Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Abnormal Psychology

1979, Vol. 88, No. 6, 611-619

Assessment of Cognitive Bias in Depression


Susan Krantz and Constance Hammen
University of California, Los Angeles
A procedure is described for the assessment of depressive distortion, a construct
hypothesized by Beck to be a central mechanism in the cause and maintenance
of dysphoria and other symptoms of depression. The questionnaire assesses in-
dividuals' interpretations of events depicted in brief stories and measures pres-
ence of a tendency to select the most negative, pessimistic, or self-deprecating
response options. The psychometric properties of the procedure are reported,
and various validity issues are addressed. There is a consistent relation between
scores on an inventory of depressive symptoms and depressive-distortion scores
across samples of college students, outpatients in therapy for depression, and
inpatients. From test-retest data, it appears that individuals with high scores
on both depression and distortion had the highest levels of depression 8 weeks
after initial testing. Although the results confirm Beck's hypothesis of a char-
acteristic cognitive bias in depression, further research is needed to analyze the
role of bias in maintaining depressed mood and to further clarify the nature
of the bias.

The last 15 years have witnessed a surge of anticipation of performance or reinforcement.


interest in cognitive models of depression. The Studies using this type of methodology have
work of Aaron Beck (1967, 1976) has been found that relatively depressed compared with
especially influential. Beck views depression nondepressed subjects underestimate their
as essentially a disorder of thinking rather than number of correct responses (Wener & Rehm,
affect and has described the "cognitive triad" 1975) and recall more negatively reinforced
characteristic of depressed persons. This triad and less positively reinforced trials (Buchwald,
consists of negative
⾃我
and pessimistic
環境
ways of 1977; De Monbreun & Craighead, 1977;
construing the self, the environment, and the Nelson & Craighead, 1977). Similarly, studies
未來
future. He postulates that these negative con- of the learned helplessness model of depression
structions set the stage for the development of have made inferences about the perception of
depression and serve to perpetuate depressed contingency by asking subjects to indicate their
mood. However, efforts to assess cognitions expectations of success on subsequent trials.
and to test their hypothesized relation to For instance, a study by Miller and Seligman
depressive mood have lagged behind the (1973) suggests that nondepressed as compared
theoretical efforts. to depressed subjects show a greater change in
Recent attempts to assess depressive cogni- their expectancy of success in skilled tasks,
tions may be categorized into two basic types. but not chance tasks.
One method compares depressed and non- A second type of methodology used in as-
depressed subjects' perception, recall, or sessing depressive cognitions involves self-
rating scales. For instance, Hammen and
Krantz (1976) used self-esteem ratings to
We are grateful to Armand Alkire of the Sepulveda assess the self-evaluative aspect of depressive
Veterans Administration Hospital for his assistance in
facilitating collection of data from the inpatient sample cognitions. They found that depressed women
and to Aaron Beck and Susan Cochran for their com- were significantly more self-critical than non-
ments on an earlier draft of this article. depressed women. Furthermore, failure feed-
Requests for reprints should be sent to Constance
Hammen, Department of Psychology, University of back intensified self-critical cognitions in
California, Los Angeles, California, 90024. depressed women, and success feedback failed
Copyright 1979 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0021-843X/79/8806-0611$00.75

