Moller 2006

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

Choice and Ego-Depletion:

The Moderating Role of Autonomy

Arlen C. Moller
Edward L. Deci
Richard M. Ryan
University of Rochester

The self-regulatory strength model maintains that all acts of self- For example, emotional self-control often has
regulation, self-control, and choice result in a state of fatigue been associated with ego-depletion. Participants who
called ego-depletion. Self-determination theory differentiates suppressed reactions to an emotionally evocative film
between autonomous regulation and controlled regulation. Because (humorous or sad) performed worse on a subsequent
making decisions represents one instance of self-regulation, the anagram task than did participants who were free to
authors also differentiate between autonomous choice and con- express their emotions and thus did not have to exert
trolled choice. Three experiments support the hypothesis that self-control (Baumeister et al., 1998, Study 3). In a study
whereas conditions representing controlled choice would be ego- by Muraven, Tice, and Baumeister (1998), participants
depleting, conditions that represented autonomous choice would were exposed to a distressing, sad film clip and were
not. In Experiment 3, the authors found significant mediation instructed either to stifle their emotional response or to
by perceived self-determination of the relation between the choice amplify it. Participants in both of these emotion-regula-
condition (autonomous vs. controlled) and ego-depletion as tion conditions (i.e., either decreasing or increasing
measured by performance. their response) persisted for less time on a subsequent
handgrip task that required physical stamina, relative to
control group participants who were not told to alter
their emotional states.
Keywords: choice; ego-depletion; self-regulation; self-control; autonomy;
Appetite regulation, another form of self-control,
self-determination
also has been regularly linked to ego-depletion in the
B aumeister and colleagues (Baumeister, Bratslavsky,
Muraven, & Tice, 1998) have argued that all acts of voli-
literature. For instance, resisting the temptation of choco-
late chip cookies caused participants to give up more
quickly on a subsequent, unsolvable, geometric, figure-
tion and self-control are effortful and draw on a limited tracing task (Baumeister et al., 1998, Study 1). Vohs and
resource, resulting in a state they refer to as ego-depletion. Heatherton (2000) found that dieters asked to sit next
Ego-depletion is theorized to be “a temporary reduction to a bowl of candies were more ego-depleted, as indi-
in the self’s capacity or willingness to engage in volitional cated later by their eating more ice cream (Study 1) and
action (including controlling the environment, control- persisting less on a demanding cognitive task (Study 2)
ling oneself, making choices, and initiating action),
caused by prior exercise of volition” (Baumeister et al.,
1998, p. 1253). Authors’ Note: We thank Thomas Cole, Laura Maruskin, John C.
The vast majority of studies supporting this ego- (or Poiarkoff, and Katie Riegel for their help with data collection and
self-regulatory) strength model have demonstrated that management. Please address correspondence concerning this article
one act of self-control can impair performance on a to Arlen C. Moller, Department of Psychology, PO Box 270266,
University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14627; e-mail: moller@psych.
subsequent, yet ostensibly unrelated, task that requires rochester.edu.
self-control (Baumeister et al., 1998; Muraven &
Baumeister, 2000; Schmeichel & Baumeister, 2004; PSPB, Vol. 32 No. 8, August 2006 1024-1036
DOI: 10.1177/0146167206288008
Schmeichel, Vohs, & Baumeister, 2003). © 2006 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

1024

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF LOUISVILLE on January 18, 2015


Moller et al. / CHOICE AND EGO-DEPLETION 1025

than were participants who sat farther from the bowl of Schmeichel et al., 2003). For instance, Muraven and
candies. There was no such effect among nondieters, colleagues (1998) made that equation in the statement,
presumably because they found the candies less tempt- “if self-regulation conforms to an energy or strength
ing and less demanding of self-regulatory resources. model, then self-control should be impaired by prior
Several studies also have linked ego-depletion to exertion” (p. 774). However, self-determination theory
self-regulation more abstractly defined. In one study (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985, 1987, 1991, 2000; Ryan, 1995)
(Baumeister et al., 1998, Study 4), participants were draws an important distinction between autonomous reg-
asked to search for and cross out each letter e in a page ulation (which is akin to what often is referred to as self-
of text, unless doing so violated one of several rules regulation) and controlled regulation (which is akin to
(e.g., do not cross off an e that is adjacent to another what is sometimes called self-control).
vowel). The rules made this a self-regulation activity Within SDT, autonomy is defined by the phenome-
because it required participants to override the impulse nological experience of an internal perceived locus of
to simply cross off every letter e they found. Participants causality (deCharms, 1968) or the self-endorsement of
in a comparison condition performed three-digit mul- one’s action. Autonomous regulation refers to regula-
tiplication problems, a task designed to be difficult and tion that is initiated and sustained by one’s integrated,
mentally taxing without requiring self-regulation. In or true, self, whereas controlled regulation encom-
phase two, participants were asked to watch a boring passes regulation by aspects of the person that are less
movie for as long as they wanted. Half of the partici- well integrated with the self. For instance, behavior
pants were told that pressing a button would stop the that stems from coercive, rigid, internalized demands
movie (i.e., an active response), whereas the other half (i.e., introjects) is an example of controlled regulation
were told that releasing a button would stop the movie (Ryan & Connell, 1989). In short, controlled regulation
(i.e., a passive response). The results showed that, rela- involves feeling pressured, coerced, or seduced into
tive to the multiplication-problem control group, par- action, whereas autonomous regulation involves doing
ticipants in the e-hunting condition stopped watching what one finds interesting or important and would be
sooner when stopping required a passive response, but inclined to do more freely.
they waited longer when stopping required an active, voli- There is also some evidence at the neurobiological
tional response. Thus, a task requiring active self-con- level that is consistent with the distinction between
trol led to the participants being passive and not actively autonomous and controlled regulation (Kuhl &
exerting themselves on a subsequent task. Fuhrmann, 1998; Ryan, Kuhl, & Deci, 1997; Walton,
In another study, Wallace and Baumeister (2002) Devlin, & Rushworth, 2004), and the empirical utility
demonstrated that working on the Stroop task, which of drawing such a distinction has been well docu-
also requires an abstract form of self-control, impaired mented in a wide variety of domains, including educa-
performance on a subsequent test of self-control relative tion (Ryan & Connell, 1989), religion (Ryan, Rigby, &
to a comparison group that did not work on the Stroop King, 1993), interpersonal relationships (La Guardia,
task. These studies also ruled out self-attribution or Ryan, Couchman, & Deci, 2000), work organizations
self-efficacy as an alternative account of ego-depletion (Deci, Connell, & Ryan, 1989), sports (Frederick &
effects because success or failure feedback following Ryan, 1995), and health care (Williams, 2002).
the initial Stroop task did not influence subsequent
performance. Furthermore, several studies have now Autonomous Regulation and Energy
demonstrated that, in general, the suppression of In contrast to the ego-depletion prediction, Ryan
thoughts can cause ego-depletion (Muraven et al., and colleagues have established a positive link between
1998). For instance, in two studies suppressing the autonomous or volitional self-regulation and the state
thought of a white bear (Wegner, Schneider, Carter, & of subjective vitality (Ryan & Frederick, 1997), which is
White, 1987) led to a tendency to give up more quickly a “positive feeling of having energy available to the self”
on unsolvable anagrams (Study 2) and impaired efforts (Nix, Ryan, Manly, & Deci, 1999, p. 266). For example,
to control the expression of amusement and enjoyment when patients at a pain clinic felt more autonomous
(Study 3). with regard to seeking treatment (i.e., had more internal
Autonomous Regulation Versus Controlled Regulation reasons), they also reported greater subjective vitality
(Ryan & Frederick, 1997, Study 4), and when morbidly
Throughout the ego-depletion literature, the terms obese patients reported more internal (autonomous)
self-regulation and self-control are used interchangeably reasons for entering a weight-loss program, they
(Baumeister et al., 1998; Muraven & Baumeister, 2000; reported higher levels of vitality at a 2-year follow-up
Muraven et al., 1998; Schmeichel & Baumeister, 2004; (Study 5). In this later study, self-reported vitality also

