Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

J Child Fam Stud

DOI 10.1007/s10826-014-0070-x

ORIGINAL PAPER

Perceptions of Parenting Styles and Their Associations


with Mental Health and Life Satisfaction Among Urban
Indonesian Adolescents
Amina Abubakar • Fons J. R. Van de Vijver •

Angela O. Suryani • Penny Handayani •


Weny Savitry Pandia

 Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Abstract The study aimed at investigating the associa- Keywords Adolescents  Indonesia  Parenting styles 
tion between maternal and paternal parenting styles and Psychological well-being
psychological well-being among Indonesian adolescents.
The Parental Authority Questionnaire (PAQ), General
Health Questionnaire-12 (GHQ-12), and the Brief Multi- Introduction
dimensional Students’ Life Satisfaction Scale, were
administered to 500 adolescents. We were unable to rep- The study of parenting styles focuses on the emotional
licate the three-factor solution of the PAQ using confir- climate that parents create while raising their children
matory factor analysis. The permissive subscale (Darling and Steinberg 1993). Since the introduction of the
demonstrated poor psychometric properties; it was there- concept, parenting styles have been a popular area of study
fore not included in any further analysis. Mothers were in developmental psychology. The influence of parenting
perceived to be more authoritative than fathers; on the has been studied across cultural contexts, including Asia.
other hand, fathers were perceived to be more authoritarian However, most Asian studies have been carried out in
than mothers. Both maternal and paternal authoritative countries with a Confucianist cultural influence (i.e.,
parenting styles were positively associated with outcomes. China, Taiwan, and Korea). With respect to the literature
Authoritarian parenting was not associated with any out- on parenting Indonesia, an Asian country with the largest
come. Scores computed to represent perceived differences Muslim population in the world, presents an interesting
between maternal and paternal use of various parenting case study. Here, religion, an important cultural variable,
styles were associated with reported GHQ-12 and life influences parenting practices, strategies, goals, and the
satisfaction scores. Our results confirm Western findings on emotional climate at home (Acevedo et al. 2013; Danso
the positive effects of authoritative parenting, but do not et al. 1997; Godina 2013; Wilcox 1998).
replicate the negative associations of authoritarian parent- Baumrind’s (1971, 1978) typology of parenting styles
ing. Future studies that examine different parenting styles has had a powerful influence on the study of parenting.
at the construct level are needed to elucidate the associa- According to this typology, the combination of warmth and
tion between parenting styles and adolescent psychological demandingness defines three kinds of parenting styles:
functioning in the Indonesian and other similar contexts. authoritative, authoritarian and permissive. Authoritative
parenting, characterized by a child-centered approach,
combines a high degree of parental warmth, structure and
autonomy support (Baumrind 1972). Authoritarian parent-
A. Abubakar (&)  F. J. R. Van de Vijver
Department of Culture Studies, Tilburg University, ing involves high demands for obedience and compliance
P.O. Box 90153, 5000 LE Tilburg, The Netherlands to a set of rules, accompanied by low levels of parental
e-mail: A.AbubakarAli@uvt.nl warmth. Permissive parenting is seen as involving few
behavioral expectations for the child and high levels of
A. O. Suryani  P. Handayani  W. S. Pandia
Atma Jaya Catholic University of Indonesia, Jl. Jenderal parental warmth. In a refinement of Baumrind’s theory, a
Sudirman 51, Jakarta 12930, Indonesia differentiation of neglectful and indulgent parents within

123
J Child Fam Stud

the permissive parenting style has been made (Maccoby self-esteem, and poor academic performance (Herz and
and Martin 1983). Gullone 1999; Lamborn 1991; Smetana 2006). Interest-
Cross-cultural research confirms the existence of these ingly, a slightly different picture emerges in some Eastern
parenting styles, as well as their salient influence on child cultures where people are more interdependent and place a
and adolescent development (Abu-Rayya 2006; Ang 2006; high value on relatedness, interpersonal harmony, and
Cheah et al. 2009; Eisenberg et al. 2009; Garcia and Gracia obedience to authority. Whereas authoritative parenting is
2009; Huver et al. 2009; Snoek et al. 2007). However, the seen to be most beneficial similarly to Western cultures,
associations between parenting style and child outcomes findings on the influence of authoritarian parenting have
are moderated by cultural and contextual factors as socio- been controversial. Some studies have observed that chil-
cultural factors may determine both the preferred parenting dren of Asian origin benefit from authoritarian parenting,
styles and their associations with outcome (Assadi et al. while others report the lack of an effect. For instance, Chao
2007; Jackson-Newsom et al. 2008). Cultural values, (1994) observed that authoritarian parenting was associated
norms, and goals influence parenting beliefs, practices, with higher academic performance among children of
goals, and styles (Harkness and Super 1996). Chinese immigrants in the US. She notes that differences in
In terms of interpersonal relationships, cultures may how the concept functions among Chinese parents may
differ in the manner in which they emphasize the relative result from an ethnocentric, Western-based conceptualiza-
contribution of autonomy and relatedness (Kagitçibasi tion. Chao proposed a different approach in conceptualizing
2006; Keller 2012). In cultures where autonomy, personal and interpreting parenting styles adopted by Chinese par-
achievement and self-directedness are emphasized, parents ents. Specifically, she explains that Chinese parents tend to
will involve children in decision-making and provide view socialization as ‘‘training’’, hence the positive effects
explanations both for the rules they set and the conse- of a controlling parenting approach in the Chinese context.
quences of one’s behavior. Authoritative parenting would In line with this view, a study by Leung et al. (1998) found
be the most ideal style in such cultures; on the other hand, that an authoritarian parenting style was positively associ-
authoritarianism or strict parenting would be viewed neg- ated with the academic achievement of Hong Kong Chinese
atively by children and contribute to adverse outcomes. students, whereas an authoritative style was unrelated to
Other cultural groups place more emphasis on group har- academic achievement. These findings have not been con-
mony, cohesion, and common goals. Respect for hierarchy sistently replicated, with researchers such as Pong et al.
and authority are considered paramount to maintaining the (2009) reporting that authoritarianism had a negative impact
cohesiveness of the group. In such cultural groups, children on children’s (both European Americans and Asian Amer-
would perceive strict parenting that is accompanied by icans) academic achievement in the US and in Taiwan.
warmth as acceptable. Recent studies among Asians (e.g., These inconsistent results provide the impetus for further
Koreans) illustrate that warm and strict parenting can co- research in this field.
exist (Choi et al. 2013). Similarly, the relationships between permissiveness and
Kagitçibasi (2006) noted that parenting styles may be adolescent outcome are not consistent. Several studies have
influenced by societal developments. Many countries with reported that permissiveness, like authoritarianism, had a
a strong tradition of relatedness and interdependency are negative impact on child outcome. It has been suggested
becoming more modern and urbanized as their populations that undercontrolled environments, typical of permissive
acquire more education. In such countries, a value pattern homes, do not foster self-regulation in children, which may
in which both economic independence and emotional result in children who are impulsive and underachieving
interdependence are valued can emerge. This leads to (Barber 1996; Onatsu-Arvilommi and Nurmi 1997).
parenting behavior that encourages an autonomous-rela- However, other studies have associated permissiveness
tional self. Parenting strategies aimed at encouraging an with positive psychological adjustment. For instance,
autonomous-related self may be especially salient for the Garcia and Garcia (2009) in an investigation involving
population in Jakarta, where they are experiencing rapid 1,416 teenagers from Spain reported that their findings
social and economic changes. suggested that indulgent (permissive) parenting was the
In largely industrialized Western countries with highly optimum style of parenting in the Spanish context. Ado-
educated populations, there is a strong socialization towards lescents whose parents favored this parenting style scored
autonomy. Authoritative parenting which is associated with higher in self-esteem and lower in anxiety than their
positive child and adolescent outcomes, such as social com- counterparts with authoritative parents.
petence, self-reliance, and achievement orientation (Baum- Understanding parenting in Indonesia requires knowl-
rind 1978; Hein and Lewko 1994; Lopez et al. 2008) is the edge of both the cultural and religious background, as
preferred style of parenting. In these countries, authoritarian religion constitutes an important component of Indonesian
parenting has been associated with maladjustment, low culture. It is important to keep in mind that religious beliefs

