Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Forest Ecology and Management 528 (2023) 120626

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Forest Ecology and Management


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/foreco

Dogs trained to recognise a bark beetle pheromone locate recently attacked


spruces better than human experts
N. Vošvrdová a, A. Johansson b, c, M. Turčáni a, R. Jakuš a, D. Tyšer a, F. Schlyter a, b, 1,
R. Modlinger a, 1, *
a
Czech University of Life Sciences, Faculty of Forestry and Wood Sciences, 16500 Prague, Czech Republic
b
Chemical Ecology, Department of Plant Protection Biology, PO Box 102, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, Alnarp, Sweden
c
SnifferDogs Sweden, Bäckvägen 26, SE-342 93 Hjortsberga, Sweden

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: One of the most promising techniques for rapid detection of bark beetle-infested trees is the use of specially
Detection dog trained dogs. Due to the novelty of using dogs in detecting bark beetle-infested trees, evaluation of success or
Forest pest management comparison with the traditional approaches is lacking. Spruces were pre-treated with a synthetic pheromone at
Ips typographus
several tree positions in six experimental forest stands of 4–12 ha. The tree positions were selected based on their
Norway spruce
arrangement in a random scheme or in patches considered suitable for bark beetle colonisation. Three dogs of
Green attack
different ages, sex, and levels of experience in detecting Ips typographus were compared with three experienced
human bark beetle specialists. We used GPS positioning of dog tracks (unleashed), handlers, human experts, and
detection points during the search under a blind-test procedure for tracking positions. The potential utility of the
search methods was estimated with three aspects: 1) search success: detection of infested trees, 2) search effort:
length of route, and 3) search efficiency: trees detected / unit time.
Dog-handler pairs were overall more successful in detecting trees attacked by bark beetles than human experts.
In particular, the success rate of dogs was higher in plots with random arrangement pre-baited trees and search
efficiency was four times higher than that of the human experts.
The most efficient time for the use of dogs for detection would be during the spring flight period, when
detection of first attacks and small hot spots could potentially prevent the development of larger infestations. The
main advantage of using the dog search method is to amplify the time-window from about one week to one
month from the detection to prompt removal of the infested trees from the forest.

1. Introduction that are outside the bioclimatic niche of Norway spruce, and this would
induce drought stress (Lindner et al. 2010). Considering these dramatic
The Eurasian spruce bark beetle Ips typographus (L.) is an intensively changes, forest management should act quickly to contain the spread of
studied, native long-term pest of Norway spruce in the entirety of its the spruce bark beetle.
natural and planted distribution in the European forests (Christiansen & Integrated pest management of spruce bark beetles traditionally in­
Bakke 1988, Grégoire & Evans 2004). Climate change has induced an volves the implementation of preventive measures and treatment ac­
unprecedented outbreak of I. typographus infestation in spruce trees in tions (Fettig & Hilszczanski 2015). Early detection and removal of bark
recent years (Hlásny et al. 2019). The epicentre of the current bark beetle-infested trees remain crucial aspects of bark beetle management
beetle outbreak is Central Europe, where the most affected regions are (Hlásny et al. 2019). However, the visual signs of infestation in the tree
now being deprived of older spruce stands (Hlásny et al. 2021a). crown are very difficult to observe with the human eye from a distance,
Drought stress has been the main driver of outbreaks in spruce forests as the foliage remains green for weeks (Kautz et al. 2022). Substantial
(Hlásny et al. 2021b). Climate change predictions show an increased progress in the identification of attacked trees was achieved with remote
frequency of shorter or longer periods of precipitation and temperatures sensing, where aerial, unmanned aerial vehicles, and satellite imagery

* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: modlinger@fld.czu.cz (R. Modlinger).
1
Authors claim shared seniority.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foreco.2022.120626
Received 28 July 2022; Received in revised form 26 October 2022; Accepted 1 November 2022
Available online 14 November 2022
0378-1127/Published by Elsevier B.V.
N. Vošvrdová et al. Forest Ecology and Management 528 (2023) 120626

