A-Baker, A. E., Lane, A., Angley, M. T., & Young, R. L. (2008) PDF

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:867–875

DOI 10.1007/s10803-007-0459-0

ORIGINAL PAPER

The Relationship Between Sensory Processing Patterns


and Behavioural Responsiveness in Autistic Disorder:
A Pilot Study
Amy E. Z. Baker Æ Alison Lane Æ Manya T. Angley Æ
Robyn L. Young

Published online: 25 September 2007


Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Abstract Sensory processing (SP) difficulties have been Autism is characterised by impairments in three main areas
reported in as many as 95% of children with autism, of development: social relatedness, communication skills,
however, empirical research examining the existence of and the presence of stereotyped behaviour, interests
specific patterns of SP difficulties within this population and activities (American Psychiatric Association 2000;
is scarce. Furthermore, little attention has been given to Szatmari 2000). Stereotyped behaviours typically associ-
examining the relationship between SP and either the core ated with autism include hand-flapping, finger flicking,
symptoms or secondary manifestations of autism. In the rocking, spinning and self-injury such as head-banging
current study, SP patterns in children with autistic disorder and hand biting (Aarons and Gittens 1999; American
(AD) were investigated via a caregiver questionnaire and Psychiatric Association 2000; Case-Smith and Bryan 1999;
findings were correlated with the social, emotional and Volkmar 1998). Self-stimulation and adopting unconven-
behavioural responsiveness of participants. Results indi- tional postures, such as walking on tiptoes, have also been
cated the presence of specific SP patterns in this sample of observed in individuals with autism (Aarons and Gittens
children with AD and several significant relationships were 1999; American Psychiatric Association 2000; Ermer and
found between SP and social, emotional and behavioural Dunn 1998). One hypothesis for the presence of stereo-
function. typed behaviours in autism is that dysfunction in
processing sensory information characteristic of the disor-
Keywords Sensory processing  Autistic disorder  der results in the adoption of aberrant behaviours in an
Pervasive developmental disorder  Behaviour  attempt to make sense of and regulate stimulation from the
Short sensory profile environment (Baranek et al. 1997; Nelson 1984; Paluszny
1979).
Sensory processing (SP) refers to the way that sensory
information e.g. visual, auditory, vestibular or proprio-
A. E. Z. Baker  A. Lane ceptive stimuli is managed in the cerebral cortex and
School of Health Sciences, Sansom Institute, University of South brainstem for the purpose of enabling adaptive responses to
Australia, Adelaide, Australia
the environment and engagement in meaningful daily life
M. T. Angley activities (Johnson-Ecker and Parham 2000). SP theory
Sansom Institute, School of Pharmacy and Medical Sciences, suggests that optimal functioning in daily environments
University of South Australia, Adelaide, Australia requires efficient reception and integration of incoming
sensory stimuli. Adaptive behaviour, learning and coordi-
R. L. Young
School of Psychology, Flinders University of South Australia, nated movement are considered products of effective
Adelaide, Australia sensory integration (Bundy et al. 2002; Kranowitz 1998).
Disorders of SP in children are increasingly discussed in
A. E. Z. Baker (&)
the literature (Bundy et al. 2002). Dunn (1997) proposed a
School of Nursing and Midwifery, University of South Australia,
Adelaide 5000, Australia model for classifying patterns of dysfunction in SP
e-mail: amy.baker@postgrads.unisa.edu.au according to individuals’ behavioural response to stimuli

