Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Personality and Individual Differences 119 (2017) 46–51

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Are perfectionism cognitions and cognitive emotion regulation strategies


mediators between perfectionism and psychological distress?
António Macedo a, Cristiana Marques a,⁎, Vasco Quaresma b, Maria João Soares a, Ana Paula Amaral a,c,
Ana Isabel Araújo a, Ana Telma Pereira a
a
Department of Psychological Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra, Portugal
b
Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra, Portugal
c
Institute Polytechnic of Coimbra, ESTESC-Coimbra Health School, Portugal

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Recent studies proposed that maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation (CER) is associated with negative dimen-
Received 31 January 2017 sions of perfectionism and mediates the relationship between this trait and negative affect dimensions. In the
Received in revised form 13 June 2017 present longitudinal study, our aim was to examine whether and which perfectionism cognitions and CER strat-
Accepted 20 June 2017
egies would mediate the relationship between perfectionism traits and psychological distress, controlling for per-
Available online xxxx
ceived stress, social support and outcome measure at one year before.
Keywords:
At T0 and after approximately one year (T1), 258 college students (79.8% female) filled in the Portuguese validat-
Perfectionistic concerns ed versions of self-report questionnaires to evaluate perfectionism trait dimensions (perfectionistic concerns and
Perfectionism cognitions perfectionistic strivings), perfectionism cognitions, CER dimensions, perceived stress, social support and psycho-
Rumination logical distress (depression, hostility-anxiety, and amiability-vigor).
Catastrophizing We found that higher perfectionistic concerns at T0 contribute significantly, after one year, to maladaptive per-
Psychological distress fectionism cognitions, which in turn are associated with higher levels of catastrophizing and rumination, and al-
together will ultimately contribute to greater anxiety/hostility (total indirect effect: 0.03; 95% CI: 0.01 to 0.07)
and depression (total indirect effect: 0.06; 95% CI: 0.02 to 0.13).
In this cognitive setting, perfectionist individuals may benefit from psychological interventions to reduce their
tendency to use maladaptive CER strategies.
© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Perfectionism actions, and organization) and two interpersonal, the latter restricted
to aspects of parental expectations and criticism.
Perfectionism is a trait characterized by a dispositional tendency to Factor analytic studies of the subscales from the Hewitt and Flett
strive for flawlessness and the setting of excessively high standards (1991) Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (H&F-MPS) and the
for performance, accompanied by a morbid fear of failure and a tenden- (Frost et al. (1990) Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (F-MPS)
cy to harshly evaluate and criticize one's behavior (Frost, Marten, Lahart, have consistently yielded two factors (Soares et al., 2014). One of
& Rosenblate, 1990). Past research has extensively shown that this mul- these higher-order perfectionism factors is composed of F-MPS – con-
tifaceted nature of perfectionism encompasses both positive and nega- cern over mistakes, F-MPS – doubts about actions, and H&F-MPS – SPP
tive dimensions, emphasizing that perfectionism is not always scales and focuses on perfectionistic concerns, and the other higher-
maladaptive (Slade & Owens, 1998; Stoeber & Otto, 2006; Terry-Short, order factor includes H&F-MPS – SOP, and F-MPS – personal standards
Owens, Slade, & Dewey, 1995). Hewitt and Flett (1991) conceptualized scales and focuses on perfectionistic strivings. These factors have been
perfectionism as consisting of both intrapersonal (i.e., self-oriented per- interpreted by researchers as representing respectively, maladaptive
fectionism – SOP) and interpersonal (i.e., other-oriented perfectionism and adaptive features of perfectionism.
– OOP; socially prescribed perfectionism – SPP) dimensions. Frost et Perfectionistic concerns have been associated with lower levels of
al. (1990) developed a six-factor measure, assessing four intrapersonal subjective well-being and higher levels of psychological maladjustment
dimensions (personal standards, concern over mistakes, doubts about and emotional disorders (Egan, Wade, & Shafran, 2011). More specifi-
cally, most of the literature concurs on the fact that maladjustment
facets of perfectionism are consistently associated with psychological
⁎ Corresponding author. distress in the form of a broad range of psychopathological conditions
E-mail address: cristiana.marques@uc.pt (C. Marques). such as depression (e.g. Maia et al., 2012), obsessive-compulsive

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.06.032
0191-8869/© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
A. Macedo et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 119 (2017) 46–51 47

disorder (e.g. Maia et al., 2009), anxiety (e.g. Soares et al., 2014), eating We have also highlighted that CER strategies mediate the associa-
disordered behavior (e.g. Macedo et al., 2007; Soares et al., 2009), sui- tions between perfectionism and negative/positive affect. Specifically,
cidal behavior (e.g. O'Connor, 2007) and other health problems such maladaptive CER strategies (rumination, self-blaming, blaming-others,
as sleep disturbances (Azevedo et al., 2010). Furthermore, several stud- catastrophizing) and low levels of adaptive CER strategies (positive re-
ies in non-Western cultures showed that maladaptive perfectionism appraisal and planning, positive refocusing, putting into perspective)
predicted eating disorder symptoms (Chan, Ku, & Owens, 2010; Chan mediated the association between doubts about actions, concern over
& Owens, 2007; Choo & Chan, 2013), and adaptive perfectionism pre- mistakes, PSP, perfectionistic concerns, and perfectionistic strivings
dicted greater vigor (Kung & Chan, 2014). Contrarily, perfectionistic and negative affect. The maladaptive CER dimensions were not signifi-
strivings have demonstrated associations with psychological adjust- cantly correlated with positive affect, but high levels of adaptive dimen-
ment (Bieling, Israeli, & Antony, 2004; Stoeber & Otto, 2006), and to a sions (positive reappraisal and planning, positive refocusing, putting
lesser extent with psychological maladjustment (Limburg, Watson, into perspective and acceptance) were associated with low levels of
Hagger, & Egan, 2016; Smith, Saklofske, Yan, & Sherry, 2015). Some au- concern over mistakes and perfectionistic concerns and mediated
thors have contended that maladaptive outcomes associated with per- their association with positive affect (Castro et al., 2016).
