Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Child and Family Studies (2018) 27:2999–3007

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-018-1130-4

ORIGINAL PAPER

Family Socio-Economic Status and Parent-Child Relationships Are


Associated with the Social Creativity of Elementary School Children:
The Mediating Role of Personality Traits
Dongjing Zhang1 Zongkui Zhou1,2 Chuanhua Gu1 Yuju Lei2 Cuiying Fan1
● ● ● ●

Published online: 19 June 2018


© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
Based on a sample of 955 elementary school children in central China, the present study examined family socio-economic
status and parent-child relationships in relation to children’s social creativity, as well as the mediating role of personality
traits in these associations. Family environment (socio-economic status and parent-child relationships) and personality traits
were significantly correlated with social creativity; children openness, conscientiousness, agreeableness and extraversion
1234567890();,:
1234567890();,:

partly mediated the relationship between family environment and social creativity when controlling for age and sex. These
results are important for explaining the roles of the family environment and individual factors in shaping children’s social
creativity.
Keywords Social creativity Family socio-economic status Parent-child relationship Personality trait Elementary school
● ● ● ●

children

Introduction Chesnokova and Subbotsky 2014; Gu et al. 2003, 2015,


2016; Mouchiroud and Bernoussi 2008; Mouchiroud and
Creativity, as a complex and diverse concept, not only Lubart 2002; Song et al. 2013).
reflects the individual act of producing novel solutions Social creativity, as an umbrella term for a number of
(Kaufman et al. 2011; McCrae 1987; Runco and Acar 2010; different forms of creativity applied in interactive situations
Sternberg and Lubart 1996; Torrance 1962), but also shapes (Mouchiroud and Lubart 2002; Mouchiroud and Bernoussi
the exchange of ideas in science, art and other areas of 2008), refers to an ability to solve social problems or
social life (Feist 1999; Furnham et al. 2006; Silvia et al. enhance social activities in a novel, unique, appropriate and
2009; Velázquez et al. 2014). As a kind of creativity, social effective way (Gu et al. 2003; Gu et al. 2016). It is well
creativity refers to is creative ability in the social domain. established that the development of social creativity is a
This type of creativity can be found in every aspect of daily very important element of good interpersonal relationships
life, which plays a key role in social and cultural change, and of adaptation to the changing society (Becker 2012;
and has been the focus of recent research (Becker 2012; Chesnokova and Subbotsky 2014; Gu et al. 2008b, 2009b,
2012). According to the Ecology of Human Creativity
Theory, creativity was influenced by both individual and
environmental factors (Harrington 1990).
* Zongkui Zhou Children’s social environments contribute to their cog-
zhouzk@mail.ccnu.edu.cn
nitive, social and emotional development, and interactions
* Chuanhua Gu
guchuanhua@mail.ccnu.edu.cn
with the social environment may promote or hinder social
creativity (Fearon et al. 2013; Soroa et al. 2015). Family
1 socio-economic status (SES) is one “objective” family
Key Laboratory of Adolescent Cyber Psychology and Behavior
(CCNU), Ministry of Education; School of Psychology, Central factor that may influence social creativity (Dai and Shen
China Normal University, 430079 Wuhan, China 2008; Gu et al. 2003, 2012; Niu 2007). According to Family
2
Collaborative and Innovative Center for Educational Technology, Investment Theory, families’ socio-economic status is a
Central China Normal University, 430079 Wuhan, China comprehensive reflection of their overall economic, labor,
3000 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2018) 27:2999–3007

