Lu Et Al (2018)

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

ORIGINAL RESEARCH

published: 10 May 2019


doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00151

Surface Acting or Deep Acting, Who


Need More Effortful? A Study on
Emotional Labor Using Functional
Near-Infrared Spectroscopy
Yongbiao Lu 1 , Wenfeng Wu 1* , Gaoxing Mei 1 , Shouying Zhao 1 , Haibo Zhou 2 , Daling Li 1
and Deng Pan 1
1
School of Psychology, Guizhou Normal University, Guiyang, China, 2 School of Education, Hunan University of Science
and Technology, Xiangtan, China

Emotional labor is characterized by two main regulation strategies: surface acting and
deep acting. However, which strategy consumes more energy? To explore this, we used
functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to measure changes in hemoglobin density
while participants performed a task requiring them to make the opposite emotional facial
expression of that presented in a picture. We found that (1) neither surface nor deep
acting led to a significant change in hemoglobin concentration in the prefrontal cortex;
(2) making negative and positive facial expressions activated the same left front and
middle areas of the prefrontal cortex; and (3) making positive facial expressions activated
the rear portion of the prefrontal cortex, but making negative facial expressions did not.
Based on these findings and past work, we can infer that deep and surface acting may
Edited by:
not significantly differ in terms of the activity in the prefrontal cortex energy consumed.
Hasan Ayaz, Furthermore, engaging in positive and negative emotional labor appear to utilize some
Drexel University, United States
of the same neurological mechanisms, although they differ in others.
Reviewed by:
Meltem Izzetoglu, Keywords: emotional labor, surface acting, deep acting, fNIRS (functional near-infrared spectroscopy), energy,
Villanova University, United States prefrontal lobe
Murat Ozgoren,
Dokuz Eylül University, Turkey
*Correspondence: INTRODUCTION
Wenfeng Wu
wwfhsd@aliyun.com Emotional labor is the act of regulating one’s emotion to conform to organizational standards.
Presently, it is central to numerous service occupations that employees are the first point of
Received: 18 August 2018 contact that customers have with the organization. Scholars have been continually seeking to
Accepted: 23 April 2019 understand the emotional labor process. Some have proposed that emotional labor consists of
Published: 10 May 2019 three components (Grandey and Gabriel, 2015): emotional requirement, emotional regulation,
Citation: and emotional performance. The emotional requirements typically involve the “integrative” goal
Lu Y, Wu W, Mei G, Zhao S, of showing positive displays and hiding negative ones [although, for certain occupations (e.g.,
Zhou H, Li D and Pan D (2019) teachers), these emotional requirements may involve negative or neutral displays; Schutz and
Surface Acting or Deep Acting, Who
Lee, 2014]. Emotional regulation refers to the effort expended by employees to comply with the
Need More Effortful? A Study on
Emotional Labor Using Functional
socioemotional demands of the job. Researchers have pointed out three essential strategies of
Near-Infrared Spectroscopy. emotional regulation: surface acting, deep acting, and genuine emotional labor. Surface acting
Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:151. involves faking the required emotions: that is, when people engage in surface acting, they do
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00151 not actually try to feel the emotions they wish to portray. They may put on “fake smiles” or

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 1 May 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 151


Lu et al. Surface and Deep Acting Consumption

other required emotional displays that do not reflect their true that surface acting and deep acting were used simultaneously to
feelings. By contrast, deep acting involves putting effort into manage emotional labor demands. Although these two studies
actually feeling and expressing the required emotions. When both used experimental methods, they still relied on participants’
engaged in deep acting, people attempt to modify feelings to self-ratings of surface acting and deep acting; in other words, they
match the required display rules. Ashforth and Humphrey (1993) could not measure the effort used for emotional labor directly.
argue that, in addition to surface acting and deep acting, that Thus, we began to wonder if there is a way of measuring the effort
there is a third form of emotional labor: spontaneous and genuine used for emotional labor directly. The answer may be “yes.”
emotional labor, wherein the person expresses naturally felt According to the definition of emotional labor, which refers
emotions that align with the emotional display rules; in this to one kind of emotional regulation, so for surface acting and
case, no acting is required (Diefendorff et al., 2005). Emotional deep acting, they can be seen as the two emotional regulation
labor research has primarily focused on surface acting and deep strategies (Grandey, 2000; Grandey and Melloy, 2017). Gross
acting (Grandey et al., 2013; Gabriel and Diefendorff, 2015). (1998) suggested that the emotional regulation strategies could
Finally, emotional performance means observable expressions of be categorized as antecedent-focused and response-focused, which
emotional labor. Pugh (2001) found that emotional performance Grandey (2000) in turn connected to surface and deep acting;
was unrelated to employees’ reported moods, supporting the deep acting is considered a form of reappraisal, which is linked to
idea that these displays were strategic rather than actual antecedent-focused regulation, whereas surface acting is linked
expressions of feeling. to suppression, which is a type of response-focused regulation.
Since Hochschild (1983) coined the term “emotional labor,” A large number of neuroimaging studies have been conducted
scholars have designed numerous studies to explore its to examine the neural functions underlying the cognitive control
characteristics and mechanisms (e.g., Grandey, 2000). Among of emotion (Hare et al., 2005; Goldin et al., 2008; Etkin
them, the different strategies of emotional labor are one of the et al., 2015; Zilverstand et al., 2017; Vandekerckhove et al.,
most studied aspects—especially surface acting and deep acting 2018). Accordingly, neuroimaging technology could be useful for
(Grandey et al., 2013; Xanthopoulou et al., 2018). Meta-analyses exploring the neural mechanisms of emotional labor. However,
have revealed that deep and surface acting have different effects there are few neural studies on emotional labor—why? We
(Hülsheger and Schewe, 2011; Mesmer-Magnus et al., 2012), infer that the most important reason is the limitations such
and some interesting questions about these two strategies still studies impose on participants’ actions; participants would have
remain to be answered. For example, what are the cognitive to utilize various expressive actions so that emotional labor can
and social differences between these two strategies? Do they be experimentally studied, but most neuroimaging equipment
consume identical amounts of psychological resources or does requires participants to remain as immobile as possible. In
one consume more? Most scholars believe that deep acting is most cases, this would rule out the use of neuroimaging
more psychologically taxing than surface acting is. However, until techniques for emotional labor. However, functional near-
now, there has been no real evidence to support this assertion. infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) can be used with less bodily
The most common way of examining these two emotion constraints than other imaging modalities (Scholkmann et al.,
regulation strategies has been to ask employees to self-report the 2012). In fact, some researchers have used fNIRS to explore
extent to which they use each strategy, and then examine the cortical areas that are associated with spontaneous facial affective
relationships between each strategy and various antecedents and expressions, human emotions, somatosensory stimulation, and
outcomes (Gabriel et al., 2015). However, self-report measures other functions (Masataka et al., 2015; Naseer and Hong, 2015;
often confound acting with affective motivational experiences; Sun et al., 2015; Hong et al., 2017). Therefore, it may be suitable
this is true for both surface acting (i.e., confounded with negative for use in emotional labor. Moreover, it is silent (i.e., it has no
affect/stress) and deep acting (i.e., confounded with motivation). operating sounds), making it somewhat more comfortable for
Furthermore, self-reported results might simply be reflecting humans (Masataka et al., 2015).
effects of participants’ memory, and not emotional labor per se. Zapf (2002) discussed surface acting and deep acting from the
For this reason, some researchers have tried experimental perspective of action theory. According to this theory, deep acting
methods to explore differences between surface acting and would partly involve conscious processes at the intellectual level
deep acting. For example, Shulei and Miner (2006) presented of action regulation. In other words, the person actively tries to
participants with differing sets of instructions, before they influence his or her inner feelings to bring them in line with
watched an emotion-eliciting film, to induce surface acting and the emotions required by the organization. By contrast, surface
deep acting. The authors found that (1) the participants devoted acting is more likely to be triggered at the level of flexible action
significant effort and found it difficult to perform both surface patterns—this implies that it is partly a routine process, and
and deep acting, but deep acting engaged more attention, and does not necessarily involve conscious processes. For example,
(2) both surface acting and deep acting led to decreased sadness, despite feeling nothing, a salesperson might automatically display
while deep acting led to a stronger physiological response (e.g., a smile. Based on Zapf ’s view of deep and surface acting,
greater increase in heart rate). In another experimental study, we hypothesized that people will need greater psychological
Gabriel and Diefendorff (2015) utilized a call center simulation to resources when engaging in deep acting than when engaging
examine how shifts in customer incivility influenced continuous in surface acting.
measures of participants’ felt emotions, surface acting, deep In this study, the purpose was to verify whether people would
acting, and vocal tone during a single interaction. They found need more psychological resources when engaging in deep acting

