Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 20

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/232543328

Repetition and frequency attenuation in lexical access

Article  in  Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition · October 1984


DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.10.4.680

CITATIONS READS

1,338 5,881

2 authors, including:

Kenneth Forster
The University of Arizona
121 PUBLICATIONS   16,860 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Kenneth Forster on 15 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1984 by the
Learning, Memory, and Cognition American Psychological Association, Inc.
1984, Vol. 10, No. 4, 680-698

Repetition Priming and Frequency Attenuation


in Lexical Access
Kenneth I. Forster and Chris Davis
Monash University, Victoria, Australia
Repetition priming effects in lexical decision tasks are stronger for low-frequency
words than for high-frequency words. This frequency attenuation effect creates
problems for frequency-ordered search models that assume a relatively stable
frequency effect. The suggestion is made that frequency attenuation is a product
of the involvement of the episodic memory system in the lexical decision process.
This hypothesis is supported by the demonstration of constant repetition effects
for high- and low-frequency words when the priming stimulus is masked; the
masking is assumed to minimize the influence of any possible episodic trace of
the prime. It is further shown that long-term repetition effects are much less
reliable when the subject is not required to make a lexical decision response to
the prime. When a response is required, the expected frequency attenuation effect
is restored. It is concluded that normal repetition effects consist of two components:
a very brief lexical effect that is independent of frequency and a long-term
episodic effect that is sensitive to frequency.

There has been much recent interest in the structure of the mental lexicon and the mech-
fact that in a lexical decision experiment, anisms of access (e.g., Morton, 1979, 1980;
where subjects are required to classify letter Scarborough, Gerard, & Cortese, 1979). On
strings as words or nonwords, there is a the other hand, it can be seen as a useful
substantial increase in both the speed and technique for studying memory processes
the accuracy of classification for words that (Jacoby & Dallas, 1981; Jaqoby & Wither-
are presented more than once during the spoon, 1982). On this view, the first presen-
experiment, even though considerable time tation of a word (the prime) establishes an
may have elapsed between successive presen- episodic memory trace mat is contacted when
tations (Forbach, Stanners, & Hochhaus, the same word (the target) is presented again.
1974; Kirsner & Smith, 1974; Scarborough, In some way this trace produces a facilitative
Cortese, & Scarborough, 1977). This phe- effect on perceptual tasks such as lexical
nomenon is referred to as the repetition effect. decision or accuracy of report with brief
There are basically two different approaches exposure. The important aspect of this ac-
to the repetition effect. On the one hand, it count for present purposes is that the repe-
is seen as the product of a temporary modi- tition effect can be explained without assum-
fication to the process of lexical access (e.g., ing that the lexical representation of the
Forbach et al., 1974). As a result of recent target word is altered in any way.
activation, the lexical representation of a These two different approaches can lead to
word is left in a state of increased accessibility. very different interpretations of the same
This priming effect can then be used as a data. Consider, for example, the problem of
powerful diagnostic tool for analyzing the cross-modal transfer. Morton (1979, 1980)
claims that there is no repetition effect at all
when the two presentations of the target word
This article is based in part on research reported by
are in different modalities. Because Morton
the second author as part of an honors thesis at Monash treats the repetition effect as a purely lexical
University. The research was supported by a grant from effect (i.e., as occurring totally within the
the Australian Research Grants Scheme. mental lexicon), he is forced to postulate that
The authors wish to thank Don Mitchell, Di Bradley, each word has a separate lexical representa-
and Don Thomson for extensive discussion and comments.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Kenneth I. tion (logogen) for each input modality. Hence,
Forster, Department of Psychology, Monash University, auditory presentation of a word leads to
Clayton, Victoria, Australia 3168. residual activation in its auditory input lo-
680
REPETITION PRIMING 681

gogen but not in its visual input logogen. 1976; Scarborough et al., 1977). This advan-
However, if repetition is seen as the result of tage for low-frequency words in episodic tasks
the influence of episodic memory traces, then may be directly responsible for the frequency
quite different conclusions are possible. For attenuation effect, perhaps because the epi-
example, it could be argued that when the sodic traces of low-frequency words can be
target word is presented visually, then only contacted more efficiently.
the memory traces of previous visual presen- From the viewpoint of theories of lexical
tations can produce a facilitative effect. Hence, access, it is therefore necessary to assess
the lack of cross-modal transfer may have no whether any particular property of the repe-
implications at all for the structure and or- tition effect reflects alterations to the organi-
ganization of the mental lexicon. The same zation of lexical entries or whether it reflects
arguments apply to the fact that there is no influences from episodic memory traces that
transfer from picture naming to lexical deci- are quite external to the lexical processor
sion. That is, saying "dog" in response to a itself. It has been argued that episodic influ-
picture of a dog produces no facilitation for ence can be ruled out on the ground that
subsequent lexical decisions on the word dog variables that normally affect the strength of
(Scarborough et al., 1979). On a purely lexical the episodic trace do not alter the repetition
interpretation, this means that quite different effect (e.g., Jacoby & Dallas, 1981; Jacoby &
logogens must be activated by pictures and Witherspoon, 1982; Scarborough et al., 1977).
by words. But on an episodic interpretation, However, these experiments might have suc-
this result may mean only that the episodic ceeded only in showing that the way in which
trace of the picture naming experience has an episodic trace is utilized might vary across
no effect in the lexical decision situation, tasks. For example, it could be that the lexical
either because the episodic trace is not acti- decision task is much more sensitive to the
vated by seeing the word dog, or because it existence of episodic memory traces than is
is not activated rapidly enough, or because it an episodic task such as old-new classifica-
is simply irrelevant. tion, and hence, trace strength is less relevant
Similar problems arise in the interpretation in the lexical decision task.
of what is referred to here as the frequency Although this evidence in inconclusive,
attenuation effect. As shown by Scarborough there is clear evidence that episodic factors
et al. (1977), low-frequency words benefit far are sometimes involved. Jacoby (1983) found
more from repetition than do high-frequency that as the proportion of words common to
words. If we adopt a purely lexical interpre- the study and test lists was increased, the
tation of this effect, then the conclusion is magnitude of the repetition effect increased.
that the accessibility of a lexical representation This is quite incompatible with a purely
is a highly dynamic, labile property. This is lexical interpretation, because the only nec-
not what would be expected from lexical essary and sufficient condition for a repetition
search theories (Becker, 1979; Forster, 1976; effect should be the previous activation of
Stanners & Forbach, 1973), which assume lexical representations. More compelling is
that the frequency effect reflects reasonably the evidence produced by Feustal, Shiffrin,
static organizational properties of the mental and Salasoo (1983). They pointed out that a
lexicon, with high-frequency words being lo- lexical view predicts no repetition effect for
cated earlier in the search path than low- nonwords because nonwords do not have
frequency words. Such theories would have lexical representations, by definition. However,
to be modified substantially to accommodate some studies have reported small effects for
the frequency attenuation effect. However, if nonwords (e.g., Besner & Swan, 1982; Kirsner
the repetition effect is not a true lexical effect, & Smith, 1974; Scarborough et al., 1977),
this may not be necessary. For example, it is although others have not (e.g., Forbach et al.,
well known that in tests of recognition mem- 1974). Feustal et al. pointed out that small
ory (old-new classification), subjects are more or non-existent effects may be expected be-
certain whether they have recently seen a cause accessing an episodic trace may well
low-frequency word than a high-frequency inhibit the no response required for nonwords.
word (e.g., Glanzer & Bowles, 1976; Gregg, By designing tasks that eliminate this prob-
682 KENNETH I. FORSTER AND CHRIS DAVIS