123
J Child Fam Stud

help shape socialization goals and parenting strategies of findings provide a sound justification for investigating
Indonesian parents, even though it is impossible to disen- paternal and maternal parenting styles separately. In cul-
tangle cultural and religious factors. Islam emphasizes tures where there is still a strong emphasis on different
group harmony, coupled with a strong leaning towards roles for mothers and fathers such as in Indonesia, these
respect and deference to the authority of elders and parents differences are likely to be amplified. In the traditional
in particular. The vast majority of Indonesian families Indonesian setting, as the head of the family, the father is
therefore emphasize closeness and harmony (rukun), loy- expected to cater for family needs, make important deci-
alty, obligation, respect (hormat), and voluntary coopera- sions on behalf of the family, and punish grave misconduct
tion (gotong-royong) (Adamson 2007; Magnis-Suseno on the part of the children (Setiadi 2006). On the other
1991; Piercy et al. 2005). It is an obligation for children to hand, mothers provide emotional support to the family
obey their parents and elders and to preserve family honor. alongside providing day to day care for the child (Setiadi
Social relationships are strictly regulated with a strong 2006). Such apparent delineation of gender roles is likely
tendency towards maintaining a respectable distance from to contribute to differences in patterns of mother–child
those in authority. The father, who is customarily the interactions compared to father–child interactions.
head of the family, makes most of the important decisions As stated earlier, the child’s gender may influence
for the family. Magnis-Suseno (1991) observed that perceived parenting styles (Chen et al. 2000). A significant
Indonesian fathers largely keep their distance from their number of studies indicate that boys report a higher score
children and are expected to punish any gross misbe- on authoritarianism in comparison to girls. It is likely that
havior by them. Mothers, on the other hand, spend more boys have a tendency towards seeking relatively more
time with children and are responsible for their care and autonomy from parents compared to girls. The differential
discipline. Despite the father’s formal ‘leadership posi- treatment of sons and daughters has been observed in
tion’ within the Indonesian family context, the mother is varying contexts. We therefore expect that the relation-
very influential. Older daughters are expected to take up ships between gender and parenting in Indonesia will be
some maternal responsibilities, such as household chores similar to what has been reported in other cultural
and sibling care. Parents’ socialization processes are contexts.
therefore geared towards ensuring that children are ready Past studies indicate that several sociodemographic
to take up these responsibilities within the family and the characteristics, including parental education and socio-
larger society. economic status, influence preferred parenting practices. In
Both parent and child gender may influence the par- one of these studies, Bluestone and Tamis (1999) observed
enting style adopted. As noted by Forehand and Nousiainen that maternal education was positively associated with
(1993), most studies have not considered the potential preferences for child-centered practices (authoritative par-
differences between mothers’ and fathers’ parenting. And enting) among working and middle class African American
yet, research has reported differences in adolescents’ per- families. Similar findings have been reported among an
ceptions of their mothers and fathers and in the influences Iranian (Assadi 2007) and Malaysian sample (Tam et al.
of mothers’ and fathers’ parenting practices on adolescent 2013). However, contrary to these findings, Lipps et al.
outcomes. For example, adolescents from the US tend to (2012) observed that parental education does not influence
link more emotional attributes to mothers and more rigid preferred parenting style in studies carried out in four
and formal attributes to fathers (Pipp et al. 1985). More- Caribbean Islands. The literature suggests that parenting
over, it has been found that there is a difference in the can be conceptualized as mediating the link between
preferred parenting styles among couples, and that these background variables and socialization outcomes (e.g.,
differences may have a significant impact on child out- Bank et al. 1993; Raver et al. 2007; Raviv et al. 2004). This
comes. In a recent study among Dutch adolescents, Hoeve link is tested in the current study.
et al. (2011) observed that about half of the parent dyads in Little if any research has been carried out on parenting
their study demonstrated discrepant parenting styles with in the Indonesian context. We could only identify two
fathers adopting different styles from mothers. Important unpublished reports (both bachelor’s theses) addressing the
differences in the relationship between adolescent delin- effects of parenting styles on adolescent outcomes. In the
quent behavior and parenting styles emerged when they first study involving adolescents aged 16–18 years, Apri-
investigated the effects of differing styles of fathers and liaty (2005) reported that there was a similar number of
mothers. Furthermore, the study found that although any authoritative and authoritarian parents (Nauthoritative = 48;
inconsistency between parenting styles tends to be dam- Nauthoritarian = 47) in her sample. This number was higher
aging to the child, the benefits of having at least one than that of permissive (N = 33) and uninvolved parents
authoritative parent outweighed the negative effects of (N = 30). The second study on parenting styles in Jakarta,
inconsistent parenting styles (Hoeve et al. 2011). These reported that most adolescents (aged 12–14 years)

