PTSD Articol Științific

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/47634341

Emotion Regulation Difficulties in Trauma Survivors: The Role of Trauma Type


and PTSD Symptom Severity

Article  in  Behavior Therapy · December 2010


DOI: 10.1016/j.beth.2010.04.004 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

516 7,392

2 authors, including:

Thomas Ehring
Ludwig-Maximilians-University of Munich
177 PUBLICATIONS   7,770 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

ECoWeB: Assessing and Enhancing Emotional Competence for Well-Being in the Young: A principled, evidence-based, mobile-health approach to prevent mental
disorders and promote mental well-being View project

Brief Imagery Rescripting vs. Treatment as Usual in Refugees with Posttraumatic Stress Disorder – A Multi-Center Randomized Controlled Trial (ReScript) View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Thomas Ehring on 22 February 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached
copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research
and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution
and sharing with colleagues.
Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or
licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party
websites are prohibited.
In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the
article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or
institutional repository. Authors requiring further information
regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are
encouraged to visit:
http://www.elsevier.com/copyright
Author's personal copy

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

Behavior Therapy 41 (2010) 587 – 598


www.elsevier.com/locate/bt

Emotion Regulation Difficulties in Trauma Survivors:


The Role of Trauma Type and PTSD Symptom Severity
Thomas Ehring
University of Amsterdam

Dorothea Quack
University of Bielefeld

ALTHOUGH POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER (PTSD) is


Two different hypotheses regarding the relationship be- mostly regarded as a reliable, valid, and clinically
tween emotion regulation and PTSD are described in the useful diagnostic category (Nemeroff et al., 2006),
literature. First, it has been suggested that emotion some aspects of the diagnosis nevertheless remain
regulation difficulties are part of the complex sequelae of controversial (e.g., McHugh & Treisman, 2007;
early-onset chronic interpersonal trauma and less common McNally, 2004). Most importantly, a number of
following late-onset or single-event traumas. Second, PTSD authors have argued that it is too much focused on
in general has been suggested to be related to emotion the sequelae of single-event traumas, but does not
regulation difficulties. Bringing these two lines of research adequately capture the more complex problems
together, the current study aimed to investigate the role of experienced by adult survivors of childhood inter-
trauma type and PTSD symptom severity on emotion personal trauma, especially if the traumatic events
regulation difficulties in a large sample of trauma survivors have occurred repeatedly or chronically (Briere &
(N = 616). In line with the hypotheses, PTSD symptom Spinazzola, 2005; Cloitre, Miranda, Stovall-
severity was significantly associated with all variables McClough, & Han, 2005; Herman, 1992a, 1992b;
assessing emotion regulation difficulties. In addition, van der Kolk, 2005; van der Kolk, Roth, Pelcovitz,
survivors of early-onset chronic interpersonal trauma Sunday, & Spinazzola, 2005). A number of addi-
showed higher scores on these measures than survivors of tional trauma-related diagnoses for this specific
single-event and/or late-onset traumas. However, when subgroup have therefore been suggested, including
controlling for PTSD symptom severity, the group differ- complex PTSD (Herman, 1992b), disorder of
ences only remained significant for 2 out of 9 variables. The extreme stress not otherwise specified (DESNOS;
most robust findings were found for the variable “lack of van der Kolk et al., 2005), and, more recently,
clarity of emotions.” Implications for future research, developmental trauma disorder (van der Kolk,
theoretical models of trauma-related disorders, and their 2005). Other authors have described groups of
treatment will be discussed. symptoms that are thought to be typical sequelae of
early-onset interpersonal trauma without suggesting
a separate diagnostic category (Briere, Kaltman, &
Green, 2008; Briere & Spinazzola, 2005; Cloitre et
We are grateful to the numerous self-help organizations, mailing al., 2005). Although the different conceptualizations
lists owners, and website hosts for their invaluable help with of complex trauma sequelae show considerable
recruitment for this study. In addition, we would like to thank
Christian Glass for his help with recruitment and data management. differences, they also agree on a number of key
Address correspondence to Thomas Ehring, Ph.D., Department of symptoms, including emotion regulation difficulties.
Clinical Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Roetersstraat 15, The suggestion that exposure to chronic inter-
1018 WB Amsterdam, The Netherlands; e-mail: t.w.a.ehring@uva.nl.
personal trauma early in life should lead to emotion
0005-7894/10/587–598/$1.00/0
© 2010 Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies. Published by regulation difficulties is based on findings from
Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. developmental psychology. Research has shown
Author's personal copy

588 ehring & quack

that adaptive emotion regulation is learned in by two factors each (Dimension 1: Lack of
interaction with primary caregivers (Calkins & Emotional Awareness and Lack of Emotional
Hill, 2007; Cole, Michel, & Teti, 1994). On the one Clarity; Dimension 3: Difficulties Engaging in
hand, caregivers' own emotion regulation behavior Goal-Directed Behavior and Impulse Control
serves as a model for the developing child as to how Difficulties).
to deal with emotional states. In addition, care- Evidence for the idea that survivors of early-
givers guide the child in understanding and labeling onset interpersonal trauma suffer from alterations
his/her own emotions and ultimately regulating in emotion regulation comes from three groups
them in a way to achieve his/her goals. In addition, of studies. First, survivors of early-onset inter-
there is evidence that compromised attachment is personal trauma were found to report higher
associated with emotion regulation deficits (Cloitre, levels of alexithymia than nontraumatized con-
Stovall-McClough, Zorbas, & Charuvastra, 2008), trols (Cloitre, Scarvalone, & Difede, 1997;
which supports the idea that the interpersonal McLean, Toner, Jackson, Desrocher, & Stuckless,
context is important for the development of 2006; Zlotnick et al., 1996). Alexithymia is
emotion regulation. On a theoretical level, it is defined as difficulty with identifying and labeling
therefore conceivable that the experience of chronic one's own emotional state and corresponds to the
interpersonal trauma in early developmental stages first dimension of Gratz and Roemer's (2004)
should disrupt the development of adaptive emo- conceptualization. Second, a number of studies
tion regulation, especially when the perpetrator is have investigated acceptance of negative emotions.
one of the key caregivers (Cloitre et al., 2005, 2008; When compared to nontraumatized controls, sur-
Ford, 2009; van der Kolk et al., 1996); however, to vivors of early-onset interpersonal trauma were
our knowledge no prospective studies testing this found to report more difficulties tolerating and
hypothesis have been conducted to date. On the regulating negative emotions (Briere & Rickards,
basis of the theoretical assumptions regarding the 2007), higher levels of fear of emotions (Tull,
effects of trauma on the development of emotion Jakupcak, McFadden, & Roemer, 2007), and
regulation, a number of authors suggest that higher experiential avoidance, defined as an un-
emotion regulation difficulties are one of the willingness to experience negative thoughts and
complex symptoms that specifically develop after feelings and high efforts to escape from them
early-onset chronic interpersonal trauma (e.g., (Batten, Follette, & Aban, 2001; Marx & Sloan,
Cloitre et al., 2005; van der Kolk et al., 2005). 2002). Furthermore, studies conducted as part of
The suggestion that survivors of early-onset inter- the DSM-IV field trial showed that a majority of
personal trauma typically show pronounced emo- survivors of early-onset interpersonal trauma
tion regulation difficulties also forms the rationale reported difficulties appropriately regulating their
for treatment approaches specifically developed for emotions (e.g., fear, anger) or impulses (e.g., self-
this group that include strategies to build up destructive behavior, sexual involvement; Pelcovitz
emotion regulation skills (Cloitre, Koenen, Cohen, et al., 1997; van der Kolk et al., 1996). Finally, a
& Han, 2002; Wolfsdorf & Zlotnick, 2001). recent study found self-reported emotion regulation
A review of evidence supporting this view is problems to be strongly associated with functional
complicated by the fact that emotion regulation is a impairment beyond PTSD symptom severity in
broad concept that has been defined in different treatment-seeking women who had experienced
ways (see Gross & Thompson, 2007; Kring & early-onset interpersonal trauma (Cloitre et al.,
Werner, 2004). The current study is based on Gratz 2005).
and Roemer's (2004) integrative conceptualization, Taken together, earlier findings support the view
which suggests four key dimensions of emotion that psychological problems following early-onset
regulation: (a) awareness and understanding of interpersonal trauma include emotion regulation
one's emotions, (b) acceptance of negative emo- difficulties. However, to our knowledge, no pro-
tions, (c) the ability to successfully engage in goal- spective studies have been conducted to date
directed behavior and control impulsive behavior investigating the suggested temporal precedence of
when experiencing negative emotions, and (d) the traumatic events leading to emotion regulation
ability to use situationally appropriate emotion difficulties. In addition, it is less clear whether
regulation strategies. On the basis of this concep- these difficulties are indeed specific for survivors of
tualization, Gratz and Roemer developed the this type of events or whether they are also present
Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS). in individuals with PTSD who have experienced
Results of factor analyses support a six-factor traumas later in life and/or only once. In line with
solution for the DERS, whereby the first and third the latter view, it is interesting to note that the
dimension suggested by the authors are represented DSM-IV diagnosis of PTSD includes symptoms that
Author's personal copy

