Baker1986 PDF

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Journal of Counseling Psychology Copyright 1986 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1986, Vol. 33, No. 1,31-38 0022-0167/86/$00.75

Exploratory Intervention With a Scale Measuring Adjustment to College


Robert W. Baker Bohdan Siryk
Clark University Wang Laboratories
Lowell, Massachusetts

An intervention into the lives of college freshmen based on a scale measuring adjustment
to college is described. The principal purposes of the study were to explore the practical
usefulness of the scale in an interview and to examine the consequences of the intervention.
The scale was used (a) to identify, for comparison through interview, students occupying
extremes of score distributions on measures of effectiveness of adjustment to college, (b)
to serve as a source of topics for discussion in interviews, and (c) to measure the effects of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

intervention through pre- and posttesting. Qualitative and quantitative findings are presented
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

regarding (a) attitude of students toward use of the scale, (b) the correspondence between
test data and effectiveness of adjustment to college, and (c) the consequences of intervention
by interviews for students indicated by the scale as well adjusted and less well adjusted.
Implications for additional research with the scale are discussed.

This article describes an intervention into the lives of college the Graduate Record Examination, a lower drop-out rate, and
freshmen based on a scale measuring adjustment to college (Baker greater subsequent improvement in grade point average. Some-
& Siryk, 1984). The primary purpose of this article is to explore what similarly, Meichenbaum and Smart (1971) found that if ac-
the practical usefulness of the scale in intervention, and second, ademically borderline students are administered ability and interest
to consider the efficacy of the intervention. tests and are subsequently given rigged test-based predictions of
The construction of the scale was occasioned originally by the future academic success, there is improvement in course grades,
realization that students who have difficulties in adjustment to in attitude toward academic work, and in self-confidence for meet-
college tend not to avail themselves of help offered by voluntary ing academic challenges.
counseling programs (e.g., Baker & Nisenbaum, 1979; Friedlan- Bednar and Weinberg (1970) and Kipnis and Resnick (1971)
der, 1980; Kirshner, 1974; Lindquist & Lowe, 1978). An essential underscored the importance of relevance of mode of intervention
interest in its construction was to provide a reliable and valid to particular characteristics or needs of students, and the latter
diagnostic instrument that would make efficient and selective in- emphasized the desirability of early diagnosis of need and prompt-
tervention possible. ness of intervention. It is expected that the adjustment-to-college
A search of the literature yields no instances of scales measuring scale employed in the present study will provide a means of early
adjustment to college being employed in any systematic way as diagnosis of need that will also carry implications for type of
bases for intervention with students. There is considerable liter- intervention that would be both need-appropriate and timely.
ature concerning consequences of interventions (see Beal & Noel, The scale is used in the present study in three ways as a basis
1980, for an overview). Of special relevance to the present study, for intervention: (a) to identify students differing in effectiveness
where the intervention employed was simply a follow-up inter- of adjustment to college; (b) to serve as a source of topics for
view, there is evidence that relatively limited efforts can produce discussion in interviews with those students, especially difficulties
surprising results. encountered in the adjustment; and (c) to measure the effects of
Bloom (1971) conducted an intervention largely by mail and intervention. Questions pertinent to each of these uses will permit
found a smaller drop-out rate from the university for a sample of evaluation of the scale's utility. Do students identified by the scale
students so treated than for a control group, indicating that per- as differing in effectiveness of adjustment to college actually see
sonal contact may not be necessary to effect desirable outcome in themselves differently or look different behaviorally with regard
a behavior of considerable consequence. A study by Wilson and to that adjustment? Are scale-based discussion topics meaningful
Linville (1982) did involve personal contact, but not a great amount to the student in terms of his or her experiences in adjusting to
of it. They identified freshmen who were worried about academic college? Is there correspondence between test findings and the
performance and conveyed to them in a group meeting information student's perceptions of his or her life circumstances, including
regarding the tendency for grades to improve beyond the freshman accurate identification of difficulties in adjustment? Does the scale
year. Students so treated, in comparison with another group of reflect change in effectiveness of adjustment to college following
freshmen similarly identified as worried but not given the infor- intervention?
mation regarding grades, had higher scores on a test resembling

Method
Bohdan Siryk is now at the Boston office of Blue Cross/Blue Shield of Materials
Massachusetts.
Correspondence Concerning this article or requests for copies of the scale The instrument used in the present study to measure adjustment to
used in this study should be addressed to Robert W. Baker, Department college is a self-rating, Likert-type scale constructed by the authors; the
of Psychology, Clark University, Worcester, Massachusetts 01610. development of an earlier version has been reported by Baker and Siryk