and social capital. The family stress model (Conger et al. may in turn, enhance children’s social problem solving
2000; Conger et al. 2002) suggests that the presence or ability. Much less research has focused on parent-child
absence of financial resources may affect developmental relationships in relation to children’s social creativity. Gu
outcomes for children and families, as low finance level et al. (2012) found a significant association between
may lead to more family conflicts, resulting in children’s parent–child relationships and social creativity, namely,
unstable emotionality and more pressures in their daily life. father’s over-protection and mother’s refusal and denial
Therefore, socio-economic status has a direct bearing on were associated with lower social creativity, and mother’s
children’s physical well-being, their ideas and attitudes as emotional warmth was associated with higher social crea-
well as their psychological growth (Currie et al. 1997; tivity in elementary school children.
Evans and Kim 2007; Matthews and Gallo 2011). According to Family Stress Model, high socio-economic
Empirical studies have found that parents with high status may release some family pressure, which can help
socio-economic status were more likely to provide a posi- children avoid parent-child conflict and bring more oppor-
tive learning environment (Carvalho 2016; Su et al. 2015) tunities for children to explore. Thus, it is important to
and a more diversified recreational environment (e.g., social determine whether family’s socio-economic status and
activities, interpersonal resources) for their children, to parent-child relationships are correlated in our sample and
consciously cultivate children’s creative ability (Gu et al. then how they are associated together and separately with
2012). By contrast, children from families with low socio- children’s social creativity.
economic status would be more likely to face more pres- Researchers have long been interested in the association
sures and difficulties(Conger et al. 2000; Conger et al. 2002; between individual factors and children’s psychological
Conger et al. 2010) because of the lack of good education growth (Miller et al. 2000). One of these individual factors
resources, accompanied by more depression, anxiety and is the creative personality. Some researchers have found that
other negative emotions, factors that might be harmful for individuals with a highly creative personality tended to have
the development of social creativity (Amabile et al. 2005; more creative performance than those with low levels of
Dai and Shen 2008; Su et al. 2015; Taylor et al. 2014). creative personality (Feist 1999; Runco and Charles 1997).
Research has shown a close connection between family’s Creative personality has also been shown to play a partially
socio-economic status and creativity (Dai and Shen 2008; Dai mediating role between family environment and creative
et al. 2012). However, the role of socioeconomic status in ability (Tang et al. 2014). One problem with this line of
relation to social creativity has been less studied. This ques- research is that creativity ability is likely part of the defi-
tion is particularly interesting in a country such as China, nition of the creative personality; this circularity makes it
where there are remarkable SES differences between rural difficult to interpret results in this area. Moreover, several
families and urban families. Research in china has shown that Big Five traits have been shown to be associated with
social creativity of rural students was significantly lower than creativity. For example, it is well established that openness
that of urban students (Gu et al. 2009b; Song et al. 2013). It is positively correlated with individual creativity in several
should be clarified whether social creativity is related to other creative domains such as creative thinking, artistic and
markers of socio-economic status in current China. scientific creativity (Feist 1999; Silvia et al. 2008, 2009).
In addition to family social-economic, many studies Conscientiousness also has been shown to be positively
suggest that parent-child relationships, as an important correlated with scientific and general creativity (Feist 1999;
family factor, also have important influences on children’s Furnham et al. 2006), whereas neuroticism is negatively
general creativity (Dasgruptas and Sonthallas 2011; Fearon correlated with social creativity (Gu et al. 2008b).
et al. 2013; Mouchiroud and Bernoussi 2008). According to Although a great many studies indicate that personality
the ecological model of authoritative parenting and ado- traits are generally stable, they are relatively changeable or
lescent adjustment (Steinberg et al. 1995), family relation- variable rather than absolutely stable (Roberts and Del-
ship influences children’s adjustment by parenting styles Vecchio 2000; Wängqvist et al. 2015). Lots of research
(Fearon et al. 2013; Guo and Wu 2011). Specifically, have also revealed that personality traits were moderately
authoritative parenting was positively associated with chil- stable(Carvalho 2016; Ilies and Judge 2003; Karmakar et al.
dren’s competence and maladaptation. Children with good 2017) and they were often influenced by environment fac-
parent-child relationship is thought to perceive more social tors such as gender, parenting styles and attachment styles
support from their parents, which increases their adjustment (Carvalho 2016; Guo and Wu 2011; Miller et al. 2000;
skills by overcoming social problems and the accompany- Münch et al. 2016). This is especially true for young indi-
ing frustrations and negative moods (Suldo and Huebner viduals who are in a constant process of growth and
2004). The increased adjustment skills can further promote development (Karmakar et al. 2017; Roberts and DelVec-
the development of their emotional regulation skills chio 2000). Sun et al. (2014) further found personality
(Lewallen and Neece 2015; Morris et al. 2011). These skills played a mediating role between adolescents’ creative
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2018) 27:2999–3007 3001

thinking and their effects on creative behavior, and con- schools during the autumn semester. We sampled four to
scientiousness especially, mediated the relationship between seven classes each from grade 4 to grade 6. Participants
family factors and adolescents’ social development (Andres were asked to complete online questionnaires consisting of
et al. 2014; Guo and Wu 2011). According to the ecological the study measures listed below. A total of 1018 students
system theory, family environment shapes many aspects of completed the questionnaire. After excluding 63 students
children’s development, perhaps including the expression of with invalid data, data from 955 children (531 boys and 424
personality (Csikszentmihalyi 2006; Dai et al. 2012). Per- girls) were analyzed. Their ages ranged from 8 to 13 years
sonality, in turn, would be expected to predict creativity. old (mean age = 10.26 years, SD = 0.94 years).
It is important to take into account children’s age and
gender when examining the relations among family envir- Procedure
onment (SES and parent-child relationships), children per-
sonality, and social creativity. Several studies suggest In order to standardize the data collection process, all
significant change in children’s social creativity over the examinations needed to go through a unified training ses-
course of the elementary school years. Mouchiroud et al. sion priority. Oral consent was obtained from school
(2002; 2008) found a significant grade difference in fluency administrators and participants before data collection. We
and originality in elementary school children’s social crea- administered the questionnaire by class; that is, we took
tivity. Other research on social creativity in real inter- classes one by one to the computer room of the school. All
personal conflicts found that age was positively correlated children participated in the survey voluntarily, and they
with social creativity, defined in terms of fluency, versati- could withdraw at any time. Participants answered all
lity, curiosity, complexity, and risk-taking. Gender is also questionnaires on the school’s online psychological eva-
found to be correlated with children’s creativity (Dasgruptas luation system.
and Sonthallas 2011).
Based on the above theoretical analyses and existing Measures
literature, we put forward the following hypotheses: First,
we hypothesized that higher family SES, and positive Family socio-economic status
parent-child relationships would be positively associated
with a high level of social creativity. Second, we hypothe- The Family Affluence Scale (FAS; Currie et al. 1997) was
sized that children’s personality traits would be associated used to assess socio-economic status (SES). This scale
with children’s social creativity. Third, we hypothesized avoids too many missing values on the traditional measures
that children’s personality traits would mediate the rela- of SES such as family income or parental education. The
tionship between family factors (SES, parent-child rela- FAS was developed as a measure of family SES (Van and
tionships) and social creativity. The proposed integrated Vedder 2011) for the Health Behavior in School-Aged
model is illustrated in Fig. 1. Children (HBSC) surveys. The FAS includes four items:
Does your family own a car, van, or truck? (Yes, two or
more = 2; Yes, one = 1; No = 0); During the past
Method 12 months, how many times did you travel away on holiday
with your family? (More than twice = 3; Twice = 2; Once
Participants = 1; Not at all = 0); How many computers does your family
own? (More than two = 3; Two = 2; One = 1; None = 0);
The research was conducted in central China. We recruited and Do you have your own bedroom for yourself? (Yes = 1;
participants from two representative normal elementary No = 0). Scores on these items were summed to a total
score, with higher scores indicating higher family SES. This
scale has demonstrated good reliability and validity among
Chinese students (Liu et al. 2012; Chen et al. 2016).
Cronbach’s α of the scale was 0.54.