123
868 J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:867–875

and neurological thresholds, describing four patterns of SP Methods


dysfunction: Low Registration, Sensation Seeking, Sensory
Sensitivity and Sensation Avoiding. Participants
Unusual responses to sensory stimuli and SP difficulties
exhibited by individuals with autism have been widely Children diagnosed with AD were recruited from the Early
documented (Baranek et al. 2006; Tomchek and Dunn Intervention Research Program (EIRP) for children with
2007). Abnormalities have been reported to occur across all autism at Flinders University, South Australia, Australia.
sensory domains, including tactile, vestibular, auditory and Children are eligible to take part in the EIRP if they are
visual (Harrison and Hare 2004; Rogers 1998) and in the under the age of 5 years and meet DSM-IV criteria for AD.
absence of known peripheral dysfunction such as a visual Inclusion criteria ensured that only children who had
or hearing loss (Baranek 2002). A recent study comparing received a diagnosis of AD by a multi-disciplinary team
SP in children with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD; using standard diagnostic tools were included. Standard
n = 235) to age-matched, typically developing controls diagnostic tools used to diagnose participants included the
(n = 221) reported that 95% of the sample with ASD Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R; Rutter et al.
versus 16.8% of the controls demonstrated some degree of 2003), the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS; Scho-
SP difficulty (Tomchek and Dunn 2007). Sensory respon- pler et al. 1988), CHAT (Baron-Cohen et al. 2000), the
ses have also been shown to fluctuate such that both hyper- Autism Detection in Early Childhood tool (ADEC; Young
and hypo-responsiveness to sensory stimuli can occur in 2006), the Adaptive Behaviour Assessment System (2nd
the same individual (Baranek 2002; Baranek et al. 2006; ed.; ABAS-II; Harrison and Oakland 2003) and the Diag-
Greenspan and Wieder 1998; Kranowitz 1998; Volkmar nostic and Statistical Manual (4th ed.; DSM-IV; American
1998). Further, findings from six studies, which specifically Psychiatric Association 2000). Children were eligible to
compared the SP patterns of individuals with autism or take part in this study if they were either on the waiting list,
another pervasive developmental disorder (PDD) with currently enrolled, or had completed the EIRP. Children
controls, all revealed the presence of significantly different who met the criteria for AD but who presented with a
SP profiles for individuals with autism/PDD (Baranek et al. co-morbid diagnosis that impaired intellectual functioning,
2006; Ermer and Dunn 1998; Kern et al. 2006; Kientz i.e., additional disorders such as a chromosomal abnor-
and Dunn 1997; Miller et al. 2005; Watling et al. 2001). mality, were excluded from the study.
These findings suggest that SP dysfunction is a feature of
autism.
Despite the abundance of descriptive literature and Materials
anecdotal reports documenting unusual responses to sen-
sory stimuli in this population, the nature of SP difficulties Short Sensory Profile (SSP):
in autism and the relationship to the core functional and
behavioural disturbances of the disorder remains poorly The SSP is a 38-item parent questionnaire designed to
understood (Baranek 2002). In particular, there is incon- measure behaviours associated with abnormal responses to
sistency in reports regarding the nature of SP patterns sensory stimuli in children aged 3–10 years (McIntosh
within autistic disorder (AD) (Rogers and Ozonoff 2005; et al. 1999). The SSP is administered as a questionnaire in
Schaaf and Miller 2005). Further, the relationship between which the caregiver rates the child’s typical responses to
SP and behavioural difficulties in autism has only recently tactile, vestibular, auditory and visual stimuli on a five
received attention from researchers and warrants further point Likert scale ranging from ‘never’ responds in this
investigation (Miller et al. 2005; Rogers et al. 2003). manner to ‘always’ responds in this manner. Item scores
Patterns of SP dysfunction may be related to core fea- cluster meaningfully into patterns of SP such as Tactile
tures of the disorder and the development of unusual Sensitivity and Low Energy/Weak, enabling sensory sen-
behaviours. They may also give insight into prognosis, sitivity and performance to be interpreted in each of these
therefore having significant implications for early diagno- areas. Higher scores relate to typical performance expected
sis and intervention (Baranek 2002; Ermer and Dunn from children whereas lower scores demonstrate that a
1998). Improved understanding of the way in which chil- definite difference is present. The SSP was derived from a
dren with autism process sensory information is required longer caregiver questionnaire, the Sensory Profile (Dunn
(Tomchek and Dunn 2007). The purpose of this pre- 1999) but is accepted as the most appropriate version to use
liminary study was to: (1) describe SP patterns of children in research protocols (Dunn 1999; McIntosh et al. 1999).
who have AD, and (2) explore the relationship between SP Good reliability of the SSP has been demonstrated in a
patterns and emotional, social and behavioural respon- previous study and acceptable internal and discriminative
siveness in this group. validity has also been established (Dunn 1999). A separate

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:867–875 869

study also demonstrated adequate construct validity (Miller University of South Australia and Flinders University
et al. 2005). The SSP takes approximately 10 min to Social and Behavioural Research Ethics Committee.
complete.

