Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

2000, Vol. 78, No. 4, 791-808 0022-3514/00/$5.00 DOI: 10.1O37//0O22-3514.78.4791

The Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal and Relationships

Susan E. Cross, Pamela L. Bacon, and Michael L. Morris


Iowa State University

Three studies describe the development and validation of a measure of the relational-interdependent
self-construal, which is defined as the tendency to think of oneself in terms of relationships with close
others. Study 1 reports the development, psychometric properties, and tests of validity of this new
measure. Individuals who scored high on the Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal (RISC) Scale
characterized their important relationships as closer and more committed than did individuals who scored
low on diis measure (Study 1) and were more likely to take into account the needs and wishes of others
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

when making decisions (Study 2). In Study 3, using a dyadic interaction paradigm with previously
unacquainted participants, the partners of persons who scored high on the RISC scale viewed them as
open and responsive to their needs and concerns; these perceptions were related to positive evaluations
of the relationship.

Cross-cultural psychologists have recently identified a basic (we explain this further below). Although this difference in self-
dimension that differentiates how people think about themselves— structure has been identified by other researchers (e.g., Lykes,
the degree to which elements of the social world (such as close 1985; Surrey, 1991), a measure of the interdependent self-
relationships, contexts for behavior, or important group member- construal appropriate for Western populations has not been avail-
ships) are included in the self (Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Shwe- able for interested researchers. In this article we describe the
der & Bourne, 1984; Triandis, 1989). They argue that members of development and validation of a new measure of the interdepen-
collectivist cultures, such as Japan or India, tend to think of dent self-construal.
themselves as interdependent with close others and as defined by
important roles and situations. In contrast, members of Western
Individual Differences in the Self-Construal
cultures, such as the United States, tend to think of themselves as
independent of relationships and as autonomous or separated from For years, Western researchers have assumed a single model of
others. the self—what has been termed the independent self-construal
There may be considerable variation within North American (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). This understanding of the self is
culture with respect to the self-construal, however. Members of based on Euro-American beliefs about individualism, personal
many ethnic and religious groups tend to think of themselves as rights, and the autonomy of the individual from social groups
interdependent or relational (Allen, Dawson, & Brown, 1989; (Bellah, Madsen, Sullivan, Swidler, & Tipton, 1985; Guisinger &
Marin & Triandis, 1985; McCombs, 1985; Oved, 1988). Women Blatt, 1994; Markus & Kitayama, 1994; Sampson, 1985). The
are more likely than men in American society to construct an underlying principle that shapes the independent self-construal is
interdependent or relational self-view (Cross & Madson, 1997; the premise that the person is essentially separate from others. The
Markus & Oyserman, 1989). The nature of this Western version of primary components of the independent self-construal are one's
the interdependent self-view is likely to be somewhat different unique traits, abilities, preferences, interests, goals, and experi-
from the group-oriented interdependence that characterizes the ences, and these are differentiated from social contexts, interper-
self-views of members of collectivist societies; North Americans sonal relationships, and group memberships. To maintain and
and Western Europeans are more likely to include representations enhance this independent view of the self one must maintain a
of close relationships rather than group membership into the self sense of autonomy from others and "be true to one's own internal
structures of preferences, rights, convictions, and goals" (Markus
& Kitayama, 1994, p. 459).
Susan E. Cross, Pamela L. Bacon, and Michael L. Morris, Department In contrast, the underlying principle that shapes the interdepen-
of Psychology, Iowa State University. dent self-construal is the premise that the person is connected to
This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health others, so that the self is defined, at least in part, by important
Grant MH 54540-01. We wish to thank Dan Russell for his help with the roles, group memberships, or relationships. For individuals with
analyses and Dan Russell and Carolyn Cutrona for their helpful comments this self-construal, representations of important relationships and
on the manuscript. We also wish to thank Angela Anderson, Paige Boland,
roles share the self-space with abstract traits, abilities, and prefer-
Summer Brunscheen, Frindee Daly, John Egan, Angela Fontanini, Amy
ences. To maintain and enhance this interdependent view of the
McGregor, Alyssa Puffer, Carrie Rosentrater, Becky Schmidt, Kelly
Southwell, and Laura Wright for their help with the data collection. self, individuals will tend to think and behave in ways that em-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Susan E. phasize their connectedness to others and that strengthen existing
Cross, Department of Psychology, W112 Lagomarcino Hall, Iowa State relationships.
University, Ames, Iowa 50011. Electronic mail may be sent to scross® These differences in self-construals raise many issues with re-
iastate.edu. gard to self-related processes. For example, cognitive processes
791
792 CROSS, BACON, AND MORRIS

may vary depending on one's self-construal. Specifically, infor- terizes North Americans. Group memberships are relatively unim-
mation for close others that is closely linked to information about portant to U.S. adults as compared with members of East Asian
the self may function much like self-relevant information in cog- cultures (Triandis, 1989). Americans treat group memberships
nitive processes. As a result, individuals with an interdependent rather casually and have relatively little loyalty to in-groups.
self-construal may attend more closely to information about close American in-groups are larger, place relatively fewer demands on
others than will individuals with an independent self-construal. members, and are more voluntary than in collectivist cultures
Similarly, individuals with an interdependent self-construal may (Triandis, 1989). Rather than in-groups, Americans are more likely
remember information about close others better than individuals to include individual relationships (e.g., a spouse, mother, best
with an independent self-construal. friend, or colleague) in their self-representations.1
Motivational processes may also vary for individuals with di- Indeed, Kashima et al. (1995) contrasted collectivist-oriented
vergent self-construals. It is generally assumed that the desire to self-construals with the concept of relatedness to argue that the two
promote or enhance the self is a central motivator of behavior. concepts are not the same. They created a 4-item measure of
However, self-promotion or self-enhancement will depend on the relatedness that focused on the emotional relatedness of the self
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

nature of the self-construal. For individuals with an independent


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

with other individuals (a sample item is, "I feel like doing some-
self-construal, standing out, being better than others on self- thing for people in trouble because I can almost feel their pains.").
defining domains, and maintaining a positive ratio of successes- Comparing samples from Australia, the United States, Japan, and
to-aspirations contribute to a positive view of the self and to Korea, they found that individualism, collectivism, and relatedness
enhanced self-esteem (Blaine & Crocker, 1993; Harter, 1993;
were empirically separable. Cultures were most likely to be dif-
James, 1890/1983; Tesser, 1988). In contrast, for the person with
ferentiated on the individualism/collectivism dimension, but men
an interdependent self-construal, positive feelings about the self
and women were best differentiated on the relatedness dimension.
will in some part derive from developing and maintaining close
It is necessary, therefore, to differentiate between the collectivist-
relationships with others. Indeed, a variety of evidence suggests
oriented interdependence that characterizes members of cultures
that positive feedback from others and harmonious relationships
like Japan and China from the relational-interdependence that
are more important for women's self-esteem than for men's (e.g.,
Moran & Eckenrode, 1991; Roberts & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1989, characterizes many North Americans.
1994; Shwalbe & Staples, 1991; Zuckerman, 1989). In addition, some measures of interdependence that have been
based on East Asian collectivist ideology are multi-dimensional.2
Individuals with interdependent self-construals may also ap- For example, Kashima et al. (1995) report that a factor analysis of
proach relationships differently than those with independent self- a collectivism scale revealed three factors: a factor they called
construals. If maintaining close relationships is central to main- "collectivism," which can be described as the willingness to sac-
taining a stable sense of self and self-esteem for people with
rifice self-interest for the sake of the group; "agency," which is
interdependent self-construals, then these individuals should strive
characterized by items indicating a willingness to be independent
to develop skills and abilities that foster close relationships. For
of the group; and "assertiveness," which is the willingness to speak
example, these persons may be more likely to self-disclose on
up in opposition to the group. Other scales, such as the Singelis
intimate topics or to consider the implications of their decisions for
(1994) interdependent self-construal measure, include additional
close others, relative to individuals with an independent self-
dimensions of collectivism, such as respect for authority figures,
construal. In addition, the needs and wishes of close others may
strongly influence the thoughts and behaviors of these individuals. that are conceptually independent of the concept of the self as
These and other implications of differences in self-construals for connected or related to specific others (see also Realo, Allik, &
cognition, emotion, motivation, and social relationships are re- Vadi, 1997). A measure that taps a relational form of interdepen-
viewed by Cross and Madson (1997). dence is needed to further explore the variation of the self-
construal and its function among Western populations.
Currently, no measure of relational interdependence operation-
Two Forms of Interdependence alizes this construct in terms of the self-concept.3 Other measures,
such as the one used by Kashima et al. (1995), tap emotional
Due to cultural differences between collectivist and individualist
consequences of investment in close relationships. Although this
cultures, the specific form of the interdependent self-construal
developed by members of these different types of cultures will emotional sensitivity to others may be a characteristic of individ-
vary. In collectivism-based interdependence, the individual's po- uals with a relational-interdependent self-construal, these items do
sition in the group or situation dictates behavior; therefore, know- not directly assess the extent to which others, especially close
ing one's place, behaving according to one's role, and putting the relationships, are a part of one's personal identity or self-concept.
needs of the group before one's own needs are central dictums that Similarly, the Communal Orientation Scale (Clark, Ouellette,
shape the self-construal. Measures designed to tap this self- Powell, & Milberg, 1987) focuses on the individual's desire for
construal, such as Yamaguchi's Collectivism scale (Yamaguchi,
1994; Yamaguchi, Kuhlman, & Sugimori, 1995), include items 1
Brewer & Gardiner (1996), however, argued that a collectivist or
such as, "I am prepared to do things for my group at any time, even
group-oriented interdependence is characteristic of men in American cul-
though I have to sacrifice my own interest" and "I respect deci- ture.
sions made by my group" (see also Matsumoto, Weissman, Pres- 2
See Triandis (1995) for an overview of the development and use of
ton, Brown, & Kupperbusch, 1997). This group-oriented notion of various measures of individualism and collectivism.
interdependence, however, does not adequately describe the 3
We do know of one related yet unpublished measure by Rude, Welch,
relationship-centered conception of interdependence that charac- and Sandere (1998).
RELATIONAL-INTERDEPENDENT SELF-CON STRUAL 793

reciprocity and expectations of equity in close relationships, but Our second goal was to assess the relation between this opera-
does not assess the person's self-views as relational or connected tionalization of the interdependent self-construal and individuals'
to others. If the relational-interdependent self-construal is concep- experiences with relationships. If the self is defined, at least in part,
tualized in terms of the self-concept, then an adequate measure in terms of one's close relationships, then the individual should be
must include this cognitive component of thinking of oneself in motivated to develop and nurture close relationships. Conse-
terms of close others or identifying with close others. quently, individuals who view themselves as interdependent with
In a related line of research, Aron and his colleagues have others may be more likely to self-disclose and to permit close
argued that individuals often include representations of a particu- others to influence how they think or behave. They are expected to
larly close relationship into their mental representations of them- be more likely to include close others in the self, in Aron's terms,
selves (Aron, Aron, & Smo 11 an. 1992; Aron, Aron, Tudor, & and to be strongly committed to important relationships. In Stud-
Nelson, 1991). In their work, individuals who include a specific ies 1, 2, and 3 we examine these hypothesized associations be-
partner in the self are more likely to view themselves as sharing tween interdependence and orientations toward relationships.
perspectives, resources, and characteristics of the other (Aron et
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