123
J Child Fam Stud

perceived their parents as practicing authoritative (N = 66) Method


rather than authoritarian (N = 46) or permissive parenting
(N = 32) (Pahur 2008). The first study found no significant Participants
relationship between parenting and self-concept (Apriliaty
2005), while the study by Pahur (2008) found that the A total of 500 participants were recruited from five schools.
social-interaction abilities of adolescents with authoritative Data from 16 pupils were excluded from further analysis
parents were more developed than those of adolescents because of incomplete questionnaires. The mean age of
with authoritarian or permissive parents. These widely respondents was 15.67 years (SD = .71, min. = 14,
discrepant results on the relationship between parenting max. = 18). Fifty-five percent of those who responded were
and outcome may be attributed to various reasons including female. The majority of the participants were Muslims
sample characteristics, sample size, analytic approach, and 89.6 % (n = 432), with Christians and Hindus constituting
measurement instruments, among others. Further evalua- 9.5 % (n = 46) and 0.4 % (n = 2) of the sample, respec-
tion of Indonesian parenting styles will address the existing tively. There was one Buddhist and one self-identified atheist
knowledge gap by providing a more in-depth understand- in the sample. Data on parental educational level showed that
ing of the association between parenting styles and ado- there were fewer fathers (n = 5, 1 %) than mothers (n = 18,
lescent outcomes. 3.7 %) with no schooling at all; 9.1 % (n = 44) of mothers
We aimed to evaluate possible differences in parenting compared to 5.6 % (n = 26) of fathers had achieved primary
styles of Indonesian mothers and fathers, as reported by school education; while the majority of both parents had
their adolescent children, and investigate the relationship completed secondary school (mothers: 52.7 %, n = 255;
between parenting styles and psychological well-being of fathers: 47.1 %, n = 228) and post-secondary school training
adolescents. Based on the reviewed literature and the (mothers: 34.5 %, n = 167; fathers: 46.5 %, n = 225).
sociocultural context of the study, we set out to test the
following hypotheses: Procedures
Hypothesis 1 There are significant differences between
Students attending five randomly selected secondary
maternal and paternal parenting styles with mothers scor-
schools in Jakarta were sampled for inclusion in this study.
ing higher on authoritative and permissive parenting
Permission for collecting data in the school was sought
compared to fathers, who score higher on authoritarianism;
from the heads of schools and departments in accordance
Hypothesis 2a There is a significant difference between with procedures laid down by the schools. After obtaining
boys’ and girls’ perceptions of parenting styles, with boys permission from the school, we sought and obtained assent
expected to report more authoritarianism than girls, and from the students. Research assistants then administered
girls expected to report more authoritativeness than boys; the questionnaires to the students during scheduled group
sessions. Three research assistants were present at each
Hypothesis 2b Parental education is negatively associ-
session to give instructions and ensure that each student
ated with the use of an authoritarian parenting style and
filled in the questionnaire independently.
positively associated with the use of authoritativeness;
Hypothesis 2c There is a significant association between Measures
a child‘s age and parenting styles. Older children are
expected to report more authoritarian parenting; Using a forward translation and back-translation approach, an
expert panel comprising the second, third, and fourth authors,
Hypothesis 3a Both maternal and paternal authoritative
together with a research assistant adapted all the measures into
parenting styles show a positive association with psycho-
Bahasa, the national language of Indonesia. Any discrepan-
logical well-being of adolescents;
cies in the translations were resolved through a committee
Hypothesis 3bi There is a significant positive association approach. A pilot study involving 60 adolescents (not inclu-
between adolescent psychological well-being and both ded in the main study) was carried out prior to the main study
maternal and paternal authoritarianism; to establish whether or not the students could understand and
respond to the questionnaires. We conducted a psychometric
Hypothesis 3bii There is a significant negative associa-
evaluation of all the measures used in the study.
tion between adolescent psychological well-being and both
maternal and paternal authoritarianism; and,
Parenting Styles
Hypothesis 4 Inconsistency in the style preferred by
mothers and fathers is negatively associated with the ado- The Parental Authority Questionnaire (PAQ; Buri 1991) is
lescents’ psychological functioning. a 30-item questionnaire designed to measure parental

123
J Child Fam Stud

authority, or disciplinary practices, from the point of view sample size, we did not consider the Chi square in the
of the child (of any age). The PAQ has three subscales: current study. We instead used the Tucker Lewis Index
permissive, authoritarian, and authoritative/flexible par- (TLI) and Comparative Fit Index (CFI) for which values
enting. The items are rated using Likert scale format, with [.95 are considered to reflect an excellent fit, while values
scores ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly between .95 and .90 are indicative of an acceptable fit. In a
agree). Sample items include ‘As I was growing up my multigroup analysis, the change in CFI is an important
mother gave me clear direction for my behavior and indicator for evaluating the suitability of hierarchically
activities, but also understood when I disagreed with her’ nested models; a CFI change of \.010 is taken to be sup-
and ‘As I was growing up my mother directed the activities portive of the more restrictive model. To supplement the fit
and decisions of the children in the family through rea- tests, the root mean square error of approximation
soning and discipline’. Given that this was a new measure (RMSEA) is also reported since it has been shown to be
in the Indonesian setting, we carried out a full validation of sensitive to model misspecification; values of \.06 are
the scale (discussed in the ‘‘Results’’ section). considered indicative of a good fit to the model while those
between .06 and .08 are considered acceptable. Second, to
Poor Mental Health investigate between group differences a series of t tests
were carried out. Finally, path analytic procedures were
The 12-item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12) was used to investigate the relationship between background
administered to evaluate mental health (Goldberg 1992). variables, parenting styles and adolescent mental health
The GHQ-12 was developed as a screening tool for outcomes.
detecting minor psychiatric disturbance or strain. The
measure assesses changes in affective and somatic symp-
toms. In the current study, a Likert-type response scale was Results
used, with scoring options of 0–1–2–3 (e.g. ‘Have you
recently felt that you couldn’t overcome your difficulties, The three-factor model proposed for the PAQ maternal and
0 = not at all, 1 = no more than usual, 2 = rather more paternal scales by the authors had poor fit indices on the
than usual, 3 = much more than usual’). CFA (see Fig. 1 for the evaluated model and fit indices).
An exploratory factor analysis (EFA) not reported here also
Life Satisfaction confirmed that the structure could not be replicated in this
sample. For both maternal and paternal models, the fit
The Brief Multidimensional Students’ Life Satisfaction problems were related to the permissiveness scale. Few
Scales (BMSLSS) which is scored on a 7-point Likert scale items loaded significantly on the permissiveness factor. To
ranging from 1 (terrible) to 7 (delighted) was also understand which of the subscales still had acceptable
administered (Huebner et al. 2006). This scale has five psychometric properties, we conducted CFAs for each
items inquiring on satisfaction with different domains (e.g., separately. The authoritative scales had an acceptable fit
school and family) as well as a global question. A sample for both maternal and paternal items, v2(33, N =
item includes ‘I would describe my satisfaction with my 488) = 92.131, p \ .001, v2/df = 2.632, TLI = .906,
school experiences as…’. CFI = .927, RMSEA = .058, and v2(34, N = 488) =
93.99, p \ .001, v2/df = 2.888, TLI = .922, CFI = .940,
Social Demographics RMSEA = .062, respectively. For the maternal scales, the
modification indices indicated the need to correlate some
We collected information on students’ age, gender, reli- errors between items. To the extent that it allowed for
gious affiliations and parental education. comparison of the scores from these groups, we carried out
a multigroup invariance analysis to check if the factor
Data Analysis Plan structure for the subscales for mothers and fathers was
similar (see Table 1 for the results of the multiple group
Three steps were followed in analyzing the data. First, analysis). Results from this analysis indicate that we
given that the PAQ is relatively new in Indonesia and that achieved partial scalar invariance. We removed constraints
no validation study has been conducted as yet, we used for one of the items (Item 11: ‘As I was growing up I knew
confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to investigate the fac- what my mother expected of me in my family, but I also
torial structure of the PAQ. We assessed the goodness of fit felt free to discuss those expectations with my mother
for each model using various parameters. The general when I felt they were unreasonable’) to achieve acceptable
guideline is that a non-significant Chi square reflects an fit indices. The authoritarian scales had an acceptable fit for
acceptable fit to the data. However, given its sensitivity to both maternal, v2(33, N = 488) = 92.131, p \ .001, v2/

