Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2003, 44, 125 –131

Gender bias in the evaluation of New Age music


Blackwell Publishing Ltd

ANN COLLEY1, ADRIAN NORTH1 and DAVID J. HARGREAVES2


1
School of Psychology, University of Leicester, UK
2
University of Surrey Roehampton, UK

Colley, A., North, A. & Hargreaves, D. J. (2003). Gender bias in the evaluation of New Age music. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 44,
125–131.
Eminent composers in Western European art music continue to be predominantly male and eminence in contemporary pop music is similarly
male dominated. One contributing factor may be the continuing under-valuation of women’s music. Possible anti-female bias in a contemporary
genre was investigated using the Goldberg paradigm to elicit judgments of New Age compositions. Since stronger stereotyping effects occur
when information provided about individuals is sparse, fictitious male and female composers were presented either by name only or by
name with a brief biography. Evidence for anti-female bias was found in the name-only condition and was stronger when liking for the
music was controlled. Other findings were the tendency for females to give higher ratings, and the association of gender differences in liking
of the music with ratings of quality in the name-only condition. These results are relevant to the design of formal assessment procedures
for musical composition.
Key words: Gender, music, stereotyping, evaluation.
Ann Colley, School of Psychology, University of Leicester, Leicester LE1 7RH, UK . E-mail: aoc@le.ac.uk

The composition of Western European art music, like many and female attributes (O’Neill & Boulton, 1996). Despite the
other areas of artistic expression, has been traditionally male greater uptake of formal training by girls, males are per-
dominated. Musicologists and music psychologists have ceived to have more ability in composition by both pupils
noted that few female classical composers have achieved and teachers (Green, 1997). Male domination in this area
eminence (Farnsworth, 1969; O’Neill, 1997). Historically, has been assisted by the introduction of music technology,
female participation in music has been inextricably linked which has opened up composition to those with less well
to gender roles, with females making music in domestic developed instrumental skills, and discouraged females, with
settings, while males occupy the public and professional their lower interest in and experience with computers and other
arena (Green, 1997). Female achievement has therefore forms of technology (Comber, Hargreaves & Colley, 1993).
received little attention, particularly in the area of composi- One important question of clear relevance to the assess-
tion. The qualities of composers like Thea Musgrave and ment of musical skill and, more generally, to the potential
Clara Schumann have now been acknowledged, but the cul- for women to receive recognition for talent as composers, is
tural and historical context in which they worked prevented whether stereotypical perceptions of compositional ability
them from achieving the recognition bestowed upon many favor males over females. Musical composition has not fea-
of their male counterparts. Prestige associated with the com- tured in studies of the evaluation of the performance of
position of music in popular genres has not been explicitly males and females, although other creative tasks, such as
studied, but it is common for performers to compose their writing (e.g. Tanner, 1977) and art (e.g. Etaugh & Sanders,
own music either as solo artists or in small groups, and 1974; Lenney, Mitchell & Browning 1983), have received
recent studies have consistently found male domination of attention. The research on performance evaluation in artistic
the rankings of performers in terms of both position and production and other areas such as vocational ability and
number appearing (North & Hargreaves, 1995, 1996). competence is difficult to digest due to variations in both
The educational system has a powerful role to play in methodology and findings.
encouraging gender equity in the development and recogni- The seminal research in this area was undertaken by
tion of musical skills. However, despite the stated position of Goldberg (1968) in a study of the evaluation by female par-
the English National Curriculum with respect to equality of ticipants of supposed journal articles in the fields of art
access for male and female pupils to all areas of the musical history, education, dietetics, linguistics, law and city plan-
curriculum (Qualifications and Curriculum Authority, ning. The same journal articles were attributed in half the
1999), it appears that gender differentiation remains in the cases to a fictitious male author (John McKay) and in the
perceptions of both pupils and teachers. Girls have con- other half to a female author (Joan McKay). The same
tinued to take up tuition in greater numbers than boys, background information was provided for both authors,
although the kinds of instruments played by the two sexes with only the forename varied. The methodology has sub-
differ such that their qualities reflect stereotypically male sequently become known as the Goldberg paradigm. The

