Ana Diaz PDF

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

Robotics and Autonomous Systems 151 (2022) 104033

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Robotics and Autonomous Systems


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/robot

Exploring factors influencing the acceptance of social robots among


early adopters and mass market representatives

Ulla A. Saari a,b , , Antero Tossavainen a , Kirsikka Kaipainen c , Saku J. Mäkinen a
a
Unit of Industrial Engineering and Management, Faculty of Management & Business, Tampere University, Korkeakoulunkatu 8, P.O. Box
541, 33014, Finland
b
Jönköping International Business School, Jönköping University, Gjuterigatan 5, 553 18, Jönköping, Sweden
c
Unit of Computing Sciences, Faculty of Information Technology and Communication Sciences, Tampere University, Korkeakoulunkatu 1, P.O. Box
553, 33014, Finland

article info a b s t r a c t

Article history: When designing social robots, it is crucial to understand the diverse expectations of different kinds
Received 21 September 2021 of innovation adopters. Different factors influence early adopters of innovations and mass market
Received in revised form 3 December 2021 representatives’ perceptions of the usefulness of social robots. The first aim of the study was to
Accepted 17 January 2022
test how applicable the technology acceptance model 3 (TAM3) is in the context of social robots.
Available online 26 January 2022
Participants’ acceptance of social robotics in a workplace environment in the fuzzy front-end (FFE)
Keywords: innovation phase of a robot development project was examined. Based on the findings for the model,
Robots we developed a reduced version of the TAM3 that is more applicable for social robots. The second
Technology acceptance model objective was to analyze how early adopters’ and mass market representatives’ acceptance of social
Early adopter robots differs. Quantitative research methods were used. For early adopters, result demonstrability has
Mass market representative a significant influence on perceived usefulness of social robots, while for mass market representatives,
Diffusion of innovations perceived enjoyment has a more significant influence on perceived usefulness. The findings indicate
Workplace
that users’ innovation adoption style influences the factors that users consider important in the
usefulness of social robots. Robot developers should take these into account during the FFE innovation
phase.
© 2022 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

1. Introduction is crucial to concentrate on the design of social robots’ func-


tionality, the human–robot interaction (HRI), and collaboration
Social humanoid robots are being gradually introduced in in workplace environments [11]. For developing these robots, it
workplaces as active partners assisting employees in such roles as is vital for robot developers to understand HRI to ensure that
reception assistants, shop assistants, waiters, and even personal it is possible for human workers to engage with social robots
assistants [1–5]. Thus, close collaboration with social robots in that are technically, socially, and emotionally sophisticated [1].
workplaces is creating completely new social situations that Research has also indicated that in some cases human workers
human workers may find challenging and difficult to accept favor collaborative and autonomous robots rather than controlled
[6,7]. The transformation and development of social robots into HRI [12].
more collaborative partners can also bring considerable change When social robots engage in social interaction with hu-
in workplaces by introducing more automation [8,9]. mans, the robots use anthropomorphic or human-like capabili-
Research on introducing robots in the workplace as co-workers ties, which are incorporated in robots’ physical design and be-
has shown that integrating robots in a team may cause some havior [13]. In workplace environments, as humans work with
issues, as human workers may have difficulties in identifying with social robots, the ways of interacting with robots go beyond
robots as teammates. These issues need to be taken into account the technical features to the extent that in the design phase
when organizations consider deploying robots in the workplace of social robots, developers should also understand human be-
in general [10]. Regarding the acceptance of robots at work, it havior, i.e., the expectations and reactions of different kinds of
users regarding robots, namely, factors outlined in technology
∗ Corresponding author at: Unit of Industrial Engineering and Management, acceptance models [14]. Therefore, it is critical that designers
Faculty of Management & Business, Tampere University, Korkeakoulunkatu 8,
and programmers of social robots understand HRI in collaborative
P.O. Box 541, 33014, Finland. contexts, how humans behave during the interaction, and how
E-mail address: ulla.saari@tuni.fi (U.A. Saari). robots can create experiences that build trust in robots [15].

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.robot.2022.104033
0921-8890/© 2022 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
U.A. Saari, A. Tossavainen, K. Kaipainen et al. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 151 (2022) 104033

Social humanoid robots deviate from earlier generations of robots The objectives of this study were to empirically test the TAM3
to that extent that they can be considered totally new products regarding acceptance of social robotics. Based on the findings,
and represent radical or disruptive innovations [5]. The actual we introduce a reduced version of the TAM3 that is more ap-
technical features of social robots are only one aspect among plicable for investigating acceptance of social robots. In addition,
the key factors impacting their acceptance. Research on social we sought to understand how the perceptions of early and late
robots in home environments has shown that perceived usability market segments of social robot users at workplaces differ. The
and enjoyment have a positive impact on the acceptance of research was conducted utilizing quantitative research methods.
robots [16]. There has been a call for more research on the The paper is divided into the following sections: Section 2
expectations that different kinds of users in varying situations presents the literature relevant to this research. Section 3 de-
and environments have for HRI when more people are familiar scribes the research setting and methodology. Section 4 reports
with robots, and they are no longer isolated cases; that is, when the results, and Section 5 discusses the relevance of the re-
mass market representatives use social robots more widely, for sults, presents the theoretical and practical implications of the
example, in workplaces [17]. study, and introduces the limitations and suggestions for future
The technology acceptance model (TAM; [18,19] has been research.
widely used and adapted to examine various kinds of technolo-
gies [20]. The TAM has been commonly applied in empirical 2. Related literature
technology acceptance research, and there are multiple variants
of the model. Some scholars consider the earlier versions too 2.1. The technology acceptance model (TAM)
simple and lacking crucial variables and behavioral processes
[14]. The third version of the TAM, i.e., the TAM3, is the latest The TAM was initially introduced by Davis [18,19] as an appli-
version of the model that has been mostly applied in IT projects. cation of Fishbein and Ajzen’s (1975) theory of reasoned action
However, its applicability in robotics has been researched less, (TRA). The TAM has been used to test various factors influencing
although in robotics there are also complex challenges, and the users’ acceptance of technology, with two important measures—
solutions should help users accomplish multiple complex tasks perceived ease of use (PEOU) and perceived usefulness (PU).
These constructs are defined by indicators that explain users’
[21]. Previous researchers have highlighted that if the TAM is
acceptance or rejection of new technology. The original TAM has
applied outside the original context where it was developed, the
been criticized for not offering tangible guidance to organizations
model should be re-validated and tested in the new context [22].
for developing technology that would engage users and promote
What has been lacking in the TAM is the perspective of social
new technology acceptance and adoption (e.g., [35]).
processes that consider individual dissimilarities between people
The first version of the TAM was extended to the TAM2 [36]
with unique cognitive or motivational drivers to use technol-
and the TAM3 [24]. The TAM3 is an integrated model of tech-
ogy [14]. However, the TAM includes factors that are relevant
nology acceptance where PEOU includes the following factors:
to workplace activities, attitudes toward technology, past ex-
computer self-efficacy, perception of external control, computer
perience of robots, trust, and perceived usefulness, specifically,
anxiety, computer playfulness, perceived enjoyment, and objec-
ease of use and the element of enjoyment [23,24]. There has
tive usability. The TAM3 includes the social influence and cogni-
been a call for more research on the influence of enjoyment on
tive instrumental processes, as well as perceived usefulness and
user acceptance of social robots [25]. Research on dimensions
its determinants, and the effects of subjective norm, image, job
of affect, such as enjoyment, in HRI has received less attention
relevance, output quality, and result demonstrability on perceived
than other HRI dimensions [26]. Especially when creating new
usefulness. The relationships added in the TAM3 include those
kinds of social robots for workplace use, it is important to test describing how experience moderates the relationships between
the products with different kinds of users in the early phases of PEOU and PU, between computer anxiety and PEOU, and between
development to understand their experiences of usefulness and PEOU and behavioral intention [24].
enjoyment. According to the theory of diffusion of innovations, Previous researchers have applied the TAM3, for example, to
users who represent early adopters and mass market users have analyze how factory workers perceive social robots and accept
different kinds of attitudes [27], and these attitudes should also them in the context of human–robot collaboration [37]. The main
be acknowledged when developing social robots. issues in this case were the employees’ fear of injury by a robot,
Research has focused on using the TAM to analyze users’ loss of job due to a robot, and general anxiety about robots
perceptions of teaching assistant robots and users’ intentions to in the workplace. To overcome these issues, it is important to
use these robots [28]. Recent research extended the TAM with analyze the use and acceptance of novel social robot applications.
measures of social responses, such as trusting [29]. In the hospi- Human–robot collaboration has been researched in industrial
tality sector, research has been conducted on visitors’ intentions production system contexts more than in the social contexts in
to use social robots in hospitality services [30]. In the workplace workplaces, such as in workplace reception areas or cafeterias
context, research has recently been conducted on risk caretakers’ [38].
perceived risk regarding adopting and accepting social robotic In the case of social robots, the studies on HRI have mostly
solutions in care facilities for older adults [31]. There has been a applied the unified theory of acceptance and use of technology
call for more research on different kinds of human–robot relation- (UTAUT) [39] to analyze the acceptance of social robots in the
ships and interactions, in various spheres of people’s everyday fields of health care and education [5,40]. However, the UTAUT
life, including workplaces [32]. There is also scant research on the and the UTAUT2 [41] incorporate such factors that are not nec-
acceptance of robots outside the social and health care fields [23]. essarily relevant for a workplace, such as the constructs effort
There are still issues with technology acceptance of social robots, expectancy and price value [21]. Therefore, the TAM3 can be
especially in contexts where there is radical change due to the considered to be better suited than the UTAUT2 model for test-
introduction of social robotics in workplaces [23,33,34]. To our ing social robots in workplaces due to the incorporation of the
knowledge, the TAM3 has not been applied among early adopters following constructs: job relevance, output quality, result demon-
and mass market representatives to compare the different dimen- strability, self-efficacy, anxiety, playfulness, and enjoyment [24].
sions of technology acceptance to provide guidance to social robot However, application of the TAM3 in business contexts has been
developers in the front-end phase of development with a tested limited, and application of the TAM3 in different types of IT
and applicable model for social robots. settings has been called for [24,35].
2
U.A. Saari, A. Tossavainen, K. Kaipainen et al. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 151 (2022) 104033

