Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

This is a chapter excerpt from Guilford Publications.

Handbook of Early Literacy Research, Volume 3.


Edited by Susan B. Neuman and David K. Dickinson.
Copyright © 2011.

Lessons from the Crib for the Classroom:


how Children really Learn Vocabulary
v

s
es
JUsTIn hArrIs

rOBErTA MIChnICk GOLInkOFF

Pr
kAThy hIrsh-PAsEk

rd
lfo
ui
G
January 2010: Nine years after the enact­ hear roughly 25% of the words that pass the
e

ment of No Child Left Behind, prekindergar­ ears of their more advantaged peers. And
Th

ten (PreK) through third-grade classrooms this lack of input has consequences for both
across America have become narrowly fo­ quick language processing (see Fernald &
cused on reading and math outcomes. A re­ Weisleder, Chapter 1, this volume) and tra­
11

cent report from the Alliance for Childhood jectories of language and literacy acquisition
(Miller & Almon, 2009) offers a portrait of (Dickinson & Freiberg, 2009; Dickinson,
20

kindergarten teachers in Los Angeles and Golinkoff, & Hirsh-Pasek, 2010; National
New York. Thirty percent claim to have no Institute of Child Health and Human De­
time for student-chosen activities or play. velopment [NICHD] Early Child Care Re­
©

These changes in school structure arose in search Network, 2005) through elementary
an attempt to narrow the achievement gap school. The methods used to increase the vo­
ht

and to raise the emergent literacy scores of cabularies of these young children, however,
disadvantaged children. Roughly 80% of the are antithetical to 40 years of research on
ig

teachers interviewed suggest that they spend early word learning.


yr

20 minutes each day in test preparation. This chapter merges the language and lit­
Furthermore, teachers often follow scripted eracy literatures by examining how lessons
op

learning plans designed to build language from the crib can teach us about strategies
skills. Students, for example, are taught a for enhancing vocabulary in the PreK–third­
C

stack of vocabulary words—along with their grade classroom. Six well-tested principles
definitions—before they hear those same of word learning emerge in the language do­
words used in a story. To further underscore main (Dickinson, Hirsh-Pasek, & Golinkoff,
the lack of developmental appropriateness, under review). After discussing each, and the
young children, using techniques developed evidence that supports them, we suggest that
to help high-school students memorize Scho­ vocabulary development can be enhanced
lastic Aptitude Test (SAT) words like syzygy not by scripted SAT-type memorization,
and synergy, practice learning new words but by classroom conversations and playful
for the upcoming test. engagement. We demonstrate how playful
The motives behind these techniques are learning flows from the language-learning
sound. Hart and Risley (1995) report that by principles to enhance vocabulary develop­
age 3, children from disadvantaged homes ment for all children.
49
50 BASIC DEVELOPMENTAL PROCESSES

Flashback: How Vocabulary sider the frequently heard word Mommy.


Learning Begins When can babies recognize that Tommy
sounds different than Mommy? Apparently,
Infants and toddlers learn vocabulary not even 6-month-old babies do more than store
from explicit instruction but in the course holistic representations of frequently heard
of their everyday interactions with parents words (Bortfeld et al., 2005) because they
and caregivers. By the time children arrive can tell the difference between Mommy and
at preschool, they have honed some amazing Tommy.
processes for vocabulary learning. Capital­ These early processes continue to be use­
izing on these processes can only enhance ful to children in vocabulary building and
vocabulary teaching. reading. In the sentence “Turn on the spig­

s
ot,” children cannot ask what spigot means

es
if they cannot segment it from the sentence.
Baby steps: Finding the sounds and Words
Sensitivity to common stress patterns helps
in Language

Pr
children to pronounce unfamiliar words
Infants face two daunting tasks that are pre­ found in text correctly. Statistical capabili­
requisite to learning vocabulary (Golinkoff ties come into play when children recognize

rd
& Hirsh-Pasek, 1999; Saffran, Werker, & which letter patterns are commonly found
Werner, 2006): segmentation and storing the together in print. For example, the ability to

lfo
sounds that comprise words. Word learning note that the letter clusters ch and ea often
cannot begin until babies segment or isolate appear together correlates with children’s
words from the sound stream. Uncover­
ing how babies do this is an active research ui
reading scores in second grade (Golinkoff &
Gibson, 1974).
G
goal (e.g., Blanchard, Heinz, & Golinkoff,
2010; Myers, Blumstein, Walsh, & Eliassen,
e
sound Patterns Turn into Words:
2009). To find the words, babies utilize a
Th

The Earliest Vocabulary


number of cues, among which are statisti­
cal cues (transitional probabilities) between When do babies invest frequently heard
syllables (Aslin, Saffran, & Newport, 1998; sound patterns with meaning? Tincoff and
11

Saffran, Aslin, & Newport, 1996) and Jusczyk (1999) showed that by 6 months
highly frequent and familiar words (e.g., of age, babies already know some frequent
20

their own names or “Mommy”) (Bortfeld, words and their meanings—words like
Morgan, Golinkoff, & Rathbun, 2005). By Mommy and Daddy. Thus, even in the first
6 months of age, babies recognize a novel year, babies find words in the language
©

word that comes after their own names, but stream and store word forms both with
not a novel word after someone else’s name. and without meanings. Table 4.1 indicates
ht

Babies also use word stress to help them find children’s progress in early word learning
words. The syllables of each language fol­ (Fenson et al., 1994). Comprehension leads
ig

low a characteristic stress pattern, and by 9 production dramatically in the first year of
yr

months, infants recognize their language’s life, suggesting that it is easier to store fre­
dominant stress pattern. A French baby, for quently heard word forms than to produce
op

example, considers a syllable pattern with them. The variability observed is enormous,
weak–strong stress (iambic) to be a two- with productive vocabularies at 24 months
C

syllable word (“guiTAR”) (Polka, Sundara, ranging from 56 to 520 words (Fenson et al.,
& Blue, 2002), while a baby hearing English 1994)! Interestingly, these enormous differ­
looks for strong–weak or trochaic stress (as ences in vocabulary have their roots partly in
in “TAble”) (Jusczyk, Houston, & News­ the nonverbal gestural interactions that take
ome, 1999). By the time children are learn­ place between babies as young as 14 months
ing to read, they have segmented literally of age and their mothers. More maternal
thousands of words from the speech stream, gestures predict more gestures by children,
even if they do not know what all the words which in turn predicts children’s school-en­
mean. try vocabulary at 54 months of age (Rowe &
Infants must also store the individual Goldin-Meadow, 2009; Rowe, Özçaliskan,
sound segments that comprise words that & Goldin-Meadow, 2008). Pointing things
emerge from the segmentation process. Con­ out in the environment and honoring chil­
how Children really Learn Vocabulary 51

TABLE 4.1. Median Number of Words (and Ranges) in the Comprehension


and Production Vocabularies of Children Ages 10, 12, 18, 24, and 30
Months, According to Parental Report from the MacArthur Communicative
Development Inventory
Comprehension Production
Age (months) Median Range Median Range
10 42 11–154 2 0–10
12 74 31–205 6 2–30
18 — — 75 14–220
24 — — 308 56–520

s
30 — — 555 360–630

es
Note. Data from Fenson et al. (1994).

Pr
dren’s communicative bids feed into vocabu­ players involved (Golinkoff & Hirsh-Pasek,

rd
lary learning. 2008; Imai et al., 2008). While verbs and
Yet learning the meaning of words is a spatial–relational terms are more difficult

lfo
lengthy process. An initial “fast mapping” than concrete nouns for children to acquire
(Carey & Bartlett, 1978; Golinkoff, Hirsh- (Golinkoff & Hirsh-Pasek, 2008; Hirsh-
Pasek, Bailey, & Wenger, 1992) must be
augmented with more exposure to words in ui
Pasek & Golinkoff, 2006), they are nec­
essary if children are to comprehend and
G
varied contexts. Discerning a word’s range of produce complex sentences. Children learn
application and the nuances of its meaning verbs and spatial terms best when these are
e

allows children to use the word generative­ presented in sentences that are typical of
Th

ly in new situations (Golinkoff, Mervis, & their language (Imai et al., 2008) and in the
Hirsh-Pasek, 1994; Maguire, Hirsh-Pasek, context of real-world events (e.g., Tomasello
& Golinkoff, 2006; Maguire, Hirsh-Pasek, & Kruger, 1992). For children to combine
11

Golinkoff, & Brandone, 2008). This is an vocabulary into sentences and narratives,
important point: The flashcard, SAT style relational words (verbs, adverbs, adjectives,
20

of vocabulary memorization often presents and spatial prepositions) need to be taught


isolated words without context and fails to in the preschool classroom and populate
invite children to learn how a word is used children’s vocabularies.
©

in a range of linguistic and environmental


contexts. Embedding words in sentences is
ht

crucial to illustrate word meaning and at the Word Learning in the Wild:

same time influences the learning of gram­ How Vocabulary Learning Continues

ig

mar. Vocabulary learning and grammatical


yr

learning are reciprocal processes. What can we learn from the crib that trans­
fers to teaching vocabulary in the classroom?
op

