Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537

www.elsevier.com/locate/learninstruc

Implementation and effects of explicit reading comprehension instruction


in fifth-grade classrooms
Rune Andreassen a, Ivar Bråten b,*
a
Østfold University College, Norway
b
Institute for Educational Research, University of Oslo, P.O. Box 1092, Blindern, N-0317 Oslo, Norway
Received 16 February 2010; revised 23 August 2010; accepted 30 August 2010

Abstract

In this intervention study, teachers tried to implement four instructional principles derived from the literature on research-based, explicit
reading comprehension instruction in their fifth-grade classrooms. The principles focused on relevant background knowledge, reading
comprehension strategies, reading-group organization, and reading motivation. Results indicated that during a five-month intervention period,
students in the intervention group increased their strategic competence and comprehension performance relative to controls. However, no effect
was found on reading motivation. The overall pattern of results is explained in relation to the implementation quality of the four instructional
principles, with implementation data indicating that the principles of reading-group organization and reading motivation were particularly
difficult for the teachers to translate into classroom practice.
Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Reading comprehension; Reading instruction; Reading strategies; Reading motivation

1. Introduction which is the mandatory framework for teaching and learning


goals in all classrooms (Norwegian Ministry of Education and
The major purpose of this study was to investigate the effects Research, 2006). Thus, reading instruction in Norwegian class-
of explicit reading comprehension instruction on students’ rooms often involves reading aloud to children (Rasmussen,
strategy use, reading motivation, and comprehension perfor- 2003), asking them control questions after reading (van Daal,
mance in relation to the quality of program implementation. Solheim, Gabrielsen, & Begnum, 2007), and using IRE-
Although reading researchers have paid much attention to sequences (Anmarkrud, 2009; Klette, 2003) where teachers
comprehension instruction in recent years (Murphy, Wilkinson, initiate a discussion, students respond, and teachers evaluate their
Soter, Hennessey, & Alexander, 2009; Pearson, 2009), elemen- responses (Cazden, 1988). At best, such activities could be
tary school teachers, in America as well as in Europe, still seem labelled implicit comprehension instruction (Dole, 2000). In
unsure about how to teach reading comprehension and often test contrast, explicit comprehension instruction is focused on giving
rather than teach comprehension by just concentrating on asking students the procedural and conditional knowledge they need to
students questions about text content after reading (Liang & Dole, learn from text (Paris, Lipson, & Wixson, 1983), and it is
2006; van Keer, 2005). This also characterizes the situation in generally associated with direct instruction in comprehension
Norwegian schools, despite an explicit emphasis on the teaching strategies (Pearson & Dole, 1987).
of reading comprehension strategies in the national curriculum, Presumably, strategies play a prominent role in compre-
hension because readers use them to construct coherent mental
representations and explanations of situations described in
* Corresponding author. Tel.: þ47 22 85 52 82; fax: þ47 22 85 42 50. texts (Graesser, 2007; Graesser, Singer, & Trabasso, 1994).
E-mail address: ivar.braten@ped.uio.no (I. Bråten). Building on the conceptualizations of strategies presented by

0959-4752/$ - see front matter Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.08.003
R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537 521

Alexander, Graham, and Harris (1998) and Weinstein and During the past decades, many studies have demonstrated
Mayer (1986), we define reading comprehension strategies that deeper-level comprehension strategies can be developed
as mental activities selected by the reader to acquire, organize, through explicit teaching (Duffy, 2002). Moreover, positive
and elaborate information, as well as to reflect on and guide effects of such instruction on comprehension performance
their own text comprehension (cf., Bråten & Samuelstuen, have been found in a number of experimental and quasi-
2004). Because readers are assumed to use strategies in experimental studies (Duke & Pearson, 2002; National
order to reduce a perceived discrepancy between a desired Reading Panel, 2000; Trabasso & Bouchard, 2002). Whereas
outcome and their current state of understanding, compre- early intervention research usually focused on the effects of
hension strategies are also regarded as deliberate and goal teaching a single comprehension strategy, such as question
oriented processes used to construct meaning from text (see generation, mental imagery, or summarization, and more often
also, Afflerbach, Pearson, & Paris, 2008). In particular, the use than not took place in laboratory settings and were managed
of deeper-level strategies such as predicting upcoming text by researchers (Trabasso & Bouchard, 2002), the teaching of
content, generating and answering questions, constructing multiple strategies has increasingly been investigated in the
self-explanations and clarifications, capturing the gist of the last two decades, with natural classroom settings and real
text, and monitoring comprehension seems to promote good teachers also replacing laboratories and researchers (Pressley,
reading comprehension (McNamara, 2007; National Reading Graham, & Harris, 2006). The idea is, of course, that more
Panel, 2000; Pressley & Harris, 2006). Such deeper-level comprehensive, multiple-strategies programs implemented in
strategies typically involve drawing inferences based on prior classrooms by teachers may have a greater impact on reading
knowledge related to text content (Pressley, 2000). comprehension than brief laboratory-based interventions
Explicit instruction in comprehension strategies requires administered by researchers that focus on a single strategy.
that teachers move beyond a mere “content approach”, Moreover, comprehension strategies have been taught in
encouraging students to attend to text ideas and build a mental combination with other instructional components, most
representation without considering specific mental processes, notably with cooperative learning activities and group
and employ a “strategies approach”, where students’ mental discussions (Murphy et al., 2009; Slavin, Cheung, Groff, &
processes are directly targeted (McKeown, Beck, & Blake, Lake, 2008). The rationale for the inclusion of social
2009). In their attempt to explain the absence of explicit components in reading comprehension instruction is largely
instruction in comprehension strategies in American schools derived from sociocultural theory, such as the work of
despite the solid evidence of its effectiveness, El-Dinary, Vygotsky (1978) and Rogoff (1990), implying that “peers can
Pressley, and Schuder (1992) argued that strategy instruction function as tutors for one another as they learn how to interpret
may differ too much from the preconceptions teachers have text” (Almasi, 1996, p. 17). Emphasized are the role of the
about skills instruction in reading. Because we believe that this learning community’s total background knowledge and the
explanation may also have merit in the Norwegian educational negotiation of meaning between different readers’ multiple
context, a main assumption in our intervention work has been interpretations of texts (Almasi, 1996; Pressley et al., 1992).
that considerable teacher preparation is needed before explicit Among the best researched and best documented multiple-
reading comprehension instruction can be successfully strategy reading comprehension programs are Reciprocal
implemented in classrooms. Teaching (RT; Palincsar & Brown, 1984), Transactional
Strategy Instruction (TSI; Pressley et al., 1992), and Concept-
1.1. Theoretical basis and prior research on explicit Oriented Reading Instruction (CORI; Guthrie et al., 1996).
reading comprehension instruction And, because the present intervention study was inspired by
those three programs, a brief description of each is in order
The main theoretical basis for this investigation of reading here.
comprehension instruction was Guthrie and Wigfield’s (2000) Essentially, RT (Palincsar & Brown, 1984) involves the
engagement model of the development of reading compre- explicit instruction of the four strategies of predicting, clari-
hension. According to this model, good reading comprehen- fying, questioning, and summarizing and organizes reading-
sion is likely to follow from engaged reading, that is, group dialogues to scaffold students’ strategy use. The
intrinsically motivated and strategic reading to build concep- reciprocity of the interaction between the teacher and the
tual knowledge in collaboration with others. Thus, engaged students and between the students themselves is emphasized as
reading that, in turn, may promote achievement in reading they collaborate in small reading groups to co-construct good
comprehension, is assumed to involve cognitive, social, and text comprehension by means of the four strategies. Gradually,
motivational dimensions: “. engaged readers in the class- group members are expected to take over responsibility by
room and elsewhere coordinate their strategies and knowledge acting as teachers and modeling strategy use for each other
(cognition) within a community of literacy (social) in order to and giving each other feedback. Expository texts related to
fulfil their personal goals, desires, and intentions (motivation)” students’ social studies or science curricula are typically used
(Guthrie & Wigfield, 2000, p. 404). Consequentially, the as reading materials in RT interventions (e.g., Lederer, 2000;
engagement model implies that all three types of engagement Takala, 2006). RT has been evaluated in quite a few studies,
processes (i.e., cognitive, social, and motivational) should be and Rosenshine and Meister (1994), when reviewing this body
supported as part of good reading comprehension instruction. of research, found effect sizes to vary from small negative to
522 R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537

large positive on reading comprehension measures, depending implementation and what happened to students’ reading
on the type of comprehension measure, the type of control strategy use, reading motivation, and reading comprehension
group, and the exact instructional approach. Because imple- as a result of the implementation are addressed in this study.
mentation data have been essentially lacking, however, The first principle, relevant background knowledge,
differences in implementation quality were not discussed as emphasizes that reading comprehension is a process where
a possible explanation for the differing effect sizes. According readers actively construct text meaning by drawing on their
to Pressley et al. (2006), this highlights the need for imple- background knowledge. Activation and generation of relevant
mentation data to better explain why particular interventions background knowledge are thus important instructional
work (or do not work) relative to comparison conditions. concerns within all three programs. While the four strategies
In relation to Palincsar and Brown’s (1984) foundational pivotal to RT involve the activation of background knowledge,
work, TSI (Pressley et al., 1992) and CORI (Guthrie et al., TSI and CORI take this perspective a step further by
1996) can be considered parts of an evolution towards more strengthening the social aspect of meaning construction
long-term, extensive multiple-strategy reading comprehension through shared knowledge activation and knowledge genera-
programs including several instructional components (Allen, tion (Allen, 2003; Guthrie, Wigfield, & Perencevich, 2004). Of
2003; Pressley et al., 2006). Thus, TSI is forged as long- note is that this emphasis on meaning construction through
term comprehension instruction taking place in a natural set- background knowledge activation also accords with several
tingdbeing taught by trained teachers as part of their ordinary prominent theoretical models of reading comprehension and
classroom instruction related to content areas. TSI has resulted learning from text, such as Anderson’s (1994) schema-theo-
in medium to large positive effects on comprehension retic view, Kintsch’s (1988) construction-integration model,
performance, with positive effects also observed on stan- Graesser et al.’s (1994) constructionist model, and Alexander’s
dardized reading tests (Pressley, 2006). In addition, a number (1997) model of domain learning. Moreover, there is solid
of instructional practices intended to promote reading moti- empirical evidence that background knowledge may be
vation and reading engagement feature prominently in CORI. a highly influential determinant of reading comprehension,
In that approach, students collaboratively read and discuss with some of this influence mediated through its positive
interesting self-selected texts to increase their knowledge influence on reading-strategy use (Cromley & Azevedo, 2007;
about science topics and concepts introduced through hands- Fox, 2009).
on science activities. While quite large positive effects of The second principle, reading comprehension strategies,
CORI on reading strategies and reading comprehension have holds that direct teaching of a limited number of reading strat-
been found in several studies, there is also substantial effect egies may provide readers with the tools they need to construct
size variation across studies, especially with respect to reading deep-level, inferential understanding of text. The four strategies
motivation (Guthrie, McRae, & Klauda, 2007). Such variation originally recommended by Palincsar and Brown (1984), that is,
is hard to explain, however, without implementation data that predicting, questioning, clarifying, and summarizing, still seem
can provide a window on what actually happens in classrooms to be main ingredients in many comprehension instruction
during an intervention, particularly on the extent to which the programs, including TSI and CORI. Across programs, such
different components of a complex intervention program are strategies are explicitly labelled and taught not only by expla-
really implemented. nation but also by repeated modeling and scaffolding support,
with the goal being self-regulated strategy use on part of the
1.2. What should explicit reading comprehension students (Hilden & Pressley, 2007). According to Taboada and
instruction be the instruction of? Guthrie (2004), strategic readers may be characterized by
competence, meaning that they can use a strategy well, aware-
Our instructional framework, simply called Explicit ness, meaning that they also know about the strategy and when
Reading Comprehension Instruction (ERCI), was based on and where this strategy works, and self-initiation, meaning that
a careful review of the literature on the three multiple-strategy they deliberately choose to use the strategy when it is appro-
programs mentioned above (i.e., RT, TSI, and CORI). That priate. To become self-regulated strategic readers, students need
review resulted in the extraction of four main principles to develop all those three qualities, which is a long-term
characterizing research-based, explicit comprehension development that may sometimes take years of strategy
instruction, with these principles also consistent with the instruction (Hilden & Pressley, 2007). Also, training teachers to
engagement model of Guthrie and Wigfield (2000). We provide comprehension strategies instruction seems to require
intentionally chose the term instructional framework to considerable time and effort and should, preferably, be collab-
describe our intervention because it rested on a set of princi- orative, gradual, and sensitive to changing contextual conditions
ples rather than a lesson-to-lesson manual, with these princi- from classroom to classroom (Block & Duffy, 2008).
ples framing teachers’ later development of more complete As indicated above, background knowledge may be needed
lessons for implementation (Liang & Dole, 2006). Thus, by for reading comprehension strategies to occur. One reason that
means of teacher - researcher cooperation prior to the inter- we still distinguish between the principles of relevant back-
vention, six Norwegian fifth-grade teachers were prepared to ground knowledge and reading comprehension strategies is
implement the four principles in their classrooms during that background knowledge has been shown to have an effect
a five-month period. How well they succeeded in their directly on comprehension in addition to an effect that is
R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537 523