2.2. Acceptance of social robots people will encounter them more in their everyday lives. The
development process of social robots requires testing in the fuzzy
As most people do not interact in their everyday lives with front-end (FFE) phase in addition to later testing and prototyping,
social robots, they are still considered radical innovations by the to develop HRI scripts. Therefore, it is critical for researchers
people who interact with them. Thus, when people interact with to examine the interaction in various contexts with different
robots, they are assessing the robots to decide whether to accept kinds of users and levels of interest in innovative technology
and adopt them [5,17]. Social robots can be differentiated from [17]. People assume social robots have the capacity to engage
other IT technologies due to their capability to conduct varying in social interaction that is meaningful, which requires from
levels of physical interaction with objects or humans via verbal robot developers understanding of the basics of anthropomor-
or non-verbal communication modes [42]. Therefore, there is phic, i.e., human-like features that the robots should portray. This
reason to question whether the technology acceptance models should be translated in HRI or human–computer interaction (HCI)
designed for IT technologies are fully applicable to social robots. that meets the user’s expectations for the robot’s performance.
In general, it has been found that people have an ‘‘anthropocentric However, the ideal target is not to create a synthetic human, but
expectancy bias’’ when communicating with human counterparts, a balanced solution that persuades humans to trust the robotic
and this expectation is used when interacting and judging the systems [13].
behavior of humanoid robots [17].
Previous research on HRI focused on prototyping. However, 2.3. Early adopters and mass market users of technology
as social robots are also designed for the mass market, different
kinds of HRI models that go beyond the stereotypes of how There have been high expectations among companies develop-
current versions of social robots interact must be developed and ing social robots that potential future users in the mass market
applied [17]. According to the computers as social actors (CASA) would follow enthusiast early adopters and acquire positive at-
paradigm [43], expectations for and acceptance of new tech- titudes toward robots as users, have personal experiences and
nology used in social robots can differ from the expectations see more robots in action in their own familiar environments
people normally have for new technology due to the element of [56]. However, research results suggest that although there may
social interaction [44]. With reference to the CASA paradigm, the be positive interest in robots, the possible negative impact they
presumption is that people perceive robots to be social actors may have in the form of job loss could generate more negative
with identities, which has led developers to try to incorporate perceptions of the robots’ actual usefulness [57]. Gnambs and Ap-
social elements in robots for people who are not comfortable pel [58] found that in recent years a negative and more cautious
with using technology [43,44]. Especially in the case of socially orientation toward robots has grown, especially in workplace
interactive robots, recognizing them as hedonic systems has been environments, and even more so toward surgical robots and
thought important, therefore the construct of perceived enjoy- autonomously driving vehicles.
ment is considered to have an impact on technology acceptance When considering the adoption of new technology, there is an
[45–47]. As the safety and trustworthiness requirements are high interplay of contextual elements impacting people in
for social robots as well, the quality of the HRI must be well workplaces—internally, externally, and from the technological
developed, technically, and on the level of social skills [5,48]. perspective [59]. Thus, the adoption process and diffusion of
Before social robots can be adopted more widely in new en- innovations among different kinds of user groups can be very
vironments and workplaces, such as offices, public buildings, and different, depending on the type of person. The theory of diffusion
stores, the challenges related to implementing social interaction of innovation [27] has been used predominantly to explain the
between social robots and humans must be understood in more adoption of technology in organizations and the diffusion process
depth. According to Yang et al. [49], there are three considerable at the market level among different kinds of adopters. A market
challenges related to the development of social robots: the way for a specific product can be categorized into market segments
social dynamics is constructed, the learning process of social based on the type of adoption rate of the main type of consumers
norms, and the creation of a robotic mind theory. Especially in or users in that segment. The adopter categorization has been
the case of social interaction that can be very complex, research based on the innovativeness of the ideal types in the specific cate-
remains scant, including quantitative analysis of human social gory. In the process of the diffusion of innovations, early adopters
behavior toward new kinds of social robots in new contexts. are the first to adopt an innovation, and later, they are followed
In the past decade, there has been an increase in the mar- by users who can be labeled mass market representatives [27,60].
ket introduction of social robots that are socially and physically According to Rogers [27] classification, there are five ideal
responsive units that can perform human tasks previously con- types of adopter categories: Innovators are among the first 2.5%
sidered possible only by humans [5]. The adoption of healthcare of users to accept an innovation. Early adopters consist of ap-
robots has become an active area of research [50]. In the con- proximately 13.5% of users who serve as role models for other
text of medical and healthcare services, social robots have been followers in a local system. The early majority (next 34%), late
designed to keep the elderly company and handle deliveries of majority, (34%), and laggards (16%) can be categorized as later
medication and meals in hospitals (e.g. [51]). In addition, there adopters who accept innovations when their peers have adopted
has been research on the adoption of robotic-assisted surgery by the innovation [27]. As the adopter categorization is based on
medical personnel [52], and older adults’ acceptance of assistive ideal types, it can vary for different technologies and products
robots [46]. In the field of education, some teaching tasks have [61]. Therefore, the innovator type of adopters in IT may not be
been introduced to social robots [40,53,54]. In the context of the same for other product categories, for example, social robots.
students with intellectual disabilities, employees’ and students’ Even early adopters have been found to share characteristics with
expectations for the robots have been examined [55]. early rejecters, for example, in the case of acceptance of solar
As the field of social robotics has been developing, novel energy systems [62].
humanoid social robots have been launched that can comprehend The market for social robotics is considered to have growth
and interact as humans that are anticipated to be pervasively potential, as robots are starting to appear in public buildings
incorporated in people’s everyday lives as social agents in dif- and service sectors, for example, in education, health and social
ferent situations [33]. As there will be more social robots in care, domestic services, transportation, and delivery services [63].
different contexts, especially in workplaces and public buildings, Although social robots can operate usefully in different social
3
U.A. Saari, A. Tossavainen, K. Kaipainen et al. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 151 (2022) 104033

contexts, people accept the robots based on personal perspectives


that are not easy to decipher [23]. The introduction of social
robots in new usage contexts in workplaces will set more de-
mands and expectations for the level of sophistication for HRI
[15]. When designing a social robot, robot developers do not
primarily aim for possible resemblance to human form or func-
tionalities, as the robot is inanimate. Instead, the primary focus is
usefulness. However, there is a need to create socially intelligent
features in social robots that respond to humans’ expectations
to facilitate interaction between people and robots [13]. How-
ever, expectations may vary between user groups, depending on
the degree to which the people are interested in and willing
to use new innovative solutions, which is linked to individual
innovativeness [27].
Social robotics research has focused on the way social robots
could be designed, for example, for improving the well-being of
vulnerable consumer groups, such as children and older adults
[64], to assist in remote rehabilitation in areas where there is a
shortage of rehabilitation clinicians and the special circumstances
during the ongoing COVID-19 pandemic [65,66], to assist in men-
tal health telemedicine configurations in low- and middle-income
countries without sufficient clinicians [67], and group therapy in
elderly care [68]. Developers must still tackle the challenge of Fig. 1. The TAM3 [24] adapted to social robots.
introducing new social humanoid robots equipped with artificial
intelligence (AI) capabilities to the mass market for wide-scale
benefit. With the current trends, there could be an opportunity to 3.2. Research design and experiment setup
exploit and launch more social robots that could serve different
kinds of users, so that HRI would be normalized and common. A social robot system based on a social humanoid robot called
In addition, in the wider deployment of robots outside industrial ‘‘Pepper’’ launched by SoftBank Robotics in 2015 was under fur-
contexts, social robots must be perceived as useful and reliable ther test development and evaluation at a university laboratory.
before they will be fully accepted [58,69]. According to the company’s website, ‘‘Pepper’’ has been adopted
in approximately 2000 companies as an assistant in reception
areas to welcome, inform, and guide visitors. The robot has been
3. Research setting and methodology ‘‘optimized for human interaction and is able to engage with
people through conversation and his touch screen’’ [70]. Newer
3.1. Research method versions of Pepper deploy AI for emotion recognition in their
interactions with humans [71].
The robot development team conducted an experiment in
The literature review showed that previous researchers did
March 2018 outside the laboratory in the university hallway,
not apply the TAM3 to test the adoption of social robot technol- which students, staff, and visitors pass through to go to the cam-
ogy in the early fuzzy front-end innovation phase. To explore how pus cafeteria. Participants who showed interest in the social robot
applicable the TAM3 is for analyzing the adoption of social robots were given a task to check the lunch menus of the university
on the markets, this study focused on early adopters and mass cafeteria presented by the robot prototype created by the devel-
market representatives according to Rogers [27] classification. To opment team. The participants were instructed to interact with
evaluate the acceptance of technology, we used the TAM3 [24]. In the robot either by speaking out loud or by touching the screen
addition to helping analyze the propensity to accept a technology, with the user interface. After the participants had performed the
the TAM allows to understand the factors that impact acceptance. tasks as instructed, they were asked to fill out a questionnaire.
The main factors of the TAM, perceived ease of use and per-
ceived usefulness of technology, are critical for understanding 3.3. Participants
how employees in workplaces perceive social robots. We re-
worded the determinants for perceived ease of use of computers Data were collected with the questionnaire from 132 end-
so that they refer to social robots: Computer self-efficacy was user participants. All participants had interacted with the robot
modified to robot self-efficacy, computer anxiety to robot anxiety, as instructed. Questionnaires that were not filled out properly
(n = 11) were omitted from the final analysis. The participants
and computer playfulness to robot playfulness [cf. 24, p. 279].
were divided into two market segments based on their answers to
The modified TAM3 for social robots is based on the relationships
questions regarding their individual innovativeness: early market
in Venkatesh and Bala [24], and there are no crossover effects
representatives, consisting of early adopters and innovators, and
between the constructs PEOU and PU; that is, they do not in- mass market representatives, who included Rogers [27] other cat-
fluence each other (Fig. 1). The following TAM3 constructs were egorizations of innovation adopters (early majority, late majority,
not included in this study: job relevance (the context of the test and laggards) (see Table 1).
was not relevant to the job of the people participating in the In the dataset, there are only a few instances where the re-
experiment), perceptions of external control (the robot used in spondents have not revealed their education (2 respondents) or
the experiment was not tested as a part of a system, and thus, occupation (1 respondent), and this is not seen as creating any
was not under external control), and voluntariness (the study bias in the reporting. For the gender, there are 19 respondents
participants were volunteers). who ‘‘Prefer not to say’’ their gender (15.7%), but the majority
4
U.A. Saari, A. Tossavainen, K. Kaipainen et al. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 151 (2022) 104033