provided useful information on the extent of bark beetle damage their development (Bakke et al. 1977, Birgersson et al. 1984, Johansson
(Wulder et al. 2006, Näsi et al. 2015, Abdullah et al. 2019, Klouček et al. et al. 2019). All three dogs were trained using the method of Johansson
2019). Still, the identification of infested trees is prone to errors due to et al. (2019), which is based on the principle of positive reinforcement;
differing accuracy and the timeliness of detection (Huo et al. 2021, that is, dogs were rewarded immediately after a correct indication of the
Zabihi et al. 2021, Bárta et al. 2022). The most reliable method of target scent (Rebmann et al. 2000, Braun 2013). For scent detection,
identifying bark beetle attack has been close visual inspection of the tree three separate components of the I. typographus aggregation pheromone,
trunk (Kautz et al. 2022). However, this method is time consuming, and specifically 2-methyl-3-buten-2-ol, 4S-cis-verbenol, and ipsdienol, as
variability in weather conditions can complicate visual inspection well as verbenone were used during the early stage of training (Bir­
because the boring dust can be washed away by the rain (Hlásný et al. gersson et al. 1984). An education scent platform was used to teach these
2019). A novel method of detection is the use of specially trained dogs; four odours. Dogs were exposed to various indoor training situations.
this is by far the most promising technique for the rapid detection of After this phase, the training lessons were moved to outdoor conditions.
bark beetle-infested trees and is already a technically proven concept for Individual substances were replaced by a mixture of the compounds
praxis (Johansson et al. 2019). during the advanced stage of training, and the search area was gradually
Dogs have been widely used in the detection of humans, cadavers, enlarged. The dogs were trained with actual bark beetle-attacked trees
drugs, and explosives, (Hepper & Wells 2015). Recently, dogs have been in the last training phase. Three bark beetle specialists were included in
increasingly used to search for insects such as termites [Termitidae] the experiment; the specialists had extensive experience in searching
(Brooks et al. 2003), bugs [Cimicidae] (Pfiester et al. 2008), ants [For­ bark beetle-infested trees in Central Europe.
micidae] (Lin et al. 2011), palm weevils [Curculionidae] (Suma et al.
2014), and longhorn [Cerambycidae] or jewel beetles [Buprestidae] 2.2. Experimental set-up
(Hoyer-Tomiczek et al. 2016, Hoyer-Tomiczek & Hoch 2020). Some of
them are considered invasive species in particular areas. Dogs have been All experimental plots were situated in the School Forest Enterprise
used to detect xylobiont beetles that are protected by law (Mosconi et al. (SFE) Kostelec and Černými lesy (Czech University of Life Sciences),
2017, Apelqvist, 2022). Identification of these insect species by dogs is near the small town of Stříbrná Skalice (Central Bohemia, Czech Re­
made possible by previous knowledge of the pheromones and other public; Fig. 1). The SFE is an upland area outside the natural Norway
semiochemicals released during host colonization (Bakke et al. 1977), spruce distribution, with suboptimal environmental conditions for the
and their quantitative changes (Birgersson et al. 1984). Dogs can detect tree species: a mean annual temperature of 7–8 ◦ C, and mean precipi­
infested trees in the early stages of bark beetle attack, even before tation of 550–600 mm / year (Tolasz et al. 2007). In 2018, when the
physiological changes visually manifest in the tree crown (Kautz et al. experiment was conducted, the outbreak of I. typographus was on a steep
2022). Dogs trained to detect bark beetle pheromones can find an rise in Central Bohemia (Hlásny et al. 2021b). Six experimental plots
infested tree up to a distance of 150 m (Johansson et al. 2019). Such were established in an 100–120-year-old Norway spruce-dominated
impressive detection ranges could be exploited for high sensitivity and forest stand, where spruce trees constituted more than 95 % of the
could be crucial in finding new bark beetle spots within forest stands, tree cover. For better orientation of the dog handlers and human spe­
which are more frequent and random during drought periods (Hlásný cialists, trees on the plot boundaries were marked using a coloured
et al. 2021b). However, due to the novelty of the dog-based detection paint.
method, no evaluations or comparisons of this method with the tradi­ For initialization of bark beetle attack, standard pheromone dis­
tional techniques have been made. Given the costs associated with long- pensers IT Ecolure® (Fytofarm, Mělník, Czech Republic) were used. The
term training and maintenance of dogs for bark beetle detection (Esti­ number of baited trees and plot areas are given in Table 1. Two types of
mation for Czech conditions is 18 000 EUR, including eight months of dispenser placements on the experimental plots were used: (a) random
training, necessary materials, travelling to forests and personal costs spatial distributions (referred to as random plots) in any place of the
based on average gross nominal wage, excluding purchase price of the experimental area (e.g. a single tree in the middle of the forest), and (b)
dog and feeding), it is crucial to gauge the efficacy of this new approach places considered suitable for beetle attacks (referred to as suitable plots),
before adopting the method for forest pest management. based on knowledge of bark beetle preferences (Wermelinger 2004,
We hypothesise that dog-handler pairs would be more efficient Fahse & Heurich 2011). The suitable plots could be forest edges (Kautz
searchers than human experts in the early detection of beetle-attacked et al. 2013) with logging residuals (Stříbrská et al. 2022), stands around
trees in commercial forests. We measured three dependent variables: trees damaged by wind (Göthlin et al. 2000), sunny parts of the forest, or
1) search success: detection of recently attacked trees, 2) search effort: southern slopes of the hills ((Netherer and Nopp-Mayr, 2005) (Table 1;
search route length travelled / unit area, and 3) search efficiency: Fig. 1).
infested trees detected / unit time. Pheromone dispensers were placed on selected spruce trees at a
height of 3 m above the ground on 27, 28, and 30 June 2018. The lo­
2. Materials and methods cations of baited trees were recorded using GPS. After 4–8 days
(depending on appropriate weather conditions for beetles flight), the
2.1. Dog-handler pairs and bark beetle specialists dispensers were removed, and the trees were checked for infestation
status (to certify if attacks had started during the short period of baiting,
The three dogs that participated in the experiment were Belgian considering that the weather controlling beetle flight intensity was a
Shepherds (FCI 2002). Details about their ages, sex, and levels of random factor). At 2–10 days after dispenser removal and two days
experience in detecting I. typographus are shown in Table S1. Two dogs before the start of the experiment, the plots were again checked for the
had five years’ experience with bark beetle detection, and the other dog presence of spontaneously attacked trees, which were also recorded
was a novice that had underwent six months of training in synthetic using GPS.
compound detection and two months training in detecting infested trees. The search for attacked trees was performed using the double-blind
The usual way to provide training odour stimuli is from natural material method (Moser & McCulloch 2010; Jendrny et al. 2020), where a bark
for this type of study, which is cumbersome. Such biological material has beetle expert/dog-handler pair was made to walk through the experi­
variable quality due to decay over time or low source material avail­ mental areas alone without knowledge of position of the infested trees.
ability, for instance during winter or coming from protected species. In The experiment was performed for eight days; dog-handler pairs were
contrast, training to detect semiochemicals of the Eurasian spruce bark allowed to search the forest for the first four days, followed by the
beetle is facilitated by the knowledge and year-round synthetic avail­ human experts for the remaining four days. The performing of the
ability of the pheromone components and the verbenone released during experiment was realised under the prepared scheme, each dog searched

2
N. Vošvrdová et al. Forest Ecology and Management 528 (2023) 120626

Fig. 1. Overview of the experimental plots in the School Forest Enterprise of Kostelec nad Černými lesy (Czech University of Life Sciences, Czech Republic) in Central
Bohemia and their location in the Czech Republic. Inside the experimental plots, the positions of bark beetle-infested trees are marked (pre-baited and spontaneously
attacked). See Table 1 and Table S1 for details of plots and search parties.