Parent-child relationship

The Parent-Child Relationship Scale was used to assess


parent-child relationships (Buchnan et al. 1991). This scale
Fig. 1 The mediating effect of personality on the relationship between includes nine items that evaluate mother-child and father-
family environment (socio-economic status (SES), parent-child rela- child relationship separately such as “how often does your
tionships) and social creativity mother express the feeling of how much she loves you?”
3002 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2018) 27:2999–3007

and “how openly do you talk to your mother?” Participants et al. 2013; Zhang et al. 2013). In the current study,
responded on a 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = Cronbach’s α for the six dimensions were 0.83, 0.78, 0.77,
strongly agree). The mean scores were calculated for each 0.70, 0.60, and 0.70, respectively.
participant, with higher values indicating a stronger parent-
child relationship. Cronbach’s α of the Parent-Child Rela- Data Analyses
tionship for the present sample was 0.91(father-child) and
0.90 (mother-child). First, we conducted descriptive statistics for children’s
social creativity, and the correlation analysis of the asso-
Children’s personality ciations of children’s social creativity with family envir-
onment (SES and parents-child relationships) and
Children’s personality was measured by the Chinese Five personality. Second, we followed the path analysis to
Personality Inventory for Children (Zhou et al. 2000). It establish the mediation effect of personality, which requires
consists of 50 items assessing five personality dimensions: (a) a significant association between the independent vari-
extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, openness, able (i.e., SES and parents-child relationships) and the
and neuroticism. Openness refers to imagination or intel- mediator variable (i.e., personality); and (b) a significant
lectual curiosity (e.g., “I have a lot of imagination”); Con- association between the mediator variable (i.e., personality)
scientiousness refers to attention, willingness to work hard, and the dependent variable (i.e., social creativity) after
or order (e.g., “I am very diligent and always try to study controlling for the independent variable (i.e., SES and
and work”); Extraversion refers to activity or facility with parents-child relationships). To further illustrate the med-
other people (e.g., “I like to play with my classmates”); iation paths between family environment (i.e., SES and
Agreeableness refers to friendliness, empathy, or kindness parents-child relationships) and children’s social creativity,
(e.g., “I can understand and appreciate others”); and Neu- we followed the bootstrapping procedure proposed by
roticism assesses anxiety, anger, or negative mood (e.g., “I Hayes (2013) in SPSS20.0.
feel dejected after a small failure”). Children rated each item
on a 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly
agree). An average score for each dimension was calculated, Results
with higher scores indicating a higher level of a specific
personality trait. This measure has demonstrated good As shown in Table 1, the social creativity of Chinese ele-
reliability and validity among Chinese elementary school mentary school children was in the mid to upper level (M =
children (An et al. 2013; Zhang et al. 2006). In the current 2.21, between 1 point and 3 points), and the means of the
study, Cronbach’s α for the five subscales were 0.83, 0.85, six dimensions could be listed from high to low as follows:
0.88, 0.82, and 0.77, respectively. communicative skills, initiative, problem solving ability,
strong persistence and drive, eminence and peer influence.
Children’s social creativity There were significant grade differences in total social
creativity score (F = 4.73, p < 0.01), initiative (F = 1.13, p
The Social Creativity Questionnaire for Elementary School < 0.05), problem solving ability (F = 1.18, p < 0.05),
Children was used to assess social creativity (Gu et al. determination aggressive (F = 1.00, p < 0.05), and emi-
2008a). This questionnaire consists of 24 items and six nence (F = 1.61, p < 0.01). Further post-hoc tests showed:
dimensions: peer influence (five items, e.g., “I am very the total social creativity of fifth-graders was significantly
popular with my classmates”), problem solving ability (five higher than that of the fourth- graders(t = 0.08, p < 0.05)and
items, e.g., “I take the initiative to solve the interpersonal sixth-graders(t = 0.10, p < 0.01); the Initiative of fifth-
conflict”), eminence (four items, e.g., “I am good at graders was significantly higher than that of the sixth-
expressing myself”), strong persistence and drive (four graders(t = 0.14, p < 0.01); the Strong persistence and drive
items, e.g., “I’m not easy to give up”), communicative skills of fifth-grade was significantly higher than that of the sixth-
(three items, e.g., “I feel easy to interact with my class- graders(t = 0.12, p < 0.01); the problem solving ability of
mates”), and initiative (three items, e.g., “I am well fifth-graders was significantly higher than that of the fourth-
responsible for various activities in school life”). Children graders(t = 0.08, p < 0.05); the eminence of sixth-graders
answered these items on a 3-point scale (1 = no, 2 = were significantly lower than that of the fourth- graders(t =
sometimes, 3 = yes). The participants’ responses on each −0.10, p < 0.05) and fifth graders(t = −0.16, p < 0.01).
item were averaged to form a scale score, with higher scores The descriptive statistics and correlations for all variables
indicating higher levels of social creativity. This measure are shown in Table 2. Social creativity was significantly
has demonstrated good reliability and validity among Chi- correlated with family socio-economic status, parent-child
nese elementary school children (Gu et al. 2008b; Song relationship, and each personality trait.
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2018) 27:2999–3007 3003