Table 1 4. Results
Participants’ sociodemographic information (N = 121).
Gender n Percentage 4.1. Partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM)
Male 55 45.5%
Female 47 38.8%
To evaluate the adapted TAM3 with the collected data on
Prefer not to say 19 15.7%
social robots, we utilized partial least squares structural equation
Educational degree
modeling (PLS-SEM) [76]. PLS-SEM is a multivariate data analysis
No university degree 44 36.4%
B.Sc. 44 36.4% method widely applied in the social and behavioral sciences [77],
M.Sc. 24 19.8% as well as in information systems research [78]. PLS-SEM is often
PhD 7 5.8% employed when the assessed model is complex [77]. In addition,
Prefer not to say 2 1.7% in confirmatory and explanatory research, a measurement model
Occupation is estimated by focusing on the assessment of the constructs
Student 52 43.0%
in a structural model. According to Hair et al. [79, p.23], the
Employed 54 44.6%
Student and employed 14 11.6% PLS-SEM approach is a suitable method when the objective of
Prefer not to say 1 0.8% the study is to predict, either the target constructs or what are
Age the main constructs influencing the target constructs. PLS-SEM
<20 years 6 5.0% can also be used when a structural model is complex, including
20–29 years 65 53.7% many constructs and many indicators, as we have in the Fig. 1
30–39 years 26 21.5% model. In addition, PLS-SEM is suitable when the sample size
40–49 years 18 14.9%
50–59 years 6 5.0%
is small and in case the data are nonnormally distributed. The
overall complexity of a structural model has little influence on
Adopter category
Early adopter 39 32.2% the sample size requirements for PLS-SEM [79]. The sample size
Mass market representative 82 67.8% recommendations for PLS-SEM to achieve a statistical power of
80% for constructs that have a maximum of 4 arrows (inde-
pendent variables) pointing at it (which is the case both in the
more elaborate version (Fig. 1) and reduced version (Fig. 3) of
have reported their gender (45.5% are male, and 38.8% female). the model) are the following: The required sample size for a
To our understanding, in this case the division of the respondents
significance level of 1% and minimum R2 0.25 is 58, and for R2
into three categories under the gender option is not critical, as in
0.50 is 24; and with a significance level of 10% the minimum R2
the recent years, there has been increasing discussion on genders
0.25 requires a sample size of 34 and R2 0.50 requires a sample
not being distributed only into two genders. Recent research
size of 15 [79, p.26]. This indicates that the sample sizes we have
guidelines even urge to go beyond a dichotomous view of the
used for testing the models in this paper are more than sufficient
concepts of gender when reporting the participants’ background
for an exploratory study.
[72]. As the main criteria in the comparison is based only on the
We used SmartPLS 3 software to estimate the model [80]. In
innovativeness of the respondents, we are not explicitly studying
the significance testing, we applied the bootstrapping procedure
gender or gender differences, therefore we are merely describ-
with 5000 samples. We checked the measurement models and
ing the demographic characteristics of the sample so that other
then focused on the structural model [77].
researchers can evaluate the possible representativeness of the
The data were analyzed for three data sets: all participants
research.
(N = 121), early adopters (EA; n = 39; 32.2%), and mass market
3.4. Variables in the TAM representatives (MM; n = 82; 67.8%). Participants who responded
5 or higher for the variable indicating willingness to adopt in-
All the constructs in the TAM3 [24] adapted for social robots novative products were grouped as EA, and participants who
(Fig. 1) and for categorizing participants according to market responded <5 were grouped as MM. A 7-point Likert scale was
segmentation [27,73] were estimated with multiple measure- used for response options for the variable (where 5 is ‘‘Somewhat
ment items in reflective measurement models [74]. Based on agree’’, 6 ‘‘Moderately agree’’, and 7 ‘‘Strongly agree’’).
the model, we used a structured questionnaire that covered the
constructs and the underlying measurement items (Table 2). The 4.2. Assessment of the measurement model based on the original
responses on a 7-point Likert scale ranged from strongly disagree TAM3 adapted to robots
(1) to strongly agree (7). Some of the measurement items in
the TAM3 were adapted and modified based on the experiment In the assessment of the measurement model, we used proce-
setting. Some items were removed due to their irrelevance to dures described by Hair et al. [77,79]. When assessing reflective
the experiment. The questionnaire was also translated into the measurement models, the following criteria are checked: item
local language of the university to ensure that the sample in- reliability, internal consistency (composite reliability), conver-
cluded visitors and non-academic staff from the university in gent validity (average variance extracted; AVE), and discriminant
addition to students. The major change in the original TAM3 validity (heterotrait–monotrait ratio of correlations; HTMT). To
questionnaire topics concerned rewording of items that refer to confirm item reliability, the item loadings are checked to verify
‘‘computer’’ or ‘‘system’’ to ‘‘robot’’. Another important change the variance shared between individual item variables and the
was the addition of more detailed instructions for the tasks for construct. The threshold value for item loadings in a reflective
the participants. The TAM3 items omitted from the questionnaire measurement model is .70 [76].
referred to constructs that were not relevant for this study. The loadings for the following items measuring the constructs
During the analysis of the results, the participants were di- result demonstrability (RES), robot self-efficacy (CSE), and robot
vided into two categories, early adopters and mass media rep- playfulness (CPLAY) were below .40: RES4, CSE3, CSE4, and
resentatives, based on their innovativeness. The market split was CPLAY4, which required that they be removed from the model as
based on an item (‘‘I’m often first to adopt an innovative product’’) they were not necessary for measuring the applicability of TAM3
measuring the participants’ attitudes toward new technologies in the case of social robots. The constructs RES, CSE, and CPLAY
[27,73,75]. were explained by the remaining items. After we eliminated the
5
U.A. Saari, A. Tossavainen, K. Kaipainen et al. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 151 (2022) 104033

Table 2
Item loadings for the measurement models adapted to robots for the three groups.
Construct Item Loading All (N = 121) Loading EA (n = 39) Loading MM (n = 82)
Subjective Norm People who influence my behavior think that I should .774 .698 .805
experiment with the robot. (SN1)
People who are important to me think that I should try out .927 .918 .936
the robot. (SN2)
Image People in my organization who use the robot have more .793 .716 .853
prestige than those who do not. (IMG1)
People in my organization who use the robot have a high .815 .900 .780
profile. (IMG2)
Testing the robot is a status symbol in my organization. .729 .871 .551
(IMG3)
Output Quality The quality of the output I get using the robot is high. (OUT1) .841 .753 .869
I have no problem with the quality of the robot’s output. .807 .829 .810
(OUT2)
I rate the results from the robot to be excellent. (OUT3) .904 .919 .879
Result Demonstrability I have no difficulty telling others about the results of using .637 .728 .573
the robot. (RES1)
I believe I could communicate to others the consequences of .736 .752 .709
using the robot. (RES2)
The results of using the robot are apparent to me. (RES3) .814 .877 .803
Robot Self-Efficacy I could investigate the lunch menus using the .908 .909 .926
robot . . .
. . . if there was no one around to tell me what to do as I
used the robot. (CSE1)
. . . if I had just the robot’s built-in help facility for assistance. .837 .875 .785
(CSE2)
Robot Anxiety Working with a robot makes me nervous. (CANX2) .899 .813 .908
Robots make me feel uncomfortable. (CANX3) .873 .703 .896
Robots make me feel uneasy. (CANX4) .830 .912 .822
Robot Playfulness The following questions ask you how you would characterize .816 .830 .561
yourself when you use robots:
. . spontaneous (CPLAY1)
. . . creative (CPLAY2) .915 .858 .991
. . . playful (CPLAY3) .834 .897 .463
Perceived Enjoyment I find using the robot to be enjoyable. (ENJ1) .886 .941 .863
The actual process of using the robot is pleasant. (ENJ2) .918 .940 .936
I have fun using the robot. (ENJ3) .729 .899 .485
Perceived Usefulness Using the robot improves my performance in my task .842 .872 .821
investigating the lunch menus. (PU1)
Using the robot enhances my effectiveness in my task .844 .824 .842
investigating the lunch menus. (PU3)
I find the robot to be useful in my task investigating the .891 .868 .902
lunch menus. (PU4)
Perceived Ease of Use My interaction with the robot is clear and understandable. .769 .842 .702
(PEOU1)
Interacting with the robot does not require a lot of my mental .632 .729 .579
effort. (PEOU2)
I find the robot to be easy to use. (PEOU3) .798 .827 .797
I find it easy to get the robot to do what I want it to do. .830 .903 .777
(PEOU4)
Behavioral Intention Assuming the robot would be permanently at the setup under .905 .887 .909
testing . .
. . . I would use the robot if it would be accessible. (BI1)
. . . I might use the robot if it would be accessible. (BI2) .884 .893 .874
. . . I would plan to use the robot in the future. (BI3) .908 .879 .916

All = All participants; EA = Early adopters; MM = Mass market representatives.