lem, they were able to demonstrate clear and though this masked context produced reliable
reliable repetition effects for nonwords. semantic priming effects.
This evidence suggests at least that the Masked repetition effects were successfully
repetition effect is a composite effect. How- obtained by Evett and Humphreys (1981). In
ever, there is also evidence for a stronger these experiments, a sequence of four brief
conclusion, namely, that there is no purely stimuli was presented: mask; prime, target,
lexical repetition effect. Oliphant (1983) mask. The prime and the target were either
compared the repetition effects in two situa- the same word or were different, but they
tions. In the first condition, subjects made were always presented in a different case.
lexical decisions on the same word on two Pattern masks were used, and performance
separate occasions, and normal repetition was measured by accuracy of report of the
effects were obtained. In the second condition, target stimulus. Significant repetition 'effects
the subjects were asked to read the instruc- were observed, despite the fact that the prime
tions for the experiment aloud. Some of the and target stimuli were in different cases.
words in the instructions were then presented The first experiment employed an adapta-
for lexical decision. No repetition effect was tion of the Evett and Humphreys technique
observed, despite the fact that the entries for to study frequency attenuation without epi-
these words must have been-accessed during sodic contamination. Instead of using accu-
the reading of the instructions. Oliphant's racy of report of a masked target as the
conclusion was that the repetition effect was dependent measure, lexical decision time was
not an automatic effect of recent exposure to used, and the target item was freely available.
a word, but depended on the subject's aware- If the frequency attenuation effect is a genuine
ness of repetition and the development of lexical effect, then elimination of the episodic
strategies to speed up processing of repeated trace of the prime should have no effect, that
words. is, masked repetition effects should also be
Once the possibility of episodic involve- greater for low-frequency words.
ment is conceded, we can no longer be certain
of any of the properties of a purely lexical Experiment 1
repetition effect because there is no way to Each trial consisted of a sequence of three
decide which properties are produced by the stimuli; the first two were in lowercase letters,
episodic component. The only way to remedy and the third was in uppercase letters. The
this situation is to attempt to minimize the third stimulus was the target stimulus that
contribution of episodic factors and to see had to be classified as a word or as a nonword.
what properties remain. The aim of the pres- The second stimulus was the prime and was
ent series of experiments is to examine the either identical to the target stimulus (but in
frequency attenuation effect under these con- lowercase letters) or was different. The first
ditions. stimulus was always a neutral word, unrelated
One way to minimize episodic influences to any other in the sequence. Both the first
is by masking the priming stimulus (the first and third items were presented for 500 ms,
presentation) so that subjects are unable either whereas the prime was presented for 60 ms.
to report it or to recognize it. Although this The only purpose of the first word was to act
does not necessarily mean that no episodic as a forward mask for the prime.
trace of the prime exists, it does imply that A pilot experiment using this technique
it is relatively inaccessible. Hence, any sub- established that words preceded by an iden-
sequent effect of this prime on the processing tical prime in a different case were responded
of the same word would be relatively free of to 35 ms faster than were words preceded by
episodic contamination. Precisely the same a nonidentical prime. There was no repetition
lope was successfully applied by Balota (1983) effect for nonwords; the difference was 4 ms
to the problem of contextual effects in epi- in the wrong direction. The durations of the
sodic recognition. Balota was able to show three stimuli were chosen by the experiment-
that the inhibiting effects of a change of ers so that the prime appeared invisible to
context on episodic recognition were absent them and the subjects of the pilot study. To
when the initial context was masked, even check this, two further pilot studies were
REPETITION PRIMING 683

conducted to assess the amount of informa- sequential display of the three members of a triad; each
tion available to awareness. In the first of durations member was displayed in the center of the screen. The
these additional pilot studies, subjects were whereas theof second the first and third members was 500 ms,
member was displayed for 60 ms.
told that the sequence in fact consisted of Each member was displayed immediately after the pre-
three stimuli. Their task was.merely to guess ceding member.
whether the second item contained the same uppercase The subject was instructed to classify the item in
sequence of letters as the third item. Twelve nonword. Itletters (the third member) as a word or a
was explained that this would always be the
subjects were tested with a set of 60 words final member, but no mention was made of the number
and 60 nonwords. The overall error rate was of items in the total sequence. Each trial was initiated
41%, Some subjects reported that they occa: by the subject, and feedback was provided concerning
sionally managed to see one letter in the their both accuracy and speed of response. Subjects indicated
decisions by a selective button press, with the
prime, and if the target stimulus contained preferred hand used for yes decisions. A different pseu-
that letter, they responded same. This might dorandom sequence was prepared for each subject, and
have accounted for the slightly better than a set of 10 practice items was included. Subjects were
chance performance. In the second additional given either the first or the second set of item's, so that
pilot study, the same materials were used, subject. item was responded to more than once by any
no test
but subjects were required to guess whether Subjects. A total of 28 volunteer undergraduates
the second stimulus was a word. Twelve sub- served as subjects. Any subject making more than 20%
jects were used, and it is worth noting that errors was rejected. No subject had participated in any
all subjects commented on the extreme diffi- of the pilot studies.
culty, if not absurdity, of the task. In fact,
the overall error rate was 50%, and no subject Results
made fewer than 45% errors. Hence, it seems
safe to conclude that very little precise infor- In this experiment, as in subsequent ex-
mation about the prime as a whole was periments, the effects of outliers were curtailed
available for conscious recall. by cutoffs established 2 standard deviations
above and below the mean of each subject.
Method Data from trials on which an error occurred
Materials and design. The target items consisted of were discarded. Separate analyses of the sub-
60 words and 60 legal nonwords, Half of the words were ject and item means were carried out and
high-frequency words, chosen at random from a frequency were combined to form F'min values according
range of 40-60 occurrences per million (Kucera & Francis, to the procedure recommended by Clark
1967). Half were low-frequency words, chosen at random
from a frequency range of 1-2 occurrences per minion. (1973).
These words were all judged by the experimenters to be Mean lexical decision times and error rates
within the working vocabulary of the typical subject (e.g., for both words and nonwords are shown in
ADORE, HEAVE, ARID, FROCK). A ,S6t Of 120 triads W8S Table 1. The results for the words were
constructed; the third member was one of the above analyzed in a 2 X 2 X 2 factorial design; the
target items in uppercase letters. The second member
was either the same letter sequence as the third or was a factors were groups (subject groups in the
different sequence of the same length. In this latter case, analysis of subject means, item groups in the
it was a word if the target item was a word, otherwise it analysis of item means), frequency (high vs.
was a nonword. The second member was presented in low), and repetition (repeated vs. nohre-
lowercase letters, and hence was physically different from peated).
the third in all triads. The first member was a randomly
chosen dummy word in lowercase letters. All targets were For words, lexical decision times were faster
4-?6 letters in length. (598 ms) if they were immediately preceded
A second set of 120 triads was constructed that had by the same word than if they were not (640
the same target items as the first but had different ms); this effect was significant, F' (l, 56) =
preceding items. These triads were constructed so that if min
a triad in the first set contained a repetition (e.g., absent 15.41, p < .001. There was also a significant
circle CIRCLE), its corresponding member in the second main effect of frequency; high-frequency
set did not (e.g.,; beam device CIRCLED and vice versa. words were classified faster (573 ms) than
Thus, it was possible to examine performance on the were low-frequency words (665 ms), F'mi«(l,
same set of items in both the repeated and nonrepeated
conditions. • 64) = 23.66, p < ,001. However, there was
Procedure, Items were presented on a computer- no significant interaction between these main
controlled video display. Each trial consisted of the effects; the repetition effect for high-frequency
684 KENNETH I. FORSTER AND CHRIS DAVIS

Table 1 scious report. In addition, the results suggest


Experiment 1: Mean Lexical Decision Times (KT that the nature of the priming effect is differ-
in Milliseconds) and Percentage of Errors for ent from that observed in more conventional
Repeated and Nonrepeated Items Using repetition studies where the priming stimulus
Masked Primes
is not masked (e.g., Scarborough et al., 1977).
The difference is that equal priming effects
Item type Example RT error are observed for both high- and low-frequency
words.
High-frequency The primary purpose of masking the prime
words
Repeated absent avoid 550 2.9 was to prevent the possibility that episodic
AVOID traces of the prime might influence the de-
Nonrepeated bullet taste 595 4.5 cision process. If this goal has in fact been
AVOIP achieved, then the results suggest that the
Repetition effect 45 1.6 attenuation of the frequency effect produced
Low-frequency
words by repetition can be attributed to episodic
Repeated cloud heave 646 15.3 factors.
HEAVE Before we consider the implications of this
Nonrepeated cape exalt 684 21.2 conclusion, there are a number of alternative
HEAVE
Repetition effect 38 5.9 interpretations that should be considered.
Nonwords First, it should be noted that Evett and
Repeated brass tovid 687 6.9 Humphreys (1981) interpreted their findings
TOVID in terms of graphemic priming effects. Lo-
Nonrepeated . complete astol 679 6.0 gogens are assumed to be partially activated
TOVID
Repetition effect -8 -0.9 by the presence of letters in specific positions,
regardless of visual format (case). Hence, the
logogen for STATE will be activated by prior
words (45 ms) was slightly larger than the exposure to a similar word, such as STARE,
effect for low-frequency words (38 ms, and even by prior exposure to a nonword
F'min < 1). such as STAFE. This increase in activation
Analysis of the error rates for words shows may well be a very temporary phenomenon,
the expected higher error rate for low-fre- only detectable with very short intervals be-
quency words, and although there is a trend tween the prime and the target. It may also
toward fewer errors for repeated items, this have nothing to do with the normal repetition
effect was not significant, FJnin(l", 68) = 3.25, effects observed in experiments employing
p > .05. The Frequency X Repetition inter- much longer intervals with many intervening
action was not significant. items, that is, the repetition effect observed
The results for nonwords were analyzed in in this experiment might be a much more
a 2 X 2 design; the factors were Groups and limited phenomenon. In the extreme case, it
Repetition. It is clear that nonwords do not could even be suggested that the priming
show a repetition effect, because repeated action is limited merely to the detection of
nonwords took slightly longer to classify than the graphemic form of the test stimulus, and
did nonrepeated nonwords, and repeated hence, it is not at all surprising that equal
nonwords produced slightly more errors. priming effects are observed for high- and
Neither of these effects was significant low-frequency words.
(tfnta < I)-
In order to show that the effects observed
in this experiment are genuine word-repetition
Discussion effects, it is therefore necessary to show that
the priming is not the result of a letter-
The results of this experiment confirm the repetition effect or of a graphemic priming
findings of Evett and Humphreys (1981) in effect. The letter-repetition interpretation
demonstrating that significant repetition ef- seems unlikely, because this should have pro-
fects can be obtained even when the prime duced priming effects for nonwords as well
is heavily masked and is unavailable for con- as words. However, this evidence is irrelevant
REPETITION PRIMING 685

