Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Australian Journal of Psychology

ISSN: 0004-9530 (Print) 1742-9536 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/raup20

An examination of the relationship between


anxiety and performance on prospective and
retrospective memory tasks

Lynne M. Harris & Steven R. Cumming

To cite this article: Lynne M. Harris & Steven R. Cumming (2003) An examination of the
relationship between anxiety and performance on prospective and retrospective memory
tasks, Australian Journal of Psychology, 55:1, 51-55, DOI: 10.1080/00049530412331312874

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00049530412331312874

Published online: 23 Aug 2006.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 646

View related articles

Citing articles: 2 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=raup20
51

An Examination of the Relationship Between Anxiety


and Performance on Prospective
and Retrospective Memory Tasks
Lynne M. Harris and Steven R. Cumming
The University of Sydney, Australia

The present study investigated the association between self-reported state anxiety, trait anxiety and perfor-
mance on closely matched prospective and retrospective memory tasks and on a working memory (reading
span) task. Eysenck and Calvo's (1992) processing efficiency theory suggests that the deleterious effects
of anxiety on cognitive task performance are mediated by worry occupying working memory. They suggest
that the adverse effects of anxiety would be most marked when people are experiencing acute anxiety states
on tasks that place high demands on working memory capacity and are treated as secondary tasks, condi-
tions that typically occur for prospective remembering. Sixty-three undergraduate students were allocated
to high, medium and low anxiety conditions using tertile splits of ranked state and trait anxiety scores.
It was found that people with elevated levels of state anxiety performed more poorly on a prospective
memory test than people with lower levels of state anxiety. No difference between these groups in retrospec-
tive memory or reading span performance was found. Trait anxiety scores were unrelated to performance on
any of the memory tasks. The results suggest that state anxiety has a direct impact on prospective memory
performance that is independent of its impact on working memory capacity.

Prospective memory refers to the ability to remember to do or the intention to remember, must be retained and retrieved
things in the future, and is usually measured by whether at the appropriate time (Einstein & McDaniel, 1990). As would
people remember to perform an action at some later time. be expected from this description, factors that influence encod-
In everyday life, prospective memory tasks include remember- ing and recall of content that are important for both prospective
ing to attend appointments, deliver messages, and take medica- and retrospective remembering, such as word frequency
tion (Dobbs & Rule, 1987). In experimental settings, and encoding specificity, have a similar impact on both kinds
an event-based prospective memory test might involve remem- of tasks (e.g., Mantyla, 1993; McDaniel & Einstein, 1993).
bering to press a key in response to the word "rake" if it It may be expected that variables affecting the attentional
appears during a short-term memory test (e.g., Einstein capacity necessary for ongoing retention and unprompted
& McDaniel, 1990). In contrast, most long-term memory tasks periodic retrieval would differentially affect prospective and
are retrospective and are concerned with remembering past retrospective remembering. Certainly, restricted attentional
experiences, such as what you had for lunch yesterday and capacity appears to interfere with the initiation of intentions
who you were with or, in more experimental situations, the to remember, and to adversely affect prospective memory task
contents of a word list presented minutes or hours before performance. For example, Park, Hertzog, Kidder, Morrell,
(Winograd, 1988). and Mayhorn (1997) reported that event-based prospective
Tests of prospective memory include both a prospective tasks had particularly high attentional requirements, exerting
component ("remembering to remember"; Dobbs & Rule, a high performance cost on a concurrent working memory task
1987) and a retrospective component (remembering the content (see also Einstein, McDaniel, Richardson, Guynn & Cunfer,
of what is to be remembered; Einstein & McDaniel, 1990). 1995; Ellis & Nimmo-Smith, 1993; Kidder, Park, Hertzog,
In practice, prospective memory tests de-emphasise the amount & Morrell, 1997).
of information that needs to be remembered and emphasise Harris and Menzies (1999) reported that elevated state
the need for the individual to spontaneously remember to per- anxiety also differentially affects prospective and retrospec-
form the action (e.g., Cherry & LeCompte, 1999; Einstein tive memory measures, being associated with reduced pro-
& McDaniel, 1990), while retrospective memory tests empha- spective memory performance, but not with retrospective
sise remembering content. For example, Mantyla (1993) used memory performance. This finding can be understood in terms
a prospective memory procedure requiring responses to 16 nouns of Eysenck and Calvo's (1992) processing efficiency theory.
and a retrospective procedure requiring remembering of 120 These authors distinguish between the impact of trait anxiety,
nouns. Similarly, Einstein & McDaniel (1990) examined a characteristic that implies a disposition towards high levels
prospective memory by looking at responses to three occurrences of psychophysiological arousal, and state anxiety, which
of the word "rake" in a 42-item list, compared to retrospective refers to the actual anxiety experienced at a particular time
free recall of 36 words and retrospective recognition of 56 words. determined by both trait anxiety and situational threat.
Retrospective remembering and prospective remembering This theory recognises that anxiety may either enhance
both require that to-be-remembered information is encoded, or adversely affect task performance, and both of these effects
and both require that the information is held and eventually are explained by resource allocation. Eysenck and Calvo
retrieved. The fundamental difference between prospective and (1992) argue that anxiety will enhance performance on tasks
retrospective memory measures, however, is that prospective treated as primary because fear of adverse outcomes will lead
remembering requires that the instruction to activate retrieval, an individual to preferentially allocate resources to ensure

