CFC Abreviada

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Personality and Individual Differences 186 (2022) 111362

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

When time is of the essence: Development and validation of brief


consideration of future (and immediate) consequences scales
Samuel Chng a, *, Han Shi Jocelyn Chew b, Jeff Joireman c
a
Lee Kuan Yew Centre for Innovative Cities, Singapore University of Technology and Design, 8 Somapah Road, 487372, Singapore
b
Alice Lee Centre for Nursing Studies, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, Level 3, Clinical Research Centre, Block MD11, 10 Medical
Drive, 117597, Singapore
c
Department of Marketing and International Business, Washington State University, Pullman, WA 99164-4730, USA

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Temporal dilemmas pose a conflict between the immediate and delayed consequences of one's actions. One trait
Time perspective related to decision-making in temporal dilemmas is the consideration of future consequences (CFC). Strathman
Consideration of future consequences et al. (1994) originally developed a (unidimensional) 12-item CFC scale (CFC-12), with five (seven) items tapping
Short scale
future (immediate) consequences. More recently, Joireman et al. (2012) created a balanced set of items (7 future,
7 immediate items), and argued that the CFC-14 scale contains CFC-Future and CFC-Immediate subscales. While
the CFC-12 and CFC-14 scales have seen widespread application, they may be too long for certain research
settings. Accordingly, the present work develops and validates brief 6-item (CFC-6) and very brief 4-item (CFC-4)
scales. Drawing on ten CFC-14 samples (N = 4789), evidence supports a two-factor structure and establishes the
internal reliability and predictive validity of the brief (3-item) and very brief (2-item) subscales in comparison to
the full-length scales: CFC-Future (3-item = 0.77, 2-item = 0.80) and CFC-Immediate (3-item = 0.83, 2-item =
0.83).

1. Introduction about the future consequences of their actions, and seven items focusing
on the tendency to base decisions on the immediate consequences of
Over the course of a day, people are faced with many decisions that one's actions. Following confirmatory factor analyses, Strathman et al.
pit the immediate against the future consequences of their actions (e.g., argued the scale was unidimensional. As such, they reverse-coded the
studying requires a sacrifice in the here and now, but can lead to better seven immediate items and combined them with the five future items to
grades and job opportunities in the distant future). One trait frequently derive an overall CFC score (CFC-Total scale), which demonstrated good
linked with decision-making in such “temporal dilemmas” is the internal reliability (>0.80).
“consideration of future consequences” (or CFC; Strathman, Gleicher, Subsequently, some research found that the CFC scale contained two
Boninger, & Edwards, 1994), which has been shown to predict academic sub-factors usually referred to as the CFC-Future and CFC-Immediate
performance (e.g., Acuff et al., 2017), health (e.g., Adams, 2012; Chew subscales (Joireman, Balliet, Sprott, Spangenberg, & Schultz, 2008).
& Chng, 2021), environmental (e.g., Enzler, 2015; Milfont, Bain, Gri­ More recently, Joireman, Shaffer, Balliet, and Strathman (2012) added
goletti, Gouveia, & Kashima, 2014), financial (e.g., Joireman, Kees, & two additional items dealing with future consequences (i.e., the CFC-14
Sprott, 2010), and work behavior (e.g., Graso & Probst, 2012), as well as scale), resulting in a balanced set of items (7 future and 7 immediate
ethical decision-making (e.g., Hershfield, Cohen, & Thompson, 2012; for items) and improved internal reliability of the CFC-Future subscale, and
a review, see Joireman & King, 2016). others have adapted this scale to specific contexts such as healthy eating
Strathman et al. (1994) defined CFC as “the extent to which people and exercise (e.g., Dassen, Houben, & Jansen, 2015; van Beek, Anto­
consider the potential distant outcomes of their current behaviors and nides, & Handgraaf, 2013). Table 1 displays the items on the CFC-14
the extent to which they are influenced by these potential outcomes” (p. scale.
743), and developed the 12-item CFC scale (i.e., CFC-12 scale). The CFC- Although the CFC-12 and CFC-14 have been widely applied, there
12 included five items dealing with the extent to which people think continues to be debate about the factor structure of the scale (e.g.,

* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: Samuel_chng@sutd.edu.sg (S. Chng).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.111362
Received 20 July 2021; Received in revised form 1 October 2021; Accepted 23 October 2021
Available online 30 October 2021
0191-8869/© 2021 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
S. Chng et al. Personality and Individual Differences 186 (2022) 111362

Table 1 Table 2
CFC-14 scale. Sample demographics.
1. I consider how things might be in the future, and try to influence those things with Sample # N Gender Ethnicity Age
my day to day behavior. (F) (representative
% % Mean SD Range
2. Often I engage in a particular behavior in order to achieve outcomes that may not outcome)
female Caucasian
result for many years. (F)
3. I only act to satisfy immediate concerns, figuring the future will take care of itself. Sample 1 256 49% 75% 21.36 1.83 18–35
(I) (preference for
4. My behavior is only influenced by the immediate (i.e., a matter of days or weeks) delayed gains)
outcomes of my actions. (I) Sample 2 (trait self- 701 56% 81% 46.30 15.00 19–83
5. My convenience is a big factor in the decisions I make or the actions I take. (I) control)
6. I am willing to sacrifice my immediate happiness or well-being in order to achieve Sample 3 (health 232 49% 81% 21.30 1.66 19–36
future outcomes. (F) eating)
7. I think it is important to take warnings about negative outcomes seriously even if Sample 4 (exercise) 119 40% 76% 21.47 1.72 18–29
the negative outcome will not occur for many years. (F) Sample 5 300 50% 91% 50.26 12.82 19–78
8. I think it is more important to perform a behavior with important distant (environmental
consequences than a behavior with less important immediate consequences. (F) values)
9. I generally ignore warnings about possible future problems because I think the Sample 6 (2008 U. 883 55% 80% 45.04 14.28 18–83
problems will be resolved before they reach crisis level. (I) S. presidential
10. I think that sacrificing now is usually unnecessary since future outcomes can be election)
dealt with at a later time. (I) Sample 7 (2012 U. 301 48% 85% 35.30 11.50 19–75
11. I only act to satisfy immediate concerns, figuring that I will take care of future S. presidential
problems that may occur at a later date. (I) election)
12. Since my day to day work has specific outcomes, it is more important to me than Sample 8 (air 1705 49% – – – >20
behavior that has distant outcomes. (I) conditioning
13. When I make a decision, I think about how it might affect me in the future. (F) usage)a
14. My behavior is generally influenced by future consequences. (F) Sample 9 (heart 150 23% – 59.70 12.71 25–88
failure self-care)
Note. Strathman et al.'s (1994) original CFC scale = items 1–12. F = CFC-Future Sample 10 (heart 142 20% – 61.14 11.77 32–85
subscale item. I = CFC-Immediate subscale item. CFC-14 Scale Instructions (for failure self-care)
online administration): “For each of the statements shown, please indicate Total 4789 49% – 41.41 17.04 18–88
whether or not the statement is characteristic of you using the scale shown EFA sample 2394 50% – 41.16 16.71 18–88
below:” 1 = extremely uncharacteristic, 2 = uncharacteristic, 3 = somewhat CFA sample 2395 49% – 41.67 17.38 18–84
uncharacteristic, 4 = neither, 5 = somewhat characteristic, 6 = characteristic, 7
Note.
= extremely characteristic. a
Age was collected in age bands.