For one thing, vocabulary learning takes


Many Word Types Are needed
place in the course of natural interaction as
for Vocabulary, Grammar, and narrative
C

children indicate their interests either vo­


While nouns make a good entry point for cally or through gestures. When parents and
lexical and grammatical learning because caregivers build on children’s interest by of­
they label many concrete and nonrela­ fering information, vocabulary comes alive.
tional concepts (Maguire, Hirsh-Pasek, & Kemler-Nelson, Egan, and Holt (2004) re­
Golinkoff, 2006; Waxman & Lidz, 2006), port that young children do not just want
other parts of speech are needed to talk to hear a name of a new object (e.g., “It’s
about relations and events (Bloom, Tinker, a toaster”) when they say, “What’s that?”
& Margulis, 1993; Nelson, 1988). Verbs, What children want is information about
for example, are the architectural center­ what the object is used for and where it is
pieces of sentences because they encode the found (e.g., “It’s a toaster—a kind of ma­
event the sentence describes and dictate the chine that cooks our bread”). Children insist
52 BASIC DEVELOPMENTAL PROCESSES

until they are offered more information. As have found that engaging with an adult in
Kemler-Nelson and colleagues write, “when dialogic reading causes children to use more
young children ask, “What is it?” . . . they words, to speak in longer sentences, to score
are more concerned with knowing what higher on vocabulary tests, and to demon­
kind of thing it is—that is, what its intended strate overall improvement in expressive lan­
function is—than what it is called” (p. 388). guage skills (Doyle & Bramwell, 2006; Har­
These findings indicate that from the child’s grave & Sénéchal, 2000; Huebner, 2000a,
perspective, vocabulary learning is not about 2000b; Huebner & Meltzoff, 2005). Con­
learning words in isolation but about acquir­ sistent with these findings, a comprehensive
ing the concepts for which the words stand. meta-analysis revealed that shared dialogic
Take, for example, a case borrowed from reading is especially beneficial to the expres­

s
Chase-Lansdale and Takanishi (2009, p. 4) sive language of young preschoolers (Mol,

es
in which they present what Hunter referred Bus, de Jong, & Smeets, 2008).
to as “three mothers and an eggplant.” They The third mother’s treatment of her child’s

Pr
write: eggplant query and episodes of dialogic read­
ing have features in common that nurture
The first mother wheels her shopping cart vocabulary. Notably they motivate children

rd
down the produce aisle, where her kindergart­ to want to learn new words by capitalizing
ner spots an eggplant and asks what it is. The on children’s focus of attention. These epi­

lfo
mother shushes her child, ignoring the ques­ sodes in the wild serve as a model for how to
tion. A second mother, faced with the same foster vocabulary learning in the classroom.

ui
question, responds curtly, “Oh, that’s an egg­
Six principles of word learning (see Table
plant, but we don’t eat it.” The third mother
4.2) emerge from the study of word learning
G
coos, “Oh, that’s an eggplant. It’s one of the
few purple vegetables.” She picks it up, hands in the crib, as well as from the vocabulary
instruction seen in preschool and kinder­
e
it to her son, and encourages him to put it on
the scale. “Oh, look, it’s about two pounds!” garten. First, children learn the words that
Th

she says. “And it’s $1.99 a pound, so that they hear most; frequency matters. Second,
would cost just about $4. That’s a bit pricey, they learn words for things and events that
but you like veal parmesan, and eggplant par­ interest them. Third, they learn best in inter­
11

mesan is delicious too. You’ll love it. Let’s buy active and responsive rather than in passive
one, take it home, cut it open. We’ll make a contexts. Fourth, they learn words in mean­
20

dish together.” ingful contexts that exemplify the meanings


of the words. Fifth, they are able to learn
The first mother ignores the child, as well words from definitions when those defini­
©

as the question. The second mother at least tions are presented in a “child-friendly”
shares the child’s eye gaze, then offers the way that takes into account children’s prior
ht

name of the new food. The third mother not knowledge. And finally, vocabulary learn-
only engages the child in a conversation but
ig

also comments on the eggplant, explains


that it is a kind of vegetable, and builds on
yr

TABLE 4.2. Six Principles of Word Learning


the child’s query. When parents talk about
1. Frequency matters: Children learn the words
op

their children’s focus of attention, they offer that they hear the most.
vocabulary and rich information (e.g., Cal­ 2. Make it interesting: Children learn words for
lanan, Siegel, & Luce, 2007; Gelman, Coley,
C

things and events that interest them.


Rosengren, Hartman, & Pappas, 1998). 3. Make it responsive: Interactive and responsive
Analogously, similar patterns charac­ contexts rather than passive contexts favor
terize storybook reading. Reading builds vocabulary learning.
4. Focus on meaning: Children learn words best in
vocabulary most when it is dialogic (e.g., meaningful contexts.
Zevenbergen, Whitehurst, & Zevenbergen, 5. Be clear: Children need clear information about
2003). Dialogic reading occurs when adults word meaning.
prompt children with questions, evaluate 6. Beyond the word: Vocabulary learning and
and expand upon children’s verbalizations, grammatical development are reciprocal
processes.
and reward children’s efforts to tell the story
and label objects in the book. Numerous in­ Note. Data from Dickinson, Hirsh-Pasek, and
tervention studies with diverse populations Golinkoff (under review).
how Children really Learn Vocabulary 53

ing and grammatical learning are recipro­ guage and reading levels (Walker, Green­
cal processes. Offering definitions or using wood, Hart, & Carta, 1994; Weizman &
words in sentences during interaction always Snow, 2001; Fernald & Weisleder, Chapter
includes a surrounding linguistic context. 1, this volume).
The rest of this chapter reviews the em­ The relationship between adult input and
pirical support for these principles. There is child output appears not only in home en­
little disembodied SAT-type “direct instruc­ vironments but also in studies of child care
tion” that takes place between parents and and early schooling (Hoff, 2006a; Hoff &
children; that is, parents typically do not Naigles, 2002; Hoff-Ginsberg, 1991; Mc-
offer children words to memorize without Cartney, 1984; NICHD Early Child Care
context. Instead, vocabulary is offered in a Research Network, 2000, 2002, 2005). In a

s
natural way as part of the conversation, or beautifully designed study by Huttenlocher,

es
specifically, prompted by children’s queries Vasilyeva, Cymerman, and Levine (2002),
(e.g., “What’s that?”). The principles of vo­ the relation between a teacher’s input and

Pr
cabulary learning offered below invite their children’s language growth was evaluated
transfer to the preschool classroom. by examining the average growth of that
class over the school year, controlling for

rd
parental language, child’s starting language,
Six Principles of Vocabulary Learning and socioeconomic status (SES). Results sug­

lfo
gest that the complexity and variety of the
As Neuman and Dywer (2009) concluded teacher’s language relate to the children’s
after conducting a review of the limited
literature on vocabulary instruction in pre­ ui
language levels, above and beyond the lan­
guage accounted for by parent language or
G
school, “pedagogical principles for teach­ SES. Given that prior research strongly sug­
ing vocabulary to young children are sorely gest that young children are very sensitive
e

needed. There appears little consensus on to statistical patterns in the language input,
Th

developmentally effective strategies for this finding is not surprising (Saffran et al.,
teaching vocabulary” (p. 391). Perhaps by 1996). When children hear varied and com­
examining the literature on early vocabulary plex language, they have more opportuni­
11

learning in toddlers and preschool environ­ ties to discover the grammatical patterns. In
ments, the principles suggested below can fact, research finds that children learn not
20

fill that gap. only language that is directed to them but


also profit from overheard speech (Akhtar,
2005; Weizman & Snow, 2001).
Children Learn the Words
©

Increased levels of exposure to vocabulary


That They hear Most
are particularly likely to have beneficial ef­
ht

As Neuman and Dwyer (2009) suggest, fects when the input includes a relatively high
“Talk may be cheap but it is priceless for density of novel words relative to total words
ig

young developing minds” (p. 384). The fact (i.e., type:token ratio) because the density of
yr

that children learn words that are used in novel words children hear is a better predic­
their ambient environment has long been tor of vocabulary growth than is a simple
op

known. The classic study by Hart and Ris­ count of word types (Hoff, 2003; Hoff &
ley (1995) found that a key variable distin­ Naigles, 2002; Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk,
C

guishing more and less educated parents is Seltzer, & Lyons, 1991; Pan, Rowe, Singer,
the sheer amount of vocabulary addressed & Snow, 2005). But even more critical than a
to children. This is best exemplified in the good ratio of novel relative to repeated words
differences in the amount of speech that the may be the inclusion of sophisticated words
third mother used relative to the others in the that children are less likely to know (Dickin­
eggplant encounter (Chase-Lansdale & Ta­ son, Flushman, & Freiberg, 2009; Malvern,
kanishi, 2009). These findings on language Richards, Chipere, & Durán, 2004).
frequency have been echoed in a number of Frequency of exposure to vocabulary
correlational studies (Hoff, 2006a; Hoff & also has been found to be an important de­
Naigles, 2002; Hoff-Ginsberg, 1991; Tamis- terminer of word learning in experimental
LeMonda & Bornstein, 2002) and seem to studies in classrooms using book reading to
have long-range consequences for later lan­ build vocabulary knowledge. While book­
54 BASIC DEVELOPMENTAL PROCESSES

reading researchers have found learning that children entered kindergarten. Nicolopou­
is associated with a single reading, most in­ lou, McDowell, and Brockmeyer (2006) also
tervention studies employ between two and found that children who engage in sociodra­
four rereadings. Some evidence suggests that matic play build the language skills required
younger children (i.e., kindergartners) ben­ for literacy. As in other areas of pedagogy,
efit more from additional exposure, but the piquing a child’s interest in language through
number of words children are taught may be playful activities increases attention, mo­
an even more potent predictor of total learn­ tivation, and real learning (Hirsh-Pasek &
ing gains (Biemiller & Boothe, 2006). Thus, Golinkoff, 2003; Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff,
book reading provides repeated exposure to Berk, & Singer, 2009; Singer, Golinkoff, &
words that children are not likely to know, Hirsh-Pasek, 2006). These are ripe contexts

s
a second reason why book experiences have for children to pick up new vocabulary from

es
been linked to stronger vocabulary (Dick­ their peers.
inson & Tabors, 2001; Weizman & Snow, The effect of free play among peers on