611
612 SUSAN KRANTZ AND CONSTANCE HAMMEN

to minimize these cognitions compared to a sions is beyond the scope of this investigation,
no-feedback control. Similarly, Nelson (1977) data are presented on the covariation of depres-
found moderate positive correlations between sive distortion and severity of depression.
scores on the Beck Depression Inventory and Samples of nondepressed and relatively de-
adherence to certain "irrational" beliefs hy- pressed college students, participants in an
pothesized by Albert Ellis (1962) to be central outpatient intervention program for depres-
to maladaptive behaviors. sion, and depressed inpatients were tested.
These studies all made important contribu- Finally, analyses of the relation between level
tions in translating Beck's concepts into opera- of depression and depressive distortion over
tional terms and then demonstrating a rela- time were undertaken to attempt to answer
tion between cognition and mood. Relatively preliminary questions about the possible
little work has been done, however, to directly maintaining role of distortions in the perpetua-
test one of Beck's central themes concerning tion of depressive affect and related symptoms.
the biased or unrealistic quality of depressive
cognitions. As a preliminary step, it would seem
useful to try to systematically document Method
Beck's clinical observations that depressed 1
individuals characteristically engage in dis- Questionnaire Development
torted interpretations of circumstances such as 詮釋
In order to assess individuals' interpretations of
selective abstraction, arbitrary inference, over- events, short descriptions were developed of potentially
generalization, and other errors. It is also im- problematic situations common to college students.
portant to more directly test his assertion that Following each story were four multiple-choice ques-
tions (with the exception of one story, which had three
these biased cognitions are substantially im- questions) pertaining to the central character's feelings,
plicated in the development and perpetuation thoughts, and expectations. Each question had four
of depression. However, no easily administered response options, constructed along two dichotomous
method of obtaining an individual measure of and crossed dimensions of depressed versus nonde-
tendency to display the cognitive distortions pressed in tone and distorted versus nondistorted in
terms of logical inference based on the information
posited to be the central features of depression provided in the story. Thus, following each question
currently exists. Weintraub, Segal, and Beck was one depressed-distorted option, one depressed-
(1974) briefly reported an independently de- nondistorted option, one nondepressed-distorted op-
veloped questionnaire assessment of depressive tion, and one nondepressed-nondistorted option. These
two different dimensions were used to evaluate dys-
distortion, but it did not differentiate dysphoric phoric content both with and without distorted form,
cognitions that were realistic from unrealistic and vice versa.
dysphoric cognitions, and issues of its validity Subjects were asked to choose the option that best
and psychometric properties apparently were represented their own response to the situations por-
描寫 trayed in the stories as if they were the central charac-
not pursued. ter. It should be noted that the dimension of depressed
The present study, therefore, describes the versus nondepressed tone as used in this questionnaire
development of a questionnaire measure of refers to the presence or absence of unhappiness and
Beck's hypothesized construct of depressive dysphoria and not the clinical disorder with its as-
distortion. The goal was to develop a measure sociated severely painful feelings, The distorted versus
nondistorted dimension denotes the presence or absence
of sufficient utility that it could be used as a of interpretations that are unwarranted in light of the
research instrument for the investigation of available information.
mechanisms of depression and of individual Thus, the construct of depressive distortion includes,
differences in onset and persistence of depres- for example, exaggeration or misinterpretation of events
sion, and as a possible outcome measure in so as to emphasize negative outcomes, and the in-
terpretation involves an inference that is not accurate
intervention studies. based on the information provided. As an example, in
In addition to analyses of the psychometric one story Peggy (or Paul) was depicted as a member
properties of the questionnaire, data are also of an organization who was encouraged by friends to
presented that bear on the occurrence of run for the presidency of the organization. She (he)
depressive distortions across various depressed
groups. Although the complex issue of con- 1
Copies of the questionnaire are available from the
tinuity in the diagnostic spectrum of depres- second author.
DEPRESSIVE BIAS 613

eventually lost the election. The subject was instructed uate student judges. Percentage of agreement across
to "put yourself in Peggy's (Paul's) place, and try to judges as to depressed or nondepressed and distorted
imagine as vividly as you can what she (he) thought or nondistorted was tabulated, and only those items
and felt." The first item was "When you first heard that achieved at least 80% agreement on both di-
you'd lost, you immediately": mensions were retained in the final version. The final
set contains six stories and a total of 23 items that
1. feel bad and imagine I've lost by a landslide reflect a high degree of interjudge consensus on content
(depressive-distorted). validity for the scoring categories.
2. Shrug it off as unimportant (nondepressive-
distorted). Groups Tested
3. feel sad and wonder what the total counts were
(depressive-nondistorted). Distortion questionnaires and Beck Depression In-
4. shrug it off, feeling I tried as hard as I could ventory scores were collected from the following groups:
(nondepressive-nondistorted). Large student samples. Group 1 consisted of 107
men and 105 women undergraduates tested in 1976, and
An effort to construct the depressed-distorted re- Group 2 consisted of 315 undergraduates—117 men
sponse options to depict equal numbers of categories of and 198 women tested in the autumn of 1977. All stu-
such logical errors as arbitrary inference, selective dents were volunteers from an introductory psychology
abstraction, overgeneralization, and maximization of subject pool; their mean age was 18 years. In Group 1,
negative or minimization of positive proved unsuc- approximately half of the subjects received the ques-
cessful. Independent judges were unable to reliably tionnaires with male story characters, and the other
separate one such response type from another. How- half received stories with female figures in order to learn
ever, the distortion responses are all examples of some if gender of characters affected responding.
type of distortion described by Beck. Test-retest samples. A group of 19 depressed and 21
An initial set of eight stories and questions was de- nondepressed subjects was randomly selected from
veloped by the authors and submitted for independent Group 1 and retested at 4-5 weeks. The 315 students in
analysis to a group of clinical psychology graduate Group 2 above were retested on the questionnaires ap-
student judges unaware of the purpose of the task. proximately 8 weeks later.
Hammen and Krantz (1976) experimental sample. In
Their job was to rate whether responses exemplified the context of an experiment on responses to success
depressed or nondepressed tone and distorted or non- and failure reported elsewhere, 32 depressed and 33 non-
distorted conclusion. On the basis of their ratings, depressed (using Beck Depression Inventory scores of
stories and questions were modified as necessary, and 10 as a cutoff) women students completed the ques-
the resulting set was evaluated by 10 additional grad- tionnaire. They had been recruited as volunteers from