items with loading below .40, only two items had loadings that values for most of the items were within the recommendation of
were clearly below the critical threshold. Thus, the model had .60–.90, with only a few exceptions. A few of the CR values for
sufficient reliability. We kept the two items that had loadings isolated cases were slightly above .90, but they were still below
under .70, as they were still close to .60 which is also considered the ultimate recommended threshold of .95 (Table 3).
acceptable [79]. The loadings for all items in the measurement The AVE criteria were used to assess convergent validity of
model were acceptable for all groups (Table 2). reflective constructs. In the TAM3, the AVE values were above
Instead of using Cronbach’s alpha for assessing internal con- the threshold of .50, and only for one item measuring result
demonstrability was the AVE value slightly below .50 (.492 for
sistency reliability, we used composite reliability (CR) to assess
the MM group). In this case, the value is acceptable to keep the
the internal consistency reliability of the reflective constructs,
item (Table 3).
because it is not as sensitive to the number of measurement To measure discriminant validity, we used the HTMT ratio of
items on the scale [79]. The threshold for acceptable CR values correlations [81]. In the HTMT, discriminant validity is verified, as
is between .60 and .70, while CR values between .70 and .90 are nearly all HTMT values are below the <.85 requirement which is
satisfactory. CR values that are above .90 or even .95 indicate that the conservative threshold value. Only for Result Demonstrability
the item variables measure a similar aspect; thus, one of the items -> Perceived Enjoyment was the value slightly above 0.85, but
is redundant and not recommended as a measure of the same still clearly below .95. For Perceived Ease of Use -> Behavioral
construct [79]. In the case of the TAM3 adapted for robots, the CR Intention, the HTMT value for the EA group was above .95.
6
U.A. Saari, A. Tossavainen, K. Kaipainen et al. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 151 (2022) 104033

Table 3
Composite reliability (CR) and average variance extracted (AVE) for the constructs.
Construct CR - All CR - EA CR - MM AVE - All AVE - EA AVE - MM
Subjective Norm .842 .796 .865 .729 .665 .763
Image .823 .871 .778 .608 .694 .547
Output Quality .888 .874 .889 .725 .699 .728
Result Demonstrability .775 .830 .741 .537 .622 .492
Robot Self-efficacy .865 .886 .848 .763 .796 .737
Robot Anxiety .901 .853 .908 .753 .662 .768
Robot Playfulness .891 .897 .732 .733 .744 .504
Perceived Enjoyment .884 .948 .820 .720 .859 .619
Perceived Usefulness .895 .891 .891 .739 .731 .732
Perceived Ease of Use .845 .896 .808 .579 .685 .517
Behavioral Intention .927 .917 .927 .809 .786 .810

All = All participants; EA = Early adopters; MM = Mass market representatives.

Table 4
Path coefficients (β ) and significance levels (p-value) for the three groups.
Path All β All p EA β EA p MM β MM p
Image > Perceived Usefulness .264 <.001 .299 .023 .229 .041
Output Quality -> Perceived Usefulness .412 <.001 .509 .034 .440 .001
Perceived Ease of Use -> Behavioral Intention .119 .228 .285 .159 .048 .644
Perceived Enjoyment -> Perceived Ease of Use .574 <.001 .661 <.001 .413 <.001
Perceived Usefulness -> Behavioral Intention .563 <.001 .396 .009 .607 <.001
Result Demonstrability -> Perceived Usefulness .262 .002 .496 <.001 .169 .128
Robot Anxiety -> Perceived Ease of Use −.194 .023 −.109 .540 −.306 .004
Robot Playfulness -> Perceived Ease of Use −.187 .053 −.108 .483 −.043 .817
Robot Self-Efficacy -> Perceived Ease of Use .151 .050 .193 .129 .118 .259
Subjective Norm -> Behavioral Intention .109 .183 .195 .170 .075 .494
Subjective Norm -> Image .400 <.001 .409 .042 .398 <.001
Subjective Norm -> Perceived Usefulness −.019 .815 .008 .953 −.041 .706

EA = Early adopters; MM = Mass market representatives.

4.3. Assessment of the structural model based on the original TAM3

In assessing the structural model, we followed the procedures


described by Hair et al. [77,79]. We checked the following criteria
first: collinearity with the variance inflation factor (VIF), the sig-
nificance, and relevance of the path coefficients. For checking the
VIF values, a bootstrapping procedure was run with 5000 rounds
for all the data sets individually. All VIF values clearly passed the
<5.00 criterion set as a threshold; therefore, collinearity is not a
critical issue for this model [79].
The three groups (all respondents, EA, and MM) were sim-
ilar and had significant paths (p < .05) between the following
constructs: Image -> Perceived Usefulness, Output Quality -> Per-
ceived Usefulness, Perceived Enjoyment -> Perceived Ease of Use,
Perceived Usefulness -> Behavioral Intention, Subjective Norm -
> Image, and Robot Self-Efficacy -> Perceived Ease of Use. The
path coefficients differed for the EA and MM groups in the case of
the following paths: Significant for EA: Result Demonstrability ->
Perceived Usefulness, and Negatively significant for MM: Robot
Anxiety -> Perceived Ease of Use. The path coefficients were not
significant for any of the groups for the following paths: Perceived
Ease of Use -> Behavioral Intention, Perceived Ease of Use ->
Perceived Usefulness, Robot Playfulness -> Perceived Ease of Use,
Subjective Norm -> Behavioral Intention, and Subjective Norm -> Fig. 2. Path coefficients and R2 values for all participants (N = 121). Note: ***
Perceived Usefulness (Fig. 2; Table 4). p < .05, ns. = non-significant.

The values of the coefficients of determination (R2 ) for the


three data sets were verified to estimate the predictive power
of the model. When R2 values are assessed, the guideline is that
the values depend on the level of complexity in the model and size, that can be used to assess the level of impact the deleted
the field. In marketing research, R2 values that are closer to .75 construct has on another endogenous construct. Effect size values
refer to substantial predictive power, those closer to .50 refer to of 0.02, 0.15, and 0.35 are considered respectively small, medium,
moderate predictive power, and those closer to .25 refer to weak and large [79]. The effect sizes (f 2 ) were analyzed for the three
predictive power [79]. The average predictive power is moderate data sets, are consistent with the other criteria reported above.
for nearly all constructs except for image (Table 5). See Table 6.
In case an exogeneous construct is left out from a model, The Q 2 values for the endogenous constructs (behavioral in-
the resulting change in the R2 value is referred as the f 2 effect tention, image, perceived ease of use, and perceived usefulness)
7
U.A. Saari, A. Tossavainen, K. Kaipainen et al. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 151 (2022) 104033

Table 5
Coefficients of determination (R2 ) values for all participants, early adopters, and
mass market representatives.
Construct All participants EA MM
Behavioral Intention .437 (**) .500 (**) .415 (**)
Image .160 .167 .158
Perceived Ease of Use .450 (**) .551 (**) .424 (**)
Perceived Usefulness .430 (**) .619 (**) .352 (*)

Fig. 4. Path coefficients and R2 values for mass market representatives (MM;
n = 82), early adopters (EA; n = 39). Note: ***p < 0.05, ns. = non-significant.

model. Items that have loadings under 0.70 can also considered
be acceptable [79]. The loadings for all the items in the mea-
surement model were now acceptable for all constructs in the
reduced model, for all respondents, including the EA and MM
Fig. 3. Conceptual model for a reduced TAM3 adapted to social robots with path
groups (Table 7). For only Perceived Enjoyment (ENJ3) and Result
coefficients and R2 values for all participants (N = 121). Note: *** p < .05, ns. = Demonstrability (RES1), the loadings for All were slightly below
non-significant. .70, and for the MM group the loadings were slightly below .50
and .60, respectively. However, these values are still acceptable
(Table 8).
were all above zero, which is an indication of the predictive rele- The reliability and convergent validity were evaluated by ver-
vance of the latent variables. However, in the case of the q2 effect ifying the AVE and CR values for the three groups. All but one
sizes, for path coefficients that did not have significant values, the of the AVE values for the reduced model were above the critical
q2 effect sizes were below the recommended threshold criteria. threshold value of .50: For the MM group, Result Demonstrability
was slightly below .50. Most of the items fulfilled the requirement
4.4. New reduced TAM3 for social robots for internal consistency, and the CR was not higher than the
.90 requirement. Only four of the items were slightly higher;
4.4.1. Conceptual model however, they were not higher than .95 (Table 9).
Due to results above indicating that the TAM3 could not be Discriminant validity was established, as all but one of the
fully adapted to social robots, we removed the path coefficients HTMT values were well below the. 85 requirement. Only for
that were not significant. Based on the findings from the testing of Result Demonstrability -> Perceived Enjoyment was the value
the TAM3 adapted to social robots, we reduced the TAM3 to a new slightly above .875.
model in which the following paths were removed: Perceived
Ease of Use -> Perceived Usefulness, Perceived Ease of Use -> 4.4.3. Assessment of the structural model of the reduced TAM3 for
Behavioral Intention, Subjective Norm -> Behavioral Intention, social robots
and Subjective Norm -> Perceived Usefulness. All the VIF values are well below 5.00 criterion; the highest
The following constructs were also removed: perceived ease value was 2.688. Therefore, collinearity is not a critical issue for
of use, as it did not have a significant influence on behavioral the reduced model [79]. When the model was tested with all par-
intention for any of the groups, and robot anxiety and robot ticipants data set and the MM data set, the path coefficient Result
playfulness, for which the effect sizes on perceived ease of use Demonstrability -> Perceived Usefulness was the only one that
were small, or there was no effect. The constructs linked to was not significant. However, when the model was tested with
perceived ease of use are instead linked to perceived usefulness the EA data set, the path coefficient for Result Demonstrability
that acts as a mediator for behavioral intention. However, the -> Perceived Usefulness was significant (.446). However, the path
construct robot self-efficacy did not have any significant influence coefficient for Perceived Enjoyment -> Perceived Usefulness was
on perceived usefulness for any of the data sets; therefore, we not significant for the EA group (–.119), but the path coefficient
removed this construct from the model. it was significant for the all participants group and the MM group
This led to a conceptual model for a reduced TAM3 adapted to (Table 10). As the R2 values for all constructs were closer to .50
social robots (Fig. 3). than .25, the model has predictive power [79] (Fig. 4).
Furthermore, we developed hypotheses to support the re- When the f 2 effect sizes for the three data groups were ana-
duced model and grounded them in previous literature relevant lyzed, for all participants, the effect for PU -> Behavioral Intention
to the TAM3 model. (Table 7). was well above the threshold for a large effect at .706, for the
EA group, the effect was .655, and for the MM group, the effect
4.4.2. Assessment of the measurement model of the reduced TAM3 was .688. For all participants, the effect sizes were small for
for social robots Perceived Enjoyment -> PU (.091), Image -> PU (.110), and Output
To confirm item reliability for the reduced model, we verified Quality -> PU (.091), while for Result Demonstrability -> PU the
the loadings to ensure that the recommended threshold value for effect was minimal (.014). In the case of EA, the effect sizes for
item loadings [76] was achieved. As none of the loadings were Image -> PU (.236) and Result Demonstrability -> PU (.276) were
below 0.4, there was no need to remove any items from the between medium and large effects, but there is only a minimal
8
U.A. Saari, A. Tossavainen, K. Kaipainen et al. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 151 (2022) 104033