© 2003 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations. Published by Blackwell Publishing Ltd., 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK
and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA. ISSN 0036-5564.
14679450, 2003, 2, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1467-9450.00330 by Cambridge University Library, Wiley Online Library on [16/05/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
126 A. Colley et al. Scand J Psychol 44 (2003)

overall finding has been widely reported as indicating a bias judgments, due to wide variation in the methodology of
toward evaluating the male author more positively. However, the studies used. The studies included in their meta-analysis
Swim, Borgida and Maruyama (1989) have pointed out that varied in the amount of information provided about the
the results are less clear-cut: what they show is qualified fictitious target, from a name only to a resume or even a video.
support for gender bias, particularly for articles concerning They also varied according to the stimulus material used,
masculine topics such as law and city planning. which could be articles, art, behaviors, resumes or biograph-
Paludi and colleagues extended Goldberg’s findings by ies, and by whether a person or product was the target of the
adding a sexually ambiguous name which used initials rather ratings. Top’s (1991) review of performance evaluations in
than a forename (Paludi & Bauer, 1983) and the ambiguous creative areas drew attention to the complexity of any effect
forename of Chris (Paludi & Strayer, 1985). Their findings of sex bias, and concluded that it is dependent upon charac-
indicated that male and female participants rated male teristics of the stimulus materials, the rater, the judgment
authors more favorably and female authors least favorably, situation, and the nature of the judgments required.
with the products of authors with ambiguous names receiv- One crucial variable in any study of the use of stereotypes
ing intermediate ratings. However, the nature of the articles is the nature and amount of information provided about the
proved to be a relevant factor. The participants tended to target. The literature distinguishes between stored informa-
interpret ambiguous names in terms of the gender character- tion about the characteristics of social categories, which pro-
istics of the articles, so that articles in masculine areas were duces stereotyping effects, and given information about an
attributed to males and those in feminine areas to females. individual, which may or may not be congruent with the
Lower ratings were given when the ambiguous author was information within the stored category. Where both types
believed to be female. of information are present, they interact in the forming of
In addition to the sex stereotyping of the stimulus article impressions or judgments and individuating information
used, other factors, such as status and familiarity with the can moderate or even negate the influence of stereotyping
field in question, have been found to influence bias in evalu- (Deaux & Lewis, 1984; Eagly & Wood, 1982; Locksley,
ations. A study by Pheterson, Kiesler and Goldberg (1971) Borgida, Brekke & Hepburn, 1980). However, in order to be
found that paintings by a female artist were regarded less influential the individuating information must have diag-
positively than those by males only when they were pre- nostic value for the judgment being made (Deaux & Lewis,
sented as contest entries rather than contest winners. Ward 1984; Macrae, Shepherd & Milne, 1992). This body of
(1981) found that art students were more critical of the work research suggests that stereotypes will be most likely to influ-
of a female artist than university students with no particular ence judgments when little or no relevant information is
expertise in art, who showed a slight positive bias. That status available about an individual.
influences evaluation is not particularly surprising, given the It is common in listening to music and in some assessment
literature on prestige effects (e.g. Crozier & Chapman, 1981). situations that little information is provided about a com-
The association of greater bias with greater expertise is poser. This is particularly true at the start of musicians’
somewhat counterintuitive, since it might be expected that careers, before articles and interviews about them appear in
greater familiarity would lead to a greater focus on the pro- the music press. In some assessment situations anonymity is
duct rather than the artist. However, as Ward concluded, for used to prevent bias from arising. This may not be entirely
those engaged with the area there may be some advantage in successful, however, when other information is available
devaluing women due to fears that their professional status which might be used to guess the gender of the person being
may be diminished by the inclusion of a lower-status minor- assessed. Studies of the anonymous marking of academic
ity in a prestigious group. In other words, those working in work have found that handwriting style may be used to
the same area may have a vested interest in preserving the attribute gender to the author (e.g. Hartley, 1992). In mu-
status quo. sical compositions similar attributions may be made for
In order to obtain an overview of studies using the stylistic features. Indeed, there is evidence that particular
Goldberg paradigm, Swim et al. (1989) undertook a meta- features can invoke gender associations (Tagg, 1989) so it is
analysis of 106 published articles. They concluded that average possible that such associations could influence assessors,
differences between the ratings of the products or behaviors who may guess the sex of a composer when no identifying
of males and females are negligible, even when other factors information is present.
such as the sex of the raters or the year of publication were In examining possible anti-female bias in judgments of
taken into account. However, they acknowledged that evalu- music, the literature on stereotyping suggests that one
ations may be influenced by a number of factors of which important variable may be the amount of information avail-
gender is only one, which might include the diagnosticity of able about the composer. The study reported here, therefore,
the material presented about the target and the nature of the used the Goldberg paradigm to examine judgments of con-
judgments required. While useful as an overall summary of temporary compositions under conditions in which either
effect sizes, their findings inevitably throw little light upon only the name of the composer was provided or a brief bio-
factors that might interact with gender in producing bias in graphy was available in addition to the name. Anti-female