31
32 ROBERT W. BAKER AND BOHDAN SIRYK

(1984). An assumption underlying the construction of the scale is that expectations to postmatriculation perceptions of self-assessed adjustment
adjustment to college is multifaceted and involves demands varying in to college as those differences in turn relate to other behavioral manifes-
both kind and degree and that these demands require a variety of coping tations of adjustment to college (Baker et al., 1985).
responses (or adjustments) that will themselves vary in effectiveness. Thus,
the scale in its current form consists of 67 statements alluding to various
aspects of the experience of adjusting to college, and the student is asked Procedure
in effect to assess on a 9-point rating scale for each statement how well
he or she is dealing with the aspect in question. Specifically, the student In the eighth week of the first semester of the academic year 1981-
is instructed to indicate the degree to which each statement applies to him 1982, the adjustment-to-college scale was distributed by campus mail to
or her. the 549 members of the freshman class of Clark University, in Worcester,
The academic adjustment subscale consists of 24 items referring to Massachusetts. It was made clear in a cover letter that (a) participation in
various facets of the educational demands characteristic of the college the research program was voluntary; (b) strict professional confidentiality
experience (e.g., "Recently I have had trouble concentrating when I try would be provided; (c) other questionnaires would be sent subsequently,
to study"). The social adjustment subscale has 20 items pertaining to and information about student progress would be obtained from university
various facets of the interpersonal-societal demands inherent in that ex- records as time went on; and (d) selected persons might be invited for
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

perience (e.g., "I am meeting as many people and making as many friends interview. The scale was completed and returned by 216 students (76 men,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

as I would like at . . ."). The personal/emotional adjustment subscale 140 women).


contains 15 items aimed at determining how the student is feeling both To construct comparison groups representing clear differences in effec-
psychologically and physically, that is, whether he or she is experiencing tiveness of adjustment to college as measured by the scale, students oc-
general psychological distress and its somatic accompaniments (e.g., "I cupying the extremes of the distribution of scores (viz., ^ ± 1 SD) on
have been feeling tense and nervous lately"). A goal commitment/insti- each of the four subscales were identified. Eighty-six students (33 men,
tutional attachment (referred to as attachment) subscale consists of 15 53 women) had at least one high-end (relatively well-adjusted) subscale
items relating to the student's feelings about being in college in general score, and 90 (26 men, 64 women) had at least one low-end (relatively
and at the college of attendance in particular, especially to the quality of less well-adjusted) score. Students thus identified could be compared across
the relationship or bond that is established between the student and the the distribution extremes within each subscale for behavioral differences
institution (e.g., "I expect to stay at ... for a bachelor's degree"). relevant to the subscale in question, as well as for differences in the way
There is no overlap of items on the academic, social, and personal/ they perceive themselves and their life circumstances.
emotional adjustment subscales, but the attachment measure contains eight To make possible an investigation of consequences of intervention, the
items that are also on the social adjustment subscale and one that is on students on the low end of the subscale distributions were assigned either
the academic adjustment subscale. The attachment subscale contains items to an interview or no-interview condition, as were those on the high end.
from other subscales because it includes any item from the original version Assignments were made in such a manner that the two pools of students
of the adjustment-to-college scale that correlated to a certain minimum within each end of the distributions were equivalent for subscale scores,
degree with attrition in two prior samples of freshmen at Clark University. sex, and size. With this design, the consequences of intervention can be
In addition to scores for each of the four subscales, the instrument also examined for relatively well-adjusted and relatively less well-adjusted stu-
yields a full-scale score as an index of overall adjustment to college, with dents separately.
higher scores indicating better adjustment. At this juncture, it is desirable to point out and clarify a complication
Reliability and validity data for the earlier, 52-item, version of the scale associated with the authors' conception of adjustment to college as mul-
are reported elsewhere (Baker & Siryk, 1984). Fifteen items were added tifaceted — reflected in the division of the measuring instrument into four
to the original scale primarily to improve the reliability of the subscales, subscales, each addressing a different aspect of that adjustment — and
especially the personal/emotional adjustment subscale, which in the orig- with the assumption that a given student's adjustment may vary from one
inal version had coefficient alpha values in the .70s (see Cronbach, 1951, area to another. One and the same student, for example, may function
for a description of coefficient alpha, a measure of internal consistency relatively well academically and relatively poorly socially, or vice versa.
reliability). Coefficient alphas for the expanded version, for three samples Sometimes this intraindividual variation occurs to the extent that one and
from two colleges in an earlier study (Baker, McNeil, & Siryk, 1985), the same student in the present study could qualify as well adjusted in one
range between .84 and .88 for the academic adjustment subscale, between area and less well adjusted in another. Indeed, 13 students (2 men, 11
.90 and .91 for the social adjustment subscale, between .81 and .85 for women) had both high- and low-end scores.
the personal/emotional adjustment subscale, between .90 and .91 for the This overlapping is not a problem for the comparison of students rep-
attachment subscale, and between .93 and .95 for the full scale. resenting clear differences in effectiveness of adjustment to college as
Correlations among the subscales range from .36 to .87; the higher measured by the scale, that is, across distribution extremes, as long as the
values occur in the comparisons of the social adjustment and attachment focus is on one particular aspect of adjustment (i.e., one subscale), because
subscales, as would be expected because they share several common items. a student cannot be both high and low simultaneously on the same sub-
The range of correlations among the three subscales that do not share scale. However, in instances where the focus is on adjustment-to-college
common items is from .36 to .64. overall or in some general sense, it will be necessary to exclude overlap-
Regarding validity, statistically significant relations have been found pers because of their ambiguous status.
between the subscales and several criterion variables expected to be dif- Because the intent in the investigation of consequences of intervention
ferentially relevant to the subscales (Baker & Siryk, 1984). Criterion var- is to look at relatively well-adjusted and relatively less well-adjusted stu-
iables — representing important behaviors or accomplishments in the lives dents separately, the 13 overlappers were excluded in this analysis. Thus,
of students — include attrition, appeals for services from a psychological of the 77 students (24 men, 53 women) with only low-end scores, 39 (12
clinic, grade point average, election to an academic honor society, in- men, 27 women) were assigned to the interview condition and 38 (12
volvement in social activities, and outcome of application for dormitory men, 26 women) were assigned to the no-interview condition. Of the 73
assistant positions. As other indications of validity, the scale has been students (30 men, 43 women) with only high-end scores, 37 (15 men, 22
found to reflect predicted differences among black students in their ad- women) were assigned to the interview condition and 36 (15 men, 21
justment to predominantly white colleges as a function of differences in women) were assigned to the no-interview condition.
prior interracial experience (Graham, Baker, & Wapner, 1984), as well The students assigned to the interview condition were sent letters in
as predicted differences among students in the relation of prematriculation mid-November of 1981 requesting a follow-up individual meeting. It was
ADJUSTMENT TO COLLEGE 33