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 2 May 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 151


Lu et al. Surface and Deep Acting Consumption

than when engaging in surface acting. To induce use of the


different strategies, we used emotional facial expressions. Because
emotional labor involves the display of positive and negative
emotions, we presented participants with positive or negative
facial expressions and asked them to make the opposite facial
expression. This also enabled us to explore whether there was a
difference in the effort for these strategies between positive and
negative emotional expressions.

MATERIALS AND METHODS


Ethics Statement
All research procedures were approved by the Ethical Committee
of Guizhou Normal University, Guiyang, Guizhou province, FIGURE 1 | Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) settings. Ten
China. All participants provided written informed consent after emitters and 10 detectors were deployed with reference to the International
fully understanding the study. 10–20 system, with a total of 31 channels. For channel 10 and channel 13, the
distance of emitter and receptor was over 3 cm, so the measurement results
Participants were canceled. The “cross” mark was used to represent the canceled results.

Twenty male and 20 female senior undergraduates were recruited


from a university in Hunan province, China. All participants
came from a teacher training specialty (e.g., education or Numerous recent studies have identified the prefrontal cortex
psychology) and were healthy and right-handed. None of (PFC) as a key region for the induction and regulation of
them had a history of psychiatric or neurological disorders. emotional responses (Doi et al., 2013). As our fNIRS instruments
Participants were randomized to the surface acting or deep could measure activity over a limited number of channels, we
acting condition. In the deep acting condition, one male and one decided to affix the optodes of the fNIRS primarily on the
female participant’s fNIRS data were not correctly obtained, so PFC. Based on that, participants would make facial expression,
the data were excluded. The average age of all participants was and refer to a study by Hong et al. (2015). Therefore, we also
21.5 ± 1.4 years. placed some optodes on the pre-motor and supplementary motor
cortexes. The details are shown in Figure 1. The optodes of fNIRS
Instrument were fixed on a full head cap designed with reference to the
We used a 24-fiber (38-channel) fNIRS system (SHIMADZU International 10–20 system. The sampling rate was set to 30 Hz.
LABNIRS System, Kyoto, Japan) in this study. Semiconductor We determined the anatomical locations of the optodes in
lasers with wavelengths of 780 nm, 805 nm, and 830 nm are relation to the standard head landmarks, including the nasion,
employed as light sources; every emitter in the LABNIRS system Cz, left tragus (T3), and right tragus (T4), using a FASTTRAK 3D
emits the three wavelength light, and as well as every detector tracking system (Polhemus). The Montreal Neurological Institute
measures the absorbance for each of these wavelengths. fNIRS (MNI) coordinates (Mazziotta et al., 2002) for the channels were
measures the amount of relative change from the initial value obtained using the NIRS-SPM software (Singh et al., 2005; Ye
of oxygenated hemoglobin (OxyHb), deoxygenated hemoglobin et al., 2009) with MATLAB 2013b (Mathworks, Natick, MA,
(deOxyHb), and total hemoglobin (totalHb), using the near- United States). The detailed MNI locations are shown in Figure 2
infrared rays. The increase in OxyHb and the concomitant and in Table 1.
decrease in deOxyHb reflects an increase in local arteriolar
vasodilatation, which increases local cerebral blood flow and Materials
cerebral blood volume, a mechanism known as neurovascular We selected 20 positive, 20 neutral, and 20 negative emotional
coupling. This produces a change in the amount of light face pictures from the Chinese Facial Affective Picture System
absorbed by this tissue, which can be measured by near (CFAPS; Bai et al., 2005) using CFAPS normative data. Among
infrared spectroscopy systems (Ferrari and Quaresima, 2012). them, eight positive, eight neutral, and eight negative pictures
These measurements are used as surrogates of brain activation were chosen to be used in the exercise phase, while the remaining
(Bulgarelli et al., 2018). The more activated, the more OxyHb were chosen to use in the formal phase. Emotion ratings were
increases, the more energy will be consumed. The adequate based on a nine-point rating scale. The average emotional valence
depth of NIR light penetration (almost one half of the source- rating of the selected pictures was 6.82 for the positive pictures,
detector distance) can be achieved using a source-detector 4.32 for the neutral, and 2.34 for the negative. Half of all the
distance around 3 cm (Dehghani et al., 2009). Compared with pictures were of men, and the others were of women.
other brain imaging devices, fNIRS is minimally restrictive and
can accurately record measurements even when individuals are Questionnaire
moving, making it useful for studying brain activity under more To identify whether participants had engaged with the tasks, we
“natural” conditions. It is also totally non-invasive. asked participants to complete a 4-item questionnaire after the