overall areas of search and the movement of the dogs to facilitate up­
Table 1
wind search, as detailed by Johansson et al. (2019). The dog handlers
Parameters of experimental plots.
were neither trained foresters nor entomologists, but were experts in dog
Plot Plot Attacked trees Arrangement behaviour and sniffer dog training; however they had the necessary
nr area of pre-baited
Artificially Number of Volume of knowledge and ample practical experience to allow them to ascertain
(ha) trees
pre-baited infested infested trees whether an indicated tree had been attacked by I. typographus. The
trees at start trees at end at end of
number of infested trees and the time taken for searching the given plot
of of experiment
experiment experiment (m3/ha)
was also recorded. At the end of each part of the experiment, all trees in
all the plots were visually inspected by the same independent person
1 10 10 44 5.3 Suitable*
from the team of authors, and the number of infested trees (pre-baited or
2 7.7 12 67 10.4 Suitable
3 4 5 9 2.7 Random not) and their GPS positions were recorded.
4 12 10 13 1.3 Random
5 12 11 14 1.4 Suitable
6 12 9 13 1.3 Random 2.3. Statistical analyses
*
Position of pre-baited trees (to become attacked) in patches of the plot that
we judged as ‘suitable’ for bark beetle colonisation of trees, as opposed to purely Statistical evaluation was performed using three dependent vari­
randomised tree positions for pre-baiting. ables, each in a separate regression model. The first dependent variable,
“Search success”, was the percentage of successfully detected beetle-
maximally-two plots per day, and to prevent tracking, only one dog was attacked trees in a plot. In dog-training terms, these were the “true
planned for each plot per day, except plot 5, where in the morning was positives”. The second variable “Length of search route” was measured
BB and afternoon Aska (the most experienced dog who does not follow a as the GPS track route length covered to search a plot (unit: km / ha). A
track on the ground but fully used wind blowing). Search routes of the third variable “Search efficiency” was calculated as the ratio of “Search
dog, the handler and bark beetle specialists were recorded using GPS success” and time spent on the plot (unit: trees detected/min.).
(Fig. 2). The handler track generally followed the dog track. Hence, only We analysed the three dependent variables using a generalised linear
the route of the dog in each dog-handler pair was evaluated. The dogs mixed model (GLMM). As the independent variables for GLMM, we used
moved faster and covered a larger search area because they were “Design”, which was the arrangement of the trees attacked by bark
directed by olfactory stimuli, whereas the handlers determined the beetles in the plot (random vs suitable) and “Searcher”, or who inspected
the plots (dog-handler pair vs human expert). Interactions of the

3
N. Vošvrdová et al. Forest Ecology and Management 528 (2023) 120626

Fig. 2. Maps of two examples of GPS routes of human experts and dog-handler pairs searching for bark beetle-attacked trees in experimental plots. A) Plot 1 was
established in the “suitable” tree arrangement spatial design, and B) plot 6 in a “random” design (Table 1, Fig. 1). See the Materials and Methods for the definitions of
the spatial arrangements. Higher resolution track examples for all 6 plots can be found in Fig. S2.

independent variables were also included in all cases, but in two models
Table 3
they were found to be not significant according to the likelihood-ratio
GLMM contrast comparison the Search success of the “Searcher” on Suitable and
test (χ2) during the model building; hence, they were not included in
Random plots.
the final models shown in Tables 3–5. All variables were explored for
basic assumptions of regression type analysis, as described by Zuur et al. Design

(2010). Violation of the assumptions was found in relation to the inde­ Searcher Suitable Random
pendence of the observations, which were influenced by the evaluation Dog-handler pair vs Human expert 0.81182 0.000406 ***
of the same experimental plots. Therefore, using GLMM, a random
structure for each experimental plot was implemented in the model.
Model formulation and prediction were realised in the package Table 4
GLMM contrast comparison the Search success of Dog-handler pairs and Human
experts on different plot “Design” arrangements.
Table 2
Searcher
GLMM likelihood-ratio test for significance independent variables including Design Dog-handler pair Human expert
their interaction. Dependent variable attacked trees detected (%,), independent
Suitable vs Random 0.217012 5.82e-06 ***
variables “Searcher” and “Design”.
Df AIC LRT Pr(>Chi)

Searcher 1 5.4687 5.5938 0.0180 * glmmTMB following the procedures described by Brooks et al. (2017).
Design 1 8.0261 8.1513 0.0043 ** Because response variables were continuous, strictly positive, and
Searcher:Design 1 1.8748 5.0634 0.0244 * without zeroes, gamma distribution was used (Zuur et al. 2009). All

4
N. Vošvrdová et al. Forest Ecology and Management 528 (2023) 120626

Table 5 experts in suitable plots, where 66 % of infested trees were detected by


GLMM likelihood-ration test for length of search route (km/ha, dependent dog-handler pairs and 63 % by human experts (GLMM p = 0.81; Table 3;
variable). “Searcher” and “Design” are independent variables. Fig. 3A). Significant difference in detection levels between human ex­
Df AIC LRT Pr(>Chi) perts and dog-handler pairs was observed in the random plots, where the
Searcher 1 1.0119 6.7232 0.0095 **
bark beetle-infested trees were at unexpected locations and mostly
Design 1 − 5.3748 0.3365 0.5619 encountered fewer attacks. Dog-handler pairs found 51 % of attacked
Searcher:Design 1 − 3.7113 0.1699 0.6801 trees, while human experts found only 23 % (GLMM p < 0.001; Table 3).
In contrast, the search success on the suitable plots was similar between
dog-handler pairs and human experts (Fig. 3A). The search success of the
GLMM analyses were performed using the R 4.0.2 environment (R Core
Human expert was significantly different between the variant arrange­
Team 2020).
ments (63 % vs 23 %; GLMM p < 0.001), whereas the Dog-handler pair
success cannot be considered different (66 % vs 51 %; GLMM p = 0.81;
3. Results
Table 4). These statistical conclusions are well illustrated by the example
of the best human expert, who was more successful than dog-handler
3.1. Search success
pairs in all the suitable plots (Fig. 3A).