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF LOUISVILLE on January 18, 2015


1026 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

was linked to an objective measure of behavioral so participants in the two conditions worked on the
energization, assessed as decreases in body mass index. same puzzles for the same amounts of time. The results
In yet another population, Kasser and Ryan (1999) of this study demonstrated that choice participants spent
demonstrated that the experience of autonomy versus more time working on the task during the behavioral
control among nursing home residents positively related free-choice period and reported more intrinsic motiva-
to greater ongoing subjective vitality, as well as to less tion, relative to comparison participants. In short, offer-
mortality, at a 1-year follow-up. In contrast, these studies ing people an optimal amount of choice enhanced
further showed that people who displayed controlled their intrinsic motivation and energy to persist.
regulation tended to show less vitality and persistence. The experience of choice also has been associated
The positive relation between autonomous self- with facilitating the process of internalization, thereby
regulation and subjective vitality also has been docu- enhancing autonomous self-regulation and the vitality
mented experimentally. In three studies, Nix et al. (1999) that accompanies it. In fact, even using language that
manipulated autonomous versus controlled motivation conveys choice (e.g., can, may, could) rather than con-
and each time found that autonomous motivation led trol (e.g., should, must, have to) has been found to
to greater subjective vitality, further suggesting that enhance autonomous motivation for an activity (Deci,
autonomous self-regulation should not be depleting. Eghrari, Patrick, & Leone, 1994; Ryan, Connell, & Deci,
Autonomous regulation has not only been associated 1985; Vansteenkiste, Simons, Lens, Sheldon, & Deci,
with the subjective experience of energization (i.e., sub- 2004). Thus, whereas the ego-depletion model suggests
jective vitality) but also with the energization of behav- that all acts of self-regulation or self-control, including
ior, both in the lab and in real-world settings. As already the exercise of choice, will tend to deplete energy for
mentioned, autonomous regulation has been related subsequent volition and action, the SDT perspective
not only to vitality but also to weight loss and seeking suggests that if the regulation is autonomous rather
treatment for pain, and it has consistently been related than controlled, it will not be depleting.
to increased persistence at various activities. For example,
a study of persistence among high school students con- Choice and Energy: A Paradox
ducted by Vallerand, Fortier, and Guay (1997) found According to the Baumeister et al. (1998) model,
that autonomous regulation predicted decreased likeli- because making choices represents one kind of self-
hood of students dropping out. Pelletier, Fortier, regulation, making choices would necessarily result in
Vallerand, and Brière (2001) found that elite swim- ego-depletion. To our knowledge, the lone published
mers’ autonomous regulation predicted greater persis- study directly investigating the influence of choice on
tence (i.e., less attrition from the team) at 22-month ego-depletion was conducted by Baumeister et al.
follow-up. (1998, Study 2). In this study, choice was manipulated
In addition to enhanced persistence, the experi- in relation to having participants select which side of a
ence of autonomy has been linked to greater effort debate they would later endorse in a persuasive speech.
and goal attainment. Sheldon and Elliot (1998) found In the low-choice condition, participants were told that
that the autonomy of personal goals predicted goal they had been assigned to make one of the two
attainment among college students and that this effect speeches because the researchers already had enough
was mediated by improved effort (i.e., greater amounts people making the other speech, “so it would not be
of time spent working on particular goals). possible to give the participant a choice as to which
Choice and Energy speech to make” (p. 1257). In contrast, participants in
the condition labeled high choice “were told that the
Experiments have suggested that manipulations decision of which speech to make was entirely up to
designed to enhance one’s experience of autonomy them [however] . . . because there were already enough
can boost intrinsic motivation and energize behavior participants in one of the groups, it would help the
(Simon & McCarthy, 1982; Swann & Pittman, 1977; study a great deal if they chose” the other speech topic
Zuckerman, Porac, Lathin, Smith, & Deci, 1978). In the (p. 1257). Half were given each of the two sides. It was
study by Zuckerman and colleagues, participants in a assumed that all students were against the proposed
choice condition were shown six puzzle configurations tuition increase, so those asked to take that side were
and were allowed to select on which three of the six assumed to be in a proattitudinal condition, and those
they would work. They also were allowed to apportion a asked to take the other side were assumed to be in a
total of 30 min of puzzle-solving time among the three counterattitudinal condition.
puzzles. Participants in a no-choice comparison condi- Of interest, all participants in these two high-choice
tion were yoked to participants in the choice condition, conditions (proattitudinal and counterattitudinal) agreed

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF LOUISVILLE on January 18, 2015


