Antropometría SW PDF

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Received: 21 February 2020 Revised: 9 May 2020 Accepted: 17 May 2020

DOI: 10.1002/ajmg.a.61750

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

A study on facial features of children with Williams syndrome


in China based on three-dimensional anthropometric
measurement technology

Chai Ji | Dan Yao | Ming-Yan Li | Wei-Jun Chen | Sheng-Liang Lin |


Zheng-Yan Zhao

Department of Pediatric Health Care,


The Children's Hospital, Zhejiang University Abstract
School of Medicine, National Clinical Research To describe special facial features of children with Williams syndrome in China by
Center for Health, Zhejiang, Hangzhou, China
using method of three-dimensional craniofacial anthropometry. Using three-
Correspondence dimensional stereo photogrammetric device, 14 craniofacial anthropometric mea-
Zheng-Yan Zhao, The Children's Hospital,
Zhejiang University School of Medicine, surements were performed and five indices were calculated in 52 children with
Department of Pediatric Health Care, Williams syndrome and 208 age and sex matched controls of Han Chinese ethnicity.
57 Zhugan Lane, Yanan Road, Hangzhou,
Zhejiang 310003, China. Except intercanthal width, mouth breadth, morphological face height, nasal height-
Email: zhaozy@zju.edu.cn breadth index, nasal breadth-depth index, morphological ear index, the Williams syn-
Funding information drome group under 3 years old were smaller than the control group in the other
Natural Science Foundation of Zhejiang 12 variables. Compared with the control group, the Williams syndrome group aged
Province, Grant/Award Number:
LQ20H090017 3–5 years old had smaller biocular breadth, nasal length, nasorostral angle, bitragal
breadth, ear width, morphological ear index and face depth. The Williams syndrome
group aged above 6 years old had smaller biocular breadth, nasal breadth, bitragal
breadth, ear width, ear length and face depth than the control group. The craniofacial
variability index of the Williams syndrome group was greater than the control group.
Greater variation was found among children with Williams syndrome than normal in
China, specifically at eye, nose, ear and face shape, which demonstrate the usefulness
of three-dimensional stereo photogrammetric analysis in supporting accurate diag-
nose of the patient with Williams syndrome.

KEYWORDS

children, Williams syndrome, three-dimensions, facial feature

1 | I N T RO DU CT I O N 20,000–50,000 of live births (Strømme, Bjørnstad, & Ramstad, 2002)


and the reported incidence in Hong Kong is 1 in 23,500 (Hirai
Williams syndrome (WS), also known as Williams–Beuren syndrome et al., 2016).
(WBS), is an infrequent genetic disorder characterized by a series of Almost every child with WS has special face, so they were called
typical physical and mental features. This disorder is mainly related to an “elfin face” (Black & Carter, 1963; Jones & Smith, 1975; McNamara
a deletion of a number of genes on chromosome 7q11.23, and the Jr., 1984) when it was first recognized. “Elfin face” usually refers to
incidence ranges from 1:7500 to 1:25000 (Riby, Kirk, Hanley, & a small face, but the head measurements and a series of other
Riby, 2014) due to inconsistent recognition of WS across different studies showed that the face of children with WS were not that small.
countries and regions. The incidence reported by Stromme is 1 in So many more physicians suggested that the designation of “elfin

Am J Med Genet. 2020;1–8. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/ajmga © 2020 Wiley Periodicals LLC 1


