Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

G Model

ERAP-447; No. of Pages 14 ARTICLE IN PRESS


Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée xxx (2018) xxx–xxx

Disponible en ligne sur

ScienceDirect
www.sciencedirect.com

Original article

Distinguishing between authenticity and personality consistency in


predicting well-being: A mixed method approach
Distinction entre authenticité et constance de personnalité dans la prévision du
bien-être : une approche de méthode mixte
A. Sutton
School of Psychology, The University of Waikato, Private Bag 3105, Hamilton 3240, New Zealand

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Introduction. – The relationship between role personality consistency and well-being is somewhat
Received 6 April 2017 ambiguous, with past research relying on methods confounded by social desirability or role stereotypes,
Received in revised form 14 May 2018 or conflating consistency with authenticity.
Accepted 1st June 2018
Objective. – This study uses a robust mixed method approach to establish the distinct impacts of authen-
ticity and personality consistency on well-being.
Keywords: Method. – In the quantitative phase, 191 participants completed questionnaires assessing contextualised
Authenticity
personality, well-being and authenticity. In the subsequent qualitative phase, a stratified sample of 22
Contextual personality
Well-being
participants completed a series of six open-ended reflective questions.
Mixed methods Results. – Findings demonstrated significant work-home personality differences at the individual, but
not group, level and revealed that authenticity, but not personality consistency, predicted well-being.
Thematic analysis demonstrated that authenticity was understood as acting in line with personal values
rather than demonstrating behavioural consistency across situations. In addition, while both personal-
ity differentiation and inauthenticity were seen as functional, their associations with well-being were
substantially different: differentiation was associated with both positive and negative feelings and inter-
pretations, while authenticity was exclusively a positive experience.
Conclusion. – Authenticity, not personality consistency, is the key contributing factor to well-being. People
who feel authentic, regardless of the extent of their contextual personality differentiation, are less stressed
and more satisfied.
© 2018 Elsevier Masson SAS. All rights reserved.

r é s u m é

Mots clés : Introduction. – Le rapport entre constance de personnalité et bien-être recèle une certaine part
Authenticité d’ambiguïté, avec des recherches passées s’appuyant sur des méthodes faussées par des biais de désir-
Personnalité contextuelle abilité sociale ou de rôle ou confondant constance avec authenticité.
Bien-être Objectif. – La présente étude fait appel à une approche de méthode mixte solide pour déterminer les
Méthodes mixtes
impacts distincts de l’authenticité et de la constance de personnalité sur le bien-être.
Méthode. – Lors de la phase quantitative, 191 participants ont rempli des questionnaires évaluant la
personnalité contextualisée, le bien-être et l’authenticité. Lors de la phase qualitative ultérieure, un
échantillon stratifié de 22 participants a répondu à une série de six questions réflexives ouvertes.
Résultats. – On relève d’importantes différences entre la personnalité au travail et celle à la maison et on
montre que l’authenticité, et non la constance de personnalité, permettait de prévoir le bien-être. Une
analyse thématique a fait apparaître que l’authenticité était perçue comme agissant en conformité avec
les valeurs personnelles plutôt que démontrant une constance de comportement au travers de situations.

E-mail address: anna.sutton@waikato.ac.nz

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2018.06.001
1162-9088/© 2018 Elsevier Masson SAS. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article in press as: Sutton, A. Distinguishing between authenticity and personality consistency in predicting well-being:
A mixed method approach. Rev. Eur. Psychol. Appl. (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2018.06.001
G Model
ERAP-447; No. of Pages 14 ARTICLE IN PRESS
2 A. Sutton / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée xxx (2018) xxx–xxx

De plus, alors que la différenciation de personnalité et l’inauthenticité étaient perçues comme fonction-
nelles, leurs associations avec le bien-être étaient substantiellement différentes : la différenciation était
associée à des sentiments et à des interprétations à la fois positifs et négatifs alors que l’authenticité était
exclusivement une expérience positive.
Conclusion. – L’authenticité, et non la constance de personnalité, est le facteur-clé déterminant du
bien-être. Les personnes possédant un sentiment d’authenticité, quelle que soit l’importance de leur
différenciation de personnalité contextuelle, sont moins stressées et plus satisfaites.
© 2018 Elsevier Masson SAS. Tous droits réservés.

Well-being is an increasingly central component of psycho- Despite these distinctions in definition, the positive relationship
logical, medical, economic and interdisciplinary research (Linton, between authenticity and well-being appears to be robust across
Dieppe, & Medina-Lara, 2016). While both authenticity and per- different models and measures (Kernis & Goldman, 2006; Lenton,
sonality consistency have been shown to influence well-being, the Slabu, & Sedikides, 2016; Sheldon et al., 1997; Wood et al., 2008).
research on this relationship is somewhat contradictory. Consis- Authenticity is also positively related to psychological adjustment.
tency in personality traits across situations has shown positive For example, it is negatively correlated with verbal defensiveness,
relationships with well-being (Bleidorn & Ködding, 2013), but perhaps because it indicates a greater tendency or willingness
including moderators or correcting for methodological flaws in the to engage with self-relevant information in an open and non-
measurement of personality consistency reduces or even reverses defensive manner (Lakey, Kernis, Heppner, & Lance, 2008). In this
this relationship (Baird, Le, & Lucas, 2006). In addition, while per- paper, Sheldon et al.’s (1997) simple definition of authenticity as
sonality inconsistency is often associated with lower well-being ‘being true to oneself’ is adopted.
and even considered pathological (see for example, Baird et al., Unfortunately, authenticity is sometimes conflated with the
2006), there is also clear evidence that ‘normal’ personality can concept of personality consistency, based on the assumption that
and does change without negative impacts on individuals: whether higher consistency across situations indicates a higher level of indi-
in different contexts or throughout adult life (Roberts, Walton, vidual authenticity. Sheldon et al. (1997) noted this potential for
& Viechtbauer, 2006; Scollon & Diener, 2006). Authenticity also confusion. Does being true to oneself involve striving for consis-
appears to be positively associated with well-being (Kernis & tency in personality or does the struggle for self-expression in
Goldman, 2006) but the evidence here too is somewhat opaque different contexts mean that inconsistency is more appropriate as
due to the frequent conflation of authenticity measures with per- a sign of authentic behaviour? Their view was that felt authentic-
sonality consistency. When defined as a subjective feeling of being ity can be an underlying source of personality differentiation and
true to oneself, authenticity can in fact be a source of personality their research demonstrated that well-being was positively related
change (Sheldon, Ryan, Rawsthorne, & Ilardi, 1997). to authenticity and personality consistency as well as low inter-role
This paper attempts to refine our understanding of these conflict.
concepts using a robust and integrated multi-method approach, Personality consistency is often highly valued in our society, as
in order to more accurately identify the relationships between seen for example in the axiomatic belief in trait theories that con-
authenticity, personality consistency and well-being. First, this sistency across situations is personality. If personality is defined
paper discusses the concept of authenticity and demonstrates its as patterns of thoughts, feelings and behaviour that are consis-
relevance to well-being. Next, personality consistency is defined, tent across situations, as in the trait approach (e.g. McCrae & Costa,
distinguishing it from authenticity and evaluating its relationship 1990), then flexibility in these patterns is seen as error, a threat
with well-being. Here, the utility of social roles as a means for to the construct of personality or even pathology. An alternative
capturing personality (in)consistency is explored. Finally, some view is to see that very situational variability as an indication of
significant methodological challenges in the study of contextual a hidden, but deeper underlying organisation of the personality
personality and authenticity are noted. The two phase, mixed system (Shoda & Mischel, 2000): a sign that the way personality
method study reported in this paper attempts to overcome these is structured includes adaptation to different situations. Changes
challenges, providing a more robust foundation for establishing in personality across the lifespan also indicate the importance of
the relationship between authenticity, personality consistency and individual interactions with the environment rather than simply a
well-being than has hitherto been possible. general maturation process (Scollon & Diener, 2006).
Authenticity is a complex concept, which has its origins in philo- There is substantial evidence for inconsistency or differentia-
sophical considerations of what it means to be human (Kernis tion in personality across different situations and social roles, of
& Goldman, 2006). Providing a concise historical review of the which ‘work’ and ‘home’ are identifiable examples. One of the most
authenticity concept, Kernis and Goldman (2006) argue for a consistent findings is significantly higher levels of conscientious-
conceptualisation of authenticity as including awareness, unbi- ness in the work role than other roles (Donahue & Harary, 1998;
ased processing, behaviour and relationship orientation. Measured Daniel Heller, Ferris, Brown, & Watson, 2009a; Sheldon et al., 1997)
using this approach, authenticity has been shown to be related though other trait differences tend to vary. For example, Heller et al.
to reduced defensiveness, improved coping, better self-concept, (2009a) reported that people were less extraverted at work than
better social role functioning and pursuit of goals, and increased home while Donahue and Harary (1998) found that extraversion
well-being. Wood, Linley, Maltby, Baliousis and Joseph (2008) was higher in the work role than roles such as spouse or child but
argue for an alternative approach in their model of authentic- lower than in friend or sibling roles.
ity, which is based in Rogers’ person-centred psychology and This variability in personality can only be understood if we
consists of three elements: lack of self-alienation (a sense of can elucidate the when and why of personality differentiation.
identity that is consistent with underlying beliefs and values), There were early calls for such a contextualised understanding of
living authentically (behaving in line with this identity) and the Big Five traits (Van Heck, Perugini, Caprara, & Fröger, 1994)
resisting external influence when it contradicts these beliefs and as a detailed and comprehensive understanding of personality
identity. necessitates a knowledge of both stability and dynamism in the