The experiment was performed a week after the first bark beetle
attack. Therefore, the infested trees did not yet show any visible changes 3.2. Search effort
in the crown; they could only be detected by the presence of boring dust
that had settled at the base of the tree trunk. Overall, dog-handler pairs During search for infested trees, dog-handler pairs covered an
were more successful than human experts in the detection of trees average distance of 2 km longer than that of human experts. The average
attacked by bark beetles (GLMM p < 0.05; Table 2). On average, 58 % of distance covered by dog-handler pairs was 0.55 km/ha, while human
bark beetle-infested trees were detected by dog-handler teams and 43 % experts walked 0.36 km/ha (Supplementary Table S2). Plot type had a
by bark beetle human experts (Table S2). The experimental tree no effect on length GLMM p = 0.56; Table 5). There was a significant
arrangement factor provided clear differences between dog and human difference in the length of the route between dog-handler pairs and
searchers. human experts (p < 0.01, Table 5; Fig. 4). Dog-handler pairs on average
A higher proportion (64 %) of attacked trees were found in the walked a greater distance than did human experts. However, if we
suitable plots. Plots with pre-baited trees in random positions were much exclude the data of the young dog in training (“BB”; Supplementary
less detected (37 %), indicating a significant difference between spac­ Table S1), which took the longest route in all the plots, the difference
ings (GLMM p < 0.01; Table 2; Fig. 3). From the factors interaction point between dog-handler pairs and human experts was minimal (upper red
of view, there was no difference between dog-handler pairs and human dot in Fig. 4). The route length did not show significant interaction
between searcher type and tree arrangement (GLMM p = 0.68; Table 5).

Fig. 3. Search success – the successfully identified attacked trees found by dog-handler pair (red) and human expert (blue) searchers in two types of spacing of
experimental tree arrangement: A)‘Suitable’ arrangement in patches considered ‘suitable’ for beetle attacks and B) ‘Random’ with a randomised spatial distribution.
The bigger dot in the middle is a posterior mean value and whiskers represent the 95 % CI of the prediction. Smaller dots are original values. (For interpretation of the
references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

5
N. Vošvrdová et al. Forest Ecology and Management 528 (2023) 120626

Fig. 4. Search efforts: Length of search route in km/ha by dog-handler pair (red) and human expert (blue) searchers in two types of spacing of experimental tree
arrangement: A)‘Suitable’ arrangement in patches considered ‘suitable’ for beetle attacks and B) ‘Random’ with a randomised spatial distribution. Bigger dots in the
middle represent posterior mean values and whiskers represent 95% CIs of the predictions. Smaller dots are original values. (For interpretation of the references to
colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

3.3. Search efficiency efficiency indicated an overall higher performance by dog-handler pairs.

Dog-handler pairs were overall more efficient, as they showed a 4. Discussion


higher number of trees detected / unit time than human experts (GLMM
p < 0.001; Table 6; Fig. 5). The mean number of trees detected per unit To our knowledge, this is the first field comparison of the efficiency
time by dog-handler pairs was more than double compared to that by of sniffer dogs and foresters (human experts) in the detection of bark
human experts (dog-handler pair 2.3 trees/10 min, human expert 1.1 beetle-infested trees, using known positions of true positive cases in
trees/10 min., Supplementary Table S2). The ‘suitable’ tree arrange­ experimental forest stands. False negatives did not occur because the
ments of the pre-baited plots were less demanding for human experts, handler in the team verified the authenticity of the trees identified by
the difference in the dog-handler pairs was relatively smaller (<1.9 the dogs. Similarly, the experts used in this study were not expected to
times) (Supplementary Table S2). give “false positives”, but “false negatives” may have occurred in the
Among the two tree arrangements, the number of trees detected / event of fresh attacks where frass was washed away by rain. The field
unit time was generally lower for the random arrangement (Fig. 5B). experiences show that the dog finds almost no false positives. Perhaps 10
There was a significant difference for tree arrangement (averaged over for the whole season. Usually, those trees have 1 or 2 bark beetle
searcher type, p < 0.01; Table 6). In the random arrangement of pre- entrance holes. The attack can develop into an infestation, or the tree
baited trees, the human experts were less efficient and the efficiency can defend itself. Both options are possible in the field. Known positions
of the dog-handler pairs during the search was greater than 4 × higher allow a higher precision during evaluation, and the two types of ar­
(dog-handler pair 1 tree/min, human expert 0.25 tree/min; Supple­ rangements of pre-baited trees, random and suitable, helps to better
mentary Table S2). According to the statistical model the interaction relate to a variety of real-life praxis conditions.
searcher with the tree arrangement factor was not significant (GLMM p We found that dog-handler pairs found 51 % of attacked trees
= 0.07; Table 6). Thus, our estimates for the three aspects of search whereas the human experts found 23 % in the case of random spatial
spacing of pre-baited trees (Supplementary Table S2). Such spatial
patterns of bark beetle attacks occur naturally during large outbreaks,
Table 6 where the predisposition of trees play a lesser role in bark beetle infes­
GLMM likelihood-ration test for efficiency of detection (trees/10 min of search, tation, and the trees are colonised rather randomly (Fahse & Heurich
dependent variable). “Searcher” and “Design” are independent variables. 2011) or in other patterns, for example presence of Armillaria root rot
Df AIC LRT Pr(>Chi) (Jakuš 1998). Similarly, we observed such patterns during 2018, when
bark beetle spots appeared inside a seemingly homogenous spruce stand
Searcher 1 96.2060 20.7422 0.0000 ***
Design 1 82.4010 6.9376 0.0084 ** with no visible predispositions. For the initialization of the bark beetle
Searcher:Design 1 77.4630 3.2172 0.0729 . attack, local water availability for trees could be an important factor
(Netherer et al. 2021), which may occur irregularly at the level of
For descriptive statistics of the input dependent variables, see Table S2.