123
J Child Fam Stud

1 6 10 13 14 17 19 21 24 28

2
4

3
5

7 8

PERMISSIVE
9 11

AUTHORITARIAN
12
15

AUTHORITATIVE
16
20

18
22

25
23

26
27

2 2
29 Maternal χ (402, N = 488) = 1308.11, p < .001, χ /df =3.254, TLI = .573, CFI =
.605,RMSEA .068
30
Paternal: χ2(402, N = 488) = 1307.18, p < .001, χ2/df =3.252, TLI = .674, CFI =
.698, RMSEA .068 .

Fig. 1 Factorial structure of the PAQ and fit indices of this model in our samples

df = 2.632, TLI = .906, CFI = .927, RMSEA = .058, exception of the maternal authoritarian scale whose value
and paternal items, v2(34, N = 488) = 93.99, p \ .001, v2/ was .66 (this scale was retained as it was because, apart
df = 2.888, TLI = .922, CFI = .940, RMSEA = .062. from good fit indices in the structural equation modeling
Moreover a multiple group analysis indicating that we analyses, its alpha level was within the acceptable range).
achieved scalar invariance justified the comparison of There was a significant difference in the scores for
scores from these two scales. The permissiveness subscale mother’s (M = 39.19, SD = 5.45) and father’s authorita-
had a poor fit for both maternal, v2(35, N = 488) = tive/flexible style (M = 37.92, SD = 5.93), t(487) = 6.06,
207.21, p \ .001, v2/df = 5.920, TLI = .495, CFI = .607, p \ .001. These results suggest that Indonesian adolescents
RMSEA = .101; and paternal items, v2(35, N = 488) = perceived their mothers to show more authoritative/flexible
335.69, p \ .001, v2/df = 9.59 TLI = .264, CFI = .428, patterns than their fathers although the effect size was
RMSEA = .133. Further analyses, using both EFA and small (Cohen’s d = .22). With the authoritarian subscale,
CFA indicated two problems with this scale. First, nega- fathers (M = 30.23, SD = 6.12) scored significantly
tively worded items formed a different subscale; in anal- higher than mothers (M = 29.70, SD = 5.43), t(487) =
yses not documented here, modeling the impact of 2.41, p \ .016. The effect size was small (d = .09). These
negatively worded items significantly improved the fit of results support the first hypothesis.
the scales. Second, two of the items in this scale performed Table 2 presents a correlation matrix of background
very badly, either by having very low or negative loadings; factors, parenting styles and child outcomes. We postulated
an example is item 1 (While I was growing up my mother a full mediation model in which background factors (age,
felt that in a well-run home the children should have their gender, and parental education) would have a direct effect
way in the family as often as the parents do’). Given the on parenting styles and parenting style would have a direct
generally problematic fit and a poor alpha (.51) for the effect on psychological well-being (life satisfaction and
permissiveness scale we did not use it any further. Further mental health).
analyses therefore only focus on the authoritarian and While the hypothesized model for the authoritative
authoritative subscales. Table 2 presents the alpha coeffi- subscale showed a good fit to the data, several path coef-
cients for each of the scales. The alphas for all our scales ficients were not significant. The model was modified
were above the recommended value of .70, with the to exclude non-significant paths (Fig. 1 shows the

123
J Child Fam Stud

Table 1 Invariance of paternal and maternal measures


Model v2 df Dv2 (df) v2/df RMSEA TLI CFI DCFI

Authoritarian model
Unconstrained model 147.76 70 – 2.11 .034 .897 .920 –
Measurement weights 160.80 79 13.04(9) 2.03 .033 .904 .916 .004
Measurement intercepts (full scalar invariance) 177.80 89 17.00(10) 1.99 .032 .908 .909 -.005
Authoritative model
Unconstrained model 223.29 70 – 3.19 .047 .895 .918 –
Measurement weights 247.21 79 24.92(9) 3.12 .047 .898 .910 .002
Measurement intercepts (partial scalar invariance) 274.12 88 26.91(9) 3.11 .047 .898 .901 .009

Table 2 Means, SDs and correlations between the key study variables
Mean (SD) Alpha 1 2 3 4 5 6

1 Maternal 39.19 (5.24) .75 1


authoritativeness
2 Maternal authoritarianism 29.70 (5.43) .66 .035 1
3 Paternal authoritativeness 37.93 (5.93) .75 .514** .108* 1
4 Paternal authoritarianism 30.23 (6.12) .73 .070 .596** .096* 1
5 Life satisfaction 27.13 (4.63) .78 .219** .055 .252** .061 1
6 Mental health 11.50 (6.28) .82 -.218** .007 -.331** -.046 -.417** 1
7 Parental education – – .019 -.071 .030 .065 -.035 -.051
*p \ .05; **p \ .01

non-significant paths using dashed lines). The modified (Kline 2005). The model indicated that in this sample,
model was tested and it showed a non-significant Chi square authoritarian parenting was not associated with psycho-
fit value (v2(1, N = 488) = 1.703, p = .977, v2/df = logical well-being. Neither Hypothesis 3bi nor 3bii were
1.703), pointing to an excellent fit. Other fit indices showed supported, since authoritarianism did not have any rela-
similar results, TLI = .977, CFI = .998, and RMSEA = tionship with psychological well-being. In addition, we
.038, suggesting a good fit of the data to the hypothesized observed only a partial confirmation of Hypothesis 2a–2c,
model (Kline 2005). The model explained 11 % of the as the background variables were only related to authori-
variance in mental health outcomes and 7 % in life satis- tarianism and some of the relationships were in an unex-
faction. The model indicated that authoritative parenting pected direction.
was positively associated with psychological well-being. To investigate the degree of concordance in the use of
Our results support Hypothesis 3a; however, Hypotheses different styles, we computed the absolute difference in
2a–2c were not supported as authoritative parenting was scores between maternal and paternal parenting styles. The
unrelated to gender, education and child’s age. difference scores were then included in a path model in
The hypothesized model for the authoritarian subscale which they were predictors of life satisfaction and mental
showed a good fit to the data; however, since several path health. The results indicated that several path coefficients
coefficients were not significant, we modified the model. were not significant. The model was modified to exclude
Figure 2 displays the model with non-significant paths path coefficients that were not significant; Fig. 3 shows the
presented as dashed lines. The modified path analytic non-significant paths using dashed lines. The modified path
model excluding all non-significant paths was tested and analytic model showed a non-significant Chi square fit
the results showed a non-significant Chi square fit value value (v2(1, N = 488) = 1.819, p = .177, v2/df = 1.819),
(v2(4, N = 488) = 3.950, p = .413, v2/df = .988), which which points to an excellent fit. Other fit indices showed
points to an excellent fit. Other fit indices showed similar similar results, TLI = .956, CFI = .996, and RMSEA =
results, TLI = 1.00, CFI = 1.00 and RMSEA = .000, .041. The model explained only 4 % of the variance in
confirming the fit of the data to the hypothesized model mental health and 1 % in life satisfaction. Our fourth