Please cite this article in press as: Sutton, A. Distinguishing between authenticity and personality consistency in predicting well-being:
A mixed method approach. Rev. Eur. Psychol. Appl. (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2018.06.001
G Model
ERAP-447; No. of Pages 14 ARTICLE IN PRESS
A. Sutton / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée xxx (2018) xxx–xxx 3

personality/situation/behaviour system (Bleidorn, 2009). Social In fact, much of the research on contextual personality uses an
roles provide a summary of the perceived situation (Bleidorn, approach epitomised by Sheldon et al. (1997), whereby participants
2009): they capture the expectations, constraints and affordances (usually undergraduate students) fill in personality questionnaires
for behaviour and are a way of operationalising contextualised for several roles at the same time. These roles are given general
personality. They are relevant to understanding the influence of names such as ‘student’, ‘friend’ and ‘employee’ and personality
traits on behaviour for two reasons. First, a social role represents differentiation is measured as the average correlation between per-
a group of situations with shared expectations and behavioural sonality trait ratings in different roles. The only elicitation of the
patterns. There is strong evidence that people perceive differ- social role therefore is in the instructions given to the respondents
ent situations as having different affordances for trait-relevant as they complete essentially the same measure several times in
behaviours (Church, Katigbak, & del Prado, 2010), i.e. certain a row. Respondents may well try to appear more consistent (in
situations allow greater expression of a trait than others. For line with the consistency equals psychological maturity hypothe-
example, a high level of extraversion will be more easily expressed sis) or more flexible across situations (to demonstrate their ability
in a group situation than when the person is alone. to adapt to different situations). This approach is further flawed in
Second, social roles offer behavioural guidance: that is, there are that it cannot control for social desirability effects.
rewards for conforming to the social expectations. An individual’s There is a clear need for research in contextual personality,
goals are essential in understanding behaviour as they direct and authenticity and well-being to use more robust methods and this
influence behaviour. This recognises an agentic view of individuals: study aims to meet that need in two ways. First, by measuring con-
we do not simply passively express certain traits in responding to textual personality while respondents are actually occupying the
situations, but actively engage in behaviours designed to achieve relevant role (stronger elicitation of the role) and second, by adopt-
specific desired outcomes. Goal-directed behaviour is central to ing a mixed method approach which can evaluate both general
a concept of personality that allows for and recognises change trends and individual experiences.
(Hennecke, Bleidorn, Denissen, & Wood, 2014). This active view A further methodological issue concerns the extent to which
of the individual is just as important in contextualised personality direct comparisons between contextual personality scores can be
research as it is in generalised personality research. According to made. Comparisons are complicated by the fact that it is a common
Dunlop (2015), individuals should be viewed as actors, agents and approach to distinguish between several different out-of-work
authors who actively construct contextualised personalities. roles such as friend, child, romantic partner or parent, yet to con-
Contextualised personality, therefore, provides a promising ceptualise ‘work’ as a single role. It could be argued that there are
avenue of research in understanding the complexities of person- as many work as non-work roles, for example, colleague, boss, sub-
ality change and stability and how they may relate to well-being ordinate, work friend or client-facing roles and that not specifying
for three reasons: work roles at the same level makes comparisons with the finer-
grained non-work roles misleading. For the sake of consistency in
• the social role can be viewed as a class of situation; comparison in this study, therefore, only two ‘meso-level’ roles are
• contextualised personality is closer to the contextualised goals used: work and home. There is evidence that this distinction in role
that are a key component of agentic behaviour; is understood by respondents and requires little further explana-
• individual differences are an important source of understanding tion to them (Daniel Heller, Perunovic, & Reichman, 2009b) and it
personality that is distinct from general maturation processes. provides a level of analysis that is more detailed than macro-level
general personality while still maintaining an approach which is
We now turn to consider the relationship of personality consis- parsimonious and not overly-arduous for respondents (Fig. 1).
tency with well-being and the critical role of measurement method Overall then, this paper seeks to evaluate individual experiences
in determining how these relationships are conceptualised. of authenticity and personality differentiation and their impact on
In their meta-analysis of personality differentiation and psycho- well-being, using robust and innovative methods to address some
logical adjustment, Bleidorn and Ködding (2013) note that there are of the challenges identified in previous research. This framework
two opposing suggestions for the role of differentiation in psycho- is represented visually in Fig. 1. It uses a two phase mixed method,
logical adjustment. The first claims that flexibility and adaptation longitudinal approach with respondents in full-time employment
is beneficial to the individual and that the ability to adapt or modu- within the contexts of ‘work’ and ‘home’ roles.
late behaviour is a sign of maturity and psychological adjustment.
The competing view sees differentiation as a sign of a divided self
or fragmentation and consistency rather than flexibility is a sign of 1. Method
maturity and adjustment. Their meta-analysis showed a negative
correlation between personality differentiation and psychological This research uses a mixed method approach. There are fre-
adjustment, thus providing support for the second view. This would quent calls for applied research to use mixed methods designs in
indicate a fairly solid, replicable finding, yet when culture was order to provide depth of understanding and increased general-
included as moderator, the relationship between differentiation isability of findings (e.g. Biron, Karanika-Murray, & Cooper, 2012)
and psychological adjustment reduced significantly for collectivist and mixed methods can prove particularly valuable in research on
cultures. complex, contextualised phenomena such as personality and self-
The apparently clear link between personality differentiation awareness (Sutton, Williams, & Allinson, 2015). As a true mixed
and reduced well-being is further called into question by Dunlop method study, the two phases of research reported here were intri-
(2015), who notes serious methodological in replications. These cately interconnected, rather than representing two independent
flaws were demonstrated convincingly by Baird et al. (2006) who studies on related topics. The two phases were designed to provide
showed that when mean-level variance in the measures was cor- generalisable nomothetic conclusions in tandem with an individ-
rected for, variability in personality was unrelated to well-being or ualised, in-depth understanding of the concepts’ impacts in lived
even slightly positively related. Following this up with an expe- experience.
rience sampling method, they again found that mean levels of Accordingly, a first phase of quantitative research was designed
personality traits were responsible for the negative association to determine the extent to which individuals differentiate between
between variability and well-being and that when this was cor- work and home personality, the relation between differentiation
rected for, the relationship reduced. and felt authenticity, and the impact of both on well-being. A

Please cite this article in press as: Sutton, A. Distinguishing between authenticity and personality consistency in predicting well-being:
A mixed method approach. Rev. Eur. Psychol. Appl. (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2018.06.001
G Model
ERAP-447; No. of Pages 14 ARTICLE IN PRESS
4 A. Sutton / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée xxx (2018) xxx–xxx

Fig. 1. Conceptual framework.

quantitative approach was considered to be best suited to evaluat- reliability (Furr & Bacharach, 2013) and considered effective in mit-
ing these relations for three reasons. First, it is important that the igating against memory effects.
findings from previous quantitative studies can be directly com- A unique link to online questionnaires (hosted on Surveymon-
pared to a study using more robust methods of eliciting contextual key) was emailed to each participant. To ensure, as far as possible,
personality. Second, a quantitative approach allows the develop- that participants completed the questionnaire while in the appro-
ment of generalizable relations between the variables. And third, priate context, the following steps were taken:
the quantitative approach enables the researcher to identify and
test precisely defined relationships between variables of interest.
The first phase was followed up with a second longitudinal, • participants provided both a work and a home email address.
qualitative phase, which addressed the detailed, day-to-day expe- The work-context questionnaire was sent to the work email
rience of workers managing the affordances and expectations of address and the home-context questionnaire emailed to the
the two contexts. This second qualitative phase was carefully built home address;
on the first by using a stratified subsample from the first phase • the emailed invitation as well as the introduction to the online
and the construction of prompt questions to further explore the questionnaire stated that participants should only complete the
findings and implications of the first study. A qualitative approach survey while in the appropriate context (i.e. work or home);
was chosen for this second phase in order to provide an in-depth, • invitations and reminders were emailed at times which would be
richer account of authenticity and personality differentiation as most likely to reach participants while they were in the appropri-
experienced by full-time workers, providing a more complex, ate work or home context. That is, work-context emails arrived
contextualised understanding of the concepts of interest. Fig. 2 pro- during the working week and home-context emails on Friday
vides a visual representation of the research design and samples at evenings.
each stage.