2 JI ET AL.

face” for children with WS was not scientific and representative diagnosed with WS due to deletion of genes on chromosome 7q11.23
(Burn, 1986; Mass & Belostoky, 1993). Currently the clinical diagnosis by FISH, MPLA or CMA technique,were collected. They were gath-
of WS mainly relies on the evaluation of facial features, vascular mal- ered from the outpatient or cardiovascular ward of the hospital or var-
formation, developmental delay and intellectual disability, then sug- ious special schools or welfare homes across the nation. They were all
gests the suspected patient with WS to take genetic detection (Ji, Han Chinese, no minority. With the consent from parents and guard-
Yao, Chen, Li, & Zhao, 2014; Martens, 2013; Sugayama, Leone, ians, their 3D facial data were collected. As children's faces vary with
Chauffaille Mde, Okay, & Kim, 2007). Conventional facial features age, children with WS enrolled in this study were divided into three
used to distinguish WS include prominent ears, large earlobes, swollen age groups, that is, under 3 years old (infants and toddlers), 3–5 years
eyelids, strabismus, full cheeks, low nasal bridge, anterior-directed old (preschool), and above 6 years old, respectively.
nostrils, long philtrum, wide mouth with full lips, protruding and con-
stantly open mouth, dental malocclusion (American Academy of
Pediatrics, 2001; Chen, Ji, Yao, & Zhao, 2017; Järvinen, Korenberg, & 2.1.2 | The control group
Bellugi, 2013). These facial features provide an initial clue for the
screening of this disease and are expected to become a breakthrough Two hundred and eight control children of the same Han ethnicity
point for noninvasive screening and diagnosis. were selected for 3D facial data collection at 1:4 matching ratio. The
In the 1990s, with the quick development of computer-aided control group with under 3 years old was composed of healthy infants
detection and the technological improvement in facial measurement, and toddlers that had taken general physical examinations in various
some scholars analyzed the facial features of eight children with WS community health centers. The 3–18 years old group were healthy
(aged 4–11 years) by collecting the data from their heads and faces. children and teenagers that had physical checkup in our outpatient
Confined by a small sample size and the availability of few features in clinic. In order to eliminate the error and the interference of exces-
respect of bone development, these data were insufficient to fully por- sively large and small human faces to data analysis, children whose
tray the facial features of children with WS (Axelsson, Kjaer, Heiberg, height and weight were above the 97th or under third percentiles on
Bjørnland, & Storhaug, 2005; Mass & Belostoky, 1993). As the devel- the Chinese Centers for Disease Control (CDC) growth standard curve
opment of soft tissue is more characteristic and observable, objective were excluded, to ensure that the physical development of each child
description of facial soft tissues and organs has become a hot direction in the same age group was at the normal level.
for research. The molecular biology technique is not an ideal choice
as it cannot be used for wide population, and has caused delay
in the diagnosis of many diseased individuals. How to screen and iden- 2.2 | Methods
tify patients with WS in a convenient, economic, and effective way
has become one of the problems that researchers are eager to solve. The 3dMD face system, manufactured by 3dMD (www.3dmd.com/
Hammond et al. were among the first applying three-dimensional 3dMDface/), was used to obtain 3D facial models. Each scan results
(3D) imaging technology to the diagnosis and adjuvant treatment of in a triangulated surface mesh model of the subject's face with
hereditary diseases with special facial features (Hammond et al., 2005). 4,000–20,000 vertices. All subjects gave informed consent, and each
In addition, Paul et al. comfirmed the special face by analyzing subject's gender, birthday, height, weight, the history of disease and
286 Asian, African, Caucasian, and Latin American individuals with WS facial surgery were recorded. Children, especially infants, often need
using facial analysis technology (Kruszka et al., 2018). the researcher's help to present the correct facial posture. Without
This study used 3D stereo-photogrammetric device to obtain the assistance, redundant data may appear which would have to be
3D images of facial surface of children with WS in China. This meth- removed after selecting the face region.
odology is characterized by its portability, high capture speed, high Research had shown the measurement error to be within as
accuracy, and had no side effects on the human body. All these little as 1/100 of an inch (Weinberg et al., 2006). Some researchers
advantages make it possible to collect objective datasets of the chil- had evaluated measurement accuracy and developed software to
dren's face images, which can aid in the recognition of children make 3dMDface a stronger tool with which to study anthropometry
with WS. (Aldridge, Boyadjiev, Capone, DeLeon, & Richtsmeier, 2005).

2 | METHOD 2.2.1 | Preprocessing the face model

2.1 | Materials The face region was isolated and segmented. As there were often
missing data on top of the head and in the nostrils and ears, such
2.1.1 | The WS group noise data points were cleaned and the holes filled. Spikes generated
by vertices leaping from the surface were reduced using a Gaussian
From June 2009 to May 2016, the data of 52 patients, including filter function. Posture positioning helped extract facial feature points
25 boys (aged 1.1 to 13.3 years) and 27 girls (aged 0.7 to 15 years), more accurately.
JI ET AL. 3

