Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 24

Psychological Bulletin Copyrittht 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1995, Vol. 118, No. 2, 199-222 0033-2909/95/13.00

Psychological Science and the Use of Anatomically Detailed Dolls in


Child Sexual-Abuse Assessments
Gerald P. Koocher Gail S. Goodman
Harvard Medical School University of California, Davis

C. Sue White William N. Friedrich


Case Western Reserve University School of Medicine Mayo Clinic

Abigail B. Sivan Cecil R. Reynolds


Rush-Presbyterian-St. Luke's Medical Center Texas A&M University

Many devices are used in child assessment and treatment as communication aids, projective tools,
and symbolic means of interaction. None are as hotly debated in their application among mental
health professionals as dolls with genital details. Anatomically detailed (AD) dolls are often used in
sexual-abuse evaluation and treatment with children, but such applications are controversial. This
article is the product of a working group formed to review AD doll research and practice. This
article reviews historical use of dolls in clinical inquiry and research on sexual behaviors in children,
normative use of AD dolls in nonreferred children, differences in children's play behavior and emo-
tional reactions to AD dolls, and memory and suggestibility issues relating to AD-dolI use. Recom-
mendations for future research are provided.

On February 8,1991, the American Psychology Association's help older children who may have difficulty expressing themselves
(APA) Council of Representatives formally adopted the follow- verbally on sexual topics.
ing Statement on the Use of Anatomically Detailed Dolls in Fo- These dolls are available from a variety of vendors and are readily
rensic Evaluations: sold to anyone who wishes to purchase them. The design, detail,
and nature of the dolls vary considerably across manufacturers.
Anatomically detailed dolls are widely used in conducting assess- Neither the dolls, nor their use, are standardized or accompanied
ments in cases of alleged child sexual abuse. In general, such dolls by normative data. There are currently no uniform standards for
may be useful in helping children to communicate when their lan- conducting interviews with the dolls.
guage skills or emotional concerns preclude direct verbal re-
sponses. These dolls may also be useful communication props to We urge continued research in quest of more and better data
regarding the stimulus properties of such dolls and normative be-
havior of abused and nonabused children. Nevertheless, doll-cen-
tered assessment of children when used as part of a psychological
Gerald P. Koocher, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical evaluation and interpreted by experienced and competent examin-
School; Gail S. Goodman, Department of Psychology, University of ers, may be the best available practical solution for a pressing and
California, Davis; C. Sue White, Department of Psychiatry, Case West- frequent clinical problem (i.e., investigation of the possible pres-
ern Reserve University School of Medicine; William N. Friedrich, De- ence of sexual abuse of a child).
partment of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN;
Therefore, in conformity with the Ethical Principles of Psychol-
Abigail B. Sivan, Department of Psychiatry, Rush-Presbyterian-St.
Luke's Medical Center, Chicago, IL; Cecil R. Reynolds, Department of ogists, psychologists who undertake the doll-centered assessment
Educational Psychology, Texas A&M University. of sexual abuse should be competent to use these techniques. We
This article is the work of the Anatomical Doll Working Group function- recommend that psychologists document by videotape (whenever
ing with financial support from the American Psychological Association and possible), audiotape, or in writing the procedures they use for each
consisting of William N. Friedrich, Gail S. Goodman, Gerald P. Koocher administration. Psychologists should be prepared to provide clini-
(Chair), Abigail B. Sivan, C. Sue White, and Cecil R. Reynolds. The content cal and empirical rationale (i.e., published studies, clinical experi-
of this article is solely our work and does not necessarily represent the policy ence, etc.) for procedures employed and for interpretation of re-
of the American Psychological Association. sults derived from using anatomically detailed dolls (American
We appreciate the detailed constructive critique provided by Barbara Boat Psychological Association, 1991).
and David Faust in response to earlier versions of this article and the sharing
of ideas and works in progress by Maggie Bruck, Steve Ceci, and Paul Meehl. This statement was the end result of a 4-year process and con-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Gerald P. siderable compromise among competing political entities
Koocher, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Children's within APA. The goal of the proponents was to place on the
Hospital, 300 Longwood Avenue, Boston, Massachusetts 02115. Electronic record for the forensic community a statement confirming that
mail may be sent via Internet to koocher@a 1 .tch.harvard.edu. anatomically detailed (AD) dolls can play an important and
199
200 KOOCHER ET AL.

valid role in the clinical assessment of child sexual abuse. The exploration of the AD doll's orifices) as indicative of abuse? Are
statement also lists a number of caveats, professional cautions, objections raised by some adults to the use of AD dolls on the
and contextual concerns. Some members of the council op- basis of valid scientific principles? Are some adults so resistant
posed the statement as potentially restricting professional dis- to believing children's reports of sexual abuse that they focus
cretion or as creating standards that might lead to litigation on the potential for misinterpretation of AD-doll play as a way
against some practitioners. During the debate on the resolution, of dismissing children's disclosures?
it became clear to many council members that there was no The answers to these questions are not simple. Although it
comprehensive review of the scientific and clinical knowledge is difficult to avoid dichotomous thinking about AD-doll use,
base on AD-doll use in the scholarly literature. The council then clearly there are both potential benefits and problems associ-
approved funding for a group of scholars to prepare such a re- ated with it. In this article, we explore the historical use of dolls
view. This article is the product of the working group. in clinical inquiry, followed by an overview of what is known
It is well-known that young children's reports of events are about sexual behaviors in childhood. Next, we review studies on
often less complete than those of older children and adults. It is nonreferred and referred children's reactions to AD dolls and
also well-known that cues and reenactment can at times result examine research on children's memory and suggestibility in
in elicitation of more complete information from children (e.g., relation to AD-doll interviews. Finally, we suggest productive
Flavell, 1985; Kail, 1990; Piaget & Inhelder, 1973; Price & arenas for future investigation.
Goodman, 1990; Ratner, Smith, & Padgett, 1990). Moreover,
there is little doubt that children's ability to express themselves Use of Dolls in Clinical Inquiry
in language improves with age. If AD dolls provide memory
cues and a means for event reenactment, thus aiding the com- The use of dolls as tools in clinical inquiry with children has
munication process, one might expect such dolls to be benefi- been documented since the 1920s (Group for the Advancement
cial for obtaining information from children. Professionals who of Psychiatry, 1982). Dolls and doll play have been regarded as
use AD dolls in child-abuse investigations emphasize these pos- integral tools and valued modes of interaction between mental
sible benefits (e.g., Conerly, 1986; Everson & Boat, 1994; Yates health professionals and children since the beginning of the
& Terr, 1988a, 1988b). Alternatively, there are others who view child-study and play-therapy movements. As early as 1928,
children as prone to sexual fantasy and highly suggestible. Anna Freud acknowledged that work with children required
These people express concern that AD dolls may be suggestive "certain modifications and alterations" (p. 2) when compared
per se and may stimulate sexual and other fantasy (e.g., Ceci & to work with adults.
Bruck, 1994; Gardner, 1991; King & Yuille, 1987; R. J. Levy, Although Anna Freud (1928) and Melanie Klein (1932)
1989; Yates & Terr, 1988a, 1988b). These opposing views un- differed in their interpretation of the play situation, both agreed
derlie much of the controversy regarding AD-doll use. that the "play technic [sic] worked out by Mrs. Klein has the great-
Other core issues in the AD-doll controversy concern in- est value for the observation of the child" (Freud, 1928, p. 30).
terviewers' possible contamination of child interviews and their
potential misinterpretation of children's responses to AD dolls. Instead of losing time and energy keeping track of the child in its
Interviewers who are not properly trained and victim sensitive domestic environment, we transplant at one stroke its entire
known world to the room of the analyst without any interference
may contaminate the validity of interview data whether they
for the time being. We thus have an opportunity to become familiar
use AD dolls or not (H. Levy, Kalinowski, Markovic, Pittman, with its various reactions, the strength of its aggressive inclinations,
& Ahart, 1991). Similarly, clinicians who are unfamiliar with its capacity for sympathy, as well as its attitude toward different
appropriate validity standards in the behavioral sciences may objects and persons which are represented by the dolls." (Freud,
also be prone to justify a forensic determination of child sexual 1928, p. 30)
abuse on the basis of doll play alone where it may not be war-
ranted (Wolfner, Faust, & Dawes, 1993). Because AD dolls are Dolls are routinely included among the essential equipment
easily purchased and used with forensic diagnostic intent by for a playroom in which to interact with young children, and
individuals with little or no behavioral science or forensic train- their use is consistent with any theory of psychotherapy
ing, considerable vigilance in interpreting data collected from (Edington, 1985). In the therapeutic context, dolls afford the
using AD dolls is warranted. However, even if considerable vig- opportunity to "recreate self, family, and day-to-day events"
ilance is expended, psychologists must determine whether AD (Wright, Everett, & Roisman, 1986, p. 93). Dolls are an "expe-
doll-aided professionals reach valid conclusions. dient" by which the child "is given the opportunity to repro-
Differing perspectives on the benefits and dangers of using AD duce the exact mental mechanisms which are disturbing him
dolls in sexual-abuse assessments have resulted in a lively de- [or her] "(Solomon, 1938, p. 492).
bate, and a number of pressing questions have emerged: Do AD In the 1930s, researchers explored the use of other stimuli,
dolls assist children in reporting or communicating about sex- such as drawings and photos, through which children could
ual experiences? Are AD dolls intrinsically suggestive of sexual project their feelings and express their developing sense of self
acts? Are they suggestive only in the context of leading question- (Horowitz, 1939). However, in their assessments of the racial
ing? Are they suggestive only with young children? Are children identification of African American children during the 1940s
so suggestible and prone to sexual fantasy that AD dolls elicit and 1950s, the Clarks (1958) returned to the use of dolls rather
false reports regardless of questioning context? Is there a subset than more abstract representations. They used a simple para-
of children for whom AD dolls elicit false reports? Do adults digm in which children were asked to choose which one of four
misinterpret children's innocent behavior with AD dolls (e.g., Black or White dolls most closely fit a series of questions de-
ANATOMICAL DOLLS 201

signed to elicit preferences and identifications. The findings sult, a study that became one of the first research articles pub-
from these interviews were highly influential (Kluger, 1975) lished on AD dolls was initiated (White, Strom, Santilli, & Hal-
and, in fact, were ultimately heard in the United States Supreme pin, 1986), and a structured interview protocol was developed.
Court in the celebrated case of Brown v. Board of Education There are undoubtedly more stories of how AD dolls became a
(1954) to illustrate the damage created by segregation. More- part of local interviewing practices in cases of suspected child
over, the Clarks's doll technique continues to be a popular sexual abuse, but these two anecdotes reflect the evolution of
means to study the racial preferences of young children as illus- clinical guidelines developed on an ad hoc basis out of urgent
trated by research on the effectiveness of interventions designed forensic and clinical need (White, Strom, & Santilli, 1985).
to reduce negative racial stereotypes (Powell-Hopson & Hop- In the strictest sense, this evolutionary pattern highlights a
son, 1988). significant ethical problem: how to develop new assessment
The collective experience of generations of clinicians as well strategies predicted on urgent clinical necessity while maintain-
as that of researchers in child development suggests that child's ing adequate scientific rigor. Because of the forensic events that
play is often a reflection of the reality of his or her experience routinely issue from disclosure of alleged sexual abuse, testi-
(Sivan, 1991). In several contexts, dolls may offer children a mony and data on the basis of unvalidated and nonstandardized
means of expressing themselves when words are not available AD-doll assessments quickly made their way into the legal sys-
either because of limited vocabulary or warnings not to speak tem. Some scholars have argued that application should "follow
about a given topic, as is often the case in circumstances of sex- knowledge gained from research, rather than precede it"
ual abuse (Sinason, 1988). (Wolfner et al., 1993, p. 9). Although we agree in principle,
practitioners on the front line of child sexual-abuse work were
Evolution of the Use of Sexually AD Dolls often in the position of choosing between urgent clinical need
and ideal standards for development and validation of assess-
Although there is no record of the precise date on which AD ment techniques. In addition, as discussed later in this article,
dolls made their appearance in clinical work, the best guess is conducting normative research presents its own set of ethical
that the first dolls specifically manufactured with genital repre- and methodological problems. The significant ethical difficulty
sentations appeared in the mid-1970s. Prior to that time, some arrived when persons without scientific training or appropriate
clinicians may have used AD dolls that were individually modi- caution (i.e., police, child welfare workers, and others including,
fied for purposes other than sexual-abuse investigations. Al- alas, some psychologists and psychiatrists) began using AD
though precise documentation is elusive, two historically sepa- dolls as though almost anything done with them yielded data of
rate events illustrate how use of AD dolls evolved. intrinsic validity for evaluation of sexual abuse (e.g., see In re
The first occurred in Eugene, Oregon in the mid-1970s when Amber B., 1987).
Virginia Friedemann, a police detective, and Marcia Morgan, In any case, by the mid-1980s, with increased reporting and
the director of a rape victim assistance program, recognized the investigation of child sexual-abuse allegations, the use of AD
need "to establish more clear communication between in- dolls became widespread. During this time, the issue arose as to
terviewers and children" (Friedemann & Morgan, 1985, p. iv) whether AD dolls constitute a "psychological test" for sexual
in cases of alleged sexual abuse. As a result, "natural dolls" were abuse.
developed, and subsequently Friedemann and Morgan formed
the first company to produce such dolls on a large scale. Other Are AD Dolls a "Psychological Test"?
companies then followed suit.
The second historical line of AD-doll development was more Over the years, one significant problem has been the assump-
serendipitous. In the late 1970s and early 1980s, Cleveland tion on the part of some that AD dolls constitute some form
(Ohio) Metropolitan General Hospital (CMGH) was unoffi- of psychological test with demonstrated potential for detecting
cially designated as the county's receiving hospital for the ma- child sexual abuse or for stating with a degree of certainty that
jority of sexual-abuse cases involving young children. Because sexual abuse occurred. Such assumptions are unfounded and
there were no guidelines or specific graduate-level mental health dangerous for a variety of reasons.
training for evaluating allegations of sexual abuse, front-line cli- To elevate AD dolls to the status of a psychological test grants
nicians were forced to create ad hoc methods of evaluation for implications of utility and benefit that are undeserved and with-
significant numbers of young children. In struggling with this out basic foundation. The National Research Council, APA, the
problem, it was noted that homemade dolls with anatomical American Educational Research Association, and the National
details were used in some hospital locations as teaching tools to Council on Measurement in Education have all endorsed the
prepare children for impending medical procedures (e.g., position that, properly used, psychological tests provide a better
catheterizations). A set of such dolls was secured for use in basis for making certain decisions than would otherwise be
abuse evaluations by a group at CMGH. By 1983, AD dolls available (Committee to Develop Standards for Educational
were a regular part of abuse evaluations with young children at and Psychological Testing, 1985). Psychological tests are tools
CMGH, and data obtained in these assessments were routinely of measurement based on psychological science. Measurement
presented in court as indicators of sexual abuse. is a set of rules for assigning numbers to objects or events such
Despite the early clinical enthusiasm, caution soon took as behavior, responses to questions, and the like. A psychological
hold. There were no scientific bases on which to reach conclu- test provides a set of standardized procedures for the presenta-
sions using AD-doll data. The clinical sample was small, and tion of common (i.e., standardized) stimuli with rules for re-
there were no normative data on nonabused children. As a re- cording responses to the stimuli and rules for then assigning
202 KOOCHER ET AL.

