Anxious Symptoms

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Anxious symptoms and cognitive function in non-demented


older adults: an inverse relationship
Ashley N. Stillman, Kelly C. Rowe, Stephan Arndt and David J. Moser
Department of Psychiatry, University of Iowa Carver College of Medicine, Iowa City, IA, USA
Correspondence to: D. J. Moser, E-mail: David-Moser@uiowa.edu

Background: The goals of this study were to determine the relationship between anxious symptoms and
cognitive functioning in a non-demented, community-dwelling older adults sample (n = 48), and to de-
termine the effect of depressive symptoms upon this relationship.
Methods: Anxious and depressive symptoms were assessed using the Symptom Checklist 90—Revised.
Cognitive functioning was assessed with the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsycholog-
ical Status.
Results: Results indicated that although both cognitive functioning and anxious symptoms were within
normal limits in this sample, anxious symptoms showed a significant, inverse relationship with global
cognitive function [r(47) = 0.400, p = 0.005]. In addition, specific relationships were noted between
severity of anxious symptoms and visuospatial/constructional ability as well as immediate and delayed
memory. With regard to the secondary objective, both anxiety and depressive symptoms together
accounted for the highest level of variance [R2 = 0.175, F(2, 45) = 4.786, p = 0.013] compared with
anxiety [R2(47) = 0.160, p = 0.005] and depression [R2(47) = 0.106, p = 0.024] alone. Nevertheless,
neither anxious nor depressive symptoms emerged as a unique correlate with cognitive ability
[r(47) = 0.278, p = 0.058; r(48) = 0.136, p = 0.363, respectively].
Conclusion: This study demonstrates that subthreshold anxiety symptoms and cognitive functioning are
significantly related even among generally healthy older adults whose cognitive ability and severity of
anxious symptoms are within broad normal limits. These findings have implications both for clinical
care of older patients, as well as for cognitive research studies utilizing this population. Copyright #
2011 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Key words: anxiety; cognitive function; older adults


History: Received 24 March 2011; Accepted 15 July 2011; Published online 15 September 2011 in Wiley Online Library
(wileyonlinelibrary.com).
DOI: 10.1002/gps.2785

Introduction 2008; Johnsen and Asbjornsen, 2008; Johnsen and


Asbjornsen, 2009; Haikal and Hong, 2010).
Until recently, research on the effects of psychopathology The current literature establishes that cognitive dys-
on cognition in older adults has focused primarily on de- function has been associated with diagnosed anxiety
pressive symptoms. Although depression has proven to disorders including obsessive–compulsive disorder
have negative effects on cognitive function (Wilson (OCD; Bannon et al., 2008), post-traumatic stress dis-
et al., 2004; Chodosh et al., 2007), the analogous rela- order (PTSD; Johnsen and Asbjornsen, 2008; Johnsen
tionship of anxiety with cognitive ability has been less and Asbjornsen, 2009), generalized anxiety disorder
thoroughly researched. To date, studies show accumu- (GAD; Mantella et al., 2007), and phobic anxiety
lating evidence of cognitive dysfunction in persons (Schultz et al., 2005) with GAD being the most preva-
with anxiety disorders (Beekman et al., 1998; Schultz lent among older adults (Beekman et al., 1998). The
et al., 2005; Mantella et al., 2007; Bannon et al., summation of this literature suggests that cognitive

Copyright # 2011 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2012; 27: 792–798.
10991166, 2012, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/gps.2785 by UNED, Wiley Online Library on [25/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Anxiety and cognitive function in the older adults 793