al., 1991). We agree that most people will include specific rela-
Study 1: Scale Development and Validation
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tionships (e.g., one's mother or spouse) as part of the self, but


individuals differ markedly in the degree to which other relation- In Study 1 we report the development and psychometric prop-
ships are self-defining—such as friendships or relationships with erties of the Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal Scale
siblings. The Aron et al. (1991) measure, the Inclusion of Others (RISC). Eight samples of students were used to examine the
in the Self Scale, focuses on a single relationship, whereas our properties of the measure, its reliability, and its convergent and
emphasis is on interdependence as a general orientation toward discriminant validity. The convergent validity of the Relational-
representing oneself in terms of close relationships. In addition, Interdependent Self-Construal scale was examined by assessing its
our goal is a better understanding of the structure and function of association with measures related to thinking of oneself as inter-
the self in contrast to Aron's focus on understanding the conse- dependent with others, such as the Communal Orientation Scale
quences of close relationships. (Clark et al., 1987) and the Interdependent Self-Construal measure
Although our discussion thus far has represented relational- (Singelis, 1994). Because other research indicates that measures of
interdependent and independent self-construals as two types, we the independent self-construal and collectivism are not correlated,
recognize that these constructs represent two continuous dimen- we expected the RISC scale to be unrelated to measures of inde-
sions. Our assumption of two dimensions (rather than one bipolar pendence or individualism. Women are more likely to include
dimension) is based on previous research with other measures, close relationships in their self-concepts than are men, so we
such as the Singelis (1994) measures of the interdependent and examined its association with a measure of traditional sex roles,
independent self-construals. Singelis reports that these two scales the Spence, Helmreich, and Stapp (1974) Personal Attributes
are orthogonal. Other studies also find a similar lack of correlation Questionnaire (PAQ). The PAQ taps self-views related to feminine
between measures of these constructs (Cross, 1995). The primary sex roles (termed "expressivity") and masculine sex roles (termed
focus of the research reported here was the development of a "instrumentality"). According to Helgeson (1994), the expressivity
measure of the relational-interdependent self-construal; therefore, subscale is a useful measure of a communal orientation, whereas
we focus on people who think of themselves in very relational or the instrumentality subscale can be viewed as a measure of the
interdependent terms compared to those who do not. We do not independent self-construal. Thus we expected the RISC scale to be
assume that persons who do not think of themselves as relational positively correlated with the expressivity scale and uncorrelated
or interdependent have necessarily constructed very independent with the instrumentality scale.
self-views. Additional theorizing and research is necessary to spell
We also examined the relations between the RISC scale and
out the implications of these two dimensions for self-related
several personality measures. Theorists have argued that a critical
processes.
mark of the relational-interdependent self-construal is empathy for
others (Surry, 1991), so we examined the relations between the
Research Aims RISC scale and measures of empathy and perspective taking. The
tendency to think of oneself in terms of close relationships may be
The first step in examining the role of the relational- a consequence of underlying personality structure; therefore, mea-
interdependent self-construal in psychological phenomena is to sures of the five-factor model of personality were included in this
devise a reliable and valid measurement tool. In much earlier research (Costa & McCrae, 1992). We expected the RISC scale to
work, interdependence has not been measured directly; instead, be moderately related to the interpersonal traits, agreeableness and
gender or cultural background have been used as proxies for the extroversion, but to be unrelated to neuroticism and openness to
interdependent self-construal. More careful tests of the hypotheses experience. (We had no hypothesis concerning conscientiousness.)
related to individual differences in self-construal can be conducted We also examined the association of the RISC scale with the
if this construct can be reliably and validly measured. Our first tendency toward responding in a socially desirable fashion.
goal was to develop a new measure that directly conceptualizes the Some researchers have argued that very relational or interde-
relational-interdependent self-construal in terms of the degree to pendent persons may be vulnerable to depression or to reduced
which individuals include close relationships in their self-concepts. well-being due to their sensitivity to the distress of others (e.g.,
In Study 1 we describe the development of this measure, its Belle, 1982; McGrath, Keita, Strickland, & Russo, 1990). There-
psychometric properties, and its relations to other personality fore, several measures of well-being, such as depression, self-
constructs. esteem, and life satisfaction, were included in this research.
794 CROSS, BACON, AND MORRIS

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics and Gender Differences for the Relational
Interdependent Self-Construal Scale

Total Men Women Difference

Sample M SD N M SD n U SD n d

1 54.10 9.29 271 52.89 8.07 111 55.11 10.03 152 -1.92* -.24
2 56.12 10.25 299 54.27 9.56 128 57.44 10.56 165 -2.65** .32
3 54.91 10.39 940 53.90 10.25 406 55.66 L0.39 525 -2.59** -.17
4 54.01 10.11 609 52.20 10.03 253 55.72 9.93 315 -4.19*** -.35
5 56.84 ,9.27 735 54.27 9.17 341 59.13 8.76 383 —7.29*** -.52
6 54.12 9.92 535 50.85 10.34 221 56.48 8.87 285 -6.59*** -.57
7 58.10 10.68 621 54.73 10.17 248 60.49 10.47 362 -6.76*** -.56
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

8 56.08 9,58 273 54.48 9.38 111 57.78 9.50 143 -2.76** -.35
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. All t tests were one-tailed. Because of participants who failed to indicate their gender, the total number
of participants may be greater than the number of women and men listed.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Finally, we used a version of the known groups validation person who has staked some of his or her identity in it. Thus we
technique (i.e., examining gender differences) to assess criterion expected individuals who scored high on the RISC scale to be
validity. We followed this with regression analyses aimed at ex- more committed to their close relationships than low scorers.
amining the unique predictive power of scores on the RISC scale. In summary, we hypothesized that individuals who scored high
We began to examine the relations between scores on the RISC on the RISC scale would view more relationships as important to
scale and individuals' descriptions of their close relationships in them than would others. We also hypothesized that they would
this study. When relationships are self-defining, the individual seek out frequent and diverse contact with close relationship
should desire to develop and maintain close and supportive ties partners, would be more likely to permit those relationships to
with others. Thus, it is likely that they will develop a greater influence their lives, and would be more likely to open up and
number of important relationships than will individuals who have reveal personal or intimate information about themselves in order
not constructed an interdependent self-construal. Individuals with to promote the relationship. We also expected that these individ-
an interdependent view of themselves may also describe their uals with a very interdependent self-construal would tend to in-
important relationships as closer than do others. Measuring close- corporate their close relationships into the serf and would be more
ness, however, is not entirely straightforward. Some researchers committed to their close relationships than other individuals. One
have defined closeness in terms of the impact of a relationship on consequence of this commitment to relationships may be that
the person's life. For example, Berscheid, Snyder, and Omoto individuals who think of themselves as interdependent with others
(1989) draw upon Kelley et al.'s (1983) definition of a close may also perceive higher levels of social support than do others.
relationship as one in which the partners are interdependent. In
their view, the three most important aspects of a close or interde-
pendent relationship are frequency of contact, involvement in a Method
diverse array of activities together, and strong impact on the
individual's decisions, plans, and behaviors. Other researchers Participants
have focused on intimacy of discussion and self-revelation as
central to a close relationship (Reis & Shaver, 1988). For example, Eight samples of undergraduate students from introductory psychology
Airman and Taylor's (1973) theory of social penetration posits that courses completed the RISC scale along with a packet of questionnaires, as
close and rewarding relationships are characterized by extensive part of large data collection efforts. Students received extra course credit
and intimate self-disclosure. for their participation. Due to time constraints, participants in each sample
completed a subset of the measures described below. Sample sizes ranged
A newer conceptualization of closeness has been suggested by from 267 to 956; the sample sizes and numbers of men and women in each
Aron and his colleagues, who argue that closeness can be under- sample are presented in Table 1. To reduce the possible influence of
stood as "including the other in the self (Aron et al., 1991). As cultural differences in self-construal, non-American participants and par-
described earlier, Aron and his colleagues have investigated the ticipants who did not indicate their citizenship were excluded from the
cognitive consequences of incorporating specific close relation- analyses (except for Samples 5, 6, and 7, where citizenship information
ships into the self, and they have developed a simple, Venn- was not obtained). This resulted in the exclusion of 22 to 40 participants
per sample.4
diagram-like measure in which the self and other are represented
as overlapping circles (Aron et al., 1991). We included measures
of each of these representations of closeness in this study in order 4
A comparison of the American students' scores (n = 2,374) on the
to assess the concept broadly.
RISC scale with the noncitizens' scores (n = 109) across all the samples
In addition, if a relationship is self-defining, the individual revealed that the Americans tended to score higher than the noncitizens,
should desire to protect and maintain it. The loss of a close AfA^oan, = 54.89, SD = 10.11, M oonddzcn9 = 50.85, SD = 9.52,
relationship may be especially threatening or difficult for the f(2481) = 4 . 0 8 , p < .001.
RELATIONAL-INTERDEPENDENT SELF-CONSTRUAL 795

Measures dent Self-Construal subscale). The reh'abilities of the Interdependent and


Independent subscales were .73 and .69, respectively (Singelis, 1994).
For all the measures described below (except where noted), participants The Collective Self-Esteem Scale (CSB; Luhtanen & Crocker, 1992) is
responded using a 7-point Likert-type scale. Indexes were created so that a 16-item measure that assesses collective self-esteem along four dimen-
high scores indicate high levels of the construct (e.g., high scores on the sions: Private Collective Self-Esteem, Public Collective Self-Esteem, Im-
RISC scale indicate higher levels of the interdependent self-construal).
portance to Identity, and Membership Esteem (each subscale is composed
Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal Scale (RISC). Many of the
of 4 items). The general positivity of one's social identity is assessed by
initial items were generated by the authors, based on the conceptualization
totaling all 16 items. Examples of items from the CSE are, "I feel good
of the relational form of the interdependent self-construal as including
about the social groups I belong to" (Private subscale; Cronbach's a =
close others in the self-concept. Additional items were created by modi-
fying items from measures of conceptually related constructs, such as .74); "In general, others respect the social groups that I am a member o f
cross-cultural measures of the interdependent self-construal. The modifi- (Public subscale; a = .80); "The social groups I belong to are an important
cation typically included changing the focus of the question from one's reflection of who I am" (Importance to Identity subscale; a = 76); and "I
relationship with people in general or a group to a close friend or family am a worthy member of the social groups I belong to" (Membership
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

members. During the development phase, some items were changed, others subscale; a = .73). Luhtanen and Crocker (1992) also report that the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

