Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Journal of the History of the Neurosciences

ISSN: 0964-704X (Print) 1744-5213 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/njhn20

Patient Tan Revisited: A Case of Atypical Global


Aphasia?

Ola A. Selnes & Argye Hillis

To cite this article: Ola A. Selnes & Argye Hillis (2000) Patient Tan Revisited: A Case of
Atypical Global Aphasia?, Journal of the History of the Neurosciences, 9:3, 233-237, DOI:
10.1076/0964-704X(200012)9:3;1-A;FT233

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1076/0964-704X(200012)9:3;1-A;FT233

Published online: 14 Jun 2011.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 264

View related articles

Citing articles: 2 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=njhn20
Journal of the History of the Neurosciences 0964-704X/00/0903-233$15.00
2000, Vol. 9, No. 3, pp. 233–237 © Swets & Zeitlinger

Patient Tan Revisited:


A Case of Atypical Global Aphasia?*
Ola A. Selnes and Argye Hillis
Department of Neurology, Division of Cognitive Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine

ABSTRACT

Broca’s first patient presented in support of a relationship between a lesion of the frontal lobe and aphasia
was patient Tan. Although Pierre Marie refers to this case as ‘‘indisputably aphasia of Broca,’’ the clinical
diagnosis of Tan’s aphasia has not been re-examined in light of current clinical criteria. Superficially, the
patient’s extremely limited verbal output and intact comprehension appear to fit with the diagnosis of
Broca’s aphasia, but a more thorough examination of the onset, evolution and nature of the patient’s speech
symptoms suggests alternate interpretations. Contemporary evidence in support of a robust relationship
between stereotypical utterances and Global aphasia suggests that patient Tan may have suffered from a
Global rather than Broca’s aphasia.

INTRODUCTION have combined to make this a pivotal case


(Sondhaus and Finger, 1988).
There is very little question that Broca’s 1861 Although there has been considerable discus-
paper entitled ‘‘Remarks on the seat of the fac- sion concerning the nature, size and location of
ulty of articulate language’’ (Broca, 1861) in the lesion in patient Tan (Marie, 1906; Moutier
which he describes a patient with aphasia sec- 1908; Signoret et al., 1984), there is some agree-
ondary to a frontal lobe lesion, represents a turn- ment that CT scans of the brain show a lesion
ing point in the history of the neurosciences. consistent with the clinical picture of Broca’s
Finger states that this may be ‘‘the most impor- aphasia (Mohr et al., 1999). Relatively little at-
tant clinical paper in the history of cortical lo- tention has been paid to the actual nature of pa-
calization’’(Finger, 1994). He also suggests sev- tient Tan’s aphasia. There appears to be a tacit
eral reasons why Broca’s initial case made such agreement that he, regardless of the specific eti-
an impact, considering the less successful previ- ology of his speech disorder, suffered from what
ous arguments in favor of cortical localization of has subsequently become known as Broca’s
speech by distinguished scientists such as Ernest aphasia. Even Pierre Marie, one of Broca’s
Auburtin and Jean-Baptiste Bouillaud. The im- toughest critics, characterized patient Tan as a
pact of Broca’s case is perhaps even more sur- case of ‘‘indisputably aphasia of Broca’’ (Marie,
prising in light of its numerous shortcomings 1906). MacDonald Critchley, on the other hand,
from a clinico-pathological point of view. None- states that ‘‘correctness of the clinical diagnosis
theless, the richness of the clinical description of has since been challenged,’’ but does not pro-
the case, the detailed anatomical description of vide any further details (Critchley, 1960). Le-
the lesion, together with Broca’s highly re- brun discussed the possibility that the speech
spected status as a clinician and scientist may disorder described by Broca corresponds to what

Address correspondence to: Ola A. Selnes, Ph.D., Meyer 222, 600 N Wolfe St., Baltimore, MD 21287, USA.
Tel.: 410-955-1696. Fax: 410-614-7472. E-mail: oselnes@jhmi.edu
* Paper presented at the 3rd Annual Meeting of the International Society for the History of the Neurosciences
Annapolis, MD June 7-9th, 1998.
234 OLA A. SELNES AND ARGYE HILLIS