2.2.2 | Set 21 facial feature landmarks 9. Middle face depth (tragion–subnasale) (t-sn): between the tragion
point and the subnasal landmark, the midpoint at the base of the
The nasion, endocanthion (left and right), exocanthion (left and right), columella.
alare (left and right), subnasale, pronasale, cheilion (left and right), sup- 10. Lower face depth (tragion–gnathion) (t-gn): between the tragion
eraurale (left and right), subaurale (left and right), postaurale (left and landmark and the chin point.
right), tragion (left and right), gnathion, and lip center (Figure 1). Using 11. Bitragal width (tragion–tragion) (t–t): between the right and left
these points, we computed distance measurements, facial indices and tragion landmarks.
the CVI. Distance was measured in the unit of millimeters. 12. Ear length (superaurale–subaurale) (spa-sba): between the level
Anthropometric measurements and calculated indices: of the highest point on the free margin of the auricle and
the level of the lowest point on the free margin of the ear lobe.
1. Intercanthal width (endocanthion–endocanthion) (en-en): between We measured the both ears length and calculated the average
the landmarks at the inner commissures of the right and left palpe- value.
bral fissures. 13. Ear width (tragion–postaurale) (t-poa): between the most anterior
2. Biocular width (exocanthion–exocanthion) (ex-ex): between the point and most posterior point of the ear. We measured the both
right and left landmarks of the exocanthions at the outer commis- ears width and calculated the average value.
sures of the palpebral fissures. 14. Nasorostral angle (NRA): < nasion, pronasale, subnasale>.
3. Nose height (subnasale–pronasale) (sn-pn): between the mid- 15. Physiognomic upper facial index (PUFI): n-gn / t–t × 100.
points at the base of the columella and the highest point at the 16. Length–breadth index of the nose (LBIN): n-sn / al-al × 100.
top of the columella. 17. Height–breadth index of the nose (HBIN): al-al / sn-pn × 100.
4. Nose width (alare–alare) (al-al): between the most lateral points 18. Morphological ear index (MEI): t-poa / spa-sba × 100.
of the nasal alare.
5. Nose length (nasion–subnasale) (n-sn): between the nasion land- Craniofacial variability index (CVI): The CVI can assist in the eval-
mark at the root of the nose and the midpoint at the base of the uation of facial anatomy and defines the range of the normal distribu-
columella. tion of craniofacial morphology (Roelfsema, Hop, van Adrichem, &
6. Mouth width (cheilion–cheilion) (ch-ch): between the right and Wladimiroff, 2007; Ward, Jamison, & Farkas, 1998). It was calculated
left cheilion landmarks. in the following steps: means and standard deviations (SDs) were com-
7. Face height (nasion–gnathion) (n-gn): between the nasion point at puted for each of the gender categories and age; each measurement
the root of the nose and the chin point. was then converted to z-scores; z-score = (measurement−age-specific
8. Upper face depth (tragion–nasion) (t-n): between the tragion point, mean value)/SD; the individual's variability index was calculated by
located just above the tragus of the ear, and the nasion landmark. taking the SD of the z-scores for the 18 variables.
Each human face was located accurately using landmarks auto-
matically extracted with manual adjustment. Landmarks on each face
were located twice, and the coordinates of the two points averaged,
to reduce measurement error.
All photo-shooting works of children were carried out under the
consent granted by themselves or by their parents or guardians by
signing an informed consent form and were subject to supervision of
the Ethics Committee of the Children's Hospital Affiliated to Zhejiang
University School of Medicine.

2.3 | Statistical method

The quantitative data in respect of three-dimensional face imaging


was expressed as mean ± SD and the qualitative data was expressed
as the number of cases (percentage). The quantitative data of these
two groups were then compared by applying the t-test for two inde-
pendent samples and the qualitative data were analyzed by chi-square
test. Benjamini-Hochberg method was used to calculated false discov-
ery rate (FDR). Multivariate analysis of variance (ANOVO) were used
to compare the overall difference. Statistical analysis was performed
F I G U R E 1 Portrait and profile views of 21 landmarks. (a) Normal
child. (b) Child with WS [Color figure can be viewed at using SAS 9.2 statistical software and adjusted p < .05 indicates statis-
wileyonlinelibrary.com] tical significance.
4 JI ET AL.

3 | RESULTS (5) Frontal and Profile Face Morphology: The WS group had
greater physiognomic upper facial index than the control group but
There was no statistically difference detected by chi-square test did not differ in terms of morphological face height. Its upper, middle,
between the WS group and the control group compared for age and and lower face depths were all smaller compared with the control
sex (p > .05). No statistically difference was detected in 18 variables group. But in under 3 years old, the WS group had smaller morpholog-
between boys and girls (p > .05). The overall features of the faces ical face height than the control group (t = 2.71, p = .014) and did not
were statistically different between the WS group and the control differ in physiognomic upper facial index (p > .05).
group in different age groups. (p < .001).