© 2003 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.


14679450, 2003, 2, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1467-9450.00330 by Cambridge University Library, Wiley Online Library on [16/05/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Scand J Psychol 44 (2003) Gender bias in evaluation of music 127

bias was predicted to be more likely to occur in the name- neutral; 10, very/very much. The presentation order of the music
only condition. and the name or description of the fictitious composers was bal-
anced across participants to counteract order effects and remove
consistent mapping between the fictitious composers and the musi-
cal extracts.
METHOD
In the name-only condition, the participants were presented with
response sheets with the names of Klaus Behne and Simon Healy,
Participants or Helena Behne and Sarah Healy, at the top. Both English and
Sixty-four undergraduate students (32 males, 32 females) with a European names were chosen to represent the international nature
mean age of 19.5 years took part in the study. Approximately 50% of the genre. In the biography condition, the same names were used
of the males and females had no formal training or recent musical followed by five-line descriptions of the family and musical back-
experience other than as listeners. The remainder had some com- ground of musicians at relatively early stages of their careers. One
petence on a variety of instruments, either through formal lessons description was used for Klaus/Helena Behne and another for
or through being self-taught. Simon/Sarah Healy. No evaluation of the music was included in the
two descriptions. The participants were instructed to read each
response sheet while the music was playing but not to write until the
music had stopped. They were also asked to make a note if they
Design, materials and procedure
recognized the piece, but none did.
The design used three independent variables: sex of participant, sex To control for any potential influence of the sex of the experi-
of fictitious composer, and amount of information provided about menter, male and female experimenters were present at all testing
the fictitious composer (name only or name plus brief biography). sessions. Participants were not told at the start of the study that the
Each participant was allocated to one of four groups on the basis composers were fictitious, and so a full debriefing took place at the
of the sex of the fictitious composers of the music to be rated and end of the study and participants were invited to destroy their
the amount of information (names or biographies) provided. Equal response sheets if they wished. None chose to do so.
numbers of males and females were allocated to each group. The
music consisted of two 30-second extracts of music composed by
different musicians. The length of extract was chosen to be suffici- RESULTS
ent for impressions of the music to be formed and was the same as
that used in past studies of musical preference (Baumann, 1960; The responses of each participant for the two musical
LeBlanc, Sims, Sivola & Obert, 1993). The contemporary genre extracts were averaged to provide an overall score on each
used in the study, New Age, was chosen for two main reasons: first, attribute for the condition to which they were allocated. All
vocal music would introduce the additional variable of sex of the attribute scores except those for “How much do you like it?”
singer(s), so a genre was selected in which instrumental pieces are
common, and, second, the nature of New Age music, a great deal
were then analyzed using a 2 × 2 × 2 × 10 (sex of participant