explained that they had a pattern of test results that we wanted to inves- pleted, the participant was asked whether if he or she were to take the
tigate further, and at the same time it was stated that feedback concerning subscale again that day, the score would be any different — higher, lower,
the questionnaire in general and their scores in particular would be offered. or about the same. Then the interviewer moved to another subscale and
Those not responding by returning a schedule card were sent follow-up repeated the foregoing procedure for presentation and discussion of scores
letters. Of the 39 low-end students invited for an interview, 36 (10 men, until all four subscales had been covered.
26 women) accepted; of the 37 high-end students invited, 31 (13 men, 18 Where there were indications of particular difficulties in the student's
women) accepted. adjustment to college, possible ways of dealing with them were explored,
The fact that 9 of the students originally assigned to the interview and in some instances specific advice, information, or recommendations
condition did not accept the invitation for an interview did not adversely were offered, such as where and from whom particular kinds of help might
affect the matching of the interview and no-interview groups. The low- be obtained. Students were also invited to ask for another interview at any
end interview and no-interview groups were now 27.8% and 31.6% male, time.
respectively, and the corresponding figures for the high-end were 41.9% The term cluster as used earlier and in the feedback to the student refers
and 41.7%. Furthermore, in no instance was there a significant difference to groups of items within a subscale that allude to a particular aspect of
by t test between the interview and no-interview groups within each dis- the adjustment area covered by the subscale, the grouping determined by
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tribution extreme on any of the four subscale scores or the full-scale score logical analysis on the part of the first author. The academic adjustment
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