3.2 | Craniofacial variability index (CVI)


3.1 | The WS group compared with the control
group The CVI of the WS group (1.25 ± 0.38) was greater than that of the
control group (0.83 ± 0.37), showing a statistically significant differ-
(1) Eyes: All the WS groups had an evidently smaller biocular breadth ence (p < .001), but the CVI between boys and girls inside the WS
than the control groups (p < .001) (Tables 1–4). There was no differ- group had no statistically difference (25 boys: 1.29 ± 0.50; 27 girls:
ence between the two groups in terms of the intercanthal width 1.21 ± 0.21; t = 0.77, p = 0.445).
except in the group under 3 years old (t = 3.36, p = .003).
(2) Nose: Compared with the control group, the WS group was
smaller in nasal height, nasal breadth, nasal length, and nasorostral 4 | DI SCU SSION
angle (for nasal height, t = 2.15, p = .049; for nasal breadth, t = 2.41,
p = .028; for nasal length, t = 2.40, p = .028; and for nasorostral angle, 3D surface imaging systems, which can generate high-density face
t = 2.89,p = 0.008) but was no difference in nasal height-breadth models and allow us to take accurate measurements, is a good way to
index and nasal breadth-depth index (p > .05). This difference faded get relatively objective data about the facial variation in children with
with age. There was no difference between the WS group and the WS. Our study found that children with WS had shorter biocular
control group in terms of nose variables above 6 years old (p > .05). breadth than the control children at any age. They were also shorter in
(3) Mouth: The mouth breadth of the WS group was no differ- intercanthal width under 3 years old. Children with WS in their infancy
ence from that of the control group in different age groups (p > .05). and toddler are more likely to have periorbital fullness, epicanthus, and
(4) Ears: Compared with the control group, the WS group was depressed nasal root, creating an illusion of wide interpupillary dis-
smaller in ear length, ear width and bitragal breadth but was no differ- tance when observed by medical staff. These symptoms accompanied
ence in terms of morphological ear index (p > .05). Small changes were by evident growth retardation and development delay lead some
found by different age groups. There was statistically difference in young children with WS to be falsely diagnosed with the most com-
morphological ear index between 3–5 years old groups and no differ- mon chromosomal disorder—Down's syndrome. However, measure-
ence in ear width between under 3 years old groups. ments suggested that children with WS had shorter interpupillary

T A B L E 1 Summery of facial features


Variable WS group (n = 52) Control group (n = 208) t value Adjusted p value
difference between WS group with
ex_ex 80.58 ± 7.70 87.60 ± 8.89 5.22 <.001 normal group (distance: mm)
sub_pro 11.93 ± 2.99 12.81 ± 2.56 2.15 .049
al_al 28.80 ± 4.00 30.32 ± 4.08 2.41 .028
n_sub 34.84 ± 6.49 36.90 ± 5.27 2.40 .028
tr_n 96.09 ± 9.54 102.08 ± 9.61 4.02 <.001
tr_sub 98.40 ± 10.38 104.08 ± 10.70 3.45 .002
tr_gn 105.66 ± 11.85 112.61 ± 13.36 3.43 .002
t_t 121.32 ± 9.86 132.13 ± 11.15 6.39 <.001
spa_spa 46.58 ± 4.71 48.86 ± 5.19 2.89 .008
t_poa 25.56 ± 2.31 27.58 ± 2.80 4.80 <.001
NRA 39.70 ± 7.75 42.73 ± 6.51 2.89 .008
PUFI 69.30 ± 5.67 65.40 ± 4.27 −5.49 <.001
F value p value
Multivariate ANOVA 12.88 <.001

Note: NRA and PUFI are not distances.