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 3 May 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 151


Lu et al. Surface and Deep Acting Consumption

while the fNIRS was operating. Subsequently, emotional face


pictures were presented on the screen (315 × 356 pixels), and
participants were asked to make the opposite facial expression
of the pictures they viewed (see Figure 3 for details). In this
phase, the participants would experience 36 trials (12 positive, 12
neutral, and 12 negative). The facial expression of the participants
was video-monitored, in order to confirm that the correct
facial expressions were displayed. To minimize the distraction
of the participants, the experimental task was implemented in
a dimly lit room.

Surface Acting Condition


In this condition, we used the followed instructions to ensure that
participants performed surface acting:
Welcome to the experiment!
First you will see a “+” sign in the middle of the screen; please
look at it and keep your mind calm. After that, pictures of people
with positive, negative, and neutral emotional expressions will be
displayed on the screen. While focusing on the pictures, please
FIGURE 2 | MNI locations for the channels. adopt the facial expression opposite to the one presented in order to
pretend to others that you are actually feeling that opposite emotion,
and keep that expression until the picture disappears. If a neutral
experiment, as follows: (1) What did you think about the difficulty expression is displayed, please do not make any expression.
of the experiment? (2) How would rate the degree of effort put
into the experiment? (3) How would you rate your degree of Deep Acting Condition
concentration on the experiment? (4) How would you rate your As with the surface acting condition, we used instructions to
performance in executing the experimental instructions? Each induce participants to perform deep acting:
item was rated on a six-point scale. Welcome to the experiment!

Experimental Procedure First you will see a “+” sign in the middle of the screen; please
look at it and keep your mind calm. After that, pictures of people
Participants were instructed to sit on a chair in front of a 17-in
with positive, negative, and neutral emotional expressions will be
(32 × 24 cm) monitor. The distance between each participant displayed on the screen. While focusing on the pictures, please
and the monitor was set to around 70 cm. The fNIRS equipment adopt the facial expression opposite to the one presented, and try
was attached to participants’ heads. The experiment was divided to actually experience the emotion you are expressing on your face.
into the exercise and formal experimental phases. In the exercise Please maintain that emotional state until the picture disappears. If
phase, participants practiced how to respond according to the a neutral expression is displayed, please do not make any expression.
instructions until they completely understood it. There were 24
facial expression pictures (eight positive, eight neutral, and eight Data Preprocessing
negative), so in this phase, the participant would experience 24 As the distance between the emitting and detecting optodes
trials under the set condition. Thereafter, the formal experiment was over 3 cm, channel 10 was interpolated by channels
began. Before the formal experiment, participants were instructed 1, 5, 6, 14, 15, and 19; and channel 13 was interpolated
to avoid any head and body movements as much as possible by channels 4, 8, 9, 17, 18, and 22. For this analysis, we
chose to focus on changes in the concentration of OxyHb,
as it is regarded as the most sensitive measure of changes
TABLE 1 | Registered positions of fNIRS measurement channels on the standard in regional cerebral blood flow (Hoshi et al., 2001). Using
brain MRI atlas. the NIRS_SPM_V4_r1 toolbox (Ye et al., 2009), SPM8, and
Brodmann area Channel MATLAB 2013b (Mathworks, Natick, MA, United States), we
processed participants’ individual data: (1) High-pass filtering:
BA 6 29, 30, 31 we selected a discrete cosine transform (DCT)-based on
BA 8 24, 25, 26, 28 detrending algorithm. Based on DCT, the high-pass filtering was
BA 9 15, 16, 17, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 27 implemented by a specified cut-off of 40 s (frequency = 0.025 Hz)
BA 10 2, 3, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 11, 12 to remove unknown global trends due to breathing, cardiac
BA 11 1, 4 events, vasomotion, or other experimental errors. These trends
BA 45 18
are the very low-frequency oscillations originating from slower
BA 46 10, 13, 14
changes in systemic cardiovascular properties, e.g., blood
The channel numbers are defined in Figures 1, 2. pressure (Gervain et al., 2011); (2) Low-pass filtering: according

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 4 May 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 151


Lu et al. Surface and Deep Acting Consumption

FIGURE 3 | One trial experimental procedure (written informed consent was obtained for the publication of this image).