fectionistic concerns may be due to their self-critical component If it could be demonstrated that the effects of perfectionism on psy-
(Dunkley, Blankstein, Masheb, & Grilo, 2006). chological distress are a function of specific mediators, this may contrib-
Perfectionism cognitions capture automatic thoughts related to dis- ute to better understand the nature of perfectionism components and to
positional perfectionism (Stoeber, Kobori, & Tanno, 2010), that are im- refine the focus of intervention trials. Thus, it is important to know
portant to understand how perfectionism is related to psychological which cognitive processes may be at work and linked with perfection-
distress, especially after individuals experiencing stressful life events ism, and consequently with the development of negative affect.
(Stoeber, Kobori, & Brown, 2014). How perfectionism cognitions oper- In the present longitudinal study, our aim was to ascertain whether
ate to produce distressful outcomes is not entirely clear, however re- and which perfectionism cognitions and CER strategies would mediate
search has shown that high levels of perfectionism cognitions are the relationship between perfectionism traits and psychological distress
associated with a ruminative response, depression, and anxiety (Flett, measured with a one-year interval. Social support and perceived stress
Madorsky, Hewitt, & Heisel, 2002). need to be controlled in the mediation models to be tested, because
these variables were associated in previous studies with both perfec-
tionism and distress (Castro et al., 2016; Dunkley, Blankstein, Halsall,
1.1. Perfectionism, cognition and emotion regulation Williams, & Winkworth, 2000). This strategy takes into consideration
the diathesis-stress model, which holds that perfectionism relates
Although emotions are biologically based, people are able to regulate more strongly to psychological distress under higher levels of stress
their emotions (Gross & Muñoz, 1995). The regulation of emotions by (e.g. Hewitt, Flett, & Ediger, 1996) and lower levels of social support
cognitions (Cognitive Emotion Regulation, CER) can be defined as the (Sherry, Law, Hewitt, Flett, & Besser, 2008).
mental processes responsible for monitoring, evaluating, and modifying Our hypotheses are that maladaptive perfectionism cognitions and
emotional reactions. This process helps people to keep control over their maladaptive CER strategies will mediate the relationships between per-
emotions during or after the experience of threatening or stressful fectionistic concerns and psychological distress (depression and anxi-
events (Garnefski, Kraaij, & Spinhoven, 2001). Garnefski and Kraaij ety/hostility). Regarding amiability/vigor outcome, we hypothesized
(2007) conceptualization of CER includes nine cognitive coping strate- that the mediational model would not be verified.
gies that are typically used in response to stressful events. Four of
them are considered maladaptive and have been positively associated 2. Method
with both anxiety and depression: self-blame, blaming others, rumina-
tion and catastrophizing. The other five dimensions are related to adap- This study was approved by the Ethic Committee of the XXXXXXXX
tive coping responses: acceptance, refocus on planning, positive [removed for the manuscript without author identities].
refocusing, positive reappraisal and putting into perspective.
Research has shown that both maladaptive and adaptive perfection- 2.1. Participants and procedure
ism are associated with negative CER dimensions, the latter to a lesser
extent. Thus, SOP, personal standards, concern over mistakes, and SPP The data were collected outside examination periods. The study
are related with self-blame (Hewitt & Flett, 1991; Ogai, 2004) and per- aims were explained to the students and the confidentiality was en-
sonal standards and SPP with rumination (Flett et al., 2002; Ogai, 2004). sured. Participants were 258 college students, enrolled in the first
SPP is associated with self-blame, rumination and catastrophizing and three years of various courses (Medicine, Dentistry, Psychology, Social
to lower levels of positive re-evaluation and putting into perspective Service and Health Technologies) (attrition rate = 30.26%). Their
(Rudolph, Flett, & Hewitt, 2007). mean age was 19.26 (±1.99), 79.8% were girls and there was no signif-
In line with these associations, we have found, in a previous cross- icant gender difference in age [female = 19.20 ± 2.06 vs. male = 19.52
sectional study, that maladaptive perfectionism dimensions were asso- ± 1.66, t(252) = −1.04]. They filled the Portuguese validated versions
ciated with high levels of maladaptive and low levels of adaptive CER of a set of self-reported questionnaires in two time periods: T0 (Septem-
strategies (Castro, Soares, Pereira, & Macedo, 2016). Concerning the ber 2012) and T1 (September 2013; mean months = 12.72 ± 1.08). The
adaptive perfectionism dimensions, unexpectedly we have found signif- perfectionism trait questionnaires were only administered at T0.