we today would call anarthria. However, he con- Was the onset associated with other symptoms
cludes that patient Tan ‘‘is an extreme case of or behavioral changes? (‘‘On n’a pu savoir si la
Broca’s aphasia’’ (Lebrun, 1982). Many apha- perte de la parole était survenue lentement ou
siologists today would distinguish anarthria rapidement , ni si quelque autre symptôme avait
from Broca’s aphasia, in that the former implies accompagné le début de cette affection.’’ p. 343)
impaired neuromuscular function in both speech [One could not find out whether this loss of
and non-speech movements. By contrast, speech came on slowly or fast, nor whether
Broca’s aphasia implies impaired planning of some other symptoms accompanied the begin-
motor sequences for speech. ning of this affection]. Broca estimates that by
At the risk of adding more confusion to an the time Tan arrived at Bicêtre, he had been un-
already complex case, we wish to expand on an able to speak for approximately 2 or 3 months.
idea previously suggested by Poeck and col- What we do know, however, is that there was no
leagues (Poeck, De Bleser, and Keyserlingk, evidence of improvement of his verbal output
1984) and also briefly mentioned by Boller over the several years that he spent at Bicêtre.
(Boller, 1978) to the effect that Broca’s first At the time when he was examined by Broca and
patient may have had an atypical Global aphasia. by Auburtin, 21 years after the onset of his
Although the absence of several critical pieces aphasia, his speech output was still limited to
of information makes it difficult to ‘‘prove’’ ‘‘tan-tan’’ and an occasional swear-word.
such a claim, we believe that several lines of Although no further details are provided
evidence are consistent with this interpretation. about the nature of Tan’s speech, Broca does
include a more general description of the speech
characteristics of aphasia in the first part of the
TAN’S SPEECH DISORDER article:

The critical piece of clinical evidence that sug- ... leur vocabulaire, si l’on peut dire, ainsi, se
gests that patient Tan may have had a Global compose d’une court série de syllabes,
aphasia is the nature of his speech output. It was quelquefois d’un monosyllabe qui exprime
described as being limited to repetitions of the tout, ou plutôt qui n’exprime rien, car ce mot
syllables ‘‘tan,’’ repeated twice. Only limited unique est le plus souvent étranger à tous les
information is available concerning the articula- vocubulaires. Certain malades n’ont même
tion of these recurrent syllables: ‘‘le timbre de pas ce vestige du langage articulé; ils font de
la voix était naturel, et les sons que le malade vains efforts sans prononcer une seule
rendait pour prononcer son monosyllabe étaient syllabe.’’ (p. 332)
parfaitment purs’’ (Brocca, 1861, p. 345) [the
timbre of the voice was natural, and the sounds [their vocabulary, if one can use that word, is
which the patient uttered to pronounce his composed of a short series of syllables, some-
monosyllable were perfectly pure]. There is no times of a monosyllable which expresses ev-
mention of articulatory struggle, slowness, or erything, or rather expresses nothing, for this
imprecise articulation, as would be expected single word is most often than not foreign to
with standard Broca’s aphasia. We do know, all vocabularies. Some patients do not even
however, that these repetitive utterances were have this vestige of articulate language; they
augmented by an occasional swear-word if lis- make vain efforts without pronouncing a sin-
teners did not appear to understand the point that gle syllable].
Tan was trying to make.
An important piece of evidence concerning Thus, Broca describes several features of im-
the patient’s speech difficulty is unfortunately paired speech that subsequently have become
missing. The nature of the onset of his symp- known as stereotypical utterances.
toms is not known. Was the onset sudden or was
there a gradual or stuttering course of onset?
PATIENT TAN’S APHASIA 235