Table 1 Descriptive statistics


Variables Grade M ± SD F
and ANOVA (N = 955)
4 5 6

Communicative skills 2.35 ± 0.51 2.42 ± 0.48 2.39 ± 0.46 2.38 ± 0.49 2.73
Initiative 2.29 ± 0.54 2.35 ± 0.50 2.22 ± 0.54 2.30 ± 0.53 4.05*
Problem solving ability 2.24 ± 0.51 2.33 ± 0.48 2.21 ± 0.50 2.27 ± 0.50 4.73**
Strong persistence and drive 2.19 ± 0.51 2.25 ± 0.49 2.13 ± 0.50 2.20 ± 0.50 3.97*
Eminence 2.15 ± 0.54 2.22 ± 0.55 2.06 ± 0.55 2.15 ± 0.55 5.35**
Peer influence 2.00 ± 0.54 2.09 ± 0.53 2.04 ± 0.54 2.04 ± 0.54 2.38
Social creativity 2.19 ± 0.42 2.26 ± 0.42 2.16 ± 0.43 2.21 ± 0.42 4.73**
Note: *p < 0.05, **
p < 0.01

Table 2 Descriptive statistics


M ± SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
and correlations among
variables SC 2.21 ± 0.42 1
SES 5.06 ± 2.10 0.27*** 1
PCR 3.44 ± 0.86 0.38*** 0.20*** 1
Extraversion 3.66 ± 0.80 0.53*** 0.26*** 0.31*** 1
*** *** ***
Agreeableness 3.87 ± 0.76 0.52 0.23 0.34 0.70*** 1
*** *** ***
Conscientiousness 3.67 ± 0.82 0.57 0.23 0.35 0.66*** 0.81*** 1
*** *** *** ***
Openness 3.60 ± 0.81 0.54 0.32 0.34 0.73 0.70*** 0.73*** 1
Neuroticism 2.95 ± 0.85 −0.13*** 0.01 −0.06* 0.10** 0.06 0.04 0.16** 1
Note: N = 995; *p < 0.05,**p < 0.01, ***
p < 0.001
SC social creativity, SES family socio-economic status, PCR parent-child relationship

To examine the relationships among family SES, parent- creativity decreased significantly once personality traits
child relationship, and social creativity reflected in the were included as mediators (β = 0.21, p < 0.01); the direct
model above, we first conducted a path analysis in Amos effect of family SES on personality was significant (β =
17.0. Multiple fit indices were used to assess model fit, 0.19, p < 0.01), the effect of personality on social creativity
including chi-square with its degrees of freedom, com- was significant (β = 0.55, p < 0.01), and the path coefficient
parative fit index (CFI), goodness of fit index (GFI), non- of family SES’s effect on social creativity decreased sig-
normed fit index (NNFI), standardized root mean square nificantly once personality traits were included as mediators
residual (SRMR), and root mean square error of approx- (β = 0.01, p < 0.01). Thus, personality might mediate the
imation (RMSEA; Hu and Bentler 1999). Our model relationships between family SES, parent-child relation-
demonstrated sufficient fit to the data, χ2/df = 2.76, ships, and social creativity.
RMSEA = 0.04, GFI = 0.98, NNFI = 0.98, CFI = 0.99, The bootstrapping procedure in SPSS was used to test
SRMR = 0.01. The direct effect of family socio-economic the mediating effect of personality (Hayes 2013). This
status on social creativity (β = 0.11, p < 0.01) and the direct method produced 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals
effect of parent-child relationship on social creativity (β = of these effects from 1000 resamples of the data. Con-
0.46, p < 0.01) were both significant. fidence intervals that do not contain zero indicate
To examine whether personality mediated the relation- effects are significant at α = 0.05. The results are shown in
ship between family SES, parent-child relationships, and Table 3. Extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and con-
social creativity, we conducted a path analysis in Amos scientiousness significantly mediated the relationship
17.0. The mediated association model demonstrated suffi- between family SES and social creativity, and the ratio of
cient fit to the data, χ2/df = 5.77, RMSEA = 0.07, GFI = indirect effect of these personality traits to total effect was
0.94, NNFI = 0.94, CFI = 0.95, SRMR = 0.04. Indirect 60.14, 77.83, 58.30, and 51.53%, respectively. Extraver-
effect path analyses are shown in Fig. 2. The direct effect of sion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness also
parent-child relationship on personality was significant (β significantly mediated the relationship between parent-
= 0.25, p < 0.01), the effect of personality on social crea- child relationships and social creativity, and the ratio of
tivity was significant (β = 0.55, p < 0.01), and the path indirect effect to total effect was 37.12, 42.24, 45.54, and
coefficient of parent-child relationship’s effect on social 39.65%, respectively.
3004 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2018) 27:2999–3007

Fig. 2 The structural equation


model of family SES, parent-
child relationships, and
personality traits as predictors of
social creativity. Standardized
coefficients are presented. All
parameter estimates in this figure
were significant (p < 0.05).
Children’s gender and age were
included in the equations as
statistical controls but are not
presented for reasons of
simplicity. Note: The brackets
represent the path coefficient
before the mediator of
personality was added; *p <
0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001