to the graphemic priming interpretation, be- (e.g., bend-BOND, peck-PACK, dusk-oiSK). For the dis-
cause this depends on the existence of a similar pairs, the prime was chosen from the same
frequency range as the target. For nonwords, the prime
logogen corresponding to the target stimulus. was always either a similar nonword (e.g., mulp-MiLP,
The only way to test this hypothesis is to cusk-cisic, tasm-TiSM), or a dissimilar nonword (e.g.,
repeat the first experiment using graphemi- leng-QRAD, trie-STOL, nout-SAMP).
cally similar prime-target word pairs. If no As in Experiment 1, these pairs were preceded by a
facilitation is produced, then the conclusion dummy word. The lengths of all three items in each
triad were the same (4 letters). Again, two sets of
that we are dealing with a genuine word- materials were constructed so that targets preceded by a
repetition effect is strengthened. The next similar prime in one set were preceded by a dissimilar
experiment was designed to examine this prime in the other, and vice versa.
issue. Procedure. The method of presentation and testing
was exactly the same as in Experiment 1.
Subjects. A total of 24 undergraduate volunteers who
Experiment 2 had not participated in any of the preceding experiments
The priming effect observed in the first was used. Half were run with one set of materials, and
half were run with the counterbalanced version.
experiment might have been a genuine word-
repetition effect in the sense that it was due
to the repeated access of the same lexical Results
entry. Alternatively, it might simply have The mean lexical decision times for the
arisen from the graphemic overlap between target items are shown in Table 2. These
the prime and the target; this overlap could results show that there is a difference of only
have produced a letter-repetition effect, be- 1 ms (in the wrong direction) for the word
cause the same letters had to be processed targets as a function of similarity, whereas
for both stimuli, or it could have produced a for nonword targets, the effect is only 7 ms.
graphemic priming effect, in which the pro- Neither of these differences was significant
cessing of the prime produces activation in (^"min < 1). There were also no significant
the logogens of all graphemically similar effects of similarity on the error rates, Further
words. If either of these alternative explana- analysis of the results for the similar words
tions is correct, then it should be possible to condition showed no effect of the frequency
demonstrate transfer effects between similar of the prime. Words preceded by a similar
primes and targets such as lump-UMP, as prime of higher frequency were responded to
shown by Evett and Humphreys (1981) in a slightly slower (528 ms) than were words
tachistdscopic accuracy task. preceded by a similar prime of lower fre-
As in the first experiment, a lexical decision quency (520 ms), but this difference was not
task was used, and the prime was masked by significant (F'mia < 1).
its surrounding context. Priming effects for
both word and nonword targets was exam-
ined. Discussion
These results indicate that graphemic over-
Method lap between the prime and the target produces
Materials and design. A total of 60 triads was con- very little impact on lexical decision times
structed as in Experiment 1 except that instead of having for the target. This indicates that the priming
half of the primes identical to the target, they differed
from the target by one medial letter. Half of the targets effect observed in Experiment 1 was not due
were words chosen from a frequency range of 20-30 either to a letter repetition effect or to gra-
occurrences per million; half were legal nonwords. Half phemic priming. In order for the prime to
of the word targets were preceded by a briefly presented influence lexical decisions, the target must be
dissimilar prime that had no more than one letter in the
same position as the target (e.g., cure-MALE, bowl-piLE, a word (because no priming occurred for
tire-LOST), The remaining targets were preceded by a nonwords in Experiment 1), and the prime
similar prime in which only one letter differed from the must be identical to the target. These prop-
target. Because it was difficult to control the frequency erties appear to meet the requirements of a
of such similar pairs precisely, it was decided to manipulate genuine word-repetition effect.
this variable systematically. Half of the similar primes
were higher in frequency than the target (e.g., line-LANE, It should be noted that these results are in
past-POST, hall-HELL), and half were lower in frequency sharp contrast to those of Evett and Hum-
686 KENNETH I. FORSTER AND CHRIS DAVIS

Table 2 it seems much less likely that such a conflation


Experiment 2: Mean Lexical Decision Times (RT of prime and target would occur. Because the
in Milliseconds) and Percentage of Errors for only effect observed with the lexical decision
Items Preceded by Graphemically Similar task is an effect of identical repetition, we
and Dissimilar Masked Primes must assume that the same effect would have
occurred in Evett and Humphreys' experi-
Item type Example RT error ment. However, the remaining effects in their
experiment may be attributable to conflation.
Words It is also worthwhile noting that Feustal et
Similar prime harm lack 524 11.0
LOCK al. (1983) reported small but significant
Dissimilar harm bowl 523 14.2 priming effects produced by graphemic over-
prime LOCK lap of the prime and target. In this case,
Priming 1
3.2 however, the prime and target were separated
effect
Nonwords by a number of intervening items, so rather
Similar prime dirt nila 552 7.1 different mechanisms must be involved. They
NELA also used an identification accuracy task, and
Dissimilar dirt wold 559 2.1 it may be that graphemic similarity effects
prime NELA
are restricted to this type of task.
Priming 7 -5.0
effect The results of this experiment are also
relevant to studies dealing with conflicting
graphemic and phonemic overlap. While
Meyer, Schvaneveldt, and Ruddy (1974) found
phreys (1981). They found that similar primes no significant facilitation for pairs such as
produced facilitation for target identification, BRIBE-TRIBE, subsequent investigators have
whether the prime was a word or a nonword. reported such effects (Hillinger, 1980; Shul-
The most obvious difference between the man, Hornak, & Sanders, 1978). One possible
experiments is that Evett and Humphreys reason for the discrepancy may be that the
used an accuracy response measure, and the location of the difference between the prime
target word also was masked. One possible and target varies (medial in the present case,
consequence of this is that the subjects might initial in all others). However, a more: likely
have combined information from the prime reason is that strategic factors play a role in
and the target. For example, suppose that the these experiments but are most unlikely to
target word is STATE, and the subject manages be involved in the current experiment because
to identify only the last three letters. Suppose,presumably, awareness of the nature of the
also, that he has identified the first two letters
prime would be a necessary precondition for
of the prime as st. If these partial identifica- the development of a successful strategy that
tions are combined, the correct word is pro- exploited the relation between the prime and
duced, whether the prime was the identical the target.
word STATE, a similar word STARE, or even a
similar nonword STAFE. Such a combination Experiment 3
strategy need not, of course, be intentional.
This interpretation is best supported by The repetition effect observed in Experi-
the fact that priming was produced by gra- ment 1 with masked primes was insensitive
phemically similar nonwords. Furthermore, to word frequency, that is, there was no
as the authors noted, subjects occasionally attenuation of the frequency effect for re-
made errors by inserting letters from the peated items. This result suggests that when
prime into the target (e.g., responding STRING episodic involvement in the repetition effect
to the prime-target pair yoing-STRiPE). The is minimized, frequency attenuation effects
fact that this involves a change of case simply are also minimized.
indicates that subjects are using abstract This conclusion would be strengthened if
properties rather than physical properties. it could be shown that frequency attenuation
However, in a lexical decision task, where effects are found for the same experimental
the target is not masked but is freely available, items when the prime is not masked, and the
REPETITION PRIMING 687