of which has roots in the folk and classical traditions, has encour- × sex of composer × amount of information × rating scale)
aged females to participate, so the genre does not have clear gender MANOVA.
associations. To illustrate the latter point, women were well rep- Females gave higher ratings than males on all attributes
resented in the recent Fourth Annual Music Awards from the except “forceful”, multivariate F(10, 46) = 2.35, p = 0.03, and
American magazine New Age Voice, winning awards for the categ-
ories of “Native American”, “Traditional World”, “Vocal”, “Celtic”
the biography produced higher ratings than the name-only
and “Top Radio Play” (New Age Voice, 2000). The extracts were condition for seven out of the ten attributes, multivariate
played through a standard stereo cassette player at constant volume F(10, 46) = 2.25, p = 0.03. In addition, the interaction
for all testing sessions. The two extracts were taken from work by between sex of composer and amount of information
male and female artists, to control for possible differences in mu- approached significance, F(10, 46) = 1.87, p = 0.07. This
sical features, the source tracks being “Someone up there likes you”
by Simple Minds and “Mosaic” by Suzanne Cianni.
interaction is shown in Fig. 1, and arises from the higher
The participants’ responses were recorded in a three-page book- ratings given to the music from the male composer in the
let. The first page provided written instructions for the experiment name-only condition on attributes that reflect musical quality.
and asked for details of age, sex and musical training. The instruc- In addition to the MANOVA, univariate ANOVAs were
tions stated that the study was examining how people judge styles conducted on each attribute. As already shown in the multi-
of music, and that participants would hear two pieces of music by
different New Age composers. Instructions were then given on how
variate result, higher ratings were awarded by females than
to complete the response sheets on the next two pages. To ensure males for a number of attributes: individualistic, F (1, 56) =
that the details were clear, the instructions were also read aloud by 11.39, p = 0.001; innovative, F (1, 56) = 8.68, p = 0.005; tech-
an experimenter. Pages 2 and 3 contained either the name or a short nically competent, F (1, 56) = 6.97, p = 0.01; and soothing,
biography of a fictitious male or female composer together with a F (1, 56) = 4.16, p < 0.05. Near-significant results were also
series of ten 10-point rating scales for the following positive attri-
butes: forceful, individualistic, innovative, warm, soothing, gentle,
obtained for expressive, F (1, 56) = 3.25, p = 0.08, and artistic
technically competent, expressive, artistic merit, interesting. In addi- merit, F(1, 56) = 3.85, p = 0.06. In addition, interaction
tion, an item to assess their own liking of the extracts was used, effects were obtained for five attributes in which similar
namely “How much do you like it?” The first six attributes listed ratings were given by both males and females in the biography
here were adapted from the Bem Sex Role Inventory (Bem, 1974) so condition, while in the name-only condition females gave
as to be stereotypically masculine or feminine and applicable to
music. The remaining attributes were drawn from the research liter-
higher, and males lower, ratings (see Fig. 2). Three of these
ature on musical aesthetics (e.g. Berlyne, 1971; North & Hargreaves, were significant – innovative, F(1, 56) = 4.16, p = 0.05; express-
1998). The scales were labeled at three points: 0, not at all; 5, ive, F (1, 56) = 6.52, p = 0.01; interesting, F(1, 56) = 3.94,

© 2003 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.