icant positive correlations with several maladaptive CER strategies
(consistently with self-blaming), as well as significant associations 2.2. Instruments
with adaptive CER. The association with self-blaming is not so surprising
taking in consideration the tendency for self-critical evaluations, even in All the questionnaires used in the present study revealed good reli-
“positive” perfectionists, corroborating previous findings showing that ability and validity (construct and concurrent) in Portuguese samples.
personal standards and SOP are not completely adaptive (DiBartolo, Li, The internal consistency coefficients (Cronbach's alpha) obtained with
& Frost, 2008; Frost et al., 1990) and may be involved in both positive the sample of this study are presented in Table 1.
and negative outcomes, processes and mechanisms (Stoeber & Otto,
2006). To explain this differential function it has been proposed that 2.2.1. Perfectionism
their role on psychological distress may be a function of specific media- The Portuguese versions of the Multidimensional Perfectionism
tors (Macedo et al., 2015; O'Connor, O'Connor, & Marshall, 2007). Scale (MPS) developed by Hewitt and Flett (1991) (Macedo et al.,
48 A. Macedo et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 119 (2017) 46–51

2007) and of the MPS developed by Frost et al. (1990) (Amaral, Soares, 3. Results
Pereira, et al., 2013) were both used to evaluate two composite trait per-
fectionism dimensions: perfectionistic concerns and perfectionistic 3.1. Descriptive and correlation analyses
strivings. These dimensions were derived from the factor analysis of
the items from both the Portuguese versions of MPS, which was per- Table 1 presents the descriptive data, internal consistencies obtained
formed using a students' sample which also included the participants for each scale, and the Pearson's correlations between all variables (per-
of the present study. Perfectionistic concerns dimension includes fectionism measured at T0 and other variables measured at T1). Perfec-
items from SPP, concern over mistakes, doubts about actions, parental tionistic strivings at T0 and personal standards (from the MPCI) at T1
expectations, and parental criticism; perfectionistic strivings dimension were not significantly correlated with the other variables (results not
is composed of items belonging to the original dimensions SOP, person- shown). Catastrophizing and rumination were the emotion regulation
al standards, and organization (Pereira et al., 2014). strategies that were correlated with both predictor and outcome
variables.
All three proposed mediators were correlated with predictor and
2.2.2. Perfectionism cognitions
outcome variables.
To assess the frequency of perfectionism cognitions during the last
week we used the Portuguese version of the Multidimensional Perfec-
3.2. Serial multiple mediation analyses
tionism Cognitions Inventory (MPCI; Macedo et al., 2014). MPCI com-
prises 15 items (to be answered on a 4-point scale from 1 = never to
Three serial multiple mediation models were tested to examine the
4 = always) of which five items each measure personal standards, pur-
mediation roles of maladaptive perfectionism cognitions (M1),
suit of perfection, and concern over mistakes. The last two dimensions
catastrophizing (M2), and rumination (M3) at T1 in the relationship be-
are considered maladaptive perfectionism cognitions (Stoeber et al.,
tween perfectionistic concerns at T0 on mood states at T1 (anxiety/hos-
2014).
tility, depression, and amiability/vigor). Maladaptive perfectionism
cognitions were hypothesized to have an enhancing effect on
2.2.3. Psychological distress catastrophizing and rumination, so maladaptive perfectionism cogni-
The Profile of Mood States (POMS) is commonly used to measure tions were entered first. Perceived stress and social support were en-
psychological distress (McNair, Lorr, & Droppleman, 1971). Following tered as covariates as well as the outcome variables in T0.
each adjective the subject is required to respond how he/she has been
feeling on a 5-point scale which varies from «not at all» to «extremely», 3.2.1. Anxiety/hostility
considering the previous month. We used a short version composed of The first model tested whether maladaptive perfectionism cogni-
36 items selected via factorial analysis from a sample of students tions, catastrophizing, and rumination at T1 would mediate the rela-
(POMS; Amaral, Soares, Bos, et al., 2013) that evaluates depression, anx- tionship between perfectionistic concerns at T0 and anxiety/hostility
iety/hostility and amiability/vigor. at T1 (Fig. 1).