Address for correspondence: Dr Lynne Harris, School of Behavioural and Community Health Sciences, Faculty of Health Science,
The University of Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia. Email: L.Harris@fhs.usyd.edu.au

- Australian Journal of Psychology Vol. 55, No. I, 2003 pp. 51-55


52 Lynne M.Harris and Steven R.Cumming

success on that task. However, anxiety may interfere with cap- imperative designed to allow the organism to attend to and
acity demanding cognitive tasks because distracting thoughts respond to potential threats. Several other research findings
(worry) associated with state anxiety compete for limited have suggested that anxiety is associated with reduced focused
resources. In support of this view, Sorg and Whitney (1992) attention and increased peripheral scanning (e.g., Fox, 1993:
examined performance on a working memory task among high Shapiro & Lim, 1989), and Hopko, Ashcraft, Ruggiero,
trait anxious compared to low trait anxious individuals either and Lewis (1998) argue that it is the failure to inhibit attention
under conditions of situational stress likely to induce anxiety to task-irrelevant distractors that accounts for performance
states or no stress. In the absence of stress, high trait anxious decrements among anxious individuals.
individuals performed better than low, but in the presence The present study sought to examine the relationship
of stressors this effect was reversed. Eysenck and Calvo between state and trait anxiety, prospective remembering and
suggest that the deleterious effects of state anxiety on cogni- retrospective remembering using memory tasks that were
tive task performance would be most marked on tasks that matched as closely as possible. In particular, the amount of to-
place high demands on working memory capacity and are be-remembered information, the nature of the cues available
treated as secondary tasks. Park et al. (1997) reported that for remembering, and the time between encoding and retrieval.
event-based prospective tasks were particularly sensitive were matched between the prospective and retrospective tasks.
to concurrent working memory load, attributing this to the The prospective and retrospective tests were both embedded
demands on working memory resources that must be diverted within a semantic association task. This is in contrast to previ-
to allow sustained and continuous monitoring to ensure that ous studies comparing prospective and retrospective remem-
responding occurs appropriately. In addition, prospective bering where prospective memory tests are carried out in the
memory tasks are always carried out in the context of other presence of other ongoing cognitive tasks, such as short-term
ongoing activity, usually under conditions in which the relative memory tests (Cherry & Le Compte, 1999) or semantic associ-
number of responses required suggests that the prospective ation tasks (Mantyla, 1993) but retrospective remembering
task is likely to be treated as secondary (e.g., McDaniel is carried out in the absence of other competing tasks.
& Einstein, 1990; Mantyla, 1993, 1994). It may therefore The relationship between working memory capacity, state
be expected that prospective memory tasks may be particularly anxiety and prospective remembering was also considered
sensitive to the effects of state anxiety. directly in the present study. It was predicted that performance
A number of features of Harris and Menzies' (1999) on the prospective memory measure, but not the retrospective
methodology should be noted. Firstly, the prospective and memory measure, would be worse among those with elevated
retrospective tasks were quite different in a range of ways state anxiety compared to those with low levels of state
apart from the relationship between the time at which the anxiety. If this effect was mediated by working memory
instruction to remember was provided and that at which the capacity, it was expected that there would be a correlation
opportunity to retrieve was available. The memory tasks varied between performance on a measure of working memory, the
greatly in the amount of information that needed to be remem- reading span task, and prospective memory performance.
bered (four exemplars of two semantic categories had to Based on Eysenck and Calvo's (1992) formulation of the
be identified in the prospective task and 60 items were avail- relationship between anxiety and cognitive task performance
able for recollection in the retrospective free recall task). it was expected that differences in trait anxiety would not
The tasks also differed in the cues that were available for adversely influence performance on the memory tasks.
remembering (exemplars of the semantic category in the
prospective task and no cues in the retrospective free recall METHOD
task). In addition, the amount of time between the encoding Participants
of the material and the retrieval of the material varied between From an original pool of 100 undergraduates who completed the