quantitative features (i.e., a score) to the elicited responses. nication process allowing children and others to demonstrate
Clearly, AD dolls do not meet any of these requirements as yet acts for which they have limited verbal descriptions and limited
(Skinner & Berry, 1993), although one can imagine developing familiarity in life or about which they are too embarrassed to
a test that includes a standardized set of AD dolls. At present, speak. How well they serve these functions and their effects on
AD dolls are not standardized as stimulus materials; they ap- the accuracy and completeness of a child's recounting of sexu-
pear in many shapes, sizes, colors, and even have varying genital ally related events are the topics of other sections of this article
and related characteristics that may affect a child's perceptions concerning research on AD dolls.
and responses to AD dolls. The questions used to elicit re-
sponses, the question sequencing, and the positioning of an AD AD-Doll Interviews: Survey Findings on Use and
doll-aided interview in the forensic process have not been stan-
Professional Training
dardized, and no quantifiable means of evaluating responses
has been proffered. Given that AD dolls do not constitute a test for child sexual
Although some suggested protocols for use of AD dolls have abuse and that no standardization exists, how then are AD dolls
been published (see, for example, American Professional Soci- used in actual practice? How much training do professionals
ety on the Abuse of Children's Guidelines, 1990; Boat & Ever- have in using AD dolls to interview children? What are the char-
son, 1986; Friedemann & Morgan, 1985; H. Levy et al., 1991; acteristics or features of the AD dolls used? In addition, what
White et al., 1986), there is no uniform acceptance of such sug- do professionals say they take as evidence of sexual abuse when
gestions. In addition, there is no documentation of validity of children interact with AD dolls? For example, if a child inserts
AD dolls or of these suggested protocols for forensic purposes. a finger into one of the doll's body openings, do professionals
No one knows if different AD dolls lead to different responses interpret such actions as a sign of sexual victimization? Fortu-
by children, how varying the questions or their sequence causes nately, these important questions have been the topic of
children to alter their responses, or how these variables may in- research.
teract with each other or the timing of using AD dolls in in- In a survey of professionals in North Carolina undertaken in
terviews during what may be months (or even a year or more) 1985, Boat and Everson (1988b) attempted to evaluate (a) who
of investigative procedures. was using AD dolls, (b) what kind of training these individuals
Some may assume AD dolls are appropriately labeled if called had received, (c) what AD dolls were being used, (d) how AD
a projective test or instrument. However, even projective tech- dolls were used, and (e) what criteria interviewers used to make
niques have a standard set of stimulus materials and specific judgments about children's responses to AD dolls. Out of 600
rules of inquiry, and most have a set of scoring criteria. AD dolls requests, they received replies from 295 professionals in the
have none of these attributes and cannot be said accurately to area of child investigation; child protection workers (N = 92),
have been derived from or fit properly even on a post hoc basis law enforcement officers (N = 46), mental health practitioners
the projective hypothesis (e.g., see Chandler, 1990). (N = 60), and physicians (N = 97). Of these, one third had
In summary, the requisite information is simply unavailable used AD dolls for more than 1 year. Child protection workers
and not known to be forthcoming to allow AD dolls to be used were the most active users (68%), law enforcement next (35%),
as a test (see Committee to Develop Standards for Educational followed by mental health professionals (28%) and physicians
and Psychological Testing's Standards for Educational and Psy- (13%). Although respondents in all four groups indicated they
chological Testing, 1985, especially Part II, sections 6 and 7). intended to increase their use of AD dolls, the child protection
Consistent with this conclusion, Everson and Boat (1994) workers and mental health practitioners were the most zealous
found that none of the 16 published guidelines or the 4 unpub- advocates of escalating their usage.
lished but widely disseminated protocols endorsed use of AD When Boat and Everson (1988b) examined respondents' lev-
dolls as a "diagnostic test." els of training with regard to AD dolls, the authors' response
If not a test, what are AD dolls? As Everson and Boat (1994) categories ranged from "formal workshops" to simple "discus-
pointed out, AD dolls can serve a variety of functions, including sions with a supervisor or colleague." Even with this broad
(a) a comforter (e.g., to create a more relaxed atmosphere dur- definition, however, fewer than 50% of each type of respondent,
ing a sexual-abuse interview), (b) an icebreaker (e.g., to intro- except mental health practitioners, had any training. Put in per-
duce in a nonleading way the topic of sexuality and to convey spective, however, there were fewer mental health practitioners
that the interviewer is comfortable talking about sex), (c) an using AD dolls (only 28% of those surveyed). One also suspects
anatomical model (e.g., a model for body-part naming), (d) a that most of the reported training involved "discussions with a
demonstration aid (e.g., props to show and tell what happened), supervisor or colleague" or reading books on interviewing
(e) a memory stimulus (e.g., to trigger memory of sexual (Harnest & Chavern, 1985) as professional workshop training
experiences), and (f) a diagnostic screen (e.g., to provide an on AD-doll usage did not begin to appear until 1985 (White,
opportunity for children to spontaneously reveal sexual knowl- 1985a, 1985b) and was not widely offered until the late 1980s.
edge but with major reliance on children's verbal statements Those who had adopted some kind of formal assessment guide-
rather than their behavioral interactions with AD dolls). lines did not exceed 20% of the Boat and Everson sample, but
In general, AD dolls might be considered a diverse set of stim- the precise nature of the guidelines is unknown because the first
uli that can function as communication and memory aids to published set came with Friedemann and Morgan's (1985) AD
children and other individuals who have immature language, dolls.
cognitive, or emotional development or impaired communica- The results of a more recent study by Kendall-Tackett and
tion skills. AD dolls are simply intended to assist in the commu- Watson (1992) of changes in professionals' training and AD-
ANATOMICAL DOLLS 203

doll use can be contrasted with the results of Boat and Everson 1987) to recommendations of at least having different races
(1988b). In 1989, Kendall-Tackett and Watson contacted by available (Boat & Everson, 1986; White et al., 1987), to actually
phone 201 mental health and law enforcement professionals in presenting a cross-section of races representative of the child's
the Boston area, achieving a 99% compliance rate for the in- community (MacFarlane & Krebs, 1986). However, Kendall-
terview. Seventy-three percent used AD dolls, and mental Tackett and Watson (1992) reported that 74% of their respon-
health professionals were more likely to use them than law en- dents still matched the complexion of the AD dolls with the race
forcement professionals. Whereas Boat and Everson found that of the child.
only 47% of professionals received training in AD-doll use, Although there are no data to indicate what is typical practice
Kendall-Tackett and Watson reported that 97% of their inter- nationwide, racial matching should be carefully thought out
viewees had received training, often from more than one source prior to the selection of AD dolls. First, one should recognize
(e.g., professional workshops and supervisors). Furthermore, that AD dolls are manufactured in a variety of complexion col-
78% followed a standardized protocol. Several possible expla- ors and features, which are marketed as Caucasian, Black/
nations for differences in their results compared with Boat and African American, Asian, and Hispanic. When one considers
Everson's results include differences in response rate, cities, that there are no validity studies to assess children's perceptions
training opportunities, and the 3-year period separating data of such presumed representations for each manufacturer's
collection across the two studies. However, regarding the latter product, trying to match the racial characteristics of an AD doll
possibility, Kendall-Tackett and Watson noted that 59% of the and alleged victim has no documented scientific or clinical basis
Boston-area professionals used AD dolls for 4 years or more, with respect to a child's actual behavior. Second, the child eval-
indicating that timing between the two surveys cannot com- uated may not have a single racial identity in his or her heritage
pletely explain the discrepancies. or may be exposed to potential perpetrators of more than one
race, or both. Thus, matching the child's and AD dolls' racial
Variable Procedures in Interviews characteristics may wrongly imply that the alleged perpetrator
is of a particular race. Third, there are no data to support the
Number of sessions and number of dolls. At the time of the conclusion that children use the cue of race to represent the
Boat and Everson (1988b) survey, single investigatory sessions alleged perpetrator. If there is a mental representation or linking
with AD dolls were the norm, although frequency varied by between the AD doll and alleged perpetrator at all (e.g., if abuse
profession (e.g., mental health professionals used a single in- has actually occurred), the scientific community has yet to pro-
terview 86% of the time; but for child protective workers and duce the data describing which children use what cues as repre-
law enforcement officers, the figures were 48% and 40% of the sentative. The implications of race matching are sufficiently
time, respectively). By the mid- to late 1980s, however, many complex that extreme caution is warranted. For example, one
evaluators were recommending more than one session for in- should not conclude that the use of any given doll from a multi-
vestigating sexual-abuse allegations (e.g., Boat & Everson, colored array constitutes valid or reliable racial identification of
1986; White, 1986). Consistent with such advise, Kendall- an alleged perpetrator per se.
Tackett and Watson (1992) found that 44% of Boston-area pro- Body-part survey. A body-part survey involves assessing the
fessionals present AD dolls for two or three sessions and 34% child's knowledge of body-part name and function. The first
always have AD dolls present. body-part survey known to have been incorporated into a set of
When AD dolls were first introduced, most practitioners rec- guidelines was in 1983 by White, Strom, and Santilli. By the
ommended using a family cluster of dolls consisting of father, end of the 1980s, most guidelines (Boat & Everson, 1986;
mother, brother, and sister figures (Boat & Everson, 1986; Friedemann & Morgan, 1985; White et al., 1987) recom-
Friedemann & Morgan, 1985; White, Strom, & Santilli, 1983) mended body-part surveys as integral components of an abuse
to represent the nuclear American family. By the end of the evaluation, so the interviewer would know the child's preferred
1980s, however, some were advocating for a larger set of dolls names for the parts which might be discussed in subsequent
that would also reflect the living circumstances of the nontradi- abuse disclosure and would have an initial assessment of the
tional family. For example, in their third protocol revision, child's level of knowledge of sexual anatomy. Indeed, Kendall-
White and colleagues (1987) recommended a set of eight dolls. Tackett and Watson (1992) found that the naming of body parts
Current practices vary widely, but our discussions with front- was indicated by professionals as the most frequent use of AD
line workers suggest that most continue to use four AD dolls, dolls compared with having children act out what happened or
primarily because of ease of availability (i.e., most manufactur- observing children in free play with AD dolls and that all of
ers sell basic sets consisting of four dolls), convenience (i.e., these uses were particularly likely with young children (e.g.,
storing and transporting) and cost (i.e., the average cost is $250 children under 6 years) compared with children 6 years and
to $300 fora set of four). above.
Matching race with victim. Do evaluators match AD dolls' Dressed or undressed presentation. Whether AD dolls
racial characteristics with that of the alleged victim? In the 1985 should be presented dressed or undressed has been a controver-
survey by Boat and Everson (1988b), mental health practition- sial issue since such dolls were first introduced. Friedemann and
ers reportedly did so 81 % of the time, physicians 63%, child Morgan (1985) have continued to assert that presentation can
protection workers 60%, and law enforcement officers 43%. be done either way without harm to the child's emotional devel-
Throughout the 1980s, recommendations about race matching opment or to the investigation of the case. Others have argued
evolved from specific matching advice (Boat & Everson, 1986; for systematic undressing of the AD dolls (Boat & Everson,
Friedemann & Morgan, 1985; White, Strom, Santilli, & Quinn, 1986; White et al., 1987) or having the AD dolls prearranged in
204 KOOCHER ET AL.

various stages of undress. Boat and Everson's (1988a, 1988b) Professionals' Interpretations of Children's Responses
data as well as Kendall-Tackett and Watson's (1992) data indi-
cate that most evaluators present AD dolls clothed. In the latter "Very convincing" evidence. As part of their survey, Boat
study, of the professionals who used AD dolls, 99% presented and Everson (1988b) asked their participants what they would
clothed AD dolls to children and 73% had children undress the accept as very convincing evidence of sexual abuse in 3- to 5-
AD dolls. year-old children. All of the physicians reported that children
Background knowledge or blind interview. Should in- who made verbal statements would be believed. Still, a substan-
terviewers be blind to background information about the al- tial 88% of child protection workers and mental health practi-
leged abuse before interviewing children with AD dolls? Some tioners would regard the child's verbal statement as very con-
professionals prefer to know almost nothing about a case except vincing evidence. Law enforcement officers were more skepti-
the child's name and age, believing (probably correctly) that the cal; only 69% reported a willingness to accept verbal report
legal system will then be more willing to accept the information unquestionably. If a child demonstrated sexual activities with
obtained (White, 1986). Other professionals feel that it is im- AD dolls but made no accompanying verbal statement, children
portant to know something about the alleged abuse (e.g., Boat under 3 years were believed by only 25% of law officers and be-
& Everson, 1986). Research specifically testing the value of tween 57% and 69% of the other professionals surveyed. AD
blind versus informed AD-doll interviews has not been doll demonstration with verbal description increased the per-
conducted. ceived credibility to 56% (law enforcement) and as much as 76-
92% for other professionals. The most convincing evidence with
AD dolls was ascribed to 6- to 12-year-old children's reports
Doll Features that included verbal description. Whether the same data would
be found were the survey undertaken today is questionable be-
With the increasing reports of allegations of child sexual cause of increasing challenges to children's statements in some
abuse, the demand for AD dolls grew significantly as the 1980s sectors.
progressed. Not all who use AD dolls for investigative work use Interestingly, in a more recent survey, Conte, Sorenson, Fo-
dolls that were specifically manufactured for this purpose. Of garty, and Rosa (1991) reported that professionals rated AD
those in Boat and Everson's (1988b) survey who actually used dolls as the assessment tool most often used by them for evalu-
AD dolls (N= 119), only about half were using a set specifically ating child sexual abuse and rated children's responses to AD
manufactured as AD dolls. Others used Cabbage Patch Dolls dolls as important in substantiating reports of child sexual
with genitalia sewn on, modified versions of Barbie, or some abuse. However, these professionals also indicated that the latter
other readily available doll. responses were not nearly as important as other signs such as
Nonsexual. A wide variety of features is available on spe- physical indicators of sexual abuse and age-inappropriate sex-
cifically manufactured AD dolls. Digitated fingers: Some AD ual knowledge.
dolls are manufactured with totally gloved hands or thumb- What is considered normal behavior? Boat and Everson's
gloved hands such that there are no individual fingers. Others (1988b) survey also produced data that described what profes-
have distinct fingers created with internal wires such that the sionals considered children's "normal" behaviors with the AD
digits can be separated or individually bent into specific posi- dolls. Almost everyone agreed that a child's undressing AD
tions, or both. Facial expressions: AD dolls come with a variety dolls and touching the doll's genitals were within normal limits.
of facial expressions; such as smiling, straight lipped, or a When, however, the child touched the AD doll's breasts or anal
turned-down mouth; some have eyes gazing forward, others area, law officers were less likely to consider this normal,
seem to glance to the side. Facial parts: Some AD dolls have all whereas most other professionals did. If the child placed the AD
of their facial features designed in detail, and some are even dolls on top of each other, 50% of the protective service workers
accentuated by cloth design. Others have all the features painted considered this normal behavior, as did 39% of the physicians
on. Still others omit some features of the normal head (e.g., and 29% of the mental health providers. Only 6% of the law
many omit ears). enforcement officers considered this juxtapositioning within
Sexual. The presentation of sexual anatomy is the primary normal limits. If vaginal penetration was demonstrated on the
reason for the use of AD dolls. How these body parts are accen- dolls, less than 15% of any professional group considered it nor-
tuated varies across manufacturers. Some are stuffed, some are mal behavior. Notably, there were individuals in all professional
given different colors from the rest of the doll, and some are groups who thought that for 2- to 5-year-olds, the demonstra-
drawn on the AD doll's body in two dimensions. Some have tion of sexual intercourse, oral-genital contact, and anal pene-
large body openings (e.g., mouth, anus, and vagina); others tration were all normal behaviors. The accuracy of such beliefs
have no openings at all. Some of the openings are large enough may be contrasted with normative data obtained from children
to accommodate a child's finger but not an adult's finger. Some with no known history of sexual abuse, discussed later in this
have secondary sexual characteristics, such as pubic hair and article.
breasts. Although critics have charged that the sexual parts on So far, this review concentrates on survey data concerning
AD dolls are exaggerated (Berliner, 1988; Freeman & Estrada- AD doll use in actual practice. We now turn to the scientific
Mullaney, 1988), Bays (1990), the only study to evaluate the research base relevant to AD dolls. However, before discussing
proportion of such parts, reported that the AD dolls had geni- such research, ethical and methodological considerations must
talia of proportional size to the human figure. be highlighted because these considerations mold and constrain
ANATOMICAL DOLLS 205

