MC Kenzie Hoyle 2008

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/229102284

The Self-Absorption Scale: Reliability and validity in non-clinical samples

Article  in  Personality and Individual Differences · December 2008


DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.07.020

CITATIONS READS

51 3,732

2 authors:

Karyn S. Mckenzie Rick H Hoyle


Georgetown College Duke University
2 PUBLICATIONS   267 CITATIONS    152 PUBLICATIONS   19,504 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Rick H Hoyle on 01 September 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Author's personal copy

Personality and Individual Differences 45 (2008) 726–731

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

The Self-Absorption Scale: Reliability and validity in non-clinical samples


Karyn S. McKenzie a, Rick H. Hoyle b,*
a
Department of Psychology, 400 East College Street, Georgetown College, Georgetown, KY 40324, USA
b
Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Box 90086, 9 Flowers Drive, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708-0086, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The lack of a measure of self-absorption, an excessive, sustained, and rigid focus on the self, has limited
Received 16 January 2008 progress in research on the role of attentional processes in psychopathology. We describe the develop-
Received in revised form 8 July 2008 ment of a measure of self-absorption that yields scores for private and public self-absorption. Factor anal-
Accepted 24 July 2008
yses indicated that the measure has a clean factor structure, with two correlated factors. In a preliminary
Available online 31 August 2008
validation study using non-clinical participants, we embedded the private and public self-absorption con-
structs in a nomological network of constructs that implicate attentional processes. Both forms of self-
Keywords:
absorption are correlated with a wide array of constructs in a pattern that corresponds to the conceptual
Self-awareness
Self-consciousness
definition of the construct. They also evidence incremental validity over their self-consciousness counter-
Self-regulation parts in associations with key constructs. These preliminary findings provide a firm basis for investiga-
tions of the role of self-absorption in psychopathology.
Ó 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction ‘‘self-absorption is defined by excessive, sustained, and rigid atten-


tion to information emanating from internal sources” (p. 169). In-
Self-awareness is a critical feature of conscious experience that gram argued that self-absorption is a common thread that runs
gives rise to adaptive capacities such as an enduring, cognitive rep- through many forms of pathological cognition that involve self-
resentation of self and the ability to establish and pursue goals that awareness.
are tied to this self-representation (Sedikides & Skowronski, 1997). Although Ingram’s (1990) analysis is persuasive, the self-absorp-
Yet, the capacity for self-awareness is a two-edged sword. Self- tion construct has not assumed a place of prominence in relevant
awareness is critical for the self-regulation of behavior (Hoyle & literatures. Two reasons for this limited influence are apparent.
Sowards, 1993) and emotional experience (Carver & Scheier, First, no measure or manipulation of self-absorption has been
1990); however, self-awareness is associated with maladaptive developed. Second, Ingram’s conceptualization does not distinguish
patterns of cognition, affect, and behavior (Baumeister, 1991; Teas- between private and public self-awareness, a distinction that has
dale & Green, 2004; Watkins & Teasdale, 2004). Virtually all of the proven useful, for example, for distinguishing manifestations of
research on the role of self-awareness in maladaptive processes self-conscious emotions (e.g. Gilbert, 2007). This distinction would
has concerned self-awareness in a form typical of adaptive func- be important for integrating the self-absorption construct into the
tioning. The goal of the present research was to develop and vali- large literature on psychopathology and self-awareness as mea-
date a measure of self-awareness tailored to the study of sured by the Self-Consciousness Scale (SCS; Fenigstein, Scheier &
maladaptive processes. Buss 1975), which yields separate private and public scores.
Research of indirect relevance to the private form of self-
1.1. Self-absorption absorption has begun to accumulate as a result of multiple factor
analyses showing two distinct forms of private self-consciousness:
Based on a comprehensive review of the literature on self- internal state awareness (ISA) and self-reflectiveness (SR; see Trap-
awareness and psychopathology, Ingram (1990) concluded that nell & Campbell, 1999, for a review). Whereas ISA reflects a basic
the form of self-awareness that accompanies maladjustment awareness of self, SR reflects an evaluative examination of self.
differs in degree and kind from ‘‘normal” self-awareness. Ingram The SR items reflect the degree component of self-absorption and
proposed and elaborated the ‘‘pathological equivalent” of self- evince a pattern of correlations similar to what might be expected
awareness, self-absorption. He posited that self-awareness varies for private self-absorption (Mor & Winquist, 2002); however, they
along three dimensions-degree, duration, and flexibility-and that do not explicitly tap the duration and flexibility components of the
construct.
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +919 660 5791; fax: +919 660 5726. Building on this finding of a reliable bifurcation of private self-
E-mail address: rhoyle@duke.edu (R.H. Hoyle). consciousness items, Trapnell and Campbell (1999) developed the