were dropped, and new items were constructed. Using data from the first reliability for the total CSE scale was .85.
sample, we examined the individual questions to identify items that (a) had The Personal Attributes Questionnaire (PAQ; Spence et al., 1974) is
low item-total correlations, (b) were strongly associated with social desir- composed of eight traits related to instrumentality (or a communal orien-
ability, or (c) were worded poorly. When an item consistently violated tation) and eight traits related to expressivity (or an independence orien-
these criteria, it was eliminated. Using this system, we narrowed the tation), which are thought to be independent constructs. The items are
number of items from 28 to 11 (see Table 2). Of these 11 items, 2 were presented in a 5-point bipolar format (e.g., 1 = not at all aggressive; 5 =
phrased negatively and are reverse-scored in the data analyses. very aggressive). Helmreich, Spence, and Wilhelm (1981) report Cronbach
When completing the RISC scale, participants were instructed to "indi- alphas ranging from .67 to .77 for the Instrumentality scale and .72 to .80
cate the extent to which you agree or disagree with each of these state- for die Expressivity scale.
ments." Participants responded using a Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 Two additional items were included to assess the validity of the RISC
(strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The possible range of scores was scale. First, participants were asked, "When you think about yourself, how
from 11 to 77.
important are your relationships with others for your self-concept?" Sec-
Measures related to interdependence and independence. The Commu-
ond, participants were asked, "Some people tend to see themselves as very
nal Orientation Scale (Clark et al., 1987) consists of 14 items; individuals
independent and separate from others, whereas others are more likely to
high in communal orientation feel responsible for the welfare of close
others and are responsive to their needs. An example of an item from the think of themselves as interdependent and connected in important ways to
Communal Orientation scale is, "I believe people should go out of their their close friends. Which type of person do you think you are?" Partici-
way to be helpful." The reliability of this scale was .78 (Clark et al., 1987). pants responded using a 5-point Likert-type scale that ranged from 1 (very
Singelis (1994) developed orthogonal, 12-item measures of the interde- independent) to 5 (very interdependent). These questions allowed us to
pendent and independent self-construals based on. the Markus and assess the associations between the RISC scale and these alternative
Kitayama (1991) conceptualization of cultural differences in the self. approaches to tapping the relational-interdependent self-construal.
Examples of these items are, "It is important for me to maintain harmony Personality measures. The 7-item Empathic Concern subscale of the
within my group" (Interdependent Self-Construal subscale) and "I prefer to Interpersonal Reactivity Index (Davis, 19S0) assesses feelings of concern
be direct and forthright when dealing with people I've just met" (Indepen- and sympathy for others, whereas the 7-item Perspective Taking subscale

Table 2
Final Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal Scale Items With
Corrected Item-Total Correlations

Corrected item-
Item total r

1. My close relationships are an important reflection of who I am. .68


2. When I feel very close to someone, it often feels to me like that person is an
important part of who I am. .69
3. I usually feel a strong sense of pride when someone close to me has an important
accomplishment. .54
4. I think one of the most important parts of who I am can be captured by looking at
my close friends and understanding who they are. .64
5. When I think of myself, I often think of my close friends or family also. .63
6. If a person hurts someone close to me, I feel personally hurt as well. .53
7. In general, my close relationships are an important part of my self-image. .69
8. Overall, my close relationships have very little to do with how I feel about
myself.3 .54
9. My close relationships are unimportant to my sense of what kind of person I am.a .52
10. My sense of pride comes from knowing who I have as close friends. .56
11. When I establish a close friendship with someone, I usually develop a strong sense
of identification with that person. .60

Note. N = 4,288. Response scale ranges from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).
a
Reverse-keyed item.
796 CROSS, BACON, AND MORRIS

measures respondents' ability to see things from another person's view- spent with the target and the activities data were converted to 1 to 10 scales
point. Cronbach's alpha ranged from .70 to .78 (Davis, 1980). as in Berscheid et al. (1989). Next, participants responded to a series of 27
The NEO-FFI (Costa & McCrae, 1992) was used to assess the dimen- items concerning the target's influence on the participant's thoughts, feel-
sions of the five-factor model: Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to ings, and behavior. Examples of influence items include "X influences
Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness. Costa and McCrae important things in my life" and "X influences how I spend my free time."
(1992) report Cronbach's alphas of .89 to .95 for the scales, which have Cronbach's alpha was .89 in this sample. Participants also responded to 7
been widely validated and have shown good convergent and discriminant items concerning the degree to which the target influenced their future
validity. goals and plans. Responses were made on a scale ranging from 1 (not at
Socially desirable responding was assessed using the 33-item Marlowe- all) to 7 (a great extent), and items included "my marriage plans" and "my
Crowne Social Desirability Scale (Crowne & Marlowe, 1960). Participants plans for achieving a particular standard of living." Cronbach's alpha
responded 'True" or "False" to each item. The Kuder-Richardson 20 was .89.
(KR-20) reliability of the Social Desirability Scale was .88 (Crowne & The 10-item self-disclosure scale (Miller, Berg, & Archer, 1983) asked
Marlowe, 1960). participants to indicate the extent to which they talked with this person
Measures of well-being. The 20-item CES-D depression scale (Rad- - about several topics. Examples of items included "My personal habits" and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

loff, 1977) assesses the frequency of depressive symptoms, such as de- "My deepest feelings." Responses were made on a 4-point scale ranging
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

pressed mood, feelings of guilt and worthlessness, problems sleeping, and from 1 (discuss not at all) to 4 {discuss fully and completely). Due to low
loss of appetite. The CES-D correlates strongly with the number of nega- correlations with other items, the "personal habits" item was dropped. With
tive life events and other measures of depression, such as the Beck this 9-item scale, Cronbach's alpha was .91.
Depression Inventory (Beck, 1967) and the Self-Rating Depression Scale The Inclusion of Other in the Self scale (IOS) is a single-item pictorial
(Zung, 1965; see Shaver & Brennan [1991] for further information on the measure of closeness (Aron et al., 1992). Participants selected a picture
scale properties). Participants responded using a 4-point scale (1 = rarely from a set of Venn-like diagrams that present two circles with varying
or none of the time; 4 = most of the time). Cronbach's alpha for this sample degrees of overlap; the circles represent the self and the relationship
was .91. partner. The IOS scale has good test-retest and alternate form reliability,
and has been shown to predict how long romantic couples stay together,
The Satisfaction With Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin,
with no relation to social desirability.
1985) is a 5-item scale that has been used extensively and has good
psychometric properties. Participants responded to the items using a Rusbult's (1983) 4-item scale was used to measure commitment. Exam-
5-point scale (1 = very slightly or not at all; 5 = extremely). Cronbach's ples of items included "How likely is it that you will end your relationship
with person X in the near future?" and "To what extent are you 'attached'
alpha in this sample was .84.
to person X?" Responses were made on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 {not
The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg, 1965), is a com-
at all likely) to 7 {extremely likely). Due to low correlations with other
monly used 10-item measure of global self-esteem. Reliabilities of the
items, the "end your relationship" item was dropped, leaving a total of 3
RSES were .77 and .88 (reported in Blascovich & Tomaka, 1991).
items. Cronbach's alpha was .65.
The Life Orientation Test (LOT; Scheier & Carver, 1985) is an 8-item
The Social Provisions Scale (SPS; Cutrona & Russell, 1987) was used to
measure of dispositional optimism. Scheier and Carver (1985) report a
assess participants' overall level of social support. This 24-item measure
Cronbach's alpha of .76 for the LOT.
taps 6 dimensions of social support identified by Weiss (1974): guidance,
Evaluations of relationships. The social network diagram (Antonucci,
reliable alliance, attachment, social integration, reassurance of worth, and
1986) was used to assess the number of important relationships in partic-
opportunity to provide nurturance. Evidence of the scale's validity is
ipants' social networks. This measure consists of four concentric circles,
provided by Cutrona (1989) and Cutrona and Russell (1987). A 4-point
the innermost one marked "YOU." Participants were asked to write in the scale was provided for the participants' responses (1 = strongly disagree;
names of people with whom they had personal relationships according to 4 = strongly agree). Cronbach's alpha for the combined scale in this
the following instructions. In the inner circle, they were to write the names sample was .92.
of people so close and important that "it is hard to imagine life without
them"; in the middle circle, they were to write the names of people who
were not quite as close but still important to them; and in the outermost Results
circle, they were to write the names of people who were important enough Basic Psychometric Properties
that they should be placed somewhere in the network. This diagram has
been used with a variety of samples that varied across age, class, and Factor structure. Principal components analysis was used to
culture and has been found to support theory in the social support literature conduct an exploratory factor analysis on the pooled data from the
(Antonucci, 1986). eight samples. Negatively phrased items were recoded, resulting in
Participants selected the most important person in their social network positive factor loadings. Only one factor emerged with an eigen-
and completed the following measures with that relationship in mind. First, value greater than 1.0. All 11 items loaded between .59 and .77 on
they described their closeness to this person using the Relationship Close- the first factor. The scree plot for the factor analysis also supported
ness Inventory (RCI). The RCI (Berscheid et al., 1989) was developed as a single factor, with the elbow of the plot occurring after the first
a multidimensional measure of closeness for specific relationships. The
factor. The first factor accounted for 47% of the total variance.
participant was asked to categorize the type of relationship into one of the
following categories: work, family, romantic, friend, or other. Thirty-three Descriptive statistics. To create an index, the negative items
percent of the most important relationships were categorized as close were reversed and responses to the 11 items were summed. Table 1
friends; 32% were romantic partners; 24% were family members; and 11% shows the mean RISC scale scores across the eight samples.
were categorized in the "other" category. Skewness ranged from —.805 to —.289 across these samples, and
Participants rated the average amount of time they had spent with the kurtosis ranged from —.017 to 1.41.
target in the mornings, afternoons, and evenings of the past week. Partic-
ipants were then presented with a list of activities and were asked to check Reliability
all those that they had initiated with the target in the past week. This was
a change from the original RCI, which asks participants to check activities Internal consistency. Evaluating the samples individually, the
that they had done alone with the target. Both the total amount of time coefficient alphas ranged from .85 to .90, with a mean of .88. The
RELATIONAL-INTERDEPENDENT SELF-CONSTRUAL 797

means of the inter-item correlations ranged from .35 to .46. The Table 3
item-total correlations from the eight samples ranged from .54 to Correlations of the Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal
.73. When the samples were combined the coefficient alpha was (RISC) Scale With Other Scales
.88 (N = 4,288), and the inter-item correlations ranged from .25 to
.66, with a mean of .41. Table 2 presents the corrected item-total Correlation
correlations for the pooled sample. Measure with RISC N
Test-retest reliability. Stability over time was assessed by Measures of collectivism and interdependence
administering the RISC scale to participants in Samples 3 and 5 Communal Orientation Scale .41 598
twice in either a one- or two-month interval. The test-retest reli- Interdependent Self-Construal Scale (Singelis) .41 1,219
abilities over the 2-month interval in the two samples were .73, Collective Self-Esteem Scale
Private .22 1,218
n = 67, p < .001 and .63, n = 317, p < .001. The test-retest Public .20 1,218
reliability over one month was .74, n = 405, p < .001 and .76, Identity .47 1,217
n = 46, p < .001. Membership .16 1,219
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Total .37 1,219


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Personal Attributes Questionnaire—Expressivity .32 604