Table 3 Mediating effect of personality 2013). As the results indicated, children from a high SES
Models Indirect Boot SE Boot Boot Ratio of family could enjoy more educational and developmental
effect LLCI ULCI indirect to resources from their parents (Currie et al. 1997; Matthews
total and Gallo 2011; Li et al. 2014); whereas, children from a
SES-E-SC 0.06 0.08 0.04 0.07 60.14% low SES family might suffer more conflicts and problems
SES-O-SC 0.07 0.01 0.05 0.08 77.83% during their growth (Ackerman et al. 2004; Conger et al.
SES-C-SC 0.05 0.08 0.04 0.07 56.30% 2000; Conger et al. 2002; Evans and Kim 2007; Taylor
SES-A-SC 0.05 0.01 0.03 0.06 51.53%
et al. 2014). Our results suggest that by improving “objec-
PCR-E-SC 0.06 0.01 0.05 0.07 37.12%
tive” family environment (family income, parents’ occu-
pation, education level, and so on), children from low SES
PCR-O-SC 0.07 0.01 0.05 0.08 42.24%
families can be helped to improve their social creativity and
PCR-C-SC 0.07 0.01 0.06 0.09 45.54%
form better interpersonal relationships.
PCR-A-SC 0.07 0.07 0.05 0.08 39.65%
In addition, parent-child relationships were related to
E extraversion, A agreeableness, C conscientiousness, O openness, N children’s social creativity. According to the ecological
neuroticism
model of authoritative parenting and adolescent adjustment
(Steinberg et al. 1995), problems in the parent-child rela-
Discussion tionship may make the child feel less supported and cared in
family life, perhaps resulting in more negative emotions
Social creativity is an important characteristic that allows (Suldo and Huebner 2004). Consistent with other related
children to solve interpersonal problems in novel ways. In studies (Fan and Zhang 2014; Fearon et al. 2013; Gu et al.
the current study, we examined the family environment and 2012) showing that positive parenting, especially parental
children’s personality traits as possible influences on social acceptance, warmth and authoritative parenting, are con-
creativity. As hypothesized, we found that family SES and ducive to children’s social creativity(Conger et al. 2010; Su
parent-child relationships were related to children’s social et al. 2015; Taylor et al. 2014). By contrast, negative par-
creativity; the results also showed that these associations enting, especially parental refusal and denial, may aggravate
were partially mediated by the personality traits of extra- the parent-child relationship and decrease children’s social
version, openness, agreeableness and conscientiousness. creativity (Gu 2009b; Song et al. 2013). Our results suggest
First, we tested the effects of family environment that good parent-child relationships might also help foster
(“objective” family SES and “subjective” or perceived social creativity.
parent-child relationships) on children’s social creativity. Second, this study found that the effects of family SES
Our results indicated that the children’s family SES and and parent-child relationship on social creativity were par-
parent-child relationships together were significant statis- tially mediated by children’s openness, conscientiousness,
tical predictors of children’s social creativity. The result can agreeableness, and extraversion. The results suggest that
be explained by the family investment theory and family high family SES and harmonious parent-child relationships
stress model. contribute to children’s personality traits development in a
The role of SES in relation to social creativity was healthy way, and these in turn affect children’s social
similar as that found for general creativity (Zhang et al. creativity. Harrington (1990) proposed the theory of the
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2018) 27:2999–3007 3005