Pr
2001). language appears to have a universal qual­
ity. An analysis of early education settings
across 10 countries found that small-group
Children Learn Words for Things and Events

rd
free play at age 4 was positively associated
That Interest Them
with multiple measures of oral language abil­

lfo
The classic work here comes from vocabu­ ity at age 7 (Montie, Xiang, & Schweinhart,
lary learning in young children acquir­ 2006). The unique demands of communi­
ing their first words. In what Lois Bloom
(2000) dubbed the “principle of relevance,” ui
cating meaning during sociodramatic play
is likely one of the reasons for vocabulary
G
she wrote, “Language learning is enhanced growth associated with such episodes. Chil­
when the words a child hears bear upon and dren work at duplicating the talk associated
e

are pertinent to the objects of engagement, with particular roles (e.g., talking like a doc­
Th

interest and feelings” (p. 19). A significant tor). They also use language to negotiate the
body of research in the joint attention lit­ play itself, covering topics such as how the
erature attests to the fact that children of play will progress, what roles each child will
11

parents who talk about what their children take, and what is allowable for those roles
are looking at have more advanced vocabu­ (i.e., what is acceptable behavior for a doc­
20

laries (Akhtar, Dunham, & Dunham, 1991; tor) (Vedeler, 1997). Pellegrini and Galda
Masur, 1982; Tomasello & Farrar, 1986). (1990) and Pellegrini, Galda, Dresden, and
A corollary finding is that children of par­ Cox (1991) also reported that preschoolers
©

ents who try to redirect children’s attention participate in much commentary about lan­
and label objects not of interest learn fewer guage when creating make-believe scenes,
ht

words (e.g., Dunham, Dunham, & Curwin, even using complex mental state verbs such
1993; Golinkoff, 1981; Hollich, Hirsh- as say, talk, tell, write, and explain.
ig

Pasek, Tucker, & Golinkoff, 2000). In the


yr

eggplant story, one would expect the third


Interactive and responsive rather Than
child to learn and remember the word egg­
Passive Contexts Favor Vocabulary Learning
op

plant because the mother capitalized on the


child’s interest. Adults who take turns, share periods of joint
C

In addition to the role of parents and focus, and express positive affect when in­
teachers, playful peer interactions feed into teracting with young children provide chil­
vocabulary development. Dickinson (2001a) dren with the scaffolding needed to facilitate
noted that the amount of time 3-year-olds language and cognitive growth (Bradley et
spend talking with peers while pretending al., 1989; Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998;
is positively associated with the size of their Clarke-Stewart, 1973; Howes, 2000; Katz,
vocabularies 2 years later, when they begin 2001; Tomasello & Farrar, 1986). The third
kindergarten. Bergen and Mauer (2000) mother in the eggplant vignette clearly built
found that 4-year-olds’ play, in the form of on the child’s interest and encouraged more
making shopping lists and “reading” sto­ conversation rather than shutting it down.
rybooks to stuffed animals, predicted both Stimulating and responsive parenting in early
language and reading readiness after the childhood are among the strongest predic­
how Children really Learn Vocabulary 55

tors of children’s later language, cognitive, found that higher-quality conversations and
and social skills (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, richer vocabulary exposure during free play
1998; Sameroff, 1983; Shonkoff & Phillips, and group book reading were related to chil­
2000). Children’s language skills are strong­ dren’s language, comprehension, and print
ly related to proximal measures of quality in skills at the end of kindergarten (Dickinson,
parent–child interaction such as sensitivity, 2001b; Tabors, Snow, & Dickinson, 2001)
cooperation, acceptance, and responsive­ and fourth grade (Dickinson, 2001b; Dick­
ness (Hirsh-Pasek & Burchinal, 2006; Lan­ inson & Porche, under review; Tabors et al.,
dry, Smith, Swank, Assel, & Vellet, 2001; 2001).
Tamis-LeMonda & Bornstein, 2002; Wak­ Finally, three studies examined this rela­
schlag & Hans, 1999). Parental warmth, tionship over time. Two held that parental

s
demonstrated as open displays of affection, sensitivity across time relates to changes

es
physical or verbal reinforcement, and sensi­ in child outcomes (see Bornstein & Tamis-
tivity to children’s requests and feelings, are LeMonda, 1989; Landry, Smith, Swank, &

Pr
also significantly associated with academic Miller-Loncar, 2000; Landry et al., 2001;
achievement and cognitive growth (Born­ NICHD Early Child Care Research & Dun­
stein & Tamis-LeMonda, 1989; Burchinal, can, 2003; Tamis-LeMonda & Bornstein,

rd
Campbell, Bryant, Wasik, & Ramey, 1997; 2002). Landry and colleagues (2001), for ex­
Clark, 2003; Cunningham & Stanovich, ample, found that children with highly sen­

lfo
1997; Howes, Phillips, & Whitebook, 1992; sitive parents in the first 3 years of life, fol­
Howes & Smith, 1995; Landry et al., 2001; lowed by lower sensitivity, did not perform as
Landry, Smith, & Swank, 2006; Landry,
Swank, Smith, Assel, & Gunnewig, 2006; ui
well as children who had consistently highly
sensitive parents across early childhood.
G
Morrison & Cooney, 2002). Hirsh-Pasek and Burchinal (2006) noted
While the role of sensitive input has been similar relationships with children in child­
e

more extensively explored in the parenting care settings. To the best of our knowledge,
Th

literature, responsive and stimulating behav­ this dimension of language learning has not
ior by caregivers also relates independently been directly explored in intervention stud­
to child outcomes (Burchinal, Roberts, Na­ ies within the preschool or early elementa­
11

bors, & Bryant, 1996; Burchinal et al., 2000; ry school setting. However, the frequency
Hirsh-Pasek & Burchinal, 2006; Howes of warmth and sensitivity in teacher–child
20

et al., 1992; Love et al., 2003; NICHD conversations in preschool classrooms was
Early Child Care Research Network, 2000; found to be correlated with the same teach­
NICHD Early Child Care Research Net­ ers’ tendency to engage in cognitively and
©

work & Duncan, 2003; Peisner-Feinberg & linguistically enriching conversations with
Burchinal, 1997; Zill, Resnick, & McKey, children (Densmore, Dickinson, & Smith,
ht

1999). Even smaller studies (Burchinal et 1995).


al., 1996, 2000; Dunn, 1993; Kontos, 1991;
ig

McCartney, 1984; Schliecker, White, &


Children Learn Words Best
yr

Jacobs, 1991) find a direct relationship be­


in Meaningful Contexts
tween environmental sensitivity and cogni­
op

tive and language outcomes. This link has After their review of how vocabulary is
been observed in child-care homes and taught in preschool, Neuman and Dwyer
C

relative care, as well as center care (Clarke- (2009) concluded: “Strategies that introduce
Stewart, Vandell, Burchinal, O’Brien, & young children to new words and entice them
McCartney, 2002; Kontos, Howes, Shinn, to engage in meaningful contexts through se­
& Galinsky, 1997). mantically related activities are much need­
Sensitive interactions are especially benefi­ ed” (p. 384). This insight is completely in line
cial when accompanied by rich lexical input. with research on memory: People learn best
In a longitudinal study researchers examined when information is presented in integrated
teacher–child conversations when children contexts rather than as a set of isolated facts
were 4, controlling at age 3 for children’s lan­ (Bartlett, 1932/1967; Bransford & Johnson,
guage ability (i.e., the mean length of their 1972; Bruner, 1972; Neisser, 1967; Tulv­
utterances), parental income, education, and ing, 1968). The same is true for children.
home support for literacy (e.g., reading), and A set of words connected in a grocery list
56 BASIC DEVELOPMENTAL PROCESSES

is better remembered than the same list of that makes vocabulary learning possible,
words without context. Meaningful con­ it is crucial to understand the guided play
nections between words are also fostered in contexts that support parents and teachers
studies that use thematic play as a prop for in the production of new words for children
language development. Christie and Roskos (Christie & Roskos, 2006; Fisher, Hirsh-
(2006), for example, find that children who Pasek, Golinkoff, Singer, & Berk, in press).
learn connected vocabulary for categories of Educational theory and research suggest
objects such as hammers, hard hats, screw­ that guided play approaches promote superi­
drivers, and tool belts (the category of build­ or learning, retention, and academic achieve­
ing) better remember and use these words ment compared to direct instruction (Burts,
than do children who do not learn in this Hart, Charlesworth, & Kirk, 1990; Burts

s
more integrative way. Additional support for et al., 1992; Hirsh-Pasek, 1991; Lillard &

es
children learning vocabulary in meaningful Else-Quest, 2006; Love, Ryer, & Faddis,
contexts comes from the work of Neuman 1992; Marcon, 1993, 2002; Roskos, Ta­