JI ET AL. 5

T A B L E 2 Facial features comparison


Variable WS group (n = 16) Control group (n = 64) t value Adjusted p value
between WS group and normal group
under 3 years old (distance: mm) en_en 28.95 ± 2.72 31.04 ± 2.10 3.36 .003
ex_ex 73.76 ± 5.56 79.24 ± 4.12 4.42 <.001
sub_pro 9.34 ± 1.13 10.91 ± 2.16 2.81 .013
al_al 25.83 ± 1.71 27.00 ± 2.76 1.61 .133
n_sub 27.75 ± 3.49 31.52 ± 2.39 5.12 <.001
ch_ch 30.95 ± 3.00 30.93 ± 3.41 −0.02 .984
gn_n 72.21 ± 6.33 76.26 ± 5.07 2.71 .014
tr_n 85.71 ± 4.99 91.70 ± 4.45 4.70 <.001
tr_sub 86.61 ± 4.76 92.46 ± 4.49 4.60 <.001
tr_gn 92.30 ± 6.29 98.31 ± 5.63 3.74 .001
t_t 111.17 ± 6.78 120.00 ± 5.39 5.55 <.001
spa_spa 41.76 ± 3.76 44.16 ± 3.04 2.69 .014
t_poa 24.45 ± 2.19 25.71 ± 2.12 2.10 .058
NRA 31.32 ± 3.82 36.31 ± 3.34 5.20 <.001
PUFI 64.99 ± 4.55 63.56 ± 3.25 −1.44 .172
LBIN 107.36 ± 10.71 118.87 ± 24.82 1.81 .103
HBIN 280.32 ± 37.15 256.98 ± 51.88 −1.69 .122
MEI 58.81 ± 5.73 58.29 ± 3.96 −0.43 .712
F value p value
Multivariate ANOVA 3.99 <.001

Note: NRA, PUFI, LBIN, HBIN and MEI are not distances.

T A B L E 3 Facial features comparison


Variable WS group (n = 15) Control group (n = 60) t value Adjusted P value
between WS group and normal group
3–5 years old (distance: mm) en_en 31.26 ± 2.82 32.03 ± 2.90 0.93 .430
ex_ex 79.46 ± 5.41 85.72 ± 4.29 4.78 <.001
sub_pro 11.60 ± 1.35 12.59 ± 2.25 1.64 .147
al_al 27.69 ± 2.13 29.39 ± 3.17 1.96 .088
n_sub 34.14 ± 3.53 36.22 ± 2.95 2.34 .040
ch_ch 37.47 ± 3.16 34.96 ± 5.73 −1.63 .147
gn_n 82.84 ± 6.84 84.89 ± 7.46 0.96 .430
tr_n 96.07 ± 5.74 101.34 ± 4.28 3.97 .001
tr_sub 98.23 ± 5.89 102.94 ± 4.42 3.44 .005
tr_gn 105.34 ± 6.71 110.68 ± 5.69 3.14 .007
t_t 120.84 ± 7.11 132.01 ± 5.33 6.77 <.001
spa_spa 48.52 ± 3.78 48.73 ± 3.49 0.21 .884
t_poa 26.18 ± 2.25 28.14 ± 2.65 2.63 .021
NRA 38.98 ± 3.99 41.79 ± 3.63 2.63 .021
PUFI 68.52 ± 3.48 64.27 ± 4.67 −3.30 .005
LBIN 123.61 ± 12.46 124.87 ± 20.51 0.23 .884
HBIN 241.17 ± 28.47 239.89 ± 43.23 −0.11 .914
MEI 54.03 ± 3.23 57.83 ± 4.61 3.00 .010
F value p value
Multivariate ANOVA 9.56 <.001

Note: NRA, PUFI, LBIN, HBIN and MEI are not distances.
6 JI ET AL.

T A B L E 4 Facial features comparison


Variable WS group (n = 21) Control group (n = 84) t value Adjusted p value
between WS group and normal group
en_en 35.67 ± 3.24 35.48 ± 4.51 −0.19 .901 above 6 years old (distance: mm)
ex_ex 86.58 ± 5.62 95.33 ± 7.36 5.08 <.001
sub_pro 14.13 ± 3.17 14.41 ± 1.95 0.51 .733
al_al 31.87 ± 4.19 33.52 ± 3.01 2.08 .081
n_sub 40.75 ± 3.49 41.50 ± 3.85 0.81 .580
ch_ch 42.52 ± 4.56 42.79 ± 5.35 0.21 .901
gn_n 94.66 ± 7.96 95.65 ± 7.54 0.53 .733
tr_n 104.01 ± 6.20 110.51 ± 6.84 3.96 <.001
tr_sub 107.50 ± 5.96 113.76 ± 7.52 3.54 .002
tr_gn 116.07 ± 6.34 124.87 ± 9.25 4.11 <.001
t_t 129.40 ± 5.19 141.46 ± 8.13 6.46 <.001
spa_spa 48.87 ± 3.00 52.54 ± 4.56 3.50 .002
t_poa 25.96 ± 2.24 28.60 ± 2.69 4.16 <.001
NRA 46.60 ± 4.96 48.30 ± 4.91 1.42 .261
PUFI 73.14 ± 5.23 67.62 ± 3.66 −5.64 <.001
LBIN 130.34 ± 24.39 124.27 ± 11.50 −1.67 .177
HBIN 235.45 ± 52.80 235.97 ± 32.44 0.06 .955
MEI 53.27 ± 5.40 54.70 ± 5.84 1.02 .465
F value p value
Multivariate ANOVA 8.89 <.001