Recruitment
Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales (VABS), Interview
Edition: Information packages were mailed to individuals who were
on the wait list, enrolled in or who had completed the EIRP
The VABS is a semi-structured interview administered to for children with autism at Flinders University and who
caregivers to assess the current adaptive behaviour of the met the inclusion criteria. Informed consent was obtained
child, defined as the development and application of and data collection occurred at the EIRP House, Flinders
abilities required for the attainment of personal indepen- University in a single face-to-face session, except for one
dence and social competence (Sparrow et al. 1984). The participant who completed the caregiver questionnaire at
VABS assesses adaptive behaviour by obtaining standard home and mailed it to the researcher.
scores in the domains of communication, daily living,
socialisation and motor skills. An optional Maladaptive
Behaviour domain measures undesirable behaviours that Data Collection and Analysis
may hinder the adaptive skills of the subject. Items in
each domain are scored from 0 to 2, with decreasing The SSP was administered to parents according to the
scores indicating skills/behaviours that are sometimes or guidelines outlined in the SSP manual (Dunn 1999).
never performed. The raw scores are then converted to DBC-P and VABS data were obtained by gathering each
standard scores. The sum of standard scores in each participant’s most recent data set for these assessments
domain yields an adaptive behaviour composite score from their EIRP records. As SP and behavioural data were
reflecting the overall ability of the subject to live inde- gathered at separate times, counterbalancing was not con-
pendently. All domains, with the exception of motor sidered to be relevant. Although the most recent data sets
skills, were analysed within the current study. The VABS for most participants were dated within 12 months of the
is routinely administered to caregivers of children enrol- current study, data for six participants were from assess-
led in the EIRP. ments conducted between 1 and 2 years prior and two
participants’ results were of assessments administered
more than two years prior. Given the relatively small
Developmental Behaviour Checklist—Parent (DBC-P): sample size within this pilot study, analysis of results
excluding the eight participants’ older data sets was not
The DBC-P is a 96-item parent checklist developed to considered appropriate. SSP results and findings from
assess a range of behavioural and emotional problems in behavioural assessments were analysed initially using
children and young people (4–18 years) with an intellec- descriptive statistics. Pearson’s Correlation Coefficient
tual disability (Einfeld and Tonge 2002). The total score method was then used to correlate data from the SSP, the
provides a threshold for clinically significant behavioural DBC-P and the VABS.
and emotional problems and sub-scales assess individuals
in the areas of disruptive and anti-social behaviour, self-
absorbed behaviour, communication disturbances, anxiety, Results
social relatedness and autism specific behaviours (DBC-
ASA). The parent edition of the DBC (DBC-P) reflects Participants
behaviours exhibited by the child over a 6-month period.
The DBC-P is routinely administered to caregivers of Twenty-two children with AD were recruited to the study
children enrolled in the EIRP. and all had completed the EIRP at the time of data col-
lection. Participants were aged between 33 and 101 months
(2 year 9 months–8 year 5 months), with a mean age of
Procedures 64.86 months (5 year 5 months; 20.70 SD). Eighteen par-
ticipants were male, resulting in an approximate ratio of
Ethics 5:1 male to female which is consistent with gender ratios
currently reported in the literature for autism (American
Ethics approval was gained from the Division of Health Psychiatric Association 2000; Dempsey and Foreman
Sciences Human Research Ethics Committee at the 2001).

123
870 J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:867–875

Performance Across SSP Sections performance in both Movement Sensitivity and Visual/
100
DD PD TP
Auditory Sensitivity sections. Remaining participants in
80 both the Movement Sensitivity and Visual/Auditory Sen-
16
% of Participal