Ecology of Human Creativity, and argued that the external personality, we acknowledge that some personality traits
environment (family SES and parents-child relationships) (especially temperament factors) may in turn have effects on
primarily acted on creative development by influencing parental-child relationships. The casual relationship
personality traits (Feist 1999; Silvia et al. 2008, 2009). As between these variables can not be determined by the cross-
studies indicated, developmental trends of personality may sectional design we used. Therefore, it is necessary to use a
be different depending on individual and environment fac- longitudinal design to examine the developmental changes
tors (Roberts and DelVecchio 2000; Wängqvist et al. 2015), and dynamic links between family socio-economic status,
and personality traits mediate the relationship between parent-child relationships and social creativity in future
family factors and children’s creative behavior(Sun et al. research. Secondly, research has identified two sides of
2014). Personality traits are very important for children’s social creativity (i.e., creative trait and creative state) using
social creativity. behavioral and neuroscience methods (Gu. et al. 2015). In
Furthermore, we not only found that the personality trait this study, we only examined the creative trait of social
of openness was positively associated with social creativity, creativity (i.e., social creativity that has been stable in daily
as documented in previous studies of creative thinking life, rather than social creativity presented in short-term
(Feist 1999; Silvia et al. 2008, 2009), but also found the specific tasks). Therefore, future studies should consider
mediation roles of conscientiousness, agreeableness, and both traits and states of social creativity. Third, family
extraversion in the association between family factors environment has a wild content. It includes not only SES
(family SES, parent-child relationships) and social creativ- and parents-child relationships. Parent’s pressure, education
ity. Empirical research shows children from a better func- level, parenting styles, and personality may also moderate
tioning family get more care and inclusiveness from their the effects of family factors on children’s creativity. Further
parents. Parents’ emotional response, timely encouragement study should consider more possible models examining the
and tolerance contribute to children’s personality openness, impact of family economic resources.
conscientiousness, agreeableness, and extraversion devel- The present study found that family SES and parent-child
opment (Wängqvist et al. 2015). Compared with other relationships were significantly correlated with children’s
children, children from positive family environments (e.g., social creativity, and personality traits played a mediation
with high family SES and harmonious parent-child rela- role in these associations. The findings have important
tionships) were more likely to communicate with others, to implications for Chinese rural children, especially for those
be responsible and outgoing, and to show higher social children who are left-behind at home because their parents
creativity at home and school. In other words, family migrate to urban areas for work. These children are suf-
environments affect children’s social creativity through fering from poor family economic situations and have to
children’s personality traits. live separately from their parents, which may hinder their
Although not the focus of our research, our descriptive social and personality development and make it difficult for
data showed that the fourth grade was one important period them to deal with interpersonal problems. Therefore, it is
for the development of children’s social creativity, which important to improve their parents’ abilities of coping and
was consistent with previous studies on creative thinking managing stress, building close parent-child relationship in
(Gu 2009a; Torrance 1962). Torrance found the “fourth the context of lower resources, which contributes to
grade slump” phenomenon in creative thinking, and the developing children’s personality and social creativity. The
current study found the same phenomenon in social crea- findings from this study may also be useful for school
tivity. The fourth grade is the key stage for children to learn psychologists to help children with interpersonal conflicts.
and master social rules (Runco and Charles 1997), and so it
might also be the key stage in which to develop social Acknowledgements This work was supported by a Major Program of
the National Social Science Foundation of P.R. China [11&ZD151],
creativity.
self-determined research Funds of Central China Normal University
from the colleges’ determined research operation of MOE
Limitations and Future Research Directions (CCNU16JCZX11 & CCNU15A02056), State Scholarship Fund
organized by China Scholarship Council (201706775076), and the
Research Program Funds of the Collaborative Innovation Center of
The findings in this study have several limitations that Assessment toward Basic Education Quality at Beijing Normal Uni-
require further research. Firstly, we used a cross-sectional versity(2016-04-003-BZK01). We also acknowledge the support of
design, but some studies have shown that the impacts of Katherine Kitzmann in preparing the manuscript.
family on children’s development and social adaptation
Author Contributions: D.Z.: designed and executed the study, assisted
were long-term effects, and childhood personality may with the data analyses, and wrote the paper. Z.Z.: collaborated in the
develop in an unstable state (Roberts and DelVecchio writing and editing of the final manuscript. C.G.: assisted with the data
2000). Moreover, although family factors such as parental- analyses, collaborated in the writing and editing of the final manu-
child relationships would exert important impacts on child script. Y.L. and C.F.: analyzed the data and wrote part of the results.
3006 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2018) 27:2999–3007