mediated by strategies (Cromley & Azevedo, 2007). More- & Wigfield, 2000). In particular, intrinsic reading motivation,
over, use of comprehension strategies is not always related to that is, the enjoyment of reading activities that are performed
readers’ level of prior knowledge about text content for their own sake (Ryan & Deci, 2000), is an outcome
(McNamara & Magliano, 2009). The distinction between the measure on a par with strategy and comprehension measures
principles of relevant background knowledge and reading in Guthrie and co-workers’ intervention studies (e.g., Guthrie
comprehension instruction strategies is further clarified in et al., 2004). From temporary positive affect and arousal
descriptions of the intervention, the seminars introducing the triggered by exciting activities and materials in the context,
principles to the intervention teachers, and how the imple- termed situational interest (Hidi & Harackiewicz, 2000),
mentation of the two principles was assessed (see below). instructors thus seek to build long-term enthusiasm or intrinsic
Obviously, activation and generation of relevant background motivation for reading in their students.
knowledge do not necessarily involve the teaching of reading Somewhat paradoxically, the components of reading inter-
comprehension strategies. vention programs are often described in great detail, but the
The third principle, reading-group organization, is related way in which the programs are implemented in the classroom
to the social aspect of comprehension instruction. Thus, RT, is more seldom extensively reported on (Gersten, Fuchs,
TSI, and CORI all incorporate variants of cooperative Williams, & Baker, 2001; Pressley et al., 2006). This lack of
learning, with development towards self-regulated strategy use implementation or process data is problematic because infor-
purportedly facilitated by cooperation in small reading groups mation is not provided concerning the quality with which the
where students share thoughts about how texts can be com- different components of the intervention are put into practice.
prehended by means of multiple strategies. In such groups, Moreover, implementation or process data may help explain
students differing with respect to abilities and skills are the varying effects on outcome variables found in different
supposed to provide scaffolding support for each other’s studies (Pressley et al., 2006; Rosenshine & Meister, 1994). As
comprehension processes. Our review of the literature also indicated above, even within the same instructional frame-
indicated that reading groups are mostly teacher-led in RT (at work, such as CORI, effects are found to vary substantially
least in the original version) and mostly student-led in CORI, with respect to both reading motivation and reading compre-
with TSI seemingly falling somewhere in-between. The fact hension (Guthrie et al., 2007). In response to this under-
that both teachers and students function as scaffolders of specification of what is going on in classrooms during an
comprehension processes in these programs (e.g., Brown, intervention, we took care to collect implementation data in
Pressley, van Meter, & Schuder, 1996) may, at least in part, this study that could help us explain the effects (or lack of
be a virtue of necessity. In real classroom situations, it is not effects) of ERCI on students.
possible for one teacher to lead and scaffold several reading It should also be noted that the contribution that different
groups at the same time and, therefore, groups have to work skills and processes make to reading comprehension may
partly independent of the teacher, acting as teachers for each depend on how individuals are asked to indicate their under-
other. The third principle is based on the premise that higher standing of what they read (Pearson & Hamm, 2005), with, for
psychological processes originate in relations between indi- example, the demands for both lower-level word recognition
viduals and, in particular, in collaboration with more knowl- skills and strategic processes varying across reading compre-
edgeable others (Vygotsky, 1978). The principle has been hension tests (Cain & Oakhill, 2006; Magliano, Millis, Ozuru,
difficult to implement in US classrooms, however (Rosenshine & McNamara, 2007). In particular, deeper-level comprehen-
& Meister, 1994), and it can also be expected to represent sion strategies are probably most needed when tests require
a challenge in the Norwegian context, with generally large that readers construct a mental representation that reflects
variations in student abilities and little experience with coop- deep, inferential understanding of text content (Graesser,
erative learning (Klette, 2003; van Daal et al., 2007). 2007; Magliano et al., 2007). Moreover, it can be assumed
The fourth principle, reading motivation, is derived from that such strategies play a more important role when text is
TSI, relating comprehension strategy instruction to authentic unavailable during testing (Andreassen & Bråten, 2010). This
subject matter, and, even more clearly, from CORI’s engage- is because readers in this situation probably have to use
ment perspective. The principle emphasizes that strategy use is comprehension strategies to construct an adequate mental
facilitated by interesting texts (Pressley, 2006) and opportu- representation of text content during reading, whereas readers
nities for choice (Guthrie et al., 2004), and that students need may rely more on strategies related to searching for the correct
to see the usefulness of strategies for reaching their reading answer when text is available during testing. Another reason is
goals when working with content area texts (Brown et al., that readers may perceive the task as more challenging when
1996). This implies that reading comprehension strategies the text is unavailable and, therefore, deem it necessary to
should be taught in social contexts characterized by a variety invest more strategic effort in order to solve it. Finally, deeper-
of interesting texts that students can choose among to increase level comprehension strategies are likely to play a larger role
their conceptual understandings. Of note is that in CORI, when assessments use longer compared to shorter passages
reading motivation is not only considered to facilitate (Andreassen & Bråten, 2010), for example because longer
comprehension strategy use but is regarded as a goal in itself, passages may require more strategic processing (e.g., pre-
which together with strategy use, conceptual understanding, dicting, summarizing) to create coherent representations
and social interaction constitute reading engagement (Guthrie (Graesser et al., 1994).
524 R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537

1.3. The present study time, however, we entertained the possibility that the effects
on our outcome variables might depend on the extent to which
Given this theoretical background analysis, we set out to the different principles were implemented by the teachers
study the effects of teachers’ implementation of the four ERCI (Hypothesis 1b). Finally, based on both theoretical accounts
principles (i.e., relevant background knowledge, reading (e.g., Graesser, 2007; Graesser et al., 1994; Magliano et al.,
comprehension strategies, reading-group organization, and 2007) and prior empirical evidence (e.g., Andreassen &
reading motivation) in the content area of social studies on Bråten, 2010; Cromley & Azevedo, 2007), we expected that
fifth graders’ reading strategy use, reading motivation, and any improvement in deeper-level strategic processing as
reading comprehension, respectively. Because we assumed a result of the intervention would coincide with an improve-
that the teachers would need considerable preparation to ment on a reading comprehension task where students read
implement those principles in their classrooms, professional one long passage of expository text and then answered an
development in the form of collaborative seminars was equal number of inferential and factual questions with text
provided in the semester before the intervention took place. unavailable during responding, but not necessarily with
Still, we collected observation and questionnaire data to assess improvement on comprehension tests involving the reading of
to what extent the principles were actually implemented in the shorter passages, a larger proportion of factual questions, and
ERCI teachers’ classrooms during the intervention period. text available during responding (Hypothesis 2). The reason
Moreover, to better isolate the effects of our instructional for this expectation is, of course, that the former task may
approach, we tried to statistically control for the potential demand more of students’ deep-level processing resources
effects of word recognition and working memory by using than the latter tasks.
those variables as covariates when comparing the intervention
group with controls on the outcome measures after the inter-
vention period. The effects of word recognition and working
memory on reading comprehension are well documented (for 2. Method
reviews, see Swanson, Howard, & Sáez, 2007; Vellutino,
2003), and there is also reason to believe that strategic pro- 2.1. Participants
cessing is influenced by word recognition and working
memory, with skilled word recognition and good working The intervention group consisted of 55 girls and 48 boys in
memory capacity presumably freeing adequate cognitive five mixed-ability fifth-grade classes at three different schools
resources for reading strategically to comprehend text in a small town in south-east Norway. The teachers of these
(Sinatra, Brown, & Reynolds, 2002; Walczyk et al., 2007). students (4 female, 1 male) volunteered to participate in the
Because it is conceivable that skilled word recognition and intervention and students participated with permission from
good working memory capacity also increase students’ moti- their parents. The teachers ranged in instructional practice
vation to read, we included word recognition and working from 5 to 34 years (M ¼ 16 years). The control group con-
memory as covariates even for reading motivation outcomes. sisted of 64 girls and 49 boys from six randomly selected
Finally, our design (see below) did not preclude pre-inter- mixed-ability fifth-grade classes at the remaining schools in
vention differences between intervention group and controls the same town. These students were taught by 5 female and 1
with respect to the outcome variables and, therefore, pre- male teachers, ranging in instructional practice from 3.5 to 30
intervention measurements of strategy use, reading motivation, years (M ¼ 23 years). Permission was also given by the
and reading comprehension, respectively, were also included parents of the control students. The participating students’
as covariates in our analyses examining the effects of ERCI. overall mean age at pre-test was 10.5 years. Because of
Specifically, we addressed the following four questions in missing values, the number of students in the two groups
our investigation: available for statistical analyses involving the outcome
measures varied somewhat, with the exact number of partici-
1. What is the effect of the ERCI intervention on fifth pants given in connection with each analysis (see Results
graders’ use of comprehension strategies? section below).
2. What is the effect of the ERCI intervention on fifth The vast majority of the participants were white native
graders’ reading motivation? speakers of Norwegian, with only 14 participants (12 in the
3. What is the effect of the ERCI intervention on fifth intervention group, 2 in the control group) having a first
graders’ reading comprehension? language other than Norwegian, Swedish and Danish (which
4. How are the effects (or lack of effects) on strategy use, have rather similar orthographies). All the participants atten-
motivation, and comprehension performance related to the ded regular classes but seven of them (5 in the intervention
quality of the implementation of the four instructional group, 2 in the control group) were given special education
principles? due to reading and writing difficulties. The average class size
was 22 students in the intervention classes and 21 students in
Given the time devoted to teacher preparation before the the control classes. At least in an international perspective, the
intervention started, we expected to see some positive effects sample was homogeneous (i.e., middle class) with respect to
on all three outcome variables (Hypothesis 1a). At the same socioeconomic status.
R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537 525