from the first-semester testing. subscale had four such subsets: six items referred to motivation for being
The 67 interviewed students were each seen once for 1 to 2 hr between in college and for doing academic work; four bore on how well that
late November and mid-March. It should be mentioned here that although motivation was being translated into actual academic effort; nine related
a long-term goal of the overall research program is to provide the diag- to the efficacy or success of that effort in various aspects of performance;
nostic means for targeted interventions — that is, interventions specifically and five asked about satisfaction with the academic environment. Within
appropriate to needs as diagnosed, for example, in the academic, social, the social adjustment subscale there were also four clusters: extent and
or personal/emotional areas — the mode of intervention employed in this success of social involvement in general; relationships with other persons
exploratory study was relatively nonspecific in this regard. Accordingly, on campus; how one was dealing with being away from home and sig-
at the outset there was no anticipation of differential consequences of the nificant persons there; and satisfaction with the social aspects of the college
interview for the several subscale scores. environment. The personal/emotional adjustment subscale contained two
Moreover, the interview format was relatively unstructured at the be- clusters: psychological feeling-states and physical feeling-states. Finally,
ginning of the study and only over time evolved into a fairly standard the attachment subscale also had two clusters: satisfaction with being in
procedure that ultimately was as described in the following paragraphs. college in general and satisfaction with being at Clark in particular.
Over the whole course of the study, however, the basic nature of the In addition to the interview, on one occasion all members of the treat-
interview remained constant in its employment of adjustment-to-college ment group were sent printed material describing the research program,
scale scores as the central focus of discussion. Changes that did occur in particularly how it came about and what it was attempting to accomplish.
the mode of interview were ones of detail rather than of essential nature Students assigned to no-interview groups were neither contacted re-
and are not considered likely to have significant consequences for findings garding interviews nor sent printed materials.
except in one instance to reduce the amount of data available for a par- After all interviews had been conducted, the adjustment-to-college scale
ticular analysis. The fact that interviews were held over a period of several was distributed again during the 11th week of the second semester to all
months — thus making for different lengths of time between the initial students who had participated in the fall testing. It was completed and
testing and the interview and between the interview and the readministra- returned by 163 students (60 men, 103 women). Of the 36 students with
tion of the adjustment-to-college scale toward the end of the academic only low-end scores who were interviewed, 23 (8 men, 15 women) com-
year — could have consequences for findings, which are considered in pleted and returned the second-semester questionnaire; of the 38 students
the following section. with only low-end scores who had been assigned to the no-interview con-
Each interview was begun with an explanation of the basic structure of dition, 29 (9 men, 20 women) did so. Of the 31 students with only high-
the questionnaire, namely, that it comprised four subscales measuring end scores who were interviewed, 26 (12 men, 14 women) completed and
different aspects of adjustment to college. It was also explained that the returned the second questionnaire; of the 36 students with only high-end
interviewer's essential interest was in examining the degree of correspon- scores who had been assigned to the no-interview condition, 32 (14 men,
dence between the impression of a student yielded by the scale and what 18 women) did so.
had been happening in the student's adjustment to college. The interviewer As finally constituted, the low-end interview and no-interview groups
said that he would try to describe from the student's responses to the were now 34.8% and 31.0% male, respectively, and the corresponding
questionnaire how things had been going for him or her at the time the figures for the high-end were 46.2% and 43.8%. Chi-square analysis re-
questionnaire had been filled out, and the student was urged to feel free vealed no significant differences in composition by sex between the in-
to react at any time by disagreeing, agreeing, offering elaboration, or terview and no-interview groups within each distribution extreme. Nor
whatever. were there any significant differences by t test between those same groups
The means of selection of persons for interviews was described, thus on any of the first-semester adjustment-to-college scale measures (see the
informing the student that he or she had at least one subscale score on one Results section for these data). That is, the original matching of the groups
or the other extreme of a distribution. The interviewer specified a subscale was not impaired by the fact that some students did not accept the invi-
that had served as a basis for selection in the particular interviewee's tation to be interviewed or by the failure of some to return the second-
instance, reminded the interviewee what the subscale attempted to mea- semester questionnaire.
sure, and asked if he or she could identify the end of the distribution on
which the score fell (the asking of this question was begun about halfway
Results
through the series of interviews). The participant was then told the z score
(explained) for the subscale in question; then averages for certain clusters
of items (concerning various aspects of the adjustment area measured by Coefficient alphas for the first (N = 216) and second (TV =
the subscale) as well as individual item scores were given and discussed. 163) administrations of the scale, respectively, are full scale, .91
Special effort was made to draw relations and patterns among item and and .92; academic adjustment, .82 and .87; social adjustment, .88
cluster scores within the subscale. and .88; personal/emotional adjustment, .82 and .79; and attach-
When the presentation of information concerning a subscale was com- ment, .89 and .86.
34 ROBERT W. BAKER AND BOHDAN SIRYK

Although some of the reported findings are quantitative, others college or choice of major; dissatisfaction with course program,
are qualitative and even anecdotal because of the basically ex- particular courses, or professors; level of difficulty of the work
ploratory nature of the study. Those presented first take the form or, conversely for some, lack of challenge by coursework or lack
of observations made by the interviewer. of intellectual stimulation by peers. In the area of personal/emo-
tional adjustment, where items are couched largely in terms of
General Observations Regarding Utility of the Scale psychological or physical feelings-states, explanatory references
were made to the same kinds of issues as previously cited or to
problems with health or personal finances.
As judged from several years of use in developmental research
Explanations of how or why things were going well by and
as well as from the present study, the scale is well received and
large made reference to the same kinds of topics, but from a
well regarded by students. Not uncommonly students interpret its
positive slant, for example, favorable and supportive relationships
use as an indication that "someone cares," and express gratitude
with others, enjoyment of dormitory living, a sense of compati-
for that. The thought is also expressed sometimes that responding
bility with the community, enjoyment of new and different people
to scale items causes a student to think about himself or herself
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and experiences, a sense of academic purpose and accomplish-


in new and beneficial ways.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ment, and satisfaction with the academic aspects of college. One