scientists' efforts to understand the impact of AD dolls on chil- gers on both sides. On the one hand, if researchers put child
dren's play behavior and reports of sexual abuse. participants in compromising situations, the children's welfare
could be harmed. On the other hand, when studies do not
sufficiently mimic actual forensic situations and their results are
Ethical and Methodological Considerations
consequently misleading, reliance on such studies for determin-
Realistic scientific research is needed to answer important ing forensic practice, influencing court decisions, and setting
questions about the use of AD dolls in the clinical and forensic social policy could lead to grave injustices for actual child vic-
context. However, a number of ethical and methodological is- tims and innocent defendants. Some argue that it is impossible
sues influence the definitiveness of such research. For example, to generalize from studies of AD dolls to actual child sexual-
investigators studying the normative behavior of children with abuse interviews because of the dramatic differences in context
AD dolls often hope to exclude from their studies children with (e.g., Ceci & Bruck, 1993, 1994); this has not stopped some of
a history of sexual victimization. Obviously, if a researcher these same investigators from applying their findings on AD
wants to study nonabused children's reactions to AD dolls, it is dolls to legal cases (Bruck & Ceci, 1993). Although the issue of
important to have a nonabused sample. Although researchers ecological validity is critically important, it should be kept in
can ask parents whether their children have been abused, it is mind that there is not just one context inherent in child sexual-
impossible to know for certain whether abused children have abuse interviews (Brigham, 1991). A range exists, and current
inadvertently slipped into a sample. Parents may not know studies may generalize about some situations but not about
whether the child was abused or may not define certain actions others.
as abusive. Moreover, the very act of asking poses ethical issues. With these considerations in mind, we now turn to a brief
If the family admits to sexual abuse, the researcher may be ob- review of empirical studies that have explored children's nor-
ligated to report it to government authorities. One way around mative sexual behaviors generally and then their normative be-
this problem is to ask parents whether any one of a number of havior with AD dolls specifically.
eligibility requirements, presented as a group, describes the
child (e.g., Is the child mentally retarded? Is the child a fluent Children's Sexual Behaviors
speaker of English? Does the child have emotional problems? Is
the child a victim of sexual abuse?) without asking them to A child's behavior with anatomical dolls is presumably re-
point out which question applied. Parents can also be fore- lated to a variety of factors, one of which is the extent and na-
warned during recruitment that they will be asked to sign a ture of sexual behavior exhibited by the child. Sexual behavior
statement that their child is not to their knowledge a victim of is influenced by the family context and the child's experience,
sexual abuse and that suspicions of abuse must be reported to normative or traumatic, with sexuality.
authorities under law. The family context is central to an understanding of sexual
In contrast, some investigators may want to study child vic- behavior in children. Mrazek and Mrazek (1981) discuss a con-
tims' use of AD dolls (e.g., to investigate how abused children tinuum of acceptable sexuality in families, ranging between the
react to AD dolls). Again, the researcher is faced with the di- extremes of complete permissiveness on the one hand and com-
lemma of knowing with certainty a child's abuse history. The plete repression on the other. Families establish a "psychosexual
researcher interested in child victims' AD doll use may also face equilibrium" derived from (a) the parent's sexual adjustment,
a perplexing situation in determining who should provide con- (b) the child's developing sexuality, (c) the impact of the child's
sent for a child's participation in research. If the child is in state sexual development on parental sexual adjustment, and (d) the
custody, must the child welfare authorities provide consent, or interaction triggered as the child's sexual development causes
is it sufficient to obtain consent from a foster parent? Should the parents to reexperience memories and feelings related to their
children's actual parents be contacted, even if they may have own sexual development.
been involved in the abuse? Such decisions are typically negoti- Psychosexual development begins at birth, although there are
ated between researchers and institutional review boards, uni- few major changes in physical sexual development prior to pu-
versity attorneys, and other various agencies involved. berty. Rutter (1971) reported that during the 2- to 5-year-old
Methodological and ethical concerns also affect the types of period, both sexes show increased genital interest and genital
situations studied. There are increasing demands for research play. He also reported findings of sexual play or genital handling
to be ecologically valid in relation to the context of child sexual- by male and female preschoolers, with male children exhibiting
abuse investigations. For example, studies of children's mem- significantly more masturbatory activity. Interestingly, sexual
ory and suggestibility have often involved artificial laboratory anatomy content responses to the Rorschach inkblots are most
situations in which children are asked to recall words or pic- frequent in childhood among 5- to 7-year-olds (Ames, Learned,
tures, thus limiting ecological validity (Goodman, Rudy, Bot- Metraux, & Walker, 1974), perhaps reflecting the increased de-
toms, & Aman, 1990). However, ethical limitations clearly exist velopmental curiosity noted by Rutter.
with respect to the ecological validity of AD-doll research. For Other research suggests that some occasional sexual behavior
example, investigators cannot contrive a totally realistic foren- directed at adults is common in young children. A recent study
sic atmosphere in which parents believe their children have based on interviews with parents of 576 children, aged 2 to 10
been abused. They cannot randomly assign children to abused years, found that a child's touching a parent's genitals was "not
and nonabused groups. Researchers must often walk a tightrope uncommon on an incidental basis" (Rosenfeld, Bailey, Siegel,
in trying to ensure that their studies are ecologically valid and & Bailey, 1986). Rosenfeld and colleagues undertook the data
that they respect participants' welfare and rights. There are dan- collection to provide a basis for testimony in a case of alleged
206 KOOCHER ET AL.

sexual abuse and concluded that such touching could be a re- lows that children might also display such behaviors with AD
sponse to normal curiosity in a child. dolls. The studies provide no evidence that children 7 years old
In a random sample of 880 children aged 2 to 12 years, or younger usually know about such adult sexual acts as inter-
screened for the absence of sexual abuse, touching a mother's course, oral sex, or anal sex, which might make demonstrations
breasts at least once in the previous 6 months was reported for of such actions with AD dolls more suspect. However, it is un-
31 % of the children, ranging from a high of 48% and 44% of the clear when children start hiding their knowledge from adults.
2- to 6-year-old girls and boys, respectively, to a low of 9% and Furthermore, studies of children's interactions with AD dolls,
12% of the 7- to 12-year-old girls and boys, respectively reviewed later, indicate that some groups of nonreferred chil-
(Friedrich, Grambach, Broughton, Kuiper, & Beilke, 1991). dren may know more than the previous studies would suggest.
These findings are even more interesting given other data from The studies previously mentioned indicate that a continuum
the same study that established a positive link between sexual exists of acceptable sexuality in families and of sexual behavior
behavior in the home (e.g., family nudity, available pornogra- and knowledge in children. Sexual abuse would be expected to
phy, opportunities to witness intercourse, etc.) and the child's affect the continuum point on which a child's sexual behavior
overall level of sexual behavior as measured by a sexual behav- and knowledge falls. Sexual abuse is an event or series of events
ior rating scale completed by parents. with psychological effects that vary along several dimensions or
Sexual games are a relatively common occurrence in child- factors (for reviews, see Browne & Finkelhor, 1986; Kendall-
hood. Lamb and Coakley (1993) studied female college stu- Tackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, 1992), including traumatic
dents' recollections of childhood sexual play. A total of 85% of sexualization, which involves the stress inherent in a precocious
the women studied remembered a childhood sexual game. The introduction to sexual behavior. Finkelhor and Browne (1985)
data also suggest a broad range of behavior, with six of the nine identified several psychological sequelae of traumatic sexualiza-
categories of play described as normal. These games included tion (e.g., confusion of sex with love, increased salience of sex-
"playing doctor," exposure, experiments in stimulation (usually ual issues, and behavioral manifestations such as sexual preoc-
of the genitals), kissing, and fantasy sexual play. Normal play cupation, sexual aggression, and the inappropriate sexualiza-
included slight persuasion or coercion. Gordon and her col- tion of parenting). In children, the sexualized behaviors that
leagues (1990a, 1990b) studied another aspect of sexual behav- can follow traumatic sexualization include excessive masturba-
ior (i.e., sexual knowledge) that is also critical for understand- tion or preoccupation with one's own or others' genitalia; devel-
ing normative sexuality, especially because clinicians have opmentally inappropriate sexual behavior with peers, younger
contended that precocious sexual knowledge may be a clinical children, or animals; sexual promiscuity; and prostitution. In
marker for sexual abuse. Gordon's research with 130 children the most current and comprehensive review of the effects of
between the ages of 2 and 7 years revealed significant age and child sexual abuse, Kendall-Tackett et al. concluded that sexu-
social class differences. A number of areas of sexual knowledge alized behavior was reported more often in sexually abused
were studied (i.e., gender, sexual and nonsexual body parts and than in nonsexually abused children, even when comparisons
functions, sexual behavior, pregnancy, and abuse prevention). involved nonabused but clinical samples of children. Posttrau-
Although none of the children demonstrated much knowledge matic stress disorder-like behavior was the only other behavior
of adult sexual behavior, younger children generally knew less to share this distinction. Although these behaviors should not
than older ones. Lower socioeconomic status (SES) children be used by themselves as confirmation of sexual abuse, they are
had less knowledge than children in middle and upper SES important clinical indicators.
groups with respect to sexual body parts, pregnancy, and abuse Consistent with Browne and Finkelhor's (1986) and Kendall-
prevention. This may have been because of more restrictive at- Tackett et al.'s (1992) conclusions, Gil and Johnson (1993) re-
titudes toward sexuality evident among lower SES parents. ported a study that was based on evaluations of children re-
Sexual knowledge includes knowing names for genitalia. ferred because of their sexual behavior. The children were
Many families teach children idiosyncratic names for body grouped into four categories: normal sexual exploration, sexu-
parts. For example, Fraley and her colleagues (1991) studied ally reactive, extensive mutual sexual behaviors, and molesting
genital naming among mothers of nonreferredchildren (that is, behaviors. The large majority of children in the latter three
children with no known history of sexual abuse) under age 4 groups had been sexually abused or exposed to high degrees of
and found that only 30% of boys and 21% of girls had been family sexuality or violence, or both. Particularly relevant to
taught to use the words penis and vagina, respectively. Fully one the AD-doll controversy, Browne and Finkelhor contended that
third of the girls and nearly a fifth of the boys they studied had inappropriate sexualized, aggressive play is the most reliable be-
been given no name for their genitals. A study that used AD havioral consequence of childhood sexual abuse. Presumably
dolls to evaluate children's knowledge of sexual body parts re- such play could be directed at dolls as well as peers and adults.
ported that among 2- to 6-year-old nonreferred children, names Does child sexual abuse result in increased sexual knowl-
for the breast, buttocks, and penis were reported more accu- edge? Although an increase might be expected, one of the few
rately than for anus or scrotum (Schor & Sivan, 1989). Clearly, studies to examine this issue did not uncover a significant
some objective basis for clarifying meaning would be needed in difference in referred and nonreferred children's sexual knowl-
interviewing such children no matter how accurate their edge (Gordon, Schroeder, & Abrams, 1990b). However, chil-
memories. dren who had experienced sexual abuse were more emotionally
Overall, these findings suggest that, in normative samples, reactive than nonabused children to pictures depicting ambig-
children will engage in exploration of genitals (e.g., playing uous sexual acts.
doctor) and certain sexually related acts (e.g., kissing). It fol- These studies are not without their faults. For example, em-
ANATOMICAL DOLLS 207

pirical literature regarding increased sexual behavior among props? This is a critical question because if nonabused children
sexually abused children has relied primarily on parent reports show evidence of sexual behaviors with AD dolls, such behav-
(e.g., Friedrich et al., 1992; White, Halprin, Strom, & Santilli, iors could be misinterpreted as evidence of sexual abuse. In an
1988). However, the empirical assessment of sexual behavior earlier article, Gabriel (1985) described observations of 16
through direct observation of behavior in interview or play set- nonreferred children, 2 to 5 years old, in free-play interactions
tings that include AD dolls is an even more controversial topic with AD dolls. Gabriel noted that many of the children showed
and is the focus of the next discussion. behaviors with AD dolls that might be considered "suggestive"
of abuse (e.g., touching and commenting on the AD dolls' geni-
tals and inserting fingers into the AD dolls' orifices); however,
Normative Behavior With AD Dolls
none of his examples are of children clearly depicting sexual
Research examining children's reactions to AD dolls has fo- acts (e.g., intercourse). Some of the ambiguous behavior evi-
cused on a variety of sexually related and emotional behaviors denced by the children could conceivably be misinterpreted as
exhibited by children when interacting with these special props signs of abuse (e.g., a 2.5-year-old child who said "choo-choo-
(e.g., the touching of AD dolls' genitals, digital penetration, in- ing" when asked what a penis is for and a girl who put a boy AD
tercourse, and emotional reactions such as avoidance of the AD doll under her skirt). The author concluded that such behavior,
dolls). Table 1 presents an outline of these studies. In some stud- combined with the "thought processes" of the young child,
ies, relatively large samples of nonabused children were ob- made evidence collected from interviews with AD dolls highly
served under a number of different conditions, and their behav- suspicious. However, for the study the observer "made a number
iors were categorized for the presence or absence of explicit sex- of inquiries" about the AD dolls, but the inquiries were unspec-
ual activity. In other studies, the frequencies of various ified. This same observer coded the children's responses, but
interactions with AD dolls including sexual behaviors were it is unclear whether the observer was blind to the hypotheses.
compared in relatively small samples of suspected or confirmed Operationally defined coding categories were not presented in
sexual abuse victims and in matched control groups of nonvic- the article (although examples are given), and reliability of cod-
tims (i.e., children with no known history of sexual abuse). ing is not indicated. Furthermore, Gabriel's conclusion con-
tains an untested assumption that some of the children's behav-
iors would be misinterpreted as signs of abuse. Thus, although
Nonreferred Samples
the study provides interesting descriptive data and was the first
How do nonabused children react to AD dolls? That is, what of its kind, methodological problems limit the conclusions that
is the nature of children's normative behavior with these special can be reasonably drawn from it.

Table 1
Summary of Research Samples and Activities for Studies of Nonreferred and Referred
Children's Interactions With Anatomically Detailed Dolls
Gender
Nonaousea Abused Type of
Study (N) (N) Age F M observation

Gabriel (1985) 16 2-5 8 8 Free play


Interview
White etal. (1986) 25 25 2-6 29 21 Interview
Jampole&Weber(1987) 10 10 3-8 16 4 Free play
Interview
Si van etal. (1988) 144 3-8 72 72 Free play
— Interview
August & Format! (1989) 16 16 5-8 32 Directed play
Story telling
Glaser& Collins (1989) 86 — 2-6 46 40 Free play with and
without peers
Interview in small
groups
Everson& Boat (1990) 209 — 2-5 103 106 Interview
Free play
Realmuto etal. (1990) 9 6 4-8 13 2 Interview
Cohn(1991) 35 35 2-6 46 24 Free play
Interview
Kenyon-Jump et al. ( 1 99 1 ) 9 9 3-5 10 8 Free play
Rudy (1991) 10 10 4-6 20 0 Interview
Story telling
Free play
Dawson etal. (1992) 20 3-8 10 10 Interview

Free play
Note. F = female; M = male.
208 KOOCHER ET AL.

Sivan, Schor, Koeppl, and Noble (1988) examined the behav- child's parents were appraised of this behavior, the father ad-
iors of a relatively large sample (N= 144) of nonreferred chil- mitted to having found his daughter examining his porno-
dren with AD dolls in three within-subject conditions: (a) with graphic literature. (The family was referred to local social ser-
an adult present, (b) without an adult present, and (c) without vices.) These examples suggest that, for at least some children,
an adult present but with the AD dolls having been undressed AD dolls may elicit reenactment of sexual or aggressive experi-
during an interview in which body-part names had been re- ences and observations.
viewed. In general, the 3- to 8-year-old children found the AD Glaser and Collins (1989) concluded that although AD dolls
dolls no more attractive than other toys provided; no instances cannot be regarded as a "clinically reliable screening test for
of role-playing explicit sexual behavior and very few instances sexual abuse" (p. 559), it was unlikely that explicit sexual play
of aggressive behavior with the AD dolls were observed. The with AD dolls arises from innate factors or that the dolls lead the
presence of a female as opposed to a male interviewer increased children to sexual play. Thus, they suggested that interviewers
the likelihood that the children would interact with the dolls. further explore the sources of learning that contribute to a given
Undressing the dolls also increased the likelihood of interaction, child's demonstrating explicit sexual acts.
particularly of dressing the dolls, perhaps suggesting that the Similarly, Everson and Boat (1990; Boat & Everson, 1994a)
nonreferred children were less comfortable when the dolls were examined the behaviors of 209 nonreferred children, ages 2 to
undressed. However, Sivan et al. did not provide details about 5 years old, who were screened for possible sexual abuse under
such activities as touching the dolls' genitals or about other am- free-play and directed-play conditions. The latter included the
biguous behavior that could conceivably be mistaken by some prompt "show me what the dolls can do together." The authors
for signs of sexual abuse. In addition, the sample group con- found that 6% (12 children) "demonstrated behavior clearly de-
sisted of middle-class children only and, as acknowledged by picting sexual intercourse" (Everson & Boat, 1990, p. 741)
Sivan et al., the conditions of the study did not mimic those of across free- and directed-play conditions. None of the 2-year-
a child-abuse investigation. Nevertheless, the Sivan et al. study olds demonstrated clear intercourse positioning. However, 18%
provided support for the claim that AD dolls in and of them- of the 5-year-olds showed suggestive intercourse positioning
selves do not lead to sexualized activity in nonreferred children. when the interviewer was present, and 12% of the 5-year-olds
Glaser and Collins (1989) observed a sample of 86 nonre- showed clear intercourse positioning when alone. Notably, as
ferred preschool children in a play situation that included fo- age increased, there was a greater likelihood that the child when
cusing the children on the novelty of the AD dolls and inviting left alone would demonstrate intercourse positioning. Genital
the children to play with the dolls during several intervals of free intercourse positioning was more likely to occur than either oral
play, undressing the dolls, naming body parts and their func- or anal intercourse positioning. African American children
tions, and redressing the dolls. Although the two scorers were with low SES backgrounds were more likely than both their
not naive to the hypotheses, independent coding by two scorers middle-class African American peers and Caucasian peers to
of the children's behavior, including touching of genitalia and demonstrate clear intercourse positioning. Male children were
so forth, was obtained. Results indicated that three fourths of equally likely to demonstrate clear intercourse positioning
the 2- to 6-year-old children spontaneously undressed the AD whether the interviewer was present or not, whereas female chil-
dolls, and 71 % eventually touched the male doll's penis, whereas dren were only seen to do so when left alone with the AD dolls.
only 4% touched the vaginal opening. There was considerable Boat and Everson (1994a) concluded that there was little sup-
variation in the children's responses to the genitalia (e.g., port for the idea that exposure to the AD dolls induced sexually
matter-of-fact noticing, avoidance, and excitement). None of naive children to fantasize about sex or to act in sexually explicit
the children inserted their fingers into the vaginal or anal ways. However, they did suggest that AD dolls "do provide sex-
opening. ually knowledgeable children with at least implicit permission
In total, five children's play actions with the AD dolls showed and perhaps, encouragement to reveal their knowledge of sexu-
sexualized qualities. Specifically, two of the 3-year-old female ality in play" (Everson & Boat, 1990, p. 741). These authors,
participants demonstrated an intercourse position (but not in- like Glaser and Collins (1989), were able to identify "relatively
tercourse itself). One of these children also demonstrated hit- benign" sources for the sexual behaviors demonstrated (e.g.,
ting the adult female doll around the breasts. It was learned later exposure to pornography and observation of sexual behavior
that this child had been made to watch pornography at home, among teenage relatives). In addition, Boat and Everson ob-
that her name appeared on a child-abuse registry, and that she served that explicit sexual play varied with the child's age and
had witnessed domestic violence against her mother. A third girl family characteristics, leading the authors to suggest that there
joined the adult dolls' genitalia and demonstrated kissing. The may be a need for differential norms to interpret AD-doll play
source of this 4-year-old's sexual knowledge remained un- of children from different backgrounds (Everson & Boat, 1990,
known. A fourth girl, also 4 years of age, demonstrated "rocking p. 742).
in intercourse motion" (Glaser & Collins, 1989) with her own Boat and Everson (1989) conducted follow-up evaluations
body when squeezing the AD doll. Glaser and Collins later dis- with 10 demonstrators, 10 avoiders, and 20 controls from the
covered that the child had observed sexual activity between her original 209 individuals. Sixteen months later, all three groups
brother and her friend. Finally, the most troubling case was a 4- still exhibited similar behaviors. None of the children were
year-old girl who, when asked about the function of the penis, avoidant of the AD dolls; about 25% of the controls and 20% of
proceeded to suck it. She later commented, regarding penises, the demonstrators showed explicit sexual positioning. None of
that they smell and "they say open your mouth." When the the avoiders showed the dolls in sexualized positions. Questions
ANATOMICAL DOLLS 209