123
J Child Fam Stud

Gender Maternal .10 Life satisfaction


Authoritativeness

Parental .51 -.36

Education
..20
Paternal
Poor Mental
Age Authoritativeness -..33 Health

Gender .11 Maternal Life satisfaction


.09+ Authoritarianism

Parental .60 -.36


.11
Education
-.14
Paternal
Poor Mental
Age -.09 Authoritarianism
Health

Fig. 2 The standardized coefficients on the antecedents and conse- represent non-significant coefficients, solid arrows represent signif-
quences of different parenting styles. Note: All path coefficients icant coefficients. Female coded as 1, male coded as 2
presented are significant at p \ .05. ? p = .051. Dotted arrows

Fig. 3 The standardized Differences


coefficients on the relationship Authoritativeness -.08+
Life
between differences in scores
.14 Satisfaction
between maternal and paternal
parenting styles and
psychological well-being
Note: ? p = .051, The dotted
arrows represents non-
significant coefficients, solid Differences
arrows represent significant Authoritarianism
coefficients. Parenting styles -.14
were allowed to correlate and all
these correlations were
significant
Poor Mental
Health

hypothesis was only partially supported. The relationships adaptation of Indonesian adolescents. We carried out a
between parental inconsistency and adolescent outcomes series of confirmatory analyses to examine the factorial
are much more nuanced than hypothesized as its associa- structure of the PAQ. Consistent with earlier findings, such
tion with psychological function seems to vary with par- as a recent study in India by Raval et al. (2013), we were
enting style. not able to replicate the underlying three-factor structure of
the PAQ. In terms of psychometric properties, Raval et al.
(2013) also observed that the authoritarian scale showed
Discussion the best performance whereas the permissiveness scale was
the most problematic. Our conclusions differ slightly from
The current study aimed at investigating the influence of those of a recent study from Japan which was able to
maternal and paternal parenting styles on the psychological replicate the three-factor solution of the PAQ (Uji et al.

123
J Child Fam Stud

2013). Closer scrutiny however reveals that the authors is that the construct as defined may not be relevant to the
excluded up to five items on the problematic permissive- Chinese culture; rather, it may be better perceived as ‘train-
ness scale. We offer two potential explanations for the ing’. Our study generally supports the view that, like parents
problematic nature of the permissiveness scale. Firstly, the in the West, most Indonesian parents prefer to use the
permissiveness construct is probably not sufficiently clear authoritative style. Three aspects are worth highlighting.
as currently assessed by PAQ. The lack of a distinction First, is that some of the associations we noted in our study
between neglectful and indulgent parenting is potentially a may be due to the urban nature of our sample. Compared to
problem, especially in a cross-cultural study. Secondly, their rural counterparts, parents in Jakarta, being largely
item wording may also be potentially problematic. We middle class, with higher educational levels and a relatively
were not able to examine the first explanation further in our high exposure to the Western cultural context, were more
sample; however, we found strong evidence to support the likely to experience change in the way they structure their
second explanation. Several negatively worded items family life and their parenting behavior, in line with the
within the permissive scale seem to create a spurious sec- family change model (Kagitçibasi 2005). Second, a study
ond factor within the scale. Future attempts to simplify the where vignettes (which ensure ecological validity and reduce
wording of this scale may produce a better measure of measurement bias due to social desirability) rather than a
permissiveness in a cross-cultural context. standard scale (such as the PAQ) were used reported that
Consistent with our first hypothesis and evidenced by Indian students preferred authoritative parenting over other
studies from elsewhere (Shek 1998), mothers were per- styles (Barnhart et al. 2013). This convergence of results
ceived to be more authoritative and permissive, whereas indicates that the preference for authoritative parenting may
fathers were perceived to be more authoritarian. The be more common in non-Western cultures than previously
observed results concur with our knowledge of the Indo- acknowledged. Third, the positive influence of authoritarian
nesian cultural context. Social interactions are strictly parenting that has been reported among the Chinese may be
regulated and the father, the main disciplinarian in the due to the role of training. Confucian cultures tend to value
family, is shown respect and deference. Within such a training and self-improvement highly; in such a context,
context, fathers are likely to appear to be more authoritarian parents are probably more focused on teaching
authoritarian (i.e., less warm) than the mother. Our findings their children. Confucianism is not salient in Indonesia.
are however in contrast with findings from studies in other Therefore, authoritarianism effects may be comparable to
largely Muslim, collectivistic cultures like the Arab world, those in most parts of the world.
where no differences were observed between maternal and Our findings demonstrate that the authoritative parenting
paternal style (Dwairy et al. 2006). Noteworthy is the style is most likely to be associated with positive sociali-
findings from studies within cultures that are geographi- zation outcomes across cultural contexts. Both maternal
cally and culturally similar to Indonesia. A study in and paternal authoritative parenting styles were positively
Malaysia observed other differences in reported parenting associated with mental health and life satisfaction of the
styles (Elias and Yee 2009). Mothers in that study were adolescents. Contrary to earlier findings, we did not find
more likely to display authoritarian styles compared to any relationship between authoritarianism and psycholog-
fathers. These differing patterns could be attributed to ical well-being (Chao 1994; Leung 1998). Whereas
methodological issues. Elias and Yee (2009) classified authoritarianism has an adverse effect in the West, these
parents according to the dimension in which they scored negative effects have not been reported through studies in
highest whereas we used the highest mean score to deter- several Middle-Eastern and Chinese samples. Two major
mine which parenting style was more common (we also did lines of thinking may explain the observed patterns of
not allocate parents to specific styles based on scale results. First, it has been suggested that there is a need to
means). The use of mean scores and midpoints is ques- interpret these results within the larger cultural context. In
tionable because of its overreliance on sample-specific Western cultures where strict parenting practices may be
parameters. This can lead to misclassification especially in construed as rejection and denial of love, children will have
homogeneous samples where all scores may be relatively adverse outcomes due to the distress created by these
high or low (see Arends-Tóth and van de Vijver 2007 for practices. On the other hand, in cultures where parental
the same classification issues in acculturation research). authority and respect for hierarchy is the norm, children
The literature has not conclusively established what the may expect and accept strict parenting. Based on this
preferred parenting style in non-western contexts is (Juang hypothesis, Dwairy et al. (2006; see also Dwairy 2008,
et al. 2013). A few studies have suggested that authoritari- 2010) posit that the negative impact of parenting styles
anism may be relatively common; however, other researchers adopted by parents may result from inconsistencies
have questioned the cultural invariance of the concept itself. between the parenting style and the cultural context.
For instance, Chao (1994) argues that one of the main issues Dwairy et al. (2006) concluded that the ‘inconsistency