to the NIRS-SPM developer’s recommendation (Ye et al., deep acting, channels 2, 9, 10, and 14 (BA10 and BA 46) were
2009), we selected the precoloring method (Worsley and activated. When participants viewed a negative facial expression
Friston, 1995) and the hemodynamic response function (hrf ) (and therefore, made a positive expression in response), those
to smooth the data. In this method, the intrinsic temporal in the surface acting group showed activations in channels 14,
correlations are swamped by an imposed temporal correlation 29, and 30 (BA 6 and BA 46); by contrast, those in the deep
structure, as smoothing the data with the temporal filtering will acting group showed no activated channels. When comparing
attenuate high frequency components; hence, this is a “low- surface acting and deep acting, the number of activated
pass filtering.” Since the transfer function of hrf is in the channels was slightly higher in the deep acting group than the
frequencies of modeled neuronal signals, cut-off frequency did surface acting group.
not need to be set in this step; (3) Estimate: In the individual
analysis, general linear model (GLM) parameters and temporal Repeated Measures ANOVA
correlations were estimated, and beta values (average signal We then subjected the OxyHb data to a 2 × 3 repeated measures
value of each channel) obtained under four conditions were analysis of variance (rmANOVA). The independent variables
calculated: condition 1 (a positive facial expression is presented); were group and facial expressions. Group was a between-subject
condition 2 (a negative facial expression is presented); condition variable, and included two levels: surface acting and deep acting.
3 (a neutral facial expression is presented); and condition 4 Facial expression was a within-subject variable, and had three
(no facial expression, just a “+” sign is presented). Using levels: positive, negative, and neutral valence pictures. The results
these beta values, we conducted a group level analysis using indicated a non-significant interaction of the group and facial
SPSS Statistics 20. expressions for all channels (all P > 0.05), and a non-significant
main effect of the group expression (all P > 0.05). However,
the main effect of the facial expression was significant for some
RESULTS channels. We further merged the surface acting (n = 20) and
deep acting group (n = 18) into one group (n = 38), and
FNIRS Results used facial expression as an independent variable to run a one-
Activation of Channels way repeated measures ANOVA. The results showed that facial
We compared the beta value of each channel with a 0 value. If the expression was significant for some channels. For details, please
p-value was significant, and the t-value of the one simple t-test see Table 3. Specifically, according to BH_p results, a significant
was positive, then the channel was defined to be activated; if the effect of facial expression was found for channel 1 (located in
t-value was negative, the channel was defined to be deactivated; if left orbitofrontal area, BA11); channels 5 (located in the left
the p-value was not significant, then the channel was defined to frontopolar area, BA10); and channels 10, 14 [located in the
not be activated. left dorsolateral PFC (L-DLFPC), BA46, please see Figure 2]. In
To determine which channels were activated while addition, in channels 24, 25, and 28 (located in the frontal eye
participants engaged in surface or deep acting, we subjected the fields, BA8) and channels 29, 30, and 31 (located in the pre-motor
beta values estimated from the NIRS-SPM tool to a one sample and supplementary motor cortexes, BA6), facial expression had a
t-test and determined the level of activation when displaying significant effect.
positive, negative, and neutral emotional face pictures. The At channels 5, 10, and 14, the OxyHb concentration change
results are shown in Table 2. As shown in Table 2, according for negative and positive facial expressions were significantly
to the BH_p results, when participants viewed a positive facial higher than those for neutral facial expressions. Furthermore, at
expression (and therefore, made a negative expression), we channels 28, 29, 30, and 31, the OxyHb concentration change was
found that no channel was activated for surface acting. For higher for positive facial expressions than for negative or neutral

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 5 May 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 151


Lu et al. Surface and Deep Acting Consumption

TABLE 2 | Comparison of activated channels among different conditions.

Surface acting (n = 20) Neutral (n = 38) Deep acting (n = 18)

Positive–negative Negative–positive Positive–negative Negative–positive

Channel t P BH_p t p BH_p t p t p BH_p t p BH_p

1 1.676 0.110 0.341 −0.398 0.695 0.798 −1.313 0.197 2.626 0.018∗ 0.110 1.521 0.147 0.301
2 −0.325 0.749 0.987 0.090 0.930 0.935 −0.719 0.477 3.348 0.004∗∗ 0.041∗ −0.398 0.696 0.768
5 1.431 0.169 0.475 0.786 0.441 0.624 −0.841 0.406 1.639 0.120 0.265 2.336 0.032∗ 0.138
9 −0.22 0.982 0.987 1.275 0.218 0.562 −0.225 0.823 3.330 0.004∗∗ 0.041∗ 0.951 0.355 0.440
10 1.910 0.071 0.276 0.882 0.389 0.624 −0.578 0.567 3.139 0.006∗∗ 0.046∗ 2.416 0.027∗ 0.138
14 2.327 0.031 0.265 3.245 0.004∗∗ 0.044∗ 1.048 0.301 4.440 0.000∗∗∗ 0.011∗ 3.571 0.002∗∗ 0.073
15 1.083 0.292 0.620 0.992 0.334 0.624 0.897 0.376 2.255 0.038∗ 0.167 2.916 0.010∗ 0.133
18 1.766 0.093 0.322 −0.427 0.674 0.798 1.043 0.304 2.436 0.026∗ 0.135 0.367 0.718 0.768
23 −0.856 0.403 0.734 2.218 0.047∗ 0.272 1.158 0.254 1.386 0.184 0.380 2.777 0.013∗ 0.133
24 −2.283 0.034∗ 0.265 1.334 0.198 0.562 −0.181 0.857 −1.769 0.095 0.263 1.237 0.233 0.344
25 −2.011 0.059 0.266 2.247 0.037∗ 0.272 −0.571 0.571 −0.319 0.754 0.899 1.355 0.193 0.340
26 −2.615 0.017∗ 0.264 0.973 0.343 0.624 −0.162 0.872 0.060 0.953 0.992 0.977 0.342 0.440
28 −2.811 0.011∗ 0.264 1.918 0.070 0.272 0.951 0.348 −1.318 0.205 0.397 2.282 0.036∗ 0.138
29 −1.999 0.060 0.266 3.519 0.002∗∗ 0.035∗ −0.659 0.514 −0.627 0.539 0.795 2.509 0.023∗ 0.138
30 −2.135 0.046∗ 0.266 4.306 0.000∗∗∗ 0.012∗ −0.683 0.499 0.010 0.992 0.992 2.061 0.055 0.155
31 −1.370 0.187 0.482 1.963 0.064 0.272 0.579 0.566 −0.541 0.596 0.839 2.626 0.018∗ 0.137

BH_p is the multiple comparison p-value correction using the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure; < 0.001;
∗∗∗ p < 0.01;
∗∗ p ∗p < 0.05. The asterisk marks annotation for
the following tables are the same as this table. Only the channels with significant p-value are displayed in this table.