dysfunction, although evident, manifests differently in thorough neuropsychological battery. Given that de-
varying types of anxiety. For example, in OCD, cogni- pression is highly associated with anxiety (Lowe
tive facilitation and inhibition are most associated et al., 2008) and subclinical depressive symptoms
with anxiety (Bannon et al., 2008), whereas PTSD is alone have been associated with cognitive impairment
mostly related to verbal and delayed memory (Johnsen (Wilson et al., 2004; Chodosh et al., 2007), it is impor-
and Asbjornsen, 2008; Johnsen and Asbjornsen, 2009). tant to assess the relationship of both variables with
Phobic anxiety is less definitive suggesting that phobic cognitive function.
anxiety is associated with overall cognitive ability It has been suggested that the current criteria for
(Schultz et al., 2005), and in GAD, anxious symptoms anxiety disorders do not adequately classify anxiety
appear to be primarily related to short term and in the older population (Pachana et al., 2007); Flint
delayed memory function (Beekman et al., 1998). (1994) proposed that anxiety manifests itself differ-
One significant limitation of the existing literature ently in older persons compared with younger adults
is that it tends to focus on younger adults. Of the liter- and consequently may not be adequately measured.
ature reviewed, one study, closest in relationship to the Flint advised that if diagnosed anxiety must interfere
current study, did assess cognition and anxiety in an with daily living, then the older adults may be under-
older adults sample. Wetherell et al. (2002) assessed diagnosed as their daily living activities tend to be-
self-reported state (current) versus trait (chronic) anx- come more limited. Parallel to the theory given by
iety symptoms and their relationships to cognitive Flint, research has suggested that “subthreshold” anx-
function in a healthy older adults sample. The neuro- iety symptoms are more prevalent in an aging popula-
psychological battery used in that study consisted of tion than diagnosed anxiety disorders (Beekman et al.,
tests of verbal reasoning, knowledge, visuospatial 1998; Forsell and Winbald, 1998; Smalbrugge et al.,
skills, perceptual speed, and attention. The authors 2005). Given that anxiety symptoms present on a
demonstrated a linear inverse relationship between spectrum ranging from mild to severe, it is vital to
state anxiety and cognition in this sample. This linear assess less severe symptoms to better determine at
relationship is a unique finding compared with the what point anxiety has a significant relationship with
inverted U relationship seen between state anxiety cognitive function.
and cognition in previous studies (Bierman et al.,
2005; Bierman et al., 2008). Briefly, the inverted U ide-
ology suggests that an optimal level of anxiety is neces- Objective
sary to obtain peak cognitive function. Nevertheless,
no definitive conclusions could be made regarding Given that subthreshold anxiety symptoms are
trait anxiety in the study conducted by Wetherell more common in a normal older population than
et al. This non-significant finding of trait anxiety and anxiety diagnoses (Beekman et al., 1998; Forsell and
cognitive decline may be because of a relatively low in- Winbald, 1998; Smalbrugge et al., 2005), the present
cidence of late-life anxiety disorders. Additionally, the study was designed primarily to characterize the associ-
sample given by Wetherell et al. was composed of only ation of anxious symptoms and cognitive functioning in
twin adults, who may systematically differ from single- a sample of relatively healthy, community-dwelling
birth individuals, limiting the generalizability of these older adults. Because previous studies have found an as-
findings. sociation between subclinical levels of anxiety in young
An additional limitation to the existing literature is adults (Elliman et al., 1997), we hypothesized that
that it concentrates on formerly diagnosed anxiety dis- there exists a significant inverse relationship between se-
orders. Elliman et al., 1997, however, examined young verity of anxious symptoms and level of cognitive func-
adults ages 18–31 without formally diagnosed anxiety tioning in a normal aging, non-demented sample of
disorders. In a cross-sectional study, they found that older adults.
even subclinical levels of anxious symptoms were asso- A secondary objective of the current study was to
ciated with lower levels of cognitive performance, spe- determine if depressive symptoms played an active
cifically related to sustained attention. Their findings role in the relationship of anxiety with cognition.
suggest that even subclinical levels of anxiety may neg- Lowe et al., 2008 found an independent effect of diag-
atively impact cognitive ability. Although this study nosed anxiety in participants with comorbid depres-
did find a relationship between subclinical levels of sion on daily functioning. Even though daily
anxious symptoms and cognitive ability, it cannot be functioning is not equivalent to cognitive function,
generalized to the aging population and it lacks they are closely related. Therefore, we hypothesized
depression as a covariate. Additionally, it lacks a that anxiety will share a significant relationship with

Copyright # 2011 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2012; 27: 792–798.
10991166, 2012, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/gps.2785 by UNED, Wiley Online Library on [25/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
794 A. N. Stillman et al.