Moller et al. / CHOICE AND EGO-DEPLETION 1027

to make the speech they had been asked to take. In a an internal perceived locus of causality). Of course it is
fourth, no speech condition, there was no mention of possible that a person who did what the experimenter
the speech or of a choice about what to do. In the requested would have experienced autonomous choice,
second phase of the experiment, participants were but we would argue that the very fact that every partici-
asked to work on an unsolvable, figure-tracing, puzzle pant chose what the experimenter requested indicates
activity. The results of the experiment indicated that, that participants probably felt compelled to do what
relative to the low-choice and no choice conditions, was suggested. This, then, would be considered a con-
participants given high-choice persisted for less time trolled choice that would be expected to deplete par-
and made fewer attempts at solving puzzles before ticipants’ energy and vitality. The critical difference
quitting, regardless of whether this illusion of choice between this and the choice in the Zuckerman et al.
manipulation had them assigned to a proattitudinal (1978) study was that in the latter there were no
or counterattitudinal position. The researchers con- requests, suggestions, or pressures to select a particular
cluded that “making a meaningful personal choice” is puzzle. As such, participants would likely have experi-
ego-depleting. enced an internal perceived locus of causality with
respect to their selection.
Autonomous Choice Versus Controlled Choice There are two other noteworthy differences between
We suggest that these seemingly contradictory find- the Zuckerman et al. (1978) study and the Baumeister
ings result primarily from an undifferentiated con- et al. (1998) study. Zuckerman and colleagues consid-
ceptualization of self-regulation and choice in the ered the effects of choice on intrinsic motivation for
ego-depletion literature. We have argued for the con- the interesting activity about which people were given
ceptual importance and empirical utility of distinguish- choice. In contrast, Baumeister and colleagues looked
ing between autonomous and controlled forms of at the effects of choice related to one activity on the
regulation, with the latter being energy depleting but energy for a different, uninteresting activity. Thus,
the former not being. We now suggest that the concept the first difference is that the dependent measure in
of “choice” or “decision” requires a similarly precise or the ego-depletion studies was not intrinsic motivation
differentiated definition that pays greater attention to because the persistence activities were not interesting.
participants’ phenomenological experience. The second difference was that the dependent measure
In the Zuckerman et al. (1978) study, participants assessed energy for a task different from the one about
were given an unrestricted choice of three out of six which people had choice.
puzzle problems, with no cues about which ones to Present Investigations
select. We herein refer to that as autonomous choice.
However, in the Baumeister et al. (1998) study, partici- Three experiments were conducted in an attempt to
pants were subtly pressured to choose one of the reconcile the seeming discrepancies between the SDT
options. We call that controlled choice because people and self-regulatory strength model positions on the
may feel compelled to select that option so they will not relation of choice to psychological energy. These exper-
appear unhelpful to the experimenter. In fact, a very iments follow up on Baumeister et al. (1998, Study 2)
similar manipulation was found to lead to an external using concepts from SDT to moderate the ego-deple-
perceived locus of causality by Pittman, Davey, Alafat, tion effect. In each of these studies, we used both an
Wehterill, and Kramer (1980). Just as the regulation of autonomous-choice condition and a condition similar
behavior can be controlled by introjects or external to the one used by Baumeister and colleagues, which
contingencies, so too can people’s decisions. One we refer to as controlled choice. We intended the
might argue that in such a situation, the people make a autonomous-choice condition to facilitate the experi-
behavioral selection or decision, but they may not ence of volition and choice. Our expectation was that
experience a true sense of autonomy or choice. the autonomous-choice condition would be vitalizing
Simply stated, the objective presence of multiple relative to the controlled-choice condition, which we
options from which to select is not a sufficient condi- expect to be depleting. Thus, we hypothesized that we
tion for the experience of autonomy or choice as would replicate the ego-depletion effect for the con-
defined by SDT. For example, in the Baumeister et al. trolled-choice condition but that the autonomous-
(1998) experiment, people were told they were free to choice condition would not lead to depletion relative to
choose between two options, but at the same time they the no-choice comparison condition and would result
were being subtly pressured to select one of two. We in greater energy and persistence than the controlled-
suggest that this was probably not experienced as a choice condition. In Experiment 3, we examined
true, or autonomous, choice (i.e., one accompanied by whether the relation between the experimental choice

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF LOUISVILLE on January 18, 2015


1028 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

conditions and ego-depletion would be mediated by self- controlled-choice condition were given a procedure
reported experiences of autonomy or self-determination. scripted directly from the Baumeister et al. method sec-
tion for the condition they labeled “high choice.” Our
participants in this condition, which we called con-
EXPERIMENT 1 trolled choice, were yoked to the option chosen by the
participant in the autonomous-choice condition who
The first experiment was virtually a direct replication immediately preceded them. Again, controlled-choice
of Baumeister and colleagues’ (1998, Study 2) study participants were initially told that the decision was
on choice and ego-depletion, using the same choice entirely up to them, but then the experimenter
manipulation activity (i.e., a decision relating to speech explained that because there were already enough par-
topics) and the identical dependent measure of ego- ticipants in one of the groups, it would help the study a
depletion (i.e., persistence at an unsolvable puzzle), with great deal if they would choose to read one folder
the addition of a key autonomous-choice condition. In rather than the other. The experimenter then stressed
this study, we contrasted an autonomous-choice condition again that the final decision remained theirs. As in the
with the controlled-choice condition used by Baumeister original study (Baumeister et al., 1998, Study 2), all par-
and colleagues (the latter having been labeled “high ticipants chose to make the speech they had been
choice” by them). We expected a significant difference assigned. Participants in the no-choice condition also
between the two. In addition, we included a no-choice were yoked to the option chosen by the previous par-
condition comparable to that used by Baumeister and ticipant in the autonomous-choice condition. The exper-
colleagues, which we expected to fall between the other imenter explained that the researchers already had
two conditions.1 enough people making the speech for (or against) so it
would not be possible to give the participant a choice of
Method which speech to make. Participants in each condition
expressed their choice (or acceptance of a speech) ver-
Participants. Data were collected in individual ses- bally to the experimenter and were then given the appro-
sions from 37 undergraduate college students (12 men, priate folder of materials. It took the experimenter 1 to
25 women) in exchange for extra course credit. The 2 min to convey the cover story to each participant, and
average age of participants was 19.3 years old, with a participants typically took less than 1 min to make their
range of 18 to 22 years. Each participant was randomly choice (or articulate consent).
assigned among three experimental conditions: auto- At this point, participants were presented with the
nomous choice, controlled choice, and no choice. task for the second part of the experiment. The exper-
Procedure. Participants signed up for a study on per- imenter explained that there was some evidence for a
suasion and problem solving. The experimenter greeted link between persuasiveness and problem-solving abili-
each participant and explained that the purpose of the ties. Accordingly, the next part of the experiment
study was to see how people responded to persuasion. would contain a measure of problem-solving ability.
Participants were told that they would be making stimuli Participants were told that the problem-solving activity
that would be played to other people to try to alter the would precede the speech.
others’ attitudes. In particular, they would be making an The problem-solving task was the same one that had
audiotaped recording of a persuasive speech regarding been used by Baumeister et al. (1998, Studies 1 & 2).
whether psychology should be taught at the high school The puzzle requires the person to trace a geometric
level. The following cover story was borrowed directly figure without retracing any lines and without lifting his
from the procedures employed by Baumeister and col- or her pencil from the paper. Multiple slips of paper
leagues (1998, Study 2). The topic of the debate was were provided for each figure, and participants were
changed from the one used by Baumeister et al. so par- instructed to use one slip of paper per attempt. Each
ticipants would be likely to vary in terms of which side participant was initially given a solvable practice figure
was consistent with their attitudes. to learn how the puzzles worked, with the experimenter
The experimenter then showed the participants two present to answer any questions. After the practice period,
folders, labeled “for psychology in high school” and the experimenter gave the participant the test figure
“against psychology in high school.” Participants in the with the following instructions:
autonomous-choice condition were told that it was
entirely up to them which side of the debate they would You can take as much time and as many trials as you
choose to argue so they could take their time to think want. You will not be judged on the number of trials or
about it and then make a choice. Participants in the the time you take. You will be judged on whether or not

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF LOUISVILLE on January 18, 2015


Moller et al. / CHOICE AND EGO-DEPLETION 1029

you finish tracing the figure. If you wish to stop before TABLE 1: Means and Standard Deviations for the Two Measures
you finish (i.e., before you solve the puzzle), ring the of Persistence on Unsolvable Puzzles in the Three
bell on the table. Experimental Conditions (Experiment 1)