Pr
and Roskos (1992), who found that enrich­ bors, & Lenhart, 2004, 2009; Schweinhart
ing play centers with literacy-related objects & Weikart, 1988; Schweinhart, Weikart,
increased the frequency, duration, and com­ & Larner, 1986). In guided play contexts,

rd
plexity of peer verbal exchanges around lit­ educators structure an environment around
eracy objects and literacy themes. a general curricular goal by encouraging

lfo
New research by Han, Moore, Vukelich, children’s natural curiosity, exploration, and
and Buell (in press) finds that children given play with learning-oriented objects/materi­
an opportunity to use vocabulary in a playful
context learn it better than those who learn ui
als (Fein & Rivkin, 1986; Hirsh-Pasek et al.,
2009; Marcon, 2002; Schweinhart, 2004).
G
only under explicit instruction. By way of Conversations that take place between adults
example, low-income children in the explicit and children in the context of a playful ac­
e

instruction group heard a reading of Wart­ tivity, and that build on children’s interests,
Th

hogs in the Kitchen. Following the reading, offer children new lexical concepts that are
they heard the word bake while being shown more likely to be retained than unbidden ver­
a picture of the word in the storybook. They bal explanations (e.g., Golinkoff, 1986).
11

were then offered a “child-friendly” defini­ In a study in which children and parents
tion of the word bake and asked to repeat it were asked to build block structures together
20

and point to an instance of the concept. This (Ferrara, Shallcross, Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff,
group spent a full 30 minutes on the book & Newcombe, in preparation), the nature of
and on receiving the explicit vocabulary in­ the task influenced the quantity and rich­
©

struction. The playgroup spent 20 minutes ness of the spatial language parents offered.
on the book and the associated definitions, For example, when the task was structured,
ht

and so forth, but had 10 minutes to engage with the goal of reproducing a figure from a
in guided play with props. Subsequent vo­ picture, parental spatial language was richer
ig

cabulary tests revealed that the group that (e.g., “Put the big one on the little one”) than
yr

played remembered the target vocabulary when the task was more open-ended and
better and included more children who dyads built without a model. Play is the ideal
op

reached vocabulary benchmark levels on the context for word learning because the child
standardized Peabody Picture Vocabulary is actively engaged in a meaningful and plea­
C

Test (PPVT). surable activity, eager to participate with an


As Neuman and Dwyer (2009) pointed interested adult, and the language used often
out, experimental research comparing vo­ has instrumental purposes the child wants to
cabulary learning in meaningful versus less achieve. Of course, children can also learn
meaningful contexts is scant. Yet correla­ vocabulary from didactic instruction (e.g.,
tional studies in language, play, and memory Biemiller, 2006). In the Han and colleagues
research converge to suggest that teaching (in press) study, the didactic group and the
vocabulary in integrated and meaningful playgroup did not differ significantly in the
contexts enriches and deepens children’s particular words upon which they were
background knowledge and, hence, their trained. However, the performance of chil­
mental lexicons (Hirsh-Pasek et al., 2009). dren in the playgroup, who had experienced
Since parents and teachers provide the input guided pretend-play vocabulary-learning ep­
how Children really Learn Vocabulary 57

isodes, exceeded that of the didactic group words simply from hearing them in a story
on the PPVT months later. (De Jong & Bus, 2002; Elley, 1989; Elley &
Mangubhai, 1983), telling children the defi­
nitions of words consistently increases word
Children need Clear Information
learning substantially (Biemiller, 2006; Bi­
about Word Meaning
emiller & Boote, 2006; Brabham & Lynch-
Words can be understood in different ways Brown, 2002; Elley, 1989; Penno, Wilkin­
and to different degrees. For many words, son, & Moore, 2002). Children with weaker
a fast mapping (Carey & Bartlett, 1978) language skills seem to be especially likely
comes first. This is when the child might be to benefit from such explicit information
offered the meaning of a word ostensively or (Penno et al., 2002), perhaps because they

s
infer that the novel, unnamed object or ac­ have more difficulty making inferences about

es
tion is the one to which the new label should word meaning. However, there is evidence
be attached (Golinkoff et al., 1992, 1994; that older children benefit more than young­

Pr
Golinkoff, Jacquet, Hirsh-Pasek, & Nanda­ er children from explicit language-based
kumar, 1996). Fast mapping, however, yields information (Dickinson, 1984), possibly re­
a relatively cursory understanding of word flecting the greater metalinguistic abilities of

rd
meaning; repeated exposures to a new word older children. If book reading devolves into
in varied contexts, or the provision of defi­ an extended vocabulary lesson, the highly

lfo
nitions to which children can relate (Booth, explicit teaching that results in the greatest
2009), lead to a deeper, more nuanced un­ gains in short-term interventions with older
derstanding of word meaning. The field
knows a great deal about factors that influ­ ui
children could paradoxically have a negative
long-term impact on children’s enjoyment of
G
ence fast mapping, such as perceptual factors books and teacher’s use of books to deepen
or what a child finds attractive (Hollich et comprehension.
e

al., 2000; Pruden, Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff, Research by Booth (2009) represents an


Th

& Hennon, 2006), grammatical contexts in attempt to uncover those factors in explicit
which a word is embedded (Gleitman, 1990; definitions that foster retention and exten­
Hirsh-Pasek & Golinkoff, 1996), and so­ sion of newly learned word meanings. Booth
11

cial cues speakers offer about what they are reports that providing definitions to 3-year­
discussing (Hollich et al., 2000; Tomasello, olds about what one can do with an object or
20

1999). Observation of parent–child con­ action promotes better vocabulary learning


versations revealed that children benefited than providing static, noncausal definitions.
when parents provided quick explanations These findings dovetail with the prior prin­
©

about the meanings of words, and suggested ciple that word learning takes place best in
that young children may not require elabo­ a meaningful context. Seeing objects and ac­
ht

rated, decontextualized word definitions to tions embedded in a causal sequence appears


gain some understanding of the meaning of to be a powerful impetus to word learning.
ig

a word (Weizman & Snow, 2001). Weizman Even acting out the meanings of words with
yr

and Snow (2001) also found that adults are props in pretend play (Han et al., in press)
often sensitive to those words a child might contributes to children’s understanding of
op

not understand and can therefore support word meaning.


understanding by providing additional hints
C

as to word meaning. Such was the case with


Vocabulary Learning and Grammatical
the third mother’s explanation of the egg­
Development Are reciprocal Processes
plant; the word was couched in familiar rou­
tines, such as eating veal parmesan. The amount and diversity of verbal stimu­
The field knows less about how to fos­ lation fosters earlier and richer language
ter conceptual understanding and decon­ outcomes in terms of both vocabulary and
textualized word meanings. Most efforts grammar (Beebe, Jaffee, & Lachman, 1992;
have used book reading as the instructional Hart & Risley, 1995, 1999; Huttenlocher
context, and one suggested way is to offer et al., 1991; Snow, 1986; Tamis-LeMonda,
explicit definitions. Work with kindergar­ Bornstein, & Baumwell, 2001). Important­
ten and early primary grade children has ly, in these and many more recent studies,
revealed that while children learn some vocabulary and grammar are not divorced.
58 BASIC DEVELOPMENTAL PROCESSES

They feed one another. Dixon and March- about word meanings (Elley, 1989; Penno et
man (2007), for example, based on a large al., 2002; Robbins & Ehri, 1994).
sample of children ages 16–30 months (N = To summarize, word learning requires that
1,461), argue that words and grammar are children learn the sounds of the word, the
“developing in synchrony across the first word’s part of speech, and the word’s mean­
few years of life” (p. 209). This relationship ing. However, memorization of these facts
between grammar and vocabulary learning is not enough. To claim that children really
is also celebrated in research with bilingual know a word, we must show that they have
children. Conboy and Thal (2006) found not only acquired a minimal grasp of the
that toddlers’ English vocabulary predicted word but can also transfer the word to new
their English grammar and the reverse, and contexts, and retain the word and its mean­

s
their Spanish vocabulary predicted their ing over time. Too few studies hold word

es
Spanish grammar. learning to these high standards. However,
Children learn vocabulary through gram­ the literature does permit us to extract six

Pr
mar and grammar through vocabulary in principles about vocabulary learning that
two ways: By noting the linguistic context in can guide our research in the future.
which words appear, children gain informa­ Unfortunately, children who are at risk

rd
tion about a word’s part of speech (Imai et al., for reading problems are likely to have
2008) and, once a word is known, children limitations in the language skills on which

lfo
detect nuances in word meaning by observ­ reading draws. For example, children from
ing the diverse linguistic contexts in which lower-SES backgrounds are at risk due to a
words are used (Gillette, Gleitman, Gleit­
man, & Lederer, 1999; Naigles, 1990). Fur­ ui
substantially decreased vocabulary size (see
Hoff, 2006a, 2006b, 2009). Therefore, to
G
thermore, oral language measured as both the extent that we understand the processes
vocabulary and grammar (NICHD Early that contribute to vocabulary learning, the
e

Child Care Research Network, 2005) is cru­ more effective will be our interventions for
Th

cial for early literacy. Building vocabulary is children who lag behind. Ironically, while
not a matter of learning words in isolation the research shows that word learning
but one of hearing words in sentences. Re­ takes place best in meaningful and playful
11

search shows that exposure to complex lan­ contexts where child engagement is high,
guage throughout a school year can improve the educational system appears to be mov­
20

the syntactic comprehension of 4-year-old ing in the opposite direction, increasing the
children (Huttenlocher et al., 2002), a find­ amount of definition memorization required
ing supported by an experimental study that of children.
©

employed books to foster syntactic develop­


ment (Vasilyeva, Huttenlocher, & Waterfall,
ht

2006). Back to Basics: Natural Interaction


An important extension of this language- and Playful Learning as the Platform
ig

learning principle is that children’s current for Vocabulary Learning


yr

language abilities condition their ability to


learn new words. This premise is central Taken collectively, the six principles of vo­
op

to the emergent coalitionist perspective cabulary development derived from the crib
(Golinkoff & Hirsh-Pasek, 2006; Hirsh- and the classroom in effect dictate the kind
C