global neuropsychological function, independent of Depression subscale of the SCL-90-R has also been
depressive symptoms. shown as a valid measurement of depressive symp-
toms (Clark and Friedman, 1983; Steer et al., 1994).
Methods Each participant was instructed to indicate how much
they have been distressed by each symptom or prob-
Participants lem in the past 7 days. Scores range from 0 (not at
all) to 4 (extremely) for each item. The SCL-90-R pro-
The study sample was composed of 48 cognitively in- vides a T-score, corrected for gender, for nine psychi-
tact participants (35 women, 13 men; mean age = 69 atric domains, including anxiety and depression.
years, SD = 8; mean education = 15 years, SD = 3). All Cognitive functioning was assessed with the Repeat-
participants were healthy comparison participants in able Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological
a larger, ongoing study on vascular disease and cogni- Status (RBANS; Randolph, 1998). This 30-min assess-
tion conducted at the University of Iowa Hospital and ment includes 12 subtests that are represented by five
Clinics. To meet inclusion criteria as healthy compar- age-corrected index scores testing a range of cognitive
isons, participants were required to be between the abilities: immediate memory, visuospatial/construc-
ages of 55 and 90. Exclusion criteria included diagnosis tional, language, attention, and delayed memory.
of atherosclerotic vascular disease, history of myocardial These five age corrected scores are used to calculate a
infarction, percutaneous transluminal coronary angio- total scale score reflecting global neuropsychological
plasty, placement of coronary artery stent, or peripheral function. The SCL-90-R was administered just prior
vascular disease, organ transplant, chronic heart failure, to cognitive assessment for all participants.
known history of stroke, dementia, learning disability,
diagnosis of schizophrenia or bipolar disorder, other di- Statistical analysis
agnosis of neurological disorder that may affect cogni-
tive functioning, and history of head injury with loss The SCL-90-R Anxiety T-scores, SCL-90-R Depres-
of consciousness for more than 30 min. sion T-scores, RBANS total scale scores, as well as
At the time of evaluation, 28 participants endorsed at RBANS Index and subtest scores, were non-normally
least some anxious symptoms and 27 endorsed at least distributed, and thus, non-parametric analyses were
some depressive symptoms. Nine participants were tak- employed to minimize the effect of non-normal distri-
ing psychopharmaceuticals. Of those nine, seven partici- bution and outliers. In order to use covariates, all vari-
pants were taking antidepressants, one participant was ables were ranked to allow for non-parametric
taking an anticonvulsant, and one was taking an anxio- analyses. Spearman’s correlations were calculated be-
lytic medication. Five of those participants who were tween SCL-90-R Anxiety Scale T-score and RBANS to-
taking psychopharmaceuticals received current or past tal scale score. After it was apparent that anxiety levels
care from a psychiatrist for anxious or depressive symp- were significantly associated with RBANS total scale
toms. However, none of the participants identified score, follow-up Spearman’s correlations were calcu-
themselves as having active symptoms that they deemed lated to further characterize the relationships among
to be significant problems at the time of enrollment. severity of anxiety and the five RBANS index scores.
This research was approved by the University of Iowa In addition, partial correlations were calculated to
Institutional Review Board. All participants provided demonstrate the relationship of the 12 individual
written informed consent prior to participation. RBANS subtests scores with anxiety.
Age, education, and gender were not used as covari-
ates in the primary analyses for several reasons. Age
Anxiety, depression, and neuropsychological had already been adjusted for RBANS total scale scores
assessment and was not correlated with SCL-90-R Anxiety
T-scores. Age was used as a covariate only in follow-
Anxious and depressive symptoms were measured us- up correlations involving the 12 RBANS subtests be-
ing the Anxiety and Depression subscales of the Symp- cause, unlike the RBANS total scale score and index
tom Checklist-90—Revised (SCL-90-R; Derogatis, scores, the individual subtest scores are not age-cor-
1994). This self-report questionnaire consists of 90 rected. Education, although correlated with the
items that ask about psychological symptoms. The RBANS total scale score, did not significantly affect
Anxiety subscale has been shown to be a valid indica- the main outcome described below. Gender was not
tor of anxious symptoms (Cameron and Hudson, correlated with RBANS total scale score but was al-
1986; Koeter, 1992; Steer and Ranieri, 1993). The ready adjusted in SCL-90-R Anxiety T-scores.