Condition Time (s) Attempts


Unbeknownst to the participant, the test figure had
Autonomous choice (n = 12)
been prepared so as to be impossible to solve.
M 1440.42 46.83
The experimenter then left the room and timed how SD 154.01 5.92
long the participant worked on the task before giving No choice (n = 12)
up (signified by ringing the bell). Thirty minutes was M 1278.00 35.00
set as the maximum time, and the 9 participants who SD 154.01 5.92
Controlled choice (n = 13)
were still working at 30 min were stopped by the exper-
M 896.39 19.15
imenter at that point.2 For the rest, when the experi- SD 147.97 5.69
menter heard the bell, he reentered the room and
administered the Brief Mood Inspection Scale (BMIS; NOTE: Higher numbers indicate greater persistence.
Mayer & Gaschke, 1988) and the Intrinsic Motivation
Inventory (IMI; Ryan, 1982). This was used to ensure
that any ego-depletion effects were not a function of
changes in affect or intrinsic motivation. When the par- persisted for significantly longer than did participants
ticipants finished, the experimenter debriefed, in the controlled-choice condition, F(1, 35) = 6.69,
thanked, and dismissed them. p = .02. Also as expected, the no-choice condition
was midway between the autonomous-choice and
Measures. The BMIS (Mayer & Gaschke, 1988) is a controlled-choice conditions. The autonomous-choice
mood-adjective scale with an item sample of 16 adjec- condition did not differ significantly from the no-
tives, 2 selected from each of the following eight mood choice condition, F(1, 35) = .48, p = .49, but the con-
states: (a) happy, (b) loving, (c) calm, (d) energetic, trolled-choice condition was marginally significantly
(e) fearful/anxious, (f) angry, (g) tired, and (h) sad. lower than the no-choice condition, F(1, 35) = 3.12,
Participants rated the degree to which they felt the p < .09, thus nearly replicating the Baumeister et al.
affect in each item on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (1998, Study 2) finding.
(definitely did not feel) to 4 (definitely felt). Mayer and As in the Baumeister et al. (1998) study, we also used
Gaschke (1988) identified two subscales that emerged a second dependent measure, namely, the number
as unrotated factors: pleasant-unpleasant (α = .86) and of attempts made before giving up (i.e., the number
arousal-calm (α = .63). This measure was included to of pieces of paper used). A one-way ANOVA again
confirm that any effects on energy resulting from the revealed significant variation among the three condi-
manipulation were not mediated by mood. tions, F(2, 34) = 5.74, p < .01. The pattern of results was
The IMI (Ryan, 1982) measures dimensions related essentially the same as with duration of persistence, as
to intrinsic motivation. The interest/enjoyment sub- can be seen in Table 1. Pairwise comparisons among the
scale of the IMI consists of four items that most closely groups indicated that participants in the autonomous-
represent the experience of intrinsic motivation and choice condition persisted for significantly longer than
thus were used in the current study to determine did participants in the controlled-choice condition,
whether any effects found were a function of changes in F(1, 35) = 11.72, p < .01. The no-choice condition did
intrinsic motivation for the activity used to measure not differ significantly from the autonomous-choice
ego-depletion. Participants rated the degree to which condition, F(1, 35) = 1.61, p = .21, but again it was mar-
they found the unsolvable problem interesting using a ginally different from the controlled-choice condition,
7-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 7 (very F (1, 35) = 3.35, p = .08.
true) (α for the current sample = .88).
Mood. As in many studies on ego-depletion, we
Results assessed mood to rule it out as a mediator. The BMIS
mood measure contained two subscales (pleasant-
Persistence. The first dependent measure was the unpleasant and arousal-calm). A MANOVA was con-
amount of time participants spent on the unsolvable ducted using condition as the independent variable
puzzles. A one-way ANOVA indicated significant varia- and the two BMIS subscales as dependent measures.
tion among the three conditions, F(2, 34) = 3.45, p < The overall effect was nonsignificant using Wilks’s
.05.3 The means are presented in Table 1. As predicted, lambda, F(4, 66) = 0.15, p = .96, and neither of the sub-
pairwise comparisons among the groups indicated scales approached significance, all Fs < 1.0. Thus, affect
that participants in the autonomous-choice condition did not mediate the results on ego strength.

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF LOUISVILLE on January 18, 2015


1030 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

Intrinsic motivation. We also assessed intrinsic motivation they were ready to quit. This hand-raising element
(i.e., interest/enjoyment for the activity) to rule it out effectively changed the decision to quit from an active
as a mediator of ego-strength effects. Specifically, this one (i.e., reaching out to ring a bell) to a passive one
measure was intended to differentiate persistence that (i.e., letting their hand fall) because Baumeister and
reflects self-regulatory strength from persistence that colleagues (1998, Study 2) found that ego-depletion
reflects intrinsic motivation. An ANOVA was conducted made people more prone to desist when the termina-
using condition as the independent variable and inter- tion behavior was passive rather than active. Using
est as the dependent variable. The effect was not signif- the passive termination and the physical-exertion task
icant, F(2, 34) = 0.71, p = .50, indicating that intrinsic was intended to increase generalizability of the results
motivation on the persistence activity was not affected and allow an examination of whether autonomous ver-
by choice on the initial activity. sus controlled choice would affect a physical-exertion
behavior.
Discussion
Method
The main hypothesis tested in Experiment 1 was sup-
ported. Participants in the autonomous-choice condi- Participants. Data were collected in individual ses-
tion persisted longer and made a greater number of sions from 25 undergraduates (9 men, 16 women) who
attempts relative to those in the controlled-choice con- received extra course credit. They averaged 19.3 years
dition, with those in the no-choice condition falling in old, with a range of 18 to 23. Each participant was ran-
between. The controlled-choice condition was the same domly assigned to one of two experimental conditions:
as the one that Baumeister et al. (1998, Study 2) had autonomous choice or controlled choice.
called the high-choice condition. Thus, the fact that
participants in the controlled-choice condition per- Procedure. Participants signed up for a study on “cog-
sisted for marginally less time and made marginally nitive exercise.” The experimenter greeted each partic-
fewer attempts relative to the no-choice comparison ipant and explained that this study was probably
condition essentially replicated the ego-depletion effect different from other studies being run on campus
reported by Baumeister and colleagues. But the fact because the participants would be given some choice
that the autonomous-choice condition led to signifi- about what they do in the second phase of the experi-
cantly greater persistence than the controlled-choice ment. Participants were then presented with a form
conditions indicates that some choices are not deplet- inviting them to choose between several potential activ-
ing. Both mood and intrinsic motivation were ruled out ities (i.e., either watching a film or listening to music
as mediators of ego-depletion. and then either writing about it or talking about it).
Each participant in the controlled-choice condition
was yoked to the choices made by the previous partici-
EXPERIMENT 2
pant in the autonomous-choice condition. Again, par-
ticipants were initially told that the decision was entirely
In Experiment 2, we extended this work by replicat- up to them. Then, the experimenter explained that
ing the autonomous-choice versus controlled-choice because there were already enough participants in one
results reported in Experiment 1 using a different task of the groups, it would help the study a great deal if
to increase generalizability and to show the effect with they chose a particular set of activities. The experi-
a physical indicator of energy. Again, the choice manip- menter then again stressed that the final decision
ulation related to an activity that participants believed would remain entirely up to them. As in Study 1, par-
would take place in a second phase of the experiment. ticipants in each condition expressed their choice ver-
This time, participants made choices related to what bally to the experimenter, and the timing was about the
activities they would like to engage in later. Again, per- same as in Study 1. As in the original study (Baumeister
sistence was used as a dependent measure of ego-deple- et al., 1998, Study 2), all controlled-choice participants
tion. However, to increase generalizability, we made the selected the activities they were subtly pressured to do.
following changes. First, we used a solvable, yet tedious, At this point, participants began the task that con-
persistence activity similar to some that have been used tained the dependent measure of ego-depletion.
for measuring ego-depletion in the past. Second, we Participants were presented with two sheets of paper,
instructed participants to raise their nonwriting hands each covered with a relatively dense matrix of letters
above their heads while working on the task to make it and symbols, and they were asked to search for differ-
a physically demanding task similar to the handgrip ences between the two pages. Participants were instructed
task used by Muraven et al. (1998). Participants were to keep their nonwriting hand raised clearly above their
instructed to drop their hand back onto the desk when head while working on the exercise. They were asked to