repetition effect is produced in the normal Table3


way. This is the aim of the present experiment. Experiment 3: Mean Lexical Decision Times (RT
The design of the experiment consisted of in Milliseconds) and Percentage of Errors for
two phases. In Phase 1, words and nonwords Words and Nonwords in Phase 2 Preceded
by Primes in Phase 1
were-presented in lowercase letters for lexical
decision. In Phase 2, the same items were Item type RT % error
presented again, along with an equal number
of items that appeared for the first time. All High-frequency words
items in Phase 2 were presented in uppercase Repeated 50L 2.9
Nonrepeated 531 4.8
letters, so that there was a shift in case Repetition effect 30 1.9
between the first and second presentations. Low-frequency words
Although changes in the physical format of Repeated 552 12.4
the stimuli are not particularly critical for Nonrepeated 615 21.9
Repetition effect 63 9.5
the repetition effect (Feustal et al, 1983; Nonwords
Scarborough et al., 1977), this procedure Repeated 606 4.3
makes the conditions of this experiment more Nonrepeated 598 2.0
comparable with those of Experiment 1, Repetition effect -8 -2.3
where primes and test stimuli also differed in Note, Lexical decision required for primes.
case.
The two-phase procedure was chosen in
preference to a single list design with intralist Results and Discussion
repetition in order to minimize possible stra-
tegic effects. In the two-phase design, all The mean lexical decision times for both
primes will have been presented before any words and nonwords in Phase 2 are given in
repetition occurs, and hence the encoding of Table 3.
the primes cannot be affected by the subject's The effect of word frequency (68 ms) was
perception of repetition. However, in a single significant, F'min(l, 57) = 26.05, p < .001, as
list design, the subjects will have noted the was the effect of repetition (47 ms), F'min(l,
fact that items are repeated quite early in the 40) = 21.56, p< .001. There was also a sig-
list, and hence might attend more closely to nificant interaction between these main ef-
subsequently presented primes. fects, Fmin(l, 44) = 5.06, p < .05; high-fre-
quency words snowed a smaller repetition
Method effect (30 ms) than did low-frequency words
(63 ms). However, both of these individual
Materials and design. The experimental items were repetition effects were highly significant: for
the same as those used in Experiment t. Throughout the high-frequency words, Fmin(l, 51) = 8.85,
experiment, subjects were presented with a single word p < .01, and for low-frequency words, F'minU,
or nonword for lexical decision. The experiment consisted
of two phases. In Phase 1, subjects were presented with 39)= 17.87, p<. 001.
15 high-frequency words, 15 low-frequency words, and For nonwords, the effect of repetition was
30 nonwords, all in lowercase letters. In Phase 2, the slightly inhibitory (-8 ms), but this effect
same items were presented again in uppercase letters, was significant only in the analysis of item
along with an equal number of words and nonwords that means, F(l, 58) = 6.07, p < .05, not subject
appeared for the first time. Two sets of materials were
constructed. For both sets, Phase 2 consisted of the' same means, f{l, 26) = 3.16, p > .05. This suggests
set of 120 items. Half of these items were randomly that repetition produces a spurious familiarity
chosen for inclusion in Phase 1 for the first set, and the effect for some subjects but not all subjects.
other half were chosen for the second set. Thus, materials The error results show a very similar pat-
were counterbalanced across the Repetition factor. From
the subject's point of view, the division of the experiment tern. As expected, more errors were made on
into phases was to clearly separate the lowercase items low-frequency words than were made on high-
from the uppercase items. frequency words, FJninO, 81) = 22.75, p <
Subjects. A total of 28 volunteer undergraduates (the .001. Repetition reduced the total number of
same number as in Experiment 1) served as subjects and errors for words, Fmin(l, 57) = 6.87, p < .05,
were paid. Subjects were assigned to one set of materials
or the other in the order of their appearance at the and tended to reduce the number of errors
laboratory. for low-frequency words to a greater extent
688 KENNETH I. FORSTER AND CHRIS DAVIS

than it did for high-frequency words, although quency words were included as a control,
this interaction effect was marginal, F'mm(l, half of the subjects showed, a greater repeti-
62) = 3,40, p < .10 (both subject and item tion-induced increase in variance for low-
analyses were significant). For nonwords, frequency words than they did for high-
however, the inhibitory effect of repetition frequency words, and half showed the opposite
was very strong, f"min (1, 60) = 12.36, effect.
Masking the prime was intended to mini-
These results clearly demonstrate that the mize any possible episodic contamination of
experimental items used in Experiment 1 the repetition effect. If this was the only
produce a frequency attenuation effect when consequence of masking, then we should
the repetition effect is induced in the standard infer that frequency attenuation is a by-
manner. Hence, the absence of frequency product of an episodically mediated process.
attenuation in Experiment 1 cannot be at- However, it may be that the procedures of
tributed to item sampling errors nor can it Experiments 1 and 3 also differ in other ways.
be attributed to a lack of power, because the For example, the delay between the prime
power of this experiment was designed to be and target was only 60 ms in Experiment 1,
the same as that of Experiment 1. Hence, it whereas in this experiment, it could have
seems reasonable to conclude that the pro- ranged from 5 min to 10 min. It is therefore
cedure of masking the prime is responsible. possible that frequency attenuation only oc-
It may be suggested that masking the prime curs with long delays between presentations.
does not really alter the size of the repetition This possibility cannot be ignored, although
effect at all but instead prevents access for a it has been argued that the standard repetition
proportion of the low-frequency primes, effect is quite unaffected by:lag (e.g., Feustal
thereby lowering the average repetition effect et al., 1983; Jacoby & Dallas, 1981; Scarbor-
for low-frequency words from the 63-ms value ough et al., 1977, 1979). In addition, Berger
observed in the present experiment to the (1980) showed clear frequency attenuation
value of 38 ms observed in Experiment 1. effects when the lag in a standard repetition
This value of 38 ms represents a composite design was only one item. A more crucial
value from items where the prime was ac- difference between the two experiments is the
cessed (each producing a "normal" repetition fact that in the current experiment, subjects
effect of 63 ms) and from items where the were required to make a lexical classification
prime was not accessed (producing a repeti- of both the prime and the target, whereas in
tion effect of zero). Two features of the results the first experiment, no specific response to
suggest that this is not the correct interpre- the prime was required (nor was it possible
tation. First, it would be expected that mask- make such a requirement, because the prime
ing would also reduce the repetition effect was not visible).
for high-frequency words, although not to the This raises the interesting possibility that
same degree. However, the reverse is the case, frequency attenuation effects may be restricted
because the effect for high-frequency words to designs that require a specific decision to
with masked primes in Experiment 1 (45 ms) be made about the prime. This possibility
was greater than in the present experiment can be examined by repeating the present
(30 ms). Second, it should be the case that experiment without requiring the subject to
for low-frequency words with masked primes, respond in any way to the prime. This was
the variances of the decision times for re- attempted in the next experiment.
peated items should be much greater than
those for nonrepeated items (because the Experiment 4
former are bimodally distributed). To test The purpose of this experiment was to
this hypothesis, the item variances were cal- determine whether the frequency character-
culated separately for each subject in Exper- istics of the standard repetition effect depend
iment 1. The obtained mean variances were on the type of response that must be made
very similar: 160.7 for repeated low-frequency to the prime. The design of this experiment
words and 150.6 for nonrepeated low-fre- paralleled that of Experiment 3 in that there
quency words (12 of the 28 subjects failed to were two phases: The primes were presented
show this trend). Moreover, when high-fre- in Phase 1, and the effects of these primes
REPETITION PRIMING 689

were assessed in Phase 2. The only difference Table 4


between the experiments was that the primes Experiment 4: Mean Lexical Decision Times (PT
were not presented for lexical decision. In- in Milliseconds) and Percentage of Errors for
stead, they were presented as part of the Word Targets in Phase 1 Preceded by a Masked
Prime and for Word Targets in Phase 2 Preceded
context for another target word altogether. by a Context Prime in Phase 1
So, for example, a subject might have received
the following combination: Item type RT error
Phase 1: doctor-(annoy)-ANNOY,
Phase 2: DOCTOR. Phase 1 word targets:
Masked priming
In both phases, the task is to make a lexical Repeated 558 1.0
decision to the target in uppercase letters. Nonrepeated 610 1.6
The items in Phase 1 were presented under Repetition effect 52 0.6
the same conditions as in Experiment 1; the Phase 2 targets:
parentheses indicate the masked word. The High-frequency words
Repeated 569 5.0
question is whether the initial experience of Nonrepeated 577 4.5
doctor as a context item would serve as a Repetition effect 8 -0.5
prime for later classification of DOCTOR in Low-frequency words
Phase 2, and whether this effect would be Repeated 618 12.6
Nonrepeated 646 19.1
sensitive to frequency. Repetition effect 28 6.5
Method
The design of this experiment was similar to that of
Experiment 3. There were two phases; the primes were in Phase 1 preceded by a masked prime and
presented in Phase 1, and the targets were presented in for the word targets in Phase 2 preceded by
Phase 2, The materials and procedure for Phase 2 were an earlier presentation of the same word as
identical to Experiment 3; 60 words and 60 nonwords a context item in Phase 1.
were presented for lexical decision. The items in Phase
1 were structured in the same way as the items in Consider first the masked repetition effects
Experiment 1: two context items followed by a target during Phase 1. It is shown in Table 4 that
item that had to be classified as a word or as a nonword. primed items were classified faster (558 ms)
The duration of the first and last members of this triad than were unprimed items (610 ms). This
was 500 ms; the middle member was presented for 60 masked repetition effect of 52 ms was highly
ms. The first two members were in lowercase letters,
while the third member was in uppercase letters. significant, Fmin(l, 53) = 15.54, p< .001,
Half of the words and nonwords presented for lexical and confirms the results of Experiment 1.
decision in Phase 2 were also presented in Phase 1 as an Considering next the results in Phase 2, it
initial member of a triad (for which no response was can be seen that overall, words repeated
required). As before, two sets of materials were constructed
with a different half of the items repeated across the two across phases were responded to faster (593
phases. In addition, IS of the word targets in Phase 1 ms) than were unrepeated words (611 ms),
were preceded by a masked repetition of the same word but this 18-ms effect of•. repetition was only
in different case, and a further 15 of the word targets marginally significant, Fmin(l, 76) = 3.57,
were preceded by a different word of the same length. p< .10 (significant effects were obtained in
Items repeated in this way in the first set of materials
were nonrepeated in the second set, and vice versa. The both the subject and item analyses). High-
remaining targets in Phase 1 were irrelevant to the frequency words produced faster responses
experiment. than did low-frequency words, f'min(l, 81) =
The instructions to subjects emphasized that in Phase 25,76, p < .001, but there was no significant
1, a lexical decision was required only for the item in
uppercase letters, but subjects were asked to read the Repetition X Frequency interaction (jF'min <
material that preceded this item. It was further explained 1; nonsignificant results were obtained in
that in the second phase, only single words, and nonwords both subject and item analyses). Considering
would be presented. the repetition results for high- and low-fre-
As in Experiments 1 and 3,28 volunteer undergraduates quency words separately, there was an effect
served as subjects.
of only 8 ms for high-frequency words, which
Results and Discussion
was not significant (F'min < 1), while for low-
frequency words, there was a nonsignificant
The mean lexical decision times and error effect of 28 ms, F'mln(l, 37) - 2.77, p > .05
rates are given in Table 4 for the word targets (a significant subject effect was obtained).
690 KENNETH I. FORSTER AND CHRIS DAVIS