6
N. Vošvrdová et al. Forest Ecology and Management 528 (2023) 120626

Fig. 5. Efficiency of search (correctly detected trees/10 min) by dog-handler pairs (red) and human experts (blue) under two types of experimental spatial tree
distributions: A)‘Suitable’ arrangement in patches considered ‘suitable’ for beetle attacks and B) ‘Random’ with a randomised spatial distribution. Bigger dot in the
middle is a posterior mean value and whiskers represent 95 % CI of the prediction. Smaller dots are original values. (For interpretation of the references to colour in
this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

individual trees. Such random occurrence corresponded to our experi­ only two months before the experiment. The search route of the inex­
mental design, where prior knowledge about the preference of bark perienced pair was the longest of all pairs, except in one case. Data
beetles was not used for selecting trees in our random plots. Our results excluding the inexperienced pair indicate the search route of dog-
showed that dogs were generally successful in detection of infested trees handler pairs would be similar to that of the human experts (0.35 vs
in random plots, likely due to their ability to detect pheromones over 0.36 km/ha). Regardless, the novice dog-handler pair, still found more
long distances, typically over 100 m (Johansson et al. 2019). Notably, infested trees than human experts with many years of experience.
the dog-handler pairs did not move in any ‘random walk’ when Furthermore, the relatively short search route of the human experts was
searching. Instead, the handlers followed direct search routes to cover due to their reliance on the visible markings in the attacked trees
plots in a structured way (Fig. 2, Supplementary Fig. S1). (different shades of green, resin flow under the tree crown) or their
Human experts searched the experimental areas for approximately knowledge of suitable areas for bark beetles. Such a strategy was rela­
twice as long as dog-handler pairs, regardless of the plot design (Sup­ tively successful in plots with places judged as suitable, but frustrating in
plementary Table S2). Random plots were searched slightly faster than the case of plots with random arrangement of pre-baited trees. Human
suitable plots possibly due to the relatively lower number of attacked experts stopped searching on random plots earlier due to lack of direc­
trees / hectare (Supplementary Table S2). Completing the search was tion in exploring attacked trees and the practical difficulty of visiting
entirely up to the discretion of the searcher. Dog-handler pairs could each tree.
finish the search of an area sooner due to the ability of dogs to detect There have not been enough studies comparing the efficiency of dog-
attacked trees from a distance that was approximately-two orders of handler pairs with other techniques. For detecting mammals was the
magnitude longer than that of a human – 100 m vs 1 m (Johansson et al. higher success of dogs, also found by Long et al. (2007) and Cristescu
2019). The shorter time to find an infested tree during the early stages of et al. (2015). Unfortunately, here are no studies that evaluate the reli­
infestation allows control of a larger endangered forest area at the start ability of humans (foresters) in detecting trees attacked by bark beetles.
of a beetle flight. One of the main reasons for failure to contain a bark A field survey of infested trees by foresters is considered a reliable
beetle outbreak is the lack of human resources during beetle flight pe­ detection method (Bárta et al. 2022) but it is demanding both in terms of
riods (Hlásný et al. 2021a). Higher search success in a shorter amount of time and workforce. In our experiment, the success of foresters and dog-
time favours the use of sniffer dogs over humans for the timely detection handler pairs in finding trees attacked in random plots differed sub­
of infested trees. stantially, with success rates of 23 and 63 %, respectively. In suitable
We expected a longer search route for the human experts due to the plots, the success rates were more similar, but still lower for humans
need for close-range inspection of the trunk. The contrasting results (Fig. 3A).
could be caused by: (i) use of a young dog still in training, and (ii) the An impressive success rate with respect to dog-handler pairs was
confidence of the forest experts in their knowledge. Regarding the first observed, even though a week had passed between the field survey of
point, this study used a relatively inexperienced dog-handler pair dogs and humans; more infested trees developed more visible signals
(Supplementary Table S1), who was trained on real infested trees for (mainly boring dust) during this time, which could be easier to detect for