were also asked to ascertain the sources of the children's knowl- of 25 nonreferred children of the same ages. Using a 5-point
edge of genital intercourse. Avoiders had less explicit sexual scale with indicators ranging from no suspicion to very high sus-
knowledge than did the other groups, an observation confirmed picion, the authors reported that the nonreferred group had a
by their parents, who reported never having given their children significantly lower rating of suspected abuse as indicated by sex-
descriptions of sexual intercourse. ually explicit behaviors demonstrated with AD dolls. The 3-
Finally, Dawson, Vaughan, and Wagner (1992) examined 20 year-olds were the most reactive to sexual features on the dolls.
nonreferred, middle-class Caucasian children's interactions Jampole and Weber (1987), with a small sample of 10 re-
with AD dolls. The children named body parts on the AD dolls, ferred and 10 nonreferred children between the ages of 3 and 8
demonstrated with dressed and then undressed AD dolls such years, introduced the children to the AD dolls and asked them
activities as what the child and the child's father do alone to- to name body parts. The children were then left to play with the
gether, and engaged in free play. There were no instances of chil- dolls for 15 min without an adult present; after the 15 min, the
dren reenacting sexual intercourse, oral sex, or fondling. Low researcher returned to the playroom, and the child was allowed
levels of sexual aggression (e.g., hitting the dolls' genitals) were to continue playing for 1 hr from the beginning of the session.
observed; high levels of sexual exploratory play and nonsexual Ninety percent of sexually abused children demonstrated sex-
aggression were reported, especially when the AD dolls were ual behaviors with the AD dolls, whereas only 20% of the non-
undressed. This small sample study again shows low levels of referred children (N = 2) showed these behaviors. Increased
explicit sexual demonstration with AD dolls in middle-class, sexual behavior with the dolls was noted when the researcher
Caucasian preschool children whose families agreed to partici- was out of the room. The sample in this study raises questions
pation in such research projects. of ascertainment bias because the nonabused children were ei-
In summary, normative studies of children's interactions ther the sons or daughters of social service agency employees or
with AD dolls have focused on spontaneous and directed play were in legal custody of social services for reasons other than
in nonreferred children, analyzing the children's behavior for sexual abuse. Not only was the sample of nonreferred children
evidence of sexualized or aggressive play. Virtually all of these unrepresentative of such children generally but also the sam-
studies found that many of the children inspected and touched pling raises questions about whether the nonreferred children
sexual body parts. However, play demonstrating explicit sexual in state custody who demonstrated sexual acts were in fact un-
activity such as intercourse or oral-genital contact was rare in detected victims of sexual abuse or had been prematurely ex-
these nonreferred samples. The exception was a study in which posed to sexual behavior in other ways.
the children were primed with the prompt "show me what these August and Forman (1989) conducted another comparative
dolls can do together." In the study, a group of low SES African- study of referred and nonreferred children. Two groups, each
American male 5-year-olds displayed clear sexual intercourse with 16 girls between the ages of 5 and 8 years, were presented
positioning at rates higher than their more socially advantaged with a set of AD dolls and an extra set of clothes. The children
peers. were asked to change the clothes on the dolls when the examiner
In drawing conclusions from extant research, one must keep left the room; on the examiner's return, the children were asked
in mind that most studies have not varied such background fac- to tell a story about the family of AD dolls. Results indicated
tors as SES and ethnicity, which Everson and Boat's (1990) re- that when left alone, the referred children played less with the
sults indicate may be significant influences on children's behav- dolls but were more aggressive and investigated the genital parts
ior. In future research, it will be important for investigators to more than the nonreferred children. The authors concluded
include a diverse, representative sample of nonabused children that children referred for evaluation of suspected sexual abuse
and specify clearly the refusal rate or to target certain groups of respond differently than nonreferred children to AD dolls, par-
children who are likely to become involved in child sexual- ticularly when left alone with the dolls. Furthermore, they sug-
abuse investigations. Studies of children's normative behavior gested that there may be a subgroup of sexually abused children
with AD dolls have not attempted to mimic the context created who respond with high levels of aggression and another sub-
by actual child-abuse investigations and have not systematically group that is avoidant of interaction with AD dolls. The small
varied the extent of children's exposure to AD dolls. Non- sample size, as well as the lack of delineation of the participants'
abused children's behavior with these dolls may be affected by characteristics, was a significant limitation to the conclusive
such factors. statements on the basis of this study, as was the use of paramet-
ric statistics in a study with category data.
Referred Versus Nonreferred Groups Rudy (1991) also compared referred and nonreferred chil-
dren's behavior with AD dolls in a study that included twenty
A related area of research focuses on comparing referred and 4- to 6-year-old girls matched in age, race, SES, and receptive
nonreferred children's behavior with AD dolls. The main goal vocabulary. Children's behavior with AD dolls was observed
of such research is to determine whether abused and nonabused during a three-phase session, consisting of AD-doll introduc-
children interact differently with AD dolls. White et al. (1986) tion and body-part naming, the child telling a story with the AD
conducted the first systematic study comparing the behavior of dolls, and the child being left alone with the AD dolls. Children's
children suspected of being sexually abused (referred) with the behavior with AD dolls was coded by raters naive to the chil-
behavior of children with no known history of sexual abuse dren's abuse status and to the study's hypotheses. Referred chil-
(nonreferred). The researchers used a structured AD-doll in- dren displayed significantly more sexualized behavior with AD
terview and compared the responses of twenty-five 2- to 6-year- dolls than did nonreferred children during the introductory and
old children referred for abuse evaluations with the responses body-part naming phase but not during the other two phases.
210 KOOCHER ET AL.

Moreover, referred children engaged in sexual behavior for (Realmuto & Wescoe, 1992) to conclude that discrepancies in
longer periods of time than did nonreferred children, and they training and experience may be contributing to the variance.
also engaged in more and longer aggressive behavior toward the The authors made several useful suggestions about the direction
AD dolls than did nonreferred children. The two groups of chil- of research on clinical decision making and determinations of
dren did not significantly differ in exploration of the AD dolls' child sexual abuse, but because of experimental constraints
sexual parts. Thus, despite the small sample size (10 children (e.g., no abuse-oriented questions and brief interviews), care
in each group) and despite the limited amount of time referred should be taken regarding generalized application of these
children spent engaging in sexualized and aggressive behaviors results.
(i.e., M - 3.5 and 1 s, respectively), significant differences in Two studies provide information regarding front-line clinical
sexual and aggressive behaviors were obtained. Rudy concluded use of AD dolls when suspicions of abuse arise. Although these
that referred children exhibit different behaviors with AD dolls studies do not technically compare referred with nonreferred
than do nonreferred children. children, they may have included both groups because abuse
Although significant differences in referred and nonreferred was not yet verified. Regardless, the studies provide important
children's behavior with AD dolls were detected in the previous information concerning the amount of information obtained
studies, such differences are not always found. For example, from children in sexual-abuse assessments conducted with and
Cohn (1991) compared the behaviors of 36 children referred without AD dolls. Leventhal, Hamilton, Rekedal, Tebano-
for suspected abuse with 36 nonreferred children matched on Micci, and Eyster (1989) examined records of interviews with
age and gender. No statistically significant differences were re- young children (under 7 years old) who had been seen for sex-
ported between the groups; however, similar to Everson and ual-abuse evaluations. The children had been interviewed first
Boat's (1990) results, trends in the data and demographic without and then with AD dolls. Unfortunately, the confound-
differences lead Cohn to suggest further studies of the linkage ing of order-of-interview format and AD-doll use makes it
between sexuality in the home and AD-doll interactions of difficult to distinguish AD-doll versus reinterviewing effects. In
young children. any case, without the dolls, 13% gave detailed descriptions of
Kenyon-Jump, Burnette, and Robertson (1991) compared the alleged abusive behavior. In contrast, 48% of these children
the behaviors of nine sexually abused preschoolers with nine reported details of the alleged abuse when interviewed with the
control children matched on age, gender, race, family status, dolls. In the no AD-doll condition, the alleged perpetrator was
and SES. Like Cohn (1991), Kenyon-Jump and colleagues named 32% of the time, whereas 67% of those in the AD-doll
found no statistically significant difference in explicit sexual be- condition named the alleged abuser. If the adult who accompa-
havior. Suspicious behaviors such as putting one's hands be- nied the child strongly suspected abuse, the child was more
tween one's legs when combined with explicit sexual behavior likely to provide a detailed description with AD dolls; this result
did yield a statistically significant difference between the could reflect either a suggestibility effect on the child's part or a
groups, despite the small sample size; this finding was not ex- valid suspicion of abuse and greater parental support on the
plained by the authors. adult's part (Lawson & Chaffin, 1992).
Finally, a lack of difference in AD-doll interaction was also Unfortunately, the authors did not present information about
noted between two small samples of emotionally disturbed pre- whether the adults' suspicions of abuse correlated with objec-
schoolers: one group of children who had been sexually abused tive indicators of abuse, such as physical findings; detailed de-
and another who had not (Allen, Jones, & Nash, 1989). Allen scription and demonstration of abuse were unrelated to the
et al. suggested that pathogenic family histories may mask the presence of physical evidence. Children were more likely to be
differences seen with samples not matched on degree of family rated as definitely abused when the AD-doll information was
disturbance. included in the assessment than when it was excluded, and of
An important question related to the one previously ad- the children considered to have been definitely abused, 38% did
dressed concerns professionals' ability to reliably categorize a not provide evidence of abuse except during the AD-doll in-
child as abused or nonabused on the basis of the child's behavior terview. These results led the investigators to conclude that the
with AD dolls. Realmuto and his colleagues (Realmuto, Jensen, use of the AD dolls gave the children the freedom to provide
& Wescoe, 1990; Realmuto & Wescoe, 1992) studied a small more details of their experiences. The authors did note, how-
group of 15 children under the age of 7 years, including 6 chil- ever, that children younger than 3 years old were generally un-
dren with a documented history of sexual abuse and 9 without able to provide detailed verbal descriptions about the alleged
a known history, with the purpose of examining the reliability abuse experience under either condition, although a number of
of professional decision making when using AD dolls as "a sole these young children demonstrated what had allegedly hap-
test of sexual abuse." The children were interviewed with a stan- pened when provided with AD dolls.
dardized protocol that avoided direct questioning about abuse However, in a study conducted by Britton and O'Keefe
incidents. Instead, Realmuto and colleagues analyzed the chil- (1991), 2- to 10-year-old children were interviewed with either
dren's spontaneous remarks and the behaviors elicited in 15- AD dolls or nondetailed dolls (e.g., regular Ken and Barbie or
min interactions with AD dolls. The interviewer was blind to Cabbage Patch). In general, there was no difference between the
the abuse status of the child and rated the child's interview ac- AD dolls and other dolls in eliciting information; two thirds of
cording to the criteria provided by White et al. (1986). Proper each group were able to give detailed accounts. Girls tended to
classification was obtained for 53% of the sample. When 14 pro- use AD dolls as props to report their allegations, whereas boys
fessionals rated the interview, there was little agreement in their were more likely to describe their experiences using nondetailed
determinations (K = .355), a finding that lead the authors dolls (66% vs. 44%).
ANATOMICAL DOLLS 211

In summary, although a number of studies have uncovered child victim-witness's testimony by asserting, for example, that
significant differences between referred and nonreferred chil- the child's memory is inaccurate or that the child was led or
dren's interactions with AD dolls, the findings are mixed. The coached into making a false allegation, is confused (e.g., con-
diversity of findings across studies of referred and nonreferred fused a sexual dream with reality), or is identifying the wrong
children's displays of sexual behaviors with AD dolls are not person (see Goodman, 1993). Moreover, although prosecutors
surprising. The studies differ widely in the size and composition report that AD dolls are useful in reducing children's trauma
of their samples and the methodology used. One would not ex- when testifying, they also report that such use is a source of
pect all sexually abused children to display increased sexual be- defense challenges.
havior with AD dolls. Likewise, one might expect that certain Although a number of legal issues are relevant to a general
groups of nonabused children will show evidence of more sexu- discussion of children's memory and suggestibility when AD
alized behavior than other nonabused groups. Interestingly, in a dolls are used in interviews, such discussion would lead too far
number of studies in which no difference was found, the data afield. The issues of leading questions and coercive interviewing
point to the possible importance of family circumstances or require mention, however. There is substantial concern that
psychological disturbance in nonabused samples as contribut- leading questions may be necessary to facilitate disclosure of
ing to a lack of difference between referred and nonreferred abuse by some children in formal legal settings (e.g., Keary &
children's display of sexual behavior with AD dolls. Fitzpatrick, 1994), but such leading questions may also result
The studies are also in general agreement in several respects. in false reports by nonabused children, especially when AD
First, clearly using AD dolls in evaluations does not inherently dolls are used (Raskin & Yuille, 1989). Thus, it is important
distress or overstimulate children. Second, using the dolls can to understand what constitutes a leading question from a legal
clearly assist in identifying children's preferred or idiosyncratic perspective. Although a precise definition remains elusive, ques-
names for body parts. Third, using AD dolls often results in tions that introduce or imply information not already offered
increased verbal productions during standardized research in- by a witness are typically considered leading. Under this nebu-
terviews. Regarding the important question of whether more or lous definition, if a child were asked "What happened?" (an
less accurate determinations of child sexual abuse are made open-ended question) and then "What else happened?" (which
when children are interviewed with AD dolls, the studies by might seem innocuous enough), the latter question can techni-
Leventhal et al. (1989) provide relevant information but not cally be defined as leading. A question that included specific
conclusive answers. The clear message is consistent with a key reference to a person and an act (e.g., "Did Uncle Henry touch
point in the statement adopted by the APA Council and pre- your bottom?") would be a relatively clear case of a leading
sented at the beginning of this article: "Such dolls may be useful
question. Because so many questions qualify as leading, in-
in helping children to communicate when their language skills
terviewers face a challenging task in questioning children. In
or emotional concerns preclude direct verbal responses. These
any case, when leading questions are combined with AD dolls
dolls may also be useful communication props" (American Psy-
in an interview, the child's credibility as a witness may be espe-
chological Association, 1991,p.722).
cially likely to come under forensic attack.
Legal and psychological debate also revolves around what
Memory and Suggestibility Concerns constitutes a coercive or contaminated interview versus a sup-
A central concern about the role of AD dolls in forensic in- portive or sufficiently probing interview (e.g., White & Quinn,
terviews is that children's memory may be contaminated and 1988). In actual cases, coercive-interviewing techniques have
their suggestibility increased when AD dolls are introduced to been used in conjunction with AD dolls (e.g., State of New Jer-
the interview context. What does research indicate about the sey v. Michaels, 1993). Many would agree that when interview-
impact of AD dolls on children's memory and suggestibility? ers pressure children (e.g., "Tell me about the time he kissed
As an important backdrop for answering this question, we first you and then you can go home") or badger children with overly
consider certain legal issues and interviewing practices that in- repeated questions about the abuse but not taking no for an
fluence the research questions asked and the procedures used. answer, such techniques add to the leading nature of an in-
We then discuss several critical issues concerning children's cog- terview (Quinn, White, & Santilli, 1989; White & Quinn,
nitive development, memory, and suggestibility as they relate 1988); how often such techniques are actually used with AD
to children's use of AD dolls. Finally, we review studies that dolls and how often they result in false reports of abuse is a
specifically investigated effects of AD dolls on children's mem- related but distinct issue, however. Ironically, the same tech-
ory reports and suggestibility. niques that some consider coercive and that may result in inac-
curacies from nonabused children might be successful in ob-
Legal Issues and Interviewing Considerations Relevant taining accurate and complete information from frightened,
to Children embarrassed, or guilt-ridden children (Goodman & Clarke-
Stewart, 1991). Research empirically validating what consti-
Concerns about children's memory and suggestibility play a tutes coercive versus sufficiently probing techniques concerning
prominent role in investigations and trials of child sexual abuse, sexual experiences is surprisingly sparse. Keep in mind, how-
whether AD dolls are involved or not. However, these concerns ever, that the relevant concern for research on AD dolls is
may be exacerbated when AD dolls are used. Prosecutors report whether AD dolls add to error rates above the use of leading
that when child sexual-abuse cases go to trial, defense attorneys questions and coercive-interviewing techniques. Ideally, appro-
"almost always" or "frequently" challenge the accuracy of a priate comparisons should be included within studies (e.g., ex-
212 KOOCHER ET AL.