14679450, 2003, 2, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1467-9450.00330 by Cambridge University Library, Wiley Online Library on [16/05/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
128 A. Colley et al. Scand J Psychol 44 (2003)

Fig. 1. Mean ratings of music from male and female composers for the 10 descriptive attributes used in the study in (a) the biography
condition and (b) the name-only condition.

p = 0.05 – while the remaining two just failed to reach signi- interaction between composer gender and amount of informa-
ficance – individualistic, F(1, 56) = 3.52, p = 0.07, and artistic tion revealed a tendency for the music from the female
merit, F(1, 56) = 2.84, p = 0.09. composers to be given higher ratings on a number of adject-
A univariate ANOVA was also conducted on the ratings ives in the biography rather than in the name-only condi-
given for “How much do you like it?”, which revealed tion. The nature of the effect is in accordance with the kind
higher ratings by females and lower ratings by males for the of stereotyping effect which might be predicted from the
name-only condition, F(1, 56) = 6.51, p = 0.01 (see Fig. 2f ), literature, that is, where no information other than social
suggesting a link between liking of the music and ratings of category is provided, there is greater pro-male bias.
other attributes in the name-only condition. The MANOVA In addition to the main finding relating to composer gen-
and ANOVAs were repeated, therefore, with “liking” used der, unexpected findings associated with the sex of the raters
as a covariate. The tendency for females to give higher, emerged. The results revealed a general tendency for females
and males lower, ratings in the name-only condition was to give higher ratings, even when liking was controlled for,
no longer present. The multivariate analysis again showed and this issue warrants further exploration in the context of
higher ratings from females, multivariate F(10, 45) = 2.04, artistic assessment, as it represents another potential source
p = 0.05, with the univariate analyses achieving significance of bias. The results also highlighted the importance of con-
for individualistic, F(1, 55) = 9.36, p = 0.003, innovative, sidering how much assessors like the material they are
F(1, 55) = 7.51, p = 0.008, and technically competent, F(1, 55) assessing. The analyses which did not control for liking
= 4.57, p = 0.04. The biography condition again produced revealed significant and near-significant interactions
higher ratings than the name-only condition, multivariate between the sex of the raters and the amount of information
F(10, 45) = 2.22, p = 0.03, with a significant univariate result provided for many of the attributes. Both males and females
for forceful, F(1, 55) = 6.65, p = 0.01. The effect shown in gave similar ratings in the biography condition, but females
Fig. 1 for ratings of musical quality to be higher for male gave higher ratings and males lower ratings in the name-only
composers than female composers in the name-only con- condition for “innovative”, “expressive”, “interesting”,
dition achieved significance, multivariate F(10, 45) = 2.15, “individualistic” and “artistic merit”. The attributes con-
p = 0.04, with a significant univariate interaction for inter- cerned all relate to the quality of the composition. However,
esting, F(1, 55) = 4.20, p = 0.04. the ratings for “How much do you like it?” followed the
same pattern, indicating a strong link between judgments of
quality and liking. Therefore, in the absence of individuating
DISCUSSION information about the composer, females tended to like the
Some evidence was found for anti-female bias in this study, compositions more than males and rate their quality more
in that there was a tendency for higher ratings on adjectives highly.
relating to musical competence to be given to music by male The most likely interpretation of this result is that, in the
composers in the name-only condition. The covariance name-only condition, liking of the music was a more import-
analyses revealed slightly stronger effects of bias due to ant contributor to judgments of quality than in the bio-
composer gender, indicating that control of other relevant graphy condition, in which background information about
variables is important in examining aesthetic judgments. The the composer could be used. There is substantial evidence of

© 2003 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.


14679450, 2003, 2, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1467-9450.00330 by Cambridge University Library, Wiley Online Library on [16/05/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Scand J Psychol 44 (2003) Gender bias in evaluation of music 129

Fig. 2. Means of the ratings given by male and female participants in the name-only and biography conditions for the following attributes:
(a) individualistic, (b) innovative, (c) expressive, (d) artistic merit, (e) interesting, and (f ) “How much do you like it?”.

© 2003 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.