0191-8869/$ - see front matter Ó 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.07.020
Author's personal copy

K.S. McKenzie, R.H. Hoyle / Personality and Individual Differences 45 (2008) 726–731 727

Rumination-Reflection Questionnaire (RRQ). As with the SR items, items, to which participants in the main sample responded. Among
the rumination items tap aspects of Ingram’s (1990) model of self- the candidate items were 16 concerning private 19 concerning
absorption but do not fully reflect the construct. Although public self-absorption. The goal of analyses was the selection of
perseveration of specific self-referent thoughts is one form of the 8–10 items for private and public self-absorption that yielded
self-absorption with maladaptive consequences (Pyszczynski & the cleanest factor structure.
Greenberg, 1987), it is not difficult to imagine excessive, sustained, Participants indicated the degree to which the statements de-
and rigid attention to self more generally and without rumination scribed them on five-point scales anchored by not at all like me
(Lyubomirsky & Tkach, 2004). Moreover, rumination does not and very much like me. For the main sample, the candidate items
always concern the self (Papageorgiou & Wells, 2004). Thus, a were embedded in a battery of questionnaires; the order of appear-
measure of self-absorption would complement, but not duplicate, ance of the candidate item set in the battery was randomized. Par-
measures of rumination. ticipants in the cross-validation sample completed only the final
Neither the RRQ nor the public subscale of the SCS provide set of items.
items that tap public self-absorption. A measure of maladaptive
attention to public self-aspects would be valuable for research on 2.2. Results and discussion
disordered cognition, affect, and behavior (e.g. social anxiety,
excessive shame and guilt) that stems from attention to self from 2.2.1. Exploratory factor analysis
the imagined perspective of others. Such a measure also would Initially, we examined communality estimates from an unrotat-
contribute to more theory-oriented research on the components ed principal axes factoring, eliminating seven items because they
of self-consciousness by offering parallel self-absorption measures. generated relatively little common variance (h2 < .25). Further
exploratory factor analyses focused on the 28 remaining candidate
1.2. The present studies items.
Because our goal was a clean two-factor solution, we extracted
Our goal was to develop and conduct preliminary validation and obliquely rotated two factors. The elimination of items with
work on a brief self-report measure of private and public self- loadings less than .50 or secondary loadings greater than .20 re-
absorption that follows directly from Ingram’s (1990) conceptual sulted in eight private and 13 public items. In order to achieve sub-
definition of self-absorption. We expected our measure to be mod- scales of equivalent length, we eliminated the four remaining
estly correlated with measures that reflect normal self-awareness. public self-absorption items with the lowest loadings. This left a
Moreover, we expected our measure to afford incremental predic- set of 17 items, of which eight formed a private subscale and nine
tion over such measures for constructs that involve a maladaptive a public subscale.
focus on the self. We initially evaluated the factor structure of the 17-item set
using exploratory factor analysis. A comparison of eigenvalues
from a principal axis extraction with random-data eigenvalues
2. Study 1 (Hoyle & Duvall, 2004) suggested five factors. An oblique rotation
of five factors indicated that the public and private item sets split
Our efforts at developing the measure were guided by two into two highly correlated factors (interfactor rs = .71. and .72).
goals: (1) ensure that items tap the excessive, sustained, and rigid No item loaded above .40 on the fifth factor. The public items split
qualities that distinguish self-absorption from normal self-aware- into four and five item sets according to whether the items explic-
ness; (2) include items for private and public self-absorption in itly mentioned evaluation rather than simply viewing self from the
sufficient numbers to enable subscale scores to be generated. To perspective of others. The private items split into three and five
achieve these goals, we wrote a large number of candidate items, item sets that differed in terms of whether they explicitly men-
to which responses were provided by a large sample of nonclinical tioned self as opposed to one’s life more broadly.
participants. We used judges’ ratings as well as exploratory and Although a strictly exploratory analysis pointed to four factors
confirmatory factor analysis to reduce the candidate item pool to rather than two, the four factors were simple splits of the subscales
a final set. on qualities not relevant to the self-absorption construct. An anal-
ysis in which we extracted and obliquely rotated two factors
2.1. Method yielded a near simple-structure pattern of loadings. The primary
loadings of the eight private self-absorption items ranged from
2.1.1. Participants .52 to .65, and the highest secondary loading was .05. The primary
Nine hundred and twelve undergraduates provided responses loadings for the public self-absorption items ranged from .59 to
to the candidate items for course credit. Data from 12 participants .73. The highest secondary loading was .15, but the average sec-
were unusable; hence, the main analysis sample comprised 900 ondary loading across the nine items was only .08. The inter-factor
participants (592 females, 302 males, 6 unspecified). correlation was .53.
An additional 234 undergraduates constituted a cross-valida- A subset of the main sample (N = 210) also completed the SCS,
tion sample. which permitted us to conduct an exploratory factor analysis that
included composite private and public self-consciousness scores as
2.1.2. Item generation marker variables. We repeated the two-factor analysis and exam-
Our goal was a measure comprising 15–20 items, with private ined the alignment of the self-consciousness composites with the
and public self-absorption subscales of equivalent length. To this self-absorption factors. The public self-consciousness composite
end, we generated a large pool of candidate items, each designed loaded with the public self-absorption items (loading = .66) but
to reflect primary characteristics of self-absorption as defined by not the private self-absorption items (loading = .10). The private
Ingram (1990). After training in the definition of the construct, self-consciousness composite aligned more closely with the
two psychology graduate students rated the degree to which each private self-absorption items (loading = .27) than the public self-
candidate item reflected the construct definition and a private or absorption items (loading = .18), but the absolute value of the
public focus. A candidate item was retained in the pool if the loading was smaller than the standard salience criterion of .30.
judges agreed that it unambiguously referenced private or public Following the norm in the self-consciousness literature, we re-
self-absorption. This process resulted in a pool of 35 candidate scored the private self-consciousness subscale into internal state
Author's personal copy