Copyright # 2011 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2012; 27: 792–798.
10991166, 2012, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/gps.2785 by UNED, Wiley Online Library on [25/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Anxiety and cognitive function in the older adults 795

To investigate whether the relationship between Table 2 Relationships of the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised Anxiety
scores with the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised Depression scores and
anxiety and cognition was independent of depressive the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status
symptoms, a multiple regression model was devel- Index and sub-test scores
oped. In this model, SCL-90-R Depression T-scores,
Correlation
and SCL-90-R Anxiety T-scores were entered as inde- Test names coefficient p
pendent variables, and RBANS total scale score was
the dependent variable. A follow-up bivariate and a SCL-90-R Depression T-score 0.581 <0.000
partial correlation were conducted between cognitive RBANS total scale score 0.400 0.005
RBANS immediate memory index 0.284 0.050
ability and depressive symptoms. The partial correla- score
tion was controlled for anxious symptoms. List learning raw score 0.200 0.179
Story memory raw score 0.317 0.030
RBANS visuospatial/construc- 0.515 <0.000
tional index score
Results Figure copy raw score 0.352 0.015
Line orientation raw score 0.431 0.002
RBANS language index score 0.138 0.348
Descriptive statistics for RBANS and SCL-90-R Anxi- Picture naming raw score 0.139 0.350
ety scores are shown in Table 1 and indicate that, as Semantic fluency raw score 0.100 0.502
a group, participants were within normal limits with RBANS attention index score 0.122 0.407
Digit span raw score 0.038 0.800
regard to cognitive functioning and severity of anxious Coding raw score 0.170 0.254
symptoms. RBANS delayed memory index 0.305 0.035
As shown in Table 2, anxious symptoms (SCL-90-R score
List recall raw score 0.170 0.252
Anxiety Scale score) were significantly and directly as- List recognition raw score 0.097 0.515
sociated with depressive symptoms (SCL-90-R De- Story recall raw score 0.287 0.050
pression Scale score) [r(47) = 0.581, p ≤ 0.000]. Figure recall score 0.168 0.260
Anxious symptoms were also significantly but in-
Index score names are rendered in boldface, with the names of the
versely associated with global neuropsychological
subtests that contribute to each index score listed beneath them.
functioning (RBANS total scale score) [r(47) = 0.400, Subtest scores are raw scores, and therefore, their correlations with
p = 0.005]. Additionally, anxious symptoms were sig- Symptom Checklist-90—Revised (SCL-90-R) Anxiety score were
nificantly and inversely associated with the RBANS adjusted for age. RBANS, Repeatable Battery for the Assessment
immediate memory and visuospatial/constructional of Neuropsychological Status.
index scores, and with four of the RBANS subtest
scores that assess immediate and delayed story recall, Results of the secondary objective assessing anxious
ability to copy a geometric figure, and ability to judge and depressive symptoms’ relationship to cognition
and match the angles of lines (p < 0.05 for all). Given were less clear. A multiple regression analysis of the re-
that nine participants were taking psychotropic medi- lationship between anxious symptoms, depressive
cation, analyses were run both with and without those symptoms, and cognitive ability indicated that, to-
participants. All significant correlations between gether, both anxious symptoms and depressive symp-
RBANS tests and anxiety symptoms with inclusion of toms were significantly associated with RBANS total
medicated participants remained significant when scale score [R2 = 0.175, F(2, 45) = 4.786, p = 0.013]. Al-
medicated participants were excluded. though neither anxious symptoms nor depressive

Table 1 Demographics of study sample

Mean Range Standard deviation Percentile

RBANS total scale score 103.54 80–134 13.10 61


RBANS immediate memory index score 99.88 65–129 14.75 50
RBANS visuospatial/constructional index score 103.90 75–131 14.421 61
RBANS language index score 100.73 82–125 11.95 53
RBANS attention index score 104.71 82–135 15.58 63
RBANS delayed memory index score 103.83 84–127 10.34 61
SCL-90-R Anxiety T-score 45.23 37– 67 8.02 32

Percentile scores were obtained from the relevant test manuals. Higher percentile scores for Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsy-
chological Status (RBANS) scores indicate better cognitive functioning. A higher percentile score for the Symptom Checklist-90—Revised
(SCL-90-R) Anxiety score indicates more severe anxiety.