14679450, 2003, 2, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1467-9450.00330 by Cambridge University Library, Wiley Online Library on [16/05/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
130 A. Colley et al. Scand J Psychol 44 (2003)

gender differences in liking of musical styles. For example, REFERENCES


Hargreaves, Comber and Colley (1995) found that girls in
Archer, J. (1992). Sex bias in evaluations at college and work. The
the 11–12 and 15 –16 age groups showed stronger liking than Psychologist, 5, 200 –204.
boys for a number of styles, including reggae, jazz, classical, Baumann, V. M. (1960). Teen-age music preferences. Journal of
opera, folk and chart pop. Boys showed stronger liking for Research in Music Education, 8, 75– 84.
rock and heavy metal. Similar findings were obtained in earl- Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny.
ier studies (e.g. Christenson & Peterson, 1988). Folk, chart Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162.
Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York:
pop and classical, all of which are liked more by females, are Appleton-Century-Crofts.
closest in style to New Age music. An association between Christenson, P. G. & Peterson, J. B. (1988). Genre and gender in the
liking of music and ratings of its quality has intuitive appeal structure of musical preferences. Communication Research, 15,
and is supported by previous research by North and 282–301.
Hargreaves (1996), who found highly significant correlations Comber, C., Hargreaves, D. J. & Colley, A. M. (1993). Girls, boys
and technology in music education. British Journal of Music
between art works nominated as “favorite” and “greatest”. Education, 10, 123–134.
In summary, the unexpected findings from this study, of Crozier, W. R. & Chapman, A. J. (1981). Aethetic preferences,
higher ratings from females, and of an association between prestige and social class. In D. O’Hare (Ed.), Psychology and the
liking for the stimulus material with higher ratings of quality arts (pp. 242–278). Brighton: Harvester.
in the name-only condition, both have potentially important Deaux, K. & Lewis, L. L. (1984). Structure of gender stereotypes:
Interrelationships among components and gender label. Journal
implications for the assessment of creative products and of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 991–1004.
warrant further investigation with genres with different gen- Eagly, A. H. & Wood, W. (1982). Inferred sex differences in status
der associations (e.g. heavy metal). If gender differences in as a determinant of gender stereotypes about social influence.
preferences for different musical genres produce different Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 915–928.
patterns of liking, which are in turn associated with quality Etaugh, C. & Sanders, S. (1974). Evaluation of performance as a
function of status and sex variables. Journal of Social Psycho-
ratings, this should be taken into account in designing pro- logy, 94, 237–241.
cedures for formal assessment. As for the main focus of the Farnsworth, P. R. (1969). The social psychology of music (2nd edn).
study, other investigators (e.g. Archer, 1992; Swim et al., Ames, IA: Iowa State University Press.
1989; Top, 1991) have concluded that, since gender bias can Goldberg, P. (1968). Are women prejudiced against women? Trans-
be demonstrated under certain conditions, the challenge is action, 5, 28–30.
Green, L. (1997). Music, gender, education. Cambridge: Cambridge
to identify the moderating variables. University Press.
Two limitations of our study are the use of only one mu- Hargreaves, D. J., Comber, C. & Colley, A. (1995). Musical likes
sical style and short musical extracts. Although our particip- and dislikes: The effects of age, gender and training in British
ants were able to make the required judgments, it might be secondary school pupils. Journal of Research in Music Educa-
argued that the use of short extracts would encourage stereo- tion, 43, 242–250.
Hartley, J. (1992). Sex bias, blind marking and assessing students.
typing effects by providing relatively little musical experi- Psychology Teaching Review, 1, 66 –75.
ence of a composer’s work. While further work with a wider LeBlanc, A., Sims, W. L., Sivola, C. & Obert, M. (1993). Musical
range of musical styles and longer exposure is necessary style preferences of different age listeners. Paper presented at the
before any sweeping conclusions can be drawn, we have 9th National Symposium on Research in Musical Behavior,
found evidence of greater pro-male bias in evaluations of Cannon Beech, Oregon.
Lenney, E., Mitchell, L. & Browning, C. (1983). The effect of clear
compositions attributed to male and female composers evaluation criteria on sex bias in judgements of performance.
under a condition in which little information about the Psychology of Women Quarterly, 7, 313 –328.
composers was available. The effect was stronger when liking Locksley, A., Borgida, E., Brekke, N. & Hepburn, C. (1980). Sex
of the stimulus material was taken into account. stereotypes and social judgement. Journal of Personality and
As other researchers have pointed out, even slight bias can Social Psychology, 39, 821– 831.
Macrae, C. N., Shepherd, J. W. & Milne, A. B. (1992). The effects
have a substantial impact upon outcome, particularly when of source credibility on the dilution of stereotype-based
few opportunities for advancement exist (Archer, 1992; Top, judgements. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 765–
1991). Relatively few musicians achieve eminence, and a high 775.
level of selectivity operates in entry to musical careers. Top New Age Voice (2000). http://www.newagevoice.playhear.com/html/
(1991) discussed the resistance of sex stereotypes to change main.isx. Retrieved 4 May 2001.
North, A. C. & Hargreaves, D. J. (1995). Eminence in pop music.
and our results add weight to her conclusion that, rather Popular Music and Society, 19, 41– 66.
than attempt to counter these, it may be more productive to North, A. C. & Hargreaves, D. J. (1996). Affective and evaluat-
design “judgment procedures and contexts that minimize ive responses to the arts. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 14, 207–
their influence” (p. 103). 222.
North, A. C. & Hargreaves, D. J. (1998). Affective and evaluative
The assistance of Steve Barrett, Helen Davies and Heather Myles in responses to pop music. Current Psychology, 17, 102–108.
data collection is gratefully acknowledged. Preparation of the study O’Neill, S. (1997). Gender and music. In D. J. Hargreaves & A. C.
was assisted by grant reference no. SOC(100)/197 from the Nuffield North (Eds.), The social psychology of music (pp. 46–63).
Foundation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