Table 1
Mean Scores for Depression and Depressive Distortion

Depressive
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) distortions
Sample Category M SD M SD
Student Sample 1 (N = 212) Depressed (BDI > 9) 11.7 5.1 2.15 2.3
Nondepressed (BDI < 9) 2.3 1.7 1.56 2.2
Student Sample 2 (N = 315) Depressed (BDI > 9) 13.1 4.8 3.02 2.5
Nondepressed (BDI < 9) 3.8 2.4 1.71 1.9
Hammen & Krantz (1976)
experiment (N = 67) Depressed (BDI > 9) 14.5 5.7 4.55 3.2
Nondepressed (BDI < 9) 3.0 2.4 2.41 2.7
Hammen & Peters (1978) role
play (N = 63) Depressed role players — — 3.68 5.17
Nondepressed role players — —- 1.13 2.14
Clinical intervention sample
(GlasB, 1978) (N - 29) Depressed 20.4 7.9 4.32 3.53
Nondepressed
Veterans Administration Hospital
inpatients (N = 20) Depressed 28.2 9.5 7.0 4.4
Nondepressed 10.8 7.9 1.5 2.0
614 SUSAN KRANTZ AND CONSTANCE HAMMEN

the introductory psychology subject pool for what was pressive-nondistorted responses were the most
labeled as an experiment on "personality and thera- frequent, as expected.
peutic functioning."
Ham/men and Peters (1978) depression role-playing In terms of general descriptive characteris-
experiment. In the course of a study described else- tics, depressive-distorted scores in the large
where, student volunteers from the subject pool who student samples ranged from 0 to 14, with a
had been instructed to enact a depressed or nonde- mean score of 1.85 (SD = 2.25), in Group 1;
pressed role in an interaction completed the
questionnaire. and from 0 to 11, with a mean of 1,95
Clinically depressed intervention sample. Volunteers (SD = 2.06), in Group 2. Thus, in normal,
in a six-session brief depression intervention study generally nondepressed student samples, de-
based on reduction of learned helplessness (Glass, 1978) pressive distortions or biases were relatively
completed the distortion questionnaire prior to treat- rare, and the distributions of scores were posi-
ment, after treatment, and at a 1-month follow-up.
Participants had been screened (on the basis of a tively skewed.
clinical interview) from self-referred applicants for an In the clinically depressed intervention
experimental program for problems with depression. sample of depressed volunteers, the range was
Individuals were included if they met the research 1-14, with a mean of 4.32 (SD = 3.53), and
diagnostic criteria for a major depressive disorder
(Feighner, et al., 1972). Persons with bipolar disorders, the depressed inpatient sample had a mean of
those whose depression seemed secondary to other 7.00 (SD = 4.37).
psychiatric or medical problems, and those currently Analyses of sex differences. In a 2 X 2
receiving psychotropic medication were referred for analysis of variance of sex differences in the
treatment elsewhere. Of the final sample, 14 were men, subject or story character based on Large
IS were women, and their mean age was 26 years.
Inpatient sample. Data were collected from 10 Sample Group 1, there were no significant
depressed and 10 nondepressed male psychiatric in- main effects or interactions of the factors. Thus,
patients at a metropolitan Veterans Administration depressive-distorted responding was not more
Hospital. All patients who were admitted to one of four prevalent in men or women, and the gender of
wards over a period of several months were evaluated
for suitability. The final depressed sample included the character of the story did not signifi-
those who met the research diagnostic criteria (Feighner cantly affect degree of depressive-distorted
et al., 1972) for major or minor depressive disorders, responding.
and the nondepressed patient sample contained a Analysis of story effects. Three of the six
mixture of diagnoses other than depression as a primary stories in the questionnaire have mainly social-
or secondary diagnosis, including schizophrenia, charac-
ter disorder, and hypomanic phase of bipolar affective interpersonal themes, and three have pre-
disorder. All of the participants had to be able to give dominantly achievement-competence themes.
informed consent. All of the nondepressed and seven of Analyses of possible differences between stories
the depressed subjects were receiving psychotropic in their likelihood of eliciting depressive distor-
medication. The depressed and nondepressed groups of
inpatients did not differ in age (M = 44 years), length tion were undertaken in order to clarify the
of current hospitalization (approximately 4 weeks), or possibility that the test stimuli contributed
other potentially relevant demographic characteristics. more to distorted responding than did charac-
The questionnaire content was altered slightly so teristics of the cognitive processes of individ-
that the settings depicting college situations were re- uals. Based on the analysis of results from
placed by settings more relevant to the patients, such
as work or hospital. Large Sample Group 1, there were no differ-
The means and standard deviations of scores on the ences in distortion levels elicited by each story,
distortion questionnaire and the Beck Depression In- nor between interpersonal and achievement
ventory are presented in Table 1. themes. Nor were there subject sex differences
in depressive-distortion scores elicited by each
Results story. Thus, story theme did not appear to be
a critical determinant of depressive-distortion
Questionnaire Characteristics level,
Depressive-distorted scores. In the results
that follow, the analyses consider only depres- Reliability
sive-distortion scores. Nondepressive-distor-
tion (unrealistically positive responses) were Internal consistency. The coefficient of in-
quite rare in all samples except the nonde- ternal consistency of the depressive-distorted
pressed hospitalized sample, whereas nonde- responses on the scale was computed by the
DEPRESSIVE BIAS 615