Note: NRA, PUFI, LBIN, HBIN and MEI are not distances.

distance than control children and this trait was particularly evident nose tip are commonly used. In our study, we found that children
under 3 years old. In fact, abnormal interpupillary distance is a highly with WS had smaller nose, lower nasal bridge, and anterior-directed
frequent clinical manifestation in many chromosomal disorders and nostrils compared with the control children but differ little in terms of
should be measured objectively rather than judged subjectively. Due their proportions. The feature of chubby nose was not found in these
to technical limitations, objective measurement of the periorbital full- young patients. We found that children with WS had shorter and
ness remains a problem. It requires the further refinement of eye fea- lower nose than the control children in the infancy and toddler period.
tures before they can be used as basis for identification. They had the normal nasal height in the 3–5 years old group and to
Wide mouth with full lips in children with WS has always been the normal nasal shape in the above 6 years old group. These features
recognized as one of the highly frequent facial manifestations. Chil- indicated that the nose of children with WS gradually closed to normal
dren with WS enrolled in this study visually exhibit typical mouth fea- as age increased. Nose growing from small to normal level with age
tures but showed no statistically significant difference from control was also one of the notable features distinguishing children with WS
children across all age groups in terms of mouth breadth. Considering from children with Down's syndrome.
low cognitive ability and over-friendly behaviors, children with WS Common descriptions about ear features in children with WS are
displayed rich facial expressions during the photo moment, including prominent ears and large earlobes. Our study found that the ears of
deliberately compressing their lips or opening or closing their mouths. children with WS were smaller than those of the normal group but dif-
Nevertheless, the 3dMDface system captured facial images at an fer little in terms of their morphological proportion. For the size of the
ultra-fast speed of 1.5 ms and the pictures collected exhibited basi- earlobe, no objective indicator could be accurately embodied in this
cally qualified natural expressions. By analysis, this phenomenon may study. Our findings indicated that the ear width and bitragal breadth
be attributed to the sharp contrast between the relatively narrow face were smaller in children with WS above 3 years old than the normal
commonly existing in children with WS and their normal mouth groups. These two indicators were the comparatively stable and mea-
breadth, which result to visually wider mouth. Hence in subsequent surable items and be used clinically to distinguish children with WS
studies, it will be necessary to take into account mouth movements of from normal children.
children with WS, and add the lip thickness index for the purpose of Many facial descriptions of children with WS are mainly bitemporal
better analyzing and evaluating the mouth shape in an objective narrowness, full cheeks, and small jaws. Small jaw is an easy feature
manner. to judge, but the concepts of bitemporal narrowness and full cheek are
When describing the nose of children with WS in prior studies, less understandable for many medical staffs. Our study found that
low nasal bridge, short nose, anterior-directed nostrils, and/or chubby infants and toddlers with WS were small in face shape but had normal
JI ET AL. 7