A second general observation — one that assumes special sig-
high-end scorer told of not wanting to go home for vacation be-
nificance in light of the previously cited fact that students, even
cause of good feelings for college, and another told of being
very needy ones, are not likely to take advantage of voluntary
"lonesome for school" while home for winter vacation and look-
counseling programs — is that because the students previously
ing forward to returning to college.
completed and returned the scale, there is a legitimate and appar-
An interesting confirmation for the interviewer of the accuracy
ently fruitful basis for follow-up. If the scale accomplishes nothing
of the impression of a student yielded by scale data was a frequent
else, it provides an excuse for such access, and students respond
experience of pleasure, sometimes even exhilaration, during or
favorably in terms of numbers. In the present study, 86% of the
after interviews with well-functioning, high-scoring students.
students invited for an interview accepted (71 out of 83, including
overlappers), and that proportion would have been higher except When asked to identify the end of the distribution on which the
for scheduling difficulties in some instances. A problem still faced score fell for the subscale serving as the basis for selection for an
by users of such a scale, of course, is to get the student to complete interview, all but 1 of the 13 well-functioning, high-scoring stu-
and return it in the first place. dents so queried made correct identifications. The reader will re-
A third general observation is that the data from the scale made call that the interviewer began asking the question about halfway
possible an easy and comfortable approach to, and productive through the series of interviews. The misidentifier (selected on
discussion of, the topic of a student's adjustment to college. The the basis of a high score on the personal/emotional adjustment
subscale) explained — once he had gotten things clarified in mem-
provision of significant issues for discussion is no small advan-
ory — that shortly after completing the questionnaire he had sus-
tage, especially in those instances in which a student has been
experiencing difficulties and the subscale, item cluster, or indi- tained two incapacitating injuries adversely affecting his formerly
good spirits. He incorrectly recalled having completed the ques-
vidual item scores offer useful means of specifying and tracking
down the areas and sources of difficulty. tionnaire during the period of distress. (It should be kept in mind
in evaluating these data concerning recollected status at time of
testing that at least 4 weeks and sometimes more than 4 months
More Specific Observations and Quantitative Findings elapsed between completion of the scale and the interview.)
Concerning Utility: Correspondence Between Scale Data Thirteen of the 19 less well-functioning, low-scoring students
and Effectiveness of Adjustment to College made correct identifications. In all but 1 of the 6 instances of
misidentification it became clear in the course of the interview
The descriptions offered by the interviewer based on question- that the problem was one of recall and not one of inaccuracy of
naire responses were generally seen by students as meaningful and inference from scale data. That is, as the student discussed what
accurate representations of what had been going on in their lives had been going on in his or her life at the time the questionnaire
at college. Not only did students offer statements affirming the was filled out, the questionnaire responses were seen to be con-
appropriateness of such inferences but they also gave explanations sonant with those circumstances. For the 6th misidentifier, self-
of how or why things had been going poorly or well. They ac- described as "confused," as well as 1 of the others, the inter-
counted for low scores in the social area by telling about problems viewer had made a note in the course of discussion that the student
with making friends or keeping friends in general, or in particular, did not seem very self-aware.
problems with boyfriends, girlfriends, or roommates; difficulties Another example of correspondence between impressions from
with parents; ethnic, religious, or sociocultural differences with scale data and events in the life of the student comes from a
peers, sometimes regarding alcohol or other drugs or sexual be- discussion that the interviewer had with 1 of the members of the
havior; missing friends or relatives from home; geographical-cul- no-interview group a year after the interviewing had been com-
tural displacement, occasionally with reference to urban-rural pleted. That student told how she had come very close to trans-
differences; problems in living arrangements; and unavailability ferring in the second semester of her freshman year and how
of preferred extracurricular activities. indeed she had applied to another college and was accepted. She
Low scores in the academic area were explained as reflecting visited the other college for 3 days in the middle of March, came
problems with goal-setting, as in personal motivation for being in to the conclusion during the visit that "Clark wasn't so bad after
ADJUSTMENT TO COLLEGE 35

all," and decided not to transfer. Her z score on the attachment in the interview and no-interview groups. Male and female par-
subscale for the first semester testing was — 1.73 and her score ticipants are combined in Table 1 because there were no significant
was — 1.44 on the social adjustment subscale, in which area she sex differences in this interaction.
said her dissatisfaction lay. In the second-semester testing, which The reader will note in the left half of Table 1 that the scores
took place shortly after her visit to the other college, the scores of less well-adjusted students as a whole increase from the first
on the two subscales cited had improved to -0.34 and —0.56, to the second semester, to some extent very likely reflecting
respectively. regression to the mean in extreme scores. However, on four of
Two more instances of consistency between test data and other the five measures there are significant interactions indicating greater
behavior lend themselves to statistical test. The first concerns rate net increase, that is, greater improvement in adjustment, for the
of return of the second questionnaire toward the end of the aca- interviewed students. The increases in those four measures range
demic year. As might be expected for groups of students differing from 4.0% to 6.2% for the noninterviewed students; for the in-
in effectiveness of adjustment, a higher proportion of the students terviewed students, they range from 16.0% to 25.2%.
identified for the purposes of the present study as well adjusted For those students who had been identified as well adjusted in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(at least one subscale S + 1 SD and none 5 - 1 SD) returned the the first-semester testing, the noninterviewed students showed de-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