amination of effects of leading questions with vs. without AD tional skills (DeLoache, Kolstad, & Anderson, 1991), develop-
dolls); unfortunately, they too seldom are. mental level may place important limits on children's ability to
Willingness, to use leading questions, AD dolls, and even co- recount an event using AD dolls. Fischer and Lazerson (1984)
ercive techniques in an interview may be influenced by concern describe a sequence of children's pretend play involving props
about how to balance risks of false reports versus risks of con- that may also apply to AD-doll use. According to this sequence,
tinued abuse. Professionals and laypersons differ in how they in the transition from sensorimotor to preoperational intelli-
weigh these two risks. Some appear more willing to err on the gence, children can act out familiar actions with their own body
side of child protection, whereas others appear more willing to (e.g., to use their own body to pretend to go to sleep). Once the
err on the side of protecting the rights of defendants (e.g., Bot- child is capable of mentally coordinating single representations
toms, 1993; Finlayson & Koocher, 1991). Members of the for- (often by about 2 years of age), the child can act out a single
mer group might be less opposed to using leading questions, a action and then later (by around 3 years of age) act out a series
certain degree 'of coaxing, and AD dolls, whereas members of of related actions with a prop. As the child's cognitive develop-
the latter group might be more troubled by using leading ques- ment advances further, the child of about 4 years of age is able
tions, pressuring, and AD dolls. to use a prop to relate one social category (e.g., mother or
The previous interviewing considerations affect profession- father) to another social category (e.g., daughter or son). It
als' use of AD dolls. For fear that a child's accuracy or credibil- would be of considerable interest to test whether this develop-
ity (or both) will be damaged, many interviewers try to .avoid mental sequence applies to children's use of AD dolls to report
leading questions and leading approaches to AD-doll use. Some experienced events from memory.
professionals avoid AD dolls altogether. The previous issues also Although studies of adults' use of AD dolls to reenact experi-
affect research on memory and suggestibility when children are enced events are lacking, note that task and context affect
confronted with AD dolls and the conclusions that different re- adults' memory performance as well (e.g., Chi, 1978; Loftus,
searchers are willing to reach. 1979; Tulving, 1983). Moreover, individual differences within
any age group can be striking; when recounting an event, some
Developmental Issues children are more accurate than others, and some children are
more accurate than some adults (Leippe, Manion, & Romanc-
The use of AD dolls with children is, at its core, a develop- zyk, 1993). Still, the optimal level of performance a young child
mental issue. Therefore, it is important to include a brief dis- can reach is unlikely to match that of an older child or adult
cussion of cognitive development and its interaction with mem- (Fischer, 1980). For example, older children and adults, on av-
ory and suggestibility as they relate to AD-doll use. erage, can be expected to reenact complex events with dolls and
Research confirms that complex interactions involving many props with greater completeness and accuracy than can younger
factors (e.g., developmental level, task, and context) influence children.
children's performance on cognitive tasks (e.g., Fischer & Bul- Memory. If young children were incapable of remembering
lock, 1984; Melton & Thompson, 1987). It follows that com- their experiences, using AD dolls to elicit memory reports
plex interactions also affect children's behavior with AD dolls. would be futile. However, even infants show evidence of prever-
Within this framework, memory, suggestibility, and AD-doll bal memory (e.g., Rovee-Collier, 1989), and 2- and 3-year-olds
use have multiple determinants, developmental level being an can verbalize memories for events that occurred at least 6
important but not the sole influence. Consistent with this view, months earlier (Fivush, Gray, & Fromhoff, 1987). Behavioral
when a task is simplified, when appropriate cues are available, indexes, such as interactions with toy props, reveal that 2-year-
and when the socioemotional context is supportive, young chil- olds have traces of nonverbal memories for experiences that oc-
dren typically provide more complete and accurate memory re- curred in infancy (Myers, Clifton, & Clarkson, 1987). However,
ports than when a task is complex requiring coordination of for young children (e.g., 2-year-olds), parental discussion of
multiple higher level representations, when appropriate cues events may be important for retention of specific experiences;
are unavailable, and when the socioemotional context is non- without such discussion, an enduring, consciously accessible
supportive (e.g., Donaldson, 1979; Fischer & Bullock, 1984; episodic memory of a specific experience in early childhood
Goodman, Bottoms, Schwartz-Kenney, & Rudy, 1991; Kail, may not form (Nelson, 1993).
1990; Price & Goodman, 1990). For example, memory perfor- When compared with older children and adults, preschool
mance may falter when inaccurate or irrelevant cues in the form children tend to recall significantly less information in free re-
of toy props are introduced (Pipe, Gee, & Wilson, 1993; Say- call and make more errors in answering questions (e.g., Good-
witz, Goodman, Nicholas, & Moan, 1991; i.e., in some situa- man'& Reed, 1986; Poole & Lindsay, 1994), including ques-
tions, AD dolls could constitute misleading or irrelevant cues), tions that deal with abuse-related information (e.g., being
when the language used in an interview is unfamiliar and ab- kissed and having one's bottom touched). Nevertheless, by the
stract (Brennan & Brennan, 1988; Carter, 1991; Perry et al., age of about 4 years, children can show surprising accuracy in
1993), and when the emotional context is intimidating answering questions about abuse-related actions (e.g., Rudy &
(Goodman et al., 1991; Hill & Hill, 1987; Saywitz & Na- Goodman, 1991; Tobey & Goodman, 1992), although accu-
thanson, in press). racy is not guaranteed (e.g., Lepore & Sesco, 1994). As studies
Some very young children may not yet have the representa- reviewed later in this article indicate, to date the presence versus
tional skills to appreciate the full symbolic nature of props, such absence of AD dolls has not been proven to significantly in-
as AD dolls (DeLoache, 1990, in press). Although task diffi- crease children's commission errors to such questions.
culty can affect a child's ability to activate emerging representa- Some of children's memory deficits reflect a limited knowl-
ANATOMICAL DOLLS 213

edge base compared with that of adults (Chi, 1978). Children Schwartz-Kenney & Goodman, 1991), delay (Goodman,
who have experienced sexual abuse would be expected to have Hirschman, et al., 1991; Loftus, 1979), participation in an
relevant knowledge of the experience, a knowledge that might event (Baker-Ward, Hess, & Flannagan, 1990; Lindberg, McCo-
be reflected in their AD-doll reenactment. Although repeated mas, Jones, & Thomas, 1993; Rudy & Goodman, 1991; Tobey
experience with an event generally strengthens memory, it can & Goodman, 1992), expectations and discrepancy (Baker-
also result in confusions across similar instances, especially for Ward, Gordon, Ornstein, Larus, & Clubb, 1993; List, 1986;
young children (e.g., Farrar & Goodman, 1992), in part be- Pezdek, 1994), repeated interviewing (Leichtman & Ceci,
cause their semantic knowledge tends to intrude into their epi- 1995; Warren, Hagood, & Snider, 1993), and rehearsal
sodic reports of a specific experience (Mandler, 1984). As a re- (Brainerd & Ornstein, 1991). Moreover, although memory
sult, in some situations a young child might reenact general strength may be linked to suggestibility, it is as yet unclear
knowledge with an AD doll rather than a specific experience. whether it is a singularly determining factor or whether social
When semantic-based AD-doll reenactment reflects age-inap- factors (e.g., demand characteristics; Zaragoza, 1991) or per-
propriate sexual knowledge, suspicion of premature exposure sonality factors (e.g., attachment status and temperament;
to sexuality or sexual abuse might reasonably be raised. Alter- Goodman, Batterman-Faunce, Quas, Riddlesberger, & Kuhn,
natively, when reenactment is based on inaccurate stereotypes 1993; Gordon et al., 1993) are even more controlling. These
or inaccurate interpretations of events, errors may result (e.g., same factors can be expected to affect children's suggestibility
Clarke-Stewart, Thompson, & Lepore, 1989; Leichtman & in response to questions asked in interviews that contain AD
Ceci, 1995). dolls.
Although young children's memories may be more fragile In actual cases, forensic interviewers have at times combined
and fade more quickly than adults' memories (Brainerd, Re- repeated abuse suggestions, an accusatory context, and biased
nya, Howe, & Kingma, 1991; Flin, Boon, Knox, & Bull, 1992; interviewing with the use of AD dolls (e.g., State of New Jersey
Howe, 1991), traumatic and stressful events can be retained v. Michaels, 1993), a combination that could enhance the
over significant periods of time by young children (e.g., Good- chances of obtaining a false report of sexual abuse. There is
man, Hirschman, Hepps, & Rudy, 1991; Steward, 1993; Terr, growing research that when an interviewer repeatedly commu-
1988), and it is well documented that central features of trau- nicates accusation or intimidation, increased memory distor-
matic events may be reenacted by children in their doll play tion and suggestibility can result, at least in young children
(e.g., Terr, 1988). Thus, even if a young child cannot be brought (Clark-Stewart et al., 1989; Goodman, Bottoms, et al., 1991;
to articulate a traumatic memory in words or has difficulty us- Leichtman & Ceci, 1995; Tobey & Goodman, 1992). However,
ing AD dolls to reenact an event in response to an interviewer's the influence of intimidation, accusatory context, and repeti-
command, a child's posttraumatic play with AD dolls may re- tion of misleading questions has not been put to scientific test
flect a child's experiences. specifically in relation to interviews that include AD dolls.
For both neutral and traumatic events, because children typ- Moreover, it is unknown whether similar interviewing tech-
ically retain more information than they report in free recall niques are successful, or even necessary, in obtaining disclo-
(Kail, 1990), scientists and professionals who work with chil- sures of actual abuse from frightened or embarrassed children
dren have long relied on specific questioning and use of props, and how best to balance the risk of obtaining a false report with
such as dolls, to help elicit more complete memory reports from the need to detect actual abuse.
children. Children's limited memory-retrieval abilities may lag Distinguishing fantasy from reality. Children's accuracy of
behind their abilities to reenact simple events (Piaget & In- reports when interviewed with AD dolls is not related to mem-
helder, 1973; Price & Goodman, 1990). Although props at ory alone; it may also be influenced by children's ability to dis-
times provide communication and memory interference (e.g., tinguish fantasy (i.e., play, imagination, and cognition) from
distraction and misleading cues), a number of studies also indi- reality. Such influences are of particular concern when children
cate facilitative effects of props on memory (Pipe et al., 1993; are interviewed with AD dolls because of the fear that toys such
Price & Goodman, 1990). as dolls may be associated in children's minds with fantasy play.
Suggestibility. Of particular concern in the AD-doll debate Differentiation between fantasy and reality has a developmental
is children's suggestibility. Young children, on average, tend to course, but children nevertheless evidence a basic understand-
be more suggestible than older children or adults (for reviews, ing of "pretend" at young ages (DiLalla & Watson, 1988; Well-
see Ceci & Bruck, 1993; Saywitz & Goodman, in press). To the man & Estes, 1986). Research on "reality monitoring" also in-
extent that children's compared to adults' memories possess dicates that younger children may have more difficulty than
less memory strength, there is an increased possibility of sug- older children differentiating their own thoughts from reality
gestibility and incorporation of misleading postevent informa- (e.g., Johnson & Foley, 1984), but literature focuses chiefly on
tion (Loftus, 1979; Schwartz-Kenney & Goodman, 1991). mundane events rather than those of personal significance to
However, when forgetting rates are controlled, age differences in children. To the extent that children might confuse fantasy with
some forms of suggestibility may disappear (Howe, 1991). reality, and to the extent that certain props such as AD dolls
Thus, for salient features of an event to which children attend, might promote such confusion, caution in interpreting chil-
consider important, and thus encode well, children may at times dren's reports from AD-doll interviews is warranted. However,
be no more susceptible to forgetting, memory impairment, and there is little solid research evidence indicating that young chil-
suggestibility effects than adults. A number of factors influence dren have spontaneous sexual fantasies (e.g., of copulation) that
memory strength and resistance to suggestion, such as the cen- they then confuse with reality, which is an issue of greater fo-
trality and salience of event information (e.g., Loftus, 1979; rensic concern in many situations in which AD dolls are used.
214 KOOCHER ET AL.

Communication and language. Use of age-inappropriate under a variety of interviewing contexts, young children—espe-
language by an interviewer can lead to confusion and increased cially preschoolers—can be expected to provide less complete
error (Carter, 1991; Goodman & Aman, 1991; Perry et al., reports, to make more errors, and to be more suggestible on
1993), which could affect an interview that includes AD dolls. average compared with older children and adults. Age differ-
Moreover, some young children provide incoherent narratives ences may be exacerbated by language and communication
or recall a different event than the one in question, without com- difficulties, intimidation, and an accusatory context. Children's
municating the difference to the interviewer. Young children cognitive limitations may at times adversely affect interviews
will often expect adults to know and understand what they that include AD dolls; however, such deficits also underlie why
mean when communicating. Such children, usually under the AD dolls can, at other times, lead to more complete and accu-
age of 7 years, may not sense or comprehend incompleteness or rate reports from children.
ambiguity of their communication and may not indicate that
they do not understand an interviewer's question (Saywitz & Studies Concerning Memory and Suggestibility
Snyder, 1993). Confusions resulting from such language and
communication problems in interviews that include AD dolls Only a handful of studies have directly investigated the effects
may enhance errors of false reports as well as false denial. In of AD dolls on nonabused children's memory and suggestibility.
some studies, including ones that involved AD dolls, language In one of the first studies to explore this issue (Goodman &
was implicated as one of the main causes of children's errors in Aman, 1991; Goodman et al., 1990), eighty 3- and 5-year-old
answering questions about an experienced event (e.g., Good- middle-class children participated individually in games such
man & Aman, 1991; Saywitz etal., 1991). as Simon Says with a male confederate. A week later, children
Recounting sexually related events. One rationale for AD- were individually interviewed in one of four conditions: (a) no
doll use is that children may be hesitant to discuss sexual dolls (AD or regular) present, (b) AD dolls in view but out of
matters outright. Most studies of children's cognitive and mem- reach, (c) regular dolls present for acting out the event, and (d)
ory development have not focused on sexually related informa- AD dolls present for acting out the event. During the interview,
tion, and thus it is unclear how the findings of such studies apply children were first asked to describe the earlier play session us-
to children's disclosure of sexual abuse. Although most young ing free recall; those children in the two doll reenactment con-
children have quite limited sexual knowledge (Gordon, ditions were asked to demonstrate as well as tell what happened
Schroeder, & Abrams, 1990a, 1990b; but see Boat & Everson, as part of their free recall. All children were then asked a set of
1988a; Everson & Boat, 1990), with development, children gain specific and misleading questions, including some questions
awareness that sexually related activity is taboo, not generally that might be of particular interest in abuse investigations such
talked about, embarrassing, and best kept a secret (Goldman & as "Did he touch your private parts?" and "Did he spank you
Goldman, 1982). Older children's reports of genital touch in like this?" (with the researcher demonstrating the act with the
particular may thus be inhibited relative to their reporting of AD doll).
other types of information. In support of this assertion, Saywitz When verbal responses in free recall were considered, a sig-
et al. (1991) found that 7-year-olds were less likely than 5-year- nificant age difference but not a significant doll-condition
olds to report genital touch. Again consistent with the idea that difference emerged. When behavioral responses in the two doll
older children may be inhibited when reporting sexually related conditions were contrasted, again only a significant age effect
information, ^9- to 10-year-olds in Edwards and Forman's was uncovered. However, further analyses indicated that the
(1989) study tended to avoid AD-doll reenactment of informa- amount of accurate information provided by older children in
tion concerning intrafamilial sexual abuse. Although older chil- free recall was increased by reenactment with regular and AD
dren's advanced cognitive skills often make them better able dolls compared with the no-doll conditions, whereas the
than younger children to recount events completely and accu- amount of accurate information provided by younger children
rately, beliefs and feelings about the appropriateness of sexual was increased primarily by regular rather than AD dolls (see
activity and about discussion of sexual matters may inhibit chil- Goodman et al., 1990). Importantly, there were no significant
dren's reports of sexual abuse. AD dolls might make it easier differences in the amount of incorrect recall as a function of
for such children to report sexual events (e.g., by permitting doll condition. In response to the specific and misleading ques-
them to reenact rather than verbalize events or by providing tioning, although few 3-year-olds made commission errors to
social cues that discussion of genitally related information is abuse-related questions, they did so more often than 5-year-olds
acceptable). (e.g., 3-year-olds were more likely to falsely affirm the question
Summary. Children do show cognitive and memory deficits "He took your clothes off, didn't he?"). Commission errors
compared with adults, and these deficits are likely to affect chil- were particularly likely when the children did not understand
dren's performance in interviews that include AD dolls. Some the terms used (e.g., "private parts"). In any case, this error
of these deficits are due to limits in cognitive and language de- rate did not significantly differ as a function of doll condition.
velopment; others may be due to a more limited knowledge base Goodman and Aman (1991) observed that some of the 3-
concerning a particular domain. However, factors related to task year-olds did not seem to understand that the dolls were sup-
complexity, cues, and socioemotional context can potentially posed to be used to symbolically represent themselves. For ex-
reduce age differences in memory. Age differences in children's ample, when asked to act out the event with AD dolls or regular
willingness to report sexually related information may also dolls, some young children made statements such as "but I'm
affect typical developmental patterns, with older children not not a doll." DeLoache and Marzolf (1994) explored this finding
necessarily providing more complete and accurate reports. Still, further in a study of 2.5- to 4-year-olds' use of AD dolls (see also
ANATOMICAL DOLLS 215