Results indicated that the total effect of perfectionistic concerns on
anxiety/hostility was significant (Effect = 0.07, SE = 0.02, t = 3.00, p
2.2.4. Perceived stress and social support = 0.003). As can be seen in Table 2, the total indirect effect was 0.03
To measure perceived stress, the Portuguese version of the Perceived and statistically different from zero (95% CI: 0.01 to 0.07). The specific
Stress Scale-10 (PSS-10; Amaral et al., 2014) was used. To evaluate so- indirect effect of perfectionistic concerns on anxiety/hostility through
cial support we included an item – “Do you feel that, in general, you maladaptive perfectionism cognitions and catastrophizing was the
have had the support and help you need?” using the same response only different from zero, so this specific indirect effect is significantly
scale of PSS-10, from “Never” (0) to “Very often” (4) (Macedo et al., positive. This model explained 49% of anxiety/hostility variance.
2015).
3.2.2. Depression
The second model examined whether maladaptive perfectionism
2.2.5. Cognitive emotion regulation
cognitions, catastrophizing, and rumination at T1 would mediate the
The Portuguese version of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Ques-
tionnaire (Castro et al., 2013) is composed of 36 items that evaluate:
positive reappraisal and planning, positive refocusing, rumination, Table 1
blaming others, putting into perspective, self-blame, acceptance and Descriptive statistics, internal consistencies, and correlations.
catastrophizing. The items were answered on a 5-point scale from 1 2 3 4 M SD α
“Never” to “Always”.
1. Perfectionistic – 82.78 18.35 0.90
concerns
2. Maladaptive 0.46⁎⁎⁎ – 19.47 6.29 0.91
2.3. Statistical analyses
PCog (MPCI −
PP + MPCI −
Data were analyzed using SPSS 23. To examine serial multiple medi- CM)
ation models we used the Process macro developed by Hayes (2013). 3. CERQ − 0.30⁎⁎⁎ 0.44⁎⁎⁎ – 7.24 2.98 0.80
The PROCESS macro utilizes the bootstrapping method, which is a catastrophizing
4. CERQ − 0.27⁎⁎⁎ 0.43⁎⁎⁎ 0.48⁎⁎⁎ – 12.87 3.97 0.84
method of assessing direct and indirect effects of variables in a way
rumination
that maximizes power and is robust against non-normality. The indirect 5. POMS − AH 0.37⁎⁎⁎ 0.41⁎⁎⁎ 0.46⁎⁎⁎ 0.42⁎⁎⁎ 10.51 7.96 0.93
effect represents the impact of the mediator variable on the original re- 6. POMS – D 0.40⁎⁎⁎ 0.51⁎⁎⁎ 0.62⁎⁎⁎ 0.51⁎⁎⁎ 14.34 13.08 0.96
lation (i.e., the relation of the independent variable on the outcome var- 7. POMS − AV −0.25⁎⁎⁎ −0.23⁎⁎⁎ −0.23⁎⁎⁎ −0.15⁎ 21.20 5.77 0.87
iable). If 0 is not contained within the Confidence Interval (CI), this Note. PCog = perfectionism cognitions; MPCI = Multidimensional Perfectionism Cogni-
suggests that the difference between the total and direct effects was dif- tions Inventory; PP = pursuit of perfection; CM = concern over mistakes; CERQ = Cogni-
ferent from 0 and so, that the indirect effect is significant. tive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; POMS = Profile of Mood States Questionnaire; AH
= anxiety/hostility; D = depression; AV = amiability/vigor; M = mean; SD = standard
Typically, 95% bias-corrected (BC) bootstrap confidence intervals are deviation; α = Cronbach's alpha.
used to judge the significance of the indirect effect, with confidence in- ⁎ p b 0.05.
tervals resampled 5000 times for each analysis (Hayes, 2013). ⁎⁎⁎ p b 0.001.
A. Macedo et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 119 (2017) 46–51 49

Fig. 1. Serial multiple mediation model with anxiety/hostility as an outcome. Perceived stress (b = 0.51, SE = 0.09, t = 5.64, p b 0.001), and social support (b = −0.99, SE = 0.41, t =
− 2.39, p = 0.018) at T1 and anxiety/hostility (b = 0.29, SE = 0.05, t = 5.64, p b 0.001) at T0 were controlled. Numbers represent unstandardized coefficients. Numbers in
parentheses represent standard errors. PCog = perfectionism cognitions. **p b 0.01. ***p b 0.001.

relationship between perfectionistic concerns at T0 and depression at the following pathways (illustrated in Fig. 2): a1b1, a1d21b2, a1d31b3,
T1 (Fig. 2). a1d21d32b3 (Table 2). This model explained 58% of depression variance.
Results indicated that the total effect of perfectionistic concerns on
depression was significant (Effect = 0.08, SE = 0.04, t = 1.99, p = 3.2.3. Amiability/vigor
0.048). The total indirect effect was 0.06 and statistically different The third model evaluated whether maladaptive perfectionism cog-
from zero (95% CI: 0.02 to 0.13), which supports the predicted model nitions, catastrophizing, and rumination at T1 would mediate the rela-
of the serial indirect effect of perfectionistic concerns on depression tionship between perfectionistic concerns at T0 and amiability/vigor at
via maladaptive perfectionism cognitions, catastrophizing, and rumina- T1. Perceived stress (Coefficient = −0.12, SE = 0.07, t = −1.63, p =
tion. The specific indirect effects statistically different from zero were 0.104), and social support (Coefficient = 0.58, SE = 0.34, t = 1.74, p
= 0.083) at T1 and amiability/vigor (Coefficient = 0.57, SE = 0.06, t
= 9.82, p b 0.001) at T0 were controlled.