second questionnaire (82.2%; 60 out of 73) than of those identified clines on all five measures, whereas those who were interviewed
as less well adjusted (at least one subscale 5 — 1 SD and none showed either a smaller decline or a slight increase (see Table 1).
s + 1 SD) (67.5%; 52 out of 77). The chi-square analysis revealed None of the interaction terms were statistically significant, how-
the following results: x2 (1, N = 150) = 3.52, p = .06, two- ever, although the F value of 3.86 obtained for the full scale
tailed (a two-tailed test was employed in this instance because the closely approximates the value of 4.02 needed for significance at
direction of the difference had not been predicted in advance). the .05 level. Possibly the positive effects of interviewing are not
Also, consistent with findings in earlier studies regarding the limited to the less well-adjusted students. The general tendency
relation of scale data to attrition (Baker et al., 1985; Baker & toward decline may again be assumed to reflect regression to the
Siryk, 1984), a higher proportion of the less well-adjusted students mean.
(33.8%; 26 out of 77) than of the well-adjusted students (16.7%; The findings regarding change in scale scores from the first to
12 out of 72) had withdrawn from college as of the time of writing the second testings are paralleled for the less well-adjusted stu-
this article, shortly after the class's eighth semester, x2 (1, N = dents by data concerning withdrawal from college. As of the time
149) = 4.86, p < .05. (The sample size of the well-adjusted of writing, shortly after the end of the class's eighth semester,
group is reduced by 1 in this comparison because 1 member was 22.2% (8 out of 36) of the interviewed less well-adjusted students
a temporary foreign student who had intended to stay for 1 year had withdrawn, as compared with 44.7% (17 out of 38) of their
only and should not be regarded as a dropout.) If only the no- noninterviewed counterparts, x2- (1, N = 74) = 3.24, p < .05,
interview groups are considered, which would be a purer test of one-tailed. There is no difference in this variable between inter-
the relation of scale scores to attrition because possible effects of viewed and noninterviewed well-adjusted students; both groups
intervention would be set aside, the results are more dramatic. had drop-out rates of 16.7% (5 out of 30 and 6 out of 36, re-
The drop-out rate in the less well-adjusted no-interview sample spectively). Thus, for the less well-adjusted students, an interview
(44.7%; 17 out of 38) is almost three times the rate in the well- is not only associated with a significant increase in scale scores
adjusted no-interview sample (16.7%; 6 out of 36), •%* (1, N = but also with a decrease in dropping out of the college of original
74) = 5.55, p < .01, one-tailed. enrollment.
Data presented in the following section concerning change as a A final finding regarding consequences of intervention concerns
consequence of intervention can also be regarded as pertinent to the relation of the timing of the interview to the amount of change
the issue of correspondence between test findings and adjustment- in scale scores from pretesting to posttesting. For purposes of
related events in the life of the student. statistical analysis, the number of weeks that elapsed for each
student between the first testing and the interview was determined,
as was the change in scale scores. The latter variable — a differ-
ence score with constants added to eliminate negative values —
Consequences of Intervention was constructed in such a way that higher scores indicated more
beneficial outcome, that is, a relatively greater increase or rela-
Multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAS) for repeated mea- tively smaller decrease in score. For both the well-adjusted and
sures on the first- and second-semester adjustment-to-college scale less well-adjusted groups, the interviews occurred as soon as 4
scores (all four subscales) of interviewed and noninterviewed stu- weeks and as late as 19 weeks following the first-semester testing.
dents were performed separately for the groups identified as well Pearson product-moment correlations between number of weeks
adjusted or less well adjusted on the basis of the first-semester and magnitude of score change for the less well-adjusted students
testing. For the less well-adjusted group, the MANOVA resulted in were negative on all but one of the five adjustment measures (i.e.,
F(4, 47) = 2.55, p = .051. The corresponding analysis for the the four subscales and the full scale), but were statistically sig-
well-adjusted group yielded F(4, 53) = 1.89, p = .13. nificant only in the case of the attachment subscale, r(23) =
Results of univariate analyses of variance (ANOVAS) for each - .47, p < .05. Corresponding values for the well-adjusted stu-
of the four subscales and the full scale separately are presented in dents were all positive but again were significant only in the case
Table 1. The statistic of particular interest in these analyses is the of the attachment subscale, r(26) = .43, p < .05. Thus, there is
interaction term, which tells whether there is differential change some evidence that early follow-up is more advantageous for the
36 ROBERT W. BAKER AND BOHDAN SIRYK

Table 1
Analysis of Variance of Adjustment-to-College Scores for First- and Second-Semester Testings of Interviewed (I) and Noninterviewed
(NI) Freshmen Differing in Level of Adjustment on the First Testing

Adjustment Less well adjusted8 Well adjusted*1


measure n First semester Second semester F(\, 50) n First semester Second semester F(l,56)
Academic
subscale
I 23 127.26 133.61 26 163.04 158.04
0.31 1.72
NI 29 130.48 133.48 32 160.34 148.09
Social
subscale
I 23 103.43 129.48 26 136.88 141.88
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