7
N. Vošvrdová et al. Forest Ecology and Management 528 (2023) 120626

human experts. Moreover, according to Johansson et al. (2019), dogs the most effective methods for the protection of forest stands from bark
can detect infested trees from the first hours after the attack, which beetles is currently an important mitigation measure that forest man­
agrees with the chemically determined rate of pheromone production agement must address to preserve carbon storage in spruce forests in the
(Birgersson et al. 1984). In a recent comparison between human expert coming decades (IPCC 2022).
and aerial hyperspectral detection methods, bark beetle-infested trees
were detected 23 days later via remote sensing compared to human 5. Conclusion
detection (Bárta et al. 2022). This shows the problem of timeliness with
remote sensing owing to reflectance of the crown; see also Kautz et al. Dog-handler pairs showed an overall higher efficiency in fulfilling
(2022). the demands in the three aspects of search potency: higher detection
Managing bark beetle population levels in most forests is realised by success, lower search effort, and a resulting higher efficiency. Plots with
sanitation felling (Hlásny et al., 2021a). The first and most important the two arrangements of briefly pre-baited tree positions, random and
rule formulated by Wermelinger (2004) for efficient sanitation felling is suitable, differed strongly: the random pattern was difficult to search for
that “trees must be found and cut before the new beetle generation both dog-handler pairs and humans alone, but more so for humans. Our
emerges”, optimally before parental adult females leave the tree for the results show that dog-handler pairs were four times more efficient in
sister-brood establishment (Davídková & Doležal 2017). A critical detection within random plots, probably because they orient to single-
element of this rule is the early detection of infested trees. The sooner tree sources of pheromones over long distances. The shorter time to
the infested trees are found, the greater the chances of timely prevention find an early bark beetle-attacked tree allows proactive management of
of bark beetle infestation spread. The use of sniffer dogs brings a new a larger endangered forest area at the start of beetle flight. The use of
dimension where early and rapid detection of infested trees, immedi­ sniffer dogs brings a new, larger window of very early and rapid
ately after occupancy by bark beetles, is achieved. detection and further management of infested trees, even immediately
If intensive detection and processing management are carried out after the first occupancy by bark beetles.
immediately at the beginning of the first bark beetle attack activity, the
spread of later bark beetle outbreak can be prevented. Under warm and CRediT authorship contribution statement
dry conditions, serious landscape scale outbreaks can arise from the first
few spring bark beetle attack hotspots (tree groups). The use of dogs for N. Vošvrdová: Investigation, Data curation, Funding acquisition,
bark beetle detection is very effective and time-efficient, as they can find Writing – original draft. A. Johansson: Investigation, Writing – original
small starting points (Johansson et al. 2019); in other words, they can draft. M. Turčáni: Funding acquisition, Resources, Writing – original
locate a single tree within the forest. Detection of infested trees in the draft. R. Jakuš: Investigation, Writing – original draft. D. Tyšer: Visu­
forest using classical visual methods can be challenging in areas with a alization. F. Schlyter: Conceptualization, Supervision, Validation,
lower occurrence of bark beetles or in newly infested areas. Conversely, Funding acquisition, Writing – original draft. R. Modlinger: Method­
in areas with larger, more continuous beetle disturbances, the benefit of ology, Conceptualization, Investigation, Visualization, Funding acqui­
dog-handler pairs is less meaningful. These include larger areas with sition, Data curation, Writing - original draft, Writing - review & editing.
ongoing bark beetle outbreaks, such as newly created forest edges, that
are usually easily traceable by visual methods alone.
The practical use of dogs for identifying bark beetle infested trees is Declaration of Competing Interest
limited by the number of handlers, number of trained dogs, and time
necessary for dog training; however, in this study, the success of a young Authors declare to doń t have a competing interest.
dog trained only for a short time to detect infested trees is promising.
The most efficient time for use of dogs is almost certainly in the spring Data availability
flight period, when dog-handler pairs can detect initial attacks and small
hot spots (single or a few trees) and stop the development of incipient Data will be made available on request.
larger infestations. Later, when larger bark beetle infestation spots
occur, dog-handler pairs could delineate foci of infestation hot spots, Acknowledgements
narrowing down the area for forestry experts to focus the search activity
for new attacks, rather than searching the whole forest again. This research was supported by the EU project “EXTEMIT-K”, No.
Using dogs as searchers is crucial for active forest management at the CZ.02.1.01/0.0/0.0/15_003/0000433 financed by OP RDE and NAZV
beginning of an outbreak. The main advantage of using the dogs is to QK1920435 funded by Ministry of Agriculture. It was also supported by
amplify the time-window for detection and timely removal of the trees the Internal Grant Agency Czech University of Life Sciences, Prague,
from forest, and/or their felling and debarking. Faster identification of Faculty of Forestry and Wood Sciences, Kamýcká 129, Praha 6 - Suchdol,
infested trees within the longer period (from the 1st day after attack to 165 00, Czech Republic - projects GA FLD CZU A_19_10. AJ and FS were
9th week after attack, as opposed to when clear symptoms are visible, i. partly supported by Swedish Research Council FORMAS grant 2013-
e. in the 4–6th weeks) could be a key factor in bark beetle management 01583.
in the initial phase of outbreak. According to our experience, in both
Central Europe and Scandinavia, the forestry staff in the period of late
Appendix A. Supplementary material
April to the first half of May are heavily stressed by other work, mainly
with artificial afforestation. This can result in relatively lower prioriti­
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.
sation to identify and cut infested trees. Thus, the use of sniffer dogs is a
org/10.1016/j.foreco.2022.120626.
convenient way to effectively complement the work of foresters during
the demanding spring season.
In particular, the present global climate change is increasing the References
effect of temperature on forest health (Hartmann et al. 2022) and has
Abdullah, H., Skidmore, A.K., Darvishzadeh, R., Heurich, M., 2019. Timing of red-edge
made spruce forests more vulnerable. Observations over the past few and shortwave infrared reflectance critical for early stress detection induced by bark
decades have indicated an increase in the number of I. typographus beetle (Ips typographus, L.) attack. Int. J. Appl. Earth Obs. Geoinf. 82, 101900
generations from one to two in Scandinavia (Jönsson et al. 2011) and https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jag.2019.101900.
Apelqvist, M.. Training dogs to indicate synthetic pheromone from Plagionotus detritus,
three in Central Europe (Hlásný et al. 2011), which necessitates a strong Coleoptera, Cerambycidae to detect living beetles. SLU, Alnarp. https://stud.epsilon.
reduction of the bark beetle population in the spring period. The use of slu.se/17501/1/apelqvist_m_220201.pdf.