© 2003 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.


14679450, 2003, 2, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1467-9450.00330 by Cambridge University Library, Wiley Online Library on [16/05/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Scand J Psychol 44 (2003) Gender bias in evaluation of music 131

O’Neill, S. & Boulton, M. J. (1996). Boys’ and girls’ preferences for Swim, J., Borgida, E. & Maruyama, G. (1989). Joan McKay versus
musical instruments: A function of gender? Psychology of John McKay: Do gender stereotypes bias evaluations? Psycho-
Music, 24, 171–183. logical Bulletin, 105, 409– 429.
Paludi, M. A. & Bauer, W. D. (1983). Goldberg revisited: What’s in Tagg, P. (1989). An anthropology of stereotypes in TV music. Swedish
an author’s name? Sex Roles, 9, 387–396. Musicological Journal, 19– 42.
Paludi, M. A. & Strayer, L. A. (1985). What’s in an author’s name? Tanner, L. R. (1977). Sex bias in children’s response to literature.
Differential evaluations of performance as a function of author’s Language Arts, 54, 48 –50.
name. Sex Roles, 12, 353 –361. Top, T. (1991). Sex bias in the evaluation of performance in the
Pheterson, G. I., Kiesler, S. B. & Goldberg, P. A. (1971). Evaluation scientific, artistic and literary professions: A review. Sex Roles,
of the performance of women as a function of their sex, achieve- 24, 73–106.
ment and personal history. Journal of Personality and Social Ward, C. (1981). Prejudice against women: Who, when, and why?
Psychology, 19, 114 –118. Sex Roles, 7, 163–171.
Qualifications and Curriculum Authority (1999). The National
Curriculum: Inclusion. Retrieved 10 June 2001, from http:// Received 29 August 2001, accepted 7 December 2001
www.nc.uk.net/inclus.html.

© 2003 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.

You might also like