Convergent Validity Importance of relationships item .56 1,216
Self-categorization item .31 1,212
Other measures of collectivism and interdependence. The Measures of independence or individualism
RISC scale is intended to provide a better measure of the Independent Self-Construal Scale (Singelis) .08 604
relational-interdependent self-construal for Americans than current Personal Attributes Questionnaire—Instrumentality -.06 604
Personality measures
measures of collectivism or the interdependent self-construal, Interpersonal Reactivity Index
which tend to focus on group identity. It should, however, be Empathic Concern .34 2,726
correlated with these other measures. As shown in Table 3, the Perspective Taking .13 2,986
RISC scale correlated moderately with the Communal Orientation NEO-FFI
Scale (r = .41), and Singelis's (1994) group-oriented Interdepen- Agreeableness .35 628
Extraversion .28 628
dent Self-Construal Scale (r = .41). The RISC scale was moder- Conscientiousness .23 628
ately related to the total Collective Self-Esteem score (r = .37). It Neuroticism .08 628
was most strongly related to the Identity subscale (r = .47) and the Openness to Experience .09 628
Private subscale (r = .22). Similarly, the correlation of the RISC Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale -.05 1,785
scale with the PAQ measure of expressivity (which has also been Measures of well-being
Depression .03 235
described as a measure of communal orientation) was .32. These Satisfaction With Life .07 235
moderate correlations suggest that although the RISC is related to Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale .01 3,555
other measures of collectivism or communal orientation, it is Life Orientation Test (Optimism) .17 749
clearly not identical to those measures. Evaluation of closest relationship
RCI time spent together .12 165"
Because individuals with a very interdependent self-construal RCI activities .11 247
should find relationships more important to how they think of RCI influence .27 247
themselves than do others, we expected a positive correlation RCI future plans .15 246
between the RISC scale and the item that asked "When you think Self-disclosure .26 245
1OS .26 243
about yourself, how important are your relationships with others Commitment .22 247
for your self-concept?" The correlation was .56, indicating that the Social Provisions Scale (Social Support) .26 235
RISC taps this relational dimension of the self-concept. In addi-
tion, the correlation between RISC scores and degree of self- Note. Eight samples are represented in this table. None of the measures
reported categorization as independent or interdependent was .31. was administered to every sample, which accounts for the differences in
sample size for each correlation. RCI = Relationship Closeness Inventory;
Measures of independence. The interdependent self-construal IOS - Inclusion of Other in the Self; NEO-FFI = NEO Five-Factor
and the independent self-construal have been described as two Inventory.
a
orthogonal constructs (Singelis, 1994); therefore, we predicted that This correlation excludes participants whose closest relationship partner
our measure would not be related to a measure of the independent lived in another city.
self-construal. Supporting our prediction, the RISC was not related
to Singelis's (1994) Independent Self-Construal measure (r = .08). Personality measures. In order to maintain self-defining rela-
Hotelling tests showed that the relation between the RISC and tionships, individuals with highly interdependent self-construals
Singelis's measure of the interdependent self-construal (r ~ .41) should be aware of the feelings and thoughts of close others. Thus,
was stronger than the relation between the RISC and his measure we predicted that the RISC scale would be related to empathic
of the independent self-construal (r - .08), r(601) = 6.67, p = concern and perspective-taking. As predicted, the RISC was pos-
.001. The RISC scale was also unrelated to the instrumentality itively related to empathic concern (r = .34), but had a weak
dimension of the PAQ, which has been described as indicating an relation to perspective taking (r = .13). Hotelling tests revealed
orientation toward independence (r = —.06). A Hotelling test that the relation between the RISC scale and empathic concern was
showed that the relation between the RISC and the expressivity significantly stronger than the relation with perspective taking,
dimension of the PAQ was stronger than the relation between the *(2723) = \i.92,p< .001.
RISC and the instrumentality dimension, r(601) = -6.79, p < The RISC scale was also correlated with each of the five
.001. subscales of the NEO-FFI measure of the Five Factor Model. As
798 CROSS, BACON, AND MORRIS

expected, the RISC scale was positively correlated with Agree- Table 4
ableness (r = .35) and Extraversion (r = .28). It was also posi- Correlations Among the Variables in the Hierarchical
tively correlated with Conscientiousness (f = ,23); but it was not Regressions (Stuiiy 1)
correlated with Neuroticism (r = .08) or Openness to Experience
(r = .09). Hotelling tests revealed that the correlations of the RISC Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6
scale with agreeableness, extraversion, and conscientiousness were 1. RISC scale _
significantly stronger than the relations with neuroticism and open- 2. PAQ Expressivity .32 —
ness to experience, rs(625) > 2.44, ps < .02. 3. Empathic Concern .28 .61 —
There was not a significant relationship between the RISC and 4. Singelis Interdependent .40 .34 .33 —
the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability scale (r = —.05). These Self-Construal
5. Collective Self-Esteem .30 .24 .33 .30 —
results suggest that respondents are not attempting to present 6. Communal Orientation .41 .61 .67 .40 .43 —
themselves in socially approved ways when they answer the RISC
scale, thus demonstrating discriminant validity. Note. N = 582. RISC = Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal;
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Measures of well-being. The RISC scale was unrelated to the PAQ = Personal Attributes Questionnaire. All correlations are significant
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

at p < .001.
measures of depression (r = .03), life satisfaction (r = .07), and
self-esteem (r = .01). There was a weak correlation between the
RISC scale and the measure of optimism (the LOT; r = .16). Thus, center, was used to assess the hypothesis that individuals who
individuals with a very interdependent self-construal appear no scored high on the RISC scale would view a greater number of
more at risk of reduced well-being than are others. relationships as very important to them than would others. As
Group differences. The relational-interdependent self-construal is expected, scores on the RISC scale and the number of people listed
thought to be more characteristic of Western women than of men in the inner circle (i.e., people it is hard to imagine life without)
(Cross & Madson, 1997). In all eight samples, women scored signif- were positively associated (r = .24, p < .05). There were no
icantly higher than men on the RISC scale (see Table 1). The size of associations between the RI^C scale and the number of people in
these differences ranged from small (d = —.17) to moderate (d — the middle and outer circles (r = .12, p > .05 for the middle circle,
-.57). In the pooled sample, d = - . 4 1 . r = .09, p > .05 for the outer circle). This suggests that high
scorers on the RISC scale tend to have more close and important
Examination of Incremental Utility relationships than others, but have similar numbers of more distant
relationships.
Although the zero-order correlations presented in Table 3 pro- Based on their responses to the social network diagram, partic-
vide some evidence that the Relational-Interdependent Self- ipants picked their most important relationship. We predicted that
Construal Scale is related to other measures as predicted, it is people who scored high on the RISC scale would view their most
unclear from these analyses the extent to which this measure taps important relationship as closer than would others (as indicated by
a unique construct. We expected that the RISC scale would ac- the RCI subscales, the self-disclosure scale, and the IOS scale) and
count for additional variance in relevant dependent variables be- as more committed than would others. As shown in Table 3, the
yond that accounted for by related constructs, such as expressivity, RISC scale was positively related to the degree to which the
empathic concern, and a measure of the group-oriented interde- individual reported being influenced by their relationship partner
pendent self-construal. In these analyses, our criterion variables (r = .27), their self-disclosure in the relationship (r = .26), the
were the relatively global measures of collective self-esteem and degree to which the other is included in the self (the IOS scale; r -
communal orientation. We selected these measures because they .26), and commitment to the relationship (r = .22). There were not,
tap constructs that are fairly broad or inclusive in scope (i.e., however, strong relations between the RISC scale and the time
attitudes toward relationships and evaluations of one's in-groups). spent with the relationship partner, the number of activities initi-
In these analyses we examined the incremental predictive power of ated with this person, or the degree to which this person influenced
the RISC scale in predicting these constructs, controlling for the the participant's future plans.
expressivity subscale of the PAQ, empathic concern, and the
Singelis Interdependent Self-Construal Scale. In separate hierar- If individuals with an interdependent self-construal are more
chical regression equations, the criterion variable was regressed on committed to and close to their relationship partners, they may also
the covariates in Step 1, and scores on the RISC scale were added perceive higher levels of social support than others. As predicted,
at Step 2. The correlations among the variables are presented in individuals who described themselves as high on the RISC scale
Table 4, and the results of the hierarchical regressions are pre- tended also to perceive higher levels of social support from others,
sented in Table 5. As shown in Table 5, the RISC scale is a r = .26. The correlations with the individual subscales of the
significant predictor of these criterion measures after controlling Social Provisions Scale ranged from a low of r = .13 with the
closely related constructs. reassurance of worth subscale to a high of r = .30 with the
opportunity for nurturance subscale.

Evaluations of Relationships Discussion


In the final phase of this study, we examined the relations The results of Study 1 indicate that the relational-interdependent
between the RISC scale and participants' perceptions of their self-construal is a relatively stable individual difference construct
social network and closest relationship. The social network dia- that can be measured with self-report items. Factor analyses indi-
gram, consisting of a set of concentric circles with "YOU" in the cated that a single factor underlies the RISC scale. The scale also
RELATIONAL-INTERDEPENDENT SELF-CONSTRUAL 799

Table 5 In the studies that follow, we examine the implications of the


Hierarchical Regression of Collective Self-Esteem and relational-interdependent self-construal for relationship processes.
Communal Orientation on PAQ Expressivity, Empatkic A key assumption of this perspective is that when others are
Concern, Singelis Interdependent Self-Construal Scale, included in one's self-representations, attention to these relation-
and RISC Scores: Beta Weights and R2 (Study 1) ships and efforts to maintain and enhance them will serve to
maintain and enhance the self. In Study 2, we examine the process
Collective Communal of making important decisions in terms of the relational-
Self-Esteem Orientation
interdependent self-construal.
Predictor j3 Aft2 /3 AR2
Study 2: The Role of Relationship Considerations in
Step 1 .54***
PAQ Expressivity .03 .28*** Decision Making
Empathic Concern 24*** ,45***
Singelis Interdependent For individuals with an interdependent self-construal, the wishes
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Self-Construal .21*** .15** and opinions of close relationship partners are likely to be taken
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Step 2 .02*** .02*** into account when the individual is making important decisions.
RISC scale 17*** .17** For example, the student who is considering going out of state for
Total/? 2 27*** .56***
a summer job may think of the implications of the decision for a
Note. N = 582. PAQ = Personal Attributes Questionnaire; RISC = romantic relationship. The student making a decision about taking
Relational-Independent Self-Construal. a summer job versus going to summer school may consider his or
**/> < .01. ***p < ,001. her family's wishes in the matter. The person who is low in
interdependence may not be as likely to consider other people's
wishes or reactions or to consult other people for information or
exhibited both high internal consistency and good test-retest sta- advice.
bility. In addition, the results provided evidence of the scale's Consequently, in this study we asked students to describe an
convergent and discriminant validity. As expected, the RISC scale important decision they were in the process of making and to
was moderately associated with other measures of conununalism elaborate the factors that were influencing their decision. After
and interdependence and was unassociated with measures of in- describing their decision, they were asked several follow-up ques-
dependence. The scale related as expected to other personality tions about it. Embedded in the list of items were questions
constructs: Scores on the RISC scale were moderately related to designed to elicit directly the extent to which close others influ-
measures of empathy, agreeableness, and extroversion, but were enced their decisions. We hypothesized that individuals who
unrelated to neuroticism and social desirability. In addition, RISC scored high on the Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal scale
scale scores did not correlate with measures of well-being, such as would tend to list relational factors that had influenced their
depression or satisfaction with life. Criterion validity was also decision making and to indicate on the close-ended items that other
shown in the pattern of sex differences: Women consistently people and close relationships were taken into account in then-
scored higher on the RISC scale than did men. When controlling decision process.
for closely related constructs, the RISC scale predicted a signifi-
cant proportion of incremental variance in relatively global mea-
sures of relatedness (collective self-esteem and communal orien- Method
tation), thus demonstrating its discriminant validity. Participants
Scores on the RISC scale were also related to the size of
participants' social networks and to their evaluations of their Participants were introductory psychology students who participated in
exchange for extra course credit. Two hundred sixty-six participants (120
closest relationship. Persons with a very interdependent self-
men, 145 women, and 1 unstated) provided complete data for all portions
construal reported a greater number of relationships that were very
of the study. The open-ended descriptions of 18 participants could not be
important to them and higher levels of social support than did coded either due to poor handwriting or failure to follow directions.
others. High scores on the RISC scale were also associated with
high levels of self-disclosure, perceived closeness, and commit-
ment in the relationship. Unexpectedly, scores on the RISC scale Procedure and Materials
were not related to the amount of time participants spent with their The RISC scale and the decision questionnaire were administered in
relationship partner or to the number of activities they initiated large group sessions of 30 to 200 as part of a mass-testing process. The first
with this person. These results suggest that whereas most people packet that the students completed included the RISC scale and several
enjoy being with close friends and seek out opportunities to do other instruments, many of which were designed for other studies. Cron-
things together regardless of the degree of interdependence of their bach's alpha for the RISC scale was .89.
self-construal, the quality of that time together may differ depend- The participants were given the decision questionnaire last. On the first
ing on individuals' self-construals. Individuals with very interde- page, they were asked to "Describe a decision that you are in the process
pendent self-construals may be more likely than others to spend of making. Please describe a decision that has a fairly significant impact on
their time with significant relationship partners disclosing impor- your life." The instructions went on to give several examples of possible
decisions one might be making (e.g., how to spend one's summer or with
tant or sensitive information and may be more responsive to their
whom to room next year). The participants were asked to describe as
partner's needs and desires (as evidenced by their willingness to let
thoroughly as possible the factors that they were considering in making
their partner influence their choices and behaviors). their decision—both major factors and relatively minor factors. No men-
800 CROSS, BACON, AND MORRIS