Table 6
The effect sizes for the paths (f 2 ).
Path All participants EA MM
Image -> Perceived Usefulness .099 (*) .171 (**) .068 (*)
Output Quality -> Perceived Usefulness .138 (*) .175 (**) .164 (**)
Perceived Ease of Use -> Behavioral Intention .021 (*) .116 (*) .004 ( †)
Perceived Ease of Use -> Perceived Usefulness .002 ( †) .068 (*) .000 (†)
Perceived Enjoyment -> Perceived Ease of Use .411 (***) .553 (***) .215 (**)
Perceived Usefulness -> Behavioral Intention .448 (***) .210 (**) .539 (***)
Result Demonstrability -> Perceived Usefulness .075 (*) .324 (**) .031 (*)
Robot Anxiety -> Perceived Ease of Use .057 (*) .023 (*) .116 (*)
Robot Playfulness -> Perceived Ease of Use .049 (*) .017 (†) .003 ( †)
Robot Self-Efficacy -> Perceived Ease of Use .035 (*) .059 (*) .021 (*)
Subjective Norm -> Behavioral Intention .019 ( †) .059 (*) .009 (†)
Subjective Norm -> Image .191 (**) .201 (**) .188 (**)
Subjective Norm -> Perceived Usefulness .000 ( †) .000 ( †) .002 ( †)

† = no effect, * = small effect, ** = medium effect, and *** = large effect.

Table 7
Hypotheses with reference to supporting literature.
Hypothesis Supporting references
H1: The image of a person using robots influences positively how useful a [24]
robot is perceived.
H2: The output quality from using robots influences positively how useful a [24]
robot is perceived.
H3: The demonstrability of the results from using robots influences positively [24]
how useful a robot is perceived.
H4: The perceived enjoyment found in using robots influences positively how [25,45–47]
useful a robot is perceived.
H5: The perceived usefulness of robots has a positive influence on the future [23,24]
potential use of a robot.

Table 8
Item loadings for the measurement models of the new reduced model.
Construct Item Loading All (N = 121) Loading EA (n = 39) Loading MM (n = 82)
Image People in my organization who use the robot have more .770 .719 .831
prestige than those who do not. (IMG1)
People in my organization who use the robot have a high .780 .886 .738
profile. (IMG2)
Testing the robot is a status symbol in my organization. .781 .879 .638
(IMG3)
Output Quality The quality of the output I get using the robot is high. (OUT1) .841 .753 .869
I have no problem with the quality of the robot’s output. .806 .829 .809
(OUT2)
I rate the results from the robot to be excellent. (OUT3) .904 .919 .880
Result Demonstrability I have no difficulty telling others about the results of using .638 .728 .574
the robot. (RES1)
I believe I could communicate to others the consequences of .736 .753 .710
using the robot. (RES2)
The results of using the robot are apparent to me. (RES3) .814 .877 .803
Perceived Enjoyment I find using the robot to be enjoyable. (ENJ1) .905 .943 .884
The actual process of using the robot is pleasant. (ENJ2) .925 .945 .923
I have fun using the robot. (ENJ3) .679 .890 .478
Perceived Usefulness Using the robot improves my performance in my task .839 .872 .815
investigating the lunch menus. (PU1)
Using the robot enhances my effectiveness in my task .849 .823 .848
investigating the lunch menus. (PU3)
I find the robot to be useful in my task investigating the .890 .870 .902
lunch menus. (PU4)
Behavioral Intention Assuming the robot would be permanently at the setup under .905 .891 .910
testing . .
. . . I would use the robot if it would be accessible. (BI1)
. . . I might use the robot if it would be accessible. (BI2) .881 .887 .873
. . . I would plan to use the robot in the future. (BI3) .911 .879 .916

All = All participants; EA = Early adopters; MM = Mass market representatives.

effect for Perceived Enjoyment -> PU (.016). For the MM group, This was also the case for the q2 effect sizes where the path
for Perceived Enjoyment -> PU, the effect size (.323) was nearly coefficients had significant values.
at the large effect level (.35), however, the effect sizes are small Based on recommendations for PLS-SEM to achieve statistical
for Image -> PU (.085), Output Quality -> PU (.112), and for Result power [79], it can be stated that for the MM representatives the
Demonstrability -> PU, there was hardly no effect (.005). significance level is on the level of 1%, and for the EA group the
The Q 2 values for the endogenous constructs Behavioral In- significance level is at 10%. For this reason, the bootstrapping
tention and Perceived Usefulness were above zero, which is an method is used to further test the significance with additional
indication of the predictive relevance of the latent variables. 5000 samples. The results from the bootstrapping indicate that
9
U.A. Saari, A. Tossavainen, K. Kaipainen et al. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 151 (2022) 104033

Table 9
Composite reliability (CR) and average variance extracted (AVE) for the constructs.
Construct CR, All CR, EA CR, MM AVE, All AVE, EA AVE, MM
Image (IMG) .820 .870 .782 .603 .692 .547
Output Quality (OUT) .888 .874 .889 .725 .699 .728
Result Demonstrability (RES) .775 .830 .741 .537 .622 .492
Perceived Enjoyment (ENJ) .879 .948 .821 .712 .858 .620
Perceived Usefulness (PU) .895 .891 .891 .739 .731 .732
Behavioral Intention (BI) .927 .916 .927 .808 .785 .809

All = All participants; EA = Early adopters; MM = Mass market representatives.

Table 10
Path coefficients (β ) and significance levels (p-value) for the three groups.
Path All β All p EA β EA p MM β MM p
H1: Image -> Perceived Usefulness .247 <.001 .339 .001 .206 .008
H2: Output Quality -> Perceived Usefulness .278 .002 .323 .022 .284 .002
H3: Result Demonstrability -> Perceived Usefulness .110 .149 .446 .001 - .066 .459
H4: Perceived Enjoyment -> Perceived Usefulness .300 .001 -.119 .411 .513 <.001
H5: Perceived Usefulness -> Behavioral Intention .643 <.001 .629 <.001 .639 <.001

EA = Early adopters; MM = Mass market representatives.

the results of this exploratory study are of significant relevance of the mass market representatives (H4), while for the early
and justifies the call for more research to validate the findings adopters the element of enjoyment is not significant. In this case,
with a larger sample size in future research. The minimum sample it is important to understand that mass market representatives
size has been fulfilled in this study to ensure that the results from seek more enjoyment related to their work, so that the use of
the PLS-SEM analyses are robust, and the model is generalizable. the robot is enjoyable, pleasant, and even fun. In the literature,
with reference to the CASA paradigm, people are considered to
5. Discussion and conclusions perceive robots as social actors with identities; therefore, social
elements in robots are developed to ease their use [43,44]. In
In this study, one objective was to test how applicable the addition, social robots have also been viewed as hedonic systems,
TAM3 [24] is in the context of social robots. The other objective and the construct Perceived Enjoyment has been found to influ-
was to analyze how early adopters’ and mass market representa- ence technology acceptance [45]. However, early adopters were
tives’ acceptance of social robots differs [27]. Based on the results differentiated from the mass market representatives by result
from testing the original TAM3 adapted to robots, a reduced demonstrability (H3). Early adopters consider it important that
model was introduced, with similarities and differences between they can tell others about the robot, communicate about its use,
the two types of innovation adopters. The reduced version of the and that the results from using the robot are apparent, which is
TAM3 focuses on perceived usefulness which is crucial in work fully in line with the drivers for technology acceptance included
environments. The context is the FFE innovation phase, where in the TAM3 model [24].
robot developers test new robot solutions with new users. To get The findings indicate that participants’ innovation adoption
a more comprehensive view of the potential success of the novel style affected the factors that users considered important when
robot solution, testing with different kinds of users is crucial. they assessed the usefulness of social robots, and robot devel-
First, we discuss the generalizability of the reduced TAM3 opers should consider these styles during the FFE innovation
model for both kinds of innovation adopters, the early adopters phase. For both adopter groups, image and output quality had a
(EA) and mass market representatives (MM). Hypotheses 1, 2, and significant impact on perceived usefulness, which is important to
5 are all supported for the full data set. This indicates that gen- consider in all phases of the introduction of social robots on the
erally ‘‘The image of a person using robots influences positively market. This may have a strong impact on the successful intro-
how useful a robot is perceived’’ (H1) and that ‘‘The output quality duction of social robots in workplaces. The findings also provide
from using robots influences positively how useful a robot is valuable information on the applicability of the TAM3 in social
perceived’’ (H2), and also that ‘‘The perceived usefulness of robots robot design. Robots may need to be introduced to workplaces in
has a positive influence on the future potential use of a robot’’ stages, so that the workforce gets used to working with robots.
(H5). This is in line with the TAM3 literature [24] indicating that Thus, introducing social robots in simple tasks in the workplace
there are some common dimensions that apply to the technology environment, such as in guidance tasks in the reception area or
acceptance of different kinds of technology, such as computers cafeteria, or providing light entertainment, can be good places to
that were the focus originally in TAM3 and in this study so- start.
cial robots. This is also an important finding for technology and Regarding the constructs that were dropped out from the orig-
social robot developers to focus on the output quality as well inal TAM3 model to create the reduced model adapted to social
as showcasing the use of technology with the help of different robots, perhaps perceived ease of use is not critical for adoption
kinds of people to build a positive image of technology users. and interaction with social robots. It could be that participants
Additionally, it should be highlighted, that understandably the did not consider themselves users when social robots perform
perceived usefulness and benefits from using a robot will have without human guidance, and the participants did not operate the
an impact on the future use of social robots among people who robots. When robots are independent and have been programmed
have tested one at some point [23]. to interact, humans are similar to ‘‘interactors’’ who then consider
After further testing the model with the two data sets sepa- the ease of the interaction rather than ease of use; that is, more
rately (EA and MM), the results indicate that perceived enjoyment research on the perceived ease of interaction with social robots is
has a significant influence on perceived usefulness in the case needed. In the reduced model, the constructs linked to perceived
10
U.A. Saari, A. Tossavainen, K. Kaipainen et al. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 151 (2022) 104033