2.2. Design and procedure communities of learners interacting to construct background


knowledge that can facilitate comprehension of the topic in
We used a non-equivalent groups pretesteposttest design. question.
Although schools and classes were not randomly assigned to
conditions, they were comparable in the sense that they were 2.3.2. Strategies instruction through modeling and
located in the same municipality and included students in the scaffolding
same grade taught according to the same curriculum by The second practice refers to the explicit teaching of pre-
experienced teachers in mixed-ability classrooms. Moreover, dicting, questioning, clarifying, and summarizing (Palincsar &
possible pre-test differences were statistically controlled for. Brown, 1984), with those strategies used to connect readers’
Before the intervention started, professional development was background knowledge to the content of the texts. Specifically,
provided during five 3-h meetings over a period of three modeling involves teachers thinking aloud during reading, and
months. ERCI was then implemented in five 45-m social scaffolding involves gradually shifting responsibility for stra-
studies lessons a week over a period of 18 weeks, from tegic text processing to the students (cf., Wood, Bruner, &
January to May in Grade 5. In the same period, the students in Ross, 1976). Inspired by Oczkus (2005), the intention was to
the control group were taught according to the same social make modeling more motivating and understandable to
studies curriculum using ordinary practices, that is, with no students by presenting them with four characters representing
special emphasis on reading comprehension instruction. the strategies. After the initial presentation and modeling of
Pre-test data on reading strategy use, reading motivation, the strategies, teachers should thus put these characters on
and reading comprehension were collected during two weeks classroom posters, bookmarks, and cue cards reminding the
in the autumn term of Grade 5, and post-test data on the same students to use the strategies to help them comprehend and
variables were collected five months later, that is, after the learn from text. Specifically, a fortune teller represented pre-
intervention. Pre-test data were also collected with respect to dicting, a curious boy represented questioning, a detective
word recognition and working memory. All materials were represented clarifying, and a cowboy with a lasso represented
group-administered to students in the intervention and control summarizing. The aim of the second practice is to help
groups in their ordinary classrooms by the first author. students appropriate the four strategies on the basis of exten-
Implementation data were collected by means of classroom sive modeling and scaffolding on part of the teachers.
observations and questionnaires at the beginning and at the
end of the intervention period. The intervention, the profes- 2.3.3. Small-group interactions during strategic reading
sional development, the measures, and the assessment of The third practice refers to small groups of students
implementation quality are further described below in that working together while predicting what the author will discuss
order. next, generating good questions to text, clarifying challenging
terms and expressions, and summarizing previously read
2.3. Intervention passages. After a period of initial training, the groups are
expected to complete assigned reading tasks in this way
The instructional content consisted of topics from the without any direct supervision from the teacher, requiring that
Norwegian national fifth-grade social studies curriculum. This the group work is well structured and that the structure is
content area was chosen because all teachers involved in the learned in advance. Ideally, the groups should consist of 4e5
intervention taught social studies in their fifth-grade class- students, and the reading task should be clearly defined to the
rooms. Moreover, social studies require extensive expository students and require the participation of all group members
text comprehension. The specific topics of instruction were (cf., Cohen, 1994). In ERCI, the group talk should be char-
Nordic Stone Age, Bronze Age, and Iron Age, as well as acterized by students thinking aloud about their own and other
Norwegian natural landscape and geography. The same topics students’ strategy use to make text comprehension processes
were taught in the same order to the control group during the an object of discussion. In accordance with the general
same period. Within ERCI, teachers attempted to use the recommendations of the Norwegian national curriculum,
following four instructional practices, which were derived mixed-ability reading groups are the rule in ERCI.
from the principles of relevant background knowledge,
reading comprehension strategies, reading-group organization, 2.3.4. Relating reading to student interest in subject matter
and reading motivation (see above). As particularly emphasized in CORI, the fourth instructional
practice refers to the promotion of interest-driven, concept-
2.3.1. Classroom dialogues to activate and generate oriented reading. ERCI teachers were thus encouraged to
relevant background knowledge identify the central social studies concepts taught during the
The implementation of relevant background knowledge intervention and to stimulate students’ interest and curiosity to
involves that teachers focus on activating prior and building read and learn about those concepts by means of real world
new, related knowledge in the students that is relevant to what interactions, demonstrations, and field trips. Moreover,
they read. Through deeper-level questions, classroom dia- students’ interest in developing conceptual understanding
logues, and engaged discussions teachers thus seek to promote through reading is supposed to be supported by allowing them
knowledge-driven reading (Guthrie et al., 2004) within choice among subtopics and texts according to their interests
526 R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537

rather than having them stick to one single textbook common to activities provided by the teachers and the researcher in
all students. For this purpose, relevant trade books to supplement cooperation in parentheses):
the textbook were provided through cooperation with the local
library. ERCI teachers were encouraged to guide the students to How can the four strategies be introduced to my students
match texts to their interests and their actual reading levels, (e.g., explaining the importance of using the strategies,
however. The fourth practice is especially targeting intrinsic teacher think-alouds, use of four characters to represent the
motivation in attempting to catch and hold their interest in strategies, posters with the characters on classroom walls)?
reading about substantive concepts (Guthrie & Wigfield, 2000; How can my students be taught to use the strategies (e.g.,
Hidi & Harackiewicz, 2000). repeated modeling, reading aloud and employing strategies
together with the students, cooperative reading groups,
2.4. Professional development scaffolding students’ strategy use, reminding strategy use)?
How can modeling help my students learn the strategies
The four principles derived from RT, TSI, and CORI were (e.g., observing best practice, modeling for each other by
introduced to the ERCI teachers as a group by the first author thinking aloud)?
and personalized by means of five 3-h collaborative seminars How can I create progression (e.g., from teacher modeling
from September through November the semester before the and scaffolding to students modeling for each other to
intervention started. In these seminars, instructional practice students self-regulating strategy use, from teaching one
related to each principle was discussed, resulting in a list of strategy at a time to teaching all strategies together)?
example activities that was handed out to the teachers after the
last meeting. Except for one teacher who attended four
seminars, all teachers attended five seminars. In the following,
we briefly describe the content of the five seminars. 2.4.3. Fourth seminar
This seminar focused on reading-group organization with
the aims of providing the teachers with an understanding of
2.4.1. First seminar the social aspect of comprehension instruction and discussing
This seminar focused on the importance of background examples of how cooperative learning can be used in the
knowledge. After a short introduction on the role of back- classroom. The following questions guided the discussion
ground knowledge in reading comprehension, examples of (examples provided by the teachers in cooperation with the
how students’ background knowledge could be activated and researcher in parentheses):
complemented during classroom dialogues and social inter-
actions were discussed. The three questions guiding this How can social interactions during reading and strategy
discussion were (example activities provided by the teachers use be organized in my classroom (e.g., small groups (4e5
and the researcher in cooperation are given in parentheses): students), peer teaching, progression from teacher-led
whole class activities to student-led cooperative groups)?
How can my classroom practice focus on students’ diverse Which criteria can be used to create groups (e.g.,
background knowledge and the construction of new heterogeneity regarding reading ability, separation of
knowledge (e.g., classroom dialogues, concept maps, think- particular students)?
alouds)? What are good cooperation skills (e.g., using low voice,
How can classroom dialogues activate relevant back- listening, asking for explanations, giving positive feedback,
ground knowledge (e.g., asking deeper-level questions, specific and concrete criticism)?
accepting and praising different points of view, avoiding How can students be taught to cooperate (e.g., teacher
right/wrong responses, more time for student-talk opposed demonstrations, more proficient groups modeling for less
to teacher-talk, modeling own thinking)? proficient groups, cooperation support by teacher, group
What can deeper-level questions be like (e.g., why ques- evaluation at the end of lessons)?
tions rather than who-what-where questions, thought- How can struggling readers be helped (e.g., better readers
provoking questions rather than text-based questions, reading aloud or together with struggling readers, groups
in-depth questions rather than surface questions)? reading together, temporary homogeneous groups for
struggling readers)?

2.4.2. Second and third seminar


These seminars focused on the four comprehension strate-
gies, aiming at giving the teachers an understanding of those 2.4.4. Fifth seminar
strategies and discussing how they can be effectively taught in The last seminar focused on the motivational aspect of
the classroom. Moreover, the strategies were modeled by the comprehension instruction. After a brief lecture on the moti-
researcher, and the teachers were encouraged to model their vational components emphasized in CORI, the following
own strategic thinking for each other during reading. The questions guided the discussion (examples provided by the
following questions guided the discussion (examples of teachers and the researcher in cooperation in parentheses):
R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537 527

How can students be introduced to and become interested vertical lines between the words. The scoring was done by
in the topic of study? (e.g., classroom dialogues, real-world counting the number of correct word chains. Høien and
experiences, field trips to historical places, visiting Tønnesen (1997) reported a split-half reliability of .99 for
museums, diverse learning materials, interesting books)? this measure. In the present study, the reliability estimate
To what extent may students choose subtopics of partic- (Cronbach’s alpha) for scores on the Word Chain Test was .94.
ular interest (from few choices in the beginning to gradu- With respect to validity, prior research (e.g., Bråten, Lie,
ally more choices according to interests)? Andreassen, & Olaussen, 1999) indicates that this task
To what extent may students choose books according to favors readers with accessible and well-established ortho-
interests (topic-relevant trade books that the students might graphic representations in the lexicon, and it can thus be
select made available through the local library, guidance of assumed to assess orthographically (rather than phonologi-
students to select texts according to interests and reading cally) based word recognition (Bråten, Lie, & Andreassen,
skills)? 1998). Also according to Jacobson (1995), this task is valid
for assessing word recognition skills, correlating with
No professional development was provided for the teachers measures of both silent reading and oral word reading.
of the control group. They were briefly informed about the
project and their role as control group teachers before the 2.5.2.2. Working memory measure. To assess working
intervention started. In addition, they were only contacted to memory as the simultaneous storage and processing of infor-
make appointments in connection with the pre- and post-tests. mation (Baddeley, 1990), we used a Norwegian adaption of
After the post-test, a questionnaire concerning their reading Swanson and Trahan’s (1992) Working Memory Span Task,
lessons and social studies instruction was sent to them. To try which is an adaptation for children of the Reading Span Test
to avoid compensatory rivalry in participants not receiving developed for adults by Daneman and Carpenter (1980), with
treatment (Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002), the control no reading required for the children. Seven sets of unrelated
teachers were offered similar professional development the sentences were read aloud to the participants with a 2-s
next school year. interval between the sentences. The number of sentences in
a set was gradually increased from two to five. The students
2.5. Measures were required to remember the final word in each sentence so
that they could later write it down on a response form handed
2.5.1. Overview of the assessment out prior to reading. To insure that the students comprehended
Word recognition, working memory, strategy use, reading (i.e., processed) the sentences and did not merely treat this
motivation, and reading comprehension were assessed before task as one of verbal memory for the final words, they were
the intervention started, during two weeks in November/ also required to write down the answer to a question related to
December in Grade 5. After the intervention, during two one of the sentences (randomly selected) just after a set was
weeks in May/June in Grade 5, strategy use and reading presented and before they wrote down the final words. All the
motivation were assessed once more with the same measures, sentences and the comprehension questions were presented
and reading comprehension was assessed with three other orally to the students by the test administrator.
measures. At both pre-test and post-test, the first author group Three practice trials were administered in dialogue with the
administered all measures to both the ERCI and the control students. Items were scored zero if the question was not
classes, assisted by one teacher or teacher assistant in each answered correctly, no matter how many final words were
class. The assessment required two 45-m periods at both pre- remembered. If the question was answered correctly, the
test and post-test. Next, we describe each specific measure scoring was done by counting the number of final words
included in the assessment. remembered. Words were not judged to be errors if spelled
incorrectly. Maximum score was 23. Cronbach’s alpha for
2.5.2. Pre-test measures scores on the measure was .79 in this study.
Please note that the measures of word recognition, working
memory, strategy use, and reading comprehension adminis- 2.5.2.3. Comprehension strategy measure. As a measure of
tered at pre-test and described in the following were used as comprehension strategies, we used a Norwegian adaptation of
covariates in statistical analyses. Lederer’s (2000) strategy assessment tool, with this approach
also used by Guthrie et al. (2004) in assessing the effects of
2.5.2.1. Word recognition test. To assess word recognition CORI. The assessment focused on the strategies described by
skills, we used a standardized measure called the Word Chain Palincsar and Brown (1984), and the participants were
Test (Høien & Tønnesen, 1997), designed to assess the ability instructed to write down their predictions, questions, clarifi-
to recognize whole words rapidly. Participants viewed 90 word cations, and summaries during the study of a particular text. In
chains, each containing four words written without any space the sense of Taboada and Guthrie (2004), the strategy
between the words. The words were high-frequency words assessment presumably targeted strategic competence more
presumably well known to the participants, both phonologi- that awareness and self-initiation. Two different expository
cally and semantically. Participants were instructed to indicate texts with the same length and difficulty level were used
as many words as possible during a 4-m period by drawing during strategy assessment, one about knights in the Middle
528 R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537