tion was made of the potential influence of others in this decision. The decisions separately, decisions that were coded by the participants
participants were encouraged to be explicit and to write in essay style, not into either the relational or family category were included together
to simply list pros and cons. Students who could not think of a decision in a single relational category (n = 126); all other decisions were
they were in the process of making were encouraged to describe a recent included in a nonrelational category (n = 135). There were no
decision they had already made. Most of the first page was empty for
gender differences in the type of decision described, x2 = .00.
students to write out their thoughts.
As expected, the correlations between the RISC scale and the
On the second page, participants answered several questions about their
decision. They were enjoined not to add anything to their written descrip- items indicating the influence of others were generally stronger for
tion after beginning these items. First they were asked if this was a decision nonrelational decisions than for the relational decisions. When
they were still making or had already made. Then they were asked to describing nonrelational decisions, the RISC scale was positively
categorize the decision as to whether it had to do with personal relation- related to the number of relational factors mentioned (r = .21
ships, academics, family, jobs, housing, or other. They were allowed to versus r = .11 for relational decisions) and to the importance of
select as many categories as were appropriate. The remainder of the family members' opinions (r = .37 compared with r = .15 for
questionnaire consisted of items that assessed their decision. Crucial to this relational decisions). Although the relation between the RISC scale
investigation were three items that addressed the role of others in the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and reports of the extent to which the needs and wishes of others
decision. They were "To what extent do you take into account the needs
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and wishes of others when thinking about this decision?" "How important were taken into account was larger for the nonrelational decisions
in your decision process are the opinions of your close friends?" and "How (r = .24) than for the relational decisions (r = . 18), this difference
important in your decision process are the opinions of your family mem- was small. The exception to this pattern was the item assessing the
bers?" These items were embedded with other filler items (e.g., "How importance of friends' opinions. For this item, the relation with
important are financial concerns in your decision making?"). interdependence was stronger for the relational decisions (r = .25)
than for the nonrelational decisions (r = .11), indicating that
students who scored high on the RISC scale were more likely to
Coding the Open-Ended Descriptions
turn to their friends for advice when making decisions about their
Each of the factors mentioned by participants as an influence on their relationships than when making other sorts of decisions.5
decision was coded into one of two categories: a relational category or a
nonrelational category. (Statements that were not factors in the decision
Discussion
were not coded.) Examples of relational statements were "I'd like to be
close to my family" and "My roommates wanted the first apartment." This study supported the hypothesis that individuals who tended
Examples of nonrelational statements were "I could get a better job here" to think of themselves in relational, interdependent terms would be
and "The second apartment was cheaper." Two trained judges coded the
more likely to consider the consequences of their decisions for
descriptions; 25 percent of the protocols were coded by both judges to
establish interrater reliability. (The coders were blind to participants' other people or to take into account the opinions or needs of close
scores on the RISC scale.) The number of relational and nonrelational others. When describing an important decision, individuals who
factors in each protocol was determined independently by the two judges; scored high on the RISC scale were more likely than those who
interjudge reliability was .92 for the relational factors and .89 for the scored low to freely generate relationship-oriented factors in their
nonrelational factors. decision. The instructions for the decision description task were
carefully designed to permit the generation of a wide variety of
factors that could influence decisions; relational factors were not
Results
specifically prompted in the instructions to the participants. Thus,
Participants generated an average of 3.3 relational factors that any mention of the role of others in this decision was self-
influenced their decisions (SD = 3.96, range = 0 to 23) and an generated by the respondents and not likely to have been a con-
average of 5.3 nonrelational factors (SD = 3.71, range = 0 to 19). sequence of subtle demand factors. In addition, participants who
As hypothesized, the RISC scale was correlated with the number
of relational factors that were described (r = .18, p < .01, 5
two-tailed), but it was not related to the number of nonrelational We also examined the extent to which the relational-interdependent
factors included in the descriptions (r = .01). The RISC scale was self-construal mediated sex differences in the ratings of the influences on
nonrelational decisions. Independent sample t tests revealed sex differences
also related to the extent to which the participants took into
in ratings of the extent to which others were taken into account,
account the needs and wishes of others when making the decision
Wwt>men = 3.07, SD = 1.09, M men = 2.66, SD = 1.12, «133) = -2.15,p <
(r = .24, p < .01) and the importance of the opinions of family .04, d = — .37; in the importance of family members' opinions,
(r = .27, p < .01) and friends (r = .21, p < .01) in the decision. M womeo = 3.58, SD = 1.01, M men = 3.11, SD = 1.00, /(133) = 2.68, p <
We also reasoned that the relation between the RISC and the .01, d = -.47; and on the RISC scale, Mwomen = 55.19, SD = 8.11,
extent to which others are considered when making decisions Wmcn = 48.36, SD = 10.16, f(133) = -4.35, p < .001, d = -.15. Sex
would be stronger for decisions that did not directly concern a alone accounted for 3% of the variance in the extent to which the needs and
relationship (e.g., where to work for the summer or whether to take wishes of others were considered in the decision. When the RISC was
added to the equation, sex was reduced to accounting for only an additional
summer school classes). When making decisions about relation-
1% of the variance in the extent variable. TTius, scores on the RISC scale
ships or family issues, virtually everyone is likely to mention the accounted for 71% of the relation between sex and the criterion variable.
influence of the other persons involved in the decision. When For the item assessing the importance of family members' opinions, sex
making decisions that are not explicitly relational, however, indi- alone accounted for 5% of the variance in the dependent variable. When the
viduals with more interdependent self-construals may be more RISC was added to the equation, sex was reduced to accounting for only
likely to think about and take into account the opinions or desires 1% of the variance. Thus, scores on the RISC scale accounted for 78% of
of their friends or family members. To look at these types of the relation between sex and the criterion variable.
RELATIONAL-INTERDEPENDENT SELF-CONSTRUAL 801

scored high on the RISC scale also indicated on the experimenter- We hypothesized that the partners of individuals who scored
provided items that they were more likely to take others into high on the RISC scale would perceive them as disclosing more
account when making their decision. about themselves and as more responsive than would the partners
This study supported the hypothesized role of the relational- of individuals who scored low on the self-construal measure.
interdependent self-construal in decision-making. Study 3 exam- Given that self-disclosure and sensitive responding are central
ines the association between the self-construal and relationship- ingredients in the development of a satisfying or close relationship,
development strategies. we reasoned that individuals paired with a high scorer on the RISC
scale would also tend to evaluate the relationship and the interac-
tion more positively than would others. Therefore, we examined
Study 3: The Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal the direct relationship between individuals' RISC scale scores and
and Relationship Development their partners' satisfaction with the interaction, their liking for the
individual, and their perceptions of closeness to their partner. In
Two key components of the process of relationship building are addition, we examined a mediational model, in which this ex-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

self-disclosure and responsiveness to one's partner's needs and pected relation between one individual's self-construal and her
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

concerns (Reis & Patrick, 1996; Reis & Shaver, 1988). Cross and partner's evaluations of the relationship are mediated by the indi-
Madson (1997) hypothesized that individuals with an interdepen- vidual's self-disclosure and her partner's perceptions of her re-
dent self-construal would be more likely to self-disclose to others, sponsiveness. Previous research has shown that perceptions that
to be sensitive to others, and to describe their relationships as one's partner is responsive to one's needs mediate the effects of
close, and the studies described above provide self-report evidence self-disclosure on the closeness of a relationship (Laurenceau,
in support of these claims. But do other people concur with these Barrett, & Pietromonaco, 1998). Thus, we hypothesized that indi-
self-descriptions? In this study of dyadic interactions, we exam- viduals who score high on the RISC scale will be more likely to
ined the degree to which individuals' self-construals were associ- self-disclose and will consequently be viewed as responsive by
ated with their partners' evaluations of their openness and respon- their partners in a get-acquainted task. As a result, their partners
siveness in the interaction and to their general evaluation of the will evaluate their relationship more positively than will partners
relationship. of low scorers.
To address this issue, one might look at long-term relationships, This study, therefore, goes beyond self-reports to examine in-
asking the partners of people who are high or low in the RISC formants' ratings of an interaction designed to promote closeness
scale to describe them. Although close friends or romantic partners in a controlled setting. It permits us to test the question posed
can provide helpful information on these dimensions, their re- earlier: Do the partners of individuals with a very interdependent
sponses may be biased. Individuals tend to choose friends and self-construal concur with their self-reports of open self-disclosure
relationship partners who are similar to themselves (Byrne, 1971) and sensitivity to their relationship partners? In addition, this study
and who see them as they see themselves (Swann, De La Ronde, allows us to examine the processes whereby individuals with an
& Hixon, 1994). As a result, a close friend of a person with a very interdependent self-construal begin building a relationship. We
interdependent self-construal may also have a very interdependent expect that these individuals will view this situation much like any
self-construal and may tend to project his or her attitudes and other opportunity to get to know a new acquaintance and so will
beliefs onto the person. The friend also may be aware of the open up about themselves and will be perceived as responsive by
target's self-views, and respond as he or she thinks the target their partners, resulting in positive evaluations of the relationship
would. by their partners.
One way around this problem is to provide an experimental
setting in which randomly paired individuals take time to get to Method
know each other, much like two acquaintances might begin to form
Participants and Procedure
a friendship. Using this approach, researchers can simulate the
process of developing a friendship but reduce some of the biases Participants were 181 women from introductory psychology classes who
inherent in the perceptions of close friends or partners. Fortu- participated in exchange for course credit. They signed up for the study in
nately, Aron, Melinat, Aron, Vallone, and Bator (1997) have groups of four, and when they arrived at the lab, they were told that the
study addressed problem solving by pairs. Specifically, they were told that
shown that a temporary feeling of closeness among two previously
the study addressed whether or not knowing one's partner had any impact
unacquainted individuals can be developed in an experimental on how effectively pairs solved problems. Participants then provided
context. Drawing on research emphasizing self-disclosure as the informed consent. The experimenter checked at this point to determine if
defining feature of a close relationship, they describe a structured any of the participants knew each other. Participants were randomly
set of activities that asks participants to describe their thoughts, assigned to partners (except for instances in which two participants knew
feelings, and experiences during a 45-min period. They found that each other previously; in this case, they were randomly assigned to one of
the participants in the experimental group described their relation- the two remaining participants).
ships as closer than did members of a control group, in which After completing the RISC scale in individual cubicles, the participants
participants engaged in relatively superficial small talk for the were seated in cubicles with their randomly assigned partner. (When an
same amount of time. Certainly the closeness generated in the odd number of participants showed up for the study, one person was paired
laboratory is not the same as that created in the context of an with a confederate. These participants' responses were dropped from all
ongoing relationship, but this method permits one to model the analyses.) Participants were told that they had been assigned to the con-
dition in which the partners spent some time getting to know each other
processes involved in the development of close relationships and
before the problem solving task (in fact, all participants got to know either
to reduce some of the biases inherent in ongoing relationships.
another participant or a confederate).
802 CROSS, BACON, AND MORRIS