Pasek & Golinkoff, 1996; Hollich et al., of pedagogical approach that will yield opti­
2000), which posits that children use mul­ mal vocabulary development. Although chil­
tiple available cues when learning words, dren can learn definitions, relatively passive
and that employed cues shift as children memorization will not yield the depth and
become more competent language learn­ long-term retention needed to allow children
ers. The impact of current language status to recognize the appropriateness of a word
on word learning has been seen in studies for a range of situations. The six principles
in which children are taught new words by of vocabulary learning encourage a combi­
reading stories. Children with stronger lan­ nation of pedagogical approaches that offer
guage skills are more apt to gain more from clear and easily digestible definitions and
the stories, unless there are special efforts to that allow children to explore the meaning
provide redundant and explicit information of words via playful interaction.
how Children really Learn Vocabulary 59

Thus, research suggests that vocabulary ac­ adults: They instantiate the six principles
quisition occurs most effectively in preschool of vocabulary learning. Take the case of a
classrooms that mimic the way vocabulary pair of children pretending to play doctor
learning takes place in the home—through and baby. When children are at play they
events that spark children’s motivation to not only hear words for topics that inter­
learn new words and heighten their engage­ est them (e.g., stethoscope) (Principle 2) but
ment. Often, though not always, these inter­ they also frame sentences to convey mean­
actions occur in a playful context—between ings and comprehension of the sentences of
children and adults or between peers (Hirsh- others (e.g., when the stethoscope is brought
Pasek et al., 2009). In fact, many of these to the baby’s chest) (Principle 6). They are
principles point in the direction of playful involved as active, constructive participants

s
learning—both free play and guided play— (Principle 3), making the meaning of words

es
as they describe how presenting words in clear by them acting out and using their
meaningful contexts, in which children are bodies to reflect their understanding (Prin­

Pr
engaged, enhances vocabulary development. ciple 4) or to infer meaning of words they
Representing a broad array of activities, might not know by watching how their co­
including object play, pretend and socio­ players bring those meanings to life (Prin­

rd
dramatic play, and rough-and-tumble play, ciple 5). Crucially, and perhaps most impor­
free play has been notoriously difficult to tant of all, they are deeply engaged in the

lfo
define (see Hirsh-Pasek et al., 2009). Con­ co-constructed narrative, learning words for
temporary play researchers generally agree things and events they are keenly interested
that free-play activities are fun, voluntary,
flexible, have no extrinsic goals, involve ui
in representing (Principle 2). And when chil­
dren repeatedly engage in such make-believe
G
active engagement of the child, and often play, they hear some of the same words
contain an element of make-believe (John­ over again, heightening their opportunity
e

son, Christie, & Yawkey, 1999; Pellegrini, to learn them (Principle 1). Play heightens
Th

2009; Sutton-Smith, 2001). Guided play, engagement and enjoyment, increasing the
on the other hand, is seen when teachers (1) likelihood that new learning will occur. This
provide materials in the classroom to spur situation is very different from an adult of­
11

children’s engagement and discovery, and fering words in a way that does not explic­
(2) comment or query children about their itly link to children’s experiences. Various
20

play by providing the words to describe it. learning theories (e.g., information process­
Thus, adults who interact with children use ing, constructivism, Vygotskian scaffolding)
the vocabulary demanded by the children’s suggest that new learning occurs best when
©

situation. There is no disembodied memori­ it builds on and expands what children (or
zation of vocabulary words under a guided adults) already know.
ht

play approach. Play that is adult-supported


leads to more conversation (Levy, 1992)
ig

and, when combined with book reading, Conclusions


yr

helps to direct children’s attention to specific


vocabulary words. Wasik and Bond (2001) Early language development—including
op

embedded concrete vocabulary-related ob­ both vocabulary and syntax—is crucial for
jects into story reading and subsequent play. children’s school success and acquisition
C

This combination allowed the adults to sub­ of literacy. There is no doubt that the new
tly shape the children’s play to support the focus on language and vocabulary is impor­
mastery of specific, important vocabulary tant and has serious implications for later
words. Wasik and Bond attribute the posi­ communication skills and literacy outcomes
tive impact of this vocabulary intervention throughout a child’s school years. As we
to the meanings children understood as a move to more academically rich curricula,
function of the play context they created. however, we must be mindful that how one
Note what these playful contexts do, learns is as important as what one learns.
whether in the context of storybook read­ A considerable bank of scientific data exists
ing, conversation between parents or teach­ to guide us in knowing how children learn
ers and children, guided play with adults, or words and master their native tongue. In­
free play between children or children and deed, the literature here is sizable enough
60 BASIC DEVELOPMENTAL PROCESSES

to formulate principles for how to optimize Aslin, R. N., Saffran, J. R., & Newport, E. L. (1998).
vocabulary and language learning. It is time Computation of conditional probability statistics
by 8-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 9,
that we use what we know in evidence-based 321–324.
practice. Assel, M. A., Landry, S. H., Swank, P. R., & Gun­
Although additional research is sorely newig, S. (2007). An evaluation of curriculum, set­
needed, research points us in the direction of ting, and mentoring on the performance of children
natural interactions as the source of vocabu­ enrolled in pre-kindergarten. Reading and Writing,
lary learning. Whether through free play be­ 20, 463–494.
Bartlett, F. C. (1967). Remembering: A study in ex­
tween peers arguing about who plays what
perimental and social psychology. Cambridge, UK:
role in sociodramatic play or an adult intro­ Cambridge University Press. (Original work pub­
ducing literacy terms (e.g., sentence, word), lished 1932)

s
as children engage in play with literacy tools, Beebe, B., Jaffee, J., & Lachman, F. M. (1992). A dy­

es
the likelihood that vocabulary will “stick” is adic systems view of communication. In N. Skolnick
heightened when children’s engagement and & S. Warshaw (Eds.), Relational perspectives in
psychoanalysis (pp. 61–81). Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic

Pr
motivation for learning new words is high.
Press.
Embedding new words in activities that chil­ Bergen, D., & Mauer, D. (2000). Symbolic play, pho­
dren want to do recreates the conditions by nological awareness, and literacy skills at three age

rd
which vocabulary learning takes place in the levels. In K. Roskos & J. F. Christie (Eds.), Play and
crib. literacy in early childhood: Research from multiple

lfo
Given the data, we strongly suggest that perspectives (pp. 45–62). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
didactic SAT learning formats will not pro­ Biemiller, A. (2006). Vocabulary development and in­

ui
struction: A prerequisite for school learning. In D.
duce good speakers or good readers. Just K. Dickinson & S. B. Neuman (Eds.), Handbook
as we quickly forgot the meaning of syzygy
G
of early literacy research (Vol. 2, pp. 41–51). New
after the test, children who memorize mean­ York: Guilford Press.
ingless words and definitions will not re­ Biemiller, A., & Boote, C. (2006). An effective method
e

tain these words or be able to use them in for building meaning vocabulary in primary grades.
Th

new contexts. When words are presented Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 44–62.
Blanchard, D., Heinz, J., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2010).
frequently in contexts meaningful to chil­ Modeling the contribution of phonotactic cues to
dren, and with clear information about their
11

the problem of word segmentation. Journal of Child


meaning, children really learn—even com­ Language, 37, 487–511.
plex words like eggplant. As we translate the Bloom, L. (2000). The intentionality model of word
20

lessons of vocabulary learning in the crib to learning: How to learn a word, any word. In R. M.
the classroom, we create more playful and Golinkoff, K. Hirsh-Pasek, N. Akhtar, L. Bloom,
G. Hollich, L. Smith, et al. (Eds.), Becoming a word
conversational contexts for learning. While
©

learner: A debate on lexical acquisition (pp. 19–50).


children may outgrow their cribs, the prin­ Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
ciples that govern vocabulary learning in Bloom, L., Tinker, E., & Margulis, C. (1993). The
ht

young children, based on playful interaction words children learn: Evidence against a noun bias
and capitalizing on children’s interests and in early vocabularies. Cognitive Development, 8(4),
ig

proclivities, remain useful. 431–450.


Booth, A. E. (2009). Causal supports for word learn­
yr

ing. Child Development, 80, 1243–1250.