These results appear to indicate that the (1983), who failed to find repetition effects
normal repetition effect is very weak under when the primes were presehted as part of
the conditions of this experiment. The same the instructions for the experiment. This result
materials in Experiment 3 produced highly implies that subjects need to perceive the
significant repetition effects for both high- primes as somehow relevant; to task perfor-
frequency words and low-frequency words. mance before a strong repetition effect occurs.
Comparison across the two experiments It would perhaps be premature to conclude
showed that the effect for low-frequency words from these experiments that there is no rep-
in Experiment 3 (63 ms) was significantly etition effect with nontarget priming (more
greater than the corresponding effect with the powerful designs might well'detect a small
same items in the present experiment, Fmin(l, effect) nor would it be proper to conclude
77) = 4.14, p < .05, while the effect for high- that targets are the only adequate primes.
frequency words uv Experiment 3 (24 ms) What can be concluded is that nonlexical
did not differ significantly from the present factors are almost certainly involved in the
effect, F'minO, 76) = 2.46,> > .05, although standard repetition effect.
both subject and item analyses did produce
significant differences. Experiment 5
These results strongly suggest that the mag-
nitude of the repetition effect is influenced The masked repetition effects observed in
by the response required to the prime (espe- Experiment 1 and in Phase 1 of Experiment
cially for low-frequency words). If subjects 4 were obtained when the prime and target
are merely required to silently read the prime, were immediately adjacent. Obviously, if this
then the repetition ..effect is weak compared effect really reflects a temporary increase in
with a condition in which subjects must the accessibility of a lexical entry, then the
classify the prime. Unfortunately, this weak- same result should be obtained when other
ening of the effect makes it difficult to answer items are interpolated between the prime and
the question of central interest: Do low- target. If a masked repetition effect occurs
frequency words show the same repetition under these conditions, then several alternative
effect as high-frequency words? The lack of explanations of the effect can be eliminated.
a significant Repetition Effect X Frequency One such possibility is that the effects are
interaction is not very informative, because really due to some kind of differential masking
there is no clear repetition effect for either effect. For example, it could be that the visual
word class. similarity of the lowercase and uppercase
Nevertheless, the results of this experiment versions of the same letter is greater than the
fit quite well with the general argument that average similarity of the lowercase and up-
the standard repetition effect is contaminated percase versions of different letters. Hence,
by episodic factors. If repeated access of the "repeated" items are really successive presen-
same lexical entry was the sole determinant tations of visually similar forms. Because the
of repetition effects, then the present experi- prime presumably produces a forward mask-
ment should have produced results compa- ing effect on the target, it is possible that this
rable with those of Experiment 3, which it masking effect is less for repeated items due
clearly did not. It seems that the only way to to greater visual similarity. However, if the
resist this argument is to argue that subjects prime is no longer adjacent to the target and
did not necessarily process the context words if the target is always preceded by a constant
in Phase 1 and hence did not access the stimulus, then any such effects should be
relevant entries. But if this were the case, it eliminated.
would be hard to explain why there was such A similar argument applies to the possibil-
a strong masked repetition effect for the ity of graphemic priming effects, because
target items in Phase 1 (if the masked item these would not be expected to persist across
was processed enough to have an effect, the intervening items. It should be noted that
much more visible context item should have evidence has already been produced against
been processed also). The failure to find a both of these proposals. If suich effects oc-
clear repetition effect using nontarget primes curred, then nonwords should have shown
is consistent with the findings of Oh'phant the same repetition effects as did words in
REPETITION PRIMING 691

Experiment 1, and graphemically similar Tables


primes should have produced facilitative ef- Experiment 5: Mean Lexical Decision Times (KT
fects in Experiment 2. in Milliseconds) and Percentage of Errors for
For these reasons, it was decided to attempt Items Preceded by Identical or Different
to replicate the findings of Experiment 1 Primes Using Both Masked and
Unmasked (no mask) Primes
using nonadjacent primes and targets. Quite
apart from the arguments just presented, it Masked Unmasked
would also be desirable to replicate the in- prime prime
dependence of masked repetition effects and
frequency. , Item type RT error RT error
Target items in this experiment were pre-
ceded by five context items, with the prime High-frequency
located in Positions 2, 3, or 4. A further words
manipulation involved the duration of the Repeated 475 3.5 459 1.9
Nonrepeated 488 4.2 477 3.8
prime; This was presented either for 60 ms Repetition effect 13 0.7 18 1.9
(as in Experiment 1) or for the same duration Low-frequency words
as the other context items (500 ms). These Repeated 500 9.6 484 4.5
latter primes are, of course, perfectly visible Nonrepeated 513 13.8 519 11.2
Repetition effect 13 4.2 35 6.7
and correspond to the unmasked primes used Nonwords •
in Experiment 4. It will be recalled that these Repeated 550 9.8 548 7.9
primes did not produce a clear repetition . Nonrepeated 538 6.1 549 6.7
effect. The present experiment examines Repetition effect -12 -3.7 1 -1.2
whether the same result occurs with a much
reduced lag.
random to these conditions. In each set, there were eight
Method word conditions (Frequency X Masking X Repetition),
with 12 items in each condition. For nonwords, there
Materials and design. A total of 192 target items was were four conditions (Masking X Repetition), with 24
selected; half were words, and half were legal nonwords. items in each condition.
The words were either drawn from a high-frequency Procedure. The sequence of five context items was
range (65-90 occurrences per million) or a low-frequency presented at a rate of 500 ms per item except for the
range (below 5 occurrences per million). A context of 5 masked prime, which was presented for only 60 ms,
items was constructed for each target, consisting of 4 Unmasked primes were presented for 500 ms, as was the
words and 1 legal nonword. For all targets, either the target. The only departure from the general conditions
2nd, 3rd, or 4th context item was the prime. In repeated of Experiment 1 was that a 500-ms warning signal was
trials/the prime was the same as the target but was in a introduced immediately prior to the target, in order to
different case. For nonrepeated trials, the prime was a reduce uncertainty about which item was the target.
completely different item. For repeated nonword trials, Subjects were instructed to read passively the context
only 1 nonword was included in the context (the prime), items, and to classify the uppercase item following the
and this occurred equally often in Positions 2-4. thus, warning signal (a pair of separated angled brackets) as a
every context contained t nonword, thereby preventing word or nonword. '
the presence of a nonword from signalling to the subject Subjects. A total of 26 volunteer undergraduates
that the target item would be a nonword. served as subjects and were-paid for their participation
Context items were presented in lowercase letters, and in the experiment. Subjects were assigned to the two
target items were presented in uppercase letters. The different sets of materials in the order of their appearance
lengths of the context items were always matched to the at the laboratory.
length of the target. For repeated trials, half were desig-
nated as masked repetitions, and half were designated as Results
unmasked repetitions. Two sets of materials were con-
structed, so that materials were counterbalanced across The mean lexical decision times and error
the Repetition factor, Thus, a repeated item in one.setrates for each condition are shown in Table
was preceded by the same context in the second set
5. The results for words were analyzed sepa-
except for the repetition. For example, if the first set
contained the following repeated word item: rately for the masked and unmasked prime
contact-kitehen-chomple-trivial-dentist-KiTeHEN, theconditions and were collapsed over the posi-
second, set contained the corresponding nonrepeated word
tion of the prime. For the masked priming
item:
contact-develop-chomple-trivial-dentist-KiTCHEN, condition, there was a significant main effect
However, materials were not counterbalanced acro'ss of frequency, Fmin(l, 65) « 9.72, p < ;01, and
the Masked-Unmasked factor; items were assigned at also a significant main effect of repetition,
692 KENNETH I. FORSTER AND CHRIS DAVIS