Compliance with Ethical Standards Currie, C. E., Elton, R. A., Todd, J., & Platt, S. (1997). Indicators of
socio-economic status for adolescents: The WHO health behavior
Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of in school-aged survey. Health Education Research, 12, 385–397.
intersts. Dai, D. Y., & Shen, J. L. (2008). Cultivating creative potential during
adolescence: A developmental and educational perspective.
Ethical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving Korean Journal of Thinking and Problem Solving, 18, 83–92.
human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of Dai, D. Y., Tan, X., Marathe, D., Valtcheva, A., Pruzek, R. M., &
the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Shen, J. (2012). Influences of Social and Educational Envir-
Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical onmentson Creativity During Adolescence: Does SES Matter?
standards. Central China Normal University had provided IRB Creativity Research Journal, 24(23), 191–199.
approval for the study. Dasgruptas & Sonthallas (2011). Role of parent-child relationship in
creativity of adolescent boys and girls. Indian Journal of Com-
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual munity Psychology, 7(1), 155–172.
participants included in the study. Evans, G. W., & Kim, P. (2007). Childhood poverty and health
cumulative risk exposure and stress deregulation. Psychological
Science, 18(11), 953–957.
Fan, J., & Zhang, L. F. (2014). The role of perceived parenting styles
References in thinking styles. Learning and Individual Differences, 32(3),
204–211.
An, Y. Y., Wu, X. C., Liu, C. H., & Lin, C. D. (2013). Neuroticism Fearon, D. D., Copeland, D., & Saxon, T. F. (2013). The Relationship
personality and posttraumatic growth in adolescent: The moder- between Parenting Styles and Creativity in a Sample of Jamaican
ating effect of coping style and mediating effect of social support. Children. Creativity Research Journal, 25(1), 119–128.
Psychological Development and Education, 6, 657–663. Feist, G. J. (1999). The influence of personality on artistic and sci-
Andres, F., Castanier, C., & Le, S. C. (2014). Attachment and alcohol entific creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity.
use amongst athletes: the mediating role of conscientiousness and (pp. 273–296). New York: Cambridge University Press.
alexithymia. Addictive Behaviors, 39(2), 487–490. Furnham, A., Zhang, J., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2006). The rela-
Amabile, T. M., Barsade, S. G., Mueller, J. S., & Staw, B. M. (2005). tionship between psychometric and self-estimated intelligence,
Affect and creativity at work. Administrative Science Quarterly, creativity, personality, and academic achievement. Cognition and
50, 367–403. Personality, 25, 119–145.
Ackerman, B. P., Brown, E. D., & Izard, C. E. (2004). The relations Gu, C. H., Chen, H. C., & Xu, J. J. (2003). The historiometric study of
between persistent poverty and contextual risk and children’s the early family environment and parental behavioral style of
behavior in elementary school. Developmental Psychology, 40(3), Chinese social creative personalities who passed away after A. D.
367–377. 1840. Psychological Development and Education, 4, 17–22.
Becker, J. C. (2012). The system-stabilizing role of identity manage- Gu, C. H., Zhou, Z. K., & Hu, J. Y. (2008a). Psychometric analysis of
ment strategies: social creativity can undermine collective action social creative potential questionnaire for primary school children
for social change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (Chinese). Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 16(4), 340–343.
103(4), 647–662. Gu, C. H., Zhou, Z. K., & Wang, F. (2008b). The relationship of social
Buchnan, C. M., Maccoby, E. E., & Dornbush, S. M. (1991). Caught creative tendency and personality attribute in primary school
between parents: Adolescents’ experience in divorced homes. children. Chinese Journal of Special Education, 93(3), 91–95.
Child Development, 62, 1008–1029. Gu, C. H. (2009a). The development of social creativity tendency of
Carvalho, R. G. G. (2016). Gender differences in academic achieve- primary school children. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 7
ment: the mediating role of personality. Personality and Indivi- (3), 198–202.
dual Differences, 94, 54–58. Gu, C. H. (2009b). The difference in social creative tendency between
Conger, K. J., Rueter, M. A., & Conger, R. D. (2000). The role of primary school children from urban areas and their counterparts
economic pressure in the lives of parentsand their adolescents: from rural areas and its implications to education. Chinese
The family stress model. In L. J. Crockett & R. K. Silbereisen Journal of Special Education, 110(8), 85–89.
(Eds.), Negoti-ating adolescence in times of social change (pp. Gu, C. H., Fan, C. Y., Zhang, D. J., Yang, S., & Song, J. J. (2012). The
201–223). New York: Cambridge University Press. effect of parenting styles and personality on primary school
Conger, R. D., Ge, X., Elder, Jr, G. H., Lorenz, F. O., & Simons, R. L. children’s social creativity and social preferences. Chinese
(2010). Economic stress, coercive family process, and develop- Journal of Special Education, 149(11), 79–83.
mental problems of adolescents. Child Development, 65(2), Gu., C. H., Wang, Y. L., & Wu, C. F. (2015). Brain correlates
541–561. underlying social creative thinking: EEG alpha activity in trait vs
Conger, R. D., Wallace, L. E., Sun, Y., Simons, R. L., Mcloyd, V. C., state creativity. Journal of Psychology, 47(6), 765–773.
& Brody, G. H. (2002). Economic pressure in african american Gu, C., Hu, B., Ngwira, F., Jing, Z., & Zhou, Z. (2016). The effect of
families: a replication and extension of the family stress model. general creative personality and freedom of task choice on ado-
Developmental Psychology, 38(2), 179–193. lescents’ social creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 50
Chen, W., Niu, G. F., Zhang, D. J., Fan, C. Y., Tian, Y., & Zhou, Z. K. (2), 132–149.
(2016). Socioeconomic status and life satisfaction in Chinese ado- Guo, M., & Wu, Q. (2011). Parenting styles and academic achieve-
lescents: Analysis of self-esteem as a mediator and optimism as a ment: The mediating role of conscientiousness and motivation.
moderator. Personality and Individual Differences, 95, 105–109. Psychological Research, 4(2), 44–47.
Chesnokova, O., & Subbotsky, E. (2014). Social creativity in primary- Harrington, D. M. (1990). The ecology of human creativity: A psy-
school children: How to measure, develop and accept it. Procedia chological perspective. Paper presented at the Based on a collo-
- Social and Behavioral Sciences, 146, 141–146. quium, Toward a Psychology of Creative Environments: An
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2006). La créativité: Psychologie de la EcologicalPerspective, presented at the Institute of Personality
découverte et del’invention [Creativity: Psychology of discovery Assessment and Research. Berkeley: University of California.
and invention]. Paris: Robert Laffont. Sep–1984.
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2018) 27:2999–3007 3007