ease of use in the original model were all tested as constructs real-world work contexts, interactions that require cooperation
impacting perceived usefulness. related to work tasks with social robots may require human
According to the results, perceived usefulness had a significant participants to perform more complex tasks in cooperation with
influence on participants’ behavioral intentions. These findings a robot. Therefore, future research should focus on doing more
are in line with the original TAM3 research results. However, testing in real-world work environments. Finally, due to the small
the factors influencing the perceived usefulness vary between the number of participants in the early adopter group, the reliabil-
innovation adopter types. When introducing the first versions of ity and validity of the results compared to those for the mass
a new social robot in the market, to attract early adopters’ accep- market representatives should be verified with a larger group of
tance, robot developers should focus on creating such features participants, so that the number of participants in both groups of
that allow early adopters get an impression of result demon- adopter types would be even. Future studies should also develop
strability (i.e., so they can understand and see the features and further appropriate indicators for measuring perceived ease of
functionality of the robot clearly which will then allow them interaction with social robots, which would allow to refine a more
to communicate about the use of robots to others and show- applicable TAM for social robots in workplaces.
case that the results from using the robot are apparent). When In the FFE innovation phase, new product initiatives require
early adopters acting as influencers in the market have explained special attention and input from various perspectives within the
robots’ functionality to other potential users who are then mass team as well as from potential target users [85–87]. Active align-
market representatives, new versions of the robot should be ment and testing according to potential user requirements can
released in the market. To ensure mass market adopters per- help reduce major risks and uncertainties [88] and have a strong
ceive the robots as useful, the second version of the social robot positive influence on the innovativeness and financial success
should include features and functionality that increase perceived of a product [89,90]. However, there should be a balance and
enjoyment among mass market adopters. This requires that they an understanding of the relevant features to be implemented as
consider the interaction with the social robot enjoyable, pleasant, fulfilling all user needs and requirements often is not feasible
and fun. [91]. Therefore, the results show that there is an opportunity
Future workplaces will be different, as the nature of work is to divide markets into early and late adopters and inquire their
changing with technological advancements in ways of working differences in preferences and acceptance behavior.
and as a result of the development of new kinds of jobs [2]. These results pave the way for interesting and fruitful research
Recent research indicated that in the future more AI solutions will avenues in further investigating the differences in adoption be-
be introduced in workplaces, and human workers will cooperate havior between adopter categories, and experimental setups for
more with social humanoid robots instead of having them replace testing social robotics and acceptance of different adopter cate-
the human workforce [82]. Researchers have shown that humans gories. Furthermore, theoretical explanations for differing accep-
are content to work with robots if they are not in superior or tance factors should be investigated in future research.
in leading roles but in assisting roles. In these situations when
a robot is a follower, it is often considered competent and intel- Funding
ligent for the task [32]. Especially in such situations as created
by the COVID-19 crisis, with a risk of spreading the infection This research did not receive any specific grant from funding
in workplaces where active social interaction is required, there agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
could be an opportunity to make use of more social robots in
work environments. However, the most common robots in work- Declaration of competing interest
places are industrial robots that are usually placed in different
spaces than where human employees operate due to safety pre- The authors declare that they have no known competing finan-
cautions. With the development in the field of social robotics, cial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared
more interactive social robots have been introduced to work- to influence the work reported in this paper.
places to work closer with human workers [10,23,83]. There could
also be potential to extend social robots to workplaces where References
there are multiple human encounters and touch points where
there is a risk of getting infected. Currently especially in hospitals, [1] S. Bankins, P. Formosa, When AI meets PC: Exploring the implications of
smart healthcare management systems could be extended with workplace social robots and a human–robot psychological contract, Eur.
social robots assisting in the simple healthcare services, such as, J. Work Organ. Psychol. 29 (2) (2020) 215–229, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
diagnosis and monitoring of patients’ simple health indicators 1359432X.2019.1620328.
[2] S.R. Barley, B.A. Bechky, F.J. Milliken, The changing nature of work: Careers,
(for example, body temperature, heart rate) and measurements of identities, and work lives in the 21st century, Acad. Manag. Discov. 3 (2)
room humidity, carbon level and CO2 gases in healthcare facilities (2017) 111–115, http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amd.2017.0034.
[84]. [3] N. Beheshtian, K. Kaipainen, K. Kähkönen, A. Ahtinen, Color game: A
This study had several limitations. The research setting was collaborative social robotic game for icebreaking – towards the design of
robotic ambiences as part of smart building services, in: Proceedings of the
artificially designed outside a research laboratory on a university
23rd International Conference on Academic Mindtrek, 2020, ACM., 2020,
campus, although individuals outside the academic community pp. 10–19, http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/3377290.3377292.
also participated in the experiment. Thus, the design incorporated [4] T. Park, How we’ll stereotype our robot coworkers, Harv. Bus. Rev.
representatives who truly can be considered mass market rep- (2014) October 02, 2014. https://hbr.org/2014/10/how-well-stereotype-
resentatives. As this research is highly explorative, the sample our-robot-coworkers.
[5] N. Piçarra, J.C. Giger, Predicting intention to work with social robots at
size is fairly small, and even though it does give an indication
anticipation stage: Assessing the role of behavioral desire and anticipated
of validity of the model, more research with larger sample sizes emotions, Comput. Hum. Behav. 86 (2018) 129–146, http://dx.doi.org/10.
is still called for. In addition, future research should investigate 1016/j.chb.2018.04.026.
how the two innovation adopter types perceive social robots in [6] P.A. Hancock, D.R. Billings, K.E. Schaefer, J.Y. Chen, E.J. De Visser, R.
different kinds of working environments and situations, as well Parasuraman, A meta-analysis of factors affecting trust in human–robot
interaction, Hum. Factors 53 (5) (2011) 517–527, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
as different levels of cooperation. The tasks in the experiment 0018720811417254.
were simple, and the development team was present to help [7] T.B. Sheridan, Human–robot interaction: Status and challenges, Hum. Fac-
participants with possible problems, technical or otherwise; in tors 58 (4) (2016) 525–532, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0018720816644364.

11
U.A. Saari, A. Tossavainen, K. Kaipainen et al. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 151 (2022) 104033