F'min(l, 54) = 4.39, p < .05. Because exactly only observed for some subjects. A similar
the same repetition effect (13 ms) was ob- pattern of results was obtained for the errors,
served for both high- and low-frequency although none of the effects approached sig-
words, there was, of course, no interaction nificance.
between these main effects. The analysis of
errors showed the usual effect of frequency; Discussion
low-frequency words produced more errors,
•F'minO, 63) = 10.06, p < .01. However, no The results of this experiment provide a
other effects were significant or even ap- clear replication of the results of Experiment
proached significance. 1. Masked primes again produce equal rep-
For the unmasked condition, there was etition effects (though small) for high- and
again a significant main effect of frequency low-frequency words. Because these effects
on lexical decision times, Fmin(l, 64) = 10.22, were obtained when the prime and the target
p < .01. There was also a significant repetition were separated by at least one intervening
effect, .FminO, 66) = 13.93, p < .001. In this item, a graphemic priming interpretation is
case, the repetition effect for low-frequency made less plausible. In addition, we can rule
words (35 ms) was nearly twice as large as out visual interference effects because the
the effect for high-frequency words (18 ms), material immediately prior to the target is
although this interaction effect failed to reach held constant.
significance, F'mia(l, 59) = 2.11, p> .05. Sig- The second result of interest is the fact
nificance was reached for the item analysis, that the unmasked primes do not show a
F(\, 44) = 4.56, p < .05, but not for the significant frequency attenuation effect, al-
subject analysis, F(\, 24) = 3.92, p > .05. though there is an obvious tendency in this
Analysis of the errors showed significant ef- direction. Although it would obviously be
fects for frequency and repetition, but no unwise to conclude that there is no attenua-
significant interaction. tion at all, it does seem clear that the reliability
Several features should be noted. Although of the frequency attenuation effect is affected
the repetition effect with masked primes (13 by removing the requirement to respond to
ms) is once again insensitive to frequency, it the prime. This is consistent with the argu-
is much smaller than the effect observed in ment that frequency attenuation is primarily
Experiment 1 (43 ms), the first pilot study an episodic phenomenon and that episodic
(35 ms), and the effect observed in Phase 1 effects are enhanced by requiring a response
of Experiment 4 (52 ms). It could be that the to tne prime. Clearly, more evidence is re-
increase in the interval between the prime quired to determine whether context items
and the target from 60 ms in the earlier are capable of producing a standard, long-
experiments to a range of 1,060-2,060 ms in term, frequency-sensitive effect, but this goes
the present experiment is responsible. If so, beyond the scope of the present article.
then it suggests that the duration of the Finally, it should be noted that this exper-
masked repetition effect is very limited. iment raises the possibility that lag effects
The results for nonwords are shown in can be demonstrated when subjects do not
Table 5. In the masked condition, there is respond specifically to the prime. The un-
clearly no repetition effect, whereas in the masked primes in this experiment produced
unmasked case, there is a reverse effect of a highly significant repetition effect, whereas
repetition. Analysis of these results in a 2 X in Experiment 4, they did not. The most
2 X 2 factorial design, with Groups, Masking, obvious explanation is that the present ex-
and Repetition as factors, showed no signifi- periment involved a much shorter lag (an
cant main effects (F'min < 1). The Masking X average of 2 intervening words) than did
Repetition interaction effect also was nonsig- Experiment 4 (an average of 90 intervening
nificant, Fmin(l, 58) = 2.12, p > .05, although items). If this proves to be the correct expla-
the item analysis did show a significant effect, nation, then we have further evidence that
F(\, 92) - 5.40, p < .05. The failure of the the nature of the repetition effect changes
subject analysis suggests that the interference when subjects do not respond -specifically to
effect observed for unmasked primes was the prime, because the standard repetition
REPETITION PRIMING 693

effect is insensitive to lag (e.g., Feustal et al., Table 6


1983; Jacoby & Dallas* 1981; Scarborough Experiment 6: Mean Lexical Decision Times (RT
et al., 1977, 1979). in Milliseconds) and Percentage of Errors for
Similar arguments apply to the masked Words Preceded by Masked Primes at a Short
repetition effect, which we have already sug- lag (1 Item) or a Long lag (17 Items)
gested may be a very short-lived phenomenon. Item type RT % error
The next experiment attempts to establish
this point more precisely. High-frequency words
Repeated (short lag) 445 5.7
Repeated (long lag) 460 3.3
Experiment 6 Nonrepeated 458 5.0
When the masked prime occurs immedi- Low-frequency words
Repeated (short lag) 490 14.0
ately prior to the target, a much larger repe- Repeated (long lag) 501 11.7
tition effect is observed compared with the Nonrepeated 511 15.3
conditions of Experiment 5, where as many
as 3 words intervened. This suggests the
possibility that the masked repetition effect At a lag of 17 items, however, the masked
is short-lived and that if sufficient items'in- repetition effect of only 4 ms was not signif-
tervened, no repetition effect would be ob- icant (F'min < 1; no significant item or subject
served. The present experiment was designed effects were obtained). There was a significant
to test this hypothesis. Two lag values were effect of frequency, Fmin(l, 79) = 16.04, p <
used. In the first condition, 17 items were .001, but no Frequency X Repetition inter-
interpolated between the prime and the target, action (F'min < 1).
and in the second, only 1 item intervened. These results confirm the expectation that
the masked repetition effect dissipates rapidly
Method because there is no detectable effect at a lag
of 17 items (a delay of 9s). However, the
Items were constructed in the same way as in Experi-
ment 5 except that the length of the context was increased effect at a lag of 1 item (17 ms) is still well
to 19 words. The prime was located either in the second below the values obtained in Experiments 1
position (a lag of 17 items), or in the second-to-last and 4 (35-52 ms), where a lag of 0 was used.
position (a lag of 1 item). The duration of the prime was If the decay is a time-dependent process, then
always 60 ms, and the duration of all other items was this suggests that most of the decay takes
500 ms. The target items consisted of 60 high-frequency
words and 60 low-frequency words (drawn from the same place within the first second. However, it is
ranges as in Experiment S), and 120 legal nonwords. As also possible that the decay is not time-
in Experiment S, a warning signal was provided 500 ms dependent but is the result of an interference
before the onset of the target. Materials were counterbal- effect produced by the intervening items. The
anced across the factors of Repetition and Lag. Subjects present results do not permit a choice between
were told that it was important for them to read the
context items. A total of 30 volunteer undergraduates these alternatives.
served as subjects.
General Discussion
Results and Discussion
The experiments reported in this article
The results are shown in Table 6. have demonstrated that it is possible to pro-
With a short lag, a masked repetition effect of duce repetition effects in a lexical decision
17 ms was obtained. This was significant, task when the prime is masked and is un-
F'm-m(l, 68) = 7.36, p < .01. There was a sig- available for conscious report. This repetition
nificant effect of frequency, F'min(l, 79) = effect differs from the normal effect in that it
16.53, p < .001, but there was no significant is insensitive to frequency (Experiments 1, 5,
Repetition X Frequency interaction (Fmin < and 6) and is extremely short-lived (Experi-
1; no significant effects were obtained in ment 6). It is not produced by some sublexical
either the subject or item analyses). Apart process such as letter priming, because no
from a significant effect of frequency, there repetition effects were observed for nonwords,
were no other significant effects in the analysis and there was no priming effect for graphe-
of errors. mically similar words (Experiment 2). On the
694 KENNETH I. FORSTER AND CHRIS DAVIS