DeLoache, in press). Seventy-two middle-class children in- After either 1 week or 1 month, the children were interviewed
teracted with a male confederate by engaging in play activities, about the checkup; the interview involved free recall, AD-doll
some of which involved touching and placing stickers on the reenactment, and directive questioning. The directive question-
child. In an immediate interview, children were asked to place ing included two AD doll-aided leading questions about the
stickers on the AD doll in the same place where the stickers had checkup that were of special interest. Specifically, the in-
been placed on them. The children were then asked to answer terviewer held up a naked AD doll and asked "Did the doctor
questions about where they were touched (e.g., "Where did Don touch you here?" (pointing to the AD doll's vaginal area) and
touch you?") and then to indicate these places on the AD doll "Did the doctor touch you here?" (pointing to the AD doll's
(e.g., "Show me on the doll where Don touched you"). anal area). Note that the comparisons across interview format
Four-year-old children were very accurate in sticker place- (e.g., free recall and AD doll) were within subject. There was
ment on the AD doll (92% correct), but 2.5-year-olds were not not a "no AD doll" group, and therefore no leading question
(41% correct). Many 2.5-year-olds did not constrain sticker effects comparisons were possible.
placement by their own experiences and instead placed stickers Of most concern to this article is the children's reports of
incorrectly on the AD doll. Importantly, response accuracy to genital touch. To the free-recall question "What happened last
questions about touch was higher when children were asked to time you went to the doctor's office?", only 8 of the 36 children
describe where they had been touched than when they had to who had the vaginal and anal examination reported vaginal
show the same thing on the AD doll, a pattern that was espe- touch, and only 4 of the children reported anal touch. When
cially true for 2.5-year-olds. Interestingly, when asked mislead- asked to demonstrate what happened using AD dolls, only 6 of
ing questions about where they were touched, both 2.5- and 4- the 36 children reported vaginal touch, and only 4 reported anal
year-olds were more accurate in their verbal responses than 3- touch. It was only when asked the specific (leading) questions
year-olds. These findings indicate that some young children may during which the AD doll was used that most of the children
not understand the dolls' symbolic function in terms of serving reported genital contact; at that point, 31 children affirmed the
as a model to indicate an experienced event (see also Gordon et vaginal touch, and 25 children affirmed the anal touch.
al., 1993). However, because the AD dolls remained clothed Of the 36 children who did not have vaginal or anal touch as
throughout the DeLoache and Marzolf (1994) study, their ex- part of the checkup, none of the children falsely reported genital
periment is arguably more about children's use of dolls than contact in free recall or AD-doll reenactment, but 3 children
about children's use of AD dolls. Possible effects of the AD falsely affirmed genital touch when the leading questions using
dolls' genital features (e.g., greater avoidance of or greater inter- the AD doll were asked. Only 1 of the 3 children provided false
est in the dolls) remain unknown on the basis of this otherwise detail, claiming that the doctor had used a stick to conduct the
informative research. anal examination and that it tickled.
It should be kept in mind that the children in the Goodman The Saywitz et al. (1991) study identifies advantages and dis-
and Aman (1991) and DeLoache and Marzolf (1995) studies advantages to using AD doll-aided leading questions with chil-
were not interviewed about an event involving genital touch; dren. On the one hand, it was only when the AD doll-aided lead-
AD dolls would be expected to be of particular benefit precisely ing questions were asked that the majority of the children re-
for such events. For example, the Goodman and Aman study vealed the genital touch they actually experienced. The
concerned whether children who did not experience genital omission error rate without use of the AD doll-aided leading
contact would be more likely to falsely report genital touch and questions was substantial at about 60%. Reenactment with AD
abuse-related actions when interviewed with AD dolls than dolls was not particularly facilitative of disclosure of genital
without such dolls. The results indicated that AD dolls did not touch, but then again these were not children who had been
intrinsically heighten the chances of obtaining a false report of sexually violated or traumatized. On the other hand, for chil-
sexual abuse, although some young children may have difficulty dren who did not experience genital touch, there was a relatively
understanding the AD doll's symbolic function. small risk of a false report when the AD doll-aided leading ques-
Children's use of AD dolls to accurately report an event in- tions were asked; specifically, 8% of the children falsely affirmed
volving genital touch was first investigated by Saywitz et al. genital touch, with only 3% (one child) providing false detail.
(1991). The Saywitz et al. study also explored the reports of None of the children falsely indicated genital touch when asked
children who had not experienced genital contact. Seventy-two to reenact the event with AD dolls. This study thus indicates
5- and 7-year-old girls were given a complete medical checkup that AD doll-aided leading questions can help elicit accurate
by a pediatrician at a university-affiliated hospital. Standard reports of genital touch from children who have experienced
medical checkups of girls by the pediatrician at the hospital typ- such touch, but that at the same time such questioning may
ically involved a vaginal and anal examination (i.e., visually in- lead to an increase in false reports by children who have not
specting and touching the labia and checking for an anal reflex). experienced genital contact.
For purposes of the study, this part of the checkup was omitted A study that was recently completed on forty 3-year-old chil-
for half of the children at each age. The advantage of this design dren underscores the need for caution in interpreting some
is that for the children who had the vaginal and anal examina- young children's behavior with AD dolls as indicative of abuse
tion, the researcher could explore the children's reports, includ- (Bruck, Ceci, & Francoeur, 1994). Borrowing the methodology
ing AD doll-aided reports, of actual genital touch, whereas for developed by Saywitz et al. (1991), Bruck and colleagues also
children who did not have vaginal or anal touch, the researchers examined children's use of AD dolls to recount medical exam-
could explore children's tendency to make false reports of geni- inations that involved or did not involve genital touch. All of
tal contact. the children were nonreferred and had no history or suspicion
216 KOOCHER ET AL.

of sexual abuse. The 3-year-olds were examined by their regular physician, there may have been confusion on the children's part
pediatrician with their mothers present. For half of the children, about former examinations by him or her.
the examination included inspection and touching of the child's The Bruck et al. (1994) study also raises questions about how
buttocks and genitals. During this portion of the examination, children's responses should be scored. It may be very difficult
the pediatrician never inserted anything into the genital open- for children (and adults, for that matter) to tell if they were
ing. Immediately following the examination, the children were touched externally or if slight penetration occurred when lying
interviewed by a research assistant using an AD doll of the same in a prone position that makes visual observation difficult. Doc-
gender as the child. The interviewer asked the child to name all umenting that such differentiation is not always made by chil-
body parts, including anus and genitalia. During this part of the dren is of interest, but at what point should such responses be
inquiry, the child was asked a leading yes-no question, "Did the scored as errors? Furthermore, combining across different types
doctor touch you here?" Each child was also offered an AD doll of errors that have very different legal implications is potentially
and some other props and asked to demonstrate how the doctor misleading. Decisions about what constitutes an error and how
touched the child's "genitals" (e.g., "Show me on the doll how to report one's data can affect conclusions reached.
Dr. Francoeur touched your penis"), an even more strongly Although the Saywitz et al. (1991) and Bruck et al. (1994)
leading question and misleading for children in the non-genital- studies concern children's reports of genital touch, the children
exam condition. Finally, children who experienced the genital in those studies did not experience painful, invasive touch or
and anal examination were asked to demonstrate on their own actual genital penetration. A recent study (Goodman, 1994)
bodies parts of the examination. (Those children who had not examined children's use of AD dolls to report invasive, painful
had their buttocks and genitals checked were not asked to dem- urethral penetration. In the study, approximately 46 children
onstrate on their own bodies the examination of these areas.) were interviewed after experiencing a radiological test that was
When the experimenter pointed to the AD doll's buttocks conducted on a physician's order for medical purposes in a hos-
and genital area and asked, "Did the doctor touch you here?", pital. The interview, which took place 1 to 3 weeks after the
45% of the children who had been touched in those areas accu- medical procedure, consisted of free-recall questions and
rately answered yes. Fifty percent of those who had not been prompts (e.g., "Tell me everything that happened when you got
examined on the buttocks or genital area answered correctly that medical test" and "What else happened?") followed by a
with a no, but the remaining 50% incorrectly affirmed such request for a reenactment of what happened using an AD doll
touch in response to the leading question, a potentially danger- and props. Findings revealed that older children provided, on
ous error were suspicions of criminal behavior involved. average, more correct information than younger children re-
During the second phase of the interview, which involved gardless of the type of interview (free recall vs. AD-doll
reenactment). Additionally, younger children overall tended to
children using the AD doll and props to show what happened
make more errors than older children.
under force of more highly leading questioning, about half
Specifically, 3- to 4-year-olds provided somewhat more cor-
(55%) of the children who did not receive the genital examina-
rect information when using AD dolls than in free recall, but
tion were accurate by not showing such touch, but the remain-
they also provided more incorrect information. The younger
ing 45% erred. For the children who actually experienced the
children's errors were especially associated with the distractor
genital examination, the error rate reached as high as 75% when props provided (e.g., a tongue depressor). In contrast, AD dolls
the researchers included omission errors and incorrect inser- aided older children (5- to 10-year-olds) in providing additional
tions of the children's fingers into anal or genital cavities in dem- correct information without a significant increase in error. Al-
onstrating the touch actually experienced. The children's accu- though this study did not include a comparison group of non-
racy did not significantly differ when demonstrations with the genitally touched children, thus failing to address the issue of
AD doll and demonstrations with the children's own bodies false reports of genital touch, it is relevant to the important
were compared. question of whether AD dolls aid children who have experi-
These data indicate that under certain conditions of testing, a enced invasive genital contact report such experiences. The
significant number of 3-year-olds can be lead to make false re- findings suggest that AD dolls may result in additional correct
ports of genital touch in interviews when AD dolls are used and information from older children, but the increase in correct in-
that a similar number who actually experienced noninvasive formation for younger children may be offset by an increase
genital touch do not report it when questioned with AD dolls. in error. However, Goodman and colleagues (Goodman, 1994)
However, in this study it cannot be determined whether the chil- noted that the additional correct information provided by even
dren who did not experience genital touch would have provided the young children was often helpful in specifying more exactly
as many false affirmations to leading questions had the AD dolls how the child's urethra was penetrated.
not been present. The children's scores may reflect children's Only a few other studies have appeared in the literature in
general suggestibility or younger children's lack of understand- which investigators who have an objective record of an event
ing of the task (e.g., the children's response rate tended to hover have collected data relevant to children's suggestibility with AD
around chance levels) as opposed to heightened suggestibility dolls. Two such studies compare use of AD dolls with other in-
because of the presence of AD dolls. That they provided about terviewing techniques. Steward and Steward (1989) were inter-
as many errors to leading questions when demonstrating on ested in 3- to 6-year-old children's reports of bodily touch when
their own bodies as with AD dolls also indicates that the AD children were interviewed with a verbal interview versus with
dolls did not significantly add to the children's error rates. AD dolls, drawings, or a computer-interviewing protocol. They
Moreover, because the children were examined by their regular found that all three "stimulus supported" interviews were su-
ANATOMICAL DOLLS 217

perior in eliciting information from children compared with a ciated with false reports of abuse or with interviewers forming
purely verbal interview. Edwards and Forman (1989) invited incorrect conclusions of abuse regardless of a child's age. On
forty-five 9- to 10-year-old girls to watch a 12 min videotape the other hand, the current research examining children's
that provided information on sexual abuse and its prevention. memory and suggestibility in interviews that include AD dolls
After a delay of between 4 to 6 hr, the children were interviewed does not address situations in which children attempt to re-
about the videotape in one of three conditions: verbal, AD doll, count actual abusive events and is just beginning to study pain-
and drawing. For the verbal condition, the children were asked ful or repeated genital touch or both, factors that also might
to recall everything they could about the videotape. The chil- affect children's performance. Further research may inform in-
dren in the AD-doll condition were also asked to show and tell terviewers about how to optimize children's disclosures of ac-
what happened in the videotape. The children in the drawing tual abuse and at the same time minimize children's false re-
condition were asked to draw pictures of what happened in the ports of abuse, whether AD dolls are used or not.
videotape as well as to recall everything about it. Although the
children who used the AD dolls or who produced drawings re- A Final Dilemma
called somewhat more than the children in the verbal condition,
the differences were not statistically significant. On average, the The nature of clinical cases involving allegations of child sex-
children provided little incorrect information regardless of in- ual abuse often demand binary categorization (i.e., the abuse
terview condition, although one child produced much more in- did or did not occur). Unlike medicine, where one might use a
accurate information than the other children tested. bacterial culture to document a streptococcal infection,
In summary, research to date mainly supports use of AD whether the infection is minor or life-threatening, there is no
dolls as a communication or memory aid for children 5 years or "gold standard" criterion or litmus test of whether alleged abuse
older, albeit with a certain risk of contributing to some chil- took place. In some instances, clinicians may disagree about
dren's errors if misleading questions are used. Interestingly, it whether acts acknowledged to have taken place were abusive. In
seems that many children beyond the preschool years are un- a rare case of alleged child sexual abuse, a gold standard may be
likely to make commission errors to abuse inquiries, at least approached (e.g., when an articulate child makes an unambig-
under the conditions examined in the previous studies. Greater uous report, corroborated by both physical findings and adult
caution is needed when preschool children are interviewed with witnesses). However, for the most part, clinicians are stuck with
AD dolls because of these younger children's greater tendency contradictory assertions and fallible indicators on which expe-
toward suggestibility and difficulties with symbolic representa- rienced practitioners tend to disagree vociferously. In the words
tion. Further research that includes preschool children will pro- of Paul Meehl (personal communication, April 6, 1993), "clin-
vide important clues about interviewing children ages 4 years ical experience simply has not sufficed to produce convergence
and under with AD dolls. Although findings to date indicate of expert opinion" on this issue. Meehl suggests the application
that young preschoolers are more prone than older children to of a taxometric approach to address this problem in a boot-
falsely report in a leading AD doll-aided interview, these results strapping manner (see, e.g., Meehl, 1992). Although such an
do not rule out AD dolls' potential usefulness in forensic inves- application is desirable in the long run, methodological barriers
tigations involving young children. For example, if a young abound.
child evidenced inappropriate sexual knowledge by spontane- In the face of these observations, one must consider whether
ously acting out a sexual event with AD dolls, such behavior the incremental validity provided by using AD dolls in an in-
might still aid the interviewer to accurately infer what happened terview "adds to the predictive accuracy of existing methods"
even if the child had difficulty forming a symbolic representa- (Wolfner et al., 1993, p. 8). One must also consider the cogni-
tion of her- or himself in relation to an AD doll when directed tive capability of human beings. Clinicians often claim to base
to do so by an interviewer. If a child provides a verbal disclosure their judgments on configural analysis and integration of many
of abuse, use of AD dolls after the disclosure as a communica- arrays of data; however, research suggests that such subjective
tion aid for clarifying what occurred may be helpful. In any beliefs are largely illusory (Faust, 1989). Reviewing clinicians'
case, because AD dolls are particularly likely to be used with ability to integrate complex data interactions, Wedding and
young children (Kendall-Tackett & Watson, 1992), further re- Faust (1989) found that problems involving the integration of
search with preschool populations is clearly warranted, as is "even two to three variables may outstrip human cognitive ca-
particular caution in interpreting young children's responses to pacities" (p. 246). Such cognitive research indicates that clini-
AD dolls. cal judgments that are purportedly based on the integration of
It could be legitimately argued, however, that extant research many variables can generally be duplicated by merely adding
has not included interviews that are as suggestive as some actual the variables together (Faust, 1989; Wedding & Faust, 1989).
forensic investigations, that the forensic context of the in- Much human "data processing" is automatic, subject to
terviews was not realistic (e.g., the children did not think any- habit, or otherwise occurs outside of conscious awareness, thus
one was in trouble, nor were they subjected to repeated limiting insight about potential biases. The arena of child sexual
interrogations), and that the events studied did not approxi- abuse is one in which many of the performers have strong affec-
mate closely enough the events about which children might tive beliefs or attitudes, which may influence them outside of a
show confusion and thus greater-error. Extant studies have also conscious-rational process. Are the data to be gleaned in in-
not examined cumulative effects of AD-doll exposure. On the terviews featuring AD dolls of sufficient validity in this context
one hand, because any tool or technique can be misused, there that they ought to be among the few given weighty consideration
may be situations in actual practice in which AD dolls are asso- in arriving at clinical conclusions? Are such data significant
218 KOOCHER ET AL.