The procedure for the interaction task was adapted from the Aron et al. before the interaction. Responses were made on a 6-point Likert-type scale
(1997) closeness task. Each partner was given a set of cards, and each card ranging from 1 (not at all) to 6 (we knew each other pretty well). No
had a different question on it, such as "Given the choice of anyone in the participants indicated that they knew their partners pretty well. Five cou-
world, whom would you want as a dinner guest?" The Aron et al. (1997) ples were dropped for failing to follow directions appropriately. The final
questions range in the sensitivity of the information elicited; after piloting sample was comprised of 68 pairs.
these questions On a separate group of participants, we selected a set of 15
questions that varied in the degree of disclosure elicited. (The cards were
arranged with the least revealing questions first.) Members of the pairs
Results
were asked to alternate asking and answering the questions, with each
The first issue addressed in these analyses was whether an
person responding to every question. Participants were instructed that they
actor's RISC scale score was related to her partner's perceptions of
could take as long as they wanted on any single question and should feel
free to skip any questions mat they preferred not to answer. They were the actor's degree of self-disclosure and responsiveness. Kenny
given a maximum of 15 min to work through the set of questions. (1996) recommends that when analyzing data from interchange-
After the interaction, participants returned to separate cubicles and able dyad members, researchers should first examine the correla-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

completed several questionnaires about their interactions. They were as- tions between the two dyad members1 scores on the variables of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sured that these evaluations would not be seen by their partner. Following interest to test for nonindependence. As one would expect, pair-
completion of these questionnaires, participants were carefully and thor- wise intraclass correlations (Griffin & Gonzalez, 1995) revealed
oughly debriefed and dismissed. significant correlations between partners' scores for self-disclosure
(r = .31), perceptions of one's partner's disclosure (r = .34),
perceptions of one's partner's responsiveness (r = .44), satisfac-
Measures tion with the interaction (r = .37), liking for the partner (r - .39),
and closeness to the partner (r — .19) (all ps < .05).
For all instruments except one of the selection variables (see below)
responses were provided on a 5-point Likert-type scale, with responses To examine dyadic effects, Kenny (1996) recommends the use
ranging from 1 (not at all/strongly disagree) to 5 (very much/strongly of pooled regressions, which allow for the computation of the
agree). effect of an actor's characteristics on her own evaluations of the
Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal (RISC). Responses to the interaction (termed the "actor effect") and the effect of her part-
11-item RISC scale demonstrated a good internal consistency with a ner's characteristics on the actor's evaluations (termed the "partner
Cronbach's alpha of .84. effect"). The values computed in this analysis represent unstand-
Disclosure. Disclosure was measured with eight items from the Self- ardized regression coefficients. As the first two lines of Table 6
Disclosure Index (Miller et al., 1983). (Two items that were judged to be
show, participants whose partners scored high on the RISC scale
inappropriate for this situation were dropped from the scale.) Participants
perceived their partners as disclosing more about themselves and
were asked to complete this scale twice, once for themselves and once for
how much their partner disclosed. Cronbach's alpha was .76 for own as being more responsive to their needs and concerns than did
disclosure and .83 for perceptions of one's partner's disclosure. participants whose partners scored low on the RISC scale (sum-
Partner's responsiveness. In this study, responsiveness was operation- marized in the "partner effect" column). In addition, an individu-
alized as the demonstration that one understands, cares for, and values al's own RISC score was related to how she perceived her partner
one's partner (Reis & Shaver, 1988). Perceptions of the partner's respon- (summarized in the "actor effect" column).
siveness were measured with six items adapted from a scale by Cutrona,
Hessling, & Suhr (1997), In addition, the authors created six items de-
signed to tap the perceptions that one has been understood, cared for, and
validated (Reis & Patrick, 1996). The items are listed in the Appendix. Table 6
Cronbach's alpha was .92. Estimation of Actor and Partner Effects of the RISC Scale on
Liking. Liking was assessed with three items from Miller et al. (1983) Evaluations of the Dyadic Interaction (Study 3)
and one additional item constructed by the current researchers. The addi-
tional item was "How much would you like to work with your partner on Actor Partner
the upcoming problem solving task?" Cronbach's alpha was .88. Dependent variable effect* effect* t df
Satisfaction. Satisfaction was measured with five commonly used
items. Examples of the items included "How much did you enjoy the Perceptions of partner's .54 7 22*** .19 2.55** 70
interaction with your partner?" and "How satisfied were you with your disclosure
interaction with this partner?" Cronbach's alpha was .88. Perceptions of partner's .69 13.26*** .36 6.97*** 71
responsiveness
Closeness. The Subjective Closeness Index (Berscheid et al., 1989)
Self-disclosure .45 6.74*** .25 3.75*** 70
was used to measure perceived closeness. This measure includes the items Satisfaction .55 7 JI*** .44 6.17*** 70
"Relative to all your other relationships, how would you characterize your Liking of partner .55 6.54*** .42 4.98*** 70
relationship with this person?" and "Relative to what you know about other Closeness to partner .40 4.45*** .17 1.90* 69
people's close relationships, how would you characterize your relationship
with this person?" A third item added by the current authors was "Right Note. N = 68 pairs. RISC = Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal.
now, how close do you feel to your partner?" Cronbach's alpha of the The values in the actor effect and partner effect columns represent un-
3-item scale was .84. standardized regression coefficients. The formula suggested by Kenny
(1996) to approximate the degrees of freedom results in different values for
Selection variables. As a check on participants' attitudes toward the
each analysis.
interaction task, they were asked to rate the extent to which they and their 11
The actor effect is the effect of an actor's RISC scale score on her own
partners took the discussion time seriously. If either member of a pair evaluations of the interaction.
indicated that they did not take the discussion time seriously, the couple b
The partner effect is the effect of an actor's partner's RISC scale score on
was excluded from all analyses. A total of three couples were excluded for the actor's evaluations of the interaction.
this reason. Participants were also asked how well they knew their partners *p < .05. **p < .01. ***/? < .001, one-tailed.
RELATIONAL-INTERDEPENDENT SELF-CONSTRUAL 803

Participants' scores on the RISC scale were positively related to were expected to predict the individual's overall satisfaction (paths
their own ratings of their self-disclosure (see Table 6 under "actor d). In addition, we examined the possible direct effects of an
effect"), replicating the findings of Study 1. There was also a actor's RISC scale scores on her perceptions of her partner's
significant partner effect on ratings of self-disclosure: Individuals responsiveness (paths e) and on her partner's perceptions of the
paired with a person who scored high on the RISC scale described actor's responsiveness (paths f). We also included possible direct
themselves as self-disclosing more than did individuals paired with effects of an actor's self-disclosure on her perceptions of her
a low-scoring partner. Table 6 also shows that participants' ratings partner's responsiveness (paths g), on her own overall satisfaction
of their satisfaction with the discussion, their liking of their part- with the relationship (paths h), and on her partner's overall satis-
ner, and feelings of closeness to their partner were significantly faction (paths i).
related to their own RISC scale scores and to their partner's scores. To test this model, structural equation analyses were conducted
As hypothesized, individuals paired with a person who scored high using maximum likelihood estimation from the LISREL 8 pro-
on the RISC scale were more satisfied with the interaction, liked gram. Members of the dyads were randomly assigned to be per-
their partner more, and felt closer to their partner than did indi- son 1 (PI) or person 2 (P2); Table 7 presents the correlation matrix
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

viduals paired with a person who scored low on the RISC scale. and the standard deviations for the variables used in this model. In
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

We hypothesized that an actor's self-disclosure and her part- these analyses, the path coefficients for the two participants were
ner' s ratings of the actor's responsiveness would mediate the direct constrained to be equivalent (e.g., the path from Pi's RISC score
effects of the actor's self-construal on her partner's overall eval- to Pi's self-disclosure score was set to be equivalent to the path
uations of the interaction. To create a single index of participants' from P2's RISC score to P2's self-disclosure score). In addition,
overall evaluations of the interaction, the measures of satisfaction, the model included the correlations between the parallel measures
liking, and closeness were standardized and summed together. The from the two participants (e.g., the correlation between P i ' s self-
reliability for this composite satisfaction score was .95. The hy- disclosure and P2's self-disclosure was included in the model).
pothesized relations among partners' RISC scale scores, self- Although this initial model presented a relatively good fit to the
disclosure, perceptions of their partner's disclosure, and overall data, the modification indices and the path coefficients indicated
satisfaction with the relationship are shown in Figure 1. This that the paths from RISC scores to perceptions of the partner's
model includes the direct effects of an actor's RISC scale scores on responsiveness (paths f) and the paths from self-disclosure to one's
her partner's overall satisfaction (paths a), as well as mediators of partner's overall satisfaction score (paths i) were nonsignificant
this relation. The most important relations in the model are cap- and should be dropped from the model.
tured by paths b, c, and d: An actor's RISC scale scores are This revised model was found to fit the data well, ^ ( 1 6 , JV =
expected to be related to her own self-disclosure (paths b), which 68) = 26.83, p < .05, GFI = .92, CFI = .93. The standardized
will in turn predict her partner's perceptions of the actor's respon- path coefficients from this model are presented in Figure 2. Par-
siveness (paths c). Perceptions of one's partner's responsiveness ticipants' RISC scale scores were directly related to their partner's

Pi's overall
satisfaction with the
Pis perception of relationship
her partner's
responsiveness

P2's overall
satisfaction with the
relationship
P2 s perception of
her partner's
responsiveness

Figure 1. Initial path model. The bold arrows represent the paths in the hypothesized mediation model. RISC =
Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal scale; PI = Partner 1; P2 = Partner 2.
804 CROSS, BACON, AND MORRIS