McKay, Cole, & Percy, 2015; McKay, Morgan, van Exel, & Worrell,
(n = 2394) and one for Confirmatory Factor Analysis (n = 2395),
2015). Moreover, the length of the scales may be a barrier to their
respectively.
application in certain research settings, where time is limited. The
present work addresses these issues by first assessing the factor structure
of the CFC-14 scale, drawing on a pooled sample of ten studies (N = 2.2. Dependent variables
4789). We subsequently develop two brief and reliable CFC scales,
namely the 6-item CFC-short-form (CFC-6) and 4-item CFC-very short- In addition to completing the CFC-14, participants in the ten samples
form (CFC-4), each comprised of a CFC-Future and CFC-Immediate completed a variety of other measures. To test the validity of the brief
subscale with a proportionate number of items. We then evaluate the (vs. longer) scales, we selected a representative outcome variable from
predictive validity of the three scales (i.e. CFC-14, CFC-6, CFC-4) and each sample to use as a criterion variable. Our aim was to examine how
their respective subscales across multiple domains (trait delayed grati­ the brief and longer scales predicted outcomes across a range of settings,
fication and self-control, healthy eating behavior, exercise behavior, including basic traits (i.e., preference for delayed gains, trait self-
environmental values, U.S. Presidential voting intentions, habitual use control), health behaviors (i.e., healthy eating, exercise, and self-care),
of air-conditioning and heart failure self-care). Results indicate that the environmental behavior (i.e., biospheric values and use of air-
CFC-6 and CFC-4 perform as well as the CFC-14 in predicting outcomes conditioning), and political contexts (i.e., voting intentions in two U.S.
in the specified contexts. Presidential elections).
In sample 1, participants completed a 21-item decision task to assess
2. Materials and method preference for smaller immediate (e.g., $32 today) vs. larger delayed
outcomes (e.g., $55 in 20 days). We averaged the 21 choices (1 =
2.1. Sample description smaller immediate, 2 = larger delayed) to derive an index reflecting
preference for delayed gains (α = 0.91, M = 1.44, SD = 0.25). In sample
Ten samples of data from the U.S. and Singapore were utilized for our 2, participants completed the brief (13-item) measure of trait self-
analyses (N = 4789). This data was collected as part of several broader control (e.g., I am good at resisting temptation; 1 = extremely charac­
projects focused on health behaviors, environmental decision-making, teristic to 7 = extremely uncharacteristic; α = 0.82, M = 4.69, SD = 1.00;
and political issues (some published, some unpublished). Samples 1, 3 Tangney, Baumeister, & Boone, 2004). Sample 3 participants rated how
and 4 were college student samples, samples 2, 5, 6, 7 and 8 were healthy they intended to make their future breakfasts, lunches, and
recruited via Qualtrics panels and Amazon's Mechanical Turk, and dinners on a 10-point scale (1 = not health to 10 = very healthy; α =
samples 9 and 10 were hospital patient samples. Gender, ethnicity (% 0.81, M = 8.38, SD = 1.24). Sample 4 respondents indicated the number
Caucasian), and age for the ten samples are summarized in Table 2. of exercise sessions they planned for the next week (M = 4.07, SD =
Overall, the merged sample was reasonably well balanced on gender 1.88). In Sample 5, participants completed a scale (de Groot & Steg,
with a mean age of 41.4 (SD = 17.0), ranging from 18 to 88 years old. 2008) assessing the importance of four environmental (biospheric)
Participants in all studies completed the CFC-14, responding to the items values (e.g., preventing pollution; protecting natural resources; 1 = not
on a 7-point scale (1 = extremely uncharacteristic of me to 7 = important to 7 = very important; α = 0.93, M = 4.80, SD = 1.03). In
extremely characteristic of me). The merged sample was randomly split Samples 6 and 7, participants provided their rating of the two main U.S.
into two analytic samples, including one for Exploratory Factor Analysis Presidential candidates in the 2008 (1 = strongly prefer Obama to 7 =