emotion regulation in trauma survivors 589

may directly reflect difficulties in emotion regula- when compared to survivors of late-onset interper-
tion, with emotional hyperreactivity to trauma- sonal trauma and/or noninterpersonal trauma (but
related cues on the one hand and hyporeactivity see also Ford, Stockton, Kaltman, & Green, 2006,
in the form of emotional numbing on the other who failed to find differences in emotion regulation
hand (see Frewen & Lanius, 2006; Litz, Orsillo, between different types of trauma survivors). Thus,
Kaloupek, & Weathers, 2000). A number of studies results of most studies directly comparing different
have shown that PTSD in general, even following types of trauma survivors support the view that
adult-onset traumas, is related to alterations in emotion regulation difficulties are more pro-
emotion regulation. Specifically, PTSD has been nounced in survivors of early-onset interpersonal
found to be related to (a) alexithymia (Frewen, than late-onset or single-event traumas. However,
Dozois, Neufeld, & Lanius, 2008); (b) a negative as none of the existing studies has controlled for
attitude towards emotional expression and a levels of PTSD, it is unclear whether the emotion
tendency to suppress and/or withhold negative regulation difficulties observed are indeed due to
emotions (Lowery & Stokes, 2005; Moore, Zoellner, the type of event experienced or simply due to
& Mollenholt, 2008; Nightingale & Williams, 2000; higher levels of PTSD in the early-onset group.
Roemer, Litz, Orsillo, & Wagner, 2001), as well as In addition, past studies differ considerably
high levels of experiential avoidance (Kashdan, regarding their definition of the different trauma
Morina, & Priebe, 2009; Marx & Sloan, 2005; types that were compared. Whereas some studies
Morina, Stangier, & Risch, 2008; Plumb, Orsillo, & compared survivors of early-onset versus late-onset
Luterek, 2004); and (c) self-reported problems interpersonal trauma regardless of the number of
regulating one's emotions (Tull, Barrett, McMillan, events experienced (e.g., Cloitre et al., 1997;
& Roemer, 2007). Taken together, these findings Pelcovitz et al., 1997; van der Kolk et al., 2005),
show an association of emotion regulation difficul- others distinguished between single incidents vs.
ties with PTSD following late-onset single-event ongoing/chronic traumatization within the group of
traumas, which may call into question the wide- early-onset interpersonal traumas (e.g., Ford et al.,
spread assumption that they are a specific charac- 2006; Roth, Newman, Pelcovitz, van der Kolk, &
teristic of early-onset chronic interpersonal trauma. Mandel, 1997). Most studies used the age of 14 as
An alternative hypothesis would be that emotion the cutoff to distinguish between early vs. late
regulation difficulties are simply related to the traumas. However, some studies chose the higher
severity of PTSD symptoms irrespective of the type cutoff of 18 years of age (e.g., Cloitre et al., 1997).
of traumatic event experienced. In the current study, self-reported characteristics
We suggest that studies testing these two of emotion regulation were assessed in a large
hypotheses against each other need to fulfill two group of trauma survivors via a web-based survey.
requirements. First, they need to directly compare The assessment battery for emotion regulation was
survivors of early-onset interpersonal traumas with compiled with the aim to assess all four groups of
survivors of late-onset and/or noninterpersonal emotion regulation variables suggested by Gratz
traumas regarding characteristics of emotion regu- and Roemer (2004), with at least two measures per
lation that are thought to be implicated in trauma- group. In addition, emotion suppression and
related disturbances. Secondly, PTSD symptom cognitive reappraisal were assessed as two specific
severity needs to be controlled for in order to emotion regulation strategies that have been found
ensure that possible differences between different in earlier research to be significantly related to
types of trauma survivors are not just due to PTSD symptomatology (Moore et al., 2008) as well
differences in the severity of PTSD symptoms as other types of emotional problems (e.g., Gross &
experienced. To our knowledge, there is no John, 2003). As emotion suppression can be
published study that fulfills both requirements. assumed to be related to low acceptance of negative
However, a small number of studies have directly emotions, it was included in the group of variables
compared different types of trauma survivors assessing emotion acceptance. Reappraisal, on the
regarding characteristics of emotion regulation. other hand, was added to the fourth group of
Results showed that survivors of early-onset variables assessing the functional and flexible use of
interpersonal trauma report higher levels of alex- emotion regulation strategies. In line with most
ithymia (Cloitre et al., 1997), more difficulties earlier studies (e.g., Pelcovitz et al., 1997; van der
tolerating and regulating negative emotions (Briere Kolk et al., 2005), early-onset interpersonal trauma
& Rickards, 2007), and more problems with the was defined as sexual or physical violence experi-
regulation of anger as well as more self-destructive enced before the age of 14. We also distinguished
behavior (Pelcovitz et al., 1997; Scoboria, Ford, whether the trauma was “chronic” (i.e., for the
Lin, & Frisman, 2008; van der Kolk et al., 1996) duration of at least 1 year) or not, as done in earlier
Author's personal copy