15 15 sitivity sections were equally divided between the Definite


12
60
11 11 Difference and Probable Difference performance
10

40
8 9 8 categories.
5 6 6 5 5
Other notable SP performance patterns included (a) an
4 3 3
20 equal number of participants showed a definite difference
1 1
0 and typical performance in Low Energy/Weak, with no
0
1. TS 2. TSS 3. MS 4. USS 5. AF 6. LEW 7. VAS participants demonstrating probable difference in this sec-
SSP Section tion, and (b) performance by the sample on Tactile
Sensitivity and Taste/Smell Sensitivity was relatively
Fig. 1 Sensory processing performance by SSP section. Key: equally distributed between definite, probable and typical
TS = Tactile Sensitivity; AF = Auditory Filtering; TSS = Taste/
Smell Sensitivity; LEW = Low Energy/Weak; MS = Movement performance.
Sensitivity; VAS = Visual/Auditory Sensitivity; USS = Under- Table 1 displays the results of the correlational analyses.
responsive/Seeks Sensation; DD = definite difference; PD = probable As seen in Table 1 moderate, significant associations
difference; TP = typical performance were observed between Movement Sensitivity and Tactile
Sensitivity, Tactile Sensitivity and Visual/Auditory Sensi-
Patterns of Sensory Processing Performance tivity and Taste/Smell Sensitivity and Low Energy/Weak.
Moderate correlations, which were significant at the 0.01
For the 22 children recruited to the study, SSP scores were level included Movement Sensitivity and Visual/Auditory
classified as an overall score, into three performance cat- Sensitivity, Taste/Smell Sensitivity and Under-responsive/
egories and across seven areas of SP. The mean overall Seeks Sensation and Under-responsive/Seeks Sensation
score for the SSP was 133.36 (SD = 20.70) and scores and Low Energy/Weak. Two clusters of association
ranged between 83 and 160. Eighteen participants (82%) between areas of SP performance were revealed in this
demonstrated either a probable or definite difference in SP analysis—Cluster One: Visual/Auditory, Tactile and
(probable, n = 6; definite, n = 12). Figure 1 shows the Movement Sensitivity and Cluster Two: Low/Energy
distribution of participant scores across SSP sections. Most Weak, Taste/Smell Sensitivity and Under-responsive/Seeks
participants demonstrated a definite difference in the Sensation.
Under-responsive/Seeks Sensation and Auditory Filtering
sections (n = 15, 68%). Twelve (80%) of these participants
exhibited a definite difference in both sections. Typical Behavioural Responsiveness
performance was demonstrated by 16 participants (73%) in
Movement Sensitivity and by 12 (55%) in Visual/Auditory From Table 2 it is evident that most participants showed
Sensitivity. Ten (45%) participants demonstrated typical low levels of performance for each of the VABS domains

Table 1 Correlational analysis of SP performance patterns


SSP section TS MS VAS LEW TSS USS AF

Tactile r=1 *r = .440, *r = .447, r = .397, r = .192, r = .049, r = .241,


sensitivity (TS) p = 0.041 p = 0.037 p = 0.067 p = 0.392 p = 0.830 p = 0.281
Movement *r = .440, r=1 *r = .615, r = .303, r = .021, r = –.023, r = .071,
sensitivity (MS) p = 0.041 p = 0.002 p = 0.171 p = 0.927 p = 0.920 p = 0.754
Visual/auditory *r = .447, *r = .615, r=1 r = .281, r = .135, r = .169, r = .168,
sensitivity (VAS) p = 0.037 p = 0.002 p = 0.205 p = 0.550 p = 0.451 p = 0.454
Low energy/ r = .397, r = .303, r = .281, r=1 **r = .510, **r = .652, r = .294,
weak (LEW) p = 0.067 p = 0.171 p = 0.205 p = 0.015 p = 0.001 p = 0.184
Taste/smell r = .192, r = .021, r = .135, **r = .510, r=1 **r = .639, r = .238,
sensitivity (TSS) p = 0.392 p = 0.927 p = 0.550 p = 0.015 p = 0.001 p = 0.286
Under-responsive/seeks r = .049, r = –.023, r = .169, **r = .652, **r = .639, r=1 r = .260,
sensation (USS) p = 0.830 p = 0.920 p = 0.451 p = 0.001 p = 0.001 p = 0.243
Auditory filtering (AF) r = .241, r = .071, r = .168, r = .294, r = .238, r = .260, r=1
p = 0.281 p = 0.754 p = 0.454 p = 0.184 p = 0.286 p = 0.243
* Cluster One; ** Cluster Two

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:867–875 871

Table 2 Participants’ scores on VABS and DBC-P


Low n (%) Mod. Low n (%) Adequate n (%) Mod. High n (%) High n (%)

Communication (n = 22) 14 (63.64%) 4 (18.18%) 3 (13.64%) 0 (0%) 1 (4.55%)


Daily living skills (n = 22) 18 (81.82%) 3 (13.64%) 1 (4.55%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
Socialisation (n = 22) 12 (54.55%) 5 (22.73%) 5 (22.73%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
Total VABS (n = 22) 13 (59.09%) 7 (31.82%) 1 (4.55%) 1 (4.55%) 0 (0%)
VABS maladaptive behaviour
Significant n (%) Intermediate n (%) Non-significant n (%)

Maladaptive behaviour (n = 22) 12 (55%) 8 (36%) 2 (9%)


DBC-P

Significant level Non-significant level


of behavioural of behavioural/ emotional
and emotional problems; n (%)
problems; n (%)