TABLE 3 | Main effects of facial expression (n = 38).

Channel F p BH_p η2 PHMC Channel F p BH_p η2 PHMC

1 6.473 0.004∗∗ 0.018∗ 0.27 a>c 17 0.071 0.931 0.931 0.004 None
2 0.861 0.431 0.534 0.047 None 18 2.693 0.082 0.146 0.133 None
3 1.087 0.348 0.490 0.058 None 19 3.020 0.062 0.124 0.147 None
4 3.314 0.048∗ 0.106 0.159 None 20 3.420 0.044∗ 0.105 0.163 None
5 7.621 0.002∗∗ 0.012∗ 0.303 a > c, b > c 21 2.115 0.136 0.211 0.108 None
6 2.976 0.064 0.124 0.145 None 22 0.244 0.785 0.811 0.014 None
7 0.997 0.379 0.490 0.054 None 23 2.649 0.085 0.146 0.131 None
8 1.023 0.370 0.490 0.055 None 24 5.908 0.006∗∗ 0.021∗ 0.252 a < b, a < c
9 0.747 0.481 0.552 0.041 None 25 6.172 0.005∗∗ 0.019∗ 0.261 a < b, a < c
10 7.822 0.002∗∗ 0.012∗ 0.309 a > c, b > c 26 4.433 0.019∗ 0.054 0.202 None
11 3.922 0.029∗ 0.075 0.183 None 27 2.514 0.095 0.156 0.126 None
12 0.686 0.510 0.565 0.038 None 28 10.775 0.000∗∗∗ 0.002∗∗ 0.381 a < b, a < c, b > c
13 0.357 0.702 0.750 0.020 None 29 13.710 0.000∗∗∗ 0.000∗∗∗ 0.439 a < b, a < c, b > c
14 6.903 0.003∗∗ 0.015∗ 0.283 a > c, b > c 30 14.414 0.000∗∗∗ 0.000∗∗∗ 0.452 a < b, b > c
15 1.712 0.195 0.288 0.089 None 31 5.599 0.008∗∗ 0.025∗ 0.242 a < b, b > c
16 0.775 0.468 0.552 0.042 None

a, presented with a positive facial expression picture (requiring participants to make a negative expression); b, presented with a negative facial expression picture
(requiring participants to make a positive expression); c, presented with a neutral facial expression picture (requiring participant to not make any expression). PHMC,
post hoc multiple comparisons.

facial expressions. In channels 24, 25, 28, and 29, the OxyHb Topoeasy toolbox (Tian et al., 2013) of MATLAB 2013b to plot a
concentration change was lower for negative facial expressions topographical picture of the significant t-values (see Figure 4).
than for positive and neutral facial expressions. As shown in the topographical maps, compared to presenting
To further visualize the OxyHb concentration change a neutral face picture (thereby requiring participants to make
differences between positive, negative, and neutral expressions, no emotional expression), presenting positive and negative face
we used the results of the main effects analysis to calculate pictures significantly increased OxyHb concentration in the left
t-values of the comparison between condition 1 and condition front and left middle areas of the PFC. Additionally, when
3 and between condition 2 and condition 3. Then, we used the presenting a positive face picture (thereby requiring participants

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 6 May 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 151


Lu et al. Surface and Deep Acting Consumption

FIGURE 4 | Topographical map of significant t-values (P < 0.05). Con 1, presenting a positive emotional face picture and asking participants to make a negative
expression; Con 2, presenting a negative emotional face picture and asking participants to make a positive expression; and Con 3, presenting a neutral face picture
and asking participants to not make any expressions.

to make a negative expression), the OxyHb concentration PFC based on this study. However, this does not mean that
significantly decreased in the left pre-motor and supplementary we can absolutely make a conclusion: there is no difference of
motor cortex near the rear area of the PFC. Finally, when energy consumption between surface and deep acting. This is
presenting a negative face picture (thereby requiring participants because in addition to the PFC, other brain regions may also
to make a positive expression), the OxyHb density significantly be associated with emotional labor processes, such as subcortical
increased in the pre-motor and supplementary motor cortex near tissue amygdala, hippocampus, and so on. In addition, in this
the rear area of PFC. study, we only used the instruction to ask participants to perform
surface acting and deep acting, and used a video monitor to
Questionnaire Results record their facial expressions. However, for the deep acting, we
Using the Student’s t-test, we examined participants’ subjective did not monitor whether the participants were experiencing the
feelings after completing the experiment, which could be a emotion corresponding to their facial expression (in fact it is very
supplement to the results of fNIRS. The results are shown difficult to do). This made it difficult to confirm whether the
in Table 4. participants really performed deep acting.
For the means of all four questionnaire items, we found In addition to action theory, we might consider the results
no significant difference between the surface acting and in light of the conservation of resources theory. According to
deep acting groups. this theory, surface acting, because it involves the suppression
of emotions, consumes more resources than deep acting does
(Richards and Gross, 2000), making it a strategy with greater
DISCUSSION cognitive investment (Brotheridge and Lee, 2002). However, our
results do not appear to support the conservation of resources
Energy Consumption of Surface and theory. We suggest three possible reasons for this. First, there
might in fact be no significant difference in energy consumption
Deep Acting
The main hypothesis of our study, which was based on action
theory, is that surface acting and deep acting consume different
amounts of psychological resources—particularly, deep acting TABLE 4 | Status of participants after completing the experiment.
leads to greater energy consumption. However, the study results
appear somewhat contradictory. When we consider only the Comparison between groups n = 20 n = 18
number of activated areas in the brain, deep acting activated
Surface acting Deep acting
more channels than surface acting did, thus supporting our study
hypothesis. However, when looking at the ANOVA results, we M ± SD M ± SD t
observed neither significant differences in activation between
surface and deep acting nor a significant interaction of the group Q1 1.750 ± 0.910 1.944 ± 0.938 −0.648

and facial expression. In other words, these results do not support Q2 1.800 ± 1.005 1.889 ± 1.023 −0.270
Q3 4.100 ± 0.788 4.167 ± 1.043 −0.224
our hypothesis. Thus, when using a more strict criterion (the
Q4 5.150 ± 0.745 5.056 ± 0.725 0.395
ANOVA results), it is possible that there is no difference in
energy consumption between surface and deep acting in the Q1, Q2, Q3, and Q4 represent the four items on the questionnaire.