2
S. Chng et al. Personality and Individual Differences 186 (2022) 111362

strongly prefer McCain; M = 3.80, SD = 2.40) and 2012 elections (1 = (Fig. 1) and parallel analysis (Fig. 1).
strongly prefer Obama to 7 = strongly prefer Romney; M = 2.53, SD = Finally, using a similar logic, we identified a reasonable four-item
2.01). In Sample 8, participants reported their habitual use of air- scale (consisting of 2-item very short subscales). The very short scale
conditioning (e.g., using the air-conditioning is something I do included items 13 and 14 (CFC-Future) and items 3 and 4 (CFC-Imme­
without thinking; 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree; α = 0.94, diate). An exploratory factor analysis on these four items yielded two
M = 3.57, SD = 1.54). In samples 9 and 10, participants provided their principal components, based on the eigenvalues (>1; Table 3), scree plot
ratings on ten items reflecting the level of heart failure self-care (e.g. (Fig. 1) and parallel analysis (Fig. 1).
daily weighing) measured on a four-point scale (1 = never/rarely; 4 =
daily/always) and computed as a composite score scaled to a score of 3.3. Confirmatory factor analysis
0–100 (αsample 9 = 0.63, Msample 9 = 51.2, SDsample 9 = 17.96 αsample 10 =
0.73, Msample 10 = 53.0, SDsample 10 = 17.26) (Chew, Sim, Choi, & Chair, Drawing on our second subsample (n = 2395), we next conducted
2021). confirmatory factor analyses on the original CFC-14 scale as well as the
shorter CFC-6, and CFC-4 scales identified in the EFAs described earlier.
3. Results Fit statistics for the one-factor and two factor models are summarized in
Table 4. Hierarchical chi-square difference tests indicated that the two-
3.1. Analytic overview factor model fit significantly better than the one-factor model for each
version of the scale (CFC-14, CFC-6, and CFC-4).
We conducted an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) on the CFC-14
scale to identify the number of underlying factors, and highest loading 3.4. Reliabilities
items suitable for abbreviated 6-item and 4-item scales. Subsequently,
on the second subsample, we conducted confirmatory factor analysis Based on the EFAs and CFAs, results suggest the presence of two
(CFA) on the CFC-14, CFC-6, and CFC-4 scales to assess the fit of one- factors on each of the three versions of the scale (CFC-14, CFC-6, and
factor and a two-factor models. CFC-4). Next, we computed internal reliabilities for the three scales,
which are shown in Table 5. Examining the internal reliability of the
3.2. Exploratory factor analysis CFC-total scale in the CFC-14, CFC-6 and CFC-4, we observed that the
internal reliability of the CFC-6 and CFC-4 were lower than the CFC-14
Utilizing the first subsample of 2394 participants, an initial EFA on (for the “total” scale). However, the internal reliability of the immediate
the CFC-14 scale, using the principal component method, suggested the and future subscales in the CFC-14, CFC-6 and CFC-4 scales were com­
presence of two underlying principal components. This determination is parable and were all above 0.70, demonstrating good internal
based on the presence of only two eigenvalues >1 (see Table 3), a visual reliability.
inspection of the scree plot (shown in Fig. 1), and as indicated by the
parallel analysis (Horn, 1965; see Fig. 1). The pattern of component 3.5. Regressions
coefficients (following an Oblimin rotation), shown in Table 3, corre­
sponded clearly to the two commonly identified CFC subscales (CFC- To assess the relative predictive validity of the CFC-14, CFC-6, and
Immediate and CFC-Future). CFC-4 scales, we conducted a series of four regression analyses to
Next, based on the items with the highest loadings, we identified a compare the strength of relationship between the scales and the
reasonable six-item short scale (consisting of 3-item short subscales). On respective outcome variables. Table 6 summarizes the results of these
this short scale, the CFC-Future items were items 14, 13, and 1, while the analyses for the ten samples. As can be seen, across all ten samples, the
CFC-Immediate items were items 3, 11, and 4. We subsequently sub­ brief scales did as well as the long scale in predicting the outcome of
mitted these six items to an exploratory factor analysis to determine the interest, with minor changes in the standardized regression coefficients
number of underlying factors. Consistent with our EFA of the full scale, and significance levels, and no change in the overall conclusion reached.
the EFA on the six-item scale also suggested the presence of two prin­ An identical pattern emerged when examining the CFC-Future and CFC-
cipal components based on the eigenvalues (>1; Table 3), scree plot Immediate subscales, with no meaningful changes to the coefficients,