the two tasks. The opportunity to remember on the prospective tasks, 63 had perfect immediate retrieval of the to-be-remem-
task occurred about 10 minutes after the information to be bered word list. This group formed the sample for the present
remembered was presented, but there was no delay between study. The rationale for this selection of participants is described
encoding and retrieval in the retrospective task. The tasks also in detail below. The mean age of participants was 19.4 years
differed in the amount of competing activity expected at the (range 18-35 years) and 83% of the sample was female.
time of retrieval. The prospective task was carried out in the
background of a semantic generation task, while the retrospec- Materials
tive task was carried out alone. It should be emphasised that Two matched lists of 35 words were constructed. All of the
such differences between prospective and retrospective words were nouns between five and seven letters in length
memory measures are common in the literature. However, the with an average Kucera-Francis word frequency of 39.3 (range
many differences between the prospective and retrospective 30-50) list 1, and 38.7 (range 30-50) list 2 (MRC Psycholin-
memory tasks make it difficult to clearly determine why guistic database: Coltheart, 1981 ). A total of five targets were
anxiety is associated with prospective remembering only. embedded within each 35 word list. The two sets of five
Cumming and Harris (2001) examined the predictions targets were matched for word frequency (means were 42.8,
of Eysenck and Calvo's processing efficiency theory in tasks range = 36-50, for list 1 and 43.2, range = 38-50, for list 2).
analogous to diagnostic decision-making. Cumming and Harris Each list was later used in either the prospective or retrospec-
compared task performance among groups of people with and tive memory procedure (counterbalanced between partici-
without experimentally-induced anxiety (Study I) and with pants), so that the maximum score for the prospective and
and without naturally-occurring elevated state anxiety (Study retrospective memory tests was 5. The reading span task
2). In both studies performance on the primary decision- required participants to listen to short sentences read aloud in
making task was disrupted by anxiety and the findings could groups of two, three, four or five sentences. After each
not be explained by competition for limited working memory sentence was read, a judgement had to be made as to whether
resources. Rather, it appeared that the effects of anxiety on per- the sentence was true or false (e.g., "You sit on a chair") and
formance were more direct, disengaging attention from an the final word in the sentence had to be retained (e.g.,"chair").
ongoing cognitive activity. These authors argued that a direct After all sentences in the group were read aloud participants
impact of anxiety on attention was an adaptive biological wrote down the final words from all sentences in the group.
Australian Journal of Psychology - April 2003
Anxiety and Memory 53
Participants were provided with a set of materials comprising correct on the prospective memory task t(62) = 5.8, p < 0.05;
the following: means were 0.56 (SD = 0.48) and 0.91 (SD = 0.17)
1. A sheet of paper containing 20 words, half of which were for prospective and retrospective memory respectively). 3
the 10 targets. This was used as an encoding check. There was a significant correlation between State and Trait
anxiety scores (r = 0.52, p < 0.01 one-tailed). However, no
2. A sheet of paper to record responses to the reading span task.
correlation emerged between scores on the prospective and
3.The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger, retrospective memory test (r = 0.13, p > 0.1) or between the
Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1983), a commonly used measure reading span task and either the prospective or retrospective
of state and trait anxiety. memory tasks (r = 0.12 and r = 0.12, p > 0.1 for prospective
4. A sheet of paper for recording responses to the semantic and retrospective memory respectively). 4
association task / prospective memory task with the numbers
1-35 in three columns. State Anxiety and Memory
5. A sheet of paper for recording responses to the semantic The relationship between anxiety and the memory measures
association task / retrospective memory task with the numbers was examined using analysis of variance. Participants were
1-35 in three columns and the instructions with the numbers divided into three approximately equal groups using a tertile
36-70 in three columns. split on the ranked state anxiety scores. This methodology
is commonly used in examining the relationship between
Procedure cognitive task performance and anxiety (e.g., see Elliman,
Participants were tested in groups of approximately 15. Green, Rogers, & Finch, 1997; Hopko et al., 1998). The low