1.1.3. Measures
1.1. Phase 1 Demographic information including working hours, job tenure,
sex and age was collected, along with the following scales.
1.1.1. Sample Personality was evaluated using the IPIP 50-item Big Five per-
Participants in full time employment (defined as 37.5 hours a sonality inventory (IPIP, 2001) with 10 items per trait and a 5-point
week or more) were recruited through adverts in professional body Likert-type response scale. Psychometric properties of this measure
newsletters (e.g. Chartered Institute of Personnel and Develop- are good, including reported alphas for each trait > 0.75 and robust
ment), the researcher’s personal contacts, Linkedin, and word of and replicable factor structure (Buchanan, Johnson, & Goldberg,
mouth. In return for their time, participants were offered a small 2005). Scores were calculated as a mean for each trait across all
Amazon voucher and individualised feedback on their personality 10 items.
questionnaire. Volunteers made initial contact with the researcher Three different measures were created from this inventory. First,
by providing demographic data including a work and home email work and home role personality trait scores were calculated. Sec-
address, job title, current employer and working hours per week. ond, differentiation scores were calculated for each individual using
This was used to verify respondents and exclude any who did not the absolute difference between the trait scores for each role (i.e.
meet the requirements of the study. Two hundred and thirteen par- the magnitude of the difference in trait scores regardless of the
ticipants completed the first questionnaire, with 191 going on to direction of the difference). Third, an overall personality differen-
complete the second. The analyses reported here are based on par- tiation score was calculated for each participant from the mean of
ticipants who provided a complete data set. Mean age was 36.8 these five differentiation scores. This overall differentiation score
years (SD = 10.5), 69% of the sample were female and mean job was used for the stratified sampling of participants for the second
tenure was 4.49 (SD = 5). phase of the research, which is described in phase two.
Negative well-being was assessed using the 4 item Perceived
1.1.2. Procedure Stress Scale (Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983, reported
Participants were randomly allocated to two groups. The first alpha > 0.8) which asks participants about their thoughts and feel-
group completed a series of online questionnaires contextualised ings over the past month. Example item: in the last month, how
for work and then, after a delay of two weeks, completed the same often have you felt that you were unable to control the important
series contextualised for home. This was reversed for the second things in your life? Responses are made on a five point scale from
group, who completed the home questionnaire first and the work 1 (never) to 5 (very often) and reverse scored where necessary so
questionnaire second, ensuring that administration was counter- that a higher score indicates greater stress. Scores were calculated
balanced across the sample to combat order effects. The two week for the work role and home role as well as an overall mean for each
delay is common in psychometric studies establishing test-retest participant.

Please cite this article in press as: Sutton, A. Distinguishing between authenticity and personality consistency in predicting well-being:
A mixed method approach. Rev. Eur. Psychol. Appl. (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2018.06.001
G Model
ERAP-447; No. of Pages 14 ARTICLE IN PRESS
A. Sutton / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée xxx (2018) xxx–xxx 5

Fig. 2. Summary of research design and samples.

Positive well-being was measured using two questions on sat- 1.2.1. Sample
isfaction with and preference for the role, based on Sheldon et al. In order to more clearly identify and explore participants’ expe-
(1997) and contextualised as appropriate: how satisfied are you rience of authenticity and personality differentiation, a stratified
with your work/home life as a whole? and would you prefer to sampling technique was employed which allowed the selection
spend more or less time in your work/home role. Responses were of 12 participants from each of 4 different ‘strata’ representing
made on a 7-point scale with higher scores indicating greater sat- the most extreme (high or low) scores on the two scales of self-
isfaction and preference for the role. Scores were calculated for integration and personality differentiation. If participants’ scores
the work role and home role as well as an overall mean for each made them eligible for membership of more than one subgroup
participant. (e.g. high self-integration and low personality differentiation), they
Authenticity was measured using the Sheldon et al. (1997) were allocated to the subgroup based on their most extreme score.
authenticity scale, which evaluates participants’ feelings of authen- This was only the case for three respondents.
ticity within each role. It consists of 5 items (e.g. I have freely chosen The final sample consisted therefore of four subgroups of 12
this way of being) scored on a 7 point scale from 1 (strongly dis- participants each: low self-integration (M = 3.32, SD = .48), high
agree) to 7 (strongly agree), with a reported alpha > 0.7. Scores were self-integration (M = 6.61, SD = .21), low personality differentia-
calculated from this scale in two ways: a role authenticity score was tion (M = .70, SD = .1) and high personality differentiation (M = 4.1,
calculated for each role using the mean of the 5 items and an overall SD = .31). Mean age was 36.8 years (SD = 9.7) and 70% of the sample
self-integration score from the mean of the two role authenticity were female. Respondents had a mean tenure of 4 years in their
scores. current jobs (SD = 4.6).

1.2. Phase 2
1.2.2. Procedure
The second part of the study was designed to provide a Respondents completed a series of six open-ended reflective
detailed, in-depth understanding of the personal experiences and questions in an online questionnaire, one per week, as shown
implications of authenticity and personality differentiation, par- in Table 1. The questionnaire consisted of a front page, which
ticularly within the work context, which built on the findings explained the purpose of the study along with assurances of confi-
from the first phase. It used a longitudinal, diary-type approach dentiality and anonymity for both the respondents and any others
where respondents gave written discussions in response to open- they might mention in their responses and a second page contain-
ended questions. This longitudinal approach enabled participants ing the prompt question and a free response text box. A reminder
to develop their reflections over time and providing further depth email was sent three days after the original invitation for each week
than a single data collection point would allow. and participants were able to ‘unsubscribe’ or withdraw from the

Please cite this article in press as: Sutton, A. Distinguishing between authenticity and personality consistency in predicting well-being:
A mixed method approach. Rev. Eur. Psychol. Appl. (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2018.06.001
G Model
ERAP-447; No. of Pages 14 ARTICLE IN PRESS
6 A. Sutton / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée xxx (2018) xxx–xxx