Table 3
Rotated loadings (pattern matrix) from EFA, including subscale internal reliabilities.
CFC-14 Future CFC-6 Future CFC-4 Future

Immediate Immediate Immediate

Immediate items
CFC-3r 0.789 0.040 0.877 0.920
CFC-11r 0.775 0.115 0.673
CFC-4r 0.762 − 0.003 0.850 0.937
CFC-10r 0.655 0.111
CFC-12r 0.636 − 0.045
CFC-9r 0.623 0.077
CFC-5r 0.594 − 0.155
Future items
CFC-14 − 0.018 0.716 0.809 0.917
CFC-13 0.027 0.716 0.829 0.904
CFC-1 0.006 0.674 0.537
CFC-7 0.042 0.636
CFC-2 0.029 0.631
CFC-6 0.022 0.598
CFC-8 − 0.058 0.588
Eigenvalue 4.22 2.29 2.34 1.24 2.02 1.37
% of variance 30.13% 16.35% 39.02% 20.67% 50.45% 34.22%
Internal reliabilities 0.87 0.84 0.84 0.76 0.84 0.79

Note. CFC-3r to CFC5r and CFC9r to CFC12r are recoded ‘immediate’ items.

3
S. Chng et al. Personality and Individual Differences 186 (2022) 111362

using the CFC-14 scale (Joireman et al., 2012). While a large literature
CFC-14 supports the validity of this scale, it is also arguably longer than re­
searchers might like or be able to administer in some settings. In an
effort to facilitate more widespread use of the CFC scales, we developed
short and very short versions of the scale. Utilizing ten samples from the
U.S. and Singapore, exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses sup­
ported the presence of two factors (CFC-Future and CFC-Immediate) and
identified short and very short scales that had both acceptable re­
liabilities and the ability to predict a range of outcomes on par with the
full (CFC-14) scale. In so doing, the present paper contributes to work on
CFC.
In addition to linking CFC with various outcomes (e.g., health,
financial, environmental), researchers interested in CFC have focused
much attention on its measurement. Consistent with a growing number
of studies (e.g., Adams, 2012; Joireman et al., 2008; Joireman et al.,
2012; McKay, Morgan, et al., 2015), the present analyses suggest the
presence of two (rather than one) factors. While this was expected, and
while it is not the main purpose of the present paper, it is worth reit­
erating why the distinction between future and immediate consequences
CFC-6 is important. If researchers recode the immediate items and combine
them with the future items, the result is a global CFC scale, where the
interpretation is not always clear. If it is primarily the future items that
are driving an association, the interpretation based on a global (com­
bined) scale is not misleading. However, if an association is primarily
driven by the recoded immediate items, a researcher might conclude
that “people low in future-oriented concerns engage in X” (e.g., alcohol
use) whereas the data suggest “people high in immediate-oriented
concerns engage in X.” An inspection of the regression analyses in
Table 6 reveals that the primary predictor (CFC-Future or CFC-
Immediate) varies based on the outcome. In addition, it is worth
noting that the correlation between the two subscales was relatively
small (r = − 0.32 in each case), further supporting the distinction. Again,
while this is not the primary purpose of the present study, it is a useful
contribution to the discussion over the factor structure of the scale.
The more important contribution of the present work is the devel­
opment of short (6-item) and very short (4-item) forms of the CFC scale,
with acceptable reliabilities and good predictive ability on par with the
full scale. Notably, our findings overlap with recent work by Vilar et al.
CFC-4 (2020), who also proposed “ultra-short” CFC scales based on college
student data largely from Brazil, with a smaller sample from New Zea­
land. Through their analyses, Vilar proposed a 3-item CFC-Future scale
(items 1, 13, 14; identical to ours), and a 3-item CFC-Immediate scale
(items 3, 10, and 11; with two of the same three items in our scale, which
was composed of items 3, 4, and 11). By comparison, the reliabilities in
the current study (CFC-Future = 0.76, CFC-Immediate = 0.84) tended to
be higher than those reported by Vilar et al. (CFC-Future = 0.72, CFC-
Immediate = 0.68). Complementing and extending Vilar et al., we also
developed very short versions of the subscales (2 items each), which
continued to show good reliability and predictive validity.
It is unclear why our three-item CFC-Immediate scale was slightly
different than the one proposed by Vilar et al. (2020). One possibility is
that Vilar et al.'s data focused mainly on college students from Brazil,
whereas the present data were based on samples with a broader age
range from the U.S. and Singapore. Thus, age and language translation
may have played a role in the different results (e.g., item 10, on Vilar
Fig. 1. EFA scree plots from principal component analysis and parallel analysis et al.'s short scale, may have been easier to translate than item 4, on our
for the CFC-14 (top), CFC-6 (middle), and CFC-4 (bottom). short scale). Future research could test both Vilar et al.'s scale and the
present scale to determine which fits best in a given context.
significance levels, or conclusions. Future research could also examine the predictive validity of the full
and short scales in different domains. While we aimed to test this across
4. Discussion a range of contexts (self-control traits, health, environment, political
preferences), future work is warranted to confirm or qualify the present
Many decisions pose a conflict between short-term and long-term results. If additional studies can support the validity of the short scales,
consequences. One individual difference variable shown to predict this could further advance work on CFC and decision-making in tem­
such choices is the consideration of future consequences, often assessed poral dilemmas.