Table 2
Indirect effects of the serial multiple mediations.
Results indicated that the total effect of perfectionistic concerns on
amiability/vigor was not significant (Effect = − 0.01, SE = 0.02, t =
Coefficient SE Bootstrapping −0.73, p = 0.469). The total indirect effect was 0.00 and not statistically
BC 95% CI
different from zero (95% CI: −0.02 to 0.01), which does not support the
Lower Upper model of the serial indirect effect of perfectionistic concerns on amiabil-
OUTCOME: anxiety/hostility ity/vigor via maladaptive perfectionism cognitions, catastrophizing, and
Total effect (c) 0.07 0.02 0.02 0.11 rumination. No specific indirect effects were statistically different from
Direct effect (c′) 0.03 0.02 −0.01 0.08 zero (Table 2). This model explained 36% of amiability/vigor variance.
Indirect effects
Total indirect effect 0.03 0.01 0.01 0.07
a1b1 0.02 0.01 −0.00 0.04 4. Discussion
a1d21b2 0.01 0.00 0.00 0.02
a1d31b3 0.00 0.00 −0.00 0.01 The primary aim of this study was to test the potential mediator role
a1d21d32b3 0.00 0.00 −0.00 0.01
of perfectionism cognitions and CER strategies in the link between per-
a2b2 0.00 0.01 −0.01 0.02
a2d32b3 0.00 0.00 −0.00 0.00 fectionism traits and psychological distress (anxiety/hostility, depres-
a3b3 0.00 0.00 −0.00 0.01 sion and amiability/vigor). To our knowledge, the mediation effect of
OUTCOME: depression both perfectionism cognitions (contents) and emotion regulation strat-
Total effect (c) 0.08 0.04 0.00 0.15 egies (processes) have not been studied before.
Direct effect (c′) 0.02 0.04 −0.05 0.09
Indirect effects
In order to perform this, we started by selecting the most relevant
Total indirect effect 0.06 0.03 0.02 0.13 variables to be entered in the mediation models as predictors and medi-
a1b1 0.03 0.02 0.01 0.08 ators. Correlation analyses led us to use perfectionistic concerns as the
a1d21b2 0.02 0.01 0.01 0.04 unique predictor, as perfectionistic strivings were not significantly cor-
a1d31b3 0.01 0.00 0.00 0.02
related with the outcomes. Likewise, personal standards, which is the
a1d21d32b3 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.01
a2b2 0.00 0.01 −0.03 0.02 perfectionism cognitive dimension viewed as more adaptive (Stoeber
a2d32b3 0.00 0.00 −0.01 0.00 & Gaudreau, 2017), as expected was not significantly correlated with
a3b3 0.00 0.01 −0.01 0.02 the affect dimensions. Regarding the CER dimensions, only rumination
OUTCOME: amiability/vigor and catastrophizing were significantly correlated with perfectionistic
Total effect (c) −0.01 0.02 −0.05 −0.02
concerns, perfectionism cognitions and affect dimensions.
Direct effect (c′) −0.01 0.02 −0.05 −0.03
Indirect effects These results emphasize the clear maladaptive nature of perfection-
Total indirect effect 0.00 0.01 −0.02 0.01 istic concerns, which unambiguously revealed to be a risk factor for psy-
a1b1 0.00 0.01 −0.01 0.01 chological distress. Although in previous cross-sectional studies,
a1d21b2 0.00 0.00 −0.01 0.00
perfectionistic strivings were also significantly associated with negative
a1d31b3 0.00 0.00 −0.01 0.00
a1d21d32b3 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 affect dimensions (Castro et al., 2016; Macedo et al., 2015), it seems that
a2b2 0.00 0.00 −0.01 0.00 this association is fleeting and not sustained over time. Furthermore, in
a2d32b3 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 line with the evidence showing that perfectionistic strivings are an in-
a3b3 0.00 0.00 −0.01 0.00 consistent predictor of psychological distress, other studies did not
Note: The pathways described are illustrated in Figs. 1 and 2. found significant associations (Soares et al., 2014).
50 A. Macedo et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 119 (2017) 46–51

Fig. 2. Serial multiple mediation model with depression as an outcome. Perceived stress (b = 0.60, SE = 0.13, t = 4.44, p b 0.001), and social support (b = −2.39, SE = 0.62, t = −3.88, p b
0.001) at T1 and depression (b = 0.20, SE = 0.06, t = 3.46, p b 0.001) at T0 were controlled. Numbers represent unstandardized coefficients. Numbers in parentheses represent standard
errors. PCog = perfectionism cognitions. **p b 0.01. ***p b 0.001.