Table 1 activity, it has since developed into a useful technique for examin-
Weekly questions.
ing psychological experiences, especially retrospective self-report
Week Topic Prompt question and reflections (Butterfield, Borgen, Amundson, & Maglio, 2005).
1 Personality The title of this research project is “Work Remaining within an organisational psychology context and in line
differentiation and Home Personality”. How different do with these more recent developments in its use, CIT is used in this
you personally feel you are in the two study primarily as a data collection tool to encourage participants to
different contexts? Is this how you would recall incidents in a structured and comprehensive manner, rather
ideally like to be? Why or why not?
than the combined collection and analysis approach first proposed
2 Experience of We all take on different roles in our normal
inauthenticity lives, whether at work, with friends or by Flanagan.
family. Sometimes this can mean “playing Of the original sample, 22 participants completed all 6 question-
a part” or acting in a way that suits the naires and in total 159 individual responses were collected across
situation rather than a way that feels like
the 6-week period. Responses were analysed using thematic anal-
the “real you”. Can you think of a time you
did this at work? ysis. Although some have suggested that thematic analysis is a tool
3 Experience of In the last questionnaire, we asked you to to be used across or within different qualitative analysis methods,
authenticity write about a time when you behaved in a there is strong argument for it being an analytical method in its
way that seemed to suit your work role but own right which has the advantages of flexibility, theoretical free-
was not necessarily your “natural” or
dom and accessibility (Braun & Clarke, 2006). These advantages
preferred way of doing things. This time,
we would like you to think of a time when make thematic analysis well suited to the current realist mixed
you felt able to really “be yourself” at work method study, allowing the researcher to reflect and report on
and were not just taking on a role the experiences, meanings and reality of the participants and pro-
4 Efforts towards In the last couple of questions, we were
viding greater richness and detail to contextualise and extend the
self-integration talking about what it’s like to play a role or
at work to behave in a way that feels more “you”.
statistical analysis.
This week, we’d like you to think about
how important it is to you to “be yourself”
1.2.3. Analysis
at work
a) Some people want their work to be an Inductive thematic analysis was undertaken on the text
extension of who they are, while others see responses from each week by the author and a research assistant,
work as just something they do and are following the general steps outlined by Braun and Clarke (2006).
happy to behave in whatever way suits the After familiarisation with the data (step 1) by both researchers, ini-
situation best. How do you personally view
your work? How important is it to you to
tial coding and searching for themes (step 2 and 3) was conducted
“be yourself” at work? by the research assistant aware of the study’s aims but blind to the
b) And finally for this week, what does it participants’ group membership. This was done in order to intro-
mean to you to “be yourself” at work? Is it duce a level of objectivity into the analysis, attempting to reduce
just about how you behave, or is there
the influence of preconceived notions about which groups might
more to it?
5 Authenticity in Over the past few weeks you’ve been have particular views or understandings of the concepts. While
work reflecting on how you behave in different subjectivity is an inherent part of the qualitative approach (and,
relationships roles and what kind of impact this might it could be argued, any analytical approach), it has been recog-
have on you and your work. This week, nised that attempts to increase objectivity in qualitative methods
we’re asking you to think specifically about
your relationships with people at work.
where possible can increase the credibility of qualitative research
These people could be colleagues, (Jonsen & Jehn, 2009). Step 4 (reviewing themes and generating an
managers and customers/clients initial thematic map) and 5 (defining and naming themes) were car-
Please consider the following questions: ried out by the author and included comparative analysis by group
How authentic do you feel in your
membership.
relationships at work? (By authentic we
mean: able to be yourself, honest with
others about who you are and how you 2. Results phase 1 statistical analysis
feel)
Do you think this level of authenticity in
your work relationships is about right for The statistical analysis of phase 1 is presented first, followed
you or would you prefer more or less? by the thematic analysis of the longitudinal reflective diary study
Have you had any positive experiences as a of phase 2. Table 2 presents the means, standard deviations and
result of being honest and open about
bivariate correlations for all measures, along with the alpha reliabil-
whom you are with people at work? Have
you experienced any drawbacks or ities for all scales. All scales showed high reliability (˛ ≥ .75). Within
negative results? role, authenticity was positively correlated with the majority of the
6 Reflection on Having been thinking about your home contextualised personality traits, the exceptions being agreeable-
the process and work personality over the last several ness in the work context and conscientiousness and openness in the
weeks, to what extent do you think they
home context. In addition, within each role, higher levels of authen-
are different now? Have you noticed any
changes? ticity were associated with greater satisfaction and preference for
Is this how you would ideally like to be? that role as well as lower stress.
Why?

2.1. Personality differentiation and authenticity

study at any time. Participants were offered an additional Amazon Work and home personality traits were significantly and
voucher for completing phase 2 of the study. strongly correlated across roles (r = .6 to .7, p < .01), as expected from
In week 2 and 3, participants were guided through a critical inci- an understanding of trait personality as capturing cross-situational
dent technique reflection (CIT) (Flanagan, 1954) using additional consistency. In order to evaluate whether this consistency exists
prompts. Originally developed within the organisational psychol- alongside role differentiation, paired t-tests were conducted on all
ogy literature as a means for gaining a detailed description of an five traits. The only trait to demonstrate a difference across roles

Please cite this article in press as: Sutton, A. Distinguishing between authenticity and personality consistency in predicting well-being:
A mixed method approach. Rev. Eur. Psychol. Appl. (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2018.06.001
G Model
ERAP-447; No. of Pages 14 ARTICLE IN PRESS
A. Sutton / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée xxx (2018) xxx–xxx 7

Table 2
Descriptive, alpha reliabilities (given in brackets on the diagonal) and bivariate correlations for all scales.

Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

Work role
Extraversion 3.45 .65 (.83)
Agreeableness 4.15 .54 .356** (.84)
Conscientiousness 3.87 .55 .047 −.024 (.75)
Emotional stability 3.18 .81 .176* −.049 .345** (.89)
Openness 3.74 .52 .333** .092 .012 .060 (.75)
Stress 2.53 .75 −.153* .124 −.206** −.595** −.094 (.76)
Satisfaction 5.12 1.55 .217** .063 .205** .470** .071 −.562**
Preference 2.98 1.23 .011 .008 .175* .298** .052 −.355** .483**
Authenticity 4.92 1.11 .336** .124 .226** .550** .159* −.533** .659** .438** (.79)
Home role
Extraversion 3.48 .65 .658** .260** .102 .157* .138 −.148* .164* −.019 .256**
Agreeableness 4.15 .54 .283** .697** .055 −.013 .137 .065 .055 −.032 .073
Conscientiousness 3.48 .71 .052 −.041 .618** .236** −.132 −.146* .016 .181* .184*
Emotional stability 3.14 .82 .114 .021 .200** .628** .035 −.349** .266** .125 .317**
Openness 3.72 .57 .268** .038 −.081 .068 .718** −.026 −.038 .022 .036
Stress 2.33 .81 −.050 .043 −.130 −.401** .085 .283** −.199** .013 −.173*
Satisfaction 5.60 1.54 .088 .058 .167* .344** −.032 −.161* .163* −.004 .181*
Preference 5.26 1.37 −.009 .015 .061 −.033 −.034 .107 −.084 −.341** −.070
Authenticity 5.55 1.13 .153* .162* .177* .351** .066 −.194** .196** −.032 .263**

10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18

Home role
Extraversion (.83)
Agreeableness .392** (.83)
Conscientiousness .148* .122 (.82)
Emotional Stability .266** .157* .206** (.89)
Openness .173* .187** −.039 .119 (.78)
Stress −.143* .006 −.077 −.617** −.006 (.83)
Satisfaction .192** .141 .069 .474** .013 −.665**
Preference .014 .097 −.052 .121 −.011 −.293** .321**
Authenticity .294** .219** .096 .645** .116 −.723** .642** .376** (.81)

Alpha reliabilities are given in brackets where relevant.


*
Indicates p < .05.
**
Indicates p < .01.

Table 3 Table 4
Differentiation between work and home personality traits (i.e. absolute differences). Regression analyses: predicting overall well-being from self-integration and per-
sonality differentiation.
Mean SD T value (df = 190) Effect size (d)
Stress Satisfaction Preference
Extraversion .41 .35 16.18* 1.17
Agreeableness .32 .27 15.90* 1.19 Model 1
Conscientiousness .53 .45 16.26* 1.18 Self-integration (␤) −.64** .69** .39**
Emotional stability .51 .48 14.79* 1.06 R2 .41** .48** .15**
Openness .30 .28 14.41* 1.07 F 131.21** 174.14** 33.12**
* Model 2
Indicates p < .001.
Self-integration (␤) .69**
Differentiated conscientiousness (␤) .12*
R2 .02*
was conscientiousness, which was significantly higher at work than F 91.82**
home (t190 = 9.39, p < .001, d = .71). *
Indicates p < .05.
These minimal differences in contextual personality scores at **
Indicates p < .001.
the group-level were, however, not reflected in the analysis of indi-
viduals’ absolute differences in trait scores. Differentiation scores
for each trait (i.e. the absolute value of the difference between for the emotional stability trait, with differentiation in this trait
home and work traits for each individual) were tested against a positively correlated with self-integration (r = −.16, p < .05). This
null hypothesis of zero difference using a one-sample t-test. Highly indicates that while authenticity is unrelated to the degree of
significant differences were revealed (Table 3), all with large effect differentiation in the majority of the Big Five traits, those with
sizes (d > 1.0). greater levels of similarity in their work and home emotional
Participants’ role authenticity scores were significantly pos- stability also feel a greater sense of self-integration.
itively correlated (r = .26, p < .01), demonstrating that people
reporting high levels of authenticity in one context also felt they 2.2. Effects on well-being
were being authentic in the other. It should be noted, however that
this correlation is not strong enough to indicate that felt authentic- Work authenticity was positively correlated with satisfaction
ity is independent of the context. There is clearly some variability (r = −.66, p < .01) and preference for the role (r = .44, p < .01) and
in how authentic people feel across different situations. negatively correlated with stress (r = −.53, p < .01). A similar pat-
Differentiation scores were then correlated with self- tern emerged for the home role, with satisfaction and preference
integration scores (i.e. mean authenticity across the two roles) positively correlated with authenticity (r = .64 and .38 respectively,
to examine the extent to which personality differentiation was p < .01) and stress negatively (r = −.73, p < .01). Overall, authenticity
related to authenticity. A significant correlation was found only in each role was associated with higher well-being in that role.