4
S. Chng et al. Personality and Individual Differences 186 (2022) 111362

Table 4
Confirmatory factor analysis results.
Model fit CFC-14 Two factors CFC-6 Two factors CFC-4 Two factors

One factor One factor One factor

Chi-square 6263.92 1628.36 2353.29 63.48 1393.20 6.06


df 77 76 9 8 2 1
p <.001 <.001 <.001 <.001 <.001 0.014
RMR 0.331 0.120 0.506 0.074 0.523 0.015
GFI 0.610 0.902 0.732 0.991 0.814 0.999
AGFI 0.468 0.864 0.375 0.977 0.074 0.987
CFI 0.551 0.887 0.548 0.989 0.559 0.998
RMSEA 0.184 0.093 0.332 0.054 0.542 0.015
r (CFC-F w/ CFC-I) − 0.32 − 0.32 − 0.32

Note. Chi-square difference tests (one vs. two factors): CFC-14 (χ2(1) = 4635.56, p < .001); CFC-6 (χ2(1) = 2289.91, p < .001); CFC-4 (χ2(1) = 1387.14, p < .001).

Table 5
Internal reliabilities by individual sample and for merged sample.
CFC-total scales CFC subscales

CFC-14 CFC-6 CFC-4 CFC-14 CFC-6 CFC-4

Immediate Future Immediate Future Immediate Future

Sample 1 0.86 0.83 0.76 0.85 0.77 0.86 0.73 0.83 0.72
Sample 2 0.89 0.82 0.75 0.88 0.87 0.86 0.80 0.85 0.83
Sample 3 0.84 0.76 0.68 0.84 0.82 0.81 0.77 0.76 0.70
Sample 4 0.85 0.79 0.70 0.84 0.80 0.85 0.69 0.79 0.68
Sample 5 0.80 0.69 0.64 0.78 0.76 0.71 0.63 0.82 0.71
Sample 6 0.85 0.88 0.82 0.87 0.81 0.84 0.71 0.84 0.73
Sample 7 0.92 0.75 0.68 0.90 0.89 0.90 0.83 0.90 0.85
Sample 8 0.75 0.64 0.53 0.86 0.84 0.82 0.76 0.81 0.82
Sample 9 0.82 0.68 0.57 0.78 0.88 0.66 0.84 0.66 0.85
Sample 10 0.86 0.78 0.72 0.80 0.87 0.70 0.87 0.71 0.98
Total (merged sample) 0.85 0.76 0.68 0.87 0.84 0.83 0.77 0.83 0.80