590 ehring & quack

studies (e.g., Roth et al., 1997). Based on the research to be equally reliable (e.g., Fortson, Scotti,
developmental literature, we expected chronic Del Ben, & Chen, 2006; Nosek, Banaji, &
interpersonal trauma experienced at an early age Greenwald, 2002). The final sample consisted of
to be specifically associated with emotion regula- 616 participants (see Table 1 for demographic
tion deficits. information). Recruitment took place in two ways.
The hypotheses were as follows: First, information about the study and a link to the
online questionnaire were posted on 15 websites,
1. Emotion regulation difficulties are significantly which were either websites advertising web-based
related to levels of PTSD symptoms. studies in general or websites providing informa-
2. Survivors of early-onset chronic interpersonal tion about trauma-related and/or other emotional
trauma report higher levels of emotion regu- disorders. Second, self-help organizations, mailing
lation difficulties than survivors of late-onset, lists, and online communities for trauma survivors
early-onset single/repeated and noninterper- were contacted and asked to distribute study
sonal traumas. information and the link to the online question-
3. The differences in emotion regulation be- naire among their members. The online question-
tween the different types of trauma survivors naire was active between July 2006 and February
remain significant when controlling for PTSD 2007. A total of 2,423 individuals accessed the
symptoms. welcome page providing information about the
study: 2,022 individuals agreed to take part in the
Method survey and started filling in the questionnaire, 772
participants of which provided complete data. For all events
The study was conducted as a web-based survey. In reported in the Trauma History Questionnaire (see
this way it was hoped to reach a larger and more below), we checked whether these events met
diverse sample of trauma survivors than when DSM-IV trauma criterion A1 (experiencing, wit-
recruiting participants via clinical services. Past nessing, or being confronted with an event that
studies directly comparing web-based versus involved actual or threatened death or serious
paper-and-pencil surveys found both types of injury, or a threat to the physical integrity of self or

Table 1
Demographic Variables and PTSD Symptom Severity by Trauma Type
Total sample Non- Late Early single Early chronic Statistic
(N = 616)1 interpersonal interpersonal interpersonal interpersonal
(n = 135)2 (n = 211)3 (n = 101)4 (n = 169)5
Age: M (SD) 32.03 (11.16) 34.86 (13.52) 31.24 (10.49) 30.67 (12.01) 32.31 (9.97) F(3, 417) = 2.02, p = .11
Sex
Female 82.9% 63.8% 85.2% 77.6% 92.2% χ2 (3, N = 463) = 29.49,
Male 17.1% 36.2% 14.8% 22.4% 7.8% p b .001

Marital Status
Single 65% 69.6% 62.6% 70.6% 62.3% χ2 (6, N = 463) = 6.21,
Married 27.2% 27.5% 30.3% 21.2% 27.3% p = .40
Divorced/ widowed 7.8% 2.9% 7.1% 8.2% 10.4%

Employment Status
Not working 46.2% 47.8% 45.2% 43.5% 48.1% χ2 (6, N = 463) = 5.49,
Employed 44.7% 43.5% 7.1% 15.3% 44.2% p = .48
Self-employed 9.1% 8.7% 47.7% 41.2% 7.8%

IES-R
Mean score 2.55 (0.84) 1.94 (.68)1 2.46 (.83)2 2.59 (.75)2 3.11 (.63)3 F(3, 615) = 64.74, p b .001
Sum score 56.1 (18.48) 42.68 (14.96) 54.12 (18.26) 56.98 (16.5) 68.42 (13.86)
Note. Different superscripts denote differences between groups at p b .05.
1
Age: n = 421; Sex/Marital Status/Employment: n = 463.
2
Age: n = 63; Sex/Marital Status/Employment: n = 66.
3
Age: n = 140; Sex/Marital Status/Employment: n = 155.
4
Age: n = 78; Sex/Marital Status/Employment: n = 85.
5
Age: n = 140; Sex/Marital Status/Employment: n = 154.
Author's personal copy

emotion regulation in trauma survivors 591

others). Participants who did not report any shows good psychometric properties (Creamer,
traumatic event according to this definition were Bell, & Failla, 2003; Weiss, 2004). Based on an
excluded from the analyses (n = 156). The final explicit decision by the test authors (see Weiss,
sample therefore consisted of 616 participants. 2004), no norms or clinical cutoff scores are
Participants did not receive a compensation for available for the IES-R. However, the test authors
their participation. suggest comparing mean scores to the anchor
Based on participants' answers to the Trauma points of the response scale (e.g., 0 = not at all;
History Questionnaire (Green, 1996) including 2 = moderate; 4 = extremely) when interpreting the
their descriptions of the traumatic events, the IES-R results.
sample was divided into four trauma groups: (1)
survivors of noninterpersonal traumas (e.g., acci- Characteristics of Emotion Regulation
dents, fire, observed traumatic events in others; A number of questionnaires were used to assess
n = 135); (2) survivors of late-onset (age ≥ 14) characteristics of emotion regulation.
interpersonal traumas (n = 211; 51.7% physical Self-reported emotion regulation difficulties were
assault, 19.5% sexual assault, 28.8% physical and assessed with the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation
sexual assault); (3) survivors of early-onset Scale (DERS; Gratz & Roemer, 2004). Six subscale
(age b 14) single or repeated interpersonal traumas scores can be computed from the 36 items:
that lasted for less than 1 year (n = 101; 59.8% Nonacceptance of Emotions (6 items; e.g., “When
physical assault, 21.8% sexual assault, 17.8% I'm upset, I feel guilty for feeling that way”; α = .90);
physical and sexual assault); (4) survivors of early- Difficulties Engaging in Goal-Directed Behavior
onset (age b 14) chronic interpersonal traumas that When Distressed (5 items; e.g., “When I'm upset, I
lasted for at least 1 year (n = 169; 30.2% physical have difficulty getting work done”; α = .91); Im-
abuse, 33.1% sexual abuse, 36.7% physical and pulse Control Difficulties (6 items; e.g., “When I'm
sexual abuse). To classify participants who had upset, I become out of control”; α = .91); Lack of
experienced several traumatic events that belonged Emotional Awareness (6 items; e.g., “I pay atten-
to different categories, a hierarchy was used with tion to how I feel” [reversed]; α = .82); Limited
“early-onset chronic interpersonal trauma” at the Access to Emotion Regulation Strategies' (8 items;
top, followed by “early-onset single or repeated “When I'm upset, I believe that there is nothing I
interpersonal trauma,” “late-onset interpersonal can do to make myself feel better.”; α = .93); and
trauma,” and finally “noninterpersonal trauma.” Lack of Emotional Clarity (5 items; “I am confused
For example, a participant who had experienced a about how I feel”; α = .90). Participants rate each
single sexual assault at the age of 10 and a hurricane item on a scale from 1 (almost never, 0 − 10%) to 5
at the age of 35 was assigned to the “early-onset (almost always, 91 − 100%). Earlier research has
single or repeated interpersonal trauma” group. shown good psychometric properties for the scale
(Gratz & Roemer, 2004).
questionnaires In addition, participants filled in the Emotion
Trauma History Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John,
Participants' trauma history was assessed with the 2003). The ERQ assesses the typical use of emotion
Trauma History Questionnaire (THQ; Green, suppression (4 items, e.g., “I keep my emotions to
1996), which inquires about 24 different traumatic myself”) and reappraisal (6 items, e.g., “When I
events. For each event, participants are asked to want to feel less negative emotion, I change the way
indicate whether they have experienced it, and if so, I'm thinking about the situation”). Each item is
the number of times and approximate age(s) of rated on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7
occurrence. In addition, they are asked to write (strongly agree). The scale has been shown to
down a brief description of the event that has possess good psychometric properties (Gross &
happened to them. John, 2003). Internal consistencies in the current
study were good (emotion suppression: α = .81;
PTSD Symptom Severity reappraisal: α = .87). There is correlational as well
The Impact of Event Scale−Revised (IES-R; Weiss as experimental evidence supporting the classifica-
& Marmar, 1997) was used to assess the severity of tion of emotion suppression as a primarily dys-
PTSD symptoms. Participants are asked to rate 22 functional emotion regulation strategy and
items representing trauma-related symptoms (e.g., reappraisal as a primarily functional one (Gross,
“Pictures about it popped into my mind”; “I had 2002; Gross & John, 2003).
trouble falling asleep”) on a scale from 0 (not at all) Experiential avoidance was assessed with the
to 4 (extremely). The IES-R has extensively been Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ;
used in clinical and nonclinical populations and Hayes et al., 2004). The 9-item self-report
Author's personal copy