Table 7
Correlations and Standard Deviations of Partner 1 (PI) and Partner 2 (P2) Variables Used in
the Mediation Model Predicting Overall Satisfaction With the Interaction (Study 3)

Variable 1

1. PI RISC scale (-56)


2. PI self-disclosure .23* (.58)
3. PI perceptions of partner's .13 .32** (-55)
responsiveness
4. PI overall satisfaction .17 .31** .77** (2.23)
5. P2 RISC scale .19 .26* -.04 .13 (.47)
6. P2 self-disclosure -.08 .25* .04 .10 .22* (-60)
7. P2 perceptions of partner's -.03 51** 31** .40** .40** .45** (-47)
responsiveness
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

8. P2 overall satisfaction .05


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

,38** .27* .24* .24* .54* .64* (2.21)

Note. RISC = Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal. Standard deviations of each variable are on the
diagonal.
*p < .05. **p < .01, one-tailed.

overall satisfaction with the relationship such that individuals cantly related to their partner's perceptions of their responsiveness
paired with a person who had a very interdependent self-construal (path c), which were strongly related to their partner's satisfaction
were more satisfied and liked their partner more than did individ- with the relationship (path d).
uals paired with a partner who scored low on the RISC scale. In
addition, there were significant indirect effects of RISC scale
Discussion
scores on participants' partners' composite satisfaction scores,
= =
Ppi RISC-P2 evaluation - ^ 0P2 RISC-PI evaluation -03> P» < .05, Disclosing personal information about oneself and being sensi-
one tailed. As shown by the bold arrows in Figure 2, individuals tive and responsive to one's partner's disclosures are central pro-
who scored high on the RISC scale tended to self-disclose more cesses in the development of relationships (Laurenceau et al.,
than did others (path b); the self-disclosure scores were signifi- 1998; Reis & Shaver, 1988). We hypothesized that individuals

Pi's overall
satisfaction with the
Pi's perception of relationship
her partner's
responsiveness

P2's overall
satisfaction with the
relationship
P2 s perception of
her partner's
responsiveness

Figure 2. Final path model. All paths are significant atp < .05, one-tailed. The bold arrows represent the paths
in the hypothesized mediation model. RISC = Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal scale; PI = Partner 1;
P2 = Partner 2. Correlations among the parallel measures for PI and P2 were as follows: RISC scale, r = .18,
ns; self-disclosure, r = .20, ns; perceptions of partner responsiveness, r = .21, p < .05; overall evaluation,
r = .07, ns.
RELATIONAL-INTERDEPENDENT SELF-CON STRUAL 805

who scored high on the RISC scale would be more likely than these studies ranged from fairly small (d = -.17) to moderate
others to use these strategies to develop new relationships. To (d - -.57). These effect sizes are similar in magnitude to many
avoid the biases inherent in ongoing relationships, we asked ran- other gender differences in behavior (Hyde, 1996).
domly assigned strangers to take some time becoming acquainted We have mentioned throughout the article that individuals who
and then to indicate how open and responsive their partners had have a very interdependent self-construal should be motivated to
been. As expected, individuals' RISC scale scores were positively develop and maintain close relationships, but we unfortunately
related to their partner's ratings of the person's self-disclosure and overlooked measures of related motives, such as the intimacy
responsiveness. In addition, participants who were paired with a motive or the need for affiliation, in our examination of the
person who scored high on the RISC scale evaluated their rela- convergent and discriminant validity of the RISC scale. Me Adams
tionship with their partner more positively than did participants describes the intimacy motive as "the desire for warm, close, and
who were paired with a person who scored low on the RISC scale. communicative relations with others" (1989, p. 53), and we would
Structural equation analyses showed that the relation between expect the RISC scale to correlate fairly highly with a measure of
individuals' RISC scale scores and their partners' overall satisfac- this motivation. This relation should not be symmetric, however: A
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tion with the interaction and liking of their partner were mediated person who has constructed an interdependent self-construal
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

by the individual's degree of self-disclosure and the partner's should also score high on the measure of intimacy motivation, but
perceptions of the individual's responsiveness. This model sug- the reverse may not be true. As McAdams (1989) explains, many
gests that persons with a very interdependent self-construal are people who score high on intimacy motivation may not have
more likely to self-disclose than are others; individuals who are identified this tendency or have incorporated it into their
very disclosing are in turn evaluated as responsive by their part- self-views.
ners. Finally, participants who view their partners as responsive We focused the remainder of the research on the associa-
and sensitive to their concerns evaluate the relationship more tion between the relational-interdependent self-construal and
positively. In short, these results suggest that individuals with a relationship-oriented thoughts and behaviors. Consistent with the
very interdependent self-construal develop and nurture new rela- hypothesis that individuals with a very interdependent self-
tionships by being open about themselves and by showing sensi- construal will have a larger social network, we found that individ-
tivity and concern for their relationship partners, even when these uals who scored high on the RISC scale listed more people who
partners are randomly assigned strangers. were very important to them and perceived higher levels of social
support than did lows (Study 1). Individuals who scored high on
the RISC scale also described their most important relationships as
General Discussion closer and more committed than did others. These results provide
We report the development of a self-report measure that focuses criterion validity for the RISC scale in that they support the
on individuals' self-representations as relational or interdependent hypothesis that individuals with a very interdependent self-
with close others. In Study 1, we found strong evidence of the construal should be especially motivated to develop and maintain
scale's reliability and validity. The 11-item measure taps a single, close relationships. Additional research is necessary to uncover
general factor and evidences good internal and test-retest reliabil- more about the processes and motivations underlying these asso-
ity. An examination of the RISC scale's associations with other ciations. The findings of Study 1, however, suggest that quality
measures of interdependence, independence, and personality at- (i.e., self-disclosure and mutual influence), not quantity, of time
tests to its convergent and discriminant validity. The RISC scale spent with close others differentiates the approach to relationships
predicted unique variance in global measures of collective self- adopted by very interdependent persons from that adopted by those
esteem and communal orientation when other related constructs who do not think of themselves as interdependent with others.
were controlled. Thus, we have good evidence that the RISC scale If close relationships are part of the self, then individuals should
measures a construct that is reasonably distinct from other similar take care to promote and protect these relationships. In Study 2,
measures. highly interdependent persons were more likely than others to take
These results revealed no zero-order correlations between the into account the opinions or needs of friends and family when
RISC scale and measures of well-being or self-esteem. Other making important decisions. When close relationships are self-
research has found that individuals who are very needy or depen- defining, then the needs and wishes of close others may be nearly
dent on others tend to be vulnerable to depression (Rude & as important or salient as one's own needs and wishes. In addition,
Burnham, 1995). Our results suggest that defining oneself in terms decisions made without heeding their implications for close others
of close relationships need not necessarily lead to these negative may cause conflict or threaten relationships.
consequences. Further research is necessary, however, to explore Study 3 supported the findings of Studies 1 and 2, showing that
the conditional relationships between the interdependence of the when individuals with a very interdependent self-construal were
self-concept and well-being. For the person with a very interde- asked to get to know a new person, they were more likely to reveal
pendent self-construal, overall well-being may depend at least in information about themselves and to be viewed as responding
part on the well-being of close relationships. If close relationships sensitively to their relationship partners than were others. These
are threatened or undergoing periods of difficulty, the person with conclusions were based not only on self-reports, but also on the
a very interdependent self-construal may suffer more than the reports of their partners in these interactions. Thus, this study
person whose self-concept is not based on close relationships. provides important criterion validity for the RISC scale.
These studies also supported the hypothesis that women would In addition, Study 3 begins to answer the question of how
be more likely to define themselves in terms of their close rela- individuals with an interdependent self-construal create and main-
tionships than would men. The effect sizes for the RISC scale in tain close relationships. The structural equation analyses of these
806 CROSS, BACON, AND MORRIS

dyadic interactions revealed that individuals with a very interde- Altman, I., & Taylor, D. A. (1973). Social penetration: The development of
pendent self-construal tended to self-disclose more, which was interpersonal relationships. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
related to their partners' perceptions that they were concerned and Antonucci, T. C. (1986). Hierarchical mapping technique. Generations, 10,
caring; these perceptions that one's partner was responsive were 10-12.
strongly related to positive evaluations of the relationship. Thus, Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollen, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self
scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality
individuals' RISC scale scores are not only related to their own
and Social Psychology, 63, 596-612.
perceptions and evaluations of themselves and their relationships,
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., Tudor, M , & Nelson, G. (1991). Close relationships
but also to others' perceptions of them. These findings are cross- as including other in the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
sectional and based on a sample of college women; replication in chology, 60, 241-253.
longitudinal studies with a more diverse sample will promote Aron, A., Melinat, E., Aron, E. N., Vallone, R. D., & Bator, R. J. (1997).
further understanding of the role of the self-construal in relation- The experimental generation of interpersonal closeness: A procedure
ship processes. Future studies should also examine the relationship and some preliminary findings. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
of the self-construal to other relationship-enhancing processes in letin, 23, 363-377.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ongoing close relationships. Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia:
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

University of Pennsylvania Press.


Bellah, R. R , Madsen, R., Sullivan, W. M., Swidler, A., & Tipton, S. M.
Research Applications
(1985). Habits of the heart: Individualism and commitment in American
When a relational orientation is conceptualized at the level of life. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
the self-concept, researchers can draw upon the extensive research Belle, D. (Ed.). (1982). Lives in stress: Women and depression. Beverly
on the self to more clearly specify the mechanisms and processes Hills, CA: Sage.
that underlie the relations between interdependence and other Berscheid, E., Snyder, M., & Omoto, A. M. (1989). The relationship
phenomena. There has been much speculation about how viewing closeness inventory: Assessing the closeness of interpersonal relation-
oneself as relational or interdependent may influence behavior, but ships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 792-807.
Blaine, B., & Crocker, J. (1993). Self-esteem and self-serving biases in
empirical tests of these hypotheses have been thwarted by the lack
reactions to positive and negative events: An integrative review. In R. F.
of an appropriate measure. For example, Cross and Madson (1997)
Baumeister (Ed.), Self-esteem: The puzzle of low self-regard (pp. 5 5 -
suggest that many theories and research findings in the area of
85). New York: Plenum Press.
self-enhancement processes have assumed an independent self- Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1991). Measures of self-esteem. In J. P.
construal. They argue that the strategies used to enhance the self Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of
may vary, depending on the structure of the self. With this new personality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 115-160). San Di-
measure, researchers are better equipped to test these and other ego, CA: Academic Press.
hypotheses specifically, thus better understanding the sources of Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this "we"? Levels of
social behavior. Similarly, some theorists have attributed gender collective identity and self-representations. Journal of Personality and
differences in behavior to differences in the self-construal (Cross Social Psychology, 71, 83-93.
& Madson, 1997; Markus & Oyserman, 1989; Surrey, 1991), but Byrne, D. (1971). The attraction paradigm. New York: Academic Press.
others contend that most gender differences in behavior are due to Clark, M. S., Ouellette, R., Powell, M. C , & Milberg, S. (1987). Recipi-
differences in status or power (Ridgeway, 1988; Snodgrass, 1992). ent's mood, relationship type, and helping. Journal of Personality and
With this measure, researchers can more carefully identify the Social Psychology, 53, 94-103.
independent and joint effects of the self and the situation in social Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Professional manual: Revised NEO
behavior. Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO five factor inventory (NEO-
FFI). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
A critical next step is to examine the relation between this Cross, S. E. (1995). Self-construals, coping, and stress in cross-cultural
construct and individuals' self-views as independent and autono- adaptation. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26, 673-697.
mous. As Trafimow, Triandis, and Goto (1991) have suggested, Cross, S. E., & Madson, L. (1997). Models of the self: Self-construals and
these two types of self-representations may be stored separately gender. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 5-37.
from each other in memory and accessed with different frequen- Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (I960). A new scale of social desirability
cies. The effects of the interdependent self-construal on independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24,
relationship-oriented behavior may depend on the extent to which 349-354.
an individual has developed independence-related self-views. For Cutrona, C. E. (1989). Ratings of social support by adolescents and adult
example, the person with a very interdependent self-construal who informants: Degree of correspondence and prediction of depressive
symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 3-14.
also has well-elaborated self-views as independent and self-reliant
Cutrona, C. E., Hessling, R. M., & Suhr, J. (1997). The influence of
may be best able to balance the needs of relationship partners with
husband and wife personality on marital social support interactions.
his or her own needs (see Helgeson, 1994, for a review). A
Personal Relationships, 4. 379-393.
consideration of the relational-interdependent self-construal may Cutrona, C. E., & Russell, D. (1987). The provisions of social relationships
open the doors to new understanding of the role of the self in and adaptation to stress. In W. H. Jones and D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances
cognition, motivation, well-being, and social interaction. in personal relationships (Vol. 1, pp. 37-68). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differ-
References ences in empathy. JSAS: Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychol-
ogy, 10, 85.
Allen, R. L., Dawson, M. C , & Brown, R. E. (1989). A schema based Deiner, EM Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The
approach to modeling an African American racial belief system. Amer- Satisfaction with Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49,
ican Political Science Review, 83, 421-442, 71-75.
RELATIONAL-INTERDEPENDENT SELF-CONSTRUAL 807