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF LOUISVILLE on January 18, 2015


Moller et al. / CHOICE AND EGO-DEPLETION 1031

TABLE 2: Means and Standard Deviations for the Two Measures Intrinsic motivation. An independent-samples t test
of Persistence on the Solvable Difference-Finding was conducted using condition as the independent vari-
Activity in the Two Experimental Conditions
(Experiment 2)
able and interest/enjoyment as the dependent variable.
The effect was nonsignificant, t(22) = 0.76, p = .46, indi-
Condition Time (s) Task score cating that intrinsic motivation on the persistence activ-
Autonomous choice (n = 13)
ity was not affected by choice on the initial activity.
M 545.58 27.83
SD 62.33 3.51
Discussion
Controlled choice (n = 12)
M 316.08 15.31
Experiment 2 replicated the critical difference
SD 59.89 3.38 observed in Experiment 1 using an alternative manipu-
lation of choice and an alternative measure of ego-
depletion that included objective performance and
physically demanding persistence. Again, participants
find as many differences as they could, but they were in the controlled-choice condition were significantly
told that whenever they were ready to quit they could ego-depleted (i.e., persisted less and performed worse)
simply drop their hand. The experimenter then relative to those in the autonomous-choice condition.
covertly started a stopwatch to measure the duration of The results support our hypothesis that different kinds
persistence. Fifteen minutes was set as the maximum of choice (autonomous vs. controlled) must be consid-
time, and the 2 participants who were still working at 15 ered with regard to understanding the relation between
min were stopped by the experimenter at that point. choice and ego-depletion.
Following this behavioral measure of ego-depletion, the
experimenter administered the BMIS and IMI. When EXPERIMENT 3
participants finished, the experimenter debriefed,
thanked, and dismissed them. In Experiment 3, we extended this work by repli-
Measures. The BMIS (Mayer & Gaschke, 1988) and cating the results reported in Experiments 1 and 2,
the interest/enjoyment subscale of the IMI (Ryan, with the addition of an important manipulation check
1982) were used in this experiment as they had been in and test of mediation. Again, we focused on the
Experiment 1 to ascertain whether either mood or autonomous-choice and controlled-choice conditions
intrinsic motivation mediated ego-strength effects. to further illustrate this conceptual distinction. The
choice manipulation was identical to that used in
Results Experiment 2. Participants made two decisions related
to which activities they would like to engage in later.
Persistence. The primary dependent measure was the Again, performance and persistence were used as the
amount of time participants spent on the difference- dependent measures of ego-depletion.
finding exercise while keeping their hand raised. An One potential critique of the dependent measure
independent-samples t test indicated that the autonomous- used in Experiment 2 is that keeping one’s hand raised
choice condition persisted significantly longer than those could have been affected by individual differences in
in the controlled-choice condition, t(23) = 7.05, p < .05. physical strength, so in this experiment, we used a pas-
The means are presented in Table 2. Performance scores sive response that involved relatively little physical
on the search task were calculated by subtracting the strength. Participants were asked to press the spacebar
number of differences incorrectly identified from the on a computer keyboard and keep the bar pressed
number correctly identified. An independent-samples down until they were ready to quit working. This
t test indicated that the mean for the autonomous-choice retained a passive response to quit while eliminating
condition was significantly higher than that for the the error variance contributed by variation in physical
controlled-choice condition, t(23) = 6.61, p < .05. strength. An additional advantage of this approach was
Mood. A MANOVA was conducted using condition as that time on task could be measured via the computer.
the independent variable and the two BMIS subscales Second, to increase generalizability, we used a third
as dependent measures. Neither the overall effect nor task for obtaining the dependent measures. It was a search
the individual effects approached significance. The task similar to the one used in Experiment 2, but
overall effect using Wilks’s lambda was nonsignificant, because the previous search task could be accomplished
F(2, 19) = .39, p = .68, and for subscales, all Fs were less with an algorithmic strategy, we used one in this exper-
than 1.0, indicating that ego-depletion effects were not iment that required more complex cognitive activity.
a function of mood. Specifically, we adapted a task used by Baumeister and

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF LOUISVILLE on January 18, 2015


1032 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

colleagues (1998, Study 4) that involves searching a page TABLE 3: Means and Standard Deviations for the Performance
of letters but using more complex problem solving and Persistence Measures on the e-Hunting Activity in
the Two Experimental Conditions (Experiment 3)
when doing so.
Condition Time (s) Task Score
Method
Autonomous choice (n = 15)
Participants. Data were collected in individual ses- M 878.2 210.3
SD 50.2 43.3
sions from 33 undergraduates (6 men, 27 women) who Controlled choice (n = 15)
received extra course credit. They averaged 19.5 years M 685.7 153.47
old, with a range of 18 to 22. Each participant was ran- SD 50.2 69.0
domly assigned to autonomous choice or controlled
choice.
In this sample, 3 participants in the controlled-choice
condition selected activities other than those suggested
the rules. Alternatively, someone could avoid making
by the experimenter. We reclassified these 3 as “resisters”
mistakes by simply circling very few es. For these rea-
and ran an additional 3 “compliant” participants in the
sons, performance scores on the e-hunting task were
controlled-choice condition. All analyses reported were
calculated by subtracting the number of false alarms
run after dropping these 3 resisters and using a sample
(incorrectly crossed off es) from the number of hits
of 30 (i.e., 15 per cell).
(correctly crossed off es).
Procedure. Participants signed up for a study on “cog- Following the behavioral measure of ego-depletion,
nitive exercise”. The experimenter greeted participants the experimenter administered three questionnaires.
individually, obtained consent, and explained that the First, participants completed the BMIS (Mayer &
participants would be given a choice among several Gaschke, 1988) and the IMI (Ryan, 1982), as in
activities that they could do in the second phase of the Experiments 1 and 2. Then, we reminded participants
experiment (i.e., either watching a film or listening to of the choices they made earlier in the session and
music and then either writing about it or talking about assessed their experience of self-determination with
it). Participants were then presented with a form and regard to these choices (a variable used as a manipula-
invited to choose. tion check and potential mediator). When participants
Participants in the controlled-choice condition were finished, the experimenter debriefed, thanked, and dis-
again yoked to the choices made by the previous par- missed them.
ticipant in the autonomous-choice condition. Again,
Measures. The BMIS (Mayer & Gaschke, 1988) and the
participants were initially told that the decision was
interest/enjoyment subscale of the IMI (Ryan, 1982)
entirely up to them. Then, the experimenter explained
again were used and are described in Experiment 1.
that because there were already enough participants in
The nine-item self-report measure of self-determination
one of the groups, it would help the study a great deal
was adapted from Reeve, Nix, and Hamm (2003). State-
if they chose a particular set of activities. The experi-
ments were rated on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging
menter again stressed that the final decision was
from 1 (not at all true) to 7 (very true). The scale includes
entirely up to them.
the following three subscales: perceived locus of causality,
At this point, participants were presented with the
volition, and perceived choice, which combine for a total
activity that included the dependent measure of ego-
self-determination score (α = .93).
depletion (adapted from Baumeister et al., 1998, Study
4). For this task, participants were presented with two Results
sheets of printed text taken from an introductory statis-
tics textbook. They were asked to search for the letter e Ego-depletion was measured in two ways: (a) time
and to cross it out, unless doing so violated any of three of persistence and (b) performance on the e-hunting
rules. The rules were as follows: (a) do not cross out an task. Independent-samples t tests indicated that the
e if it is adjacent to another vowel, (b) do not cross out autonomous-choice participants persisted significantly
an e if it is the first letter of a word, and (c) do not cross longer than the controlled-choice participants, t(28) =
out an e if it is followed by two consonants in the same 2.71, p < .05 (see Table 3 for the means). On the per-
word. Participants were told that although they were formance measure, an independent-samples t test indi-
not expected to find every e, they were to find as many cated that the autonomous-choice participants also
qualifying es as they could before quitting. scored significantly higher than those in the controlled-
On the e-hunting exercise, someone could find many choice condition at the e-hunting activity, t(28) = 2.70,
qualifying es by simply crossing off every e and ignoring p < .05.4