Given this, we used the serial mediation models to examine the me- As we have hypothesized, the mediational model with amiability-
diation role of maladaptive perfectionism cognitions, catastrophizing vigor as outcome was not significant. This finding is consistent with pre-
and rumination in the relationship between perfectionistic concerns vious research that suggested that perfectionistic concerns are more
and psychological distress. Perfectionism cognitions were the first me- strongly related to maladaptive outcomes, rather than positive ones
diator because we understand, as Stoeber et al. (2010) that the assess- (Egan et al., 2011).
ment of perfectionism cognitions is a complement of the assessment Although the prospective design constitutes a strength, the present
of dispositional perfectionism, thus it will provide a better comprehen- study is not without limitations. It is based on a sample exclusively com-
sion of how the perfectionistic contents relates to psychological out- posed of students. It is our intention to test this and other mediation
comes, such as cognitive processes and psychological distress. We models of the relationship between perfectionism and psychological
introduced catastrophizing before rumination in the serial model, as it distress in a clinical sample of patients with anxiety and/or depressive
is known that one way in which rumination (and excessive worry) oc- disorders. Besides, the sample was predominantly composed of femi-
curs is through the process of catastrophizing, in which individuals per- nine subjects, which may limit the generalization of these findings. Con-
sistently reiterate the problematic features of their maladaptive sequently, future studies will be necessary to demonstrate if these
cognitions (McMillan & Fisher, 2004). findings are moderated by the effect of gender. Data were obtained
In the first serial mediation model, the results showed the indirect through self-report measures, so relationships between variables may
effect of perfectionistic concerns on anxiety/hostility via maladaptive be inflated.
perfectionism cognitions and catastrophizing. Individuals with higher This study has implications for both clinical practice and research. In
perfectionistic concerns presented higher maladaptive perfectionism terms of implications for research, this study adds to the literature
cognitions, which in turn was associated with a greater tendency to concerning the link between perfectionism, cognitive processes and
catastrophize. In this cognitive framework, the individual is persuaded psychological distress. In which respects clinical implications, it high-
that the worst possible outcome will possibly occur, contributing to in- lights that perfectionist individuals may benefit from psychological in-
creased levels of anxiety/hostility. terventions aimed at reducing their tendency to engage in
Regarding the second serial mediation model with depression as the dysfunctional cognitions and to use negative emotional regulation
outcome, we found that the higher perfectionistic concerns were, the mechanisms.
more it would contribute to maladaptive perfectionism cognitions, The role of negative perfectionism as a casual risk factor for psycho-
which in turn relate to greater levels of catastrophizing and rumination, logical distress and of dysfunctional cognitions and negative CER as in-
and altogether will ultimately contribute to greater depression. The in- tervening processes will be better established within a clinical trial
direct effects of perfectionistic concerns were via maladaptive perfec- aimed at the reduction of perfectionism levels through the improve-
tionism cognitions; via maladaptive perfectionism cognitions and ment of CER strategies in patients with anxiety and/or depressive
catastrophizing; via maladaptive perfectionism cognitions and rumina- disorders.
tion and via maladaptive perfectionism cognitions, catastrophizing and
rumination.
The mediation model with anxiety/hostility as outcome suggests References
that rumination did not play a significant role in the mediation between
Amaral, A. P., Soares, M. J., Bos, S. C., Pereira, A. T., Marques, M., Valente, J., ... Macedo, A.
perfectionistic concerns and anxiety/hostility. Taking this into account, (2013a). The profile of mood states (POMS): New factor-structure in a shorter version.
we hypothesized that people who tend to engage more promptly in (Faro, Portugal).
thinking dreadfully about the consequences of an event that they have Amaral, A. P., Soares, M. J., Pereira, A. T., Bos, S. C., Marques, M., Valente, J., ... Macedo, A.
(2013b). Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale: Versão Portuguesa. Revista de
experienced will be more prone to be concerned about future threats, Psiquiatria Clinica, 40(4), 144–149 http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0101-
which is a typical feature of anxiety. 60832013000400004.
Overall, it seems that when college students with perfectionistic Amaral, A. P., Soares, M. J., Bos, S. C., Pereira, A. T., Marques, M., Valente, J., ... Macedo, A.
(2014). The Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-10) - A Portuguese version. XVI world con-
concerns and maladaptive perfectionism cognitions after one year
gress of psychiatry (pp. 390) (Madrid).
only catastrophize over a past event they present anxiety/hostility or Azevedo, M. H., Bos, S. C., Soares, M. J., Marques, M., Pereira, A. T., Maia, B., ... Macedo, A.
depression, but when they catastrophized and then ruminate about (2010). Longitudinal study on perfectionism and sleep disturbance. The World
Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 11(2), 476–485 http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/
their feelings and thoughts associated with the negative event they ex-
15622970903304467.
hibit a depressive mood state. This is in line with several studies, in Bieling, P. J., Israeli, A. L., & Antony, M. M. (2004). Is perfectionism good, bad, or both? Ex-
which there is a link between ruminative cognitive style and depression amining models of the perfectionism construct. Personality and Individual Differences,
(Joormann & Arditte, 2014; McMillan & Fisher, 2004). 36, 1373–1385http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00235-6.