123
J Child Fam Stud

between authoritarian parenting and the liberal culture in contexts; however, despite using a comprehensive
the west, rather than authoritarianism per se, constitutes the approach to evaluating the scale, we were not able to fully
main reason behind the negative impact of authoritarian replicate its factor structure. Two characteristics of the
parenting in western society’ (p. 23). Second, a new line of scale may account for this failure. The first, with regards to
research indicates that the Western conceptualization of item formulation, relates to the potential problems arising
parenting styles may not adequately capture the construct from the length of the items, complexity of wording,
in other cultural groups. Choi et al. (2013) observe that negatively worded and double-barreled items. The second,
‘results present a nuanced and detailed picture of Korean at the construct level could be the bias introduced through
American parenting as a blend of Western concepts of the use of items that are too similar to clearly differentiate
Authoritative and Authoritarian styles and show the coex- between permissive and authoritative parenting styles. Our
istence of positive and-although limited-negative parent- study findings therefore suggested that the PAQ does not
ing’ (p. 26). seem to function well in the Indonesian context, and that a
Children experiencing inconsistency in parenting styles more emic approach is needed to develop satisfactory
were more likely to experience poor mental health and items. Future research concerned with refinement of the
lowered life satisfaction. These results are in line with items and scale is required if the scale is to be used in a
empirical evidence (Dwairy 2010; Fletcher et al. 1999) and non-Western context.
theory, particularly the family systems theory which pro- Another limitation of this study is the use of an exclu-
poses that mother’s and father’s parenting styles can sively urban sample. This may have precluded the oppor-
interdependently contribute to adolescent outcomes. A tunity to obtain information on parenting styles and its
more refined analysis of the different patterns of incon- influences from a more traditional Indonesian sample.
sistency or a study of the family parenting typology will Future efforts should focus on a broader sample. Moreover,
provide a clearer understanding of the differential impact our study involved only a limited number of antecedents and
of various combinations of parenting styles. It is note- outcome variables. The inclusion of more antecedent factors
worthy that the amount of variance explained by our ana- such as parental working hours, family size and structure
lysis was small. Given the differences in methods (both may have explained a greater proportion of the variance
data collection and analytic), it is difficult to carry out a observed, thus improving our study findings. A study of
direct comparison of our findings with those of earlier other aspects like academic achievement could also poten-
studies. However, an examination of one of the most tially yield more information on the impact of different
thorough investigations of this topic in the literature parenting styles on adolescent outcomes. Lastly, our use of
(Dwairy 2010) indicates that in general, the association cross-sectional data in the form of single informant reports
between parental inconsistency and psychological func- (rather than observations) means we can only base our
tioning of adolescents was weak. conclusions on associations rather than causations.
Fathers are often ignored in the parenting literature; yet, As measured by the PAQ, authoritative and authoritar-
their influence on adolescent development may be stronger ian, but not permissive, parenting seems to be valid con-
than mothers in some instances (Hoeve et al. 2009). Our structs the Indonesian context. A further refinement of the
dataset demonstrated a stronger relationship between scales to adequately capture the different parenting styles is
paternal authoritativeness and poor mental health than that necessary. The differences between maternal and paternal
found for maternal authoritativeness. These results are parenting styles were small; however, their relationships to
consistent with findings from other populations. A meta- adolescent development point to meaningful and partially
analysis of the influence of parenting on delinquency universal patterns (notably the positive associations of
observed that although only 20 % of the studies included authoritative parenting with children’s well-being and
fathers in their samples, the lack of paternal support was mental health) that may be specific to the context of our
found to be potentially more detrimental to outcomes than study (the lack of influence of authoritarianism). These
lack of maternal support (Hoeve et al. 2009). These results results emphasize the need to study maternal and paternal
provide the impetus for more research involving fathers parenting styles separately in order to understand their
especially in understudied populations such as Indonesia. influence within a given socio-cultural context.

Limitations

Several factors may limit the generalizability of the current References


findings. The first relates to the measurement instrument.
The PAQ has been widely used in the investigation of Abu-Rayya, H. M. (2006). Acculturation and well-being among Arab-
Baumrind’s typology of parenting across different cultural European mixed-ethnic adolescents in Israel. Journal of