8
N. Vošvrdová et al. Forest Ecology and Management 528 (2023) 120626

Bakke, A., Froyen, P., Skattebol, L., 1977. Field response to a new pheromonal compound Johansson, A.M., Birgersson, G., Schlyter, F., 2019. Using synthetic semiochemicals to
isolated from Ips typographus. Naturwissenschaften 64, 98–99. https://doi.org/ train canines to detect bark beetle–infested trees. Ann. Forest Sci. 76, 262386
10.1007/BF00437364. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13595-019-0841-z.
Bárta, V., Hanuš, J., Dobrovolný, L., Homolová, L., 2022. Comparison of field survey and Jönsson, A.M., Harding, S., Krokene, P., Lange, H., Lindelöw, Å., Økland, B., Ravn, H.P.,
remote sensing techniques for detection of bark beetle-infested trees. For. Ecol. Schroeder, L.M., 2011. Modelling the potential impact of global warming on Ips
Manage. 506, 119984 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foreco.2021.119984. typographus voltinism and reproductive diapause. Clim. Change 109, 695–718.
Birgersson, G., Schlyter, F., Lofqvist, J., Bergstrom, G., 1984. Quantitative variation of https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-011-0038-4.
pheromone components in the Spruce Bark Beetle Ips typographus from different Kautz, M., Schopf, R., Ohser, J., 2013. The “sun-effect”: microclimatic alterations
attack phases. J. Chem. Ecol. 10, 1029–1055. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00987511. predispose forest edges to bark beetle infestations. Eur. J. Forest Res. 132, 453–465.
Braun, B., 2013. Wildlife detector dogs - A guideline on the training of dogs to detect https://doi.org/10.1007/s10342-013-0685-2.
wildlife in trade. World Wildlife Fund. Kautz, M., Peter, F.J., Harms, L., Kammen, S., Delb, H., 2022. Patterns drivers and
Brooks, M.E., Kristensen, K., van Benthem, K.J., Magnusson, A., Berg, C.W., Nielsen, A., detectability of infestation symptoms following attacks by the European spruce bark
Skaug, H.J., Mächler, M., Bolker, B.M., 2017. glmmTMB Balances speed and beetle. J. Pest. Sci. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10340-022-01490-8.
flexibility among packages for zero-inflated generalized linear mixed modeling. The Klouček, T., Komárek, J., Surový, P., Hrach, K., Janata, P., Vašíček, B., 2019. The use of
R Journal. 9, 378–400. https://doi.org/10.32614/RJ-2017066. UAV mounted sensors for precise detection of bark beetle infestation. Remote
Brooks, S.E., Oi, F.M., Koehler, P.G., 2003. Ability of canine termite detectors to locate Sensing. 11, 1561. https://doi.org/10.3390/rs11131561.
live termites and discriminate them from non-termite material. J. Econ. Entomol. 96, GRéGOIRE, J.-C., EVANS, H.F., 2004. Damage and control of bawbilt organisms, an overview,
1259–1266. https://doi.org/10.1603/0022-0493-96.4.1259. in: Lieutier, F. (Ed.), Bark and Wood Boring Insects in Living Trees in Europe: A
Christiansen, E., Bakke, A., 1988. The Spruce Bark Beetle of Eurasia. In: Berryman, A.A. Synthesis. Kluwer Academic Publishers, pp. 19–40. Doi: 10.1007/978-1-4020-2241-
(Ed.), Dynamics of Forest Insect Populations. Springer, Boston, MA, pp. 479–503. 8.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-0789-9_23. Lin, H.M., Chi, W.L., Lin, C.C., Tseng, Y.C., Chen, W.T., Kung, Y.L., Lien, Y.Y., Chen, Y.Y.,
Cristescu, R.H., Foley, E., Markula, A., Jackson, G., Jones, D., Frère, C., 2015. Accuracy 2011. Fire ant-detecting canines: A complementary method in detecting red
and efficiency of detection dogs: A powerful new tool for koala conservation and imported fire ants. J. Econ. Entomol. 104, 225–231. https://doi.org/10.1603/
management. Sci. Rep. 5, 8349. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep08349. EC10298.
Davídková, M., Doležal, P., 2017. Sister broods in the spruce bark beetle, Ips typographus Lindner, M., Maroschek, M., Netherer, S., Kremer, A., Barbati, A., Garcia-Gonzalo, J.,
(L.). For. Ecol. Manage. 405, 13–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foreco.2017.08.040. Seidl, R., Delzon, S., Corona, P., Kolström, M., Lexer, M.J., Marchetti, M., 2010.
Fahse, L., Heurich, M., 2011. Simulation and analysis of outbreaks of bark beetle Climate change impacts, adaptive capacity, and vulnerability of European forest
infestations and their management at the stand level. Ecological Modeling. 222, ecosystems. For. Ecol. Manage. 259, 698–709. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
1833–1846. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2011.03.014. foreco.2009.09.023.
FCI 2002. Belgian Shepherd Dog. FCI-Standard N◦ 15. 1–12. https://www.fci.be/Nomen Long, R.A., Donovan, T.M., Mackay, P., Zielinski, W.J., Buzas, J.S., 2007. Comparing Scat
clature/Standards/015g01-en.pdf. detection dogs, cameras, and hair snares for surveying carnivores. J. Wildl. Manage.
Fettig, C.J., Hilszczański, J., 2015. Management Strategies for Bark Beetles in Conifer 71, 2018–2025. https://doi.org/10.2193/2006-292.
Forests. In: Vega, F.E., Hofstetter, R.W. (Eds.), Bark Beetles: Biology and Ecology of Mosconi, F., Campanaro, A., Carpaneto, G.M., Chiari, S., Hardersen, S., Mancini, E.,
Native and Invasive Species. Elsevier, pp. 555–584. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978- Maurizi, E., Sabatelli, S., Zauli, A., Mason, F., Audisio, P., 2017. Training of a dog for
0-12-417156-5.00001-0. the monitoring of Osmoderma eremita. Nat. Conserv. 20, 237–264. https://doi.org/
Göthlin, E., Schroeder, L.M., Lindelöw, A., 2000. Attacks by Ips typographus and 10.3897/natureconservation.20.12688.
Pityogenes chalcographus on Windthrown Spruces (Picea abies) During the Two Years Moser, E., McCulloch, M., 2010. Canine scent detection of human cancers: A review of
Following a Storm Felling. Scand. J. For. Res. 15, 542–549. https://doi.org/ methods and accuracy. J. Veterinary Behav. 5, 145–152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
10.1080/028275800750173492. jveb.2010.01.002.
Hartmann, H., Bastos, A., Das, A.J., Esquivel-Muelbert, A., Hammond, W.M., Martínez- Näsi, R., Honkavaara, E., Lyytikäinen-Saarenmaa, P., Blomqvist, M., Litkey, P.,
Vilalta, J., McDowell, N.G., Powers, J.S., Pugh, T.A.M., Ruthrof, K.X., Allen, C.D., Hakala, T., Viljanen, N., Kantola, T., Tanhuanpää, T., Holopainen, M., 2015. Using
2022. Climate change risks to global forest health: emergence of unexpected events UAV-based photogrammetry and hyperspectral imaging for mapping bark beetle