728 K.S. McKenzie, R.H. Hoyle / Personality and Individual Differences 45 (2008) 726–731

awareness and self-reflectiveness composites and re-ran the anal- women and men. Two hundred and three participants from the
ysis. Consistent with the literature on these forms of private self- main sample completed the measure a second time about seven
consciousness, internal state awareness loaded on neither factor weeks later. The test-retest correlations were .60 for private and
(loadings were .03 and .09 for private and public, respectively), .73 for public self-absorption.
but self-reflectiveness loaded with the private (loading = .40) but Comparisons of mean scores for women and men revealed a sig-
not the public items (loading = .24). nificant sex difference on private self-absorption, F(1, 892) = 4.40,
p < .05. Women (M = 1.74), on average, scored higher than men
2.2.2. Confirmatory factor analysis (M = 1.66); however, the difference was small (effect size r = .07).
In order to more rigorously evaluate the factor structure of our Mean scores for women (M = 2.44) and men (M = 2.55) on public
item set, we used confirmatory factor analysis (Hoyle, 2000) to fit a self-absorption were not significantly different, F(1, 892) = 3.13,
two-correlated-factor model using data from the cross-validation p = .08, r = .06.
sample. We contrasted this hypothesized model with two more
parsimonious models, one with a single factor and one with two
uncorrelated factors. To evaluate model fit, we consulted the com- 3. Study 2
parative fit index (CFI; Bentler, 1990), and the root mean square er-
ror of approximation (RMSEA; Steiger, 1990). We use the v2 The purpose of study 2 was to examine the convergent and dis-
estimate only for comparing nested models. A value of .90 is com- criminant validity of the Self-Absorption Scale.
monly used as a cutoff for CFI, but .95 has been recommended for
measurement models (Hu & Bentler, 1999). At this preliminary 3.1. Method
stage, we used .90 for models that were fully hypothesis driven
and included no adjustments to maximize model fit. For RMSEA, 3.1.1. Participants
values between 0 and .08 indicate acceptable fit (MacCallum, Two hundred and three participants, who were part of the main
Browne, & Sugawara, 1996). sample for study 1, provided data for the study.
Values of the fit indices indicated that the hypothesized model
provided an acceptable account of the data, v2(118, 3.1.2. Materials and procedure
N = 234) = 203.65, p < .001; CFI = .91, RMSEA = .07. This model Participants completed a battery of questionnaires in which the
clearly outperformed both a one-factor model, Dv2(1, Self-Absorption Scale appeared first; the order of the remaining
N = 234) = 133.35, p < .001, and a model with uncorrelated factors, questionnaires was randomized.
Dv2(1, N = 234) = 68.57, p < .001. These results provided initial We assessed five constructs that primarily concern intraper-
support for the hypothesized structure of the Self-Absorption sonal processes. Private self-consciousness was assessed by the pri-
Scale, which is shown in Table 1. vate subscale from the SCS (Fenigstein et al., 1975). The 10 items
The full set of loadings (all significant at p < .001) appears in the were scored both as a single composite and as separate internal
last two columns of Table 1. The average loading was .58 for the state awareness and self-reflectiveness composites. Self-esteem
eight private items and .66 for the nine public items. The inter-fac- and self-efficacy were measured using the Self-Esteem Scale
tor correlation was .67. (Rosenberg, 1965) and the General Self-Efficacy Scale (Sherer
et al., 1982), respectively. Depressive symptoms were assessed using
2.2.3. Subscale characteristics the Beck depression inventory (Beck, 1978). Finally, the ruminative
Coefficient alpha was .81 for the private items and .89 for the responses subscale from the response styles questionnaire (Nolen-
public items. Internal consistency estimates were equivalent for Hoeksema & Morrow, 1991) was used to measure rumination.