Overall DBC score (n = 22) 4 (18%) 18 (82%)

included in this study as well as significant scores for in children with autism to range between 69% and 95%
maladaptive behaviour however no significant behavioural (Baranek et al. 2006; Schaff and Benevides 2007; Tomchek
difficulties as measured by the DBC-P were evident. and Dunn 2007). Despite the lack of a control group,
normative data reported in the SSP suggests these SP dif-
ficulties are of greater magnitude than might be expected in
Relationship Between SP Performance Patterns and the typically developing population. In order to assess the
Behavioural Responsiveness uniqueness of these difficulties to autism, these data need
to be compared with data collected from other disability
Pearson’s correlation analyses of SSP and VABS and groups. Results from the current study were also based on
DBC-P scores showed several significant relationships, as the chronological age of children only and previous
displayed in Table 3. research supports a stronger relationship between SP and
Total SSP scores and total DBC-P scores were found to developmental age than SP and chronological age (Baranek
be strongly negatively associated as were total SSP and et al. 2006). SSP results from the current study showed that
VABS Maladaptive Behaviour domain scores. A moderate, the majority of participants had marked impairment in
positive correlation was calculated between total SSP and several areas of SP while demonstrating typical respon-
VABS Daily Living Skills scores. Moderate, negative siveness in others, indicating the presence of specific
correlations were found between the VABS Maladaptive patterns of SP functioning.
Behaviour domain and Movement Sensitivity, Under-
responsive/Seeks Sensation, Auditory Filtering and Low
Energy/ Weak. Total DBC-P scores were also moderately, Areas of Impairment in SP
negatively correlated with several SSP sections, including
Underresponsive/Seeks Sensation, Auditory Filtering, Low Most participants showed a definite difference in the
Energy/Weak and Visual/Auditory Sensitivity. These Under-responsive/Seeks Sensation and Auditory Filtering
results indicate that, in general, higher VABS Maladaptive sections with only one participant in each of these sections
Behaviour domain and DBC-P total scores were associated demonstrating typical performance. Kern (2002) suggested
with lower SSP scores. that under-responding to sensory stimuli in individuals with
autism may be explained by the depression of sensory
afferents in the cerebellum, resulting in inconsistent sen-
Discussion sory modulation. In a study by Watling et al. (2001),
Sensation Seeking was identified as a section in which
In this study, overall SSP results showed that most par- children with a PDD demonstrated significantly lower
ticipants in this sample (82%) exhibited some degree of SP scores from the typically developing group, congruent with
difficulty. These results are consistent with the findings of the findings of the current study. However, conflicting
recent studies that report the prevalence of SP dysfunction results were observed by Ermer and Dunn (1998), who

123
872

123
Table 3 Analyses between sensory and behavioural data
n = 22 r VABS VABS VABS VABS VABS DBC DBC DBC DBC DBC DBC ASA DBC total
(p-level) commu- daily socialisa- maladaptive total disruptive/ self- comm. anxiety social
nication living tion behaviour antisocial absorbed disturbance relating
skills

Tactile –.216 (0.335) .268 (0.227) –.211 (0.346) –.342 (0.119) –.055 (0.808) –.432 (0.045) –.340 (0.122) –.364 (0.096) –.517* (0.014) –.322 (0.143) –.221 (0.324) –.366 (0.094)
sensitivity
Taste/smell –.053 (0.814) .228 (0.307) –.150 (0.560) –.416 (0.054) –.054 (0.811) –.331 (0.132) –.324 (0.141) –.105 (0.641) –.347 (0.114) –.246 (0.270) –.408 (0.059) –.370 (0.090)
sensitivity
Movement .166 (0.460) .312 (0.157) .247 (0.268) –.487* (0.022) .342 (0.120) –.208 (0.353) –.425* (0.048) –.426* (0.048) –.590** (0.004) –.414 (0.055) –.306 (0.166) –.420 (0.051)
sensitivity
Under– .069 (0.759) .301 (0.174) –.018 (0.938) –.482* (0.023) .115 (0.609) –.408 (0.059) –.523* (0.012) –.280 (0.206) –.302 (0.172) –.370 (0.090) –.533* (0.011) –.491* (0.020)
responsive
/seeks
sensation
Auditory –.120 (0.595) .341 (0.120) –.071 (0.752) –.438* (0.042) .034 (0.880) –.252 (0.258) –.313 (0.156) –.252 (0.258) –.096 (0.670) –.295 (0.182) –.549** (0.008) –.503* 0.017
filtering
Low Energy/ –.306 (0.166) .261 (0.241) –.104 (0.644) –.596** (0.003) –.038 (0.866) –.482 (0.023) –.516* (0.014) –.470* (0.027) –.515* (0.014) –.365 (0.095) –.447* (0.037) –.558** (0.007)
weak
Visual/ .145 (0.519) .396 (0.068) –.007 (0.977) –.407 (0.060) .225 (0.314) –.553** (0.008) –.554** (0.008) –.620** (0.002) –.671** (0.001) –.319 (0.148) –.319 (0.148) –.508* (0.016)
auditory
sensitivity
Total SSP –.120 (0.594) .433* (0.044) –.101 (0.656) –.702** (0.000) .076 (0.735) –.608** (0.003) –.665** (0.001) –.541** (0.009) –.649** (0.001) –.492* (0.020) –.616** (0.002) –.705** (0.000)

* Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed); ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed)
J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:867–875
J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:867–875 873

reported a low incidence of atypical behaviours in the was observed within the Low Energy/Weak section in
Sensory Seeking category for the AD/PDD group. Behav- which exactly half of the participants demonstrated a
iours associated with sensory seeking, such as definite difference and half showed typical performance,
inappropriate smelling, licking and rubbing, may account illustrating distinct patterns of both hypo- and hyper-
for stereotyped and repetitive behaviours observed in this responsiveness within the same section. Taste/Smell
population. Sensitivity was another area in the current study in which
Auditory Filtering has also been shown to be an area of participants demonstrated varying performance, with
difficulty for children with AD and other PDDs in previous almost even numbers of participants receiving scores for
studies. Findings from studies by Ermer and Dunn (1998) definite difference and typical performance. Large variance
and Watling et al. (2001) showed a high frequency of in the Taste/Smell Sensitivity and Low Energy/Weak sec-
behaviours in the Inattention/Distractibility category of the tions was also noted in a study by Watling et al. (2001) but
Sensory Profile, an equivalent category to the Auditory contrasting results were observed in a study by Ermer and
Filtering section within the SSP, congruent with current Dunn (1998) where children with AD/PDD exhibited a
findings. When a child has difficulties with auditory fil- high frequency of behaviours within the SSP Taste/Smell
tering, he or she may be either hyper-responsive i.e., Sensitivity section.
sensitive to sounds, or hypo-responsive, i.e., oblivious to
sounds (Dunn 1999). Children with auditory filtering dif-
ficulties may appear to be distracted and inattentive, Clusters of SP Responsiveness
particularly in busy environments, which contain numerous
stimuli such as the school environment. Several clusters of SP responsiveness were noted in the
current study, one of which wholly replicated, and the other
partially replicated, findings from a previous study (Miller
Areas of Typical Performance in SP et al. 2005). The first cluster was observed between Tactile
Sensitivity, Movement Sensitivity and Visual/Auditory
Most participants showed typical performance in the Sensitivity, which were also detected by Miller et al. who
Movement Sensitivity and Visual/Auditory Sensitivity titled this cluster ‘Sensory-Over-Responsivity’. Another
sections of the SSP. It is noteworthy that most participants cluster of SP symptoms observed by Miller et al. titled
who demonstrated typical performance in these sections ‘Sensory Under-Responsivity’ included Taste/Smell Sen-
showed typical performance in both of these sections, sitivity, Low Energy/Weak and Visual/Auditory Sensitivity
suggesting that these areas may be associated in some way. to a lesser degree. Results from the current study partially
A Pearson’s correlation coefficient of .615 (p = .002) support this cluster, with a moderate correlation observed
confirmed that a significant, moderate relationship existed between Low Energy/Weak and Taste/Smell Sensitivity
between these sections. Typical responsiveness in move- but with the Under-responsive/Seeks Sensation section
ment sensitivity was also observed for a group of children replacing Visual/Auditory Sensitivity.
with AD/PDD in another study (Watling et al. 2001),
whilst a further study yielded contrary results (Ermer and
Dunn 1998). These conflicting results highlight the need SP and Behavioural Responsiveness
for further investigation of patterns of typical responsive-
ness as this may assist clinicians to maximise areas of Various significant relationships were found between SP
relative strength in SP and consequently support learning patterns and behavioural responsiveness. Strong, negative
and adaptation for individuals with AD. Individuals with correlations between SSP scores and total DBC-P and
AD often show superior skills and repetitive behaviours in VABS Maladaptive Behaviour domain scores indicated
visual tasks such as building blocks, pattern recognition that in general, poor SP ability was associated with higher
and puzzles, which may be associated with the high level levels of behavioural and/or emotional problems for this
of performance observed in the Visual/Auditory Sensitivity sample. A moderate correlation found between total SSP
section in the current study. and VABS Daily Living Skills scores, indicated that within
this sample, poorer SP ability was associated with
decreased functioning in the area of daily living skills.
Areas of Varied Response Within SP Other authors have hypothesised that SP difficulties limit
participation in activities of daily living, work and leisure
In the current study, several SSP sections elicited a mixed activities (Schaaf and Miller 2005). Both the VABS Mal-
response from participants, with a greater spread of scores adaptive Behaviour domain and total DBC scores showed
occurring in these sections. The most pronounced example moderate correlations with the following SSP sections:

123
874 J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:867–875

Under-responsive/Seeks Sensation, Auditory Filtering and her involvement in data collection of behavioural data. This study was
Low Energy/Weak. The DBC Autism subset (DBC-ASA), supported in part by the Channel 7 Children’s Research Foundation,
South Australia.
which measures autistic behaviour, was also found to be
moderately correlated with these three SSP sections. In a
study by Miller et al. (2005), sensory seeking behaviours References
were found to be strongly and consistently related to a
higher incidence of repetitive behaviours. Repetitive Aarons, M., & Gittens, T. (1999). The handbook of autism: A guide
behaviours are likely to constitute much of the maladaptive for parents and professionals. London: Routledge.
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical
behaviour seen in the current sample. If an individual manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR, (4th ed.). Washington
demonstrates under-responsive or sensory seeking behav- DC: American Psychiatric Association.
iours, more stimulation is required in order to regulate and Baranek, G. T. (2002). Efficacy of sensory and motor interventions
process sensory information received from the environ- for children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 32(5), 397–422.
ment, which in turn may manifest in the form of repetitive Baranek, G. T., David, F. J., Poe, M. D., Stone, W. L., & Watson, L.
behaviours. R. (2006). Sensory experiences questionnaire: Discriminating
sensory features in young children with autism, developmental
delays, and typical development. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 47(6), 591–601.
Conclusion Baranek, G. T., Foster, L. G., & Berkson, G. (1997). Tactile
defensiveness and stereotyped behaviors. The American Journal
In summary, several areas of marked impairment, typical of Occupational Therapy, 51(2), 91–95.
responsiveness and varied responsiveness were observed in Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., Charman, T.,
Swettenham, J., Drew, A., & Doehring, P. (2000). Early
this study, suggesting the presence of distinct SP patterns identification of autism by the Checklist for Autism in Toddlers
for children with AD. In general, participants demonstrated (CHAT). Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 93(10), 521–
SP difficulties in auditory filtering and a high incidence of 525.
sensation seeking behaviours, areas, which mainly focused Bundy, A. C., Lane, S. J., Murray, E. A., & Fisher, A. G. (2002).
Sensory integration: theory and practice. Philadelphia: F.A.
on under-responsiveness in relation to movement and Davis Company.
vestibular input. Other distinctive SP patterns in this Case-Smith, J., & Bryan, T. (1999). The effects of occupational
sample included typical responsiveness in the areas of therapy with sensory integration emphasis on preschool-age
Visual/Auditory and Movement Sensitivity and varied children with autism. The American Journal of Occupational
Therapy, 53(5), 489–497.
responses for Low Energy/Weak and Taste/Smell Sensi- Dempsey, I., & Foreman, P. (2001). A review of educational
tivity SSP sections. Of the two distinct clusters observed approaches for individuals with autism. International Journal
which either wholly or partly replicated groupings found of Disability, Development and Education, 48(1), 103–116.
by Miller et al. (2005), most participants in the current Dunn, W. (1997). The impact of sensory processing abilities on the
daily lives of young children and their families: A conceptual
study demonstrated typical performance for all SSP sec- model. Infants and Young Children, 9(4), 23–35.
tions within the Sensory-Over-Responsivity cluster but Dunn, W. (1999). Sensory profile: User’s manual. US: The Psycho-
showed a definite difference for sections within the Sen- logical Corporation.
sory-Under-Responsivity cluster. Einfeld, S. L., & Tonge, B. J. (2002). Manual for the developmental
behaviour checklist (2nd ed.). University of New South Wales
Noteworthy findings observed between SP and behav- and Monash University.
ioural data include significant moderate correlations Ermer, J., & Dunn, W. (1998). The sensory profile: A discriminant
between both maladaptive behaviour and emotional/ analysis of children with and without disabilities. The American
behavioural problems, and the following SSP sections: Journal of Occupational Therapy, 52(4), 283–290.
Greenspan, S. I., & Wieder, W. (1998). The child with special needs:
Under-responsive/Seeks Sensation, Auditory Filtering and Encouraging intellectual and emotional growth. Reading, MA:
Low Energy/Weak. Replication of this study with a larger Addison-Wesley Publishing Company.
sample size is warranted to further clarify the observed Harrison, J., & Hare, D.J. (2004). Brief report: Assessment of sensory
relationships and to develop hypotheses regarding the abnormalities in people with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(6), 727–730.
impact of SP on behavioural responsiveness in autism. Harrison, P. L., & Oakland, T. (2003). Adaptive behavior assessment
Testing of these hypotheses will then assist in the identi- system – Second Edition. San Antonio: The Psychological
fication of specific SP and behavioural typologies within Corporation.
autism that will enable more targeted approaches to inter- Johnson-Ecker, C. L., & Parham, L. D. (2000). The evaluation of
sensory processing: A validity study using contrasting groups.
vention and therefore improved allocation of resources for The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 54(5),
this population. 494–503.
Kern, J. K. (2002). The possible role of the cerebellum in autism/
Acknowledgements We wish to express our gratitude to the par- PDD: Disruption of a multisensory feedback loop. Medical
ticipating children and their parents, and to Ms Carrie Partington, for Hypotheses, 59(3), 255–260.