of elevated tree mortality worldwide. Annu Rev Plant Biol 73, 673–702. https://doi. damage at tree-level. Remote Sensing. 7, 15467–15493. https://doi.org/10.3390/
org/10.1146/annurev-arplant-102820-012804. rs71115467.
Hepper, P., Wells, D., 2015. Olfaction in the Order Carnivora: Family Canidae. In: Netherer, S., Kandasamy, D., Jirosová, A., Kalinová, B., Schebeck, M., Schlyter, F., 2021.
Doty, R.L. (Ed.), Handbook of Olfaction and Gustation. Wiley-Blackwell, Interactions among Norway spruce, the bark beetle Ips typographus and its fungal
pp. 591–603. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118971758. symbionts in times of drought. J. Pest. Sci. 94, 591–614. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Hlásny, T., Zajíčková, L., Turčáni, M., Holuša, J., Sitková, Z., 2011. Geographical s10340-021-01341-y.
variability of spruce bark beetle development under climate change in the Czech Netherer, S., Nopp-Mayr, U., 2005. Predisposition assessment systems (PAS) as
Republic. J. Forest Sci. 57 (No. 6), 242–249. supportive tools in forest management-rating of site and stand-related hazards of
Hlásny, T., Krokene, P., Liebhold, A., Montagné-Huck, C., Müller, J., Qin, H., Raffa, K., bark beetle infestation in the High Tatra Mountains as an example for system
Schelhaas, M.-J., Seidl, R., Svoboda, M., 2019. Living with bark beetles: impacts, application and verification. For. Ecol. Manage. 207 (1–2), 99–107. https://doi.org/
outlook and management options. European Forest Institute. 8 https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.foreco.2004.10.020.
10.36333/fs08. Pfiester, M., Koehler, P.G., Pereira, R.M., 2008. Ability of bed bug-detecting canines to
Hlásny, T., Merganicova, K., Modlinger, R., Marušák, R., 2021a. Prognosis of bark beetle locate live bed bugs and viable bed bug eggs. J. Econ. Entomol. 101, 1389–1396.
outbreak and a new platform for the dissemination of information about the forests https://doi.org/10.1603/0022-0493(2008)101[1389:AOBBCT]2.0.CO;2.
in the Czech Republic. Forestry Reports. 66, 197–205. R CORE TEAM, 2020. The R Project for Statistical Computing [WWW Document]. URL
Hlásny, T., Zimová, S., Merganičová, K., Štěpánek, P., Modlinger, R., Turčáni, M., 2021b. https://www.r-project.org/ (accessed 3.4.22).
Devastating outbreak of bark beetles in the Czech Republic: Drivers, impacts, and Rebmann, A., David, E., Sorg, M.H., Song, M.H., 2000. Cadaver Dog Handbook: Forensic
management implications. For. Ecol. Manage. 490, 119075 https://doi.org/ Training and Tactics for the Recovery of Human Remains. CRC Press, p. 232.
10.1016/j.foreco.2021.119075. Stříbrská, B., Hradecký, J., Čepl, J., Tomášková, I., Jakuš, R., Modlinger, R., Netherer, S.,
Hoyer-Tomiczek, U., Hoch, G., 2020. Progress in the use of detection dogs for emerald Jirošová, A., 2022. Forest margins provide favourable microclimatic niches to
ash borer monitoring. Forestry 93, 326–330. https://doi.org/10.1093/forestry/ swarming bark beetles, but Norway spruce trees were not attacked by Ips typographus
cpaa001. shortly after edge creation in a field experiment. For. Ecol. Manage. 506, 119950
Hoyer-Tomiczek, U., Sauseng, G., Hoch, G., 2016. Scent detection dogs for the Asian https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foreco.2021.119950.
longhorn beetle. Anoplophora glabripennis. EPPO Bulletin. 46, 148–155. https://doi. Suma, P., la Pergola, A., Longo, S., Soroker, V., 2014. The use of sniffing dogs for the
org/10.1111/epp.12282. detection of Rhynchophorus ferrugineus. Phytoparasitica. 42, 269–274. https://doi.
Huo, L., Persson, H.J., Lindberg, E., 2021. Early detection of forest stress from European org/10.1007/s12600-013-0330-0.
spruce bark beetle attack, and a new vegetation index: Normalized distance red & TOLASZ, R., MÍKOVá, T., VALERIáNOVá, A, VOŽENÍLEK, V. (eds) 2007. Climate Atlas of Czechia.
SWIR (NDRS). Remote Sens. Environ. 255, 112240 https://doi.org/10.1016/j. 1st edition. Prague, Olomouc: Czech Hydrometeorological Institute, Palackého
rse.2020.112240. University. pp 255. ISBN 978-80-86690-26-1.
IPCC, Climate Change 2022. Impacts, Adaptation, and Vulnerability. Contribution of Wermelinger, B., 2004. Ecology and management of the spruce bark beetle Ips
Working Group II to the Sixth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on typographus - A review of recent research. For. Ecol. Manage. 202, 67–82. https://
Climate Change [WWW document]. 2022. URL https://www.ipcc.ch/report/ar6/ doi.org/10.1016/j.foreco.2004.07.018.
wg2/. Wulder, M.A., Dymond, C.C., White, J.C., Leckie, D.G., Carroll, A.L., 2006. Surveying
Jakuš, R., 1998. Types of bark beetle (Coleoptera: Scolytidae) infestation in spruce forest mountain pine beetle damage of forests: A review of remote sensing opportunities.
stands affected by air pollution, bark beetle outbreak and honey fungus (Armillaria For. Ecol. Manage. 221, 27–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foreco.2005.09.021.
mellea). Anzeiger Fur Schadlingskunde Pflanzenschutz Umweltschutz. 71, 41–49. Zabihi, K., Surovy, P., Trubin, A., Singh, V.V., Jakuš, R., 2021. A review of major factors
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02770619. influencing the accuracy of mapping green-attack stage of bark beetle infestations
Jendrny, P., Schulz, C., Twele, F., Meller, S., von Köckritz-Blickwede, M., Osterhaus, A.D.
M.E., Ebbers, J., Pilchová, V., Pink, I., Welte, T., Manns, M.P., Fathi, A., Ernst, C.,
Addo, M.M., Schalke, E., Volk, H.A., 2020. Scent dog identification of samples from
COVID-19 patients – a pilot study. BMC Infect. Dis. 20, 5281. https://doi.org/
10.1186/s12879-020-05281-3.

9
N. Vošvrdová et al. Forest Ecology and Management 528 (2023) 120626

using satellite imagery: Prospects to avoid data redundancy. Remote Sens. Appl.: Zuur, A.F., Ieno, E.N., Elphick, C.S., 2010. A protocol for data exploration to avoid
Soc. Environ. 24, 100638 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rsase.2021.100638. common statistical problems. Methods Ecol. Evol. 1, 3–14. https://doi.org/10.1111/
Zuur, A.F., Ieno, E.N., Walker, N., Saveliev, A.A., Smith, G.M., 2009. Mixed effects j.2041-210x.2009.00001.x.
models and extensions in ecology with R. Statistics for Biology and Health. Springer
Science & Business Media, p. 574.

10

You might also like