KR-20 formula (see Guilford, 1954). The Table 2


coefficient was calculated for the largest stu- Item-Total Correlations
dent sample, Group 2 (N = 315), as .62. The
second largest sample, Group 1 (N = 212), Story no. Question no. ra
was used as a cross-validation sample and 1 1 .42
yielded a coefficient of internal consistency of 2 .48
.69. Item-total correlations (using a point- 3 .31
biserial correlation procedure appropriate to
the form of the data) based on Group 2 of the 2 1 .39
2 .49
large student sample ranged from .12 to .5, and 3 .19*
the mean correlation of the 23 items with total 4 .31
scores was .34. Twenty-one of the 23 item-total
correlations were significant at or beyond the 3 1 .51
2 .41
.01 level, as shown in Table 2. 3 .48
The coefficients of internal consistency were 4 .35
only moderate and possibly reflect the lack of
homogeneity of the items (e.g., selective ab- 4 1 .12 ns
straction, arbitrary inference, and the like) as 2 .19*
3 .35
well as the short length of the questionnaire 4 .52
(23 items). Although the reliability of the in-
strument seems to be adequate for research S 1 .17*
purposes, the construct of depressive distortion 2 .31
3 .12 ns
itself is heterogeneous and requires further 4 .48
clarification.
Test-retest reliability. The simple test- 6 1 .34
retest correlations between depressive-distor- 2 .28
tion scores at different points reflect a sub- 3 .30
4 .33
stantial degree of stability. In the Large Sample
Group 2 of undergraduates, the test-re test cor- Note. The five items with correlations not significant
relation was .60, (p < .001) for 8 weeks. A at p < .001 may be omitted for improved internal
randomly selected group of students from Large consistency when only the depressive-distortion
Sample Group 1 who scored above 9 on the scores are to be used.
• All correlations are significant at p < .001 unless
Beck Depression Inventory (n = 19) and a otherwise noted.
randomly selected group who scored below 9 *p <.01.
(n — 21) were retested at 4-5 weeks. The
test-retest correlation of depressive-distortion
scores was .48 (p < .001). sive-distorted replies. Table 1 presents some
Although depressive-distortion scores ap- samples.
peared to be relatively stable, they nevertheless In the original Hammen and Krantz (1976)
seemed to vary according to changes in mood experimental sample, depressed women had
level. This information is discussed in the significantly greater depressive-distortion
validity section. scores than did nondepressed women, F(l, 61)
= 8.24, p < .01. Large Sample Group 2, using
a similar Beck cutoff score of 10 to designate
Validity the relatively more depressed students, also
revealed a significant difference between de-
Depressive-distortion scores and level of depres- pressed and nondepressed groups, /(232)
sion. Of critical interest are relations between = 3.91, p < .001. The overall correlation be-
depressed mood and degree of depressive-dis- tween the variables was .39 (p < .001). In
torted responding. Data from all the samples Large Sample Group 1, a simple median split
strongly support the hypothesized tendency of on Beck scores was used to divide subjects, and
more depressed persons to give more depres- greater depressive-distortion scores were found
616 SUSAN KRANTZ AND CONSTANCE HAMMEN