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 7 May 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 151


Lu et al. Surface and Deep Acting Consumption

between deep and surface acting (at least in the short term). differences in the pattern of activation (Barrett and Bliss-Moreau,
This is backed by the questionnaire results: the mean score for 2009; Trambaiolli et al., 2018). Lane et al. (1997a,b) presented
the question “How would you rate the degree of effort put into participants with happy, sad, and disgusting film clips, and asked
the experiment?” was slightly higher in the deep acting group them to feel the relevant target emotion. They found increased
than in the surface acting group, but not to a significant degree activity in the left medial PFC for both positive and negative
(see Table 1). In other words, based on participants’ subjective emotions, measured using PET. Honda et al. (2018) measured
feelings, there was no difference in deep acting and surface acting the change in OxyHb concentration in the PFC while participants
in terms of effort. Second, the fNIRS can only measure the outer commented on a disgusting film they had seen, and found
cortex, surface of the brain. However, surface and deep acting significant differences in OxyHb concentration in the left PFC
may also be involved with the deeper brain structure. For this, between when they engaged in emotional suppression and when
a fNIRS study cannot discover whole brain activities related to they did not. Our study results are in line with the studies of Lane
emotional labor, and it remains difficult to verify the action theory et al. (1997a,b) and Honda et al. (2018), and support the affective
and the conservation of resource theory. Third, emotional labor is workspace hypothesis.
such a complex activity that it is possible that we have not actually Additionally, compared to making no facial expressions, when
measured its energy consumption in this study. We simulated participants engaged in a positive facial expression, the posterior
only the surface and deep acting strategies of emotional labor; in frontal lobe (mainly the frontal eye fields and supplementary
real emotional labor, the goals of the organization are exceedingly motor cortex) showed activity; however, this was not found
important factors to consider, as they will doubtlessly influence when they made a negative facial expression. This finding
the emotional labor process (including surface and deep acting). might indicate that positive facial expressions are related to
With our design, we would have found it difficult to present stronger motion control. Lane et al. (1997a) also found that
participants with a definite organizational goal. happiness was distinguished from sadness by greater activity in
the vicinity of the ventral medial frontal cortex. However, we
cannot make any conclusions based such few studies, so further
Differences Between Making Negative research is necessary.
and Positive Facial Expressions
We found that facial expression had a significant main effect for Limitations
some channels (see Table 3). To clarify the role of emotional First, in this study, we only presented participants with emotional
valence, we compared the changes in OxyHb concentration face pictures, and asked them to display the opposite facial
between the presentation of a positive facial expression and expression to the pictures they saw, using instructions to
the presentation of a neutral facial expression, and between manipulate whether they would engage in surface or deep acting.
the presentation of a negative facial expression and a neutral Accordingly, our findings may not reflect real-life emotional
facial expression. Compared with the neutral facial expression, labor. Further studies should consider motivational factors in
presenting either the positive or negative facial expression (and order to improve the external validity of the study. Then, in this
making the corresponding opposite facial expression in response) study, we primarily focused on PFC activity. Although the PFC
led to a notable activation in the left front and left middle parts of is the most important zone for emotion regulation (Braunstein
the PFC (BA46 and BA10). According to the valence hypothesis, et al., 2017), emotional responses to visual stimuli are regulated
the left PFC is activated during positive emotions, whereas the through neural pathways in multiple brain regions, including the
right PFC is activated during negative emotions (Silberman amygdala and PFC, and emotional labor involves other parts of
and Weingartner, 1986; Jansari et al., 2011). Using functional the brain (e.g., the motor zone of the parietal lobe). In order
magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), Goldin et al. (2008) found to explore whether there is a difference in energy consumption
that the right ventrolateral PFC (VLPFC) was significantly between surface acting and deep acting, more areas of the
activated during emotional suppression while viewing disgusting brain should be monitored in the future. Finally, we did not
images. Kreplin and Fairclough (2013), in applying fNIRS during monitor whether the participants were experiencing the emotion
the viewing of visual art, selected to induce positive and negative corresponding to their facial expression. This made it difficult
emotions and found a significantly higher increase in OxyHb in to confirm whether the participants really performed a deep
the medial rostral PFC (rPFC) when viewing positive images, acting. So, in future studies, it is worth exploring how to use
as compared to viewing negative images. Furthermore, Rodrigo behavioral and physiological indicators to measure participants’
et al. (2016), also applying fNIRS, found that when viewing fearful surface acting and deep acting.
as compared to neutral faces, participants demonstrated higher
levels of activation within the right medial PFC. On the other
hand, participants demonstrated lower levels of activation within CONCLUSION
the left medial PFC and left lateral PFC when viewing fearful
faces, as compared to neutral faces. Our results differ from both Based on the results of this study, we infer that deep acting and
these previous studies and do not support the valence hypothesis. surface acting may not show a significant difference in energy
However, according to the affective workspace hypothesis, the consumption. Furthermore, engaging in positive and negative
activity patterns of the same core neural network implement emotional labor may rely on some of the same psychological
both positive and negative valences, which are determined by mechanisms, though there could be differences as well.

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 8 May 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 151


Lu et al. Surface and Deep Acting Consumption

ETHICS STATEMENT data. YL, WW, and SZ wrote the main manuscript.
YL and WW prepared the figures. All authors
All research procedures were approved by the Research Ethical reviewed the manuscript.
Committee of Guizhou Normal University Educational School
according to the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants
were given written informed consent after they fully FUNDING
understand the study.
This study was supported by Startup Foundation for Advanced
Talents in Guizhou Normal University for WW; and also
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS funded by Philosophy and Social Sciences Key Research
Base Open Fund Project of Hunan Provincial University
WW designed the experiments. YL, HZ, DL, and (13K093) and by Hunan Provincial Social Science Fund
DP collected the data. YL and GM analyzed the Project (13YBA138).