[8] S. Warta, K. Kapalo, A. Best, S. Fiore, Similarity, complementarity, and [32] X. Lei, P.L.P. Rau, Effect of relative status on responsibility attributions in
agency in HRI: Theoretical issues in shifting the perception of robots from human–robot collaboration: Mediating role of sense of responsibility and
tools to teammates, in: Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics moderating role of power distance orientation, Comput. Hum. Behav. 122
Society 2016 Annual Meeting, 2016, pp. 1230–1234, http://dx.doi.org/10. (2021) 106820, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2021.106820.
1177/1541931213601287. [33] L. Royakkers, R. van Est, A literature review on new robotics: Automation
[9] World Economic Forum, The future of jobs report 2020, 2020, https: from love to war, Int. J. Soc. Robot. 7 (5) (2015) 549–570, http://dx.doi.
//www.weforum.org/reports/the-future-of-jobs-report-2020. org/10.1007/s12369-015-0295-x.
[10] N. Savela, M. Kaakinen, N. Ellonen, A. Oksanen, Sharing a work team with [34] K.J. Vänni, A.K. Korpela, Role of social robotics in supporting employees
robots: The negative effect of robot co-workers on in-group identification and advancing productivity, in: Tapus A., E. André, JC. Martin, F. Ferland,
with the work team, Comput. Hum. Behav. 115 (2021) 106585, http: M. Ammi (Eds.), Social Robotics. ICSR 2015, in: Lecture Notes in Computer
//dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106585. Science, vol. 9388, Springer, Cham, 2015, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-
[11] C. Breazeal, A. Takanishi, T. Kobayashi, Social robots that interact 319-25554-5_67.
with people, in: B. Siciliano, O. Khatib (Eds.), Springer Handbook of [35] K.M.S. Faqih, M-I.R.M. Jaradat, Assessing the moderating effect of gender
Robotics, Springer., 2008, pp. 1349–1369, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3- differences and individualism-collectivism at individual-level on the adop-
540-30301-5_59. tion of mobile commerce technology: TAM3 perspective, J. Retail. Consum.
[12] M.Q. Azhar, E.I. Sklar, A study measuring the impact of shared decision Serv. 22 (2015) 37–52, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jretconser.2014.09.006.
making in a human–robot team, Int. J. Robot. Res. 36 (5–7) (2017) 461–482, [36] V. Venkatesh, F.D. Davis, A theoretical extension of the Technology Accep-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0278364917710540. tance Model: four longitudinal field studies, Management Science 46 (2)
[13] B.R. Duffy, Anthropomorphism and the social robot, Robot. Auton. Syst. 42 (2000) 186–204, http://dx.doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.46.2.186.11926.
(3–4) (2003) 177–190, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0921-8890(02)00374-3. [37] V. Lotz, S. Himmel, M. Ziefle, You’re my mate – acceptance factors for
[14] R.P. Bagozzi, The legacy of the technology acceptance model and a proposal human–robot collaboration in industry, in: D. Dimitrov, D. Hagedorn-
for a paradigm shift, J. Assoc. Inf. Syst. 8 (4) (2007) 244–254, http: Hansen, K. von Leipzig (Eds.), Knowledge Valorisation in the Age of
//dx.doi.org/10.17705/1jais.00122%20. Digitalization. Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Com-
[15] D. Cameron, S. de Saille, E.C. Collins, J.M. Aitken, H. Cheung, A. Chua, E.J. petitive Manufacturing, COMA ’19, Stellenbosch, South Africa, ISBN:
Loh, J. Law, The effect of social-cognitive recovery strategies on likability, 078-0-7972-1779-9, 2019, pp. 405–411.
capability and trust in social robots, Comput. Hum. Behav. 114 (2021) [38] C. Bröhl, J. Nelles, C. Brandl, A. Mertens, C.M. Schlick, TAM Reloaded: A
106561, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106561. Technology Acceptance Model for Human–Robot Cooperation in Production
[16] D. Fischinger, P. Einramhof, K. Papoutsakis, W. Wohlkinger, P. Mayer, P. Systems, Institute of Industrial Engineering and Ergonomics, RWTH Aachen
Panek, et al., Hobbit, a care robot supporting independent living at home: University. Springer-Verlag, 2011.
first prototype and lessons learned, Robot. Autonom. Syst. 75 (2016) 60–78, [39] V. Venkatesh, M.G. Morris, G.B. Davis, Davis F.D., User acceptance of
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.robot.2014.09.029. information technology: Toward a unified view, MIS Q. 27 (2003) 425–478,
[17] A. Edwards, C. Edwards, D. Westerman, P.R.. Spence, Initial expectations, https://www.jstor.org/stable/30036540.
interactions, and beyond with social robots, Comput. Hum. Behav. 90 [40] A. Istenic, I. Bratko, V. Rosanda, Are pre- service teachers disinclined to
(2019) 308–314, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.042. utilise embodied humanoid social robots in the classroom? Br. J. Educ.
[18] F.D. Davis, A Technology Acceptance Model for Empirically Testing New Technol. (2021) http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjet.13144.
End-User Information Systems: Theory and Results, Doctoral dissertation, [41] V. Venkatesh, J. Thong, X. Xu, Consumer acceptance and use of informa-
Massachusetts Institute of Technology MIT, 1985. tion technology: Extending the unified theory of acceptance and use of
[19] F.D. Davis, Perceived usefulness, perceived ease of use, and user acceptance technology, MIS Q. 36 (1) (2012) 157–178, https://www.jstor.org/stable/
of information technology, MIS Q. 13 (3) (1989) 319–339, http://dx.doi.org/ 41410412.
10.2307/249008. [42] C. Breazeal, K. Dautenhahn, T. Kanda, Social robotics, in: B. Siciliano,
[20] N. Marangunic, A. Granic, Technology acceptance model: A literature O. Khatib (Eds.), Springer Handbook of Robotics, Springer., 2016, pp.
review from 1986 to 2013, Univ. Access Inf. Soc. 14 (1) (2015) 81–95, 1935–1972, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-32552-1_72.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10209-014-0348-1. [43] C. Nass, J. Steuer, E.R. Tauber, Computers are social actors, in: Proceedings
[21] A. Tossavainen, Reduction of Market and Technology Uncertainty Dur- of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, 1994,
ing the Front End of New Product Development [Doctoral disser- pp. 72–78, http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/191666.191703.
tation, Tampere University], Tampere University Dissertations, ISBN: [44] Y. Kim, S.S. Kwak, M.S. Kim, Am I acceptable to you? Effect of a robot’s
978-952-03-1537-5, 2020, p. 243. verbal language forms on people’s social distance from robots, Comput.
[22] M. Turner, B. Kitchenham, P. Brereton, S. Charters, D. Budgen, Does the Hum. Behav. 29 (3) (2013) 1091–1101, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.
technology acceptance model predict actual use? A systematic literature 2012.10.001.
review, Inf. Softw. Technol. 52 (5) (2010) 463–479, http://dx.doi.org/10. [45] M. Heerink, B. Kröse, V. Evers, B. Wielinga, Influence of social presence
1016/j.infsof.2009.11.005. on acceptance of an assistive social robot and screen agent by elderly
[23] N. Savela, T. Turja, A. Oksanen, Social acceptance of robots in different users, Adv. Robot. 23 (14) (2009) 1909–1923, http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/
occupational fields: A systematic literature review, Int. J. Soc. Robot. 10 016918609X12518783330289.
(4) (2018) 493–502, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12369-017-0452-5. [46] M. Heerink, B. Kröse, V. Evers, B. Wielinga, Assessing acceptance of assistive
[24] V. Venkatesh, H. Bala, Technology acceptance model 3 and a research social agent technology by older adults: The almere model, Int. J. Soc.
agenda on interventions, Decis. Sci. 39 (2) (2008) 273–315, http://dx.doi. Robot. 2 (4) (2010) 361–375, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12369-010-0068-
org/10.1111/j.1540-5915.2008.00192.x. 5.
[25] M.M. De Graaf, S.B. Allouch, Exploring influencing variables for the ac- [47] D.H. Shin, H. Choo, Modeling the acceptance of socially interactive robotics:
ceptance of social robots, Robot. Auton. Syst. 61 (12) (2013) 1476–1486, Social presence in human–robot interaction, Interact. Stud. 12 (3) (2011)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.robot.2013.07.007. 430–460, http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/is.12.3.04shi.
[26] R. Kirby, J. Forlizzi, R. Simmons, Affective social robots, Robot. Auton. Syst. [48] T. Fong, I. Nourbakhsh, K. Dautenhahn, A survey of socially interactive
58 (3) (2010) 322–332, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.robot.2009.09.015. robots, Robot. Auton. Syst. 42 (3–4) (2003) 143–166, http://dx.doi.org/10.
[27] E.M. Rogers, The Diffusion of Innovations, fifth ed., The Free Press, 2003. 1016/S0921-8890(02)00372-X.
[28] E. Park, S.J. Kwon, The adoption of teaching assistant robots: A technology [49] G.Z. Yang, J. Bellingham, P.E. Dupont, P. Fischer, L. Floridi, R. Full, N.
acceptance model approach, Program-Electr Libr. Inf. Syst. 50 (4) (2016) Jacobstein, V. Kumar, M. McNutt, R. Merrifield, B.J. Nelson, B. Scassellati,
354–366, http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/PROG-02-2016-0017. M. Taddeo, R. Taylor, M. Veloso, Z.L. Wang, R. Wood, The grand challenges
[29] A.S. Ghazali, J. Ham, E. Barakova, P. Markopoulos, Persuasive robots of science robotics, Science Robotics 3 (14) (2018) 1–14, http://dx.doi.org/
acceptance model (PRAM): Roles of social responses within the acceptance 10.1126/scirobotics.aar7650.
model of persuasive robots, Int. J. Soc. Robot. 12 (2020) 1075–1092, http: [50] A. Alaiad, L. Zhou, The determinants of home healthcare robots adoption:
//dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12369-019-00611-1. An empirical investigation, Int. J. Med. Inform. 83 (11) (2014) 825–840,
[30] R. De Kervenoael, R. Hasan, A. Schwob, E. Goh, Leveraging human–robot http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijmedinf.2014.07.003.
interaction in hospitality services: Incorporating the role of perceived [51] T. Klamer, S.B. Allouch, Acceptance and use of a social robot by elderly
value, empathy, and information sharing into visitors’ intentions to use users in a domestic environment, in: 2010 4th International Conference
social robots, Tour. Manag. 78 (2020) 104042, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. on Pervasive Computing Technologies for Healthcare, IEEE, 2010, pp. 1–8,
tourman.2019.104042. http://dx.doi.org/10.4108/ICST.PERVASIVEHEALTH2010.8892.
[31] S.M.S. Khaksar, R. Khosla, S. Singaraju, B. Slade, Carer’s perception on [52] C. BenMessaoud, H. Kharrazi, K.F. MacDorman, Facilitators and barriers
social assistive technology acceptance and adoption: Moderating effects to adopting robotic-assisted surgery: Contextualizing the unified theory
of perceived risks, Behav. Inf. Technol. 40 (4) (2021) 337–360, http://dx. of acceptance and use of technology, PLoS One 6 (1) (2011) e16395,
doi.org/10.1080/0144929X.2019.1690046. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0016395.