592 ehring & quack

questionnaire measures the tendency to negatively Associations between emotion regulation difficul-
evaluate unpleasant feelings, thoughts and sensa- ties and PTSD symptom severity (Hypothesis 1) were
tions, and unwillingness to tolerate these private assessed using Pearson product-moment correlations.
events and attempts to suppress or escape them In order to test differences in emotion regulation
(example items: “Anxiety is bad”; “If I could between the different trauma groups (Hypothesis 2),
magically remove all the painful experiences I've separate MANOVAs were carried out for each of the
had in my life, I would do so”). Participants are four groups of emotion regulation variables. Signif-
asked to rate each item on a scale from 1 (never icant main effects of trauma type were then followed
true) to 7 (always true). Acceptable to good up by separate ANOVAs for each variable and
psychometric properties have been found in earlier planned simple contrasts comparing each trauma
research using the measure (Hayes et al., 2004; α in group with the early chronic interpersonal group.
this sample: .71). Similarly, MANCOVAs with the IES-R score as the
covariate were conducted to test Hypothesis 3,
procedure followed by ANCOVAs and simple contrasts.
The study was conducted by means of a web-based, Power analyses for the MANOVAs and follow-
secured and encrypted survey, using the Unipark up ANOVAs based on α = .05 and N = 616 partici-
platform (www.unipark.com). When following the pants revealed high power (N.99) to detect a
link to the study, participants accessed a welcome moderate effect size (f 2 = .25).
page. On this page, respondents were briefed about
the survey (Title: “Dealing with Emotions”). They Results
were told that the study aimed to investigate how
preliminary analysis: group differences on
people deal with their emotions and how this is demographic variables
related to negative life experiences. They were
informed that part of the questionnaire would be Before testing the hypotheses, we first checked
about traumatic experiences they may have had, and whether the groups differed on any demographic
it was emphasized that participation was voluntary, variables. As shown in Table 1, no difference in age,
that they could stop filling in the survey at any point marital status, or employment between the groups
and that the data would remain anonymous. At the emerged. However, the rate of female participants
end of the welcome page, participants were explicitly was higher in the interpersonal trauma groups than
asked to indicate whether they wanted to participate the noninterpersonal trauma group. As would be
in the study or not. Participants could give their expected, the groups also differed regarding the
informed consent by clicking on “yes.” severity of PTSD symptoms assessed with the IES-
The questionnaires were then presented in the R. Survivors of early chronic interpersonal trauma
following order: AAQ, DERS, ERQ, THQ, IES-R, showed the highest IES-R mean scores (between
and a questionnaire asking participants to provide anchor points quite a bit and extremely), followed
demographic data. The closing page of the survey by early single interpersonal and late interpersonal
thanked participants and gave them the opportunity trauma survivors (between anchor points moder-
to leave a comment for the investigators. In addition, ately and quite a bit), and noninterpersonal trauma
the first author's contact details were provided in survivors showed the lowest scores (between
case participants had further questions or comments anchor points a little bit and moderately).
related to the study or felt distressed by it. hypothesis 1: relationship between
emotion regulation difficulties and
statistical analyses ptsd symptom severity
The emotion regulation variables assessed were Correlations between the different emotion regula-
divided into four groups, following Gratz and tion scores and PTSD symptom severity are shown
Roemer's (2004) model: (a) awareness and clarity in Table 2. In line with Hypothesis 1, IES-R scores
(DERS subscales: Lack of Emotional Awareness showed significant correlations with all indices of
and Lack of Clarity); (b) acceptance (DERS emotion regulation difficulties in the expected
subscale: Nonacceptance; AAQ total score; ERQ direction.
subscale: Emotion Suppression); (c) difficulties with
goal-directed behavior and impulse control (DERS hypothesis 2: differences in emotion
subscales: Difficulties Engaging in Goal-Directed regulation according to trauma type
Behavior and Impulse Control Difficulties); and (d) As shown in Table 3, results of MANOVAs showed
strategy use (DERS subscale: Limited Access to significant main effects of trauma type for all four
Strategies; ERQ subscale: Reappraisal). groups of emotion regulation variables. Follow-up
Author's personal copy

emotion regulation in trauma survivors 593

Table 2
Pearson Correlations between PTSD Symptom Severity and Characteristics of Emotion Regulation
Variable 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10.
1. IES-R −

Awareness and Clarity


2. DERS − lack of awareness .34⁎⁎ −
3. DERS − lack of clarity .48⁎⁎ .55⁎⁎ −

Acceptance
4. DERS − non-acceptance .48⁎⁎ .40⁎⁎ .61⁎⁎ −
5. AAQ − experiential avoidance .49⁎⁎ .41⁎⁎ .62⁎⁎ .61⁎⁎ −
6. ERQ − emotion suppression .36⁎⁎ .46⁎⁎ .46⁎⁎ .44⁎⁎ .35⁎⁎ − -

Difficulties with goal-directed behavior and impulse control


6. DERS − goal-directed behavior .49⁎⁎ .34⁎ .59⁎⁎ .57⁎⁎ .70⁎⁎ .33⁎⁎ −
7. DERS − impulse control difficulties .49⁎⁎ .36⁎⁎ .60⁎⁎ .58⁎⁎ .65⁎⁎ .28⁎⁎ .76⁎⁎ −

Strategy Use
8. DERS − limited access to strategies .52⁎⁎ .42⁎⁎ .66⁎⁎ .72⁎⁎ .74⁎⁎ .42⁎⁎ .77⁎⁎ .74⁎⁎ −
10. ERQ − reappraisal -.23⁎⁎ -.38⁎⁎ -.39⁎⁎ -.25⁎⁎ -.45⁎⁎ -.15⁎⁎ -.44⁎⁎ -.42⁎⁎ -.50⁎⁎ −
Note. IES-R = Impact of Event Scale−Revised; DERS = Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; ERQ = Emotion Regulation Questionnaire;
AAQ = Acceptance and Action Questionnaire.
⁎ p b .01.
⁎⁎ p b .001.