REFERENCES Grandey, A. A., Diefendorff, J. M., and Rupp, D. E. (2013). “Bringing emotional
labor into focus: A review and integration of three research lenses,” in
Ashforth, B. E., and Humphrey, R. H. (1993). Emotional labor in service roles: the Organization and Management. Emotional Labor in the 21st Century: Diverse
influence of identity. Acad. Manag. Rev. 18, 88–115. doi: 10.2307/258824 Perspectives on Emotion Regulation at Work, eds A. A. Grandey, J. M.
Bai, L., Ma, H., Huang, Y., and Luo, Y. (2005). The development of native chinese Diefendorff, and D. E. Rupp (New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis
affective picture system–A pretest in 46 college students. Chin. Mental Health J. Group), 3–27.
19, 719–722. Grandey, A. A., and Gabriel, A. S. (2015). Emotional labor at a crossroads: where
Barrett, L., and Bliss-Moreau, E. (2009). Affect as a psychological primitive. Adv. do we go from here? Ann. Rev. Organ. Psychol. Organ. Behav. 2, 323–349.
Exp. Soc. Psychol. 41, 167–218. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)00404-8 doi: 10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032414-111400
Braunstein, L. M., Gross, J. J., and Ochsner, K. N. (2017). Explicit and implicit Grandey, A. A., and Melloy, R. C. (2017). The State of the heart: emotional labor as
emotion regulation: a multi-level framework. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, emotion regulation reviewed and revised. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 22, 407–422.
1545–1557. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx096 doi: 10.1037/ocp0000067
Brotheridge, C. M., and Lee, R. T. (2002). Testing a conservation of resources Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review.
model of the dynamics of emotional labor. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 7, 57–67. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 271–299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271
doi: 10.1037//1076-8998.7.1.57 Hare, T. A., Tottenham, N., Davidson, M. C., Glover, G. H., and Casey, B. J. (2005).
Bulgarelli, C., Blasi, A., Arridge, S., Powell, S., Klerk, C. D., Southgate, V., et al. Contributions of amygdala and striatal activity in emotion regulation. Biol.
(2018). Dynamic causal modelling on infant fNIRS data: a validation study Psychiatry 57, 624–632. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.038
on a simultaneously recorded fNIRS-fMRI dataset. Neuroimage 175, 413–424. Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The Managed Heart: Commercialization of Human
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.04.022 Feeling. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Dehghani, H., White, B. R., Zeff, B. W., Tizzard, A., and Culver, J. P. (2009). Honda, M., Tanaka, H., Sakti, S., and Nakamura, S. (2018). “Detecting suppression
Depth sensitivity and image reconstruction analysis of dense imaging arrays of negative emotion by time series change of cerebral blood flow using fNIRS,”
for mapping brain function with diffuse optical tomography. Appl. Opt. 48, in Proceedings of the 2018 IEEE EMBS International Conference on Biomedical
D137–D143. doi: 10.1364/AO.48.00D137 & Health Informatics (BHI), (Las Vegas, NV: IEEE).
Diefendorff, J. M., Croyle, M. H., and Gosserand, R. H. (2005). The dimensionality Hong, K. S., Bhutta, M. R., Liu, X., and Shin, Y. I. (2017). Classification of
and antecedents of emotional labor strategies. J. Vocat. Behav. 66, 339–357. somatosensory cortex activities using fNIRS. Behav. Brain Res. 333, 225–234.
doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2004.02.001 doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.06.034
Doi, H., Nishitani, S., and Shinohara, K. (2013). NIRS as a tool for assaying Hong, K. S., Naseer, N., and Kim, Y. H. (2015). Classification of prefrontal and
emotional function in the prefrontal cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:770. doi: motor cortex signals for three-class fNIRS–BCI. Neurosci. Lett. 587, 87–92.
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00770 doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.12.029
Etkin, A., Büchel, C., and Gross, J. J. (2015). The neural bases of emotion regulation. Hoshi, Y., Kobayashi, N., and Tamura, M. (2001). Interpretation of near-
Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 693–700. doi: 10.1038/nrn4044 infrared spectroscopy signals: a study with a newly developed perfused rat
Ferrari, M., and Quaresima, V. (2012). A brief review on the history of brain model. J. Appl. Physiol. 40, 1657–1662. doi: 10.1152/jappl.2001.90.5.
human functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) development and fields 1657
of application. Neuroimage 63, 921–935. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012. Hülsheger, U. R., and Schewe, A. F. (2011). On the costs and benefits of emotional
03.049 labor: a meta-analysis of three decades of research. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 16,
Gabriel, A., and Diefendorff, J. (2015). Emotional labor dynamics: a momentary 361–389. doi: 10.1037/a0022876
approach. Acad. Manag. J. 58, 1804–1825. doi: 10.5465/amj.2013.1135 Jansari, A., Rodway, P., and Goncalves, S. (2011). Identifying facial emotions:
Gabriel, A. S., Daniels, M. A., Diefendorff, J. M., and Greguras, G. J. (2015). valence specific effects and an exploration of the effects of viewer gender. Brain
Emotional labor actors: a latent profile analysis of emotional labor strategies. Cogn. 76, 415–423. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.009
J. Appl. Psychol. 100, 863–879. doi: 10.1037/a0037408 Kreplin, U., and Fairclough, S. H. (2013). Activation of the rostromedial prefrontal
Gervain, J., Mehler, J., Werker, J. F., Nelson, C. A., Csibra, G., Lloyd-Fox, S., cortex during the experience of positive emotion in the context of esthetic
et al. (2011). Near-infrared spectroscopy: a report from the McDonnell infant experience. An fNIRS study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:879. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.
methodology consortium. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 1, 22–46. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn. 2013.00879
2010.07.004 Lane, R. D., Reiman, E. M., Ahern, G. L., Schwartz, G. E., and Davidson, R. J.
Goldin, P. R., Mcrae, K., Ramel, W., and Gross, J. J. (2008). The neural bases (1997a). Neuroanatomical correlates of happiness, sadness, and disgust. Am.
of emotion regulation: reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Biol. J. Psychiatry 154, 926–933. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.7.926
Psychiatry 63, 577–586. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.031 Lane, R. D., Reiman, E. M., Bradley, M. M., Lang, P. J., Ahern, G. L., Davidson,
Grandey, A. A. (2000). Emotional regulation in the workplace: a new way to R. J., et al. (1997b). Neuroanatomical correlates of pleasant and unpleasant
conceptualize emotional labor. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 5, 95–110. doi: 10.1037/ emotion. Neuropsychologia 35, 1437–1444. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)
/1076-8998.5.1.95 00070-75