assumption that the main effect of masking the effect is strategic, it could be suggested
the prime is to decrease the influence of any that the locus of the episodic influence is at
possible episodic trace of the prime, it can the decision stage of the lexical decision task.
be concluded that the frequency attenuation For example, as suggested by Jacoby and
normally produced by repetition is due to Dallas (1981), the fact that a repeated item
episodic influences. accesses both a lexical entry and an episodic
Increasing the duration of the prime (and trace may increase the perceived familiarity
hence making it visible) does not necessarily of the word, which may in turn decrease
restore the frequency attenuation effect. At decision time. To explain the frequency at-
long lags (Experiment 4), the repetition effect tenuation effect, we would need to postulate
itself was so weak that there was no reliable that the familiarity effect is enhanced for
effect for either high- or low-frequency words low-frequency words. There are several pos-
considered separately, and hence no interac- sible ways in which this may occur. For
tion could be expected. At very short lags example, high-frequency words are already
(Experiment 5), a highly significant repetition highly familiar, and hence the added famil-
effect was obtained, but there was no signifi- iarity of a recent encounter may have little
cant interaction with frequency, although impact. Or, it could be that the episodic
there was an obvious trend in the expected traces of low-frequency words are either
direction., These experiments differ from the stronger or are accessed more rapidly.
standard design in that the primes are not To explain why an increase in familiarity
presented as target items but as part of the should decrease decision time, we need to
context of another irrelevant target word. show that there is some postaccess process
Hence, it appears that frequency attenuation that could be abbreviated of omitted. One
is more marked when the primes have been possibility is the postaccess orthographic
presented as target items, as was the case in check (e.g., Forster, 1976), in which a candi-
Experiment 3, where a significant frequency date entry is finally checked to confirm that
attenuation effect was obtained under the it is the correct entry. This process could be
same lag conditions as those of Experi- dispensed with altogether for repeated items,
ment 4. because it is highly unlikely that the candidate
These results can be explained by postu- entry is incorrect but also happens to be the
lating that there are in fact two types of entry for a word previously presented in the
repetition effect in lexical decision tasks: a experiment. This strategy may well depend
long-term effect and a short-term effect. The on the subject's awareness that items are
long-term effect requires that the prime be being repeated. Furthermore, the savings may
given special emphasis or salience (e.g., by be greater for low-frequency words, because
presenting the prime as a target) and is the entries for these words ;take longer to
sensitive to the frequency of the target word. access, and hence it is more;likely that the
The short-term effect requires no special sta- episodic trace will have been found in time
tus for the prime, is insensitive to frequency, to have any effect. Lexical access may be so
and dissipates rapidly. We would further argue rapid for high-frequency words that the post-
that the long-term effect is totally mediated access check is well under way before the
by episodic factors, whereas the short-term episodic trace is found.
effect is an automatic consequence of repeated An alternative proposal is that the episodic
access of the same lexical entry. This argu- trace modifies the access process itself, al-
ment implies that there are no long-term though the precise nature of such an effect is
lexical effects of repetition. This is necessary obscure. Within the framework of lexical
to explain why Oliphant (1983) obtained no search models, access time is controlled by
repetition effect at all, even though it could position within the search set,:and it is there-
be shown that the entries for the primes had fore difficult to see how the mere existence
been accessed. of an episodic trace could influence access
We now need to consider the possible time without modifying the ordering of entries
mechanisms responsible for each of these within the lexicon (the latter being ruled out
effects. Consider first the long-term effect. on the assumption that there are no long-
Following Oliphant's (1983) suggestion that term lexical effects of repetition). Without
REPETITION PRIMING 695

any strong evidence that the effect occurs From the standpoint of a search model of
during access, the decision interpretation just lexical access, one may be tempted to pos-
offered seems preferable. tulate that the entries for recently encountered
If episodic retrieval is markedly context- words are "promoted" to a higher position
sensitive, as suggested by the encoding spec- in the search path. However, as noted by
ificity principle (Tulving & Thomson, 1973), Forbach et al. (1974), this would seem to
then any change in the way in which the indicate stronger effects for low-frequency
prime and the target are presented may reduce words because high-frequency words may al-
the accessibility of the trace of the prime, ready be at the top or near the top of the
and hence may reduce the repetition effect. search set. Because the short-term effect is
Carroll and Kirsner. (1982) reported stronger insensitive to frequency, this proposal appears
repetition effects when related words were to be ruled out, as will all other explanations
presented in the same pairings on both oc- that interpret the effect in terms of the access
casions, which may be interpreted as an process. That is, the constancy of the fre-
instance of such an effect. This notion might quency effect must indicate that the access
also explain why the repetition effect was so process is unmodified, and hence the short-
weak when the prime was presented as a term effect must also be interpreted as some
context item (Experiment 4). When the target kind of postaccess effect. One possibility that
is presented, the episodic search may first meets this constraint is to suggest that im-
scan the traces of previous target items. Only mediately after an entry has been accessed,
subsequently will the traces of other words it is left in a moderately excited state so that
be examined. This may mean that the episodic information can be extracted from it more
trace of the prime is not contacted rapidly rapidly. This readout effect may be an acci-
enough to produce a reliable repetition effect. dental by-product of some neurophysiological
It is perhaps worth noting that most subjects process or it may serve some function, such
appeared to be aware of the repetition in this as decreasing the time required for other
experiment, so it could not be claimed that processing systems (such as the parser) to
the episodic trace of the prime was not extract information from the entry. The con-
accessed; it must be that it was accessed less cepts of opening and closing a file entry are
efficiently. apt analogies from computer parlance. The
If we are correct in claiming that long- task of the lexical processor is to locate the
term repetition effects are purely episodic, lexical entry appropriate to the stimulus, and
then much of the debate about the modality- to open the entry. When the contents of the
specific nature of the repetition effect loses entry are no longer required, the entry is
its significance (e,g., Morton, 1979, 1980; then closed. While an entry is in the open
Scarborough etal., 1979). Given the .contex- condition, any subsequent access of the same
tual sensitivity of episodic memory, it is no entry will be faster because the entry need
longer surprising that presenting the prime not be opened. Because this effect does not
and the target in different modalities should involve any reordering of entries and only
reduce the repetition effect. So, for example, operates after access has occurred, it should
the fact that picture naming does not facilitate be independent of frequency. Moreover, such
subsequent word naming (Scarborough et al., an effect would be expected to be extremely
1979) may indicate only that episodic retrieval short-lived, because processes such as parsing
of the picture names was relatively slow when and semantic interpretation are rapid, and
the same words were presented visually, A hence there would be no requirement to
similar argument would apply to contrasts maintain the entry in the open state beyond
between auditory and visual presentations. a period of 1-2 s.
The critical assumption is that episodic re- For the logogen model (Morton, 1980),
trieval uses features of the test stimulus situ- there is no particular difficulty in adapting
ation to direct its search, so that it first scans the account of long-term effects to cover the
episodic traces that are contextually linked short-term effect. Originally, it was assumed
to the current test situation. that discharge of a logogen leads to a tem-
We now turn to a consideration of the porary lowering of threshold proportional in
mechanism -underlying the short-term effect. size to the original threshold level. Because
696 KENNETH I, FORSTER AND CHRIS DAVIS

low-frequency words have logogens with cor- refutation of the claim that repetition effects
respondingly high thresholds, it follows that are episodic in nature. But it may be the case
larger repetition effects should occur for low- that this result shows something about the
frequency words. This account can be adapted accessibility to consciousness of episodic
to cover the short-term effect provided we traces in Korsakoff patients. Episodic traces
assume that logogen discharge can occur can be utilized effectively in carrying out
without the subject becoming aware of the certain kinds of tasks (e.g., lexical decision)
stimulus and provided we now assume that but not others (e.g., recall).
all thresholds are reduced by the same Finally, there are a number of problems
amount. However, it seems unlikely that both associated with the use of masked primes
the original account of the long-term effect that should be considered. There have been
(suitably modified to incorporate episodic a number of recent studies that have shown
influences) and this new account of the short- that masked primes are capable of producing
term effect could coexist unless some con- semantic priming effects (e.g., Balota, 1983;
vincing account is offered for proportional Humphreys, 1981; Marcel, 1980; Marcel &
threshold reduction in the case of the long- Patterson, 1978; McCauley, Parmelee, Sper-
term effect but constant reduction for the ber,'& Carr, 1980). Much of the controversy
short-term effect. surrounding these studies centers on the
As mentioned earlier, any episodic inter- problem pf deciding whether subjects were
pretation of the long-term repetition effect really unaware of the priming stimulus (e.g.,
must explain why some variables that affect Merikle, 1982). Thus, it might be suggested
the strength of the episodic trace do not affect that in the present series of experiments, the
the strength of the repetition effect (e.g., masking effect was not complete and that
Jacoby & Dallas, 1981; Jacoby & Wither- subjects in fact could have detected the
spoon, 1982; Scarborough et al., 1977). Part primes, especially after practice. This point
of the explanation may be a question of is actually irrelevant to the argument, because
timing. Episodic traces must be available all that is necessary is to argue that the mask
quickly enough to influence lexical decisions, radically reduces the accessibility of the epi-
but this property may be irrelevant in an sodic trace of the prime. However, leaving
old-new classification task. Part of the expla- this aside, it should be noted that in the pilot
nation may be a question of sensitivity. For studies (Experiment 1) where subjects at-
example, trace strength may be critical in tempted to guess the properties of the prime,
discriminating whether an item has occurred the same opportunities for improvement as a
during the experiment or on some prior function of practice were available, yet no
occasion, but for the lexical decision task, evidence of any ability to detect anything
any trace strength above some minimal value more than the most superficial aspects of the
may be sufficient to produce an effect. Hence, prime was obtained. It will be noted that the
the evidence suggesting that the long-term duration of the prime in the present experi-
effect is not episodic may only indicate that ments (60 ms) is rather longer than in previous
trace strength has different effects in different studies (less than 20 ms). This is most likely
tasks. It should be noted that the evidence is due to the fact that a combination of forward
not all negative, because there are some par- and backward masking was: used here. It
allel effects for episodic and perceptual tasks should be stressed that the masking effects
(Jacoby, 1983). In addition, the evidence are in part a function of the duration of the
from Experiments 4 and 5 suggests a further material presented before and after the prime
parallel, namely that lag effects may occur (500 ms). If these words are\ also displayed
when context primes are used. for 60 ms, then the prime becomes much
Of particular interest is the finding that more visible.
Korsakoff patients show repetition effects de- But the important point to note is that the
spite their apparent inability to recall any impact of the evidence reported here does
details of the initial presentation (Berger, not depend on the claim that subjects were
1980; Jacoby & Withenipoon, 1982). On the totally unaware of the prime. Even if it could
face of it, this fact appears to be a striking be shbwn that they were partially aware (or
REPETITION PRIMING 697