Table 3
Characteristics of Emotion Regulation by Trauma Type
Non- Late Early single Early chronic (M)ANOVA (M)ANCOVA
interpersonal interpersonal interpersonal interpersonal
F(6, 1224)a η2 F(10, 1530)a η2
(n = 135) (n = 211) (n = 101) (n = 169)
or F(3, 612) or F(5, 766)
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Awareness and Clarity 16.23⁎⁎ .07 3.93⁎⁎ .02
DERS − lack of awareness 16.33 (4.50)c 17.32 (5.20)c 17.51 (5.49)c 19.53 (5.54) 10.53⁎⁎ .05 1.19 .01
DERS − lack of clarity 11.40 (4.79)b 13.52 (5.18)b 13.99 (5.12)b 17.24 (5.16) 35.08⁎⁎ .15 7.69⁎⁎ .04

Acceptance 8.82⁎⁎ .04 1.21 .01


DERS − non-acceptance 13.78 (5.88)c 16.76 (6.66)c 17.31 (6.65)c 19.83 (6.51) 22.21⁎⁎ .10 N/A
AAQ − experiential avoidance 35.60 (7.74)c 37.96 (8.63)c 39.43 (7.82)c 42.54 (7.72) 20.10⁎⁎ .09 N/A
ERQ − emotion suppression 13.24 (5.69)c 14.33 (6.34)c 14.40 (6.75)c 16.57 (6.20) 7.86⁎⁎ .04 N/A

Difficulties with goal-directed behavior and impulse control 14.03⁎⁎ .06 2.29⁎ .01
DERS − goal-directed behavior 13.72 (5.13)b 15.35 (5.61)b 16.98 (5.03)c 18.75 (4.96) 26.08⁎⁎ .11 4.09⁎ .02
DERS − impulse control difficulties 12.65 (5.56)c 14.98 (6.20)c 16.05 (6.22)c 18.67 (6.85) 24.41⁎⁎ .11 2.51 .01

Strategy Use 13.19⁎⁎ .06 2.09 .01


DERS − limited access 18.81 (8.31)c 23.05 (8.75)c 25.35 (9.29)c 27.43 (7.37) 28.11⁎⁎ .12 N/A
to strategies
ERQ − reappraisal 25.86 (7.37)c 24.24 (8.07)c 23.14 (8.23) 22.04 (8.30) 6.14⁎ .03 N/A
PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; DERS = Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; AAQ = Acceptance and Action
Questionnaire; ERQ = Emotion Regulation Questionnaire.
a
Pillai's Trace; N/A: ANCOVA not computed because of nonsignificant MANCOVA.
b
Significant contrast between this group and early-onset chronic interpersonal trauma group in both ANOVA and ANCOVA controlling for
IES-R scores.
c
Significant contrast between this group and early-onset chronic interpersonal trauma group in ANOVA, but not significant in ANCOVA
controlling for IES-R scores.
⁎ p b .01.
⁎⁎ p b .001.
Author's personal copy

594 ehring & quack

ANOVAs revealed significant main effects of between emotion regulation variables and PTSD
trauma type on all emotion regulation variables. symptom severity remained significant. In addition,
In line with Hypothesis 2, simple contrasts com- the same pattern of group differences were found as
paring all groups to the last one showed that in the total sample, although some simple contrasts
survivors of early-onset chronic interpersonal failed to reach significance. As the trauma groups
trauma reported significantly higher emotion regu- still significantly differed regarding their IES-R
lation difficulties than all other groups for almost scores, the (M)ANCOVAs controlling for PTSD
all variables. Only one contrast was nonsignificant, symptom severity were also repeated for female
showing that survivors of early-onset chronic participants only. When controlling for IES-R
interpersonal trauma did not differ from survivors scores, Clarity of Negative Emotions was the only
of early-onset single interpersonal traumas regard- remaining variable for which significant group
ing their reappraisal scores. differences were found.

dropout analysis
hypothesis 3: group differences when A set of t tests was conducted comparing partici-
controlling for ptsd symptom severity pants with complete data (n = 616) with those who
According to Hypothesis 3, we expected that did not complete the whole online questionnaire.
survivors of early-onset interpersonal trauma would Data for the latter group were available for the
still show higher scores on measures of emotion AAQ (n = 800), the DERS (396 b n b 745 for the
regulation difficulties when PTSD symptom severity different subscales) and the ERQ (n = 308). Partici-
is controlled for. In contrast to the hypothesis, two of pants with complete data reported significantly
the four MANCOVAs conducted were nonsignifi- higher emotion regulation difficulties on the AAQ
cant, indicating that the different groups of trauma and the DERS than participants who dropped out,
survivors did not differ regarding their acceptance of all t's N 2.3, all p's b .01. No significant difference
negative emotions as well as strategy use when emerged for the ERQ subscales, both t's b 1.7, both
symptom levels of PTSD were controlled. However, p's b .09.
significant main effects remained in the MANOVAs
for Awareness and Clarity and Difficulties With
Goal-Directed Behavior and Impulse Control. Fol- Discussion
low-up ANCOVAs showed that the DERS subscale The aim of the current study was to investigate the
Lack of Clarity was the only emotion regulation role of trauma type and PTSD symptom severity in
variable for which survivors of early chronic emotion regulation difficulties experienced by
interpersonal trauma remained to show higher scores trauma survivors. In line with the first hypothesis,
than all other trauma groups. On the DERS subscale significant correlations between all emotion regu-
Difficulties Engaging in Goal-Directed Behavior, lation variables and PTSD symptom severity were
survivors of early chronic interpersonal trauma still found. Symptom levels of PTSD were related to (a)
reported higher levels than survivors of noninterper- reduced levels of clarity and awareness of emotion;
sonal and late-onset interpersonal traumas, but did (b) low levels of acceptance of negative emotions,
not differ from early-onset single interpersonal higher levels of experiential avoidance, and higher
trauma survivors. levels of emotion suppression; (c) difficulties
engaging in goal-directed behavior when distressed
testing the robustness of the findings and high levels of impulse control difficulties; and
In order to test the robustness of the findings, we (d) impaired use of functional emotion regulation
repeated the analyses with slight variations. First, strategies, including low levels of reappraisal. These
we combined all survivors of early-onset interper- findings were found to be robust and still held when
sonal trauma to one group, regardless of whether looking at female participants only. The results
the event met criteria for chronic trauma or not. replicate a series of earlier findings (e.g., Morina et
This did not change the results. al., 2008; Moore et al., 2008; Tull, Barrett, et al.,
Second, as the proportion of females was 2007) and are consistent with the view that
significantly higher for the early-onset groups successful recovery from trauma requires adaptive
than the late-onset groups, we reran all analyses emotion regulation and that therefore difficulties in
for female participants only. 1 All correlations emotion regulation are a risk factor for the
development and/or maintenance of the disorder.
1
The number of male participants in our sample was rather low.
However, in the absence of prospective studies,
Therefore, controlling for sex by entering this as an additional factor the causal nature of the association remains unclear
in the analyses would have resulted in too small a sample size. as self-reported difficulties to regulate one's
Author's personal copy