A. Macedo et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 119 (2017) 46–51 51

Castro, J., Chaves, B., Pereira, A. T., Soares, M. J., Amaral, A. P., Bos, S., ... Macedo, A. (2013). Macedo, A., Soares, M. J., Amaral, A. P., Nogueira, V., Madeira, N., Roque, C., ... Pereira, A. T.
Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: Validation of the Portuguese version, 162. (2015). Repetitive negative thinking mediates the association between perfectionism
Castro, J., Soares, M. J., Pereira, A. T., & Macedo, A. (2016). Perfectionism, cognitive emo- and psychological distress. Personality and Individual Differences, 72, 220–224 http://
tion regulation and perceived distress/coping. European psychiatry. 33S. (pp. S213). dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.08.024.
Elsevier Masson SAS http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.01.513. Maia, B. R., Soares, M. J., Gomes, A., Marques, M., Pereira, A. T., Cabral, A., ... Macedo, A.
Chan, C. K. Y., & Owens, R. G. (2007). Perfectionism and eating disorder symptomatology (2009). Perfectionism in obsessive-compulsive and eating disorders. Revista
in Chinese immigrants: Mediating and moderating effects of ethnic identity and ac- Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 31(4), 322–327 http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-
culturation. Psychology & Health, 21(1), 49–63 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ 44462009005000004.
14768320500105312. Maia, B. R., Pereira, A. T., Marques, M., Bos, S., Soares, M. J., Valente, J., ... Macedo, A. (2012).
Chan, C. K. Y., Ku, Y., & Owens, R. G. (2010). Perfectionism and eating disturbances in Ko- The role of perfectionism in postpartum depression and symptomatology. Archives of
rean immigrants: Moderating effects of acculturation and ethnic identity. Asian Women's Mental Health, 15(6), 459–468 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00737-012-
Journal of Social Psychology, 13, 293–302 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-839X. 0310-2.
2010.01326.x. McMillan, D., & Fisher, P. (2004). Cognitive therapy for depressive thinking. In C.
Choo, S. Y., & Chan, C. K. Y. (2013). Predicting eating problems among Malaysian Chinese: Papageorgiou, & A. Wells (Eds.), Depressive rumination: Nature, theory and treatment
Differential roles of positive and negative perfectionism. Personality and Individual (pp. 241–258). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Differences, 54, 744–749 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.036. McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. F. (1971). Manual for the profile of mood states.
DiBartolo, P. M., Li, C. Y., & Frost, R. O. (2008). How do the dimensions of perfectionism San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Services.
relate to mental health? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 401–417 http://dx.doi. O'Connor, R. C. (2007). The relations between perfectionism and suicidality: A systematic
org/10.1007/s10608-007-9157-7. review. Suicide & Life-Threatening Behavior, 37(6), 698–714 http://dx.doi.org/10.
Dunkley, D. M., Blankstein, K. R., Halsall, J., Williams, M., & Winkworth, G. (2000). The re- 1521/suli.2007.37.6.698.
lation between perfectionism and distress: Hassles, coping, and perceived social sup- O'Connor, D. B., O'Connor, R. C., & Marshall, R. (2007). Perfectionism and psychological
port as mediators and moderators. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47(4), 437–453 distress: Evidence of the mediating effects of rumination. European Journal of
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/AW22-0167.47.4.437. Personality, 21, 429–452 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.616.
Dunkley, D. M., Blankstein, K. R., Masheb, R. M., & Grilo, C. M. (2006). Personal standards Ogai, Y. (2004). Relationship between two aspects of self-oriented perfectionism and self-
and evaluative concerns dimensions of “clinical” perfectionism: A reply to Shafran et evaluative depression: Using coping styles of uncontrollable events as mediators.
al. (2002, 2003) and Hewitt et al. (2003). Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 63–84 Shinrigaku Kenkyu, 75(3), 199–206.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.12.004. Pereira, A. T., Soares, M. J., Amaral, A. P., Nogueira, V., Madeira, N., Roque, C., ... Macedo, A.
Egan, S. J., Wade, T. D., & Shafran, R. (2011). Perfectionism as a transdiagnostic process: A (2014). The Multidimensional Perfectionism Scales: Combined factorial analysis of
clinical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 203–212 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. the Portuguese versions. XVI World Congress of Psychiatry (pp. 443) (Madrid).
cpr.2010.04.009. Rudolph, S. G., Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2007). Perfectionism and deficits in cognitive
Flett, G. L., Madorsky, D., Hewitt, P. L., & Heisel, M. J. (2002). Perfectionism cognitions, ru- emotion regulation. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 25(4),
mination, and psychological distress. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior 343–357 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10942-007-0056-3.