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 9 May 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 151


Lu et al. Surface and Deep Acting Consumption

Masataka, N., Perlovsky, L., and Hiraki, K. (2015). Near-infrared spectroscopy Sun, Y., Ayaz, H., and Akansu, A. N. (2015). “Neural correlates of affective context
(NIRS) in functional research of prefrontal cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:274. in facial expression analysis: A simultaneous EEG-fNIRS study,” in Proceedings
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00274 of the 2015 IEEE Global Conference on Signal and Information Processing
Mazziotta, J., Toga, A., Evans, A., Fox, P., Lancaster, J., Zilles, K., et al. (2002). (GlobalSIP), (Orlando, FL: IEEE).
A probabilistic atlas and reference system for the human brain: international Tian, F., Lin, Z. J., and Liu, H. (2013). EasyTopo: A toolbox for rapid diffuse optical
consortium for brain mapping (ICBM). Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. topography based on a standard template of brain atlas. Proc. SPIE 8578:85782J.
356, 1293–1322. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2001.0915 doi: 10.1117/12.2003907
Mesmer-Magnus, J. R., DeChurch, L. A., and Wax, A. (2012). Moving emotional Trambaiolli, L. R., Biazoli, C. E., Cravo, A. M., and Sato, J. R. (2018). Predicting
labor beyond surface and deep acting a discordance–congruence perspective. affective valence using cortical hemodynamic signals. Sci. Rep. 8:5406. doi:
Organ. Psychol. Rev. 2, 6–53. doi: 10.1177/2041386611417746 10.1038/s41598-018-23747-y
Naseer, N., and Hong, K. S. (2015). Decoding answers to four-choice questions Vandekerckhove, M., Van Hecke, W., Quirin, M., and De Mey, J. (2018). Neural
using functional near infrared spectroscopy. J. Near Infrar. Spectrosc. 23, 23–31. pathways in ‘emotional approach’as experiential emotion regulation strategy.
doi: 10.1255/jnirs.1145 Behav. Brain Res. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.01.001 [Epub ahead of print].
Pugh, S. D. (2001). Service with a smile: emotional contagion in the service Worsley, K. J., and Friston, K. J. (1995). Analysis of fMRI time-series revisited–
encounter. Acad. Manag. J. 44, 1018–1027. doi: 10.2307/3069445 again. Neuroimage 2, 173–181. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1995.1023
Richards, J. M., and Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: the Xanthopoulou, D., Bakker, A. B., Oerlemans, W. G., and Koszucka, M. (2018).
cognitive costs of keeping one’s cool. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 410–424. Need for recovery after emotional labor: differential effects of daily deep and
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.3.410 surface acting. J. Organ. Behav. 39, 481–494. doi: 10.1002/job.2245
Rodrigo, A. H., Ayaz, H., and Ruocco, A. C. (2016). “Examining the Neural Ye, J. C., Tak, S., Jang, K. E., Jung, J., and Jang, J. (2009). NIRS-SPM: statistical
Correlates of Incidental Facial Emotion Encoding within the Prefrontal parametric mapping for near-infrared spectroscopy. Neuroimage 44, 428–447.
Cortex Using Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy,” in Foundations of doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.036
Augmented Cognition: Neuroergonomics and Operational Neuroscience, eds Zapf, D. (2002). Emotion work and psychological well-being: a review of the
D. D. Schmorrow and C. M. Fidopiastis (Cham: Springer). literature and some conceptual considerations. Hum. Res. Manag. Rev. 12,
Scholkmann, F., Gerber, U., Wolf, M., and Wolf, U. (2012). End-tidal CO2: an 237–268. doi: 10.1016/s1053-4822(02)00048-7
important parameter for a correct interpretation in functional brain studies Zilverstand, A., Parvaz, M. A., and Goldstein, R. Z. (2017). Neuroimaging cognitive
using speech tasks. Neuroimage 66, 71–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012. reappraisal in clinical populations to define neural targets for enhancing
10.025 emotion regulation. a systematic review. Neuroimage 151, 105–116. doi: 10.
Schutz, P. A., and Lee, M. (2014). “Teacher emotion, emotional labor and 1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.009
teacher identity,” in English as a Foreign Language Teacher Education: Current
Perspectives and Challenges, Vol. 27, eds J. de Dios and M. Agudo (Amsterdam: Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was
Rodopi), 169–186. conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
Shulei, M., and Miner, H. (2006). Emotional labor; surface acting and deep acting, be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
which one is better? Acta Psychol. Sin. 38, 262–270.
Silberman, E. K., and Weingartner, H. (1986). Hemispheric lateralization of Copyright © 2019 Lu, Wu, Mei, Zhao, Zhou, Li and Pan. This is an open-access
functions related to emotion. Brain Cogn. 5, 322–353. doi: 10.1016/0278- article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License
2626(86)90035-7 (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided
Singh, A. K., Okamoto, M., Dan, H., Jurcak, V., and Dan, I. (2005). Spatial the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original
registration of multichannel multi-subject fNIRS data to MNI space without publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No
MRI. Neuroimage 27, 842–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.019 use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 10 May 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 151

You might also like