Acknowledgments Bornstein, M. H., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (1989).
op

Maternal responsiveness: Characteristics and con­


This research was funded by joint grants to Roberta sequences. New Directions for Child and Adoles­
C

Michnick Golinkoff and Kathy Hirsh-Pasek: cent Development, 43, 31–47.


from the National Science Foundation, Grant No. Bortfeld, H., Morgan, J. L., Golinkoff, R. M., & Rath­
SBR9615391, and from the National Institutes of bun, K. (2005). Mommy and me: Familiar names
Health, Grant No. R01HD050199. help launch babies into speech-stream segmenta­
tion. Psychological Science, 16, 298–304.
Brabham, E. G., & Lynch-Brown, C. (2002). Effects
References of teachers’ reading-aloud styles on vocabulary ac­
quisition and comprehension of students in the early
Akhtar, N. (2005). The robustness of learning through elementary grades. Journal of Educational Psychol­
overhearing. Developmental Science, 8, 199–209. ogy, 94(3), 465–473.
Akhtar, N., Dunham, F., & Dunham, P. J. (1991). Di­ Bradley, R. H., Caldwell, B. M., Rock, S. L., Ramey,
rective interactions and early vocabulary develop­ C. T., Barnard, K. E., Gray, C., et al. (1989). Home
ment: The role of joint attentional focus. Journal of environment and cognitive development in the first
Child Language, 18, 41–49. 3 years: A collaborative study involving six sites and
how Children really Learn Vocabulary 61

three ethnic groups in North America. Develop­ Clarke-Stewart, K. A., Vandell, D. L., Burchinal, M.,
mental Psychology, 25, 217–235. O’Brien, M., & McCartney, K. (2002). Do regula­
Bransford, J. D., & Johnson, M. K. (1972). Contextual ble features of child-care homes affect children’s de­
prerequisites for understanding. Journal of Verbal velopment? Early Childhood Research Quarterly,
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 717–726. 17, 52–86.
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecolo­ Conboy, B. T., & Thal, D. J. (2006). Ties between the
gy of developmental processes. Handbook of Child lexicon and grammar: Cross-sectional and longitu­
Psychology, 1, 993–1028. dinal studies of bilingual toddlers. Child Develop­
Bruner, J. (1972). Nature and uses of immaturity. ment, 77, 712–735.
American Psychologist, 27, 687–708. Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). Early
Burchinal, M. R., Campbell, F. A., Bryant, D. M., reading acquisition and its relation to reading ex­
Wasik, B. H., & Ramey, C. T. (1997). Early inter­ perience and ability 10 years later. Developmental
vention and mediating processes in cognitive perfor­ Psychology, 33, 934–945.

s
mance of children of low-income African American De Jong, M. T., & Bus, A. G. (2002). Quality of book-

es
families. Child Development, 68, 935–954. reading matters for emergent readers: An experiment
Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., Nabors, L. A., & Bry­ with the same book in a regular or electronic format.
ant, D. M. (1996). Quality of center child care and Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 145–155.

Pr
infant cognitive and language development. Child Densmore, A., Dickinson, D. K., & Smith, M. W.
Development, 606–620. (1995, April). The socioemotional content of teach­
Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., Riggins, R., Jr., Zei­ er–child interaction in preschool settings serving

rd
sel, S. A., Neebe, E., & Bryant, D. (2000). Relating low-income children. Paper presented at the annual
quality of center-based child care to early cognitive conference of the American Educational Research

lfo
and language development longitudinally. Child Association, San Francisco.
Development, 71, 338–357. Dickinson, D., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K.

ui
Burts, D. C., Hart, C. H., Charlesworth, R., Fleege, (2010). Speaking out for language: Why language
P. O., Mosley, J., & Thomasson, R. (1992). Ob­ is central for reading development. Educational Re­
G
served activities and stress behaviors of children in searcher, 39, 305–310.
developmentally appropriate and inappropriate kin­ Dickinson, D., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M.
dergarten classrooms. Early Childhood Research (under review). Increasing vocabulary in preschools:
e

Quarterly, 7, 297–318. Using cognitive science to guide pedagogy: Proposal


Th

Burts, D. C., Hart, C. H., Charlesworth, R., & Kirk, to Institute for Education Science.
L. (1990). A comparison of frequencies of stress be­ Dickinson, D. K. (1984). First impressions: Children’s
haviors observed in kindergarten children in class­ knowledge of words after a single exposure. Journal
11

rooms with developmentally appropriate versus de­ of Applied Psycholinguistics, 5, 359–373.


velopmentally inappropriate instructional practices. Dickinson, D. K. (2001a). Large-group and free-play
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 5, 407–423. times: Conversational settings supporting language
20

Callanan, M., Siegel, D., & Luce, M. (2007). Conven­ and literacy development. In D. K. Dickinson &
tionality in family conversations about everyday P. O. Tabors (Eds.), Beginning literacy with lan­
objects. In C. Kalish & M. Sabbagh (Eds.), Con­ guage: Young children learning at home and school
©

ventionality in cognitive development: How chil­ (pp. 223–255). Baltimore: Brookes.


dren acquire shared representations in language, Dickinson, D. K. (2001b). Putting the pieces together:
thought, and action. New Directions in Child and The impact of preschool on children’s language and
ht

Adolescent Development, 115, 83–97. literacy development in kindergarten. In D. K. Dick­


Carey, S., & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new inson & P. O. Tabors (Eds.), Beginning literacy with
ig

word. Papers and Reports on Child Language De­ language: Young children learning at home and
velopment, 15, 17–29. school (pp. 257–287). Baltimore: Brookes.
yr

Chase-Lansdale, P. L., & Takanishi, E. (2009, Octo­ Dickinson, D. K., Flushman, T. R., & Freiberg, J. B.
ber). How do families matter?: Understanding how (2009). Language, reading and classroom supports:
op

families strengthen their children’s educational Where we are and where we need to be going. In B.
achievement. New York: Foundation for Child De­ Richards, M. H. Daller, D. D. Malvern, P. Meara,
C

velopment. J. Milton, & J. Trefers-Daller (Eds.), Vocabulary


Christie, J., & Roskos, K. (2006). Standards, science, studies in first and second language acquisition:
and the role of play in early literacy education. In The interface between theory and application
D. Singer, R. Galinkoff, & K. Hirsh-Pasek (Eds.), (pp. 23–38). Hampshire, UK: Palgrave-MacMillan.
Play = learning: How play motivates and enhances Dickinson, D. K., & Freiberg, J. G. (2009). Preschool
children’s cognitive and social-emotional growth language development and later academic suc­
(pp. 57–73). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. cess. Paper presented at the Workshop on the Role
Clark, E. V. (2003). First language acquisition. Cam­ of Language in School Learning: Implications for
bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Closing the Achievement Gap, National Academy of
Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1973). Interactions between Sciences, Menlo Park, CA.
mothers and their young children: Characteristics Dickinson, D. K., & Porche, M. (under review). The
and consequences. Monographs of the Society for relationship between teacher–child conversations
Research in Child Development, 38(Serial No. with low-income four-year-olds and grade four lan­
153). guage and literacy development.
62 BASIC DEVELOPMENTAL PROCESSES

Dickinson, D. K., & Tabors, P. O. (2001). Beginning ical principles to learn new nouns. Developmental
literacy with language: Young children learning at Psychology, 28, 99–108.
home and school. Baltimore: Brookes. Golinkoff, R. M., & Gibson, E. J. (1974, March).
Dixon, J. A., & Marchman, V. A. (2007). Grammar Children’s judgments of spelling patterns relate to
and the lexicon: Developmental ordering in language reading achievement in second grade. Presented at
acquisition. Child Development, 78, 190–212. the annual meeting of the American Educational
Doyle, B. G., & Bramwell, W. (2006). Promoting emer­ Research Association, Chicago.
gent literacy and social-emotional learning through Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1999). How ba­
dialogic reading. Reading Teacher, 59, 554–564. bies talk: The magic and mystery of language de­
Dunham, P. J., Dunham, F., & Curwin, A. (1993). velopment in the first three years of life. New York:
Joint attentional states and lexical acquisition at 18 Plume.
months. Developmental Psychology, 29, 827–831. Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2006). Baby
Dunn, L. (1993). Proximal and distal features of day wordsmith: From associationist to social sophisti­

s
care quality and children’s development. Early cate. Current Directions in Psychological Science,

es
Childhood Research Quarterly, 8, 167–192. 15, 30–33.
Elley, W. B. (1989). Vocabulary acquisition from lis­ Golinkoff, R. M., Jacquet, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., &
tening to stories. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, Nandakumar, R. (1996). Lexical principles may un­

Pr
174–187. derlie the learning of verbs. Child Development, 67,
Elley, W. B., & Mangubhai, F. (1983). The impact of 3101–3119.
reading on second language learning. Reading Re­ Golinkoff, R. M., Mervis, C., & Hirsh-Pasek, K.

rd
search Quarterly, 19, 53–67. (1994). Early object labels: The case for a devel­
Fein, G., & Rivkin, M. (1986). The young child at opmental lexical principles framework. Journal of

lfo
play: Reviews of research (Vol. 4). Washington, Child Language, 21, 125–155.
DC: National Association for the Education of Han, M., Moore, N., Vukelich, C., & Buell, M. (in

ui
young Children. press). Does play make a difference?: Effects of play
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, S., Bates, E., Thai, D., intervention on at-risk preschoolers’ vocabulary
G
& Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early commu­ learning. American Journal of Play.
nicative development. Monographs of the Society Hargrave, A. C., & Sénéchal, M. (2000). A book read­
for Research in Child Development, 59(Serial No. ing intervention with preschool children who have
e

242). limited vocabularies: The benefits of regular read­


Th

Ferrara, K., Shallcross, W. L., Hirsh-Pasek, K., ing and dialogic reading. Early Childhood Research
Golinkoff, R. M., & Newcombe, N. (under review). Quarterly, 15, 75–90.
Block talk: Parental use of spatial language during Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differenc­
11

block play. es in the everyday experience of young American


Fisher, K., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Sing­ children. Baltimore: Brookes.
er, D., & Berk, L. E. (in press). Playing around in Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1999). The social world of
20

school: Implications for learning and educational children learning to talk. Baltimore: Brookes.
policy. In A. Pellegrini (Ed.), The Oxford handbook Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1991). Pressure or challenge in pre­
of play. New York: Oxford University Press. school?: How academic environments affect chil­
©