Please cite this article in press as: Sutton, A. Distinguishing between authenticity and personality consistency in predicting well-being:
A mixed method approach. Rev. Eur. Psychol. Appl. (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2018.06.001
G Model
ERAP-447; No. of Pages 14 ARTICLE IN PRESS
8 A. Sutton / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée xxx (2018) xxx–xxx

To further explore the effect of authenticity and personality reference to comparisons and similarities with the quantitative
differentiation on overall well-being, multiple linear regression results.
was conducted. The step-wise entry method was selected as
this enables a comparison to be made between the effect of
self-integration scores (the measure of overall authenticity) on
well-being in Model 1 and the added effect of personality differen- 3.1. Personality differentiation
tiation on well-being in Model 2. Results (Table 4) demonstrate that
self-integration is a major contributor to well-being, predicting 41% 3.1.1. Differentiation is a conscious experience
of variance in stress and 48% of variance in satisfaction, as well as The majority of respondents (20 out of 34) reported that they
15% of the variance in role preference. Personality differentiation, in felt they had clear differentiation between their work and home
contrast, had limited effect on well-being, with only differentiated personality. This theme was most common (emerging in 75% of
conscientiousness emerging as a significant predictor (accounting responses) for the high personality differentiation group, and least
for an extra 2% of variance in satisfaction). common (25%) for the low personality differentiation group, pro-
The relationships between personality differentiation, authen- viding triangulation support for the findings in the quantitative
ticity and well-being were addressed in further detail through analysis: the personality differentiation captured by those scales
phase 2 of the study and the following qualitative results provide represents a consciously felt experience on the part of the respon-
both context for the quantitative findings and delve further into the dents. It was also common for both high and low self-integration
detail of the respondents’ experiences to explore the complexity of groups to report experiencing a differentiation in personality (70%
this issue. and 57% respectively), providing further evidence that authenticity
is distinct from personality differentiation.
3. Results phase 2 thematic analysis Four sub-themes were identified within this personality dif-
ferentiation experience. For about two thirds of the respondents,
Four main themes were identified from the data and they maintaining a distinction between work and home personality was
are represented with their first- and second-order sub-themes in reported as being like putting on an act at work, with the ‘real
Fig. 3. Each of these main themes is described and developed in self’ at home being more natural and in line with participants’ real
the following sections with illustrative quotes and discussed with interests.

Fig. 3. Data structure and themes.

Please cite this article in press as: Sutton, A. Distinguishing between authenticity and personality consistency in predicting well-being:
A mixed method approach. Rev. Eur. Psychol. Appl. (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2018.06.001
G Model
ERAP-447; No. of Pages 14 ARTICLE IN PRESS
A. Sutton / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée xxx (2018) xxx–xxx 9

At home I feel more true to myself, as I am able to enjoy what Again, this sub-theme emphasises personality differentiation as
really interests me [. . .] At work, I feel as if I am acting – doing an agentic process in which individuals engage in an attempt to cre-
what is expected of me to fulfil my role, rather than doing what ate boundaries for themselves and possibly even maintain a sense
I would naturally prefer to do (P20). of felt authenticity: work may require traits which are not ‘really
me’, but I am more than just my work personality. Responses here
However, for the remaining respondents, the work or pro-
also raise the important organisational issue that differentiation
fessional personality was valued as giving expression to aspects
can signal severe dissatisfaction and turnover intent.
of themselves that were important and well regarded. People
The relationship between personality differentiation and stress
reported feeling more confident, committed and conscientious at
at work was further detailed where respondents gave their reasons
work than at home, as well as reflecting on their professional per-
for modifying their personality. The first factor involved accounts
sonality as a better version of themselves. These specific comments
of the respondents’ previous work experience, which had made
reflect the higher level of conscientiousness trait that is the most
them realise that they needed to behave differently. The second
robust statistical group-level difference between work and home
involved the style and culture of the respondents’ working envi-
personality.
ronment, which did not allow them to behave as they would like
I feel more alert and motivated at work than at home [. . .] I try to:
to be a better person (P2).
It might be a reaction to the 17 years I spent in IT in Financial
[I am] more confident dealing with people and situations at
Services. It was so stressful I practically burnt myself out. I lived,
work [. . .] more motivated and organised at work [. . .] I will
breathed, ate work, and it was the only thing I had to talk about
overcome obstacles to meet work commitments, but not neces-
(P14).
sarily personal ones (P12).
. . .my line manager does not share the same values as me. . .[. . .]
In these first two sub-themes, a clear contrast was identified to better secure my position in the future of the organisation at a
between respondents in the high self-integration group, where per- time of deep financial cuts, tactically I perceive that it would be
sonality differentiation was exclusively seen as putting on an act, useful for me to come more into alignment with her approach
and those in other groups, where there was a mix of experience. This (P15).
indicates that those who have a high sense of authenticity may be
This again indicates that individuals engage in personality dif-
creating this work personality more consciously in response to the
ferentiation as a means of reducing stress: providing something
requirements and expectations of work, but also recognising that
of a buffer to a negative environment or adapting to the require-
it does not necessarily reflect what they believe to be their ‘true
ments of the situation in order to reach a later goal (such as career
self’.
advancement).
The positive associations with personality differentiation were
captured by the third sub-theme of enjoying duality, where respon-
dents expressed enjoyment of the variety it brought, as well as 3.1.2. Consistency as essential
the importance they ascribed to maintaining a distinction between A substantial minority of respondents (10 out of 43) reported
the two arenas. This sub-theme was almost exclusive to the high that they experienced little if any personality differentiation
personality differentiation group and supports the notion that between work and home, describing instead an experience of
personality differentiation or consistency is agentic, goal-directed essential consistency across the two contexts. These responses
behaviour (Dunlop, 2015). were limited to the high self-integration and low personality differ-
I do like the fact that I am different in/out of work and that not entiation group, indicating that when it comes to an experience of
everything is the same (P3). consistency, it is both the felt authenticity and the personality con-
. . .I just want to keep the 2 things separate [. . .] To me home sistency that contribute. An important feature of this sub-theme
and work are separate, and I like it that way (P14). was that the respondents felt that consistency was important for
their lives and relationships.
The negative side of maintaining differentiated personalities
was captured in the fourth and final sub-theme of personality dif- I am happy with how I behave because it helps me get along
ferentiation as a coping mechanism. This may provide an insight well with my work colleagues as well as my family and have a
into the interpretation of the findings in the quantitative phase, good relationship with both (P26).
where personality differentiation did not make a substantial con- I am fairly similar both at work and at home [. . .] it is important
tribution to stress or well-being. Responses here demonstrate that to reflect your normal personality at work (P37).
personality differentiation did not necessarily cause stress, but
rather was a response to stress or a way of mitigating the impact
of stressful environments. Creating a clearer demarcation between 3.2. Felt authenticity
work and home personality could be a way of dealing with a nega-
tive work environment. Indeed, respondents suggested that lacking 3.2.1. Expression of self at work
flexibility in personality traits could in itself be a cause of stress. When considering what it meant to be authentic at work, a num-
ber of respondents said that they view their work as an expression
Sticking by your traits when they don’t really fit with a job is of who they are. It was important to them to remain authentic
stressful, self-inflicted stress (P47). in their working environment as a matter of integrity as well as
I feel very different when at home compared to when at work. personal and psychological stability.
At home, I would say I am more relaxed and less frustrated. . . .
At work, I don’t feel that what I do is worthwhile, and I have little I do view my work as an expression of my personality, of my
time for most of my colleagues. I’m appalled and dismayed by being, of who I am [. . .] We spend a lot of time at work so it
the waste and inefficiency at work – this has made me feel rather seems important to me that it expresses something of who we
embittered. . . . I feel I’d be a lot happier if at work I could see are (P47).
‘light at the end of the tunnel’, and some opportunity for career I completely see work as an extension of myself. I am here for
progression and job satisfaction. Yes – I’m looking for a new job! longer than I am at home and I think if I had to behave in a
(P19). particular way, I would find it really hard (P25).