Table 1
Items, descriptive statistics, and factor loadings for items of the Self-Absorption Scale

Factor
Item M SD PrSA PubSA
Private self-absorption
4. I think about myself more than anything else. 1.85 1.05 .61
5. When I try to think of something other than myself, I cannot. 1.26 0.61 .55
6. When I have to perform a task, I do not do it as well as I should because my concentration is interrupted with thoughts of 1.42 0.77 .57
myself instead of the task.
9. My mind never focuses on things other than myself for very long. 1.47 0.76 .55
11. I cannot stop my head from thinking thoughts about myself. 1.70 0.96 .69
12. Sometimes I am so deep in thought about my life I am not aware of my surroundings. 1.80 1.08 .53
14. I do not spend long amounts of time thinking about myselfa 2.40 1.16 .48
15. When I think about my life, I keep thinking about it so long I cannot turn my attention to tasks that need to be done. 1.61 0.87 .65
Public self-absorption
1. I find myself wondering what others think of me even when I don’t want to. 3.15 1.29 .65
2. I have difficulty focusing on what others are talking about because I wonder what they’re thinking of me. 1.93 1.08 .68
3. I feel like others are constantly evaluating me when I’m with them. 2.39 1.24 .75
7. I wish others weren’t as critical of me as they are. 2.27 1.17 .63
8. I am very aware of what others think of me, and it bothers me. 2.07 1.09 .71
10. When I start thinking about how others view me, I get all worked up. 1.86 1.03 .73
13. It upsets me when people I meet don’t like me. 3.20 1.33 .57
16. When I’m about to meet someone for the first time, I worry about whether they’ll like me. 3.05 1.33 .59
17. After being around other people, I think about what I should have done differently when I was with them. 2.85 1.30 .63

Note: PrSA = private self-absorption. PubSA = public self-absorption. Factor loadings are completely standardized maximum likelihood estimates. Off-loadings were con-
strained to zero.
a
Reverse scored item; descriptive statistics and factor loading reflect reverse scoring.
Author's personal copy

K.S. McKenzie, R.H. Hoyle / Personality and Individual Differences 45 (2008) 726–731 729