in the relatively more depressed group, F(l, distortion should accompany reduced levels of
208) = 4.68, p < .05. depressed mood.
The clinically depressed intervention sample Within the randomly selected subset of
(Glass, 1978), with its relatively restricted Sample 1 that was retested after 4 weeks
range of Beck and depressive-distortion scores (» = 40), a planned comparison between sub-
prior to treatment, showed a degree of correla- jects whose Beck depression scores increased
tion between the two variables that approached and those whose Beck scores decreased over
significance (r = .33, p < .10). At the end of time revealed a borderline significant difference
the treatment, the correlation was significant in depressive-distortion scores at retesting,
(r = .46, p = .01). /(37) = 1.62, p «= .06, one-tailed. Persons who
When students were given instructions to became more depressed showed little change in
role play a depressed person in an experiment depressive-distortion scores, but those who
on interactions with depressed and nonde- became less depressed decreased in depressive-
pressed role players (Hammen & Peters, 1978), distortion level.
they were asked to complete the questionnaire In the clinically depressed intervention
as they imagined a depressed student would sample, the treatment and waiting-list control
answer. Depressed-role players gave a signifi- groups were initially equal both in levels of
cantly higher proportion of depressive-distor- depression and depressive distortion. At the
tion responses than did students asked to re- end of treatment, the treatment group de-
spond in a nondepressed fashion, F(l, 49) creased significantly in level of depression com-
= 5.46, p < .05. It should be noted that the pared to the waiting-list control group, and
students were not given information about there was a corresponding significant difference
depressive cognitions as such. between the groups on depressive-distortion
Finally, comparisons between depressed and scores, 1(22) = 2.51, p < .05, reflecting the
nondepressed psychiatric inpatients revealed treatment group's reduction in depressive
a significant difference in depressive-distortion distortion.
scores in the predicted direction, /(18) = 3.63, In the clinically depressed intervention
p < .01. It is of interest to note that although sample, persons who distorted relatively more
the nondepressed psychiatric patients had prior to treatment (scores of 3 or more) were
mildly elevated Beck scores, their depressive- compared on posttreatment depression scores
distortion scores were quite low, perhaps in- with those who distorted less. Those who dis-
dicating that depressive distortions are more torted relatively more prior to treatment were
specific to clinical depression than is intensity somewhat more depressed following treatment
of depressed mood. than those who distorted less, although the
In sum, across every sample tested—in- effect was nonsignificant. At the follow-up
cluding college students, a clinically depressed testing 1 month later, Beck Depression In-
intervention sample, and inpatient samples— ventory scores were significantly greater for
the relatively more depressed persons had those whose distortion scores were initially
significantly higher depressive-distortion higher, t(U) = 2.03, p < .05, one-tailed.
scores. Although the results are suggestive, it is not
Relation between changes in mood and depres- possible to draw unambiguous conclusions
sive-distortion scores. As noted previously, de- about the maintaining role of depressive-dis-
pressive-distortion scores were relatively stable tortion in depression because of the strong
over time; nevertheless, they appeared to vary tendency of distorters to be relatively more
according to changes in mood level. The theory depressed than nondistorters. The most ac-
of the role of cognitive distortion in depression curate statement of the patterns observed is
leads to the hypothesis that negatively biased that the most depressed persons (who also
thinking processes serve to perpetuate negative distort) tend to remain relatively more de-
mood. Therefore, it would be predicted that pressed over time. Depression level and depres-
higher levels of depressive distortion would be sive-distortion level both appear to contribute
associated with more persistent depression over to persisting depression. Further research is
time. Also, decreases in levels of depressive needed to investigate the possibility that the
DEPRESSIVE BIAS 617