Please cite this article in press as: Sutton, A. Distinguishing between authenticity and personality consistency in predicting well-being:
A mixed method approach. Rev. Eur. Psychol. Appl. (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2018.06.001
G Model
ERAP-447; No. of Pages 14 ARTICLE IN PRESS
10 A. Sutton / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée xxx (2018) xxx–xxx

In contrast, other respondents noted that their work did not It might be wondered why many people behave inauthentically
define who they were and talked about being paid to play a cer- when they find it such a psychologically difficult process. There
tain role: they referred to the need to respond to work tasks by were three main motivations identified in this dataset:
suppressing their true self, as something that needed to be done to
remain professional and earn a living.
• acting professionally: respondents often reported a sense that
Work is the way I earn a living to pay the bills and help you to they were expected to display a professional image (consisting
enjoy your life. I behave in a way that keeps my job and people of appearing serious and unemotional) in order to create a good
off my back (P6). impression and to effectively manage others at work.
I see it as an aspect of who I am not everything I am or something
that defines me (P44). I feel I need to maintain a professional image and cannot show
personal opinions for fear of it affecting the image I portray at
A prominent sub-theme in discussions of authenticity related work (P9);
to the tension between recognising the importance of authentic-
• avoiding conflict: the majority of respondents discussed their
ity alongside the restrictions of professionalism, highlighting that
authenticity was not simply about behaving however the individ- attempts to maintain good working relationships and a harmo-
ual wanted but could represent a personally valued yet appropriate nious climate.
response to the situation. Acting in this way was most effective for not antagonising
It is important to be myself as much as possible, situation the situation or causing further problems or an uncomfortable
permitting, whilst retaining the standards of professionalism atmosphere (P34);
required (P34). • keeping the job: some respondents reported the belief that con-
This tension and the primary role of values were emphasised as cealing their authentic self was an element of the job and a
respondents argued that authenticity was about more than simply concern that there could be negative consequences if they were
the way people behave at work. Authenticity was a matter of a to act more authentically.
certain mind-set, values and behaving in line with their own morals I felt pressured myself to give the ‘correct’ answer, rather than
and ethics. create any fuss or upheaval, which could cause more detri-
There is more to being yourself than just about how you behave ment to my role (P13).
at work. The values/morals you have also play a key part (P3).
To ‘be yourself’ at work [. . .] can well be expressed in the way It should be noted that the low PD group was unique in
you behave, but it is desirable that your values at work corre- not reporting negative experiences related to inauthenticity at
spond to your own personal values (P19). work, though they still identified reasons why they behaved in
Participants were guided through a critical incident technique an inauthentic manner. This suggests that the low PD group
to describe an experience of being authentic and inauthentic at may experience these ‘inauthentic’ moments simply as pragmatic
work. Respondents described how in certain events at work they adjustments rather than as an internal tension, and given the sim-
felt forced to play a part and construct a role, which implied a lack ilarity between their role personalities, are perhaps simply less
of authenticity in their personality. While there was some cross- likely to experience inauthenticity at work.
over in how respondents discussed personality differentiation and The quantitative results indicated a large negative impact of
authenticity, which can be expected from the layperson unfamil- inauthenticity on well-being, and this was supported by the qual-
iar with the more technical definitions we use here, there was one itative analysis, which further extends the findings by identifying
distinctive difference: inauthenticity was overwhelmingly experi- the important reasons that people engage in inauthentic behaviour
enced negatively and authenticity positively, in contrast with the despite its high costs.
more balanced feelings about and experiences of simple personality
differentiation.
3.2.3. Authenticity as a productive and positive experience
While inauthenticity was a functional but negative experience,
3.2.2. Inauthenticity as a functional necessity
authenticity was identified as both functional and overwhelmingly
Inauthenticity was perceived as a ‘functional necessity’ with
positive for the respondents. Being authentic at work contributed to
a wide range of negative psychological outcomes, again reinforc-
their work relationships and interactions with different stakehold-
ing the view that authentic behaviour is goal-driven. Respondents
ers, such as colleagues, customers, other staff and even managers.
noted that playing a part like this was generally effective in that
Respondents argued that being authentic was effective because it
they managed to remain professional and either avoid conflict or
helped them to present a more light-hearted, happy, confident and
keep other people in their job satisfied, but the associated effects of
productive self and to have better and more effective relationships
behaving inauthentically on the individuals were wide ranging and
at work.
generally negative, centred around the sense that they were playing
a part which was unnatural to them. The most common effects were I felt really confident, enthusiastic, involved and invigorated
stress, damaged self-esteem, a sense of discomfort, lower produc- (P2).
tivity at work and feeling detached from work. Illustrative quotes I felt as though I was talking to an old friend rather than a cus-
are given below: tomer [. . .] We have a definite rapport going now (P25).
It’s effective, up to a point, but not particularly satisfying and Many respondents expressed a desire for a work environ-
creates its own strain. It’s very tiring (P7). ment where they could be authentically themselves without being
Leave me feeling stressed at my lack of voice (P15). unprofessional. Responses here often recognised that this was a
I found it incredibly stressful and emotionally exhausting as this tricky balance for many organisations and that it might not be easy
role was a complete opposite to my natural way of being (P28). to accomplish, but also that it was vitally important to some of the
It felt very undermining and disempowering [. . .] it felt humil- respondents, even to the extent of being more essential to them
iating to play the ‘just be compliant’ role (P15). than the type of work or pay they earned.

Please cite this article in press as: Sutton, A. Distinguishing between authenticity and personality consistency in predicting well-being:
A mixed method approach. Rev. Eur. Psychol. Appl. (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2018.06.001
G Model
ERAP-447; No. of Pages 14 ARTICLE IN PRESS
A. Sutton / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée xxx (2018) xxx–xxx 11