enough to warrant incorporation as incremental data in an ac- 1990; Wolfner et al., 1993). In daily practice, professionals, es-
tuarial prediction model? In the context of the studies reviewed pecially mental health professionals, use a wide variety of tech-
here, it does not seem unreasonable to draw on AD-doll data niques when evaluating suspected child victims (Kendall-Tack-
in the cautious and thoughtful manner proposed by the APA ett, 1992). Most of these professionals do so in an effort to
council's resolution. Perhaps the concerns advanced by Faust achieve responsible clinical decision making. Reviews (Mann,
and his colleagues (1989) on the matter of data integration do 1991; White, 1991) of current practice attest to this goal. In
apply in the context of clinical judgments using AD-doll data in this context, we disagree with the conclusion of Wolfner et al.,
child sexual-abuse cases much as they do in neuropsychology (1993) that the APA council resolution at the beginning of this
(Wedding & Faust, 1989). Research focused on answering that article has "no merit" (p. 9). The data summarized here illus-
question seems the best solution. trate both merits and pitfalls inherent in AD-doll use. We agree
that the data on AD dolls may be equivocal when claims for
Future Research Directions diagnostic discrimination are made (Ceci & Bruck, 1993). We
agree with the APA resolution that AD dolls can still provide a
Many pressing questions about the impact of AD dolls on useful communication tool in the hands of a trained profes-
children's memory and suggestibility remain to be explored or sional interviewer with the following caveats:
have received insufficient research attention. A number of is- 1. AD dolls are not a psychological test with predictive (or
sues that require further research were mentioned; additional postdictive) validity per se.
important issues are addressed here. Research is needed on op- 2. Definitive statements about child sexual abuse cannot be
timal techniques for AD-doll use (e.g., number of AD dolls to made on the basis of spontaneous or guided "doll play" alone.
use, similarity of props to offenders and events, types of ques- A clinical interview by a skilled clinician is not play.
tions to ask, inclusion of other props, and duration of exposure 3. Particular caution is called for when interpreting the re-
to AD dolls); use of AD dolls to recount repeated versus single ports of children ages 4 years and under, at least so far as when
events; use of AD dolls to reenact traumatic versus neutral affirmations to leading questions about "being touched" are
events; further exploration of the basis for developmental concerned and when repeated misleading questioning has been
differences in AD-doll use; effects of repeated suggestions, re- used.
peated exposure to AD dolls, naming of body parts at the start 4. In light of current knowledge, we recommend that APA
of interviews, and accusatory context on children's AD-doll re- reconsider whether valid "doll-centered assessment" techniques
ports; effects of AD dolls on person identification (e.g., of cul- exist and whether they still "may be the best available practical
prits and other children); effectiveness of AD-doll use in devel- solution" (APA, 1991, p. 722) for the pressing and frequent
opmentally disabled or language-impaired children; effects of problem of investigation of child sexual abuse.
prior sexual knowledge and disorganized family life; and cul- 5. Special recognition of normative differences between
tural differences that might affect children's recounting of children of different racial groups and socioeconomic strata
events with AD dolls. In regard to referred children's use of AD should be a part of training professionals who use AD dolls in
dolls, research is needed that takes not only age but also severity clinical inquiry.
and type of sexual abuse into account, in case degree and type
of traumatization affect the likelihood of AD-doll reenactment. References
In general, research should examine separately, when possible, Allen, M. E., Jones, P. D., & Nash, M. R. (1989, August). Detection
the various functions of AD dolls articulated by Boat and Ever- of sexual abuse among emotionally disturbed preschoolers: Unique
son(1994b). effects of sexual abuse as observed on doll play and other standard
Moreover, psychologists need to know more about the use of assessment procedures. Paper presented at the Annual Convention of
AD dolls in actual practice. An ongoing study by Boat and Ev- the American Psychological Association, New Orleans, LA.
erson (1994a) on the use of AD dolls by social service profes- American Professional Society on the Abuse of Children. (1990).
Guidelines for psychosocial evaluation of suspected sexual abuse in
sionals should shed important light on the actual practices of
young children. Unpublished manuscript, Chicago, IL.
interviewers when AD dolls are used in child sexual-abuse in- American Psychological Association. (1991). Minutes of the Council
vestigations, a study that may confirm or shatter some currently of Representatives. American Psychologist, 46, 722.
held beliefs about actual practice with AD dolls. Perhaps of Ames, L. B., Learned, J., Metraux, R. W., & Walker, R. N. (1974).
most concern, there is a nearly complete void in the research Child Rorschach responses: Developmental trends from two to ten
literature concerning the process of decision making by profes- years. New York: Bruner/Mazel.
sionals and lay people and their abilities to reach the truth on August, R. I., & Forman, B. D. (1989). A comparison of sexually
the basis of children's use of AD dolls. Even if young children abused and nonsexually abused children's behavioral responses to
have difficulty using AD dolls optimally, in the end, it is the fact anatomically correct dolls. Child Psychiatry and Human Develop-
finder's (e.g., interviewer's and juror's) assessment that matters ment, 6, 39-47.
Baker-Ward, L., Gordon, B., Ornstein, P., Larus, N., & Clubb, P.
most. (1993). Ybung children's long-term retention of a pediatric exami-
It goes without saying that assessments of children who report nation. Child Development, 64, 1519-1533.
abuse or who are alleged to have been abused are complex. Baker-Ward, L., Hess, T. M., & Flannagan, D. A. (1990). The effects of
However, this complexity is often ignored by critics of the use of involvement on children's memory for events. Cognitive Develop-
AD dolls who focus on the incidence of false positives that result ment, 5, 55-69.
if one uses interactions with AD dolls as a single indicator or Bays, J. (1990). Are the genitalia of anatomical dolls distorted? Child
criterion for the evaluation of sexual abuse (Realmuto et al., Abuse and Neglect, 14, 171-175.
ANATOMICAL DOLLS 219

Berliner, L. (1988). Anatomical dolls. Journal of Interpersonal Vio- sessment of children: Vol. 2. Personality, behavior, and context. New
lence, 3, 468-470. York: Guilford Press.
Boat, B. W., & Everson, M. D. (1986). Using anatomical dolls: Guide- Chi, M. T. H. (1978). Knowledge structures and memory development.
lines for interviewing young children in sexual abuse investigations. In R. Siegler (Ed.), Children's thinking: What develops? Hillsdale,
Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina. NJ: Erlbaum.
Boat, B. W., & Everson, M. D. (1988a). Research and issues in using Clark, K. B., & Clark, M. P. (1958). Racial identification and prefer-
anatomical dolls. Annals of Sex Research, I, 191-204. ence in negro children. In E. Maccoby, J. Newcomb, & H. Hartley,
Boat, B. W., & Everson, M. D. (1988b). Use of anatomical dolls among (Eds.), Readings in social psychology. New York: Holt.
professionals in sexual abuse evaluations. Child Abuse and Neglect, Clarke-Stewart, A., Thompson, L., & Lepore, S. (1989, March). Ma-
12, 171-179. nipulating children's interpretations through interrogation. In
Boat, B. W., & Everson, M. D. (1989, August). Anatomical doll play G. S. Goodman (Chair), Can children provide accurate testimony?
among young children: A follow-up of sexual demonstrators and doll Symposium presented at the Society for Research in Child Develop-
avoiders. Paper presented at the 97th Annual Convention of the ment Meetings, Kansas City, MO.
American Psychological Association, New Orleans, LA. Conn, D. S. (1991). Anatomical doll play of preschoolers referred for
Boat, B. W., & Everson, M. D. (1994a). Exploration of anatomical dolls sexual abuse and those not referred. Child Abuse and Neglect, 15,
by nonreferred preschool-aged children: Comparisons by age, gender, 455-466.
race and socioeconomic status. Child Abuse and Neglect, 18, 139- Committee to Develop Standards for Educational and Psychological
154. Testing. (1985). Standards for educational and psychological testing.
Boat, B. W., & Everson, M. D. (1994b). Functional uses of anatomical Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
dolls in child sexual abuse investigations. Profiles of research grants Conerly, S. (1986). Assessment of suspected child sexual abuse. In K.
funded by NCCAN. National Clearinghouse on Child Abuse and Ne- MacFarlane & J. Waterman (Eds.), Sexual abuse of young children
glect Information, Washington, DC. (pp. 30-51). New York: Guilford Press.
Bottoms, B. L. (1993). Individual differences in perceptions of child Conte, J., Sorenson, M. A., Fogarty, L., & Rosa, J. D. (1991). Evaluat-
sexual assault victims. In G. S. Goodman & B. L. Bottoms (Eds.), ing children's reports of sexual abuse: Results from a survey of pro-
Child victims, child witnesses (pp. 235-247). New York: Guilford fessionals. American Journal ofOthropsychiatry, 61, 428-437.
Press. Dawson, B., Vaughan, A. R., & Wagner, W. G. (1992). Normal re-
Brainerd, C, & Ornstein, P. A. (1991). Children's memory for wit- sponses to sexually anatomically detailed dolls. Journal of Family
nessed events: The developmental backdrop. In J. Doris (Ed.), The Violence, 7, 135-152.
suggestibility of children's recollections (pp. 10-20). Washington, DeLoache, J. (1990). Young children's understanding of models. In
DC: American Psychological Association. R. Fivush & J. Hudson (Eds.), Knowing and remembering in young
Brainerd, C., Kenya, V., Howe, M., & Kingma, J. (1991). The develop- children (pp. 94-126). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
ment of forgetting and reminiscence. Monographs of the Society for Press.
Research in Child Development, 55(3-4, Serial No. 222). DeLoache, J. (in press). The use of dolls in interviewing young chil-
Brennan, M., & Brennan, R. (1988). Strange language: Child victims dren. In M. Zaragoza, J. Graham, & G. Hall (Eds.), Memory and
under cross examination. Riverina, Australia: Charles Stuart testimony in the child witness. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
University. DeLoache, J., Kolstad, V., & Anderson, K. N. (1991). Physical sim-
ilarity and young children's understanding of scale models. Child De-
Brigham. J. (1991). Issues in the empirical study of the sexual abuse of
velopment, 62, 111-126.
children. In J. Doris (Ed.), The suggestibility of children's recollec-
DeLoache, J., & Marzolf, D. (1994). The use of dolls to interview young
tions (pp. 110-114). Washington, DC: American Psychological
children: Issues of symbolic representation. Manuscript submitted for
Association.
publication, University of Illinois, Champaign-Urbana.
Britton, H. L., & O'Keefe, M. A. (1991). Use of nonanatomical dolls
DiLalla, L. F, & Watson, M. W. (1988). Differentiation of fantasy and
in the sexual abuse interview. Child Abuse and Neglect, 15, 567-573.
reality: Preschoolers' reactions to interruptions in their play. Develop-
Brown v. Board of Educ., 347 U.S. 483 (1954). mental Psychology, 24, 286-291.
Browne, A., & Finkelhor, D. (1986). Impact of child sexual abuse: A Donaldson, M. (1979). Children's minds. New York: Norton.
review of the research. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 66-77.
Edington, G. (1985). Hand puppets and dolls in psychotherapy with
-Bruck, M., & Ceci, S. J. (1993). Amicus brief on behalf of Kelly Mi- children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 61, 691-696.
chaels. Unpublished manuscript, McGill University, Montreal, Edwards, C. A., & Forman, B. D. (1989). Effects of child interview
Canada. method on accuracy and completeness of sexual abuse information
Bruck, M., Ceci, S. J., & Francoeur, E. (1994). Anatomically detailed recall. Social Behavior and Personality, 17, 237-247.
dolls do not facilitate preschoolers' reports of touching. Paper pre- Everson, M. D., & Boat, B. W. (1990). Sexualized doll play among
sented at the annual meeting of the Canadian Pediatric Society, St. young children: Implications for the use of anatomical dolls in sexual
John's, Newfoundland, Canada. abuse evaluations. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Carter, C. (1991). Influences of language and emotional support on chil- Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 736-742.
dren's testimony. Unpublished dissertation, State University of New Everson, M. D., & Boat, B. W. (1994). Putting the anatomical doll con-
York-Buffalo. troversy in perspective: An examination of the major uses and criti-
Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1993). Suggestibility of the child witness: A cisms of the dolls in child sexual abuse evaluations. Child Abuse and
historical review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 113,403-439. Neglect, 18, 113-130.
Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1994). Translating research into policy. Soci- Farrar, M. J., & Goodman, G. S. (1992). Developmental differences in
ety for Research in Child Development Social Policy Report, 7(3), 1- event memory. Child Development, 63, 173-187.
29. Faust, D. (1989). Data integration in legal evaluations: Can clinicians
Chandler, L. A. (1990). The projective hypothesis and the development deliver on their premises? Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 7, 469-
of projective techniques for children. In C. R. Reynolds & R. W. 483.
Kamphaus (Eds.), Handbook of psychological and educational as- Finkelhor, D., & Browne, A. (1985). The traumatic impact of child
220 KOOCHER ET AL.

sexual abuse: A conceptualization. American Journal of Orthopsychi- meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, New Or-
atry, 55, 530-541. leans, LA.
Finlayson, L., & Koocher, G. P. (1991). Professional judgment and re- Goodman, G. S., Bottoms, B. L., Schwartz-Kenney, B., & Rudy, L. A.
porting in child sexual abuse cases. Professional Psychology: Re- (1991). Children's memory for a stressful event: Improving chil-
search and Practice, 22, 464-472. dren's reports. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 1, 69-99.
Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control Goodman, G. S., & Clarke-Stewart, A. (1991). Suggestibility in chil-
and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, dren's testimony: Implications for child sexual abuse investigations.
477-531. In J. L. Doris(Ed.), The suggestibility of children's recollections (pp.
Fischer, K. W., & Bullock, D. (1984). Cognitive development in school- 92-105). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
age children: Conclusions and new directions. In W. Collins (Ed.), Goodman, G. S., Hirschman, J. E., Hepps, D., & Rudy, L. A. (1991).
Development during middle childhood: The years from six to twelve Children's memory for stressful events. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 37,
(pp. 70-146). Washington, DC: National Academy Press. 109-158.
Fischer, K. W., & Lazerson, A. (1984). Human development. New York: Goodman, G. S., & Reed, R. S. (1986). Age differences in eyewitness
Freeman. testimony. Law and Human Behavior, 10, 317-322.
Fivush, R., Gray, J. T., & Fromhoff, F. A. (1987). Two year olds talk Goodman, G. S., Rudy, L. A., Bottoms, B. L., & Aman, C. (1990).
about the past. Cognitive Development, 2, 393-410. Children's concerns and memory: Issues of ecological validity in the
Flavell, J. H. (1985). Cognitive development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: study of children's eyewitness testimony. In R. Fivush & J. Hudson
Prentice-Hall. (Eds.), Knowing and remembering in young children (pp. 249-284).
Flin, R., Boon, J., Knox, A., & Bull, R. (1992). The effects of a five- New \brk: Cambridge University Press.
month delay on children's and adults' eyewitness memory. British Gordon, B. N., Ornstein, P. A., Nida, R. E., Follmer, A., Crenshaw,
Journal of Psychology, 83, 323-336. M. C., & Albert, G. (1993). Does the use of dolls facilitate children's
Fraley, M. C, Nelson, E. C, Wolf, A. W., & Lozoff, B. (1991). Early memory of visits to the doctor? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 7,459-
genital naming. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 12, 301- 474.
304. Gordon, B. N., Schroeder, C. S., & Abrams, J. M. (1990a). Age and
Freeman, K. R., & Estrada-Mullaney, T. (1988). Using dolls to in- social-class differences in children's knowledge of sexuality. Journal
terview child victims: Legal concerns and interview procedures. Na- of Clinical Child Psychology, 19, 33-43.
tional Institute of Justice Reports, 107, 1-6. Gordon, B. N., Schroeder, C. S., & Abrams, J. M. (1990b). Children's
Freud, A. (1928). Introduction to the technique of child analysis. New knowledge of sexuality: A comparison of sexually abused and non-
York: Nervous and Mental Disease Publishing. abused children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 60, 250-257.
Friedemann, V., & Morgan, M. (1985). Interviewing sexual abuse vic- Group for the Advancement of Psychiatry. (1982). The process of child
tims using anatomical dolls: The professional's guidebook. Eugene, therapy. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
OR: Shamrock Press. Harnest, J., & Chavern, H. E. (1985). A survey of the use of anatomi-
Friedrich, W. N., Grambach, P., Broughton, D., Kuiper, J., & Beilke, cally correct dolls in sex education, investigation, therapy, and court-
R. L. (1991). Normative sexual behavior in children. Pediatrics, 88, room testimony. Paper presented at the Seventh World Congress of
456-464. Sexology, New Delhi, India.
Friedrich, W. N., Grambach, P., Damon, L., Hewitt, S. K., Koverola, Hill, P., & Hill, S. (1987). Videotaping children's testimony: An empir-
C., Lang, R. A., Wolfe, V., & Broughton, D. (1992). Child Sexual ical view. Michigan Law Review, 85, 809-833.
Behavior Inventory: Normative and clinical comparisons. Psycholog-
Horowitz, R. E. (1939). Racial aspects of self-identification in nursery
ical Assessment, 4, 303-311.
school children. Journal of Psychology, 7, 91-99.
Gabriel, R. M. (1985). Anatomically correct dolls in the diagnosis of
Howe, M. (1991). Misleading children's story recall: Forgetting and
sexual abuse of children. The Journal of the Melanie Klein Society, 3,
reminiscence of the facts. Developmental Psychology, 27, 746-762.
40-51.
In re Amber B. et al., 91 Cal. Rptr. 3d 682 (Calif. Ct. App. 1987).
Gardner, R. (1991). Sex abuse hysteria: Salem witch trials revisited.
Cresskill, NJ: Creative Therapeutics. Jampole, L., & Weber, M. K. (1987). An assessment of the behavior of
sexually abused and nonabused children with anatomically correct
Gil, E., & Johnson, T. C. (1993). Sexualized children. New York:
dolls. Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 187-192.
Launch Press.
Glaser, D., & Collins, C. (1989). The response of young, non-sexually Johnson, M., & Foley, M. (1984). Differentiating fact from fantasy.
abused children to anatomically correct dolls. Journal of Child Psy- Journal of Social Issues, 40, 33-50.
chology and Psychiatry, 30, 547-560. Kail, R. (1990). The development of memory in children (3rd ed.). New
Goldman, R., & Goldman, J. (1982). Show me yours. Victoria, Aus- York: Freeman.
tralia: Penguin. Keary, K., & Fitzpatrick, C. (1994). Children's disclosure of sexual
Goodman, G. S. (1993, August). Child victims, child witnesses. Invited abuse during formal investigation. Child Abuse and Neglect, 18, 543-
address presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Psycholog- 548.
ical Association, Toronto, Canada. Kendall-Tackett, K. A. (1992). Beyond anatomical dolls: Professionals'
Goodman, G. S. (1994, August). Update on developmental research on use of other play therapy techniques. Child Abuse and Neglect, 16,
child sexual abuse. Session presented at the Annual Convention of 139-142.
the American Psychological Association, Los Angeles, CA. Kendall-Tackett, K. A., & Watson, M. W. (1992). Use of anatomical
Goodman, G. S., & Aman, C. J. (1991). Children's use of anatomically dolls by Boston-area professionals. Child Abuse and Neglect, 16,423-
detailed dolls to recount an event. Child Development, 61, 1859- 428.
1871. Kendall-Tackett, K. A., Williams, L., & Finkelhor, D. (1992). Impact
Goodman, G. S., Batterman-Faunce, J. M., Quas, J., Riddlesberger, M., of sexual abuse on children: A review and synthesis of recent empiri-
& Kuhn, J. (1993, March). Children's memory for stressful events: cal findings. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 164-180.
Theoretical and developmental considerations. In N. Stein (Chair), Kenyon-Jump, R., Burnette, M. M., & Robertson, M. (1991). Com-
Emotional events and memory. Symposium presented at annual parison of behaviors of suspected sexually abused and nonsexually
ANATOMICAL DOLLS 221