123
J Child Fam Stud

Adolescent Health, 39, 745–751. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006. Dahesihsari, R., Purwanti, M., Halim, M. S. (1995). Pola Reaksi
04.008. Penanggulangan Masalah pada Remaja dengan Iklim Sosial
Acevedo, G. A., Ellison, C. G., & Yilmaz, M. (2013). Religion and Keluarga Kondusif dan Non Kondusif di DKI Jakarta [Coping
child-rearing values in Turkey. Journal of Family Issues. doi:10. behavior patterns of adolescents living with family in conducive
1177/0192513X13504921. and nonconductive social climate in DKI Jakarta]. Unpublished
Adamson, C. (2007). Gendered anxieties. Islam, women’s rights and research report. Jakarta, Indonesia: Atma Jaya Catholic
moral hierarchy in Java. Anthropological Quarterly, 80, 5–37. University.
Ang, R. P. (2006). Effects of parenting style on personal and social Danso, H., Hunsberger, B., & Pratt, M. (1997). The role of parental
variables for Asian adolescents. American Journal of Orthopsy- religious fundamentalism and right-wing authoritarianism in
chiatry, 76, 503–511. doi:10.1037/0002-9432.76.4.503. child-rearing goals and practices. Journal for the Scientific Study
Apriliaty, M. (2005). Perbedaan Konsep Diri Antara Remaja Putri of Religion, 36, 496–511.
yang Memiliki Persepsi Pola Asuh Authoritarian, Authoritative, Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style as context: An
Permissive, dan Uninvolved [Self-concept differences between integrative model. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 487–496.
female adolescent who perceived receiving Authoritarian, Dornbush, S., Ritter, P., Liederman, P., Roberts, D., & Fraleigh, M.
Authoritative, Permissive, and Uninvolved parenting styles]. (1987). The relation of parenting style to adolescent school
Bachelor thesis. Jakarta, Indonesia: Faculty of Psychology, Atma performance. Child Development, 58, 1244–1257.
Jaya Catholic University. Dwairy, M. A. (2008). Parental inconsistency versus parental
Arends-Tóth, J. V., & Van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2007). Acculturation authoritarianism: Associations with symptoms of psychological
attitudes: A comparison of measurement methods. Journal of disorders. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37, 616–626.
Applied Social Psychology, 37, 1462–1488. doi:10.1111/j.1559- doi:10.1007/s10964-007-9169-3.
1816.2007.00222.x. Dwairy, M. (2010). Parental inconsistency: A third cross-cultural
Assadi, S. M., Zokaei, N., Kaviani, H., Mohammadi, M. H., Padideh, research on parenting and psychological adjustment of children.
G., Gohari, M. R., et al. (2007). Effect of socio-cultural context Journal of Child and Family Studies, 19, 23–29. doi:10.1007/
and parenting style on scholastic achievement among Iranian s10826-009-9339-x.
adolescents. Social Development, 16, 169–180. doi:10.1111/j. Dwairy, M., Achoui, M., Abouserie, R., & Farah, A. (2006a).
1467-9507.2007.00377.x. Parenting styles, individuation, and mental health of Arab
Badan Pusat Statistik Republik Indonesia. (2009). Statistik Indonesia adolescents: A third cross-regional research study. Journal of
2009: Statistical yearbook of Indonesia. Jakarta: BPS-Indonesia. Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37, 262–272. doi:10.1177/00220
Bank, L., Forgatch, M. S., Patterson, G. R., & Fetrow, R. A. (1993). 22106286924.
Parenting practices of single mothers: Mediators of negative Dwairy, M., Achoui, M., Abouserie, R., Farah, A., Sakhleh, A. A.,
contextual factors. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 55, Fayad, M., et al. (2006b). Parenting styles in Arab societies—A
371–384. first cross-regional research study. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: Revisiting a Psychology, 37, 230–247. doi:10.1177/0022022106286922.
neglected construct. Child Development, 67, 3296–3319. Eisenberg, N., Chang, L., Ma, Y., & Huang, X. K. (2009). Relations
Barnhart, C., Raval, V., Jansari, A., & Raval, P. (2013). Perceptions of parenting style to Chinese children’s effortful control, ego
of parenting style among college students in India and the United resilience, and maladjustment. Development and Psychopathol-
States. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 22, 684–693. ogy, 21, 455–477. doi:10.1017/S095457940900025X.
doi:10.1007/s10826-012-9621-1. Elias, H., & Yee, T. H. (2009). Relationship between perceived
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Devel- paternal and maternal parenting styles and student academic
opmental Psychology Monographs, 4, 1–103. doi:10.1037/h0030 achievement in selected secondary schools. European Journal of
372. Social Sciences, 9, 181–192.
Baumrind, D. (1972). An exploratory study of socialization effects on Fletcher, A. C., Steinberg, L., & Sellers, E. B. (1999). Adolescents’
black children: Some black and white comparisons. Child well-being as a function of perceived interparental inconsistency.
Development, 43, 261–267. doi:10.2307/1127891. Journal of Marriage and Family, 61, 599–610.
Baumrind, D. (1978). Parental disciplinary patterns and social Forehand, R., & Nousiainen, S. (1993). Maternal and paternal
competence in children. Youth and Society, 9, 239–275. parenting: Critical dimensions in adolescent functioning. Journal
Buri, J. R. (1991). Parental Authority Questionnaire. Journal of of Family Psychology, 7, 213–221. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.7.2.
Personality Assessment, 57, 110–119. doi:10.1207/s15327752 213.
jpa5701_13. Garcia, F., & Gracia, E. (2009). Is always authoritative the optimum
Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style? Evidence from Spanish families. Adolescence,
parenting style-understanding Chinese parenting through the 44, 101–131.
cultural notion of training. Child Development, 65, 1111–1119. Godina, L. (2013). Religion and parenting: Ignored relationship?
doi:10.2307/1131308. Child and Family Social Work.
Cheah, C. S. L., Leung, C. Y. Y., Tahseen, M., & Schultz, D. (2009). Goldberg, D. (1992). General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12).
Authoritative parenting among immigrant Chinese mothers of Windsor: NFER-Nelson.
preschoolers. Journal of Family Psychology, 23, 311–320. Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (1996). Parents’ cultural belief systems:
doi:10.1037/a0015076. Their origins, expressions, and consequences. New York, NY:
Chen, X. Y., Liu, M. W., & Li, D. (2000). Parental warmth, control, The Guilford Press.
and indulgence and their relations to adjustment in Chinese Hein, C., & Lewko, J. H. (1994). Gender differences in factors related
children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Family Psychology, to parenting style—A study of high performing science students.
14, 401–419. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.14.3.401. Journal of Adolescent Research, 9, 262–281. doi:10.1177/
Choi, Y., Kim, Y. S., Kim, S. Y., & Park, I. J. (2013). Is Asian 074355489492008.
American parenting controlling and harsh? Empirical testing of Herz, L., & Gullone, E. (1999). The relationship between self-esteem
relationships between Korean American and Western parenting and parenting style a cross-cultural comparison of australian and
measures. Asian American Journal of Psychology, 4(1), 19. vietnamese Australian adolescents. Journal of Cross-Cultural
doi:10.1037/a0031220. Psychology, 30, 742–761.