emotion regulation in trauma survivors 595

emotions could also be the consequence of having clarity' and ‘difficulties engaging in goal-directed
the disorder. behavior' form an interesting exception as survivors
It has been suggested that emotion regulation early-onset chronic interpersonal trauma showed
difficulties are particularly prominent in survivors heightened scores on these variables independent of
of early-onset chronic interpersonal trauma, such as levels of PTSD.
childhood sexual or physical abuse by caregivers. However, alternative explanations for this pat-
The second aim of this study was to test this tern of findings are also conceivable. First, if early
hypothesis by comparing survivors of early-onset chronic interpersonal trauma leads to impaired
chronic interpersonal trauma with survivors of late- emotion regulation and if emotion regulation
onset, noninterpersonal and/or nonchronic difficulties in turn are a risk factor for PTSD, one
traumas. In line with our second hypothesis, would expect PTSD symptom severity as well as
significant group differences emerged for all vari- emotion regulation difficulties to be highest in the
ables assessing characteristics of emotion regula- early chronic interpersonal trauma group as found
tion. Simple contrasts confirmed the expected in the current study. A strong association between
pattern of group differences for all variables. all three variables may then conceal a true
Survivors of early-onset chronic interpersonal association between trauma type and emotion
trauma did not only report higher emotion regula- regulation difficulties when controlling for PTSD.
tion difficulties than nontraumatized controls, but Second, there may be some construct overlap
also higher difficulties than survivors of late-onset between the measures of PTSD symptom severity
traumas, early-onset noninterpersonal traumas, and emotion regulation difficulties, such as avoid-
and nonchronic early-onset interpersonal traumas. ance of negative affect and lack of ability to self-
This finding is in line with a small number of earlier soothe, which may have artificially increased the
studies (Briere & Rickards, 2007; Cloitre et al., effect of the IES-R as a covariate. Third, the results
1997; van der Kolk et al., 1996). It can be seen as may suggest that rather than being part of a
support for the idea that high levels of emotion syndrome of complex PTSD, emotion regulation
regulation difficulties are specific for survivors of difficulties could be a common feature of PTSD
early-onset chronic interpersonal trauma and are following any type of traumatic event.
part of the complex pattern of posttrauma sequelae The design of the current study does not allow a
found in this population (see Cloitre et al., 2005; decision between these alternative explanations.
van der Kolk, 2005; van der Kolk et al., 1996). However, the findings suggest a number of
However, because of the strong association be- promising avenues for future research. First, the
tween PTSD symptom severity and emotion regu- simultaneous consideration of trauma type and
lation difficulties, this finding could also simply be PTSD symptom severity are a strength of the
due to higher levels of PTSD in survivors of early- current study. Our results suggest that it is
onset chronic interpersonal trauma. It therefore necessary to also measure PTSD symptom severity
appears important to test whether the group when investigating the influence of trauma type on
differences hold when controlling for symptom emotion regulation and vice versa. Using groups
levels of PTSD. Our results showed that the matched by PTSD symptom severity or, ideally, a
expected group differences only remained signifi- prospective longitudinal design could then help to
cant for two variables: Lack of Emotional Clarity directly test the different ideas presented above.
and Difficulties Engaging in Goal-Directed Behav- Second, emotion regulation is a very broad
ior. However, group differences for the latter concept and has been operationalized in very
variable ceased to be significant when looking at different ways in past research. Results of the
female participants only. current study, in which different facets of the
The finding that group differences disappeared concept were assessed, showed that PTSD symptom
for most emotion regulation variables when con- severity was broadly related to all emotion regula-
trolling for levels of PTSD may be interpreted as tion variables and this remained significant when
evidence against the idea that emotion regulation controlling for type of trauma. On the other hand, a
difficulties are a specific feature of early-onset specific association between early-onset chronic
chronic interpersonal trauma. According to this interpersonal trauma and emotion regulation was
interpretation, the fact that survivors of early-onset only found for emotional clarity and difficulties
chronic interpersonal trauma showed higher scores engaging in goal-directed behavior when control-
on emotion regulation variables than survivors of ling for levels of PTSD. In future research, theory-
other types of traumatic events could be seen as driven predictions should be tested as to which
primarily the consequence of higher levels of PTSD specific aspects of emotion regulation are expected
in this group. The two variables ‘lack of emotional to be impaired as a consequence of early-onset
Author's personal copy

596 ehring & quack

chronic interpersonal trauma rather than treating emotion regulation interventions into standard
emotion regulation as a unitary phenomenon. As in treatment.
most earlier research, only self-report measures of Finally, a number of methodological limitations of
emotion regulation were included in the current the current study bear noting. Some of these limita-
study, which is a clear limitation. Future research tions are related to the fact that all measures were
should include more objective measures of emotion assessed via a web-based survey. Web-based studies
regulation and/or experimental designs. are related to reduced control over who participates
Third, the strong and robust association with and how participants fill in the measures. However, it
PTSD symptom severity raises the question as to is reassuring that studies directly comparing web-
whether emotion regulation difficulties reported by based versus paper-and-pencil surveys found both
trauma survivors should best be conceptualized as types of research to be equally reliable (Fortson et al.,
broad deficits of regulating negative emotional 2006; Nosek et al., 2002). Nevertheless, the high
states in general or as specific deficits in regulating number of participants not completing the survey is
negative emotions triggered by trauma reminders as an important challenge in web-based research. Given
reflected in the PTSD diagnostic criteria. These the population and topic of the current study, a high
alternative hypotheses could best be tested in dropout rate was predictable. Only 40% of partici-
experimental designs using emotion-eliciting stim- pants agreeing to take part completed all question-
uli that differ regarding their trauma-relatedness. naires and results of dropout analyses showed that
Fourth, it is unclear whether different types of completers showed higher levels of emotion regula-
early-onset chronic interpersonal trauma are differ- tion difficulties than noncompleters. As the high
ently associated with emotion regulation difficul- dropout may limit the generalizability of the findings,
ties. Future research investigating this issue should future studies should take measures aiming to reduce
not only focus on physical and sexual abuse but dropout and should ideally test representative samples
also include emotional abuse and neglect, which of trauma survivors.
were not assessed in the current study but have been An additional limitation of the current study
found to be related to emotion regulation difficul- concerns the fact that a nonclinical sample was
ties and symptom levels of PTSD in recent research tested and we did not assess how many participants
(e.g., Briere & Rickards, 2007). were in need of treatment for their symptoms and/
Fifth, the relationship between type of trauma, or had already received treatment. Furthermore,
PTSD, and emotion regulation difficulties is not PTSD was assessed with a self-report questionnaire
only of theoretical interest, but may also have only, which did not allow establishing a diagnosis
important clinical implications if replicated in future of PTSD. In addition, comorbid symptoms or
research. Based on the view that pronounced diagnoses were not measured. The characterization
emotion regulation difficulties are a specific feature of the current sample in terms of symptom severity
of complex symptoms that develop after early-onset is complicated by the fact that no norms or clinical
chronic interpersonal trauma, a number of authors cutoff scores are available for the IES-R (Weiss,
have suggested that these emotion regulation 2004). However, IES-R scores for survivors of late-
difficulties should be addressed in treatment before onset trauma are comparable to earlier studies
using trauma-focused strategies such as prolonged testing community samples of Type-I trauma
imaginal exposure (Cloitre et al., 2002; Wolfsdorf survivors (e.g., Beck et al., 2008; Creamer et al.,
& Zlotnick, 2001). However, it remains controver- 2003; King et al., 2009). Similarly, IES-R scores for
sial whether this phase-based approach is indeed survivors of early-onset interpersonal trauma are
necessary (for a discussion of this issue, see Cahill, comparable to those reported in earlier studies on
Zoellner, Feeny, & Riggs, 2004; Cloitre, Stovall- adult survivors of childhood abuse (e.g., Paivio,
McClough, & Levitt, 2004). The design of the Holowaty, & Hall, 2004; Shakespeare-Finch & de
current study does not allow drawing conclusions Dassel, 2009). Nevertheless, future research is
regarding this issue. Instead, research directly needed to investigate whether the findings can be
examining the effects of skilled-based versus trau- replicated in clinical samples with diagnoses estab-
ma-focused techniques on emotion regulation and lished by structured clinical interviews.
PTSD symptom severity in survivors of early-onset Despite these limitations, the results of the current
chronic interpersonal trauma is needed to resolve study emphasize the need to simultaneously consid-
this controversy. In addition, future research should er type of trauma and PTSD symptom severity when
also test the idea that emotion regulation difficulties investigating emotion regulation in trauma survi-
may be a feature of PTSD in general and that vors. The findings support a strong association
individuals with this disorder following all types of between PTSD and emotion regulation difficulties
traumatic events may benefit from the integration of and suggest new avenues for future research into
Author's personal copy