abused preschool children using anatomical dolls. Journal oj'Psycho- were very young: Almost threes remember two years ago. Infant Be-
pathology and Behavioral Assessment, 13, 225-240. havior and Development, 10, 123-132.
King, M. A., & Yuille, J. (1987). Suggestibility and the child witness. Nelson, K. (1993). The psychological and social origins of autobio-
In S. J. Ceci, M. P. Toglia, & D. F. Ross (Eds.), Children's eyewitness graphical memory. Psychological Science, 4, 7-14.
memory (pp. 24-35). New York: Springer-Verlag. Perry, N., Claycomb, L., Dostal, C., Flanagan, C., McAuliff, B., & Tam,
Klein, M. (1932). The psychoanalysis of children. London: Hogarth P. (1993, March). When lawyers question children: Is justice served?
Press. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Research in
Kluger, R. (1975). Simple justice. New York: Knopf. Child Development, New Orleans, LA.
Lamb, S., & Coakley, M. (1993). "Normal" childhood sexual play and Pezdek, K. (1994). Children's suggestibility and resistance to sugges-
games: Differentiating play from abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 17, tion. Paper presented at the Conference on Applied Cognitive Psy-
515-526. chology, Claremont Graduate School, Claremont, CA.
Lawson, L., & Chaffin, M. (1992). False negatives in sexual abuse dis- Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1973). Memory and intelligence. New York:
closure interviews. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 7, 532-542. Basic Books.
Leichtman, M. D., & Ceci, S. J. (1995). The effects of repeated ques- Pipe, M. E., Gee, S., & Wilson, C. (1993). Cues, props, and context: Do
tions and stereotypes on preschoolers' reports. Developmental Psy- they facilitate children's reports. In G. S. Goodman & B. L. Bottoms
chology, 31, 568-578. (Eds.), Child victims, child witnesses (pp. 25-46). New York: Guil-
Leippe, M. R., Manion, A., & Romanczyk, A. (1993). Discernability ford Press.
or discrimination? Understanding jurors' reactions to accurate and Poole, D., & Lindsay, D. S. (1994, April). Interviewing preschoolers:
inaccurate child and adult eyewitnesses. In G. Goodman & B. Bot- Effects of nonsuggestive techniques, parental coaching, and leading
toms (Eds.), Child victims, child witnesses: Understanding and im- questions on reports ofnonexperienced events. Paper presented at the
proving testimony (pp. 169-202). New York: Guilford Press. American Psychology and Law Society Meetings, Santa Fe, NM.
Lepore, S. J., & Sesco, B. (1994). Distorting children's reports and in- Powell-Hopson, D., & Hopson, D. S. (1988). Implications of doll color
terpretations of events through suggestion. Journal of Applied Psy- preferences among Black preschool children and White preschool
chology, 79, 108-120. children. Journal of Black Psychology, 14(2), 57-63.
Leventhal, J. M., Hamilton, J., Rekedal, S., Tebano-Micci, A., & Eyster, Price, D. W. W, & Goodman, G. S. (1990). Visiting the wizard: Chil-
C. (1989). Anatomically correct dolls used in interviews of young dren's memory for a recurring event. Child Development, 61, 664-
children suspected of having been sexually abused. Pediatrics, 84, 680.
900-906. Quinn, K. M., White, S., & Santilli, G. (1989). Influences of an in-
Levy, H., Kalinowski, N., Markovic, J., Pittman, M., & Ahart, S. terviewer's behaviors in child sexual abuse investigations. Bulletin of
(1991). Victim-sensitive interviewing in child sexual abuse: A devel- the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 17, 45-52.
opmental approach to interviewing and consideration of the use of an- Raskin, D., & Yuille, J. (1989). Problems in evaluating interviews of
atomically detailed dolls. Chicago: Department of Pediatrics, Mount children in sexual abuse cases. In S. J. Ceci, D. Ross, & M. Toglia
Sinai Hospital Medical Center. (Eds.), Perspectives on children's testimony (pp. 184-207). New
Levy, R. J. (1989). Using "scientific" testimony to prove child sexual York: Springer-Verlag.
abuse. Family Law Quarterly, 23, 383-409. Ratner, H. H., Smith, B., & Padgett, R. J. (1990). Children's organiza-
Lindberg, M., McComas, L., Jones, S., & Thomas, S. (1993, March). tion of events and event memories. In R. Fivush & J. Hudson (Eds.),
Similarities and differences in eyewitness testimonies for children who Knowing and remembering in young children (pp. 65-93). New \brk:
directly versus vicariously experience stress. Paper presented at the Cambridge University Press.
annual meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Realmuto, G. M., Jensen, J. B., & Wescoe, S. (1990). Specificity and
New Orleans, LA. sensitivity of sexually anatomically correct dolls in substantiating
List, J. A. (1986). Age and schematic differences in the reliability of abuse: A pilot study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
eyewitness testimony. Developmental Psychology, 22, 50-57. Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 743-746.
Loftus, E. F. (1979). Eyewitness testimony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Realmuto, G. M., & Wescoe, S. (1992). Agreement among profession-
University Press. als about child's sexual abuse status: Interviews with sexually ana-
MacFarlane, K., & Krebs, S. (1986). Techniques of interviewing and tomically correct dolls as indicators of abuse. Child Abuse and Ne-
evidence gathering. In K. MacFarlane & J. Waterman (Eds.), Sexual glect, 76,719-725.
abuse of young children: Evaluation and treatment (pp. 67-100). Rosenfeld, A. A., Bailey, R. R., Siegel, B., & Bailey, G. (1986). Deter-
New York: Guilford Press. mining incestuous contact between parent and child: Frequency of
Mandler, J. (1984). Stories, scripts, and scenes: Aspects of schema the- children touching parent's genitals in a nonclinical population.
ory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Journal of the A merican Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
Mann, T. (1991). Assessment of sexually abused children with anatom- 25,481-484.
ically detailed dolls: A critical review. Behavioral Sciences and the Rovee-Collier, C. (1989). The joy of kicking: Memories, motives, and
Law, 9, 43-51. mobiles. In P. R. Solomon, G. R. Goethals, C. M. Kelley, & B. R.
Meehl, P. E. (1992). Factors and taxa, traits and types, differences of Stephens (Eds.), Memory: Interdisciplinary approaches. New "Vbrk:
degree and differences in kind. Journal of Personality, 60, 117-174. Springer-Verlag.
Melton, G. B., & Thompson, R. (1987). Getting out of a rut: Detours Rudy, L. A. (1991). Interactions of sexually abused and nonabused chil-
to less traveled paths in child-witness research. In S. Ceci, M. J. Tog- dren with anatomically correct dolls. Unpublished master's thesis,
lia, & D. Ross (Eds.), Children's eyewitness memory (pp. 209-229). Ohio State University, Columbus.
New \brk: Springer-Verlag. Rudy, L. A., & Goodman, G. S. (1991). Effects of participation on
Mrazek, D. A., & Mrazek, P. B. (1981). Psychosexual development children's reports: Implications for children's testimony. Develop-
within the family. In P. B. Mrazek & C. H. Kempe (Eds.), Sexually mental Psychology, 27, 1-26.
abused children and their families (pp. 17-32). New York: Rutter, M. (1971). Normal psychosexual development. Journal of
Pergamon. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 11, 259-283.
Myers, N. A., Clifton, R. K., & Clarkson, M. G. (1987). When they Saywitz, K., & Goodman, G. S. (in press). Interviewing children in
222 KOOCHER ET AL.

and out of court: Current research and practical implications. In L. ingin neuropsychology. Archives ofClinical Neuropsychology, 4,233-
Berliner, J. Bulkley, J. Briere, & T. Reid (Eds.), Handbook of child 265.
sexual abuse. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Wellman, H. M., & Estes, D. (1986). Early understanding of mental
Saywitz, K., Goodman, G. S., Nicholas, E., & Moan, S. (1991). Chil- entities: A reexamination of childhood realism. Child Development,
dren's memories of physical examinations involving genital touch: 57,910-923.
Implications for reports of child sexual abuse. Journal of Consulting White, S. (1985a, October). Objective data collection using anatomi-
and Clinical Psychology, 59, 682-691. cally correct dolls in the assessment of sexual abuse in very young
Saywitz, K.., & Nathanson, R. (in press). Children's testimony and their children. Workshop presented at the American Academy of Child and
perceptions of stress in and out of the courtroom. Child Abuse and Adolescent Psychiatry, San Antonio, TX.
Neglect. White, S. (1985b, October). The use of anatomical dolls in child sexual
Saywitz, K., & Snyder, L. (1993). Improving children's testimony abuse evaluations. Presented at the National Summit Conference on
through preparation. In G. S. Goodman & B. L. Bottoms (Eds.), Diagnosing Child Sexual Abuse, Los Angeles, CA.
Child victims, child witnesses (pp. 117-146). New York: Guilford White, S. (1986). Uses and abuses of anatomically correct dolls. Chil-
Press. dren, Youth, and Families Newsletter, 9, 3.
Schor, D. P., & Sivan, A. B. (1989). Interpreting children's labels for White, S. (1991). Using anatomically detailed dolls in interviewing pre-
sex-related body parts of anatomically explicit dolls. Child Abuse and schoolers. In C. E. Schaefer, K. Gitlin, & A. Sandgrund (Eds.), Play,
Neglect, 7.?, 523-531. diagnosis and assessment. New York: Wiley.
Schwartz-Kenney, B., & Goodman, G. S. (1991, April). Memory im- White, S., Halpin, B. M., Strom, G. A., & Santilli, G. (1988). Behav-
pairment in children. Paper presented at the meetings of the Society ioral comparisons of young sexually abused, neglected, and non-re-
for Research in Child Development, Seattle, WA. ferred children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychiatry, 17, 53-61.
Sinason, V. (1988). Dolls and bears: From symbolic equation to sym- White, S., & Quinn, K. M. (1988). Investigatory independence in child
bol: The significance of different play material for sexually abused sexual abuse evaluations: Conceptual considerations. Bulletin of the
children and others. British Journal of Psychotherapy, 4, 349-363. American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 17, 269-278.
Sivan, A. B. (1991). Preschool child development: Implications for in- White, S., Strom, G., & Santilli, G. (1983). Research guidelines for
vestigation of child abuse allegations. Child Abuse and Neglect, 15, interviewing young children with anatomically correct dolls. Unpub-
485-493. lished manuscript, Case Western Reserve University School of Medi-
Sivan, A. B., Schor, D. P., Koeppl, G. K., & Noble, L. D. (1988). In- cine, Cleveland, OH.
teraction of normal children with anatomical dolls. Child Abuse and White, S., Strom, G., & Santilli, G. (1985). A protocol for interviewing
Neglect, 12, 295-304. preschoolers with the sexually anatomically correct dolls. Unpub-
Skinner, L., & Berry, K. (1993). Anatomically detailed dolls and the lished manuscript, Case Western Reserve University School of Medi-
evaluation of child sexual abuse allegations: Psychometric considera- cine, Cleveland, OH.
tions. Law and Human Behavior, 17, 399-422. White, S., Strom, G., Santilli, G., & Halpin, B. M. (1986). Interviewing
Solomon, J. (1938). Active play therapy. American Journal of Ortho- young sexual abuse victims with anatomically correct dolls. Child
psychiatry, 8, 479-498. Abuse and Neglect, 10, 519-529.
State of New Jersey v. Kelly Michaels, 2 NJ Super., 593 (1993). White, S., Strom, G., Santilli, G., & Quinn, K. M. (1987). Clinical
Steward, M. (1993). Understanding children's memories of medical guidelines for interviewing young children with anatomically correct
procedures: "He didn't touch me and it didn't hurt." In C. A. Nelson dolls. Unpublished manuscript, Case Western Reserve University
(Ed.), Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology: Memory and School of Medicine, Cleveland, OH.
affect in development (pp. 171-225). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Wolfner, G., Faust, D., & Dawes, R. M. (1993). The use of anatomically
Steward, M., & Steward, J. (1989). The development of a model in- detailed dolls in sexual abuse evaluations: The state of the science.
terview for young child victims of sexual abuse: Comparing the Applied & Preventive Psychology, 2, 1-11.
effectiveness of anatomical dolls, drawings, and videographics. Final Wright, L., Everett, F., & Roisman, L. (1986). Experiential psychother-
Report to the National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect, Washing- apy with children. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
ton, DC. Yates, A., & Terr, L. (1988a). Anatomically correct dolls: Should they
Terr, L. (1988). What happens to early memories of trauma? A study be used as the basis for expert testimony? Journal of the American
of 20 children under age 5 at the time of the documented traumatic Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 254-257.
events. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Yates, A., & Terr, L. (1988b). Issue continued: Anatomically correct
Psychiatry, 27, 96-104. dolls: Should they be used as the basis for expert testimony? Journal
Tobey, A. E., & Goodman, G. S. (1992). Children's eyewitness mem- of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27,
ory: Effects of participation and forensic context. Child Abuse and 387-388.
Neglect, 16, 779-796. Zaragoza, M. (1991). Preschool children's susceptibility to memory
Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. Oxford, England: Ox- impairment. In J. Doris(Ed.), The suggestibility of children's mem-
ford University Press. ory (pp. 27-39). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Warren, A., Hagood, P. L., & Snider, C. (1993, March). Repeatedly Association.
questioning children: Does practice make perfect? Paper presented at
the annual meeting of the Society for Research in Child Develop- Received February 7, 1994
ment, New Orleans, LA. Revision received January 16, 1995
Wedding, D., & Faust, D. (1989). Clinical judgment and decision mak- Accepted January 17, 1995 •

You might also like