THE SIGNIFICANCE OF STEREOTYPICAL sics was not indicated. Moreover, information


UTTERANCES was collected through questionnaires, and no
definition of what constituted a stereotypical
Starting with Hughlings Jackson, who recog- utterance was provided, which makes it difficult
nized four major subtypes of recurrent stereo- to interpret the significance of these data.
typical utterances (Hughlings-Jackson, 1880), In addition to the less well known association
several authors have since considered stereotyp- of stereotypical utterances with Global aphasia,
ical utterances as diagnostic of severe aphasia there are other reasons why contemporary apha-
(Stachowiak et al., 1977; Brunner et al., 1982). siologists may not have questioned the clinical
It is only more recently, however, that system- diagnosis of Broca’s first patient. For example,
atic studies of patients with stereotypical utter- patient Tan’s ability to comprehend speech was
ances have established an association between judged to be relatively intact, which would
this type of speech output and Global Aphasia clearly be less consistent with Global aphasia.
(Poeck, De Bleser, and Keyserlingk, 1984; Blan- Because patient Tan could neither speak nor
ken et al., 1988; Blanken, Wallesch, and write, however, the assessment of his auditory
Papagno, 1990). Therefore, it is perhaps not sur- comprehension was obviously a challenge. He
prising that the speech output of Broca’s first would indicate his responses by gestures with
patient has not been re-examined in the light of his left hand. Thus, Broca states, ‘‘notre
this more recent evidence. moribond ne pouvait pas se faire comprendre
Several studies have concluded that stereo- aussi bien qu’il comprenait les autres.’’ (p. 345-
typical utterances are characteristic of patients 6) [...our moribund patient could not make him-
with Global aphasia. For example, Poeck and his self understood as well as he could understand
colleagues (Poeck, De Bleser, and Keyserlingk, others].
1984) concluded that recurrent stereotypical ut-
terances are uniquely associated with Global
aphasia. Furthermore, they propose that patients TAN’S COMPREHENSION
who produce exclusively stereotypical utter-
ances with some degree of preserved prosody Since early reports of aphasia generally did not
have a fluent subtype of Global aphasia. Others comment in much detail on the patient’s ability
have also concluded that stereotypical utterances to comprehend speech, Broca’s description of
are characteristic of patients with Global aphasia Tan’s comprehension is surprisingly detailed
(Blanken et al., 1988). (Boller, 1978). Before Wernicke, language was
Although stereotypical utterances of the type generally thought of as verbal expression or
produced by patient Tan may in very rare in- speech, whereas language comprehension was
stances occur in patients whose speech and lan- thought to relate more to general cognitive abili-
guage characteristics otherwise fit the syndrome ties or intelligence (Whitaker and Etlinger,
of Broca’s aphasia, it is quite clear that this is 1993). Broca mentions in his paper that articu-
the exception rather than the rule. It is also late language is probably more important and
likely that whenever this does occur, it is in pa- more complex than comprehension (‘‘le plus
tients who are initially globally aphasic but important et probablement les plus complexe’’ ,
whose auditory comprehension improves to the p. 332). Broca introduces the paragraph in which
point where they can no longer be classified as he describes Tan’s level of comprehension with
Global, and thus by default become classified as the following sentence: ’’L’état de l’intelligence
having Broca’s aphasia. The only large scale n’a peu être exactement déterminé.’’ [The state
study of stereotypical utterances is retrospective of his intelligence could not be determined very
(Code, 1982). It concluded that 26% of patients precisely].
with non-word recurrent utterances were classi- He then goes on to explain that Tan appeared
fied as Broca’s aphasia; however, the number of to understand most questions asked of him, de-
these patients who were recovered Global apha- spite being limited to the use of his left hand to
236 OLA A. SELNES AND ARGYE HILLIS

indicate his responses. He did particularly well strated that patients with Global aphasia perform
with numerical responses, and could indicate, significantly above chance on tasks such as sort-
for example, how many days he had been sick. ing out the odd item from a list of semantically
He sometimes answered 5 days, sometimes 6 related items (Deloche, Andreewsky, and Desi,
days. He could indicate the correct hour when 1981).
Broca showed him his watch. Broca concludes There is another feature of Broca’s initial
from this: ‘‘Il est donc incontestable que cet case that would have made the contemporary
homme était intelligent, qu’il pouvait ré- diagnosis of global aphasia somewhat unlikely.
fléchir...’’ (p. 346) [It is therefore indisputable As Pierre Marie pointed out, Global aphasia un-
that this man was intelligent, that he could accompanied by hemiparesis is sometimes seen,
think...]. but it is not common. It is known that by the
Nonetheless, Broca expresses concern that time patient Tan arrived at Bicêtre, approxi-
Tan’s comprehension may not have been en- mately 2-3 months after the onset of his speech
tirely normal. He states that Tan gave no re- symptoms, he was not hemiplegic. Today,
sponse to several questions that ‘‘a man of aver- Global aphasia without hemiplegia is still un-
age intelligence should have been able to re- common, but certainly well-recognized as a
spond to.’’ In addition, he notes that Tan would clinical entity (Damasio, 1992; Legatt et al.,
express great irritation if the listener did not 1987). The time course of Tan’s neurological
fully understand his responses. In some cases, symptoms, with slowly progressive hemiplegia
Tan’s responses were unambiguous, yet incor- first involving his right arm and subsequently
rect. For example, he had no children, but none- his right leg after the onset of his aphasia, has
theless responded affirmatively when asked if he added further enigma to the etiology of this un-
had children. Broca concludes that there is little fortunate patient’s brain injury.
doubt that there has been a profound decline in Patient Tan’s language disorder clearly fit
Tan’s intelligence, but despite of this, Tan ap- with Broca’s concept of aphemia, as defined by
peared to have more than enough intelligence to him in his 1861 paper. The clinical concept of
carry on a conversation (p. 346). Broca’s aphasia has evolved considerably over
In discussing patient Tan’s level of compre- time, however. As a consequence, this review of
hension, Poeck and his colleagues conclude that the clinical evidence supports the somewhat par-
his comprehension may not have been ade- adoxical conclusion that although patient Tan is
quately challenged by virtue of the ‘‘limited de- generally thought of as being a prototypical ex-
mands on comprehension in a patient institution- ample of Broca’s aphasia, his clinical presenta-
alized for twenty-one years in an asylum of the tion may have been more consistent with what
mid-19th century.’’ In his review of the history we today would think of as Global aphasia.
of comprehension disorders in aphasia, Boller
(1978) also raises the possibility that patient
Tan, based on his comprehension, may fit the ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
clinical category of Global rather than Broca’s
aphasia, but does not elaborate further. The authors thank Dr. Harry A. Whitaker for helpful
There is also another consideration. Level of comments and Dr. Pamela Tallay for her editorial
assistance.
comprehension in patients with Global aphasia
depends on how it is tested. As noted by
Goodglass and Kaplan (1983), patients with
REFERENCES
Global aphasia may perform surprisingly well if
asked questions of immediate personal rele-
Blanken G et al (1988): Producing speech auto-
vance. They also note that many patients with matisms (recurring utterances): looking for what is
Global Aphasia often understand geographic left. Aphasiology 2: 545-556.
place names quite well and can locate them on Blanken G, Wallesch CW, and Papagno C (1990):
an outline map. Other authors have demon- Dissociations of language functions in aphasics
PATIENT TAN’S APHASIA 237