Gelman, S. A., Coley, J. D., Rosengren, K., Hartman, dren. In L. Rescorla, M. C. Hyson, & K. Hirsh-
E., & Pappas, A. (1998). Beyond labeling: The Pasek (Eds.), New directions in child development:
role of maternal input in the acquisition of richly- Academic instruction in early childhood: Challenge
ht

structured categories. Monographs of the Society or pressure? (No. 53, pp. 39–46). San Francisco:
for Research in Child Development, 63(Serial No. Jossey-Bass.
ig

253). Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Burchinal, M. (2006). Mother and


Gillette, J., Gleitman, H., Gleitman, L., & Lederer, A. caregiver sensitivity over time: Predicting language
yr

(1999). Human simulations of vocabulary learning. and academic outcomes with variable- and person-
Cognition, 73, 135–176. centered approaches. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly,
op

Gleitman, L. (1990). The structural sources of verb 52, 449–485.


meanings. Language Acquisition, 1, 3–55. Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. (1996). The origins
C

Golinkoff, R., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2008). How tod­ of grammar: Evidence from early language compre­
dlers learn verbs. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12, hension. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
397–403. Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. (2003). Einstein
Golinkoff, R. M. (1981). The influence of Piagetian never used flashcards: How our children really
theory on the study of the development of com­ learn and why they need to play more and memo­
munication. In I. E. Sigel, D. M. Brodzinsky, & R. rize less. Emmaus, PA: Rodale Press.
M. Golinkoff (Eds.), New directions in Piagetian Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. (2006). Action meets
theory and practice (pp. 127–142). Hillsdale, NJ: word: How children learn verbs. New York: Oxford
Erlbaum. University Press.
Golinkoff, R. M. (1986). “I beg your pardon?”: The Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Berk, L. E., & Sing­
preverbal negotiation of failed messages. Journal of er, D. G. (2009). A mandate for playful learning in
Child Language, 13, 455–476. preschool: Presenting the evidence. New York: Ox­
Golinkoff, R. M., Bailey, L., Wenger, N., & Hirsh- ford University Press.
Pasek, K. (1992). Young children and adults use lex­ Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influ­
how Children really Learn Vocabulary 63

ence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary Johnson, J. E., Christie, J. R., & Yawkey, T. D. (1999).
development via maternal speech. Child Develop­ Play and early childhood development (2nd ed.).
ment, 74, 1368–1378. New York: Addison-Wesley/Longman.
Hoff, E. (2006a). Environmental supports for language Jusczyk, P. W., Houston, D., & Newsome, M. (1999).
acquisition. In D. K. Dickinson & S. B. Neuman The beginnings of word segmentation in English-
(Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 2, learning infants. Cognitive Psychology, 39, 159–
pp. 163–172). New York: Guilford Press. 207.
Hoff, E. (2006b). How social contexts support and Katz, J. R. (2001). Playing at home: The talk of pre­
shape language development. Developmental Re­ tend play. In D. K. Dickinson & P. O. Tabors (Eds.),
view, 26, 55–88. Beginning literacy with language (pp. 53–73). New
Hoff, E. (2009). Do vocabulary differences explain York: Brookes.
achievement gaps and can vocabulary-targeted in­ Kemler-Nelson, D. G., Egan, L. C., & Holt, M. (2004).
terventions close them? Unpublished manuscript, When children ask What is it?: What do they want

s
Florida Atlantic University. to know about artifacts? Psychological Science, 15,

es
Hoff, E., & Naigles, L. (2002). How children use 384–389.
input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development, 73, Kontos, S., Howes, C., Shinn, M., & Galinsky, E.
418–433. (1997). Children’s experiences in family child care

Pr
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991). Mother–child conversation and relative care as a function of family income and
in different social classes and communicative set­ ethnicity. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 43, 386–403.
tings. Child Development, 62, 782–796. Kontos, S. J. (1991). Child care quality, family back­

rd
Hollich, G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Tucker, M. L., & ground, and children’s development. Early Child­
Golinkoff, R. M. (2000). A change is afoot: Emer­ hood Research Quarterly, 6, 249–262.

lfo
gentist thinking in language acquisition. In P. Ander­ Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., & Swank, P. R. (2006).
son, C. Emmeche, N. O. Finnemann, & P. V. Chris­ Responsive parenting: Establishing early founda­

ui
tiansen (Eds.), Downward causation (pp. 143–178). tions for social, communication, and independent
Oxford, UK: Aarhus University Press. problem-solving skills. Developmental Psychology,
G
Howes, C. (2000). Social-emotional classroom climate 42, 627–642.
in childcare, child–teacher relationships and chil­ Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., Assel, M. A.,
dren’s second grade peer relations. Social Develop­ & Vellet, S. (2001). Does early responsive parenting
e

ment, 9, 191–204. have a special importance for children’s develop­


Th

Howes, C., Phillips, D., & Whitebook, M. (1992). ment or is consistency across early childhood neces­
Thresholds of quality: Implications for the social sary? Developmental Psychology, 37, 387–403.
development of children in center-based child care. Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., & Miller-
11

Child Development, 63, 449–460. Loncar, C. L. (2000). Early maternal and child in­
Howes, C., & Smith, E. W. (1995). Relations among fluences on children’s later independent cognitive
child care quality, teacher behavior, children’s play and social functioning. Child Development, 71,
20

activities, emotional security, and cognitive activity 358–375.


in child care. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, Landry, S. H., Swank, P. R., Smith, K. E., Assel, M. A.,
10, 381–404. & Gunnewig, S. B. (2006). Enhancing early literacy
©

Huebner, C. E. (2000a). Community-based support skills for preschool children: Bringing a professional
for preschool readiness among children in poverty. development model to scale. Journal of Learning
Journal of Education for Students Placed at Risk, Disabilities, 39, 306–324.
ht

5, 291–314. Levy, A. K. (1992). Sociodramatic play as a method for


Huebner, C. E. (2000b). Promoting toddlers’ language enhancing the language performance of kindergar­
ig

development through community-based interven­ ten age students. Early Childhood Research Quar­
tion. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychol­ terly, 7, 245–262.
yr

ogy, 21, 513–535. Lillard, A., & Else-Quest, N. (2006). The early years:
Huebner, C. E., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). Intervention Evaluating Montessori education. Science, 313,
op

to change parent–child reading style: A comparison 1893–1894.


of instructional methods. Journal of Applied Devel­ Love, J., Ryer, P., & Faddis, B. (1992). Caring envi­
C

opmental Psychology, 26, 296–313. ronments: Program quality in California’s publicly


Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & funded child development programs. Portsmouth,
Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Rela­ NH: RMC Research.
tion to language input and gender. Developmental Love, J. M., Harrison, L., Sagi Schwartz, A., Van
Psychology, 27, 236–248. IJzendoorn, M. H., Ross, C., Ungerer, J. A., et al.
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & (2003). Child care quality matters: How conclu­
Levine, S. (2002). Language input and child syntax. sions may vary with context. Child Development,
Cognitive Psychology, 45, 337–374. 74, 1021–1033.
Imai, M., Li, L., Haryu, E., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, Maguire, M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R.
R. M., & Shigematsu, J. (2008). Novel noun and (2006). A unified theory of word learning: Putting
verb learning in Chinese, English, and Japanese verb acquisition in context. In K. Hirsh-Pasek & R.
children: Universality and language-specificity in Golinkoff (Eds.), Action meets word: How children
novel noun and verb learning. Child Development, learn verbs (pp. 364–392). New York: Oxford Uni­
79, 979–1000. versity Press.
64 BASIC DEVELOPMENTAL PROCESSES

Maguire, M. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Early child care and children’s development prior
Brandone, A. C. (2008). Focusing on the relation: to school entry: Results from the NICHD Study of
Fewer exemplars facilitate children’s initial verb Early Child Care. American Educational Research
learning and extension. Developmental Science, Journal, 39, 133–164.
11, 628–634. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2005).
Malvern, D., Richards, B., Chipere, N., & Durán, P. Pathways to reading: The role of oral language in
(2004). Lexical diversity and language develop­ the transition to reading. Developmental Psychol­
ment: Quantification and assessment. Palgrave- ogy, 41, 428–442.
Macmillan. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, & Dun­
Marcon, R. A. (1993). Socioemotional versus academ­ can, G. (2003). Modeling the impacts of child care
ic emphasis: Impact on kindergartners’ development quality on children’s preschool cognitive develop­
and achievement. Early Child Development and ment. Child Development, 74, 1454–1475.
Care, 96, 81–91. Nicolopoulou, A., McDowell, J., & Brockmeyer, C.

s
Marcon, R. A. (2002). Moving up the grades: Rela­ (2006). Narrative play and emergent literacy: Sto­

es
tionship between preschool model and later school rytelling and story-acting meet journal writing. In
success. Early Childhood Research and Practice, 4, D. G. Singer, R. M. Golinkoff, & K. Hirsh-Pasek
1–20. (Eds.), Play = Learning: How play motivates and

Pr
Masur, E. F. (1982). Mothers’ responses to infants’ enhances children’s cognitive and social–emotional
object-related gestures: Influences on lexical devel­ growth (pp. 124–144). New York: Oxford Univer­
opment. Journal of Child Language, 9, 23–30. sity Press.

rd
McCartney, K. (1984). Effect of quality of day care Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., & Snow, C. E.
environment on children’s language development. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vo­

lfo
Developmental Psychology, 20, 244–260. cabulary production in low-income families. Child
Miller, E., & Almon, J. (2009). Crisis in the kindergar­ Development, 76, 763–782.

ui
ten: Why children need to play in school. College Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., & Burchinal, M. R. (1997).
Park, MD: Alliance for Childhood. Relations between preschool children’s child-care
G
Mol, S. E., Bus, A. G., De Jong, M. T., & Smeets, D. experiences and concurrent development: The Cost,
J. H. (2008). Added value of dialogic parent–child Quality, and Outcomes Study. Merrill–Palmer
book readings: A meta-analysis. Early Education Quarterly, 43, 451–477.
e

and Development, 19, 7–26. Pellegrini, A. D. (2009). The role of play in human de­
Th

Montie, J. E., Xiang, Z., & Schweinhart, L. J. (2006). velopment. New York: Oxford University Press.
Preschool experience in 10 countries: Cognitive and Pellegrini, A. D., & Galda, L. (1990). Children’s play,
language performance at age 7. Early Childhood language, and early literacy. Topics in Language
11

Research Quarterly, 21, 313–331. Disorders, 10, 76–88.