We also assessed five constructs that primarily concern inter- independent correlations of private and public self-absorption
personal processes. Using the Self-Consciousness Scale (Fenigstein with depressive symptoms and rumination are considered, an
et al., 1975), public self-consciousness and social anxiety were mea- unexpected pattern emerges: Both aspects of self-absorption are
sured. Social desirability, the need for approval or avoidance of dis- independently related to depressive symptoms and rumination;
approval from others, was assessed by the Crowne–Marlowe Social however, public self-absorption is significantly more strongly re-
Desirability Scale (Crowne & Marlowe, 1960). The 25-item Self- lated to depressive symptoms than is private self-absorption
Monitoring Scale (Snyder, 1974) was used to measure self-monitor- (p < .05).
ing. We assessed extraversion using Goldberg’s (1992) lexical-based
measure. 3.2.2. Nomological network: Interpersonal constructs
Coefficient alpha for all measures is displayed in the rightmost Relevant correlation coefficients are displayed in the bottom
column of Table 2. portion of Table 2. Both forms of self-absorption were correlated
with public self-consciousness; however, the public self-absorp-
3.2. Results and discussion tion correlation was the stronger of the two. When public self-
absorption was partialed from private self-absorption, that correla-
The first two columns of coefficients in Table 2 reflect the sim- tion dropped to near zero, whereas partialing private from public
ple association between private or public self-absorption and the self-absorption affected the correlation of the latter with public
various individual differences. The next two columns include self-consciousness minimally. A virtually identical pattern
semi-partial coefficients that partial public from private and pri- emerged for social anxiety. Both facets of self-absorption are re-
vate from public self-absorption before each is correlated with lated to social anxiety, but it is the unique variability in public
the individual differences. self-absorption that drives the association.
Two interpersonal constructs, social desirability and self-moni-
3.2.1. Nomological network: intrapersonal constructs toring, concern self-presentation. Only the correlation with public
Both aspects of self-absorption were correlated with private self-absorption remained significant in the semi-partial analyses;
self-consciousness. The semi-partial coefficients indicate that the however, the two semi-partial correlations are not significantly
apparent correlation between public self-absorption and private different. Both private and public self-absorption are correlated
self-consciousness is attributable to the overlap between private with self-monitoring. In the semi-partial analyses, only the corre-
and public self-absorption. When private self-consciousness is bro- lation involving public self-absorption remains significant. Fol-
ken out into internal state awareness and self-reflectiveness com- low-up analyses indicate that this association is wholly
ponents, it is clear that the self-reflectiveness component is attributable to the other-directedness dimension of self-monitor-
responsible for these associations. ing. Taken together, these associations indicate that individuals
The zero-order correlations involving self-esteem and self-effi- high in public self-absorption are more likely than individuals
cacy are similar for private and public self-absorption. Participants low in public-self-absorption to look to other people for behavioral
who were more self-absorbed-private or public-were lower in self- cues (other-directedness) but less likely to need other people’s ap-
esteem and self-efficacy. The semi-partial analysis indicates that proval of their behavior (social desirability).
the self-esteem association is primarily attributable to public We expected a negative correlation between public self-absorp-
self-absorption. Both private and public self-absorption are inde- tion and extraversion because, although individuals high in public
pendently related to self-efficacy. self-absorption are highly aware of how others perceive them, they
Depressive symptoms and rumination are associated (e.g. are uncomfortable around others. The zero-order correlations
Lyubomirsky, Caldwell, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1998); hence, we con- involving extraversion were negative and significant for both pri-
sidered their association with self-absorption together. When the vate and public self-absorption, but the correlation with public
self-absorption was significantly stronger. When public self-
absorption was partialed from private self-absorption, the correla-
tion between private self-absorption and extraversion approached
zero. Conversely, the correlation between public self-absorption
Table 2
Correlations of public and private self-absorption with other individual differences
and extraversion remained strong even when controlling for pri-
vate self-absorption.
Zero-order Semi-partial
Measure Private Public Private Public Alpha 3.2.3. Incremental validity
Self-absorption We next undertook a set of analyses to evaluate the degree to
Private .83 which private and public self-absorption are predictive of selected
Public .53** .90 constructs beyond well-established associations of those con-
Intrapersonal Constructs structs with private and public self-consciousness. We first consid-
Private self-consciousness .37** .30** .26** .12 .53 ered the association between private self-consciousness and
Internal state awareness .09 .03 .12 .09 .32
depressive symptoms (e.g. Smith & Greenberg, 1981). Recall that
Self-reflectiveness .37** .35** .22** .18* .69
Self-esteem .31** .40** .12 .28** .83 the zero-order correlation between private self-absorption and
Self-efficacy .38** .37** .21** .20** .84 BDI scores was .35. The zero-order correlation between private
Depressive symptoms .35** .41** .15* .27** .91 self-consciousness and depressive symptoms was .37. When pri-
Rumination .36** .41** .17* .26** .93
vate self-absorption was partialed from private self-consciousness,
Interpersonal Constructs the correlation dropped to .08 (p > .20). When private self-con-
Public self-consciousness .33** .54** .05 .43** .73
sciousness was partialed from private self-absorption, the correla-
Social anxiety .32** .50** .07 .39** .68
Social desirability .27** .30** .13 .18* .84
tion between the latter and depressive symptoms was .29. At the
Self-monitoring .15* .20* .05 .14* .68 subscale level, internal state awareness was not correlated with
Extraversion .18* .32** .01 .27** .92 depressive symptoms and did not alter the association between
Note: Ns ranged from 200 to 203.
private self-absorption and depressive symptoms. Self-reflective-
*
p < .05. ness was correlated .23 with depressive symptoms, but the corre-
**
p < .01. lation dropped to.11 (p > .10) when private self-absorption was
Author's personal copy