Note. Reliabilities <0.70 shown in gray.

Table 6
Regressions comparing long, short, very short scales (for both CFC-total and CFC-subscale analyses).
n CFC-14 CFC-6 CFC-4

Total Future Immediate Total Future Immediate Total Future Immediate

Sample 1 (preference for delayed gains) 256 0.17** − 0.04 − 0.23** 0.15* − 0.06 − 0.21** 0.14* − 0.05 − 0.19**
Sample 2 (trait self-control) 701 0.46*** 0.16*** − 0.38*** 0.44*** 0.16*** − 0.36*** 0.42*** 0.17*** − 0.34***
Sample 3 (health eating) 232 0.01 0.14* 0.01 0.12 0.18** 0.02 0.10 0.15* 0.01
Sample 4 (exercise) 119 0.24** 0.24* − 0.06 0.25** 0.27** − 0.05 0.29*** 0.33*** − 0.05
Sample 5 (environmental values) 300 0.50*** 0.52*** − 0.14** 0.45*** 0.41*** − 0.18*** 0.32*** 0.18*** − 0.24***
Sample 6 (2008 U.S. presidential election) 883 − 0.05 − 0.21** − 0.14* − 0.04 − 0.21** − 0.15* − 0.03 − 0.22** − 0.16*
Sample 7 (2012 U.S. presidential election) 301 − 0.03 − 0.08* − 0.03 0.00 − 0.07* − 0.06 0.00 − 0.04 − 0.03
Sample 8 (air conditioning usage) 1705 − 0.19*** − 0.07** 0.21*** − 0.18*** − 0.04 0.19*** − 0.18*** − 0.07** 0.18***
Sample 9 (heart failure self-care) 150 0.28*** 0.22** − 0.15 0.22** 0.16 − 0.13 0.15 0.11 − 0.08
Sample 10 (heart failure self-care) 142 0.16 0.16 − 0.03 0.16 0.15 − 0.05 0.16 0.15 − 0.05

Note.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.

5. Conclusion collection, data analyses and interpretation and manuscript drafting. All
authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.
In this paper, we developed two brief and reliable CFC scales, the
CFC-6 and CFC-4, that perform as well as the CFC-14 in predicting CRediT authorship contribution statement
outcomes across multiple domains. Our findings also support a two-
factor structure of CFC, and hence, both brief scales comprised of a Samuel Chng: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Writing – orig­
CFC-Future and CFC-Immediate subscale with a proportionate number inal draft, Writing – review & editing. Han Shi Jocelyn Chew:
of items. Our CFC-6 and CFC-4 scales will provide researchers with Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing –
shorter alternatives when research in settings where time is limited or a review & editing. Jeff Joireman: Conceptualization, Formal analysis,
more efficient tool is necessary. Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Author contributions Acknowledgements

All authors contributed to the study concept, study design, data We thank the participants for their participation in this study. The