Morrison, F. J., & Cooney, R. R. (2002). Achievement: Pellegrini, A. D., Galda, L., Dresden, J., & Cox, S.
Multiple paths to early literacy. In J. G. Borkowski, (1991). A longitudinal study of the predictive re­
20

S. L. Ramey, M. Bristol-Power, & the Robert Wood lations among symbolic play, linguistic verbs, and
Johnson Foundation, National Institute of Child early literacy. Research in the Teaching of English,
Health and Human Development (Eds.), Parent­ 25, 219–235.
©

ing and the child’s world: Influences on academic, Penno, J. F., Wilkinson, I. A. G., & Moore, D. W.
intellectual, and social–emotional development (2002). Vocabulary acquisition from teacher ex­
(pp. 141–160). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. planation and repeated listening to stories: Do they
ht

Myers, E. B., Blumstein, S., Walsh, E., & Eliassen, J. overcome the Matthew effect? Journal of Educa­
(2009). Inferior frontal regions underly the percep­ tional Psychology, 94, 23–33.
ig

tion of phonetic category invariance. Psychological Polka, L., Sundara, M., & Blue, S. (2002). The role of
Science, 20, 895–903. language experience in word segmentation: A com­
yr

Naigles, L. (1990). Children use syntax to learn verb parison of English, French, and bilingual infants.
meanings. Journal of Child Language, 17, 357– Paper presented at the 143rd Meeting of the Acous­
op

374. tical Society of America: Special Session in Memory


Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: of Peter Jusczyk, Pittsburgh, PA.
C

Appleton–Century–Crofts. Pruden, S. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., &


Nelson, K. (1988). Constraints on word learning? Hennon, E. A. (2006). The birth of words: Ten­
Cognitive Development, 3, 221–246. month-old learn words through perceptual salience.
Neuman, S. B., & Dwyer, J. (2009). Missing in action: Child Development, 77, 266–280.
Vocabulary instruction in pre-K. Reading Teacher, Robbins, C., & Ehri, L. C. (1994). Reading storybooks
62, 384–392. to kindergartners helps them learn new vocabulary
Neuman, S. B., & Roskos, K. (1992). Literacy objects words. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86,
as cultural tools: Effects on children’s literacy be­ 54–64.
haviors in play. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, Roskos, K., Tabors, P. O., & Lenhart, L. A. (2004).
203–225. Oral language and early literacy in preschool: Talk­
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2000). ing, reading, and writing. Newark, DE: Internation­
The relation of child care to cognitive and language al Reading Association.
development. Child Development, 71, 960–980. Roskos, K., Tabors, P. O., & Lenhart, L. A. (2009).
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2002). Oral language and early literacy in preschool: Talk­
how Children really Learn Vocabulary 65

ing, reading, and writing (2nd ed.). Newark, DE: Maternal responsiveness and early language acqui­
International Reading Association. sition. Advances in Child Development and Behav­
Rowe, M. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). Differ­ ior, 29, 89–127.
ences in early gesture explain SES disparities in Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baum­
child vocabulary size at school entry. Science, 323, well, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and chil­
951–953. dren’s achievement of language milestones. Child
Rowe, M. L., Özçaliskan, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S. Development, 72, 748–767.
(2008). Learning words by hand: Gesture’s role Tincoff, R., & Jusczyk, P. W. (1999). Some beginnings
in predicting vocabulary development. First Lan­ of word comprehension in 6-month-olds. Psycho­
guage, 28, 182–199. logical Science, 10, 172–175.
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human
Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
274, 1926–1928. Press.

s
Saffran, J. R., Werker, J., & Werner, L. (2006). The Tomasello, M., & Farrar, J. (1986). Joint attention

es
infant’s auditory world: Hearing, speech, and the and early language. Child Development, 57, 1454–
beginnings of language. In R. Siegler & D. Kuhn 1463.
(Eds.), Handbook of child development (pp. 58– Tomasello, M., & Kruger, A. (1992). Joint attention

Pr
108). New York: Wiley. on actions: Acquiring verbs in ostensive and non-
Sameroff, A. (1983). Resilient children and how they ostensive contexts. Journal of Child Language, 19,
grew. PsycCRITIQUES, 28, 11–12. 311–333.

rd
Schliecker, E., White, D. R., & Jacobs, E. (1991). The Tulving, E. (1968). When is recall higher than recogni­
role of day care quality in the prediction of children’s tion? Psychonomic Science, 10, 53–54.

lfo
vocabulary. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Sci­ Vasilyeva, M., Huttenlocher, J., & Waterfall, H.
ence, 23, 12–24. (2006). Effects of language intervention on syntac­

ui
Schweinhart, L. (2004). The High/Scope Perry Pre­ tic skill levels in preschoolers. Developmental Psy­
school Study through age 40. Ypsilanti, MI: High/ chology, 42, 164–174.
G
Scope Educational Research Foundation. Vedeler, L. (1997). Dramatic play: A format for “liter­
Schweinhart, L. J., Weikart, D., & Larner, M. B. ate” language? British Journal of Educational Psy­
(1986). Consequences of three preschool curriculum chology, 67, 153–167.
e

models through age 15. Early Childhood Research Wakschlag, L. S., & Hans, S. L. (1999). Relation of
Th

Quarterly, 1, 15–45. maternal responsiveness during infancy to the de­


Schweinhart, L. J., & Weikart, D. P. (1988). The High/ velopment of behavior problems in high-risk youths.
Scope Perry Preschool Program. In R. H. Price, E. Developmental Psychology, 35, 569–579.
11

L. Cowen, R. P. Lorion, & J. Ramos-McKay (Eds.), Walker, D., Greenwood, C., Hart, B., & Carta, J.
Fourteen ounces of prevention: A casebook for (1994). Prediction of school outcomes based on
practitioners (pp. 53–66). Washington, DC: Ameri­ early language production and socioeconomic fac­
20

can Psychological Association. tors. Child Development, 65, 606–621.


Shonkoff, J., & Phillips, D. (2000). From neurons to Wasik, B. A., & Bond, M. A. (2001). Beyond the pages
neighborhoods: The science of early childhood de­ of a book: Interactive book reading and language
©

velopment. Washington, DC: National Academy development in preschool classrooms. Journal of


Press. Educational Psychology, 93, 243–250.
Singer, D., Golinkoff, R., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (Eds.). Waxman, S. R., & Lidz, J. (2006). Early word learn­
ht

(2006). Play = learning: How play motivates and ing. In D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of
enhances children’s cognitive and social–emotional child psychology (6th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 299–335).
ig

growth. New York: Oxford University Press. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.


Snow, C. E. (1986). Conversations with children. In Weizman, Z. O., & Snow, C. E. (2001). Lexical input as
yr

P. Fletcher & M. Garman (Eds.), Language acqui­ related to children’s vocabulary acquisition: Effects
sition: Studies in first language development (2nd of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning.
op

ed., pp. 69–89). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Uni­ Developmental Psychology, 37, 265–279.
versity Press. Zevenbergen, A. A., Whitehurst, G. J., & Zevenbergen,
C

Sutton-Smith, B. (2001). The ambiguity of play. Cam­ J. A. (2003). Effects of a shared-reading intervention
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press. on the inclusion of evaluative devices in narratives of
Tabors, P. O., Snow, C. E., & Dickinson, D. K. (2001). children from low-income families. Journal of Ap­
Homes and schools together: Supporting language plied Developmental Psychology, 24, 1–15.
and literacy development. In D. K. Dickinson & Zill, N., Resnick, G., & McKey, R. H. (1999, April).
P. O. Tabors (Eds.), Beginning literacy with lan­ What children know and can do at the end of Head
guage: Young children learning at home and school Start and how it relates to program quality. Presen­
(pp. 313–334). Baltimore: Brookes. tation at the annual meeting of the Society for Re­
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (2002). search in Child Development, Albuquerque, NM.

Copyright © 2011 The Guilford Press. All rights reserved under International Copyright Convention. No part
of this text may be reproduced, transmitted, downloaded, or stored in or introduced into any information
storage or retrieval system, in any form or by any means, whether electronic or mechanical, now known or
hereinafter invented, without the written permission of The Guilford Press.
Guilford Publications, 72 Spring Street, New York, NY 10012, 212-431-9800. www.guilford.com/p/neuman2

You might also like