The ideal would be a work place that gives us space to be truly 3.3.2. Cautionary tales
ourselves (P47). Alongside, these optimistic views were a recognition that
I would prefer a greater level of authenticity at work, and feel authenticity involved a certain level of vulnerability that might
strongly that this being true to oneself is – for me – more impor- not be appropriate or wanted in the workplace. For example, one
tant than my line of work or remuneration (P20). respondent noted how he did not feel he could share anything about
his mental health because it might adversely affect his career:
Greater authenticity was also perceived as being beneficial to
productivity, supporting stronger, more successful and support- I can’t admit to my team that I have mental health issues as
ive relationships through developing trust and communication. they may lose respect for me or think that I cannot do my job
Respondents believed it contributed to better well-being and sat- [. . .] I wouldn’t want to jeopardise my career by talking about
isfaction. anything too personal (P3).
When I am able to be authentic, I find that I can motivate and There were also cautionary tales of when respondents felt that
inspire others more convincingly. I also find that I can be more their attempts to be authentic at work had resulted in negative
productive myself and that I end up enjoying my working day outcomes.
more (P20).
I’ve had experiences where I’ve confided in someone about how
I’m feeling and they have not respected my privacy. I’ve felt hurt
and disrespected by that, and disinclined to repeat the experi-
ence! (P14).
This recognition of the vulnerability which may be associated
3.3. Safety and trust with being authentic emphasises the importance of trust and safety
as prerequisites for an authentic workplace and parallels the recog-
3.3.1. Pre-requisite for authentic behaviour and workplaces nised issue of vulnerability in attempts to develop self-awareness
Respondents distinguished between genuine relationships, found in previous research (Sutton et al., 2015).
where they felt they could be authentic, and constructed rela-
tionships, which were more guarded. Authentic relationships were
mostly with peers and close colleagues where they felt free from 3.4. Flexibility vs consistency
the fear of being judged and able to express their true feelings. Here,
authenticity can be seen to be in a virtuous circle with trust: it is The vast majority of respondents expressed a desire to be able
based on trust and it builds trust. to show their true selves, to be honest about who they were at
work and home. For many, this meant they wished for greater con-
With peers/team, colleagues, being myself help to form trusting sistency and to be able to be similar in the two contexts, believing
relationships as they also tend to be open and honest with me that this would build stronger relationships and bring more positive
(P22). results at work.
Constructed relationships in contrast were generally with In an ideal world, I would like to be exactly the same at home
clients or those at a different level in the organisational hierarchy, and work (P13).
where the respondents’ behaviour was shaped by certain feeling I think it is important to show your true personality in order to
and display rules like professionalism and respect. For some peo- build more trusting relationships with people at work (P37).
ple, this type of guarded relationship was how they experienced I would ideally like to be honest and true to myself and to my
work in general and they preferred to maintain that distance even colleagues and clients. . . Everyone works best when they can
from team colleagues. be themselves and it brings out the best in everyone (P39).
With the people I manage, I feel that I am not myself and 100% This was sometimes expressed as a desire to bridge the gap:
honest about who I am as I have to maintain professionalism having a consistency of personality in the two contexts where dif-
and respect with them (P9). ferences would not be too large or they could feel and act the same
The incidents described by participants which allowed them a in the two roles.
greater level of authenticity at work involved incidents of when I would ideally like to balance the work me and the home me
they were given more autonomy or tasks they really enjoyed and more so that I am a similar person in both environments (P22).
feel confident about. A role for autonomy in authenticity has been
demonstrated in a study which noted that the relationship between Some respondents were hoping for or working towards this
social power and authenticity was mediated by personality differ- more balanced or consistent approach, while others felt they had
entiation (Kraus, Chen, & Keltner, 2011). In addition, there was a already achieved this happy medium:
significant role for pleasant interpersonal relations, being with peo- I believe that the level [of consistency] is fine. It keeps the right
ple they could trust and communicate well with, in helping them image with my boss and the wider population in the company,
to reveal their authentic selves. at the same time in my day-to-day work relationships is rea-
I worked on a project creating training sessions [. . .] It enabled sonably authentic and strong (P6).
me to “play” rather than work [. . .] I had the freedom to laugh Others however, rather than expressing a desire for greater
and be silly and allowed to throw out ideas and explore them consistency, wished for a greater flexibility, believing this would
(P14). reduce the amount of stress they put themselves under and help
There are many people I work with who I would consider trusted them maintain boundaries.
colleagues and therefore, I feel able to “be myself” with (P22).
There are considerable rewards in being able to be flexible, ulti-
Feelings of safety and security are clearly essential to authentic mately it can be less stressful which is reason enough in itself
behaviour. As demonstrated above, reasons for behaving inauthen- but also it can help you to actually be truer to yourself and your
tically include fear of losing a job and conflict avoidance, while own values, to express who you really are to others, both at work
authentic behaviour is encouraged by trust in relationships. and at home (P47).

Please cite this article in press as: Sutton, A. Distinguishing between authenticity and personality consistency in predicting well-being:
A mixed method approach. Rev. Eur. Psychol. Appl. (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2018.06.001
G Model
ERAP-447; No. of Pages 14 ARTICLE IN PRESS
12 A. Sutton / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée xxx (2018) xxx–xxx

I think there also has to be an element of difference as well. The parallels with Lenton et al.’s (2016) study of state authenticity,
roles we perform at work are not the same as the roles we are in which demonstrated significant changes in feelings of authenticity
at home, and so we are used to wearing different hats. If there across a wide range of different situations. However, authenticity
were no boundaries, I think I would feel rather lost (P25). is still measured as a trait rather than state in the present study,
just as contextual personality is, as participants were responding
With all these responses, the aim seemed to be for respondents
based on their perceptions of themselves generally within that role
to feel comfortable with whom they were, whether this was in
rather than experience sampling across the day.
maintaining their current differentiated or consistent personalities
There are individual differences in felt authenticity and to some
in different roles, or in attempting to change and bridge the gap.
extent these differences are associated with an individual’s contex-
tual personality. Those who are more extraverted and emotionally
4. Discussion stable within each context report a greater level of authenticity
for that context. In addition, at work, higher conscientiousness and
This study aimed to develop our understanding of the rela- openness to experience are also associated with higher authentic-
tionships between personality differentiation and authenticity in ity; while at home; higher levels of agreeableness are associated
accounting for well-being, using more robust and innovative meth- with authenticity. These varying relationships between contextual
ods than have been used in previous work and drawing on a personality and authenticity within that role may be interpreted
sample of full-time employed respondents. The use of a longitudi- as providing further evidence that certain contexts may encour-
nal diary study enabled the development of respondent reflections age the expression of certain traits and that those individuals who
over a six week time period, resulting in deeper qualitative find- express these traits also feel that they are being authentically them-
ings than are possible with a single data collection point. The selves. For those who do not express these traits as highly, there are
findings emphasised the unique individual differences in contex- associated feelings of inauthenticity. This again indicates the com-
tualised personality and authenticity that may be hidden in less plexity of the inter-relationships between situation and personality
robust methodologies, as well as confirming the agentic nature in determining behaviour.
of personality differentiation and the strong relationship between Authenticity predicted a substantial proportion of well-being,
authenticity and well-being. The following discussion is organised confirming previous findings (Kernis & Goldman, 2006; Lenton
around the three themes of the study: personality consistency, et al., 2016; Sheldon et al., 1997; Wood et al., 2008), while person-
authenticity and well-being. ality differentiation was generally unrelated (the exception being
A common approach in investigating contextualised personality conscientiousness which accounted for a small proportion of vari-
is to instruct respondents to complete several personality ques- ance in role satisfaction). These results provide some insight into
tionnaires at the same time, each marked for a different context the debate as to whether personality differentiation indicates self-
(Sheldon et al., 1997). In addition, these studies are also often con- fragmentation or mature adaptation to situations. Bleidorn and
ducted with student samples rather than full-time employees and Ködding’s (2013) meta-analysis concluded that personality dif-
Shen et al. (2011); in their review of applied psychology research ferentiation was negatively related to indicators of well-being.
encouraged the use of samples, which more closely match the aims However, the criteria for inclusion in the meta-analysis included
of the research. This study therefore took the more robust approach only the requirement that self-concept differentiation (SCD) was
of delivering each contextualised personality questionnaire to full- measured in “at least two different predefined social contexts”
time employed respondents while they were in the appropriate (p. 548), rather than any requirement about how SCD was mea-
context (work or home), including a time gap of two weeks between sured. In fact, of the 54 studies included in the meta-analysis, only
completions and analysing results at both group and individual two (by Baird et al. (2006)) measured different role personalities at
level. different times and this was done via an experience sampling tech-
At the individual level, there were highly significant absolute nique. These two studies demonstrated that personality variability
differences between work and home personality on all five traits. across situations was unrelated to well-being, which is in line with
This finding emphasises the centrality of the individual’s context in the current study and provides support for the interpretation that
any consideration of personality consistency or flexibility, further flexibility or consistency in personality is not in itself related to
evidence of the flexible nature of personality even as measured by well-being. It also further underscores the need for robust mea-
traits. It also provides some support for Bleidorn’s (2009) sugges- sures in assessing contextual personality: it is not enough simply
tion that social roles can be considered summaries of the situation to ask participants to complete several questionnaires at once and
as perceived by the individual, enabling researchers to capture hope that the measures are accurately capturing their personality
some of the personality-situation interactions in a parsimonious in these different contexts. What is being measured in this kind of
way. approach is more likely to be the participants’ beliefs about how
The findings here also suggest that contextual personality differ- consistent or flexible they want to appear, rather than their actual
ences at the individual level effectively cancel out when considered consistency or flexibility.
at the group level: for example, some people are more extraverted In attempting to further delineate the role of contextual person-
at work than at home, while others are more introverted, resulting ality in well-being therefore, we need to turn to authenticity, that
in a null group difference. In fact, the only group-level difference is, a person’s feeling of being true to self. The quantitative element
found in this study was a higher level of conscientiousness in the of this study demonstrated that being authentic predicts lower lev-
work context compared to the home context, which is also the most els of stress, higher sense of satisfaction and a greater preference
consistent finding in previous research (D Heller, Watson, Komar, for a role, both within each role as well as across roles. But how
Min, & Perunovic, 2007; Sheldon et al., 1997). Other differences does this take place in people’s lived experience? And what does it
found in previous work failed to replicate when using this more mean to them to be authentic at work and home? To answer these
robust methodology. It seems likely therefore, that previous meth- questions, we turn to the qualitative analysis.
ods may have encouraged participants to respond from generally Personality differentiation as captured by the quantitative mea-
held stereotypes or schemas rather than capturing true differences. sure used here represents a conscious experience on the part of
Felt authenticity is positively correlated across contexts, though the respondents and is distinct from felt authenticity. One of the
there remains some variability in the levels of authenticity indi- strengths of the integrated multi-method design is that these com-
viduals experience in the work and home contexts. This shows plexities can be identified quantitatively and explored qualitatively