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF LOUISVILLE on January 18, 2015


Moller et al. / CHOICE AND EGO-DEPLETION 1033

Mood. A MANOVA using condition as the indepen-


dent variable and the two BMIS subscales as outcome Choice .40* Self- .46** e-Hunt
variables showed that the overall effect was nonsignifi- Condition Determination Performance
cant using Wilks’s lambda, F(2, 27) = 0.44, p = .65, and
none of the subscales approached significance, Fs < 1.0,
.27 (.46**)
again indicating that mood did not explain persistence
or performance results.
Figure 1 Mediation of ego-depletion effect by self-determination
Intrinsic motivation. An independent-samples t test (Experiment 3).
using condition as the independent variable and NOTE: In the figure, numerical values represent standardized beta
coefficients resulting from the four-step procedure described by
interest/enjoyment for the activity as the dependent Baron and Kenny (1986) for establishing mediation. The beta value
variable also yielded a nonsignificant effect, t(28) = in parentheses refers to the direct effect between choice condition
0.73, p = .91. Again, the effects were not a function of and e-hunt performance when self-determination is not being con-
trolled for.
changes in intrinsic motivation. *p < .05. **p < .01.
Self-determination. Participants reported their experi-
ence of self-determination in relation to the choice
manipulation. An independent-samples t test indicated
that the mean of the autonomous-choice condition GENERAL DISCUSSION
(M = 50.20) was significantly higher on self-determination
with regard to the choice manipulation than was the Three studies were designed to test and refute an
mean of the controlled-choice condition (M = 41.53), assertion made by Baumeister and colleagues (1998)
t(28) = 2.27, p < .05. that “all acts of volition” (p. 1253) are ego-depleting.
Mediation. Next, we tested whether perceived self- Several previous studies had isolated processes that
determination would mediate the relation between limit or counteract the ego-depletion phenomenon,
condition and ego-depletion, first done with the e- including positive affect (Baumeister, Dale, & Tice,
hunt score and then with persistence. Baron and 1998), self-regulatory practice (Muraven, Baumeister, &
Kenny (1986) presented four steps for establishing Tice, 1999), rest and sleep (Baumeister, Muraven, &
mediation. Steps 1 and 2 involve showing that the Tice, 2000), challenging expectations (Martin,
independent variable (i.e., choice condition) is Tenbuelt, Merckelbach, Dreezens, & de Vries, 2002),
related to the outcome (i.e., e-hunt score) and show- task motivation (Muraven & Slessareva, 2003), imple-
ing that the independent variable is related to the mentation intentions (Webb & Sheeran, 2004), and
mediator (i.e., self-determination). These effects were visualizing an energizing significant other (Knowles &
confirmed and reported above. Step 3 requires that Finkel, 2005). The current studies isolated autonomy as
the mediator affect the outcome variable, controlling a moderator of ego-depletion.
for the independent variable. A regression analysis The current studies also went beyond the previous
including both reported self-determination and con- ones in that they not only specified an antidote to ego-
dition confirmed that self-determination did signifi- depletion but they suggested that the strength-model
cantly predict the e-hunt score, β = .46, t(28) = 2.81, p conceptualization does not apply to all types of self-
< .01. The final step for establishing mediation looks regulation. Specifically, SDT emphasizes that there are
at the relation between the initial predictor variable multiple ways of regulating one’s self and that these reg-
and the outcome, controlling for the mediator. If this ulatory approaches have very different relations to psy-
effect drops to zero, then there is full mediation; if it chological energy and vitality. Specifically, controlled
drops significantly (Sobel, 1982), then there is partial regulation (sometimes called self-control) is not equiv-
mediation. The effect of condition on e-hunt score alent to autonomous self-regulation in terms of its
dropped from significant (β = .46) to nonsignificant, effects. SDT agrees that controlled regulation will be
β = .27, t(28) = 1.66, p = .11. A Sobel test confirmed depleting but it maintains that autonomous regulation
the significance of this mediation, z = 2.27, p < .05 (see will not. Indeed, in each of these current studies, and in
Figure 1). line with past research, there was a significant differ-
With regard to the persistence measure, Steps 1 ence in the level of energy, vitality, and persistence in
and 2 held (as reported above). However, when a the conditions representing autonomous choice
regression analysis was run including both reported (prompting autonomous regulation) versus controlled
self-determination and condition, self-determination did choice (prompting controlled regulation).
not significantly predict persistence, β = .25, t(28) = Thus, we maintain that Baumeister et al.’s (1998) claim
1.36, p = .19. that all acts of self-regulation or choice will deplete

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF LOUISVILLE on January 18, 2015