emotion regulation in trauma survivors 597

emotion regulation deficits as specific sequelae of Ford (Eds.), Treating complex traumatic stress disorders: An
early-onset chronic interpersonal trauma. evidence-based guide (pp. 31–58). New York: Guilford.
Ford, J. D., Stockton, P., Kaltman, S., & Green, B. L. (2006).
Disorders of extreme stress (DESNOS) symptoms are
associated with type and severity of interpersonal trauma
References exposure in a sample of healthy young women. Journal of
Batten, S. V., Follette, V. M., & Aban, I. B. (2001). Experiential Interpersonal Violence, 21, 1399–1416.
avoidance and high-risk sexual behavior in survivors of Fortson, B. L., Scotti, J. R., Del Ben, K. S., & Chen, Y. C.
child sexual abuse. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 10, (2006). Reliability and validity of an internet traumatic
101–120. stress survey with a college student sample. Journal of
Beck, J. G., Grant, D. M., Read, J. P., Clapp, J. D., Coffey, S. F., Traumatic Stress, 19, 709–720.
Miller, L. M., et al. (2008). The Impact of Event Scale- Frewen, P. A., Dozois, D. J. A., Neufeld, R. W. J., & Lanius, R. A.
Revised: Psychometric properties in a sample of motor (2008). Meta-analysis of alexithymia in posttraumatic stress
vehicle accident survivors. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21, 243–246.
187–198. Frewen, P. A., & Lanius, R. A. (2006). Toward a psychobiology
Briere, J., Kaltman, S., & Green, B. L. (2008). Accumulated of posttraumatic self-dysregulation: Reexperiencing, hyper-
childhood trauma and symptom complexity. Journal of arousal, dissociation, and emotional numbing. Annals of the
Traumatic Stress, 21, 223–226. New York Academy of Sciences, 1071, 110–124.
Briere, J., & Rickards, S. (2007). Self-awareness, affect Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional
regulation, and relatedness: Differential sequels of child- assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation:
hood versus adult victimization experiences. Journal of Development, factor structure, and initial validation of
Nervous and Mental Disease, 195, 497–503. the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. Journal of
Briere, J., & Spinazzola, J. (2005). Phenomenology and Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26, 41–54.
psychological assessment of complex posttraumatic states. Green, B. (1996). Trauma History Questionnaire. In B. H.
Journal of Traumatic Stress, 18, 401–412. Stamm & E. M. Varra (Eds.), Measurement of stress, trauma,
Cahill, S. P., Zoellner, L. A., Feeny, N. C., & Riggs, D. S. and adaptation (pp. 366–368). Lutherville, MD: Sidran
(2004). Sequential treatment for child abuse-related post- Press.
traumatic stress disorder: Methodological comment on Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive,
Cloitre, Koenen, Cohen, and Han (2002). Journal of and social consequences. Psychophysiology, 39, 281–291.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 543–548. Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two
Calkins, S. D., & Hill, A. (2007). Caregiver influences on emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect,
emerging emotion regulation: Biological and environmental relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and
transactions in early development. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Social Psychology, 85, 348–362.
Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 229–248). New York: Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation:
Guilford. Conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of
Cloitre, M., Koenen, K. C., Cohen, L. R., & Han, H. (2002). emotion regulation (pp. 3–24). New York: Guilford.
Skills training in affective and interpersonal regulation Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., Wilson, K. G., Bissett, R. T.,
followed by exposure: A phase-based treatment for PTSD Pistorello, J., Toarmino, D., et al. (2004). Measuring
related to childhood abuse. Journal of Consulting and experiential avoidance: A preliminary test of a working
Clinical Psychology, 70, 1067–1074. model. The Psychological Record, 54, 553–578.
Cloitre, M., Miranda, R., Stovall-McClough, K. C., & Han, Herman, J. L. (1992a). Complex PTSD: A syndrome in
H. (2005). Beyond PTSD: Emotion regulation and survivors of prolonged and repeated trauma. Journal of
interpersonal problems as predictors of functional impair- Traumatic Stress, 5, 377–391.
ment in survivors of childhood abuse. Behavior Therapy, Herman, J. L. (1992b). Trauma and recovery. New York: Basic
36, 119–124. books.
Cloitre, M., Scarvalone, P., & Difede, J. (1997). Posttraumatic Kashdan, T. B., Morina, N., & Priebe, S. (2009). Post-traumatic
stress disorder, self- and interpersonal dysfunction among stress disorder, social anxiety disorder, and depression in
sexually retraumatized women. Journal of Traumatic Stress, survivors of the Kosovo War: Experiential avoidance as a
10, 437–452. contributor to distress and quality of life. Journal of Anxiety
Cloitre, M., Stovall-McClough, C., Zorbas, P., & Charuvastra, Disorders, 23, 185–196.
A. (2008). Attachment organization, emotion regulation, King, D. W., Orazem, R. J., Lauterbach, D., King, L. A.,
and expectations of support in a clinical sample of women Hebenstreit, C. L., & Shalev, A. Y. (2009). Factor structure of
with childhood abuse histories. Journal of Traumatic Stress, posttraumatic stress disorder as measured by the Impact of
21, 282–289. Event Scale-Revised: Stability across cultures and time.
Cloitre, M., Stovall-McClough, K. C., & Levitt, J. T. (2004). Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research. Practice, &
Treating life-impairing problems beyond PTSD: Reply to Policy, 1, 173–187.
Cahill, Zoellner, Feeny, and Riggs (2004). Journal of Kring, A. M., & Werner, K. H. (2004). Emotion regulation
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 549–551. and psychopathology. In P. Philippot & R.S. Feldman
Cole, P. M., Michel, M. K., & Teti, L. O. (1994). The (Eds.), The regulation of emotion (pp. 359–385). Mahwah,
development of emotion regulation and dysregulation: A NJ: Erlbaum.
clinical perspective. Monographs of the Society for Research Litz, B. T., Orsillo, S. M., Kaloupek, D., & Weathers, F. (2000).
in Child Development, 59, 73–100. Emotional processing in posttraumatic stress disorder.
Creamer, M., Bell, R., & Failla, S. (2003). Psychometric Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 26–39.
properties of the Impact of Event Scale - Revised. Behaviour Lowery, K., & Stokes, M. A. (2005). Role of peer support
Research and Therapy, 41, 1489–1496. and emotional expression on posttraumatic stress disorder
Ford, J. D. (2009). Neurobiological and developmental in student paramedics. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 18,
research: Clinical implications. In C. A. Curtois, & J. D. 171–179.
Author's personal copy