Ages and one about Sámi in Northern Scandinavia. Both were motivation (sample item: I read to learn something new about
selected from middle-school social studies textbooks, but things that interest me) and 18 items written to measure
neither topic had been studied by the participants nor as part of extrinsic reading motivation (sample item: I like being the best
their social studies courses. The participants were assigned at reading in my class). The participants rated each item on
randomly to one of the two texts and were presented with a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from does not fit at all (1)
a brief written instruction in connection with the assessment of to fits very well (5).
each strategy. Please note that these texts were not used to An initial principal component analysis with oblique rota-
assess reading comprehension. tion on the 36 motivation items yielded 10 factors with
In the assessment of prediction, participants saw only the eigenvalues greater than 1 that explained 65.6% of the total
headline of the text and a corresponding picture, with an sample variation. Based on the sizes of the eigenvalues and the
instruction to write down what they thought the following text scree plot, we explored the underlying structure of the reading
might be about. Scores from 0 to 4 were given according to the motivation scores further by trying to force three- as well as
number of relevant concepts included in the prediction and the two-factor solutions on all the items. After having deleted
extent to which those concepts were connected in the predic- items that did not load on any factor or loaded significantly on
tion. One additional point was awarded when participants more than one factor, two factors with high loadings (>.40)
explained the basis for their prediction. and no overlap for any item were identified. This two-factor
Question generating was assessed by asking participants to solution included 26 items of the reading motivation ques-
write down three good questions to the text after reading. tionnaire. The two factors had eigenvalues of 6.52 and 3.45,
Scoring was done by awarding from 0 to 4 points for each respectively, and explained 36.9% of the total sample varia-
question, dependent on the number of important concepts tion. The first factor consisted of 16 items pertaining to
included in the questions. intrinsic motivation, with all items originally included to
Clarifying was measured by having participants write down address the intrinsic category of motivation. The second factor
explanations for two words included in the text after reading, consisted of 10 items focusing on extrinsic reading motivation,
with the meaning of those words unknown to the participants with all items originally included to assess the extrinsic
before reading the text. Scores from 0 to 3 were given for each category of motivation. On the basis of this factor analysis,
word, with 1 point awarded for a partly correct definition and 2 two scales measuring intrinsic and extrinsic reading motiva-
points awarded for a completely correct definition. One tion, respectively, were constructed. After having divided the
additional point was awarded if participants were able to scores on the scales by the number of items, the scores on each
describe how they reasoned when working on the text to scale ranged from 1 to 5. The reliabilities (Cronbach’s alpha)
clarify the meaning of the word. for the scores on the scales measuring intrinsic and extrinsic
Finally, summarizing was assessed by asking participants to reading motivation, respectively, were .87 and .83.
make a brief summary of the text after reading, with four lines
available for writing down the summary. According to the 2.5.2.5. Reading comprehension measure. At pre-test, we
number of important concepts included in the summary, and used a standardized Norwegian version of a Danish test called
the degree of connection between the concepts, summarizing the Sentence Reading Test (SRT) (Nielsen, Kreiner, Poulsen,
was scored from 0 to 5. & Søegård, 1995). This test is frequently used as a screening
The participants were given 20 min to complete the strategy test of reading comprehension in Norway as well as in the
assessment. A sum score was calculated by adding the scores other Scandinavian countries. It consists of 40 short text
for all four strategy tasks. The maximum score was 28. passages, each of them linked to five pictures printed below
Interrater reliability was estimated by two raters (the first the passage. The participants were instructed to choose among
author and a trained college student) independently deter- the five alternative pictures the one that represented the
mining the strategy scores for a randomly selected 15% of the meaning of the above printed passage by drawing a diagonal
participants, with the interrater reliability (Cohen’s kappa) across that picture. Please note that the text passage was
over items equalling .88. Cronbach’s alpha for participants’ available during responding. There was a gradually increasing
scores on the comprehension strategy measure was .65. length of the passages throughout the testing, from one to three
sentences and from seven to 27 words. The total number of
2.5.2.4. Measures of reading motivation. To assess reading words in this test was 363. Of the 40 items of the SRT, six (i.e.
motivation, we used a Norwegian adaptation of Swan’s (2003) 15%) seemed to require inferences in the sense that students
version of the Motivation for Reading Questionnaire (MRQ) had to combine information from more than one sentence to
(Wigfield & Guthrie, 1997). In Swan’s version of the MRQ, identify the correct picture. The rest of the items could be
items are divided into the two basic categories of intrinsic and classified as literal questions because the correct picture could
extrinsic motivation, with intrinsic motivation including the be identified on the basis of an explicit description in one
aspects reading efficacy, challenge, curiosity, involvement, and sentence of the passage. Participants were given 5 min to read
importance, and extrinsic motivation including the aspects of the passages and mark the corresponding pictures. Although
recognition, grades, social, competition, compliance, and the test manual for the SRT indicates a time limit of 10 min,
reading work avoidance. Accordingly, our 36-item question- piloting the SRT in two fifth-grade classrooms not included in
naire consisted of 18 items written to measure intrinsic reading the investigation indicated that a time limit of 5 min was more
R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537 529

appropriate to avoid ceiling effects. The reliability estimate three (i.e., 33%) could be categorized as inferential questions
(Cronbach’s alpha) for participants’ SRT scores was .92. while six could be categorized as literal questions, with the
last category only requiring that participants were able to
2.5.3. Post-test measures remember or locate a piece of factual information explicitly
Please note that the measures of strategy use, reading stated in the text (cf., McKenna & Stahl, 2009). In accordance
motivation, and reading comprehension administered at post- with the test manual, participants were given 4 min to read the
test and described in the following were used as dependent three text passages and answer the nine comprehension
variables in statistical analyses. questions. According to the test manual (Læringssenteret,
2003), the split-half reliability for the scores in a representa-
2.5.3.1. Comprehension strategy and reading motivation tive sample of 1587 Norwegian fifth-graders equalled .83. In
measures. The measures used to assess strategy use and reading the present sample, Cronbach’s alpha for scores on the NLCT
motivation, respectively, were the same as at pre-test. The only was .75.
exception was that during strategy assessment, each participant The third measure of reading comprehension used at post-
was assigned to the alternative text at post-test. There was no test was a multiple-choice measure with text unavailable
relationship between text alternative and strategy use, however during responding. This was a researcher-developed test con-
(pre-test: r ¼ .01, ns; post-test: r ¼ .12, ns). sisting of a 210-word expository text and 11 multiple-choice
comprehension questions. The text was selected from a fifth-
2.5.3.2. Reading comprehension measures. Reading compre- grade social studies textbook not used by the participants, and
hension was assessed by means of three tests of different the topic of the passage was Norwegian fisheries. We simply
formats, also varying with respect to their demands on infer- called this test the researcher-generated test (RGT). Each of
ence generation. The first measure was a maze task consisting the 11 multiple-choice questions had four response options,
of an 1169-word narrative text in the form of an African fable, and participants were to answer all questions after the reading
much used for assessing reading comprehension in Norwegian of the entire passage. Of the 11 questions, five (i.e., 45%)
middle-grade classrooms (Nordgreen, 1994). The maze task could be classified as inferential and six as literal questions
format is a multiple-choice variation of the cloze task, with (cf., McKenna & Stahl, 2009). In addition to differences
alternative choices for each deleted word. The format is regarding passage length and proportion of inference ques-
regarded as sensitive to reading comprehension for younger tions, an important difference between this measure and the
students (McKenna & Stahl, 2009; Shin, Deno, & Espin, two other reading comprehension measures used at post-test
2000). The items in the task that we used consisted of 33 was that it was completed without the text passage available
parentheses containing three alternative words appearing in when answering questions. Thus, the participants were
every third to eighth sentence. In each parenthesis, students instructed that they had 5 min to study the text and that the text
were to underline the correct alternative while reading. Of the had to be handed in before they were given a separate sheet
33 items of the maze task, four (i.e., 12%) could be catego- with questions to the text. The participants were given 10 min
rized as inferential questions in the sense that the answers to answer the 11 questions, with this time limit also deemed
could not be directly located in the selection but required that appropriate to avoid floor or ceiling effects on the basis of our
the readers made logical connections among facts (McKenna piloting of the instrument. The reliability estimate (Cronbach’s
& Stahl, 2009). The participants were given 10 min to alpha) for participants’ scores on the RGT was .70.
complete the task. Also on the maze task, the time limit was
chosen on the basis of piloting in two fifth-grade classrooms to
avoid either floor or ceiling effects. The reliability estimate
(Cronbach’s alpha) for participants’ scores on the maze task 2.6. Assessing implementation of the instructional
was .70. framework
The second measure of reading comprehension was
a multiple-choice measure with text available during testing. To answer the research question concerning the relationship
This was a much used standardized test published by the between implementation quality and the effects (or lack of
Norwegian Learning Center (Læringssenteret, 2003), as effects) of the intervention, both observational and question-
a comprehension subtest in a larger fifth-grade assessment naire data were collected. The first author made observations
battery of reading skills. It was called the Norwegian Learning of two ERCI lessons at the beginning (after 6e8 weeks) and
Center Test (NLCT). The students were instructed to read two at the end (after 16e17 weeks) of the intervention period
three expository text passages about spiders consisting of 91, on two different days in each classroom. Each of the ERCI
82, and 95 words, respectively. After each passage, three teachers were thus observed for four lessons in all. No
multiple-choice comprehension questions were to be observations were made of control group teachers. In addition,
answered, that is, a total of nine questions. For each question, questionnaires were answered by the teachers at the end of the
the students had to choose one of four alternative answers by intervention period. Two questionnaires were designed: One
drawing a cross next to it. They were informed that they were 21-item questionnaire was answered by all teachers in both
allowed to look back to the passage when answering the conditions (last week of May), and one 22-item questionnaire
comprehension questions. Of the nine questions of the NLCT, was only answered by the ERCI teachers (first week of June).
530 R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537