8.68** 3.50
29 104.76 110.69 32
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

NI 144.78 140.53
Personal/
emotional
subscale
I 23 74.83 91.65 26 106.58 106.65
4.84* 2.83
NI 29 78.21 83.07 32 105.75 98.34
Attachment
subscale
I 23 82.83 99.61 26 112.00 111.85
4.60* 0.80
NI 29 83.17 86.48 32 114.72 111.19
Full
scale
I 23 353.70 410.39 26 471.00 471.35
6.15* 3.86
NI 29 361.62 378.21 32 475.19 450.78
Note. Higher scores indicate higher levels of adjustment. F values cited are for the interaction terms.
"6 - 1 SD on at least one subscale and S +1 SD on none. bS + 1 SD on at least one subscale and 6 - 1 SD on none.
*p < .05. **/> < .01.

less well-adjusted students, whereas later follow-up is more ad- for follow-up, and offers a means of approach to discussion of a
vantageous for the well-adjusted students. student's adjustment to college — carry promise for future use,
but there is also an important qualification to be stated. Those
observations represent perceptions and judgments on the part of
Discussion an interviewer who was also an author of the scale and therefore
subject to bias. Final judgment regarding these aspects of the
The facts that the original questionnaire return rate was only scale's utility must await either its use by other investigators with
about 40% of the class, that only students with extreme scores less personal commitment to the instrument or more systematic
were selected for interview and study, that students with both high collection of relevant data.
and low extreme scores were eliminated from some comparisons, The qualitative observations regarding degree of correspon-
that some students did not accept the invitation for interview and dence between inferences drawn from scale data and effectiveness
others did not return the second-semester questionnaire, and that of student adjustment to college are, of course, subject to the same
there is no basis for assuming this shrinkage is random indicate reservation. Descriptions offered by the interviewer based on
that the resulting samples of students may not be representative questionnaire responses were indeed generally accepted by stu-
of the class as a whole. However, the facts that we are interested dents as meaningful and accurate reflections of relevant events in
in generalizing only to students who are willing to return a ques- their lives at college, but again in the judgment of the same po-
tionnaire such as the one used here under circumstances similar tentially biased interviewer. Students did indeed flesh out their
to those of the present study, that in most analyses our focus of confirmatory reactions with descriptions and examples of their life
interest was on the comparison of students showing clear differ- circumstances in order to explain why the findings were as they
ences in various aspects of adjustment to college, and that in most were, but the fact that the interviews were not taped precluded
of the analyses matched groups were used and the matching was more careful analysis, and no other means were used to determine
not adversely affected by discontinuance by some participants limit incidence of confirming or disconfirming responses.
the negative consequences of any nonrandom shrinkage that did There was no question, either, of the elevation of the inter-
occur. viewer's mood while talking with high-scoring students whose
The general observations regarding the utility of the scale — lives seemed correspondingly full, meaningful, and satisfying.
that is, that it is favorably regarded by students, provides a basis However, this response on the part of the interviewer was, of
ADJUSTMENT TO COLLEGE 37

course, even more subjective than the rational observations pre- of a tendency on the part of the interviewer unwittingly to com-
viously mentioned. municate to the student his interest in having the student's ad-
The limitations of the anecdote cited in support of the corre- justment change for the better and the student's tendency to oblige
spondence issue are obvious. It really is simply another example or please this authority figure who was good enough to be inter-
of illustrative material offered by students in elaboration of the ested in his or her welfare. To the extent that this was true, the
relation of scale findings to events in their lives, as previously increase in score would reflect more of a verbal, superficial phe-
described, except that it was given by a member of the noninter- nomenon than a substantial change of personal status. However,
viewed group a year after completion of the study. the fact that an interview was also associated with less attrition,
In addition to using an interviewer with less potential bias toward a behavioral variable of considerable importance, would seem to
the scale, it may be desirable in future research concerning the controvert that interpretation.
correspondence issue to include studies in which the interviewer In conclusion, what is needed in future investigations of the
also was unaware of test findings and made judgments regarding utility of the scale in interview situations is more systematic and
effectiveness of student adjustment from interview data alone, to controlled means of comparing students who differ in effective-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

be matched later with test findings. ness of adjustment to college as measured by the scale to deter-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