even totally aware) of the prime, it would Evett, L. J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1981). The use of
still be necessary to explain why the frequency abstract graphemic information in lexical access.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33,
attenuation effect is absent under these con- 325-350.
ditions. Feustal, T. C., Shiffrin, R. M., & Salasoo, A. (1983).
It is possible that masking is actually irrel- Episodic and lexical contributions to the repetition
evant to the argument. It may turn out that effect in word identification. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 112, 309-346.
subsequent experiments show that context Forbach, G. B., Stanners, R. E, & Hochhaus, L. (1974).
items in general fail to produce frequency Repetition and practice effects in a lexical decision
attenuation effects, whether masked or un- task. Memory & Cognition, 2, 337-339.
masked. The results of Experiment 5 are not Forster, K. I, (1976). Accessing the mental lexicon. In
decisive on this point: Although the repetition R. J. Wales & E. Walker (Eds.), New approaches to
language mechanisms (pp. 257-287). Amsterdam:
effect for low-frequency words was almost North-Holland.
twice as large as the effect for high-frequency Glanzer, M., & Bowles, N. (1976). Analysis of the word
words, this difference was not reliable (it is frequency effect in recognition memory. Journal of
perhaps worth adding that precisely the same Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Mem-
result was obtained in an earlier experiment ory, 2, 21-31.
Gregg, V. (1976). Word frequency, recognition and recall.
that is omitted for space reasons here). How- In J. Brown (Ed.), Recall and recognition. London:
ever, it should be stressed that even this is a Wiley.
peripheral issue, because the argument con- Humphreys, G. W. (1981). Direct vs. indirect tests of
cerning diminished accessibility of episodic the information available from masked displays: What
visual masking does and does not prevent. British
traces could be applied equally well to this Journal of Psychology, 72, 323-330.
case. Hillinger, M. L. (1980). Priming effects with phonemically
Clearly, a complete theory of the repetition similar words: The encoding-bias hypothesis reconsid-
effect will require resolution of these issues ered. Memory & Cognition, 8, 115-123.
and many others. But this should not deflect Jacoby, L. L. (1983). Perceptual enhancement: Persistent
effects of an experience. Journal of Experimental Psy-
attention from the central point of the present chology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 21-38.
argument, which is that the repetition effect Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship
should not automatically be taken as a purely between autobiographical memory and perceptual
lexical effect, and in particular, that the fre- learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
quency attenuation effect produced by repe- 110, 306-340.
Jacoby, L. L., & Witherspoon, D. (1982). Remembering
tition does not undermine the notion of a without awareness. Canadian Journal of Psychology,
frequency-ordered search process. 36, 300-324.
Kirsner, K., & Smith, M. C. (1974). Modality effects in
word identification. Memory & Cognition, 2, 637-640.
References Kucera, J., & Francis, W. N. (1967). Computational
Balota, D. A. (1983). Automatic semantic activation and analysis of present day American English. Providence,
episodic memory encoding. Journal of Verbal Learning RI: Brown University Press.
and Verbal Behavior, 22, 88-104. Marcel, A. J. (1980). Conscious and preconscious rec-
Becker, C. A. (1979). Semantic contexts and word fre- ognition of polysemous words: Locating the selective
quency effects in visual word recognition. Journal of effects of prior verbal context. In R. S. Nickerson
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Per- (Ed.), Attention and performance VIII (pp. 435-457).
formance, 5, 252-259. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Berger, J. (1980). Determinants of verbal and non-verbal Marcel, A. J., & Patterson, K. E. (1978). Word recognition
recognition performance in Korsakoff syndrome. Un- and production: Reciprocity in clinical and normal
published honor's thesis, Monash University, Victoria, studies. In J, Requin (Ed.), Attention and performance
Australia. VII (pp. 209-226). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Besner, D., & Swan, M. (1982). Models of lexical access McCauley, C., Parmelee, C. M., Sperber, R. D., & Carr,
in visual word recognition. Quarterly Journal of Ex- T. H. (1980). Early extraction of meaning from pictures
perimental Psychology, 34, 313-325. and its relation to conscious identification. Journal of
Carroll, M., & Kirsner, K. (1982). Context and repetition Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Per-
effects in lexical decision and recognition memory. formance, 6, 265-276.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, Merikle, P. M. (1982). Unconscious perception revisited.
55-69. Perception & Psychophysics, 31, 298-301.
Clark, H. H. (1973). The language-as-ftxed-effect fallacy: Meyer, D. E., Schvaneveldt, R. W, & Ruddy, M. G.
A critique of language statistics in psychological re- (1974). Functions of graphemic and phonemic codes
search. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, on visual word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 2,
12, 335-359. 309-321.
698 KENNETH I. FORSTER AND CHRIS DAVIS

Morton, J. (1979). Facilitation in word recognition: ex- tition effects across task and modality. Memory &
periments causing change in the logogen model. In Cognition, 7, 3-12.
P. A. Kolers, M. E. Wrolstad, & M. Bouma (Eds.), Shulman, H. G., Hornak, R., & Sanders, E. (1978). The
Processing of visible language (pp. 259-268). New effect of graphemic, phonetic, and semantic relation-
York: Plenum Press. ships on access to lexical structures. Memory & Cog-
Morton, J. (1980). The logogen model and orthographic nttion, 6, 115-123.
structure. In U. Frith (Ed.), Cognitive processes in stanners, R. F., & Forbach, G. B> (1973). Analysis of
spelling (pp. 117-135). London: Academic Press. _ ietter ^1^ ;„ wor<i recognition. Journal ofExperi-
Oliphant, G. W. (1983). Repetition and recency effects mental Psychology, 98, 31-35.
n
- ^a*8""" Jownal of Penology, Tfrfri ^ & Thoms(jni D M (1573) Encodjng sped.
fidtv and rettieval
rw*«. r x, c^,tv,m,.oi, H processing in episodic memory.
Gortese, C, & Scarborough, H Psychological Review, 80, 352-373.
(1977). Frequency and repetition effects in lexical
memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 3, 1-17.
Scarborough, D. L., Gerard, L., & Cortese, C. (1979). Received April 27, 1983
Accessing lexical memory: The transfer of word repe- Revision received April 20, 1984 •

Instructions to Authors

The journal publishes long, integrative articles containing multiple experiments and extensive theoretical
development, including quantitative models and computer simulations where appropriate. Alternative con-
tributions may be quite briefer very long and could include letters, comments on previous articles or books,
problems with theoretical conclusions or empirical designs of earlier publications, replies to criticism, and
brief reports of new work in cases where this new work will not be published in other journals of general
circulation. For further information on content, authors should refer to the editorial in the January 1983
issue of the Journal (Vol. 9, No. 1, pp. 1-2). For information on the other three JEP journals, authors
should refer to editorials in those journals.
Authors should prepare manuscripts according to the Publication Manual of the American Psychological
Association (3rd ed.). All manuscripts must include an abstract of 100-150 words typed on a separate sheet
of paper. Typing instructions (all copy must be double-spaced) and instructions on the preparation of tables,
figures, references, metrics, and abstracts appear in the Manual. Also, all manuscripts are subject to editing
for sexist language.
APA policy prohibits an author from submitting the same manuscript for concurrent consideration by
two or more journals. APA policy also prohibits duplicate publication, that is, publication of a manuscript
that has already been published in whole or in substantial part in another journal. Also, authors of manuscripts
submitted to APA journals are expected to have available their raw data throughout the editorial review
process and for at least 5 years after the date of publication.
Blind reviews are optional, and authors who wish blind reviews must specifically request them when
submitting their manuscripts. Each copy of a manuscript to be reviewed blind should include a separate
title page with authors' names and affiliations, and these should not appear anywhere else on the manuscript.
Footnotes that identify the authors should be typed on a separate page. Authors should make every effort
to see that the manuscript itself contains no clues to their identities.
Manuscripts should be submitted in quadruplicate (the Original and three photocopies), and all copies
should be clear, readable, and on paper of good quality. Authors should keep a copy of the: manuscript to
guard against loss.
Mail manuscripts and address all correspondence at all stages of handling to the Editor-elect, Henry L.
Roediger III, Department of Psychology, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana 47907.
For the other JEP journals, authors should submit manuscripts (also in quadruplicate) to one of the
editors at the following addresses: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, Sam Glucksberg, Department
of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey 08544; Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Human Perception and Performance, William Epstein, Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin,
W. J. Brogden Psychology Building, 1202 West Johnson Street, Madison, Wisconsin 53706; Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, Donald S. Blough, Department of Psychology, Brown
University, Providence, Rhode Island 02912. When one of the editors believes a manuscript is clearly more
appropriate for an alternative journal of the American Psychological Association, the editor may redirect
the manuscript with the approval of the author.

View publication stats

You might also like