Copyright # 2011 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2012; 27: 792–798.
10991166, 2012, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/gps.2785 by UNED, Wiley Online Library on [25/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
796 A. N. Stillman et al.

symptoms emerged as a statistically significant corre- cognitive performance overall. Elliman et al. (1997) ap-
late of global neuropsychological functioning, the plied and tested this theory in a sample of young
relationship between neuropsychological functioning adults ages 18–31. They found that with a proces-
and anxiety, controlling for depression narrowly sing-intensive measure of sustained attention in parti-
missed significance, whereas its relationship with cipants with low, medium, and high levels of anxiety,
depressive symptoms, controlling for anxiety did not. participants with higher levels of anxiety did more
[r(47) = 0.278, p = 0.058; r(48) = 0.136, p = 0.363, poorly than those with low levels. The current study
respectively]. However, the regression model did ac- did not include a specific measure of working mem-
count for a higher amount of variance than either ory. However, it is plausible that anxiety-related dis-
Spearman’s correlation between cognitive function ruption in working memory could lead to
and anxiety [R2(47) = 0.160, p = 0.005] and cognitive subsequent difficulty on other tasks including those
function and depression [R2(47) = 0.106, p = 0.024]. involving visuospatial processing and memory.
The high comorbidity of anxiety with depression
(Lowe et al., 2008) raises the question of whether anx-
Discussion ious symptoms have an independent effect on global
neuropsychological function or if depressive symp-
The aims of this study were twofold. Initially, we toms drive the relationship, leading to our secondary
assessed whether normal levels of anxiety were corre- objective. In the current study sample, the multiple re-
lated with lower levels of neuropsychological function gression analysis suggests that anxious symptoms may
in a cognitively intact sample of older adults. Addi- have an association with global cognitive function sep-
tionally, we sought to determine if anxiety had an in- arate from that of depressive symptoms; however, the
dependent relationship on global cognitive function, association did not reach the level of statistical signifi-
controlling for depressive symptoms. cance. Given the strong relationship between depres-
With regard to the primary aim, our findings sug- sive and anxious symptoms, this non-significant
gest that even within normal limits, elevated anxiety finding was not surprising. We propose, however, that
symptoms are related to poorer global cognitive func- given the recruitment of a larger sample and/or mea-
tioning as measured by RBANS total scale score. Addi- surement of anxious and depressive symptoms with
tionally, we found that visuospatial/constructional more sensitive instruments, this relationship would
ability was the cognitive function most strongly corre- resolve to the level of significance. Nevertheless, the
lated with anxiety symptoms, though significant rela- regression model, incorporating both anxious and de-
tionships were also detected in measures of immediate pressive symptoms in relation to cognitive ability,
and delayed memory. accounted for a higher level of variance than either in-
Although the significant relationship noted was of a dependent variable alone. This result suggests that al-
moderate magnitude, it is intriguing that this relation- though anxiety’s relationship may or may not be
ship exists among healthy, relatively normal, older independent from depression, accounting for both
adults. These findings, in addition to the findings variables yields a stronger outcome.
given by Wetherell et al., suggest that both lower levels The present study demonstrates that it is clinically
of verbal and visual processing are inversely associated relevant to consider subclinical levels of anxiety when
with elevated anxiety symptoms. Importantly, the assessing cognitive functioning in older adults. Al-
inverted U relationship of anxiety symptoms and cogni- though these levels of anxiety may not require treat-
tive function, established between state anxiety and cog- ment, they may be correlated with early stages of
nitive function (Bierman et al., 2005; Bierman et al., cognitive decline or poor test performance. It is im-
2008) seems not to apply in the context of the current portant for physicians to recognize this relationship
study. This could be because that relationship changes given the possibility that cognitive dysfunction could
in older age, as Flint (1994) suggested. It is also likely be partially associated with anxious symptoms, even
that the Anxiety Scale of the SCL-90 is measuring a when those anxious symptoms are not necessarily se-
combination of both state and trait anxiety. vere. Furthermore, in patients whose level of anxiety
Although the mechanism involved in the relationship does require treatment, effective treatment for the
between anxiety and cognition remains unclear, Eysenck anxiety symptoms may reverse cognitive changes.
and Calvo (1992) proposed the Processing and Effi- Implications of this research should encourage clini-
ciency Theory, which suggests that anxiety precedes cians to be more aware of subthreshold anxiety symp-
cognitive dysfunction. Their theory posits that working toms and their association with lower levels of
memory is interrupted by anxiety, leading to poorer cognitive performance in older adults.