730 K.S. McKenzie, R.H. Hoyle / Personality and Individual Differences 45 (2008) 726–731

partialed out. The association between private self-absorption and In conclusion, the Self-Absorption Scale is a viable alternative to
depressive symptoms was minimally affected by controlling for measures of normal self-awareness for research on attentional pro-
self-reflectiveness (sr = .28). This finding is consistent with In- cesses and psychological dysfunction. Unlike measures of normal
gram’s (1990) argument that it is self-absorption, not normal self-awareness and rumination, the Self-Absorption Scale captures
self-awareness, that accompanies mood disorders such as in the broadest sense the extended duration and maladaptive
depression. rigidity that distinguish pathological from normal self-awareness
We conducted a similar analysis involving public self-absorp- (Ingram, 1990; see also, Muraven (2005)). This qualitative
tion and social anxiety. At the zero-order level, public self-absorp- distinction may prove critical to an understanding of how self-
tion is correlated r = .50, and public self-consciousness r = .41, with awareness should be incorporated into process models of
social anxiety. When public self-consciousness is partialed from psychopathology.
public self-absorption, the correlation drops but remains signifi-
cant at sr = .34. When public self-absorption is partialed from pub-
References
lic self-consciousness, the correlation involving public self-
consciousness remains significant but drops to a relatively modest Baumeister, R. F. (1990). Suicide as escape from self. Psychological Review, 97,
value of r = .16. 90–113.
Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Escaping the self: Alcoholism, spirituality, masochism, and
other flights from the burden of selfhood. New York: Basic Books.
Beck, A. T. (1978). Beck depression inventory. San Antonio, TX: Psychological
4. General discussion Corporation.
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psychological
Bulletin, 107, 238–246.
Our findings provide preliminary support for the Self-Absorp- Buss, A. H. (1980). Self-consciousness and social anxiety. San Francisco: Freeman.
tion Scale, a measure of excessive, rigid, and sustained self-aware- Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative
ness. The measure is brief, self-administered, and yields separate affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19–35.
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent
scores for private and public self-absorption. Although the private of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349–354.
and public subscales are appreciably correlated, when one is par- Fejfar, M. C., & Hoyle, R. H. (2000). Effect of private self-awareness on negative affect
tialed from the other, predicted associations of each with other and self-referent attribution: A quantitative review. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 4, 132–142.
constructs persist. Importantly, associations involving the self- Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self-
absorption subscales and key outcomes persist when the corre- consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
sponding self-consciousness subscale is partialed. Although the Psychology, 43, 522–527.
Gilbert, P. (2007). The evolution of shame as a marker for relationship security: A
items do not refer specifically to self-directed affect, the subscales
biopsychosocial approach. In J. L. Tracy, R. W. Robins, & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), The
relate as expected to affective outcomes such as depressive symp- self-conscious emotions: Theory and research (pp. 283–309). New York: Guilford.
toms and social anxiety. Goldberg, L. R. (1992). The development of markers for the big-five factor structure.
Psychological Assessment, 4, 26–42.
To date, in virtually every study in which self-awareness was
Hoyle, R. H. (2000). Confirmatory factor analysis. In H. E. A. Tinsley & S. D. Brown
measured rather than manipulated, self-awareness was operation- (Eds.), Handbook of applied multivariate statistics and mathematical modeling
ally defined using the SCS. In studies that focus on adaptive behav- (pp. 465–497). New York: Academic Press.
ior this is not ill-advised because it is now well-established that the Hoyle, R. H., & Duvall, J. L. (2004). Determining the number of factors in exploratory
and confirmatory factor analysis. In D. Kaplan (Ed.), Handbook of quantitative
private self-consciousness subscale from the SCS is a proxy for the methodology for the social sciences (pp. 301–315). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
transient state of self-awareness induced by environmental factors Publications.
(e.g. Fejfar & Hoyle, 2000). In studies that focus on maladaptive af- Hoyle, R. H., & Sowards, B. A. (1993). Self-monitoring and the regulation of social
experience. A control-process model. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 12,
fect and cognition, however, self-consciousness is an unsuitable 280–306.