123
J Child Fam Stud

Hoeve, M., Dubas, J. S., Eichelsheim, V. I., van der Laan, P. H., Mussen manual of child psychology Vol. 4 (4th ed., pp. 1–102).
Smeenk, W., & Gerris, J. R. M. (2009). The relationship between New York, NY: Wiley.
parenting and delinquency: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnor- Magnis-Suseno, F. (1991). Etika Jawa: Sebuah analisa falsafi tentang
mal Child Psychology, 37, 749–775. doi:10.1007/s10802-009- kebijaksanaan hidup Jawa [Javanese ethics: A philosophical
9310-8. analysis of Javanese wisdom]. Jakarta: Gramedia Pustaka
Hoeve, M., Dubas, J. S., Gerris, J. R., van der Laan, P. H., & Smeenk, Utama.
W. (2011). Maternal and paternal parenting styles: Unique and Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self:
combined links to adolescent and early adult delinquency. Implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psycholog-
Journal of Adolescence, 34, 813–827. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence. ical Review, 98, 224–253. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224.
2011.02.004. Milevsky, A., Schlechter, M., Klem, L., & Kehl, R. (2008).
Huebner, E. S., Suldo, S. M., Valois, R. F., & Drane, J. W. (2006). Constellations of maternal and paternal parenting styles in
The brief multidimensional students’ life satisfaction scale: Sex, adolescence: Congruity and well-being. Marriage and Family
race, and grade effects for applications with middle school Review, 44, 81–98. doi:10.1080/01494920802185447.
students. Applied Research in Quality of Life, 1, 211–216. Neto, (1993). The satisfaction with life scale: Psychometric properties
doi:10.1007/s11482-006-9016-9. in an adolescent sample. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 22,
Huver, R. M. E., Otten, R., de Vries, H., & Engels, R. (2009). 125–134. doi:10.1007/BF01536648.
Personality and parenting style in parents of adolescents. Journal Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Girgus, J. S. (1994). The emergence of gender
of Adolescence, 33, 395–402. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2009. differences in depression during adolescence. Psychological
07.012. Bulletin, 115(3), 424–443. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.424.
Jackson-Newsom, J., Buchanan, C. M., & McDonald, R. M. (2008). Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Larson, J., & Grayson, C. (1999). Explaining
Parenting and perceived maternal warmth in European American the gender difference in depressive symptoms. Journal of
and African American adolescents. Journal of Marriage and the Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1061–1072. doi:10.
Family, 70, 62–75. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00461.x. 1037/0022-3514.77.5.1061.
Juang, L. P., Qin, D. B., & Park, I. J. (2013). Deconstructing the myth Pahur, R. A. (2008). Perbedaan Kemampuan Sosialisasi antara
of the ‘‘tiger mother’’: An introduction to the special issue on Remaja yang Memiliki Persepsi Pola Asuh Authoritative,
tiger parenting, Asian-heritage families, and child/adolescent Authoritarian, dan Permissive [Social skill differences between
well-being. Asian American Journal of Psychology, 4, 1. doi:10. adolescent who percieved receiving Authoritative, Authoritarian,
1037/a0032136. and Permissive parenting styles]. Bachelor thesis. Jakarta:
Kagitçibasi, C. (2005). Autonomy and relatedness in cultural context: Faculty of Psychology, Atma Jaya Catholic University.
Implications for self and family. Journal of Cross-Cultural Piercy, F., Soekandar, A., Limansubroto, C., & Davis, S. (2005).
Psychology, 36, 403–422. doi:10.1177/0022022105275959. Family therapy with Indonesians. In M. McGoldrick, J. Pearce,
Kawabata, Y., Alink, L. R. A., Tseng, W., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & & J. Giordano (Eds.), Ethnicity and family therapy (3rd ed.,
Crick, N. R. (2011). Maternal and paternal parenting styles pp. 84–99). New York, NY: Guilford.
associated with relational aggression in children and adolescents: Pipp, S., Shaver, P., Jennings, S., Lambom, S., & Fischer, I. C. W.
A conceptual analysis and meta-analytic review. Developmental (1985). Adolescents’ theories about the development of their
Review, 31, 240–278. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2011.08.001. relationships with parents. Journal of Personality and Social
Keller, H. (2012). Autonomy and relatedness revisited. Cultural Psychology, 48, 991–1001. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.48.4.991.
manifestations of universal human needs. Child Development Proctor, C. L., Linley, P. A., & Maltby, J. (2009). Youth life
Perspectives, 6, 12–18. satisfaction: A review of the literature. Journal of Happiness
Keller, H. (2013). Culture and development: Developmental path- Studies, 10, 583–630. doi:10.1007/s10902-008-9110-9.
ways to psychological autonomy and hierarchical relatedness Raval, V., Ward, R., Raval, P., & Trivedi, S. (2013). Confirmatory
(2). Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 6(1). doi:10. and exploratory factor analyses of Parental Authority Question-
9707/2307-0919.1052. naire in Urban India. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 22,
Keshavarz, S., & Baharudin, R. (2012). The moderating role of 707–718. doi:10.1007/s10826-012-9624-y.
gender on the relationships between perceived paternal parenting Raver, C. C., Gershoff, E. T., & Aber, J. L. (2007). Testing
style, locus of control and self-efficacy. Procedia-Social and equivalence of mediating models of income, parenting, and
Behavioral Sciences, 32, 63–68. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.01. school readiness for White, Black, and Hispanic children in a
011. national sample. Child Development, 78, 96–115. doi:10.1111/j.
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practices of structural equation 1467-8624.2007.00987.x.
modeling (2nd ed.). New York, NY: The Guildford Press. Raviv, T., Kessenich, M., & Morrison, F. J. (2004). A mediational
Lamborn, S. D., Mounts, N. S., Steinberg, L., & Dornbusch, S. M. model of the association between socioeconomic status and
(1991). Patterns of competence and adjustment among adoles- three-year-old language abilities: The role of parenting factors.
cents from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 528–547.
families. Child Development, 62, 1049–1065. Setiadi, B. N. (2006). Indonesian traditional family in a changing
Leung, K., Lau, S., & Lam, W. L. (1998). Parenting styles and society. In J. Georgas, J. W. Berry, F. J. R. Van de Vijver, C.
academic achievement: A cross-cultural study. Merrill-Palmer Kagitcibasi, & Y. Poortinga (Eds.), Families across cultures: A
Quarterly-Journal of Developmental Psychology, 44, 157–172. 30-nation psychological study (pp. 370–378). Cambridge: Cam-
Lipps, G., Lowe, G. A., Gibson, R. C., Halliday, S., Morris, A., bridge University Press.
Clarke, N., et al. (2012). Parenting and depressive symptoms Shek, D. T. L. (1998). Adolescents’ perceptions of paternal and
among adolescents in four Caribbean societies. Child and maternal parenting styles in a Chinese context. Journal of
Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 6, 1–12. Psychology, 132, 527–537. doi:10.1080/00223989809599285.
Lopez, S. T., Calvo, J. V. P., & Menendez, M. D. R. (2008). Parenting Snoek, H. M., Engels, R., Janssens, J., & van Strien, T. (2007).
styles. Bibliographical revision and theoretical reformulation. Parental behaviour and adolescents’ emotional eating. Appetite,
Teoria De La Educacion, 20, 151–178. 49, 223–230. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2007.02.004.
Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of Tam, C. L., Chong, A., Kadirvelu, A., & Khoo, Y. T. (2013).
the family: Parent-child interaction. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Parenting styles and self-efficacy of adolescents: Malaysian

123
J Child Fam Stud

scenario. Global Journal of Human Social Science Research, Uji, M., Sakamoto, A., Adachi, K., & Kitamura, T. (2013). The
12(14-A). Impact of authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive parenting
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Way, N., Hughes, D., Yoshikawa, H., styles on children’s later mental health in Japan: Focusing on
Kalman, R. K., & Niwa, E. Y. (2008). Parents’ goals for parent and child gender. Journal of Child and Family Studies,
children: The dynamic coexistence of individualism and collec- 1–10. doi:10.1007/s10826-013-9740-3.
tivism in cultures and individuals. Social Development, 17, Wilcox, W. B. (1998). Conservative Protestant childrearing: Authoritar-
183–209. ian or authoritative? American Sociological Review, 796–809.

123

You might also like