Copyright # 2011 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2012; 27: 792–798.
10991166, 2012, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/gps.2785 by UNED, Wiley Online Library on [25/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Anxiety and cognitive function in the older adults 797

The current findings also have additional implications Conflict of interest


for research. As a matter of protocol, researchers fre-
quently control for depressive symptoms in cognitive None declared.
performance studies, but this practice has not been typi-
cally expanded to include measures of anxiety symptoms
in most studies. Our findings demonstrate that anxiety is Acknowledgement
an important variable to take into consideration when
conducting research studies on cognitive performance This research was supported by the following grants to
in older people. Lower levels of cognitive performance Dr David J. Moser from the National Institute on
may be at least partially associated with anxiety, in addi- Aging: NIA RO1 AG030417-01A2 and NIA 1 K23
tion to depressive symptoms, in studies of mild cognitive AG020649-01A1. This publication was made possible
impairment, Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia, or by Grant Number UL1RR024979 from the National
other conditions that involve cognitive decline. Center for Research Resources (NCRR), a part of the
The present study is limited by small sample size. Ad- National Institutes of Health (NIH). Its contents are
ditionally, the use of the SCL-90-R subtest for anxiety solely the responsibility of the authors and do not nec-
and depression limited the type and range of anxious essarily represent the official views of the CTSA or NIH.
and depressive symptoms that could be measured. Fur-
thermore, because of the use of the SCL-90-R Anxiety References
Scale and the fact that extensive historical information re-
garding anxiety was not obtained, we were not able to as- Bannon S, Gonsalvez CJ, Croft RJ. 2008. Processing impairments in OCD: it is more
certain whether state versus trait anxiety is more closely than inhibition! Behav Res Ther 46: 689–700. DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.02.006.
Beekman A, Bremmer M, Deeg, D, Van Balkom A, Smit J, De Beurs E, Van Dyck R,
associated with cognitive functioning in our sample. A Van Tilburg, W. 1998. Anxiety disorders in later life: a report from the longitudinal
further limitation was that nine participants were taking aging study Amsterdam. Int J Geriat Psychiatry 13: 717–726. DOI: 10.1002/(SICI)
1099-1166(1998100)13:10<717::AID-GPS857>3.0.CO;2-M.
psychopharmaceuticals. However, as noted previously, Bierman EJM, Comijs HC, Jonker C, Beekman AT. 2005. Effects of anxiety versus de-
an identical pattern of significant results was obtained pression on cognition in later life. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 13: 686–693.
Bierman EJM, Comijs HC, Jonke, C, Beekman AT. 2008. Anxiety symptoms and cog-
both with and without inclusion of these participants. nitive performance in later life: results from the longitudinal study Amsterdam.
Future research should employ more sensitive and Aging & Ment Health 12: 517–523.
Cameron OG, Hudson CJ. 1986. Influence of exercise on anxiety level in patients with
specific measures of anxiety, along with additional his- anxiety levels. Psychosomatics 27: 720–723.
torical information regarding participants’ duration, Chodosh, J, Kado DM, Seeman TE, Karlamangla AS. 2007. Depressive symptoms as a
predictor of cognitive decline: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Am J Geriat
type, and severity of anxious symptoms. Additionally, Psychiatry 15: 720–723.
longitudinal study would allow for determination of the Clark A, Friedman M. 1983. Factor structure and discriminant validity of the SCL-90
in a veteran psychiatric population. J Pers Assess 47: 396–404.
relationship between anxious symptoms and cognitive Derogatis, LR. 1994. SCL-90-R: administration, scoring and procedures manual. Na-
decline across time. Nonetheless, the current findings tional Computer Systems: Minneapolis, MN.
Elliman N, Green M, Rogers P, Finch G. 1997. Processing-efficiency theory and the
are intriguing in that they reveal a significant relationship working-memory system: impairments associated with sub-clinical anxiety. Pers
between anxious symptoms and cognitive functioning in Indiv Differ 23: 31–35.
Eysenck M, Calvo M. (1992). Anxiety and performance: the processing Efficiency
generally healthy, cognitively intact older individuals Theory. Cognition Emotion 6: 409–434.
who are experiencing subclinical levels of anxiety. Flint A. 1994. Epidemiology and comorbidity of anxiety disorders in the elderly. Am J
Psychiatry 151: 640–649.
Forsell Y, Winbald B. 1998. Feelings of anxiety and associated variables in a very el-
derly population. Int J Geriat Psychiatry 14: 454–458. DOI: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-
1166(199807)13:7<454::AID-GPS795>3.0.CO;2-D.
Haikal M, Hong RY. 2010. The effects of social evaluation and looming threat on self-
Key points attentional biases and social anxiety. J Anxiety Disord 24: 345–352. DOI: 10.1016/j.