facial proportion and less distinct bitemporal narrowness, and these DATA AVAILABILITY STAT EMEN T
facial features tended to become evident in children with WS above Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no new data were cre-
3 years old. Also, the prevalence of widely spaced small teeth and dental ated or analyzed in this study.
malocclusion distincted their facial features as age increases in children
with WS (Axelsson, 2005; Sinha, Tamang, & Chakraborty, 2014).
OR CID
CVI is a comprehensive assessment of facial features. The higher
Zheng-Yan Zhao https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2368-0486
the CVI, the more incongruent the facial variability is. In our study, we
found the CVI in the WS group was greater than that of the control
RE FE RE NCE S
group, which confirmed that children with WS had very different
American Academy of Pediatrics. (2001). Health care supervision for chil-
facial features than normal children. Some studies found that there dren with Williams syndrome. Pediatrics, 107, 1192–1204.
was a certain correlation between CVI and age though the correlation Aldridge, K., Boyadjiev, S. A., Capone, G. T., DeLeon, V. B., &
coefficient was small, and the difference between genders was statis- Richtsmeier, J. T. (2005). Precision and error of three-dimensional phe-
notypic measures acquired from 3dMD photogrammetric images.
tically significant (Farkas et al., 2005; Zhuang, Landsittel, Benson,
American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A, 138a, 247–253.
Roberge, & Shaffer, 2010). However, we found no difference between Axelsson, S. (2005). Variability of the cranial and dental phenotype in
boys and girls with WS. This insignificant gender difference in WS Williams syndrome. Swedish Dental Journal Supplement, 170, 3–67.
may be attributed to the small sample size. Axelsson, S., Kjaer, I., Heiberg, A., Bjørnland, T., & Storhaug, K. (2005).
Neurocranial morphology and growth in Williams syndrome. European
The pathological mechanism behind facial dysplasia is not yet
Journal of Orthodontics, 27, 32–47.
known. It is thought to be related to the deletion genes that regulate Black, J. A., & Carter, R. E. (1963). Association between aortic stenosis and
elastin, and among them, the GTF2IRD1 is the most studied (Tassabehji facies of severe infantile hypercalcaemia. Lancet, 2, 745–749.
et al., 2005). The establishment of animal models had provided clues for Burn, J. (1986). Williams syndrome. Journal of Medical Genetics, 23,
389–395.
better analysis of genes and phenotypic characteristics (Osborne, 2010;
Chen, W. J., Ji, C., Yao, D., & Zhao, Z. Y. (2017). Thyroid evaluation of chil-
Palmer et al., 2007; Palmer et al., 2010). Since most experimental sub- dren and adolescents with Williams syndrome in Zhejiang Province.
jects were chosen from mice, but human facial growth is more compli- Journal of Pediatric Endocrinology & Metabolism, 30, 1271–1276.
cated. The correlation between genes and facial development in human Farkas, L. G., Katic, M. J., Forrest, C. R., Alt, K. W., Bagic, I., Baltadjiev, G.,
needs further study. … Yahia, E. (2005). International anthropometric study of facial mor-
phology in various ethnic groups/races. The Journal of Craniofacial Sur-
There are some limitations in this study. First, due to the small
gery, 16, 615–646.
sample size, different age groups could not be grouped by gender. Hammond, P., Hutton, T. J., Allanson, J. E., Buxton, B., Campbell, L. E.,
Second, the variables of periorbital fullness and mouth characteristics Clayton-Smith, J., … Tassabehji, M. (2005). Discriminating power of
were too less to reflect the characteristics of WS. More landmarks localized three-dimensional facial morphology. American Journal of
Human Genetics, 77, 999–1010.
and measurements should be added to obtain more data about these
Hirai, M., Muramatsu, Y., Mizuno, S., Kurahashi, N., Kurahashi, H., &
facial features. In the future, we plan to expand the sample size, Nakamura, M. (2016). Typical visual search performance and atypical
increase the measurement data, control the measurement error, and gaze behaviors in response to faces in Williams syndrome. Journal of
obtain more meaningful objective basis. Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 8, 38.
Järvinen, A., Korenberg, J. R., & Bellugi, U. (2013). The social phenotype
In conclusion, this study used 3D anthropometric measurement
of Williams syndrome. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23,
technology to confirm that the peculiar facial features caused by WS 414–422.
was evidently expressed in the infancy and early childhood in Chinese Ji, C., Yao, D., Chen, W., Li, M., & Zhao, Z. (2014). Adaptive behavior in
Han population. It provided objective basis to help clinicians to under- Chinese children with Williams syndrome. BMC Pediatrics, 14, 90.
Jones, K. L., & Smith, D. W. (1975). The Williams elfin facies syndrome. A
stand and get acquainted with such diseases more quickly, and bring
new perspective. The Journal of Pediatrics, 86, 718–723.
to these children and their parents a chance for timely diagnosis, Kruszka, P., Porras, A. R., de Souza, D. H., Moresco, A., Huckstadt, V.,
treatment and intervention. Gill, A. D., … Muenke, M. (2018). Williams-Beuren syndrome in diverse
populations. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A, 176,
1128–1136.
CONF LICT OF IN TE RE ST
Martens, M. (2013). Developmental and cognitive troubles in Williams
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence syndrome. Handbook of Clinical Neurology, 111, 291–293.
of any commercial or financial relationships. Mass, E., & Belostoky, L. (1993). Craniofacial morphology of children
with Williams syndrome. The Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 30,
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS 343–349.
McNamara, J. A., Jr. (1984). A method of cephalometric evaluation. Ameri-
Zheng-Yan Zhao and Chai Ji: Conception and designed the research;
can Journal of Orthodontics, 86, 449–469.
Chai Ji, Dao Yao and Ming-Yan Li: interviewed respondents and Osborne, L. R. (2010). Animal models of Williams syndrome. American
entered data; Sheng-Liang Lin, Wi-Jun Chen and Ming-Yan Lin: ana- Journal of Medical Genetics. Part C, Seminars in Medical Genetics, 154c,
lyzed the data and performed the statistical analyses; Chai Ji and 209–219.
Palmer, S. J., Santucci, N., Widagdo, J., Bontempo, S. J., Taylor, K. M.,
Zheng-Yan Zhao: wrote and revised the manuscript. All authors:
Tay, E. S., … Hardeman, E. C. (2010). Negative autoregulation of
approved the final content of the manuscript. None of the authors GTF2IRD1 in Williams-Beuren syndrome via a novel DNA binding
declared a conflict of interest. mechanism. The Journal of Biological Chemistry, 285, 4715–4724.
8 JI ET AL.