1034 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

inner resources and energy is insufficiently differentiated. incorrectly identified as differences). Experiment 3
Controlled regulation, which involves inner conflict— further extended this research by replicating the gen-
with one part of the person pressuring or controlling eral effect using yet another dependent measure of
another part—is indeed depleting. However, we argue ego-depletion. The general effect was again replicated,
that autonomous regulation is a prototypic example of such that those in the autonomous-choice condition
volition that is not depleting. With respect to making were less ego-depleted than those in the controlled-
decisions or choices, we suggest that the so-called high- choice condition, persisting longer and performing
choice condition in the Baumeister et al. (1998) research better on the e-hunting task. Collectively, these studies
was not an instance of autonomous choice. Although provide strong support for recognizing the distinction
participants were told that the choice was theirs, they between autonomous and controlled choice and their
were subtly pressured to select one of the options, and differential relations with ego-depletion.
indeed, every participant did make that selection. In all three experiments, we ruled out affect and
Presumably, the participants in that condition would intrinsic motivation as possible mediators. Then, in
have felt controlled and perceived an external locus Experiment 3, self-reported self-determination was
of causality (Pittman et al., 1980). In short, the key to examined as a potential mediator and was found to
making sense of this apparent contradiction involves an mediate the effect of autonomous versus controlled
appreciation for the distinction between autonomous choice on performance on the e-hunting task. Self-
choice and controlled choice (Deci & Ryan, 1985). determination failed to significantly mediate the effect
Both involve selecting among options, but autonomous of choice on persistence, perhaps because persistence is
choice is accompanied by the experience of volition, not as precise a measure of focused engagement with
whereas controlled choice, which involves selecting an the task (of ego-energy) as is quality of performance.
option under pressure, is accompanied by the experience Specifically, persistence does not distinguish whether
of control. In other words, choices that are accompa- people are seriously engaged with the task or are casu-
nied by demands or obligations involve a very different ally engaged with it while daydreaming. An alternative
phenomenological experience from those that simply possibility for the lack of significant mediation of the
offer opportunities. choice-persistence relations is that self-determination is
In three experiments, we illustrated the importance not sufficiently proximal to persistence. For example,
of this distinction for understanding the relation perhaps people who experience self-determination
between making choices and ego-depletion. For the would feel greater vitality (Ryan & Frederick, 1997),
first experiment, we duplicated two conditions that had which would then be more strongly related to persis-
been used by Baumeister and colleagues (1998) in the tence. Additional studies of mediation will be required
one published experiment suggesting that making a to elucidate this issue further.
choice would be depleting. The effect reported by The current studies demonstrated that making
Baumeister and colleagues was essentially replicated, choices is not always ego-depleting. When people expe-
but the study also demonstrated that conditions repre- rienced a sense of autonomy with regard to the choice,
senting autonomous choice are quite different from their energy for subsequent tasks was not diminished.
those representing controlled choice, which Baumeister An important question that deserved empirical atten-
and colleagues had labeled “high choice.” In our first tion concerns the potential for autonomous choice to
study, participants in the autonomous-choice condition vitalize or enhance self-regulatory strength for subse-
persisted longer and made more attempts at puzzle quent tasks. What, for example, are the conditions that
solving than did those in the controlled-choice condi- will lead autonomous choice to enhance people’s moti-
tion, and there was no evidence of ego-depletion for vation for new tasks? We suggest that among the factors
the autonomous-choice condition relative to a no- that are likely to affect whether choice will be vitalizing
choice comparison condition. Experiment 2 focused is the nature of the options being provided to the
on the key distinction (viz., autonomous choice vs. con- person. If a person is offered choice among options
trolled choice) using a new choice situation that provided that he or she does not value, that are trivial or irrele-
more options and a new measure of ego-depletion vant, the choice is unlikely to be vitalizing and may be
involving physical exertion. Again, those in the depleting, even if there is no subtle pressure toward a
autonomous-choice condition persisted longer at a dif- particular option. On the other hand, having autonomous
ficult activity relative to those in the controlled-choice choice among options that do have personal value may
condition. In addition, in Experiment 2, participants indeed be quite energizing.
in the autonomous-choice condition scored higher on We have framed this article in terms of understanding
an objective measure of performance (i.e., number of choice as both a phenomenological aspect of autonomous
differences found minus number of nondifferences self-regulation and as a description of situations that

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF LOUISVILLE on January 18, 2015


Moller et al. / CHOICE AND EGO-DEPLETION 1035

involve selecting among options that can vary in whether Baumeister, R. F., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (2000). Ego-depletion:
A resource model of volition, self-regulation, and controlled pro-
there are pressures toward one of the options.5 We used cessing. Social Cognition, 18, 130-150.
the terms “autonomous choice” and “controlled choice” Carmon, Z., Wertenbroch, K., & Zeelenberg, M. (2003). Option
to covey the absence versus presence of pressure toward attachment: When deliberating makes choosing feel like losing.
Journal of Consumer Research, 30, 15-29.
an option, and we maintained that only autonomous deCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation. New York: Academic Press.
choice will allow the person to experience a sense of Deci, E. L., Connell, J. P., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Self-determination in
choice. The evidence from the three studies supported a work organization. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 580-590.
Deci, E. L., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. (1994).
our contention that controlled choice is likely to be Facilitating internalization: The self-determination theory per-
ego-depleting but that autonomous choice is not. One spective. Journal of Personality, 62, 119-142.
implication of this work is that researchers would do Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-
determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.
well to avoid making sweeping statements about the Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). The support of autonomy and the
effects of choice. The concept of choice has multiple control of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53,
meanings, and effects of choice will depend on pre- 1024-1037.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). A motivational approach to self:
cisely how the term is defined. As the current studies Integration in personality. In R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska sympo-
showed, it is important to distinguish between auto- sium on motivation: Vol. 38. Perspectives on motivation (pp. 237-288).
nomous and controlled forms of choice. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pur-
Furthermore, because autonomous choice and con- suits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior.
trolled choice are instances of the general concept of Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227-268.
self-regulation, the implications of the present research Frederick, C. M., & Ryan, R. M. (1995). Self-determination in sport:
A review using cognitive evaluation theory. International Journal of
can be extended to our understanding of self-regulation Sport Psychology, 26, 5-23.
more generally. Whereas the ego-depletion literature Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (2000). When choice is demotivating:
has to date focused on what we refer to as controlled Can one desire too much of a good thing? Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 79, 995-1006.
forms of regulation, we maintain that autonomous self- Kasser, V. G., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). The relation of psychological needs
regulation is very different and that it is important to for autonomy and relatedness to vitality, well-being, and mortality in
consider types of regulation when examining vitalizing a nursing home. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29, 935-954.
Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling.
versus ego-depleting effects in general. New York: Guilford.
Knowles, M. L., & Finkel, E. J. (2005). Pumping up with mom: The
impact of interpersonal relationships on self-regulatory capacity. Poster
NOTES
presented at the Society for Personality and Social Psychology
1. Because the no-speech condition turned out to be equivalent to Conference, New Orleans, LA.
the no-choice condition in the original study by Baumeister, Kuhl, J., & Fuhrmann, A. (1998). Decomposing self-regulation and
Bratslavsky, Muraven, and Tice (1998, Study 2), we did not run the self-control: The volitional components inventory. In J. Heckhausen
no-speech condition in the current experiment. & C. Dweck (Eds.), Lifespan perspectives on motivation and control
2. Because some participants persisted for the full time, we did a test (pp. 15-49). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
for skewness and found the skewness to be −.44, which is far below the La Guardia, J. G., Ryan, R. M., Couchman, C. E., & Deci, E. L. (2000).
cutoff score suggested by Kline (1998) of an absolute value of 3.0. In Within-person variation in security of attachment: A self-
Experiments 2 and 3, we also found skewness to be below the cutoff. determination theory perspective on attachment, need fulfill-
3. We also did a 2 × 3 ANOVA with condition crossed by gender. ment, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79,
Neither the main effect for gender nor the interaction approached sig- 367-384.
nificance. Furthermore, in Experiments 2 and 3, we also analyzed for Martin, C., Tenbuelt, P., Merckelbach, H., Dreezens, E., & de Vries, N. K.
gender and found no effects, so gender will not be considered further. (2002). Getting a grip on ourselves: Challenging expectancies
4. We repeated the t tests with data from the 3 resisters included about loss of energy after self-control. Social Cognition, 20, 441-460.
and the effects were still significant. Mayer, J. D., & Gaschke, Y. N. (1988). The experience and meta-
5. The concept of choice has received considerable attention in experience of mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55,
social psychology recently and, in contrast to our approach, many 102-111.
authors have defined choice exclusively in terms of selection among Mick, D. G., Broniarczyk, S. M., & Haidt, J. (2004). Choose, choose,
options while ignoring the subjective experience of choice (Carmon, choose, choose, choose, choose, choose: Emerging and prospec-
Wertenbroch, & Zeelenberg, 2003; Iyengar & Lepper, 2000; Mick, tive research on the deleterious effects of living in consumer
Broniarczyk, & Haidt, 2004; Schwartz, 2000, 2004). hyperchoice. Journal of Business Ethics, 52, 207-211.
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and deple-
tion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle?
REFERENCES Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247-259.
Muraven, M., Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1999). Self-control as
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator vari- limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of
able distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 774-789.
strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Muraven, M., & Slessareva, E. (2003). Mechanism of self-control fail-
Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182. ure: Motivation and limited resources. Personality and Social
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Psychology Bulletin, 29, 894-906.
Ego-depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-control as
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252-1265. a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of
Baumeister, R. F., Dale, K., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Replenishing the self: Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 774-789.
Effects of positive affect on performance and persistence following ego- Nix, G. A., Ryan, R. M., Manly, J. B., & Deci, E. L. (1999).
depletion. Unpublished manuscript. Revitalization through self-regulation: The effects of autonomous