janxdis.2010.01.007.
Sub-clinical anxious symptoms are associated Johnsen GE, Asbjornsen AE. 2008. Consistent impaired verbal memory in PTSD: a
meta-analysis. J Affect Disord 111: 74–82. DOI: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.02.007.
with poorer cognitive performance in healthy Johnsen GE, Asbjornsen AE. 2009. Verbal learning and memory impairments in post-
older people traumatic stress disorder: the role of encoding strategies. Psychiatry Res 165: 68–77.

• Although depressive symptoms are also asso-


ciated with poorer cognition, current findings
Koeter M. 1992. Validity of the GHW and the SCL anxiety and depression scales: a
comparative study. J Affect Disorders 24: 271–280.
Lowe B, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Mussell M, Schellbert D, Droenke K. 2008. De-
pression, anxiety and somatization in primary care: syndrome overlap and func-
underscore the importance of taking anxiety tional impairment. Gen Hosp Psychiat 30: 191–199.
into consideration as well, in both clinical Mantella R, Butters M, Dew MA, Mulsant, B, Begley A, Tracey B, Shear MK, Reynolds
III C, Lenze, E. 2007. Cognitive impairment in late-life generalized anxiety disor-
and research settings

der. Am J Geriatric Psychiatry 15: 673–679. DOI: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31803111f2.
Longitudinal research is needed to determine Pachana A, Byrne G, Siddle H, Koloski N, Harley E, Arnold E. 2007. Development
and validation of the Geriatric Anxiety Inventory. Int Psychogeriatrics 19:
the relationship between anxious symptoms 103–114. DOI: 10.1017/S1041610206003504.
and cognitive decline across time Randolph C. 1998. Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status.
The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX.

Copyright # 2011 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2012; 27: 792–798.
10991166, 2012, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/gps.2785 by UNED, Wiley Online Library on [25/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
798 A. N. Stillman et al.

Schultz S, Moser D, Bishop J, Ellingrod V. 2005. Phobic anxiety in late-life in rela- Steer R, Clark D, Ranieri W. 1994. Symptom dimensions of the SCL-90-R: a test of
tionship to cognition and 5HTTLPR polymorphism. Psychiatr Genet 15: the tripartite model of anxiety and depression. J Pers Assess 64: 525–536.
305–306. Wetherell JL, Reynolds C, Gatz M, Pedersen N. 2002. Anxiety, cognitive performance,
Smalbrugge M, Pot A, Jongenelis K, Beekman A, Eefsting J. 2005. Prevalence and cor- and cognitive decline in normal aging. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 57: 246–255.
relates of anxiety among nursing home patients. J Affect Disorders 88: 145–153. DOI: 10.1093/geronb/57.3.P246.
DOI: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.06.006. Wilson RS, de Leon CFM, Bennett DA, Bienias JL, Evans DA 2004. Depressive symp-
Steer R, Ranieri W. 1993. Further evidence for the validity of the Back Anxiety Inven- toms and cognitive decline in a community population of older persons. J Neurol
tory with psychiatric outpatients. J Anxiety Disord 7: 195–205. Neurosur Ps 75: 126–129.

Copyright # 2011 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2012; 27: 792–798.

You might also like