598 ehring & quack

Marx, B. P., & Sloan, D. M. (2002). The role of emotion in the Roth, S., Newman, E., Pelcovitz, D., van der Kolk, B., &
psychological functioning of adult survivors of childhood Mandel, F. S. (1997). Complex PTSD in victims exposed to
sexual abuse. Behavior Therapy, 33, 563–577. sexual and physical abuse: Results from the DSM-IV field
Marx, B. P., & Sloan, D. M. (2005). Peritraumatic dissociation trial for posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic
and experiential avoidance as predictors of posttraumatic Stress, 10, 539–555.
stress symptomatology. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Scoboria, A., Ford, J., Lin, H. j., & Frisman, L. (2008).
43, 569–583. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses of the
McHugh, P. M., & Treisman, G. (2007). PTSD: A problematic Structured Interview for Disorders of Extreme Stress.
diagnostic category. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, Assessment, 15, 404–425.
211–222. Shakespeare-Finch, J., & de Dassel, T. (2009). Exploring
McLean, L. M., Toner, B., Jackson, J., Desrocher, M., & posttraumatic outcomes as a function of childhood sexual
Stuckless, N. (2006). The relationship between childhood abuse. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 18, 623–640.
sexual abuse, complex post-traumatic stress disorder and Tull, M. T., Barrett, H. M., McMillan, E. S., & Roemer, L.
alexithymia in two outpatient samples: Examination of (2007). A preliminary investigation of the relationship
women treated in community and institutional clinics. between emotion regulation difficulties and posttraumatic
Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 15, 1–17. stress symptoms. Behavior Therapy, 38, 303–313.
McNally, R. J. (2004). Conceptual problems with the DSM-IV Tull, M. T., Jakupcak, M., McFadden, M. E., & Roemer,
criteria for posttraumatic stress disorder. In G. M. Rosen L. (2007). The role of negative affect intensity and the
(Ed.), Posttraumatic stress disorder: Issues and controversies fear of emotions in posttraumatic stress symptom
(pp. 1–14). Chichester: Wiley. severity among victims of childhood interpersonal
Moore, S. A., Zoellner, L. A., & Mollenholt, N. (2008). Are violence. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195,
expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal associated 580–587.
with stress-related symptoms? Behaviour Research and van der Kolk, B. A. (2005). Developmental Trauma Disorder: A
Therapy, 46, 993–1000. more specific diagnosis than posttraumatic stress disorder
Morina, N., Stangier, U., & Risch, A. K. (2008). Experiential should be considered for children with complex trauma
avoidance in civilian war survivors with current versus histories. Psychiatric Annals, 35, 401–409.
recovered posttraumatic stress disorder: A pilot study. van der Kolk, B. A., Pelcovitz, D., Roth, S., Mandel, F. S.,
Behaviour Change, 25, 15–22. McFarlane, A. C., & Herman, J. L. (1996). Dissociation,
Nemeroff, C. B., Bremner, J. D., Foa, E. B., Mayberg, H. S., somatization, and affect dysregulation: The complexity of
North, C. S., & Stein, M. B. (2006). Posttraumatic stress adaptation to trauma. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153,
disorder: A state-of-the-science review. Journal of Psychiatric 83–93.
Research, 40, 1–21. van der Kolk, B. A., Roth, S., Pelcovitz, D., Sunday, S., &
Nightingale, J., & Williams, R. M. (2000). Attitudes to Spinazzola, J. (2005). Disorders of extreme stress: The
emotional expression and personality in predicting post- empirical foundation of a complex adaptation to trauma.
traumatic stress disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psy- Journal of Traumatic Stress, 18, 389–399.
chology, 39, 243–254. Weiss, D. S. (2004). The Impact of Event Scale - Revised. In J. P.
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Wilson & T. M. Keane (Eds.), Assessing psychological
E-research: Ethics, security, design, and control in psycho- trauma and PTSD: A practitioner's handbook (2nd ed., pp.
logical research on the internet. Journal of Social Issues, 58, 168–189). New York: Guilford press.
161–176. Weiss, D. S., & Marmar, C. R. (1997). The Impact of Event
Paivio, S. C., Holowaty, K. A. M., & Hall, I. E. (2004). The Scale - Revised. In J. P. Wilson & T. M. Keane (Eds.),
influence of therapist adherence and competence on client Assessing psychological trauma and PTSD (pp. 399–411).
reprocessing of child abuse memories. Psychotherapy: New York: Guilford Press.
Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 41, 56–68. Wolfsdorf, B. A., & Zlotnick, C. (2001). Affect management in
Pelcovitz, D., van der Kolk, B. D., Roth, S., Mandel, F., Kaplan, group therapy for women with posttraumatic stress disorder
S., & Resick, P. (1997). Development of a criteria set and a and histories of childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Clinical
structured interview for disorders of extreme stress (SIDES). Psychology, 57, 169–181.
Journal of Traumatic Stress, 10, 3–16. Zlotnick, C., Zakriski, A. L., Shea, M. T., Costello, E., Begin,
Plumb, J. C., Orsillo, S. M., & Luterek, J. A. (2004). A A., Pearlstein, T., et al. (1996). The long-term sequelae of
preliminary test of the role of experiential avoidance in post- sexual abuse: Support for a complex posttraumatic stress
event functioning. Journal of Behavior Therapy and disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 9, 195–205.
Experimental Psychiatry, 35, 245–257.
Roemer, L., Litz, B. T., Orsillo, S. M., & Wagner, A. W. (2001).
A preliminary investigation of the role of strategic with- R E C E I V E D : September 12, 2009
holding of emotions in PTSD. Journal of Traumatic Stress, A C C E P T E D : April 13, 2010
14, 149–156. Available online 30 June 2010

View publication stats

You might also like