2.6.1. Observations cooperating on text materials but not modeling strategies or


The observations used check-list procedures (Judd, Smith, giving feedback to each other, students sitting in pairs
& Kidder, 1991), focusing on instructional practices reflect- modeling strategies and giving feedback to each other, and
ing the four ERCI principles. A check-list was filled out in small group cooperation with students modeling strategies and
each lesson by marking the list when an observed practice giving feedback to each other.
matched a description (i.e., check point) on the list. After each Finally, the fourth part of the check list targeted the
observed lesson, the teacher was asked to what extent that implementation of reading motivation. The following six
particular lesson was representative of his or her ERCI points were checked: The teacher organizing real-world
instruction and in what way, if any, the lesson differed from experiences related to topics of study (i.e., the central
other ERCI lessons. The four parts of the check list are concepts), the teacher leading classroom dialogue about
described next. central concepts, the teacher allowing students to choose
In the first part of the check list, targeting the imple- subtopics according to interests, the teacher allowing students
mentation of relevant background knowledge, teachers’ verbal to choose among topic-relevant texts according to interests, the
initiatives and verbal responses were registered. The check teacher guiding individual students’ choice of text, and the
point for initiatives differentiated between five types of teacher having all students read the same text at the same time.
initiatives: Asking factual questions (topic-related or strategy-
related), asking deeper-level questions (topic-related or 2.6.2. Questionnaires
strategy-related), describing or explaining topic issues, The 21-item questionnaire given to all teachers contained
modeling thinking, and requesting students to model their own a combination of open-ended and Likert-type questions con-
thinking. The check point for responses differentiated between cerning teacher characteristics related to education and rele-
seven alternatives, four concerning responses to student vant experience as well as instructional practices related to
questions and three concerning responses to student answers. each of the four ERCI principles (e.g., concerning their
The alternatives for responses to student questions reflected teaching of summarization and their use of different books in
a continuum from providing the right answer to encouraging the classroom).
students to reflect on the question. The alternatives for The 22-item questionnaire given only to the ERCI teachers
responses to student answers varied from evaluating the also contained a combination of open-ended and Likert-type
answer as right or wrong to encouraging students to elaborate questions. These questions asked the teachers to judge the
upon the answer. ease/difficulty of implementing each of the four principles in
The second part of the check list targeted the imple- their classrooms and their success (or lack of it) in promoting
mentation of reading comprehension strategies. For each of strategic reading comprehension and cooperative learning
the four strategies, the following seven check-points were among their students during the intervention period.
used: The teacher explaining the strategy to the students, the When trying to decide whether each of the four principles
teacher modeling the strategy for the students, the teacher had been appropriately implemented in the ERCI classrooms
reminding the students to use the strategy, the teacher scaf- or not, teachers’ responses to the questionnaires were used to
folding the strategy use of an individual student, a student supplement the information about instructional practices
modeling or explaining the strategy, the teacher giving stra- gathered through the classroom observations. By utilizing
tegic reading as an assignment, and self-initiated strategy use. different data sources in this way, we hoped to strengthen the
In addition to these check-points used for each strategy, there validity of our conclusions concerning implementation quality.
was a check point for registering the integration of multiple
strategies. 3. Results
The third part of the check list, targeting the implementa-
tion of reading-group organization, focused on cooperation Correlations among variables at pre-test and post-test,
between students during strategic reading comprehension. The respectively, are shown for all participants in Tables 1 and 2.
following four check point were used for registration: Students At pre-test, word recognition and working memory correlated
sitting in small groups without any cooperation or dialogue positively with reading comprehension, as did strategy use and
concerning text materials, students sitting in small groups intrinsic motivation. Moreover, word recognition and working

Table 1
Correlations among variables at pre-test.
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Word recognition e
2. Working memory .45*** e
3. Strategy use .31*** .38*** e
4. Extrinsic motivation .10 .14 .02 e
5. Intrinsic motivation .20** .12 .18* .29*** e
6. Reading comprehension (SRT) .69*** .47*** .33*** .13 .22** e
Note. SRT ¼ Sentence Reading Test. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537 531

Table 2 Table 4
Correlations among variables at post-test. Adjusted marginal means and standard errors for dependent variables.
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 Measure Intervention Control
1. Strategy use e M SE M SE
2. Extrinsic motivation .12 e Strategy use 10.64 .34 8.38 .34
3. Intrinsic motivation .18* .36*** e Extrinsic motivation 2.92 .08 3.03 .08
4. Reading comprehension .47*** -.30*** .25*** e
Intrinsic motivation 3.22 .08 3.13 .07
(maze) Reading comprehension (maze) 22.88 .55 21.71 .54
5. Reading comprehension .42*** -.24** .27*** .71*** e Reading comprehension (NLCT) 6.15 .16 6.14 .16
(NLCT) Reading comprehension (RGT) 6.83 .24 5.92 .23
6. Reading comprehension .48*** -.21** .24** .68*** .59*** e
(RGT) Note. NLCT ¼ Norwegian Learning Center Test, RGT ¼ ResearchereGenerated
Test.
Note. NLCT ¼ Norwegian Learning Center Test, RGT ¼ ResearcherGenerated
Test. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
( p ¼ .28). Moreover, the covariates did not interact with
memory were positively correlated, and each of these variables condition ( ps > .34), indicating that our data met the
also correlated positively with strategy use. At post-test, scores assumption of homogeneous regression slopes. Table 4 shows
on the three reading comprehension measures correlated the adjusted marginal means and standard errors for the
positively. Moreover, both strategy use and intrinsic motiva- dependent variables. The ANCOVA showed a statistically
tion correlated positively with all three comprehension significant effect of condition on the comprehension strategy
measures, as well as with each other. Finally, external reading measure after adjustment by covariates. That is, consistent
motivation correlated negatively with all three comprehension with our hypothesis, students participating in the ERCI inter-
measures. vention (M ¼ 10.64) displayed more use of reading compre-
Table 3 shows descriptive statistics (means, standard devi- hension strategies than did students in the control condition
ations, and coefficients of skewness) for all measured variables (M ¼ 8.38), with F(1, 176) ¼ 21.49, p ¼ .000, partial
at pre-test and post-test in both conditions (i.e., intervention h2 ¼ .11. Moreover, the covariates of word recognition, F(1,
and control group). As can be seen, all score distributions were 176) ¼ 25.98, p ¼ .000, partial h2 ¼ .13, and pre-test strategy
approximately normal and, thus, appropriate for use in para- use F(1, 176) ¼ 11.77, p ¼ .001, h2 ¼ .06, uniquely adjusted
metric statistical analyses. comprehension strategies, while adjustment by working
To address our first research question, concerning the effect memory did not quite reach a conventional level of statistical
of our intervention on the use of reading comprehension significance, F(1, 176) ¼ 3.68, p ¼ .057, partial h2 ¼ .02. This
strategies, we performed an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) indicates that students displaying better word recognition skill
on the comprehension strategy measure with pre-test word and strategy use at pre-test were more likely to use compre-
recognition, working memory, and strategy use as covariates, hension strategies at post-test.
and with condition (intervention vs. control) as the indepen- Next, to address our second research question, concerning
dent variable. Ninety students in the intervention group and 91 the effects of the intervention on reading motivation, we per-
students in the control group had complete data sets and were formed two separate ANCOVAs, one on extrinsic reading
available for this analysis. Levene’s test indicated that the motivation and one on intrinsic reading motivation. In both
assumption of homogeneity of variances was not violated analyses, Levene’s test indicated homogeneity of variances;
p ¼ .98 for extrinsic motivation, and p ¼ .59 for intrinsic
Table 3 motivation. Moreover, the covariates did not interact with
Descriptive statistics for variables at pre-test and post-test.
condition in any of the analyses ( ps > .26 in the analysis on
Measure Intervention Control extrinsic motivation, and ps > .42 in the analysis on intrinsic
M SD Skew M SD Skew motivation), indicating that our data met the assumption of
Pre-test homogeneous regression slopes. The first analysis, using word
Word recognition 21.00 7.30 .06 23.80 8.00 .19 recognition, working memory, and pre-test extrinsic reading
Working memory 8.44 4.72 .25 10.07 6.06 .14 motivation as covariates, showed no effect of condition, F(1,
Strategy use 7.70 3.59 .21 7.42 3.35 .57
175) ¼ .95, p ¼ .33, partial h2 ¼ .005, with the adjusted marginal
Extrinsic motivation 3.17 .84 .44 3.03 .84 .00
Intrinsic motivation 3.39 .70 .19 3.30 .83 .50 means almost the same for the two groups (see Table 4). Only the
Reading comprehension (SRT) 15.47 4.43 .19 16.46 5.28 .16 covariate of pre-test extrinsic motivation uniquely adjusted the
Post-test scores on post-test extrinsic motivation, F(1, 175) ¼ 79.19,
Strategy use 10.19 3.83 .10 8.65 3.76 .25 p ¼ .000, partial h2 ¼ .31. Eighty-nine students in the inter-
Extrinsic motivation 2.99 .87 .11 3.00 .93 .02
vention group and 91 students in the control group had complete
Intrinsic motivation 3.22 .84 .19 3.12 .79 .46
Reading comprehension (maze) 22.04 7.19 .43 23.21 7.33 .77 data sets and were available for this analysis. The second anal-
Reading comprehension (NLCT) 6.01 2.13 .11 6.25 1.98 .37 ysis, using word recognition, working memory, and pre-test
Reading comprehension (RGT) 6.24 3.09 .39 5.89 2.92 .41 intrinsic reading motivation as covariates, also showed no effect
Note. SRT ¼ Sentence Reading Test, NLCT ¼ Norwegian Learning Center of condition, F(1, 165) ¼ .69, p ¼ .41, partial h2 ¼ .004 (see
Test, RGT ¼ ResearcherGenerated Test. Table 4 for the adjusted marginal means). Only the covariate of
532 R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537