level of depressive distortion contributes to the mood factor score with depressive-distortion
stability of depression. scores when the effects of the other mood states
Concurrent and discriminant validity. It was were controlled. The resulting partial correla-
anticipated that a measure of depressive dis- tions suggested that only depression (r = .15,
tortions would have a moderate positive cor- p < .01) and vigor (r = -.11, p < .05) were
relation with scores on Byrne's (1964) Repres- related to depressive-distortion scores.
sion-Sensitization (R-S) scale. Although vastly
different in concept from the current question- Discussion
naire, "sensitizers" could be expected to share
The psychometric properties of the measure
such characteristics as acknowledgment of
of cognitive bias in depression seem adequate
negative experiences and pessimistic and self-
to warrant its use in further construct valida-
critical responses with depressed persons. As
tion research. The measure has moderate in-
predicted, when the R-S scale was administered
ternal consistency, correlates positively with a
to subjects in the Large Sample Group 1 study, true-false measure of sensitization, and does
the correlation between the two scores was
not appear to be associated with disturbed
.31 (p < ,001), indicating some similarity be- mood states such as anxiety, anger, and con-
tween " sensitization" and depressive distor-
fusion except as they correlate highly with
tion. At the time the present measure of depressed mood. The measure reliably dis-
depressive distortion was developed, there
tinguishes relatively depressed and nonde-
were no other established instruments that pressed groups, including mild and clinical
assessed the construct. Work is currently
levels of depression, and also seems sensitive to
under way, however, to assess the correspond-
changes in mood level within individuals.
ence between the current measure and a Nevertheless, not all depressed individuals
measure of dysfunctional attitudes (Weissman
showed biased thinking and not all biased re-
& Beck, Note 1). sponding occurred in the presence of depres-
Although the present measure correlates sion. Thus, the instrument seems to tap an
strongly with self-report measures of depression
individual difference that could have utility in
such as the Beck Depression Inventory and the study of the occurrence and mechanism of
moderately with the R-S scale, it seems im- depression and as an outcome measure in
portant to question whether the depressive- intervention studies.
distorted responses assessed by the present
In addition to their utility in assessing de-
procedures are in fact unique to depression
pression, the results lend support to Beck's
rather than characteristic of emotional upset
(1967, 1976) emphasis on the cognitive aspects
in general. One approach to answering this of depression. The instrument specifically taps
question was to learn whether depressive-
a biased manner of evaluating situations that
distorted responses were more associated with
emphasize negative, self-critical, or pessimistic
depressed mood than with anxiety, anger, or
interpretations that are not warranted by the
other mood disturbance. The subjects in Large events themselves. The items that assess de-
Sample Group 2 were administered the Profile pressive bias were constructed to reflect such
of Mood States (McNair, Lorr, & Droppelman, errors in logical processing of information as
1971), which yields scores for factors of depres- artibrary inference, overgeneralization, maxi-
sion, anxiety-tension, anger, confusion, fatigue, mization of negative outcomes, and the like.
and vigor. Correlations between factor scores
As such, the items are a mixture of causal at-
and depressive distortion ranged from .33 for tributions for events, predictions of conse-
depression to .13 for fatigue. Although depres-
quences, and inferences about others' current
sion had the highest correlation, all the r values
feelings and attitudes. The heterogeneity of the
were significant at least at the p < .05 level.
content of the items presents a need for
One difficulty, however, is the high intercor- theoretical refinements but nevertheless seems
relation between factors in our sample (e.g., reflective of the heterogeneity in the current
depression with anxiety-tension, r — .69; and state of theory about the role of cognitions in
depression with anger, r = .72). Therefore, depression. In the same vein, it is important
analyses were undertaken to correlate each to be clear that the current instrument repre-
618 SUSAN KRANTZ AND CONSTANCE HAMMEN