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:867–875 875

Kern, J. K., Trivedi, M. H., Garver, C. R., Grannemann, B. D., Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). ADI-R: Autism
Andrews, A. A., Savla, J. S., Johnson, D. G., Mehta, J. A., & diagnostic interview—revised: Manual. Los Angeles: Western
Schroeder, J. L. (2006). The pattern of sensory processing Psychological Services.
abnormalities in autism. Autism, 15(5), 480–494. Schaff, R. C., & Benevides, T. (2007). Mechanisms of sensory
Kientz, M. A., & Dunn, W. (1997). A comparison of the performance dysfunction and impact on OT interventions. Paper presented at
of children with and without autism on the Sensory Profile. The the meeting of the American Occupational Therapy Association,
American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 51(7), 530–537. St. Louis, MO.
Kranowitz, C. S. (1998). The out-of-sync child. New York: Skylight Schaaf, R. C., & Miller, L. J. (2005). Occupational therapy using a
Press. sensory integrative approach for children with developmental
McIntosh, D. N., Miller, L. J., Shyu, V., & Hagerman, R. J. (1999). disabilities. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities,
Sensory-modulation disruption, electrodermal responses and 11(2), 143–148.
functional behaviors. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurol- Schopler, E. C., Reichler, R., & Renner, B. (1988). The Childhood
ogy, 41(9), 608–615. Autism Rating Scale (CARS). Los Angeles: Western Psycholog-
Miller, L. J., Schoen, S., Coll, J., Brett-Green, B., & Reale, M. (2005). ical Services.
Final report: Cure autism now executive summary. Los Angeles: Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). Vineland
Cure Autism Now. Adaptive Behavior Scales: Interview edition survey form man-
Nelson, D. L. (1984). Children with autism and other pervasive ual. Minnesota: American Guidance Service.
disorders of development and behavior: Therapy through Szatmari, P. (2000). The classification of autism, asperger’s syn-
activities. Thorofare: SLACK Inc. drome, and pervasive developmental disorder. Canadian Journal
Paluszny, M. (1979). Autism: A practical guide for parents and of Psychiatry, 45(8), 731–739.
professionals. New York: Syracuse University Press. Tomchek, S. D., & Dunn, W. (2007). Sensory processing in children
Rogers, S. J. (1998). Neuropsychology of autism in young children with and without autism: A comparative study using the Short
and its implications for early intervention. Mental Retardation Sensory Profile. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy,
and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 4(2), 61(2), 190–200.
104–112. Volkmar, F. R. (1998). Autism and pervasive developmental disor-
Rogers, S. J., & Ozonoff, S. (2005). Annotation: What do we know ders. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
about dysfunction in autism? A critical review of the empirical Watling, R. L., Deitz, J., & White, O. (2001). Comparison of Sensory
evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(12), Profile scores of young children with and without autism
1255–1268. spectrum disorders. The American Journal of Occupational
Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S., & Wehner, E. (2003). Parent reports of Therapy, 55(4), 416–423.
sensory symptoms in toddlers with autism and those with other Young, R. L. (2006). Autism detection in early childhood (ADEC).
developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Australian Council of Educational Research.
Disorders, 33(6), 631–642.

123

You might also like