substitute for the pathological form of self-awareness that is most Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure
relevant for psychopathology (Ingram, 1990). Although extreme analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation
Modeling, 6, 1–55.
scores on measures of self-consciousness adequately capture the Ingram, R. E. (1990). Self-focused attention in clinical disorders: Review and a
quantitative difference between self-consciousness and self- conceptual model. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 156–176.
absorption, they do not capture the qualitative distinctions— Lyubomirsky, S., Caldwell, N. D., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Effects of ruminative
and distracting responses to depressed mood on retrieval of autobiographical
namely, the sustained and inflexible nature of self-absorption.
memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 166–177.
Similarly, measures of rumination capture a specific instance of Lyubomirsky, S., & Tkach, C. (2004). The consequences of dysphoric rumination. In
self-absorption but not the construct in its broader sense. The C. Papageorgiou & A. Wells (Eds.), Depressive rumination: Nature, theory, and
treatment (pp. 21–41). West Sussex, England: Wiley.
Self-Absorption Scale captures all aspects of pathological self-
MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., & Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and
awareness, providing a means of empirically validating models of determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychological
psychopathology that implicate self-attention and self-regulation Methods, 1, 130–149.
(e.g. Baumeister, 1990; Pyszczynski & Greenberg, 1987). Mor, N., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-
analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 638–662.
A feature of our measure that extends the conceptual model Muraven, M. (2005). Self-focused attention and the self-regulation of attention:
developed by Ingram (1990) is the distinction between private Implications for personality and pathology. Journal of Social and Clinical
and public self-absorption. The distinction between private and Psychology, 24, 382–400.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and
public self-consciousness elaborated by Buss (1980) and mani- posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta
fested in the SCS has given rise to two rich and relatively distinct earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 115–121.
literatures. Whereas private self-consciousness has been central Papageorgiou, C., & Wells, A. (2004). Nature, functions, and beliefs about depressive
rumination. In C. Papageorgiou & A. Wells (Eds.), Depressive rumination: Nature,
in research on intrapersonal processes (e.g. Trapnell & Campbell, theory, and treatment (pp. 3–20). West Sussex, England: Wiley.
1999), public self-consciousness has contributed to research on a Pontari, B. A., & Schlenker, B. R. (2000). The influence of cognitive load on self-
range of interpersonal processes (e.g. Pontari & Schlenker, 2000). presentation: Can cognitive busyness help as well as harm social performance?
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1092–1108.
Similarly, we might expect private and public self-absorption to
Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Self-regulatory perseveration and the
contribute to the prediction and explanation of different forms of depressive self-focusing style: A self-awareness theory of reactive depression.
psychopathology and maladjustment. By building the private-pub- Psychological Bulletin, 102, 122–138.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
lic distinction into our measure, we offer the opportunity to build
University.
on work inspired by the SCS while extending the conceptual anal- Sedikides, C., & Skowronski, J. J. (1997). The symbolic self in evolutionary context.
ysis of pathological self-awareness. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 80–102.
Author's personal copy

K.S. McKenzie, R.H. Hoyle / Personality and Individual Differences 45 (2008) 726–731 731

Sherer, M., Maddux, J. E., Mercandante, B., Prentice-Dunn, S., Jacobs, B., & Rogers, R. Teasdale, J. D., & Green, H. A. C. (2004). Ruminative self-focus and
W. (1982). The Self-Efficacy Scale: Construction and validation. Psychological autobiographical memory. Personality and Individual Differences, 36,
Reports, 51, 663–671. 1933–1943.
Smith, T. W., & Greenberg, J. (1981). Depression and self-focused attention. Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-
Motivation and Emotion, 5, 323–331. factor model of personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal
Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 284–304.
Social Psychology, 30, 526–537. Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Adaptive and maladaptive self-focus in
Steiger, J. H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 82, 1–8.
estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25, 173–180.

View publication stats

You might also like