Griffin, D., & Gonzalez, R. (1995). Correlational analysis of dyad-level Moran, P. B., & Eckenrode, J. (1991). Gender differences in the costs and
data in the exchangeable case. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 430-439. benefits of peer relationships during adolescence. Journal of Adolescent
Guisinger, S., & Blatt, S. J. (1994). Individuality and relatedness: Evolu- Research 6, 396-409.
tion of a fundamental dialectic. American Psychologist, 49, 104-111. Oved, Y. (1988). Two hundred years of American communes. New Bruns-
Harter, S. (1993). Causes and consequences of low self-esteem in children wick, NJ: Transaction Press.
and adolescents. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Self-esteem: The puzzle of Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for
low self-regard (pp. 87-116). New York: Plenum Press. research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measure-
Helgeson, V. S. (1994). Relation of agency and communion to well-being: ment, 1, 385-401.
Evidence and potential explanations. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 412- Realo, A., Allik, J., & Vadi, M. (1997). The hierarchical structure of
428. collectivism. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 93-116.
Helmreich, R. L., Spence, J. T., & Wilhelm, J. A. (1981). A psychometric Reis, H. T., & Patrick, B. C. (1996). Attachment and intimacy: Component
analysis of the Personal Attributes Questionnaire. Sex Roles, 7. 1097- processes. In E. T. Higgins and A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psy-
1108. chology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 523-563). New York: Guil-
Hyde, J. (1996). Half the human experience. Lexington, MA: Heath. ford Press.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

James, W. (1890/1983). The principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Reis, H. T., & Shaver, P. (1988). Studying social interaction with the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Harvard University Press. Rochester Interaction Record, In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in exper-
Kashima, Y., Yamaguchi, S., Kim, U., Choi, S. C , Gelfand, M. J., & Yuki, imental social psychology (Vol. 24, pp. 269-318). San Diego, CA:
M. (1995). Culture, gender, and self: A perspective from individualism- Academic Press.
collectivism research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, Ridgeway, C. L. (1988). Gender differences in task groups: A status and
925-937.
legitimacy account. In M. Webster and M. Foschi (Eds.), Status gener-
Kelley, H. H., Berscheid, E., Christensen, A., Harvey, J. H., Huston, T. L., alization: New theory and research (pp. 188-206). Stanford, CA: Stan-
Levinger, G., McClintock, E., Peplau, L. A., & Peterson, D. R. (1983). ford University Press.
Close relationships. New York: Freeman.
Roberts, T., &. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1989). Sex differences in reactions to
Kenny, D. A. (1996). Models of non-independence in dyadic research. evaluative feedback. Sex Roles, 21, 725-747.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 13, 279-294.
Roberts, T., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1994). Gender comparisons in re-
Lauxenceau, J.-P., Barrett, L. F., & Pietromonaco, P. R. (1998). Intimacy as
sponsiveness to others' evaluations in achievement settings. Psychology
,an interpersonal process: The importance of self-disclosure, partner
of Women Quarterly, 18, 221-240.
disclosure, and perceived partner responsiveness in interpersonal ex-
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton,
changes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1238-1251.
NJ: Princeton University Press.
Luhtanen, R., &. Crocker, J. (1992). A collective self-esteem scale: Self-
Rude, S. S., & Burnham, B. L. (1995). Connectedness, neediness: Factors
evaluation of one's social identity. Personality and Social Psychology
of the DEQ and SAS dependency scales. Cognitive Therapy and Re-
Bulletin, 18, 302-318.
search, 19, 323-340.
Lykes, M. B. (1985). Gender and individualistic vs. collectivistic bases for
Rude, S. S., Welch, N., & Sanders, B. (1998). The development and
notions about the self. In A. J. Stewart & M. B. Lykes (Eds.), Gender
and personality: Current perspectives on theory and research (pp. validation of a scale measuring the construct of connectedness. Unpub-
268-295). Durham, NC: Duke University Press. lished manuscript, University of Texas.
Marin, G., & Triandis, H. C. (1985). Allocentrism as an important char- Rusbult, C. E. (1983). A longitudinal test of the investment model: The
acteristic of the behavior of Latin Americans and Hispanics. In R. development (and deterioration) of satisfaction and commitment in
Diaz-Guerrero (Ed.), Cross-cultural and national studies in social psy- heterosexual involvement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
chology (pp. 69-80). Amsterdam: North Holland. ogy, 45, 101-117.
Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for Sampson, E. E. (1985). The decentralization of identity: Toward a revised
cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224- concept of personal and social order. American Psychologist, 40, 1203-
253. 1211.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1994). A collective fear of the collective: Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health:
Implications for selves and theories of selves. Personality and Social Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies.
Psychology Bulletin, 20, 568-579. Health Psychology, 4, 219-247.
Markus, H., & Oyserman, D. (1989). Gender and thought: The role of the Schwalbe, M. L., & Staples, C. L. (1991). Gender differences in sources of
self-concept. In M. Crawford & M. Hamilton (Eds.), Gender and self-esteem. Social Psychology Quarterly, 54, 158-168.
thought (pp. 100-127). New York: Springer-Verlag. Shaver, P. R., & Brennan, K. A. (1991). Measures of depression and
Matsumoto, D., Weissman, M. D., Preston, K., Brown, B. R., & Kupper- loneliness. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. W. Wrightsman (Eds.),
busch, C. (1997). Context-specific measurement of individualism- Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 195-
collectivism on the individual level: The Individualism-Collectivism 290). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Interpersonal Assessment Inventory. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychol- Shweder, R. A., & Bourne, E. J. (1984). Does the concept of the person
ogy, 28, 743-767. vary cross-culturally? In R. A. Shweder & R. A. LeVine (Eds.), Culture
McAdams, D. P. (1989). Intimacy: The need to be close. New York: theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 158-199). Cambridge,
Doubleday. England: Cambridge University Press.
McCombs, H. (1985). Black self-concept: An individual/collective analy- Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdepen-
sis. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 9, 1-18. dent self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20,
McGrath, E., Keita, G. P., Strickland, B. R., & Russo, N. F. (1990). Women 580-591.
and depression: Risk factors and treatment issues. Washington, DC: Snodgrass, S. E. (1992), Further effects of role versus gender on interper-
American Psychological Association. sonal sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62,
Miller, L. C , Berg, i. H., & Archer, R. L. (1983). Openers: Individuals 154-158.
who elicit intimate self-disclosure. Journal of Personality and Social Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Stapp, J. (1974). The Personal Attributes
Psychology, 44, 1234-1244. Questionnaire: A measure of sex-role stereotypes and masculinity-
808 CROSS, BACON, AND MORRIS

femininity. Journal Supplement Abstract Service Catalog of Selected Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO:
Documents in Psychology, 4, 43-44. Westview Press.
Surrey, J. (1991). The "Self In-relation": A theory of women's development. In Weiss, R. S. (1974). The provisions of social relationships. In Z. Rubin
J. V. Jordan, A. G. Kaplan, J. B. Miller, I. P. Stiver, & J. L. Suirey (Eds.), (Ed.), Doing unto others (pp. 17-26). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice
Women's Growth in Connection (pp. 51-66). New York: Guilford Press. Hall.
Swann, W. B., Jr., De La Ronde, C , & Hixon, J. G. (1994). Authenticity Yamaguchi, S. (1994). Collectivism among the Japanese: A perspective
and positivity strivings in marriage and courtship. Journal of Personality from the self. In U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, C. Kagitcibasi, S. C. Choi, and
and Social Psychology, 66, 857-869. G. Yoon (Eds.), Individualism and collectivism (pp. 175-188). Newbury
Tesser, A. (1988). Toward a self-evaluation maintenance model of social Park, CA: Sage.
behavior. In L. Berfcowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy- Yamaguchi, S., KuhJman, D. M., & Sugimori, S. (1995). Personality
chology (Vol. 21, pp. 181-227). New York: Academic Press. correlates of allocentric tendencies in individualist and collectivist cul-
Trafimow, D., Triandis, H. C , & Goto, S. G. (1991). Some tests of the tures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26, 658-672.
distinction between the private self and the collective self. Journal of Zuckerman, D. M. (1989). Stress, self-esteem, and mental health: How
Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 649-655. does gender make a difference? Sex Roles, 20, 429-444.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural Zung, W. W. K. (1965). A self-rating depression scale. Archives of General
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506-520. Psychiatry, 12, 63-70.

Appendix

Items Used to Assess Perceptions of One's Partner's Supportiveness (Study 3)

1. I felt as if my partner really cared about me."


2. My partner behaved warmly toward me.a
3. My partner listened carefully when it was my turn to talk.
4. My partner tried to see things from my point of view.
5. My partner made me feel comfortable about myself and my feelings.3
6. My partner seemed sensitive to my feelings.8
7. My partner seemed uncaring.
8. My partner showed respect for my capabilities and talents.0
9. My partner did not seem to take my concerns seriously.3
10. My partner seemed sincere during our interaction.
11. My partner made me feel valued as a person.
12. My partner seemed to understand my concerns.
1
From the Interaction Supportiveness Scale. Copyright 1997 by C. E. Cutrona. Printed with permission.

Received June 28, 1999


Revision received September 2, 1999
Accepted November 2, 1999

You might also like