12
U.A. Saari, A. Tossavainen, K. Kaipainen et al. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 151 (2022) 104033

[53] A. Ahtinen, K. Kaipainen, Learning and teaching experiences with a per- [77] J.F. Hair, J.J. Risher, M. Sarstedt, C.M. Ringle, When to use and how to
suasive social robot in primary school: Findings and implications from report the results of PLS-SEM, Eur. Bus. Rev. 31 (1) (2019) 2–24, http:
a 4-month field study, in: Persuasive Technology. Designing for Future //dx.doi.org/10.1108/EBR-11-2018-0203.
Change. PERSUASIVE 2020, in: Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol. [78] J.L. Roldán, M.J. Sánchez-Franco, Variance-based structural equation mod-
12064, Springer, 2020, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-45712-9_6. eling: Guidelines for using partial least squares in information systems
[54] M. Fridin, M. Belokopytov, Acceptance of socially assistive humanoid robot research, in: M. Mora, O. Gelman, A.L. Steenkamp, M. Raisinghani (Eds.),
by preschool and elementary school teachers, Comput. Hum. Behav. 33 Research Methodologies, Innovations and Philosophies in Software Systems
(2014) 23–31, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.12.016. Engineering and Information Systems, IGI Global, 2012, pp. 193–221.
[55] D. Conti, S. Di Nuovo, S. Buono, A. Di Nuovo, Robots in education and [79] J.F. Hair, G.T.M. Hult, C.M. Ringle, M. Sarstedt, A Primer on Partial Least
care of children with developmental disabilities: A study on acceptance Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM), second ed., Sage, 2017.
[80] C.M. Ringle, S. Wende, J.M. Becker, SmartPLS 3, 2015, http://www.smartpls.
by experienced and future professionals, Int. J. Soc. Robot. 9 (1) (2017)
com.
51–62, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12369-016-0359-6.
[81] J. Henseler, C.M. Ringle, M. Sarstedt, A new criterion for assessing discrimi-
[56] M. Appel, S. Krause, U. Gleich, M. Mara, Meaning through fiction: Science
nant validity in variance-based structural equation modeling, J. Acad. Mark.
fiction and innovative technologies, Psychol. Aesthet. Creativity Arts 10
Sci. 43 (2015) 115–135, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11747-014-0403-8.
(2016) 472–480, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aca0000052.
[82] W.L. Woo, Future trends in I & M: Human-machine co-creation in the rise
[57] S. Naneva, M.Sarda. Gou, T.L. Webb, T.J. Prescott, A systematic review of AI, IEEE Instrum. Measur. Mag. 23 (2) (2020) 71–73.
of attitudes, anxiety, acceptance, and trust towards social robots, Int. J. [83] T. Haidegger, M. Barreto, P. Gonçalves, M.K. Habib, S.K.V. Ragacan, H. Li,
Soc. Robot. 12 (2020) 1179–1201, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12369-020- A. Vaccarella, R. Perrone, E. Prestes, Applied ontologies and standards
00659-4. for service robots, Robot. Auton. Syst. 61 (11) (2013) 1215–1223, http:
[58] T. Gnambs, M. Appel, Are robots becoming unpopular? Changes in attitudes //dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.robot.2013.05.008.
towards autonomous robotic systems in Europe, Comput. Hum. Behav. 93 [84] M.T. Quasim, A. Shaikh, M. Shuaib, A. Sulaiman, S. Alam, Y. Asiri, Smart
(2019) 53–61, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.11.045. healthcare management evaluation using fuzzy decision making method,
[59] A.C. Simões, A.L. Soares, A.C. Barros, Factors influencing the intention 2021, https://www.researchsquare.com/article/rs-424702/v1.
of managers to adopt collaborative robots (cobots) in manufacturing [85] J. Mohr, S. Sengupta, S. Slater, Marketing of High-Technology Products and
organizations, J. Eng. Technol. Manag. 57 (2020) 101574, http://dx.doi.org/ Innovations, third ed., Pearson Education Inc, 2010.
10.1016/j.jengtecman.2020.101574. [86] P.S. Rizova, S. Gupta, E.N. Maltz, R.W. Walker, Overcoming equivocality
[60] T.W. Valente, Social network thresholds in the diffusion of innovations, on projects in the fuzzy front end: Bringing social networks back in,
Social Networks 18 (1) (1996) 69–89. Technovation 78 (2018) 40–55, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.technovation.
[61] O. Dedehayir, R.J. Ortt, C. Riverola, F. Miralles, Innovators and early 2018.05.006.
adopters in the diffusion of innovations: A literature review, Int. J. Innov. [87] P. Conradie, L. De Marez, J. Saldien, User consultation during the fuzzy
Manag. 21 (8) (2017) http://dx.doi.org/10.1142/S1363919617400102. front end: evaluating student’s design outcomes, International Journal of
[62] D.G. Labay, T.C. Kinnear, Exploring the consumer decision process in the Technology and Design Education 4 (2017) 563–575, http://dx.doi.org/10.
adoption of solar energy systems, J. Consum. Res. 8 (3) (1981) 271–278, 1007/s10798-016-9361-4.
[88] F. Schweitzer, M. Palmie, O. Gassmann, Beyond listening: The distinct ef-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/208865.
fects of proactive versus responsive customer orientation on the reduction
[63] International Federation of Robotics, World Robotics Service Robots
of uncertainties at the fuzzy front end of innovation, R & D Manag. 48 (5)
2019: Statistics, Market Analysis, Forecasts, Case Studies, IFR Statistical
(2016) 534–550, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/radm.12252.
Department. VDMA Services GmbH, 2019.
[89] M.A. Stanko, J.M. Bonner, Projective customer competence: Projecting
[64] A.P. Henkel, M. Čaić, M. Blaurock, M. Okan, Robotic transformative service
future customer needs that drive innovation performance, Ind. Market.
research: Deploying social robots for consumer well-being during Covid-19
Manag. 42 (8) (2013) 1255–1265, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.indmarman.
and beyond, J. Serv. Manag. 31 (6) (2020) 1131–1148, http://dx.doi.org/10.
2013.05.016.
1108/JOSM-05-2020-0145. [90] B.S. Yoon, A.J. Jetter, Connecting customers with engineers for the success-
[65] D. Giansanti, The social robot in rehabilitation and assistance: What ful fuzzy front end: Requirement of tools, in: 2015 Portland International
is the future? Healthcare 9 (3) (2021) 1–10, http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ Conference on Management of Engineering and Technology, PICMET, 2015,
healthcare9030244, 244. pp. 1585–1595.
[66] M.J. Sobrepera, V.G. Lee, S. Garg, R. Mendonca, M.J. Johnson, Perceived [91] C.M. Christensen, The innovator’s dilemma, in: Collins Business Essentials,
usefulness of a social robot augmented telehealth platform by therapists 2003.
in the United States, IEEE Robot. Autom. Lett. 6 (2) (2021) 2946–2953.
[67] M.R. Lima, M. Wairagkar, N. Natarajan, S. Vaitheswaran, R. Vaidyanathan,
Robotic telemedicine for mental health: A multimodal approach to improve Ulla A. Saari is a Senior Research Fellow at the Unit
human–robot engagement, Front. Robot. AI 8 (2021) 618866, http://dx.doi. of Industrial Engineering and Management, Tampere
org/10.3389/frobt.2021.618866. University, Finland. She has received a D.Sc. (Tech)
[68] Y.H. Liao, L. Tzu-Yuan, C.C. Wu, Y.N. Shih, Can occupational therapy degree in Industrial Engineering & Management and
manpower be replaced with social robots in a singing group during a M.Sc. in Environmental Engineering from the Tam-
COVID-19? Work 68 (1) (2021) 21–26, http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/WOR- pere University of Technology. She also has a M.A.
205096. degree in Languages and Social Sciences from the
[69] Y. Weng, C. Chen, C. Sun, Toward the human–robot co-existence society: University of Helsinki, Finland. She is a research af-
On safety intelligence for next generation robots, Int. J. Soc. Robot. 1 (2009) filiate at Jönköping University, Sweden. Her research
267–282, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12369-009-0019-1. topics include technology & innovation management,
[70] SoftBank Robotics, Pepper. www.softbankrobotics.com/emea/en/pepper. sustainability-oriented innovation, sustainable produc-
[71] Venturebeat, Softbank robotics enhances pepper the robot’s emotional tion & consumption, sustainable business models. She has 20 years’ experience
in the high-tech industry in senior manager level roles.
intelligence, 2018, https://venturebeat.com/2018/08/28/softbank-robotics-
enhances-pepper-the-robots-emotional-intelligence/.
[72] J.J. Cameron, D.A. Stinson, Gender (mis) measurement: Guidelines for
respecting gender diversity in psychological research, Soc. Pers. Psychol. Antero J. Tossavainen is a Technology Manager at
Compass 13 (11) (2019) http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12506. Huawei Technologies new Device Concept Laboratory
[73] E.M. Rogers, The Diffusion of Innovations, The Free Press, 1962. based in Helsinki, Finland. Antero has work experience
[74] M. Sarstedt, J.F. Hair, C.M. Ringle, K.O. Thiele, S.P. Gudergan, Estimation from various positions in Finnish high-tech consumer
issues with PLS and CBSEM: Where the bias lies!, J. Bus. Res. 69 (2016) electronics industry since 2001 at Nokia, Microsoft and
3998–4010, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2016.06.007. Huawei Technologies. Antero participates as a delegate
[75] E.H. Noppers, K. Keizer, M. Bockarjova, L. Steg, The adoption of sustainable in ETSI 3GPP technology standardization. Antero has
innovations: The role of instrumental, environmental, and symbolic at- received a D.Sc. (Tech) degree in Business and Technol-
tributes for earlier and later adopters, J. Environ. Psychol. 44 (2015) 74–84, ogy Management from Tampere University and M.Sc.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.09.002. in Electrical and Information Engineering in University
[76] M. Sarstedt, C.M. Ringle, J.F. Hair, Partial least squares structural equation of Oulu. Antero’s academic research is in fields of
technology acceptance, fuzzy front-end, new product development, market and
modeling, in: C. Homburg, M. Klarman, A. Vomberg (Eds.), Handbook of
technology uncertainty and early adopters of technology.
Market Research, Springer, 2017, pp. 1–40, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-
3-319-05542-8_15-1.

13
U.A. Saari, A. Tossavainen, K. Kaipainen et al. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 151 (2022) 104033

Kirsikka Kaipainen is a Postdoctoral Research Fellow Saku J. Mäkinen, Ph.D., is a full professor at the Unit
at the Unit of Computing Sciences, Tampere University, of Industrial Engineering and Management at Tampere
Finland, and is also affiliated with the Department University, Finland. His research focuses on the field of
of Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, Finland. She technology and innovation management and examines
has received a Ph.D. degree in Information Technol- social change and technology forecasting.
ogy from Tampere University of Technology in 2014,
and a M.Sc. in Computer Science from University of
Tampere. Her research interests include social robotics,
sustainable mobility, sustainable urban development,
and technology-aided interventions for mental health.

14

You might also like