pre-test intrinsic motivation uniquely adjusted the scores on F(1, 174) ¼ 39.19, p ¼ .000, partial h2 ¼ .18, for the NLCT,
post-test intrinsic motivation, F(1, 165) ¼ 62.01, p ¼ .000, and F(1, 174) ¼ 16.80, p ¼ .000, partial h2 ¼ .09, for the
partial h2 ¼ .27. For this analysis, 84 students in the intervention RGT.1
group and 86 students in the control group were available. Our fourth research question concerned potential relation-
The third research question, concerning the effects of our ships between the effects (or lack of effects) of the interven-
intervention on reading comprehension, was addressed tion on student outcome variables and the quality of the
through a multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) on implementation of the four instructional principles. As
the three comprehension measures administered at post-test, summarized in Table 5, the results indicated that only the first
that is, the maze task, the Norwegian Learning Center Test two ERCI principles were appropriately implemented. Based
(NLCT), and the researcher-generated test (RGT). Covariates on the observations, the teachers of the intervention classes
were pre-test word recognition, working memory, and reading commonly initiated classroom dialogues to activate and
comprehension (i.e., the Sentence Reading Test [SRT]), and generate background knowledge, especially asking deeper-
the independent variable was condition (intervention and level questions to students, explaining topic issues, and
control). Eighty-nine students in the intervention group and 90 refraining from just evaluating student responses as right or
students in the control group had complete data sets and were wrong. Especially at the end of the intervention period,
available for this analysis. Levene’s test indicated that the deeper-level questions that required elaborated answers were
assumption of homogeneity of variances was not violated for asked rather than factual or yes/no questions, and instead of
any of the dependent measures ( ps > .10). Moreover, the just evaluating student responses as right or wrong, the
covariates did not interact with condition ( ps > .05), indi- teachers also acknowledged and tried to clarify the responses
cating that the assumption of homogeneous regression slopes and encouraged students to elaborate and reflect on their
was met. Box’s M test indicated that variance-covariance statements. In addition, the teaching of all four comprehension
matrices were not different across cells, F(6, 226909) ¼ .75, strategies, either individually or in combination, was observed
p ¼ .61. Finally, multivariate normality could be assumed in the majority of the lessons, with the teachers especially
because each dependent variable considered separately fol- giving strategic reading as an assignment, reminding the
lowed a normal distribution (see Table 3). students to use the strategy, and guiding the strategy use of
With the use of Wilks’ criterion, the combined dependent individual students. While the use of individual strategies was
variables were statistically significantly affected by condition, most emphasized at the beginning of the intervention period,
Wilks’ l ¼ .95, F(3, 172) ¼ 2.98, p ¼ .033, partial h2 ¼ .05, as the use of two or more strategies in combination was most
well as by the covariates of word recognition, Wilks’ l ¼ .83, emphasized at the end of the period. The teachers were seldom
F(3, 172) ¼ 12.05, p ¼ .000, partial h2 ¼ .17, working observed modeling strategy use for students, however. As
memory, Wilks’ l ¼ .92, F(3, 172) ¼ 4.77, p ¼ .003, partial intended, the four characters described earlier were used when
h2 ¼ .08, and pre-test reading comprehension, Wilks’ l ¼ .80, introducing and teaching the four comprehension strategies.
F(3, 172) ¼ 14.53, p ¼ .000, partial h2 ¼ .20. We performed For example, the teachers dressed up as those characters when
three follow-up ANCOVAs to investigate the effect of condi- introducing the strategies, and the classrooms contained
tion on each comprehension measure, as well as the unique posters with the characters, who were also used on bookmarks
adjustment by covariates of each dependent variable. Table 4 and cue cards that reminded students to use the strategies.
shows the adjusted marginal means and standard errors for The teachers’ answers on the questionnaires seemed to
each of the two groups on the three comprehension measures. support the conclusion that the principles of relevant back-
As expected, the ANCOVAs showed a statistically significant ground knowledge and reading comprehension strategies,
effect of condition on the RGT after adjustment by covariates, respectively, were satisfactorily implemented. Thus, the ERCI
with students in the intervention condition (M ¼ 6.83) out- teachers reportedly put much emphasis on teaching students to
performing students in the control condition (M ¼ 5.92), F(1, use their background knowledge and read strategically by
174) ¼ 7.34, p ¼ .007, partial h2 ¼ .04. However, there were means of the four strategies during the intervention period.
no statistically significant effect of condition on the maze task, Such practices were much less pronounced in the answers of
F(1, 174) ¼ 2.28, p ¼ .13, partial h2 ¼ .01, or the NLCT, F(1, the control teachers, who expressed that they had not focused
174) ¼ .001, p ¼ .98, partial h2 ¼ .00. Word recognition particularly on students’ background knowledge activation and
uniquely adjusted the scores on the maze task, F(1, use during reading, and also put much less emphasis on the
174) ¼ 33.57, p ¼ .000, partial h2 ¼ .16, and the NLCT, F(1,
174) ¼ 6.21, p ¼ .014, partial h2 ¼ .03, but not on the RGT, 1
To examine whether the effects of the intervention differed between
F(1, 174) ¼ 1.90, p ¼ .17, partial h2 ¼ .01. Working memory classrooms, we performed two separate ANCOVAs, with scores on the post-
uniquely adjusted the scores on the maze task, F(1, test strategy measure and the RGT, respectively, as dependent variables and
174) ¼ 8.65, p ¼ .004, partial h2 ¼ .05, and the RGT, F(1, ERCI classroom as the independent variable. Covariates in the first analysis
174) ¼ 12.46, p ¼ .001, partial h2 ¼ .07, but not on the NLCT, were pre-test word recognition, working memory, and strategy use; in the
F(1, 174) ¼ 2.27, p ¼ .13, partial h2 ¼ .01. Finally, pre-test second analysis, covariates were pre-test word recognition, working memory,
and SRT scores. Neither results for strategies, F(4, 82) ¼ .73, p ¼ .57, nor for
reading comprehension uniquely adjusted the scores on all the RGT, F(4, 81) ¼ .83, p ¼ .51, were statistically significant, indicating that
three post-test comprehension measures, with F(1, the intervention effects on students’ strategy use and reading comprehension (i.
174) ¼ 20.99, p ¼ .000, partial h2 ¼ .11, for the maze task, e., RGT) that we report did not differ between classrooms.
R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537 533

Table 5 Based on both observations and questionnaires, the same


Implementation of the four instructional principles. pattern of implementation seemed to be occurring in all ERCI-
Principle Implementation Problem classes during the intervention. Thus, the first two principles
Relevant background Appropriate seemed to be satisfactorily and the last two principles seemed
knowledge to be poorly implemented in all five classrooms. This pattern
Reading comprehension Appropriate Modeling of implementation is consistent with the results regarding the
strategies
Reading-group Poor Small-group text
student outcome variables. More specifically, the imple-
organization dialogues involving strategy mentation of the principles of relevant background knowledge
modeling and feedback and reading comprehension strategies, respectively, seems
Reading motivation Poor Real world experiences consistent with the findings that the intervention had a positive
Choice of subtopics effect on students strategic processing and comprehension
Choice of texts
performance as measured by the RGT. At the same time, the
lack of implementation of the principles of reading-group
organization and reading motivation, respectively, seems to be
teaching of strategies, especially with respect to the strategies consistent with the finding that no effect of the intervention
of questioning and predicting. was observed on reading motivation. Possibly, that these two
The two last ERCI principles, respectively reading-group principles were unsatisfactorily implemented may also have
organization and reading motivation, seemed to be rather limited the effect of the intervention on comprehension
poorly implemented in the intervention classrooms. performance because self-regulatory use of reading compre-
The observations indicated that there was relatively little, hension strategies was not sufficiently promoted. We will
group-based cooperation between students during strategic further elaborate upon possible relationships between imple-
reading comprehension, with students more often reading the mentation quality and student outcome data in the discussion.
text together in pairs or small groups but then continuing to
work on the text individually by, for example, generating 4. Discussion
questions or summarizing content. Apparently, this situation
did not change much over the intervention period and was due The present study contributes to the reading science liter-
to disturbances and difficulty keeping students concentrated on ature by demonstrating positive effects of complex, explicit
the task when trying to model strategies and give feedback to reading comprehension instruction directed by regular
each other. The answers to the questionnaires complemented teachers after considerable professional development on
this picture. Although the ERCI teachers’ answers emphasized middle-school students’ strategy use and comprehension.
reading-group organization more than those of the control These findings partly confirm Hypothesis 1a. Consistent with
teachers, they also expressed that they found small-group text Hypothesis 1b, the findings also indicate that the effects of
dialogues difficult to manage because of students’ unsatis- reading comprehension instruction may depend on how an
factory social skills and lack of independence. Particularly, the intervention is implemented in classrooms, also pinpointing
shift from teacher-led classroom dialogues to student-led small which of the underlying theoretical principles that may
group dialogues were found to be problematic, with especially represent particular challenges for the teachers.
academically less able and behaviorally more disruptive First, the improvement in students’ use of reading
students reportedly having problems functioning in reading comprehension strategies resulting from the intervention
groups without more tutoring or scaffolding support. Finally, seems to be in line with teachers’ adequate implementation of
the fourth principle, concerning the promotion of reading the principles of relevant background knowledge and reading
motivation, was also not realized in the ERCI classrooms. comprehension strategies in the classroom. More specifically,
According to the observations, reading was seldom inspired by both the emphasis on activating and generating relevant
real-world experiences related to the topics of study, and the background knowledge and the explicit teaching of the four
students were not allowed to choose among subtopics or texts merited strategies of predicting, questioning, clarifying, and
according to interests. Instead, one social studies textbook was summarizing over a five-month period could be expected to
used as the sole reading material for all students, who always affect students’ use of deep-level comprehension strategies
read the same text at the same time. The different trade books positively. It should be noted, however, that our strategy
made available through the local library were thus not used in assessment tool focused more on strategy competence than on
the ERCI lessons, and the comprehension strategies were awareness or self-initiation in the sense of Taboada and
always taught and practiced in relation to texts from the Guthrie (2004), meaning that the students primarily demon-
textbook. This situation was confirmed by the ERCI teachers, strated that they had improved their ability to use strategies
with information derived from the questionnaires also sug- well when requested to do so. Further development towards
gesting that students in intervention and control groups did not becoming a self-initiated or self-regulated user of a repertoire
experience much difference with respect to this principle. In of comprehension strategies requires extensive training as well
fact, the teachers of the control classes used the same textbook as motivation to use the strategies. The implementation of the
as the ERCI teachers as the only reading material in social third principle, reading-group organization, was supposed to
studies for all students during the intervention period. give the participants opportunities for extensive training by
534 R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537

practicing the comprehension strategies in social contexts, and Because different reading comprehension measures were
the fourth principle, reading motivation, was supposed to used at pre- and post-test, we cannot entirely exclude the
instigate a desire for text-based learning and comprehension. possibility that the difference between the intervention and the
The lack of adequate implementation of both these principles control condition that we observed on the RGT at post-test was
may therefore have hindered students’ development towards due to pre-existing differences in reading comprehension.
self-regulated use of comprehension strategies. However, it should be noted that the pre-test measure that was
Second, the fact that no change was observed with respect included as a covariate was a much used standardized and
to reading motivation can be explained by the poor imple- validated test of reading comprehension (e.g., Elbro, Nielsen,
mentation of principles three and four. According to Guthrie & Petersen, 1994), which has also been shown to correlate
et al. (2004), providing collaboration in reading instruction substantially with the RGT (Andreassen & Bråten, 2010).
should be regarded as an important motivational practice with Moreover, students in the control group tended to score higher
the potential to promote, in particular, intrinsic reading moti- on the SRT at pre-test that did students in the intervention
vation. Moreover, catching students’ interest through real- group, as was also the case on the pre-test measures of word
world experiences and then giving them ample opportunity to recognition and working memory. Taken together, this makes
work towards interesting and valued content goals by using it unlikely that there were pre-test differences in the favor of
a variety of textual materials at their own discretion was the intervention group with respect to reading comprehension.
supposed to support the development of intrinsic motivation Moreover, it could be conceived as a threat to internal validity
for reading. Given the general decrease in academic motiva- that we did not ensure that the students in the control condition
tion often observed over the elementary school years (Schunk, were given the same amount of reading time as the students in
Pintrich, & Meece, 2008), motivation-supporting components the intervention condition, which, admittedly, would have made
of educational interventions seem especially needed. Middle- it easier to firmly conclude that it was the quality and not the
school children, in particular, seem to prefer topics and texts quantity of reading that made the intervention effective. It
experienced as interesting and debatable for cooperative group should be noted, however, that in the intervention period, the
discussions (Alvermann et al., 1996). That such instructional control group covered the same social studies topics by means of
practices were mostly conspicuous by their absence in the the same reading material (i.e., textbook) during the same
intervention classrooms makes it understandable that no number of lessons. There is thus little reason to believe that the
improvement in intrinsic reading motivation was observed. students in the control group were actually reading less that the
Third, consistent with Hypothesis 2, the improvement that students in the intervention group, although the “ordinary
we observed in students’ comprehension performance was practices” that they experienced presumably involved more
limited to a test where students read an expository text that whole class instruction where they were asked control questions
consisted of a longer passage, contained a larger proportion of about text content after individual reading (cf., van Daal et al.,
inferential questions, and was answered without access to 2007; Rasmussen, 2003). Please also remember that teachers’
relevant text passages. This result seems reasonable given that answers to questionnaires indicated qualitative differences in
this text presumably represented a greater challenge and that the intervention teachers were more concerned about acti-
required more deeper-level processing than the two other vating and generating relevant background knowledge during
comprehension tasks that we used (cf., Andreassen & Bråten, instruction, and that they also targeted strategic processing more
2010). Accordingly, Afflerbach et al. (2008) emphasized that directly. Finally, it seems unlikely that merely a larger quantity
strategies are needed when readers struggle with the text and of reading instruction would result in a change (i.e., increase) in
their usual skills cannot do the job. By demonstrating that strategic processing such as observed in this investigation
standardized tests much used to assess reading comprehension (Pressley, 2006).
in Norwegian elementary schools were not sensitive to The difficulty that the teachers experienced implementing
performance changes resulting from the intervention, our reading-group dialogues where students model strategy use
study also highlights the importance of avoiding a “mono- and provide constructive feedback to each other is certainly
operation bias” (Cook & Campbell, 1979) when assessing the not peculiar to Norwegian middle-grade teachers. As reviewed
effects of interventions to improve reading comprehension. by Dignath, Buettner, and Langfeldt (2008), interventions to
That said, the reason that the improvement in comprehension foster self-regulated learning at elementary school level may
performance that we observed on the most challenging task even be less effective when students work together in pairs or
was not larger may be related to the poor implementation of small groups during the intervention than when group work is
principles three and four, presumably hindering the develop- not used, with this negative effect displayed on measures of
ment of self-regulated strategy use and intrinsic motivation. academic performance, strategy use, and motivation alike.
Thus, some students may have been unable or unmotivated to Although there is a solid research base indicating that group
initiate and regulate their use of strategies to comprehend text work can, indeed, improve motivation and make learning more
when not explicitly asked to do so. Of note is that reading efficient (e.g., Guthrie et al., 2004; O’Donnell, 2006; Slavin,
motivation may directly influence comprehension perfor- 1996), this also suggests that just having students sit
mance in addition to its indirect influence as mediated through together in pairs or small groups may actually do more harm
the efficient use of comprehension strategies (Anmarkrud & than good. In contrast, effective group work probably requires
Bråten, 2009). considerable commitment and skill, with both teachers’ and
R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537 535