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and Runco, M. A., & Acar, S. (2010). Do tests of divergent thinking have
conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New an experiential bias? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the
York: Guilford Press. Arts, 4(3), 144–148.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P.M. (1999). Cutoff criterion for fit indexes in Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Berg, C., Martin, C., & O’ Connor, A.
covariance structural analysis: Conventional criteria versus new (2009). Openness to experience, plasticity, and creativity:
alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. Exploring lower-order, high-order, and interactive effects. Jour-
Ilies, R., & Judge, T. A. (2003). On the heritability of job satisfaction: nal of Research in Personality, 43(6), 1087–1090.
The mediating role of personality. Journal of Applied Psychol- Silvia, P. J., Winterstein, B. P., Willse, J. T., Barona, C. M., Cram, J.
ogy, 88(4), 750–9. T., & Hess, K. I., et al. (2008). Assessing creativity with diver-
Velázquez, J. A., Segal, N. L., & Horwitz, B. N. (2014). Genetic and gent thinking tasks: Exploring the reliability and validity of new
environmental influences on applied creativity: A reared-apart subjective scoring methods. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity,
twin study. Personality and Individual Differences, 75, 141–146. and the Arts, 2, 68–85.
Kaufman, J. C., Kaufman, S. B., & Lichtenberger, E. O. (2011). Song, J. J., Gu., C. H., & Zhang, Y. X. (2013). The effect of left-at-
Finding creative potential on intelligence tests via divergent home adolescents’ Internet use on their social creativity: The
production. Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 26(2), mediating role of social relationship network. Chinese Journal of
83–106. Special Education, 161(11), 63–69.
Karmakar, M., Elhai, J. D., Amialchuk, A. A., & Tietjen, G. E. (2017). Soroa, G., Balluerka, N., Hommel, B., & Aritzeta, A. (2015). Asses-
Do personality traits mediate the relationship between childhood sing interactions between cognition, emotion, and motivation in
abuse and migraine? An exploration of the relationships in young creativity: The construction and validation of EDICOS. Thinking
adults using the add health dataset. Headache the Journal of Skills and Creativity, 17, 45–58.
Head and Face Pain, 58(2), 243–259. Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1996). Investing in creativity.
Lewallen, A. C., & Neece, C. L. (2015). Improved Social Skills in American Psychologist, 51(7), 677–688.
Children with Developmental Delays After Parent Participation in Steinberg, L., Darling, N. E., & Fletcher, A. C. (1995). Authoritative
MBSR: The Role of Parent–Child Relational Factors. Journal of parenting and adolescent adjustment: An ecological journey.
Child and Family Study, 24, 3117–3129. Washington, DC. American Psychological Association.
Li, X. W., Zou, H., Liu, Y., & Zhou, Q. (2014). The relationships of Su, Z. Q., Zhang, D. J., & Shao, J. J. (2015). Relationship between
family socioeconomic status, parent–adolescent conflict, and filial SES and social adjustment of left-over children: with perceived
piety to adolescents’ family functioning in mainland China. discrimination as the mediator. Psychological Development and
Journal of Child and Family Study, 23, 29–38. Education(Chinese), 31(2), 212–219.
Liu, Y., Wang, M., Villberg, J., Torsheim, T., Tynjälä, J., Lv, Y., & Suldo, S. M., & Huebner, E. S. (2004). The role of life satisfaction in
Kannas, L. (2012). Reliability and validity of Family Affluence the relationship between authoritative parenting dimensions and
Scale (FAS II) among adolescents in Beijing,China. Child Indi- adolescent problem behavior. Social Indicators Research, 66(1/
cators Research, 5, 235–251. 2), 165–195.
Matthews, K. A., & Gallo, L. C. (2011). Psychological perspectives on Sun, P., Zou, H., & Yao-Iin, D.U. (2014). The characteristics of
pathways linking socioeconomic status and physical health. adolescents' creative thinking and their effects on creative beha-
Annual Review of Psychology, 62(4), 1–30. vior: the mediating role of personality. Psychological Develop-
McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to ment and Education, 4, 355–362.
experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), Tang, G. R., Zou, H., Hou, K., Cai, D., & Zhang, W. J. (2014). The
1258–1265. characteristics of family innovation environment and their effects
Miller, I. W., McDermut, W., & Gordon, K. C. (2000). Personality and on everyday creative behavior: The mediating role of personality.
family functioning in families of depressed patients. Journal of Journal of Psychological Science, 37(5), 1125–1131.
Abnormal Psychology, 109(3), 539–545. Taylor, R. D., Budescu, M., Gebre, A., & Hodzi, I. (2014). Family
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Morris, M. D., Steinberg, L., Aucion, K. J., & Financial Pressure and Maternal and Adolescent Socioemotional
Keyes, A. W. (2011). The influence of mother-child emotion Adjustment: Moderating Effects of Kin Social Support in Low
regulation strategies on children’s expression of anger and sad- Income African American Families. Journal of Child and Family
ness. Developmental Psychology, 47(1), 213–225. Study, 23, 242–254.
Mouchiroud, C., & Lubart, T. I. (2002). Social creativity: A cross- Torrance, E. P. (1962). Guiding creative talent. NJ: Prentice Hall, Inc.
sectional study of 6- 11- year- old children. International Journal Van, G. M., & Vedder, P. (2011). The role of family obligations and
of Behavioral Development, 26(1), 60–69. school adjustment in explaining the immigrant paradox. Journal
Mouchiroud, C., & Bernoussi, A. (2008). An empirical study of the of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 187–196.
construct validity of social creativity. Learning and Individual Wängqvist, M., Lamb, M. E., Frisén, A., & Hwang, C. P. (2015).
Differences, 18, 372–380. Child and adolescent predictors of personality in early adulthood.
Münch, A. L., Hunger, C., & Schweitzer, J. (2016). An investigation Child Development, 86(4), 1253–1261.
of the mediating role of personality and family functioning in the Zhang, C. M., Zou, H., & Hou, K. (2006). The Relations among
association between attachment styles and eating disorder status. Respecting Behaviors, Personalities and Class Environment of
Bmc Psychology, 4(1), 1–10. Primary School Children. Psychological Development and Edu-
Niu, W. (2007). Individual and environmental influences on Chinese cation, 3, 16–22.
student creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 41, 151–175. Zhang, J. H., Man, D. H., Liu, G. R., Zhang, S., Dou, F. F., & Lin, C.
Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order con- D. (2013). A Research on the Relationship between Elementary
sistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: A School Students’ Parenting Styles and Social Creativity: the
quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulle- Mediating Role of Self-esteem. Psychological Development and
tin, 126(1), 3–25. Education, 6, 595–603.
Runco, M. A., & Charles, R. E. (1997). Developmental trends in Zhou, H., Niu, L. L., & Zou, H. (2000). A development study on five-
creative potential and creative performance. In M. A. Runco factor personality questionnaire for middle school students. Psy-
(Ed.), The creativity research handbook (vol.1) (pp. 115–153). chological Development and Education, 1, 48–54.
New Jersey: Hampton Press, Inc.

You might also like