All participants received the following instructions: anxiety group comprised people with state anxiety scores
This task must be done quietly and by yourself. Concentrate on these between 20 and 36. The moderate anxiety group comprised
instructions. Ask anything you want to now, because only some people with state anxiety scores between 37 and 45. The high
of the instructions will be repeated later. anxiety group comprised people with state anxiety scores
You need to remember a short list of words and your memory for between 46 and 60. One way analysis of variance with planned
the words will be tested. You will do some filler tasks to create a
contrasts comparing the three groups revealed that the high
delay for the memory tests. You will hear a list of words and will be
asked to write down a word that is related to the one that you hear in anxiety group performed significantly worse than the
meaning. If you hear "zoo", you might write "lion". While you are low anxiety group on the prospective memory task F(1, 60)
doing this, you may hear words from the list that you learn. If you = 5.65, p s 0.02. No other differences between groups were
hear a word that is from the list, write the word itself, rather than an significant, although the difference between the low
associate. If you hear "dog", and "dog" was on the list, write "dog" and moderate groups approached significance F(1, 60) = 3.34,
rather than, for example, "cat" on the associate task. This instruction
won't be repeated. p = 0.07. There were no significant differences between the
groups on retrospective memory performance or reading span
Participants were then presented with the 10-word study list performance (see Table 1).
at a rate of one word every 10 seconds. Following this they
performed the encoding check. Only those who scored 100% Trait Anxiety and Memory
on this task were included in the data analysis to ensure that
The analyses described above were repeated with participants
any later prospective memory failures could not be attributed to
divided into three approximately equal groups using a tertile
a failure to encode the content. Participants were asked to circle
split on the ranked trait anxiety scores. The low anxiety group
the 10 study list items from a list of 20 words. All participants
comprised people with trait anxiety scores between 24 and 37.
then completed the reading span task and completed the STAI.
The moderate anxiety group comprised people with trait
Lastly, participants performed the two memory tests embed-
anxiety scores between 38 and 43. The high anxiety group
ded in the semantic association test. For the semantic associa-
comprised people with trait anxiety scores between 44 and 57.
tion test, the 70 words were read out at a rate of one every
There were no significant differences between the groups
10 seconds and participants wrote a semantic associate of each
on prospective memory, retrospective memory or reading span
word during the interval while keeping a running total of all
measures (see Table 1).
words that began with the letter "s".2 Of the 70 words, 10 were
from the study list, and memory for these words was indicated DISCUSSION
when participants recorded the word itself, rather than an The findings of this study demonstrated that elevated state
associate, in response to study list words. During the first half
anxiety was associated with poorer performance on a prospec-
of the semantic association task participants were not re-
minded of the instruction to write down the study list word
instead of an associate. The responses to the five study list
Table I
words presented during this part of the test therefore reflected
Mean Prospective, Retrospective and Reading Span Task
prospective memory, as participants had to retain the intention Performance for Those with Low, Medium, and High State
to respond to study list words across time. During the second and Trait Anxiety Scores
half of the semantic association task the instruction to write
down the study list word instead of an associate was repeated, Task
so that responses to the five studied words presented in this Prospective Retrospective Reading Span
phase indicated retrospective memory. The memory tests were Memory (SD) Memory (SD) (SD)
always carried out in the same order, with the prospective
State Anxiety Level
memory test first. The delay between the presentation of the Low (N = 22) 0.75 (0.42) 0.93 (0.11) 4.57 (0.64)
study list and the semantic association task was about Medium (N = 22) 0.50 (0.50) 0.92 (0.22) 4.18 (0.88)
10 minutes, and the whole procedure took about 30 minutes. High (N = 19) 0.41 (0.46) 0.89 (0.17) 4.11 (0.92)
Trait Anxiety Level
RESULTS Low (N = 20) 0.66 (0.45) 0.91 (0.24) 4.50 (0.71)
The average proportion correct on the retrospective memory Medium (N = 22) 0.50 (0.51) 0.90 (0.16) 4.11 (0.90)
task was significantly better than the average proportion High (N = 21) 0.53 (0.47) 0.93 (0.09) 4.29 (0.83)