sents only one method of conceptualizing and Reference Note


assessing distortions. The Jones Irrational
Beliefs Test (Nelson, 1977) and the Dysfunc- 1. Weissman, A., & Beck, A. T. Development and valida-
tion of the dysfunctional attitude scale. Paper presented
tional Attitudes Scale (Weissman & Beck, at the meeting of the Association for the Advance-
Note 1) represent additional approaches to ment of Behavior Therapy, Chicago, November
gauging cognitions associated with depression. 1978.
The depression-inducing or -maintaining
functions of depressive bias remain unclear at References
the present time. Results from the studies that
assessed depressive bias at two points in time Beck, A. T. Depression: Clinical, experimental, and
found that initial high levels of both depressed theoretical aspects. Philadelphia: University of
Pennsylvania Press, 1967.
mood and bias were associated with persistence Beck, A. T. Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders.
of depression and that mood and depressive New York: International Universities Press, 1976.
bias were at least moderately correlated. It may Beck, A. T., & Rush, A. J. Cognitive approaches to
be speculated that current mood and current depression and suicide. In G. Serban (Ed.), Cognitive
defects in the development of mental illness. New York:
biases have reciprocal effects on each other and Brunner/Mazel, in press.
additive effects in maintaining depression. It Buchwald, A. M. Depressive mood and estimates of
might be speculated further that depression reinforcement frequency. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
without depressive bias is relatively more chology, 1977, 86, 443-446.
benign than depression with bias, and that Byrne, D. Repression-sensitization as a dimension of
personality. In B. A. Maher (Ed.), Progress in experi-
depressive bias in the absence of depression mental personality research. New York: Academic
denotes a state of vulnerability to develop de- Press, 1964.
pression in the presence of a personally mean- De Monbreun, B., & Craighead, W. E. Distortion of
ingful loss or failure experience (e.g., Beck & perception and recall of positive and neutral feedback
in depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1977,
Rush, in press). The present instrument may /, 311-338.
have some utility in the pursuit of such Ellis, A. Reason and emotion in psychotherapy. New
questions. York: Stuart, 1962.
It should be noted that errors in the in- Feighner, J. P., et al. Diagnostic criteria for use in
terpretation of information are not unique to psychiatric research. Archives of General Psychiatry,
1972, 26, 57-63.
depression. Ross (1977), for example, provides Glass, David R., Jr. An evaluation of a brief treatment
a review describing a variety of interpretive for depression based on the learned helplessness model.
errors made by normal subjects in a variety of Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
contexts, and stimulating analyses and re- California, Los Angeles, 1978.
Guilford, J. P. Psychometric methods (2nd ed.). New
search are under way to explore such behaviors York: McGraw-Hill, 1954.
(e.g., Nisbett & Wilson, 1977; Smith & Miller, Hammen, C. L., & Krantz, S. Effect of success and
1978). The present set of studies, however, failure on depressive cognitions. Journal of Abnormal
shows that persons who are depressed are Psychology, 1976, 85, 577-586.
unique in their selective use of certain types of Hammen, C. L., & Peters, S. D. Interpersonal con-
sequences of depression: Responses to men and
errors as described by Beck (1967, 1976) in women enacting a depressed role. Journal of Abnor-
previous anecdotal reports. These findings were mal Psychology, 1978, 87, 322-332.
consistent across all samples regardless of McNair, D., Lorr, M., & Droppelman, L. Profile of
whether depression was defined as a relatively mood states. San Diego, Calif.: Educational and In-
dustrial Testing Services, 1971.
normal variation in mood, a moderately in- Miller, W. R., & Seligman, M. E. P. Depression and the
tense but not incapacitating disturbance, or a perception of reinforcement. Journal of Abnormal
severe clinical disorder. Assessing depression Psychology, 1973, 82, 62-73.
by self-report measures such as the Beck Nelson, R. E. Irrational beliefs in depression. Journal of
Depression Inventory or by clinical observa- Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1977, 45, 1190-
1191.
tion did not affect results, either. Additional Nelson, R. E., & Craighead, W. E. Perception of rein-
research is under way to attempt further forcement, self-reinforcement, and depression. Jour-
specification of characteristic cognitive biases nal of Abnormal Psychology, 1977, 86, 379-388.
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. Telling more than we
in depression and their potential role in the can know: Verbal reports on mental processes.
maintenance of depressed mood. Psychological Review, 1977, 84, 231-259.
DEPRESSIVE BIAS 619

Ross, L. The intuitive psychologist and his short- vestigation of cognition and affect in the depressive
comings: Distortions in the attribution process. In experiences of normal men. Journal of Consulting and
L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social Clinical Psychology, 1974, 42, 911.
psychology (Vol. 10). New York: Academic Press, Wener, A., & Rehm, L. P. Depressive affect: A test of
1977. behavioral hypotheses. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
Smith, E. R., & Miller, F. D. Limits on perception of chology, 1975, 84, 221-227.
cognitive processes: A reply to Nisbett and Wilson.
Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 355-362. Received February 26, 1979
Weintraub, M., Segal, R. M., & Beck, A. T. An in- Revision received April 30, 1979 •

You might also like