5
S. Chng et al. Personality and Individual Differences 186 (2022) 111362

views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the Hershfield, H. E., Cohen, T. R., & Thompson, L. (2012). Short horizons and tempting
situations: Lack of continuity to our future selves leads to unethical decision making
above funders.
and behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 117, 298–310.
Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis.
Funding Psychometrika, 30, 179–185.
Joireman, J., Balliet, D., Sprott, D., Spangenberg, E., & Schultz, J. (2008). Consideration
of future consequences, ego-depletion, and self-control: Support for distinguishing
This research did not receive any specific grant from funding between CFC-immediate and CFC-future subs-scales. Personality and Individual
agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. Differences, 45, 15–21.
Joireman, J., Kees, J., & Sprott, D. (2010). Concern with immediate consequences
magnifies the impact of compulsive buying tendencies on college students’ credit
References card debt. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 44, 155–178.
Joireman, J., & King, S. (2016). Individual differences in the consideration of future and
Acuff, S. F., Soltis, K. E., Dennhardt, A. A., Borsari, B., Martens, M. P., & Murphy, J. G. (more) immediate consequences: A review and directions for future research. Social
(2017). Future so bright? Delay discounting and consideration of future and Personality Psychology Compass, 10, 313–326.
consequences predict academic performance among college drinkers. Experimental Joireman, J., Shaffer, M. J., Balliet, D., & Strathman, A. (2012). Promotion orientation
and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 25, 412. explains why future-oriented people exercise and eat healthy: Evidence from the
Adams, J. (2012). Consideration of immediate and future consequences, smoking status, two-factor consideration of future consequences 14 scale. Personality and Social
and body mass index. Health Psychology, 31(2), 260–263. Psychology Bulletin, 38, 1272–1287.
van Beek, J., Antonides, G., & Handgraaf, M. J. (2013). Eat now, exercise later: The McKay, M. T., Cole, J. C., & Percy, A. (2015). Further evidence for a bifactor solution for
relation between consideration of immediate and future consequences and healthy the consideration of future consequences scale: Measurement and conceptual
behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 785–791. implications. Personality and Individual Differences, 83, 219–222.
Chew, H. S. J., & Chng, S. (2021). COVID-19 pandemic response behaviors: A Singapore McKay, M. T., Morgan, G. B., van Exel, J., & Worrell, F. C. (2015). Back to “the future”:
experience of the “circuit breaker”. Translational Behavioral Medicine, 11, 808–813. Evidence of a bifactor solution for scores on the consideration of future consequences
Chew, H. S. J., Sim, K. L. D., Choi, K. C., & Chair, S. Y. (2021). Effectiveness of a nurse-led scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 97, 395–402.
temporal self-regulation theory-based program on heart failure self-care: A Milfont, T. L., Bain, P. G., Grigoletti, L. V. S., Gouveia, V. V., & Kashima, Y. (2014).
randomized controlled trial. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 115 (In press). Examining how projections about the future of society are related to present-day
Dassen, F. C., Houben, K., & Jansen, A. (2015). Time orientation and eating behavior: climate change action. Psico, 45, 359–368.
Unhealthy eaters consider immediate consequences, while healthy eaters focus on Strathman, A., Gleicher, F., Boninger, D. S., & Edwards, C. S. (1994). The consideration
future health. Appetite, 91, 13–19. of future consequences: Weighing immediate and distant outcomes of behavior.
Enzler, H. B. (2015). Consideration of future consequences as a predictor of Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 742–752.
environmentally responsible behavior: Evidence from a general population study. Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good
Environment and Behavior, 47, 618–643. adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of
Graso, M., & Probst, T. M. (2012). The effect of consideration of future consequences on Personality, 72, 271–324.
quality and quantity aspects of job performance. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Vilar, R., Milfont, T. L., de Carvalho Rodrigues Araujo, R., de Holanda Coelho, G. L.,
42, 1335–1352. Soares, A. K. S., & Gouvei, V. V. (2020). Consideration of future consequences (CFC):
de Groot, J. I., & Steg, L. (2008). Value orientations to explain beliefs related to Validation and proposition of an ultra-short scale. Current Psychology. https://doi.
environmental significant behavior: How to measure egoistic, altruistic, and org/10.1007/s12144-020-00840-y
biospheric value orientations. Environment and Behavior, 40, 330–354.

You might also like