Australian Journal of Psychology - April 2003


54 Lynne M.
Harris and Steven R. Cumming

tive memory task, but was not associated with performance restricted attentional capacity appears to interfere with the
on a retrospective memory measure. Trait anxiety, however, initiation of intentions to remember, and to adversely affect
was not related to either prospective or retrospective memory prospective memory task performance (Einstein et al., 1995;
task performance. The findings confirm and extend those of Ellis & Nimmo-Smith, 1993; Kidder et al., 1997; Park et al..
Harris and Menzies (1999) concerning the relationship 1997). A withdrawal of attention from ongoing cognitive tasks
between state anxiety and prospective, but not retrospective, to effectively scan the environment for potential danger may
memory task performance. While replicating those findings, be seen as extremely adaptive for organisms that are in a state
this study is the first to systematically equate the prospective of acute anxiety. Further studies investigating the relationship
and retrospective tasks in terms of the amount of to-be-remem- between anxiety, prospective memory and sustained attention
bered information, the delay between study and test and the are currently being undertaken.
presence of a competing background task.
Dissociations between prospective and retrospective ENDNOTES
memory are uncommon in the memory literature (e.g., Einstein I This list is also available at htttp://www.psy.uwa.edu.au/MRC
et al., 1995; Einstein, Smith, McDaniel, & Shaw, 1997; DataBase/uwa/mrc.htm
Mantyla, 1993; McDaniel & Einstein, 1993), and this is not 2 This task was included to increase the ongoing working memory load
surprising as performance on prospective memory tests usually during the semantic association/memory task to make the overall task
more difficult.
reflects both a retrospective memory component (remembering
3 To check for possible ceiling effects the analyses of the retrospective
the content of the prospective memory task) and a prospective data were repeated removing participants who scored 5 on the retro-
memory component (remembering the intention to remember). spective test. The pattern of results with this smaller sample was
In the present task, the use of the encoding check and the identical.
decision to select only participants who demonstrated accurate 4 There was a ceiling effect in the reading span task using the scoring
retrospective memory reduced the influence of retrospective recommended by Daneman and Carpenter (1980). When the reading
memory failure on prospective memory performance. Consis- span responses were re-scored to reflect errors at each set level.
the ceiling effect was eliminated. However, there was still no correla-
tent with this argument, there was no correlation between the tion between reading span scores and either prospective memory
prospective and retrospective memory performance. or retrospective memory scores.
One account of the relationship between anxiety and prospec-
tive memory is in terms of processing efficiency theory (Eysenck REFERENCES
& Calvo, 1992). If state anxiety adversely influences the avail- Cherry, K.E., & LeCompte, D.C. (1999). Age and individual differences
ability of cognitive resources, and if the self-initiated retrieval influence prospective memory. Psychology and Aging. 14, 60-76.
of instructions to remember imposes a cognitive "cost" upon Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. Quarterly
prospective memory that is not borne by retrospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 33A, 497-505.
it would follow that anxiety would impact upon prospective, Cumming. S.R., & Harris, L.M. (2001). The impact of anxiety on the
accuracy of diagnostic decision-making. Stress and Health, 17.
but not retrospective memory, as found in the present study.
281-286.
From processing efficiency theory an effect of state, but not trait.
Daneman, M., &Carpenter, P.A. (1980). Individual differences in working
anxiety on task performance would be expected, as reported memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
in this study. However, processing efficiency theory would also Behaviour, 19, 45(-466.
predict a relationship between anxiety and reading span perfor- Dobbs, A.R.. & Rule, B.G. (1987). Prospective memory and self-reports
mance, as has been reported in a number of previous studies of memory abilities in older adults. Canadian Journal of Psychology.
(e.g., Sorg & Whitney, 1992). Markham and Darke (1991) found 4/, 209-222.
significant differences between high and low anxiety groups only Einstein, G.O.. & McDaniel, M.A. (1990) Normal aging and prospective
memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
on difficult items from a verbal reasoning task. In the present
&Cognition, 16, 717-726.
study, participants performed well on the reading span task, Einstein. G.O., McDaniel, M.A., Richardson, S.L., Guynn, M.J..
suggesting that it was not a difficult task, and so the absence & Cunfer, A.R. (1995). Aging and prospective memory: Examining
of differences between the state anxiety groups in the reading the influences of self-initiated retrieval processes. Journal of Experi-
span data is not surprising. mental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21. 996-1007.
Alternatively, Cumming and Harris (2001) propose that state Einstein, G.O., Smith, R.E., McDaniel, M.A., & Shaw, P. (1997). Aging
anxiety impacts upon cognitive task performance directly, and prospective memory: The influence of increased task demands
at encoding and retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 12, 479-488.
without being a consequence of working memory disruption.
Elliman, N., Green, M., Rogers, P.. & Finch, G. (1997). Processing-
The present results are consistent with this suggestion. Anxiety efficiency theory and the working memory suystem: Impairments
was found to adversely affect prospective memory, but had associated with sub-clinical anxiety. PersonalitY and Individual
no significant effect on reading span performance, and there Differences. 23. 31-35.
was no relationship between reading span and prospective Ellis, J.A., & Nimmo-Smith, . (1993). Recollecting naturally-occurring
memory performance. It would therefore appear that the intentions: A study of cognitive and affective factors. Memory,
i. 107-126.
impact of anxiety on prospective memory is independent of its
Eysenck, M.W., & Calvo, M.G. (1992). Anxiety and performance:
impact on working memory capacity. An adaptive function
The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409-434.
of anxiety is to disengage attention from concurrent and Fox, E. (1993). Attentional bias in anxiety: Selective or not? Behaviour
demanding tasks in order to examine the environment for Research and Therapy, 31, 487-493.
potential threat. By this account, anxiety does not impair task Harris, L.M.. & Menzies, R.G. (1999). Mood and prospective memory.
performance by competing for working memory resources. Memor, 7. 117-127.
rather it causes a redirection of attention away from current, Hopko, D., Ashcraft, M., Ruggiero, K., & Lewis. C. (1998). Mathematics
ongoing, information processing tasks. Such a view is consistent anxiety and working memory: support for the existence of a deficient
with the suggestion of Hopko et al. (1998) that anxiety causes a inhibition mechanism. Journal ofAnxiety Disorders, 12, 343-355.
difficulty with focusing attention that accounts for poor perfor- Kidder, D.P.. Park, D.C., Hertzog, C., & Morrell, R.W. (1997). Prospec-
tive memory and aging: the effects of working memory and prospec-
mance on cognitive tasks. Prospective remembering requires tive memory task load. Aging, Neuropsycholog,. and Cognition.
retention and periodical unprompted retrieval of the intention 4,93 112.
to recall, and may therefore be more vulnerable to these atten- Mantyla, T. (1993). Priming effects in prospective memory. Memory,
tional influences than retrospective memory. Certainly. I. 203 218.