students’ experience and instruction in small group coopera- allow choices of subtopics and reading materials because they
tive learning seemingly a key condition for success (cf., take the traditional attitude that the textbook is the only
Dignath et al., 2008). authoritative source on curricular content, and that working
In particular, group work must be organized to insure that with alternative topics and texts may imply that some of the
group members explain or model to-be-learned material for topics of the textbook are not sufficiently covered or that the
each other, a practice that was essentially lacking in our inter- progression suggested by the textbook is delayed. Finally, to
vention classes, because such cognitive processes seem crucial insure better implementation of educational interventions, it
to support learning during small-group discussions (van may be necessary that the school’s administration plays a more
Blankenstein, Dolmans, van der Vleuten, & Schmidt, in press). active role. For example, De Corte, Verschaffel, and van de
The implementation of the last principle was supposed to Ven (2001) had principals participate in regular meetings
contribute to the development of intrinsic reading motivation, between researchers and teachers during their intervention to
that is, a will to read for one’s own sake (Guthrie & Wigfield, improve reading comprehension strategies. Accordingly,
2000; Ryan & Deci, 2000). That real-world experiences were Hilden and Pressley (2007) concluded that administrative
hardly used to trigger students’ interest to find out more about support for changes in teaching practice is desirable.
the topic, and that students were not allowed choices between In conclusion, this study highlights the need for imple-
subtopics and reading materials, seem consistent with the mentation and process data in intervention studies to explain
finding that no change was observed with respect to intrinsic their outcomes. Although we combined the use of classroom
reading motivation. Real-world experiences seem important observations and questionnaires, further research should
because they can initiate and frame students’ inquiry into probably assess implementation quality even more broadly, for
conceptual issues and make subsequent reading both interest example by also including in-depth interviewing of teachers
driven and concept oriented (Guthrie et al., 2004). Choices, on and students and video-based observations of classroom
the other hand, seem to promote intrinsic motivation to the instruction as well as group work. Moreover, implementation
extent that they support students’ needs for autonomy, data should not only be collected at the beginning and the end
competence, and relatedness (Ryan & Deci, 2000). This means but, ideally, in all phases of the intervention. That said, our
that all choices are not necessarily motivating and that assessment of the implementation quality in this study clearly
teachers should offer students choices that are adapted to their indicated that teachers may need extensive preparation and
interests and goals (i.e., autonomy supporting), do not cogni- support to adequately implement new approaches to reading
tively overwhelm students but allow them to demonstrate their comprehension instruction, with promoting productive group
competence (i.e., competence supporting), and are not in dialogues and motivating choices representing particular
conflict with the values of significant others (i.e., relatedness challenges. Still, much further research is required on how
supporting) (Katz & Assor, 2007). teachers may be adequately prepared for and guided through
An important question concerns why the teachers partici- the implementation of research-based, explicit reading
pating in the intervention had difficulties implementing the comprehension instruction in the classroom.
principles of reading-group organization and reading motiva-
tion despite professional development in the form of
researcherdteacher collaboration prior to the intervention. References
There are several possible reasons for those difficulties. One is
that the researcherdteacher collaboration that took place prior Afflerbach, P., Pearson, P. D., & Paris, S. G. (2008). Clarifying differences
between reading skills and reading strategies. The Reading Teacher, 61,
to the intervention should have been even more extensive,
364e373.
including, for example, the use of films to illustrate explicit Alexander, P. A. (1997). Mapping the multidimensional nature of domain
reading comprehension instruction and frame discussions learning: the interplay of cognitive, motivational, and strategic forces. In
about how underlying principles could be implemented in the M. L. Maehr, & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and
teachers’ own classrooms (cf., Anderson, 1992; Palincsar & achievement, Vol. 10 (pp. 213e250). Greenwich, CT: JAI.
Alexander, P. A., Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (1998). A perspective on
Brown, 1984). Another possibility is that the research-
strategy research: progress and prospects. Educational Psychology Review,
erdteacher collaboration should have continued throughout 10, 129e154.
the intervention period. Thus, to manage more fundamental Allen, S. (2003). An analytic comparison of three models of reading strategy
changes in instructional practice, teachers may need guidance instruction. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language
and support with classroom implementation not only prior to Teaching, 41, 319e338.
Almasi, J. F. (1996). A new view of discussion. In L. B. Gambrell, & J. F.
but also during the intervention, with this allowing for
Almasi (Eds.), Lively discussions! Fostering engaged reading (pp. 2e24).
continuous adjustments and fine-tuning of practices to Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
accommodate underlying theoretical principles (cf., De Corte, Alvermann, D. E., Young, J. P., Weaver, D., Hinchman, K. A., Moore, D. W.,
2000). Moreover, as part of professional development, it may Phelps, S. F., et al. (1996). Middle and high school students’ perceptions of
be particularly important to discuss and, if necessary, try to how they experience text-based discussions: a multicase study. Reading
Research Quarterly, 31, 244e267.
modify teachers’ attitudes towards certain instructional prin-
Anderson, R. C. (1994). Role of the reader’s schema in comprehension,
ciples and practices. For example, teachers’ attitudes towards learning, and memory. In R. B. Ruddell, & H. Singer (Eds.), Theoretical
group work may not further the implementation of this prin- models and processes of reading (pp. 469e482). Newark, DE: Interna-
ciple (Dignath et al., 2008). Also, teachers may be reluctant to tional Reading Association.
536 R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537

Anderson, V. (1992). A teacher development project in transactional strategy Duffy, G. G. (2002). The case for direct explanation of strategies. In C. C.
instruction for teachers of severely reading-disabled adolescents. Teaching Block, & M. Pressley (Eds.), Comprehension instruction: Research-based
and Teacher Education, 8, 391e403. best practices (pp. 28e41). New York: Guilford.
Andreassen, R., & Bråten, I. (2010). Examining the prediction of reading Duke, N., & Pearson, P. D. (2002). Effective practices for developing reading
comprehension on different multiple-choice tests. Journal of Research in comprehension. In A. Farstrup, & J. Samuels (Eds.), What research has to
Reading, 33, 263e283. say about reading instruction (3rd ed.). (pp. 205e242) Newark, DE:
Anmarkrud, Ø (2009). Undervisning i lesestrategier og utvikling av lesemo- International Reading Association.
tivasjon på ungdomstrinnet. [The teaching of reading strategies and Elbro, C., Nielsen, I., & Petersen, D. K. (1994). Dyslexia in adults: evidence
development of reading motivation in lower secondary school]. Unpub- for deficits in non-word reading and in the phonological representation of
lished doctoral dissertation. Oslo: University of Oslo. lexical ı́tems. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 205e226.
Anmarkrud, Ø., & Bråten, I. (2009). Motivation for reading comprehension. El-Dinary, P. B., Pressley, M., & Schuder, T. (1992). Becoming a strategic
Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 252e256. teacher: an observational and interview study of three teachers learning
Baddeley, A. (1990). Human memory: Theory and practice. Hove/London: transactional strategies instruction. In C. Kinzer, & D. Leu (Eds.), Forty-
Erlbaum. first yearbook of the national reading conference (pp. 453e462). Chicago:
van Blankenstein, F. M., Dolmans, D. H. J. M., van der Vleuten, C. P. M., & National Reading Conference.
Schmidt, H. G. (in press). Which cognitive processes support learning Fox, E. (2009). The role of reader characteristics in processing and learning
during small-group discussion? The role of providing explanations and from informational text. Review of Educational Research, 79, 197e261.
listening to others. Instructional Science. Gersten, R., Fuchs, L. S., Williams, J. P., & Baker, S. (2001). Teaching reading
Block, C. C., & Duffy, G. G. (2008). Research on reading comprehension: where comprehension strategies to students with learning disabilities: a review of
we’ve been and where we’re going. In C. C. Block, & S. R. Paris (Eds.), research. Review of Educational Research, 71, 279e320.
Comprehension instruction: Research-based best practices (2nd ed.). Graesser, A. C. (2007). An introduction to strategic reading comprehension. In
(pp. 19e37) New York: Guilford. D. S. McNamara (Ed.), Reading comprehension strategies: Theories,
Bråten, I., Lie, A., & Andreassen, R. (1998). Explaining individual differences interventions, and technologies (pp. 3e26). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
in reading: on the orthographic component of word recognition. Scandi- Graesser, A. C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences
navian Journal of Educational Research, 42, 389e399. during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101,
Bråten, I., Lie, A., Andreassen, R., & Olaussen, B. S. (1999). Leisure time 371e395.
reading and orthographic processes in word recognition among Norwegian Guthrie, J. T., McRae, A., & Klauda, S. L. (2007). Contributions of concept-
third- and fourth-grade students. Reading and Writing, 11, 65e88. oriented reading instruction to knowledge about interventions for moti-
Bråten, I., & Samuelstuen, M. S. (2004). Does the influence of reading purpose vations in reading. Educational Psychologist, 42, 237e250.
on reports of strategic text processing depend on students’ topic knowl- Guthrie, J. T., van Meter, P., McCann, A. D., Wigfield, A., Bennett, L.,
edge? Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 324e336. Poundstone, C. C., et al. (1996). Growth of literacy engagement: changes
Brown, R., Pressley, M., van Meter, P., & Schuder, T. (1996). A quasi- in motivations and strategies during concept-oriented reading instruction.
experimental validation of transactional strategies instruction with low- Reading Research Quarterly, 31, 306e332.
achieving second-grade readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, Guthrie, J. T., & Wigfield, A. (2000). Engagement and motivation in reading.
18e37. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.),
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (2006). Assessment matters: issues in the measurement Handbook of reading research, Vol. 3 (pp. 403e422). Mahwah, NJ:
of reading comprehension. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, Erlbaum.
697e708. Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., Barbosa, P., Perencevich, K. C., Taboada, A.,
Cazden, C. B. (1988). Classroom discourse: The language of teaching and Davis, M. H., et al. (2004). Increasing reading comprehension and
learning. Portsmouth: Heinemann. engagement through concept-oriented reading instruction. Journal of
Cohen, E. G. (1994). Restructuring the classroom: conditions for productive Educational Psychology, 96, 403e423.
small groups. Review of Educational Research, 64, 1e35. Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., & Perencevich, K. C. (Eds.). (2004). Motivating
Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. A. (1979). Quasi-experimental design: Design reading comprehension: Concept-oriented reading instruction. Mahwah,
and analysis issues. Chicago: Rand McNally. NJ: Erlbaum.
Cromley, J. G., & Azevedo, R. (2007). Testing and refining the direct and Hidi, S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Motivating the academically unmo-
inferential mediation model of reading comprehension. Journal of tivated: a critical issue for the 21st century. Review of Educational
Educational Psychology, 99, 311e325. Research, 70, 151e179.
van Daal, V., Solheim, R. G., Gabrielsen, N. N., & Begnum, A. C. (2007). Hilden, K. R., & Pressley, M. (2007). Self-regulation through transactional
Norske elevers leseinnsats og leseferdigheter. [The reading effort and strategies instruction. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 23, 51e75.
reading skills of Norwegian students]. Stavanger, Norway: Center for Høien, T., & Tønnesen, G. (1997). Ordkjedetesten. [Word chain test]. Sta-
Reading Research. vanger, Norway: Stiftelsen Dysleksiforskning.
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working Jacobson, C. (1995). Word Recognition Index (WRI) as a quick screening
memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, marker of dyslexia. The Irish Journal of Psychology, 16, 260e266.
450e466. Judd, C. M., Smith, E. R., & Kidder, L. H. (1991). Research methods in social
De Corte, E. (2000). Marrying theory building and the improvement of school relations (6th ed.). Orlando, FL: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
practice: a permanent challenge for instructional psychology. Learning and Katz, I., & Assor, A. (2007). When choice motivates and when it does not.
Instruction, 10, 249e266. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 429e442.
De Corte, E., Verschaffel, L., & van de Ven, A. (2001). Improving text van Keer, H. (2005). Fostering reading comprehension in fifth grade by
comprehension strategies in upper primary school children: a design explicit instruction in reading strategies and peer tutoring. British Journal
experiment. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 531e559. of Educational Psychology, 74, 37e70.
Dignath, C., Buettner, G., & Langfeldt, H. P. (2008). How can primary school Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension:
students learn self-regulated learning strategies most effectively? A meta- a construction- integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163e182.
analysis on self-regulation training programmes. Educational Research Klette, K. (2003). Lærernes klasseromsarbeid. [Teachers’ classroom practice].
Review, 3, 101e129. In K. Klette (Ed.), Klasserommets praksisformer etter Reform, 97. [Forms
Dole, J. A. (2000). Explicit and implicit instruction in comprehension. In B. of classroom practice after Reform 97] Oslo: University of Oslo, Depart-
M. Taylor, M. F. Graves, & P. van den Broek (Eds.), Reading for meaning: ment of Educational Research.
Fostering comprehension in the middle grades (pp. 52e69). New York: Lederer, J. M. (2000). Reciprocal teaching of social studies in inclusive
Teachers College Press. elementary classrooms. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 91e106.
R. Andreassen, I. Bråten / Learning and Instruction 21 (2011) 520e537 537