with speech automatisms (recurring utterances). Lebrun Y (1982): Aphasie de Broca et anarthria. Acta
Cortex 26: 41-63. Neurologica Belgica 82: 80-90.
Boller F (1978): Comprehension disorders in aphasia: Legatt AD et al (1987): Global aphasia without hemi-
a historical review. Brain and Language 5: 149- paresis: Multiple etiologies. Neurology 37: 201-
165. 205.
Broca P (1861): Remarques sur le siège de la faculté Marie P (1906): Aphasia from 1861 to 1866: Essay of
du langage articulé, suivies d’une observation Historical criticism on the genesis of the doctrine
d’aphémie (perte de la parole). Bulletin de la of aphasia. Semaine Medicale 26: 565-571.
Sociéte Anatomique de Paris 6: 330-357. Mohr JP et al (1999): Broca aphasia: pathologic and
Brunner RJ et al (1982): Basal ganglia participation in clinical. Neurology 28: 311-324.
language pathology. Brain and Language 16: 281- Moutier F. (1908): L’aphasie de Broca. Paris,
299. Steinheil.
Code C (1982): Neurolinguistic analysis of recurrent Poeck K, De Bleser R, Keyserlingk DF (1984):
utterance in aphasia. Cortex 18: 141-152. Neurolinguistic status and localization of lesion in
Critchley M (1960): Broca’s contribution to aphasia aphasic patients with exclusively consonant-vowel
reviewed a century later. In: Garland H (ed.) Scien- recurring utterances. Brain 107: 199-217.
tific aspects of neurology. Edinburgh: E.S. Living- Signoret JL et al. (1984): Rediscovery of Leborgne’s
stone, Ltd. brain: anatomical description with CT scan. Brain
Damasio AR (1992): Aphasia. New England Journal and Language 22: 303-319.
of Medicine 326: 531-539. Sondhaus E and Finger S (1988): Aphasia and the
Deloche G, Andreewsky E, and Desi M (1981): Lexi- CNS from Imhotep to Broca. Neuropsychology 2:
cal meaning: a case report, some striking phenome- 87-110.
non, theoretical implications. Cortex 17: 147-152. Stachowiak FJ et al. (1977): Die global aphasie: Kli-
Finger S. (1994): Origins of Neuroscience, A History nisches Bild und uberlegungen zur neurolin-
of Explorations into Brain Function. New York, guistischen Struktur. Journal of Neurology 214:
Oxford University Press. 75-87.
Goodglass H, Kaplan E. (1983): The assessment of Whitaker H and Etlinger SC (1993): Theodor
aphasia and related disorders. 2nd., Lea and Meynert’s contribution to classical 19th century
Febiger, Philadelphia. aphasia studies. Brain and Language 45: 560-571.
Hughlings-Jackson J (1880): On affections of speech
from disease of the brain. Brain 2: 203-220.

You might also like