Therapy, 20(1), 33–47 http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1015128904007. Sherry, S. B., Law, A., Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., & Besser, A. (2008). Social support as a me-
Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of perfection- diator of the relationship between perfectionism and depression: A preliminary test
ism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14(5), 449–468 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ of the social disconnection model. Personality and Individual Differences, 45,
BF01172967. 339–344 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.05.001.
Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2007). The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: Psy- Slade, P. D., & Owens, R. G. (1998). A dual process model of perfectionism based on rein-
chometric features and prospective relationships with depression and anxiety in forcement theory. Behaviour Modification, 22(3), 372–390 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
adults. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23(3), 141–149 http://dx.doi. 01454455980223010.
org/10.1027/1015-5759.23.3.141. Smith, M. M., Saklofske, D. H., Yan, G., & Sherry, S. B. (2015). Perfectionistic strivings and
Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., & Spinhoven, P. (2001). Negative life events, cognitive emotion perfectionistic concerns interact to predict negative emotionality: Support for the tri-
regulation and emotional problems. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, partite model of perfectionism in Canadian and Chinese university students.
1311–1327http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00113-6. Personality and Individual Differences, 81, 141–147 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.
Gross, J. J., & Muñoz, R. F. (1995). Emotion regulation and mental health. Clinical 2014.09.006.
Psychology: Science and Practice, 2, 151–164 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2850. Soares, M. J., Macedo, A., Bos, S. C., Marques, M., Maia, B., Pereira, A. T., ... Azevedo, M. H.
1995.tb00036.x. (2009). Perfectionism and eating attitudes in Portuguese students: A longitudinal
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: study. European Eating Disorders Review, 17(5), 390–398 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
A regression-based approach. New York: The Guilford Press. erv.926.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts: Conceptu- Soares, M. J., Amaral, A. P., Pereira, A. T., Bos, S., Marques, M., Valente, J., ... Macedo, A.
alization, assessment, and association with psychopathology. Journal of Personality (2014). Multidimensional Perfectionism Scales underlying higher order factors. In
and Social Psychology, 60(3), 456–470 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.60.3.456. K. Kaniasty, K. Moore, S. Howard, & P. Buchwald (Eds.), Stress and anxiety: Applications
Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., & Ediger, E. (1996). Perfectionism and depression: Longitudinal to social and environmental threats, psychological well-being, occupational challenges,
assessment of a specific vulnerability hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, and developmental psychology (pp. 115–125). Berlin: Logos Verlag Berlin GmbH.
105(2), 276–280 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.105.2.276. Stoeber, J., & Gaudreau, P. (2017). The advantages of partialling perfectionistic strivings
Joormann, J., & Arditte, K. (2014). Cognitive aspects of depression. In E. S. Friedman, & I. M. and perfectionistic concerns: Critical issues and recommendations. Personality and
Anderson (Eds.), Handbook of depression (pp. 259–276) (3rd ed ). New York: The Individual Differences, 104, 379–386 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.08.039.
Guilford Press. Stoeber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of perfectionism: Approaches, evidence,
Kung, C. S. J., & Chan, C. K. Y. (2014). Differential roles of positive and negative perfection- challenges. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 295–319 http://dx.doi.org/
ism in predicting occupational eustress and distress. Personality and Individual 10.1207/s15327957pspr1004.
Differences, 58, 76–81 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.10.011. Stoeber, J., Kobori, O., & Tanno, Y. (2010). The Multidimensional Perfectionism Cognitions
Limburg, K., Watson, H. J., Hagger, M. S., & Egan, S. J. (2016). The relationship between inventory–English (MPCI-E): Reliability, validity, and relationships with positive and
perfectionism and psychopathology: A meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology, negative affect. Journal of Personality Assessment, 92(1), 16–25 http://dx.doi.org/10.
0(0), 1–26 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22435. 1080/00223890903379159.
Macedo, A., Soares, M. J., Azevedo, M. H., Gomes, A., Pereira, A. T., Maia, B., & Pato, M. Stoeber, J., Kobori, O., & Brown, A. (2014). Perfectionism cognitions are multidimensional:
(2007). Perfectionism and eating attitudes in Portuguese university students. A reply to Flett and Hewitt (2014). Assessment, 21(6), 666–668 http://dx.doi.org/10.
European Eating Disorders Review, 15(4), 296–304 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/erv.735. 1177/1073191114550676.
Macedo, A., Soares, M. J., Amaral, A. P., Castro, J., Chaves, B., Bos, S., ... Pereira, A. T. (2014). Terry-Short, L. A., Owens, R. G., Slade, P. D., & Dewey, M. E. (1995). Positive and negative
Multidimensional Perfectionism Cognitions inventory: Validation of the Portuguese perfectionism. Personality and Individual Differences, 18(5), 663–668http://dx.doi.org/
version. In K. Kaniasty, K. Moore, S. Howard, & P. Buchwald (Eds.), Stress and anxiety: 10.1016/0191-8869(94)00192-U.
Applications to social and environmental threats, psychological well-being, occupational
challenges, and developmental psychology (pp. 107–114). Berlin: Logos Verlag Berlin
GmbH.

You might also like