Liang, L. A., & Dole, J. A. (2006). Help with teaching reading comprehension: (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed.). (pp. 265e286)
comprehension instructional frameworks. The Reading Teacher, 59, 742e753. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Læringssenteret. (2003). Kartlegging av leseferdighet: 5. klasse. [Assessment Rasmussen, J. B. (2003). Reading literacy performance in Norway: current
of reading skills: 5th grade] Oslo: Læringssenteret. practice and critical factors. European Journal of Education, 38, 427e443.
Magliano, J. P., Millis, K., Ozuru, Y., & McNamara, D. S. (2007). A multi- Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in
dimensional framework to evaluate reading assessment tools. In D. S. social context. New York: Oxford University Press.
McNamara (Ed.), Reading comprehension strategies: Theories, interven- Rosenshine, B., & Meister, C. (1994). Reciprocal teaching: a review of the
tions, and technologies (pp. 107e134). New York: Erlbaum. research. Review of Educational Research, 64, 479e530.
McKenna, M. C., & Stahl, K. A. D. (2009). Assessment for reading instruction Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: classic
(2nd ed.). New York: Guilford. definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25,
McKeown, M. G., Beck, I. L., & Blake, R. G. K. (2009). Rethinking reading 54e67.
comprehension instruction: a comparison of instruction for strategies and Schunk, D. H., Pintrich, P. R., & Meece, J. L. (2008). Motivation in education:
content approaches. Reading Research Quarterly, 44, 218e253. Theory, research, and applications (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.
McNamara, D. S. (Ed.). (2007). Reading comprehension strategies: Theories, Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and
interventions, and technologies. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston, MA:
McNamara, D. S., & Magliano, J. P. (2009). Self-explanation and meta- Houghton Mifflin.
cognition: the dynamics of reading. In D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. C. Shin, J., Deno, S. L., & Espin, C. (2000). Technical adequacy of the maze task
Graesser (Eds.), Handbook of metacognition in education (pp. 60e81). for curriculum-based measurement of reading growth. Journal of Special
New York: Routledge. Education, 34, 164e172.
Murphy, P. K., Wilkinson, I. A. G., Soter, A. O., Hennessey, M. N., & Sinatra, G. M., Brown, K. J., & Reynolds, R. E. (2002). Implications of
Alexander, J. F. (2009). Examining the effects of classroom discussion on cognitive resource allocation for comprehension strategies instruction. In
students’ comprehension of text: a meta-analysis. Journal of Educational C. C. Block, & M. Pressley (Eds.), Comprehension instruction: Research-
Psychology, 101, 740e764. based best practices (pp. 62e76). New York: Guilford.
National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: an evidence-based Slavin, R. E. (1996). Research on collaborative learning and achievement:
assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its impli- what we know, what we need to know. Contemporary Educational
cations for reading instruction. Washington, DC: National Institute of Psychology, 21, 43e69.
Child Health and Human Development. Slavin, R. E., Cheung, A., Groff, C., & Lake, C. (2008). Effective reading
Nielsen, J. C., Kreiner, S., Poulsen, A., & Søegård, A. (1995). Setningslese- programs for middle and high schools: a best-evidence synthesis. Reading
prøvene SL40 og SL60. [Sentence reading tests SL40 and SL60]. Stavanger, Research Quarterly, 43, 290e322.
Norway: Center for Reading Research. Swan, E. A. (2003). Concept-oriented reading instruction: Engaging class-
Nordgreen, P. (1994). Verktøy for lesestimulering. [Tools for reading stimu- rooms, lifelong learners. New York: Guilford.
lation]. Sandefjord, Norway: Lærerhjelp. Swanson, H. L., Howard, C. B., & Sáez, L. (2007). Reading comprehension
Norwegian Ministry of Education and Research. (2006). Læreplan for and working memory in children with learning disabilities in reading. In
grunnskolen og videregående skole. [Curriculum for elementary and K. Cain, & J. Oakhill (Eds.), Children’s comprehension problems in oral
secondary school]. Oslo: Ministry of Education and Research. and written language (pp. 157e189). New York: Guilford.
Oczkus, L. D. (2005). Reciprocal teaching at work: Strategies for improving Swanson, H. L., & Trahan, M. F. (1992). Learning disabled readers’
reading comprehension. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. comprehension of computer mediated text: the influence of working
O’Donnell, A. M. (2006). The role of peers and group learning. In P. A. memory, metacognition, and attribution. Learning Disabilities Research &
Alexander, & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology Practice, 7, 74e86.
(2nd ed.). (pp. 781e802) Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Taboada, A., & Guthrie, J. T. (2004). Growth of cognitive strategies for
Palincsar, A. S., & Brown, A. L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehen- reading comprehension. In J. T. Guthrie, A.Wigfield., & K. C. Perencevich
sion-fostering and comprehension-monitoring activities. Cognition and (Eds.), Motivating reading comprehension: Concept-oriented reading
Instruction, 1, 117e175. instruction (pp. 273e306). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Paris, S. G., Lipson, M. Y., & Wixson, K. K. (1983). Becoming a strategic Takala, M. (2006). The effects of reciprocal teaching on reading compre-
reader. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8, 293e316. hension in mainstream and special (SLI) education. Scandinavian Journal
Pearson, P. D. (2009). The roots of reading comprehension instruction. In S. of Educational Research, 50, 559e576.
E. Israel, & G. G. Duffy (Eds.), Handbook of research on reading Trabasso, T., & Bouchard, E. (2002). Teaching readers how to comprehend
comprehension (pp. 3e31). New York: Routledge. text strategically. In C. C. Block, & M. Pressley (Eds.), Comprehension
Pearson, P. D., & Dole, J. A. (1987). Explicit comprehension instruction: instruction: Research-based best practices (pp. 176e200). New York:
a review of research and a new conceptualization of instruction. Elemen- Guilford.
tary School Journal, 88, 151e165. Vellutino, F. R. (2003). Individual differences as sources of variability in
Pearson, P. D., & Hamm, D. N. (2005). The assessment of reading compre- reading comprehension in elementary school children. In A. P. Sweet, &
hension: a review of practicesdpast, present, and future. In S. G. Paris, & C. E. Snow (Eds.), Rethinking reading comprehension (pp. 51e81). New
S. A. Stahl (Eds.), Children’s reading comprehension and assessment (pp. York: The Guilford Press.
13e69). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Pressley, M. (2000). What should comprehension instruction be the instruction Press.
of? In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Walczyk, J. J., Wei, M., Griffith-Ross, D. A., Goubert, S. E., Cooper, A. L., &
Handbook of reading research, Vol. 3 (pp. 545e561) Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Zha, P. (2007). Development of the interplay between automatic processes
Pressley, M. (2006). Reading instruction that works: The case for balanced and cognitive resources in reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99,
teaching (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford. 867e887.
Pressley, M., El-Dinary, P. B., Gaskins, I., Schuder, T., Bergman, J. L., Almasi, J., Weinstein, C. E., & Mayer, R. E. (1986). The teaching of learning strategies.
et al. (1992). Beyond direct explanation: transactional instruction of reading In M. C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (pp. 315e327).
comprehension strategies. Elementary School Journal, 92, 511e555. New York: Macmillan.
Pressley, M., Graham, S., & Harris, K. (2006). The state of educational Wigfield, A., & Guthrie, J. T. (1997). Relations of children’s motivation for
intervention research as viewed through the lens of literacy intervention. reading to the amount and breadth of their reading. Journal of Educational
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 1e19. Psychology, 89, 420e432.
Pressley, M., & Harris, K. R. (2006). Cognitive strategies instruction: from Wood, D., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem
basic research to classroom instruction. In P. A. Alexander, & P. H. Winne solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89e100.

You might also like