The data regarding correspondence between scale scores and mine whether they indeed look different behaviorally or regard
other adjustment-relevant behaviors or life events become more themselves differently. Important in the accomplishment of this
quantitative in relation to four findings. The better adjusted stu- goal would be use of interviewers not biased in favor of the scale,
dents tended to have a higher rate of return of the second ques- possibly in some designs where the interviewer is unaware of the
tionnaire and had a lower rate of withdrawal from college than test findings, and preferably where there is less time lapse between
did the less well-adjusted students. Also, the less well-adjusted testing and interview. To explore whether the consequences of the
students who were interviewed had significantly higher scores on intervention in the present study are attributable to the contents
the second testing than did those who were not interviewed. Fi- of the interviews (i.e., feedback and discussion of questionnaire
nally, 78% of participants who were asked were able to correctly findings) and not simply to the fact of being interviewed, a control
identify the end of the distribution on which subscale scores fell, condition in which there are interviews but no discussion of scale
and the misidentifications of all but 1 of the remainder could be data could be employed. Of particular advantage would be re-
understood as errors of recall due in large part to passage of search showing predictable differential consequences of different
considerable time from testing to interview. kinds of interventions for the several subscales.
Taken all together, the several indications — both qualitative
and quantitative — of correspondence between scale data and
other adjustment-relevant behaviors or events in the life of the References
student can be seen as indications of validity for the measuring
instrument and of promise for its use in practical situations. Baker, R. W., McNeil, O. V., & Siryk, B. (1985). Expectation and reality
The results concerning the consequences of interviews provide in freshman adjustment to college. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
32, 94-103.
yet another example of beneficial effects of even relatively modest
Baker, R. W., & Nisenbaum, S. (1979). Lessons from an attempt to
interventions. Only one interview, approximately an hour in length
facilitate freshman transition into college. Journal of the American Col-
and designed as primarily information-giving rather than remedial, lege Health Association, 28, 79-81.
was associated not only with significantly higher scores on the Baker, R. W., & Siryk, B. (1984). Measuring adjustment to college.
adjustment scale but also in lower rates of dropping out of college. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 179-189.
One is tempted to wonder what may happen to scale scores and Beal, P. E., & Noel, L. (1980). What works in student retention (Joint
related behaviors with more elaborate interventions tailored to be project report, the American College Testing Program and the National
more specific to particular aspects of areas of adjustment to col- Center for Higher Education Management Systems). Iowa City, IA:
lege. Such a line of investigation could have considerable impli- American College Testing Program.
cations for both the utility and validity of the scale. Bednar, R. L., & Weinberg, S. L. (1970). Ingredients of successful treat-
The reader will recall, for example, that the only subscale not ment programs for underachievers. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
17, 1-7.
showing significant change after interview in the less well-ad-
Bloom, B. L. (1971). A university freshman preventive intervention pro-
justed group was the academic adjustment subscale. Quite pos- gram: Report of a pilot project. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
sibly that was due to the fact that the interview process itself, and Psychology, 37, 235-242.
especially the content of the particular interviews carried out, would Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of
more likely influence social or personal/emotional adjustment, or tests. Psychometrika, 16, 297-334.
attachment, than academic adjustment. Maybe an intervention de- Friedlander, J. (1980). Are college support programs and services reaching
signed to influence attitudes toward academic activity or to im- high-risk students? Journal of College Student Personnel, 21, 23-28.
prove academic skills would have more effect on a subscale Graham, C., Baker, R. W., & Wapner, S. (1984). Prior interracial ex-
measuring academic adjustment than on the other subscales. In perience and black student transition into predominantly white colleges.
the broader outlook, predicted differential effects on the subscales Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1146-1154.
Kipnis, D., & Resnick, J. H. (1971). Experimental prevention of under-
of interventions varying in degree of relevance to those subscales
achievement among intelligent impulsive college students. Journal of
could be of great value not only practically and psychometrically Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 36, 53-60.
but also heuristically. Kirshner, L. A. (1974). A follow-up of a freshman group counseling
It is possible to argue that the increase in scale scores after an program. Journal of the American College Health Association, 22, 279-
interview is less an indication of improvement in adjustment than 280.
38 ROBERT W. BAKER AND BOHDAN SIRYK

Lindquist, C. U., & Lowe, S. R. (1978). A community-oriented evalu- Wilson, T. D., & Linville, P. W. (1982). Improving the academic per-
ation of two prevention programs for college freshmen. Journal of formance of college freshmen: Attribution theory revisited. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 25, 53-60. Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 367-376.
Meichenbaum, D. H., & Smart, I. (1971). Use of a direct expectancy to
modify academic performance and attitudes of college students. Journal Received February 8, 1985
of Counseling Psychology, 18, 531-535. Revision received July 9, 1985 •
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

New Look for the APA Journals in 1986


Beginning with this issue, the APA Journals have a new look. All the journals will be 8'/4 x 11
inches — a little larger than the American Psychologist is now. This change in trim size will help
reduce the costs of producing the journals, both because more type can be printed on the larger
page (reducing the number of pages and amount of paper needed) and because the larger size
allows for more efficient printing by many of the presses in use today. In addition, the type size
of the text will be slightly smaller for most of the journals, which will contribute to the most
efficient use of each printed page.
These changes are part of continuing effort to keep the cost of producing the APA journals
down, to offset the escalating costs of paper and mailing, and to minimize as much as possible
increases in the prices of subscriptions to the APA journals.

You might also like