Palmer, S. J., Tay, E. S., Santucci, N., Cuc Bach, T. T., Hook, J., Ward, R. E., Jamison, P. L., & Farkas, L. G. (1998). Craniofacial variability
Lemckert, F. A., … Hardeman, E. C. (2007). Expression of Gtf2ird1, the index: A simple measure of normal and abnormal variation in the head
Williams syndrome-associated gene, during mouse development. Gene and face. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 80, 232–240.
Expression Patterns, 7, 396–404. Weinberg, S. M., Naidoo, S., Govier, D. P., Martin, R. A., Kane, A. A., &
Riby, D. M., Kirk, H., Hanley, M., & Riby, L. M. (2014). Stranger danger Marazita, M. L. (2006). Anthropometric precision and accuracy of digi-
awareness in Williams syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability tal three-dimensional photogrammetry: Comparing the Genex and
Research, 58, 572–582. 3dMD imaging systems with one another and with direct anthropome-
Roelfsema, N. M., Hop, W. C., van Adrichem, L. N., & Wladimiroff, J. W. try. The Journal of Craniofacial Surgery, 17, 477–483.
(2007). Craniofacial variability index in utero: A three-dimensional Zhuang, Z., Landsittel, D., Benson, S., Roberge, R., & Shaffer, R. (2010).
ultrasound study. Ultrasound in Obstetrics & Gynecology, 29, 258–264. Facial anthropometric differences among gender, ethnicity, and age
Sinha, P., Tamang, B. K., & Chakraborty, S. (2014). Craniofacial anthropom- groups. The Annals of Occupational Hygiene, 54, 391–402.
etry in newborns of Sikkimese origin. The Journal of Laryngology and
Otology, 128, 527–530.
Strømme, P., Bjørnstad, P. G., & Ramstad, K. (2002). Prevalence estimation
of Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology, 17, 269–271. How to cite this article: Ji C, Yao D, Li M-Y, Chen W-J,
Sugayama, S. M., Leone, C., Chauffaille Mde, L., Okay, T. S., & Kim, C. A. Lin S-L, Zhao Z-Y. A study on facial features of children with
(2007). Williams syndrome: Development of a new scoring system for Williams syndrome in China based on three-dimensional
~ Paulo, Brazil), 62, 159–166.
clinical diagnosis. Clinics (Sao
anthropometric measurement technology. Am J Med Genet
Tassabehji, M., Hammond, P., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Thompson, P.,
Thorgeirsson, S. S., Durkin, M. E., … Donnai, D. (2005). GTF2IRD1 in cra- Part A. 2020;1–8. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.61750
niofacial development of humans and mice. Science, 310, 1184–1187.

You might also like