Australian Journal of Psychology - April 2003


Anxiety and Memory 55
Mantyla, T. (1994). Remembering to remember: Adult age differences in Sorg, B., &Whitney, P. (1992). The effect of trait anxiety and situational
prospective memory. Journal of Gerontology: PsychologicalSciences, stress on working memory capacity. Journalof Research in Personal-
49, 276-282. ity, 26, 235-241.
Markham, R., & Darke, S. (1991). The effects of anxiety on verbal Spielberger, C.D., Gorsuch, R.L., & Lushene, R.E. (1983). Manualforthe
and spatial task performance. Australian Journal of Psychology, State-TraitAnxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psycholo-
43, 107-111. gists Press.
McDaniel, M.A., &Einstein, G.O. (1993). The importance of cue familiar- Winograd, G. (1988). Some observations on prospective remembering.
ity and cue distinctiveness in prospective memory. Memory, 1, 23-41. In M.M. Gruneberg, P.E., Morris & R.N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical
Park, D.C., Hertzog, C, Kidder, D.P., Morrell, R.W., Mayhorn, C.B. aspects of memory: Current research and issues, Volume I
(1997). Effect of age on event-based and time-based prospective (pp. 349-353). Chichester: Wiley.
memory. Psychology andAging, 12, 314-327.
Shapiro, K.L., &Lim, A. (1989). The impact of anxiety on visual attention
to central and peripheral events. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
27, 345-351.

Australian Journal of Psychology - April 2003

You might also like