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF LOUISVILLE on January 18, 2015


1036 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

and controlled motivation on happiness and vitality. Journal of Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1998). Not all personal goals are “per-
Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 266-284. sonal”: Comparing autonomous and controlling goals on effort and
Pelletier, L. G., Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., & Brière, N. M. (2001). attainment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 546-557.
Associations among perceived autonomy support, forms of self- Simon, T., & McCarthy, B. (1982). Choice and the enhancement of intrin-
regulation, and persistence: A prospective study. Motivation and sic motivation. Unpublished manuscript, University of Sheffield,
Emotion, 25, 279-306. England.
Pittman, T. S., Davey, M. E., Alafat, K. A., Wehterill, K. V., & Kramer, Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect
N. A. (1980). Informational versus controlling verbal rewards. effects in structural equation models. In S. Leinhardt (Ed.),
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 228-233. Sociological methodology (pp. 290-312). Washington, DC: American
Reeve, J., Nix, G., & Hamm, D. (2003). Testing models of the experi- Sociological Association.
ence of self-determination in intrinsic motivation and the conun- Swann, W. B., & Pittman, T. S. (1977). Initiating play activity of
drum of choice. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 375-392. children: The moderating influence of verbal cues on intrinsic
Ryan, R. M. (1982). Control and information in the intrapersonal motivation. Child Development, 48, 1128-1132.
sphere: An extension of cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Vallerand, R. J., Fortier, M. S., & Guay, F. (1997). Self-determination
Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 450-461. and persistence in a real-life setting: Toward a motivational model
Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation of inte- of high-school drop out. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
grative processes. Journal of Personality, 63, 397-427. 72, 1161-1176.
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K. M., & Deci, E. L.
internalization: Examining reasons for acting in two domains. (2004). Motivating learning, performance, and persistence: The
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 749-761. synergistic role of intrinsic goals and autonomy-support. Journal of
Ryan, R. M., Connell, J. P., & Deci, E. L. (1985). A motivational ana- Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 246-260.
lysis of self-determination and self-regulation in education. In C. Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2000). Self-regulatory failure: A
Ames & R. E. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in education: The resource-depletion approach. Psychological Science, 11, 249-254.
classroom milieu (pp. 13-51). New York: Academic Press. Wallace, H. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2002). The effects of success ver-
Ryan, R. M., & Frederick, C. M. (1997). On energy, personality, and sus failure feedback on self-control. Self and Identity, 1, 35-42.
health: Subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being. Walton, M. E., Devlin, J. T., & Rushworth, M.F.S. (2004). Interactions
Journal of Personality, 65, 529-205. between decision making and performance monitoring within
Ryan, R. M., Kuhl, J., & Deci, E. L. (1997). Nature and autonomy: An prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 1259-1265.
organizational view on the social and neurobiological aspects of Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2004). Identifying good opportunities to
self-regulation in behavior and development. Development and act: Implementation intentions and cue discrimination. European
Psychopathology, 9, 701-728. Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 407-419.
Ryan, R. M., Rigby, S., & King, K. (1993). Two types of religious inter- Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987).
nalization and their relations to religious orientation and mental Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality
health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 586-596. and Social Psychology, 53, 5-13.
Schmeichel, B. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2004). Self-regulatory strength. Williams, G. C. (2002). Improving patients’ health through support-
In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: ing the autonomy of patients and providers. In E. L. Deci & R. M.
Research, theory, and applications (pp. 84-98). New York: Guilford. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 233-254).
Schmeichel, B. J., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Intellectual Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
performance and ego-depletion: Role of the self in logical rea- Zuckerman, M., Porac, J. F., Lathin, D., Smith, R., & Deci, E. L. (1978).
soning and other information processing, Journal of Personality and On the importance of self-determination for intrinsically moti-
Social Psychology, 85, 33-46. vated behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, 443-446.
Schwartz, B. (2000). Self-determination: The tyranny of freedom.
American Psychologist, 55, 79-88.
Schwartz, B. (2004). The paradox of choice: Why more is less. New York: Received February 15, 2005
HarperCollins. Revision accepted January 30, 2006

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF LOUISVILLE on January 18, 2015


554914
research-article2014
PSPXXX10.1177/0146167214554914Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

Personality and Social

Corrigendum
Psychology Bulletin
2014, Vol. 40(12) 1711
© 2014 by the Society for Personality
and Social Psychology, Inc
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0146167214554914
pspb.sagepub.com

Moller, A. C., Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M. (2006). Choice and ego-depletion: The moderating role of autonomy. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(8), 1024–1036. (Original DOI: 10.1177/0146167206288008)

The above article published in Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin included incorrect standard deviations reported
in Tables 2 and 3 on pages 1031 and 1032, respectively. The significance levels reported in the article and conclusions drawn
from the authors’ findings remain unchanged. The revised tables are shown below.

Table 2.  Means and Standard Deviations for the Two Measures Table 3.  Means and Standard Deviations for the Performance
of Persistence on the Solvable Difference-Finding Activity in the and Persistence Measures on the e-Hunting Activity in the Two
Two Experimental Conditions (Experiment 2). Experimental Conditions (Experiment 3).

Condition Time (s) Task Score Condition Time (s) Task Score
Autonomous choice (n = 12) Autonomous choice (n = 15)
  M 545.58 27.83  M 878.20 210.27
  SD 254.47 13.87  SD 171.85 43.27
Controlled choice (n = 13) Controlled choice (n = 15)
  M 316.08 15.31  M 685.73 153.47
  SD 173.16 10.37  SD 214.45 68.99

You might also like