Please cite this article in press as: Sutton, A. Distinguishing between authenticity and personality consistency in predicting well-being:
A mixed method approach. Rev. Eur. Psychol. Appl. (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2018.06.001
G Model
ERAP-447; No. of Pages 14 ARTICLE IN PRESS
A. Sutton / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée xxx (2018) xxx–xxx 13

within the same sample. The qualitative findings provide some promoting authenticity at work. Building a foundation of safety and
indication that those with higher authenticity were able to cre- trust can allow each individual to engage with his or her work in
ate a work personality to suit their particular environment but that an authentic manner, developing a virtuous cycle of further trust,
it was recognised as not necessarily reflecting their ‘true self’. In better relationships and communication.
addition, personality differentiation was perceived as enjoyable In seeking to make an internally consistent yet practically viable
by those with high differentiation scores. Finally, differentiation distinction between contextual personalities, this study considered
was also described as a coping mechanism for situations where an the personality-situation interaction at a meso-level, making fairly
individual perceived that his or her natural approach might lead a broad-stroke distinction between work and home roles. This deci-
to negative outcomes. All these findings underscore the agentic sion does limit the nature of these findings: there is likely to be even
or goal-directed nature of contextual personality, as suggested by further differentiation between more finely-grained social roles. A
Hennecke et al. (2014): differentiation is undertaken in order to be more precise understanding of the personality-situation-behaviour
a more effective worker, for the enjoyment of expressing a different triad could be achieved by the use of further role descriptions and
side of the self, to reduce stress, or even as a way of maintaining a an interesting line of future research could be the comparison of
boundary between work and home. Personality differentiation as a roles such as work friend and non-work friend.
coping mechanism for stress may also help to explain the some- Secondly, although the mixed method approach reported here
times contradictory results in previous cross-sectional research attempts to use the complementary strengths of quantitative and
(Bleidorn & Ködding, 2013) about the relationship of differentia- qualitative methods, there remains the fact that this necessarily
tion with well-being: there could well be a substantial element of involves a certain degree of compromise in both. For example, the
any population who will report higher stress alongside higher dif- use of a stratified sample for the qualitative phase based on the
ferentiation because they are attempting to use the latter to cope findings from the quantitative phase, while ensuring that important
with the former, rather than because the former causes the latter. comparisons could be made, may well have excluded respondents
It is important to note that personality differentiation is not with very different experience and understanding of the concepts
synonymous with authenticity: feeling authentic does not mean under study.
one must behave in the same way across all situations. In fact, the Finally, although every effort was made to ensure that respon-
level of an individual’s personality differentiation across the work dents completed the questionnaires in the appropriate context,
and home contexts was generally unrelated to the level of self- there remains the possibility that this was not done and the elic-
integration (i.e. mean authenticity). Those with high authenticity itation of the social role was not as distinct for each respondent.
were no more likely to have high or low personality differentia- Future research could perhaps adopt an approach more similar
tion than those with low authenticity. The exception to this was to experience-sampling, where respondents are asked to indicate
the emotional stability trait, where higher authenticity was associ- their context at the same time as responding to personality items.
ated with a lower difference between emotional stability at work
and home. In addition, the qualitative findings demonstrated that
people identify authenticity as involving values and behaving in 5. Conclusion
line with deeper morals rather than similarly in behaviour patterns
across different situations. Prior research into role or contextual personalities has utilised
Significantly, personality differentiation was associated with methods, which are open to problems with confounding variables
a range of feelings and interpretations, both positive and nega- such as role stereotypes or social desirability. This study used more
tive, while authenticity was exclusively identified as a positive robust methods to reduce the impact of these possible confounding
experience and inauthenticity negative. Both differentiation and variables. Specifically, contextualised personality was measured
inauthenticity were described by participants as functional in some while the respondents were in the relevant context and with a two
way, underlining the agentic nature of both, but their associations week gap between questionnaire completions. Another strength of
with well-being were substantially different. the design was the use of a sample consisting of a relatively mature
One of the significant implications of this research is to empha- group of full-time employees rather than undergraduate students.
sise the importance of the individual experience in personality Finally, the utilisation of a six week qualitative diary design enabled
differentiation and authenticity. Personality differentiation simply the collection of in depth, highly developed reflections on the con-
disappears when averaged across a group and there are distinct cepts under study.
differences in people’s personal experiences of differentiation: for Overall, the study supports the finding that, at a group level,
some, it is flexibility and adaptability that is an essential while for people are more conscientious at work than home. But these more
others it is consistency. In addition, it is worth noting it was not the robust methods suggest that other differences found in previous
case in this research that people only felt they could ‘be themselves’ studies may be artefacts of the research design rather than reflect-
at home. For some of those who made a clear distinction between ing consistent differences in role personality. The studies reported
work and home personality, the home personality was more valued here find excellent quantitative and qualitative evidence that at an
and for others, the work personality was. individual level, employees actively adapt themselves to suit their
Just as personality differentiation was shown to be agentic and own particular work or home context. While previous research
goal directed, so too is authentic behaviour. Here, the goals tended in the area has proposed a link between personality differentia-
to be more internal, involving acting in line with one’s inner values tion and lower well-being, this study indicates that it is in fact
and moral code, rather than in order to meet external requirements authenticity rather than consistency, which is the key to well-being.
or material benefits (such as promotion). Inauthentic behaviour Employees who feel authentic, irrespective of the degree of differ-
was motivated by three aims: acting professionally, avoiding con- entiation in their role personalities, report lower stress and greater
flict and keeping the job, but was also reported as having negative satisfaction in their roles.
impacts on well-being, confirming the quantitative results. Adapting to work and home roles by differentiating one’s
An important finding for work organisations is that authenticity personality was recognised by many participants as a necessity,
is not only associated with higher well-being, but also high pro- helping them to maintain a harmonious working environment and
ductivity, confidence and effectiveness at work. In addition, many a sense of professionalism at work. However, the negative effect of
respondents had a strong desire to be authentic at work. Taken as inauthenticity on productivity and well-being was also apparent. In
whole, this research indicates that organisations could benefit from summary, personality differentiation is a functional response and

Please cite this article in press as: Sutton, A. Distinguishing between authenticity and personality consistency in predicting well-being:
A mixed method approach. Rev. Eur. Psychol. Appl. (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2018.06.001
G Model
ERAP-447; No. of Pages 14 ARTICLE IN PRESS
14 A. Sutton / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée xxx (2018) xxx–xxx

can be experienced positively whereas inauthenticity, also engaged Heller, D., Perunovic, W. Q. E., & Reichman, D. (2009). The future of person–situation
in as a functional response to the environment, is universally expe- integration in the interface between traits and goals: A bottom-up framework.
Journal of Research in Personality, 43(2), 171–178.
rienced negatively. Heller, D., Watson, D., Komar, J., Min, J.-A., & Perunovic, W. (2007). Contextualized
personality: Traditional and new assessment procedures. Journal of Personality,
Disclosure of interest 75(6), 1229–1253.
Hennecke, M., Bleidorn, W., Denissen, J. J. A., & Wood, D. (2014). A three-part frame-
work for self-regulated personality development across adulthood. European
The author declares that she has no competing interest. Journal of Personality, 28(3), 289–299. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.1945
(2001). International personality item pool. (Retrieved from http://ipip.ori.org/)
Jonsen, K., & Jehn, K. A. (2009). Using triangulation to validate themes in qualitative
Acknowledgements studies. Qualitative Research in Organizations and Management: An International
Journal, 4(2), 123–150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/17465640910978391
This research was supported by grant number 1101 from the Kernis, M. H., & Goldman, B. M. (2006). A multicomponent conceptualization of
authenticity: Theory and research. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,
Richard Benjamin Trust. 38, 283–357. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38006-9 (Editor: P.Z.
Mark, Academic Press)
References Kraus, M. W., Chen, S., & Keltner, D. (2011). The power to be me: Power elevates self-
concept consistency and authenticity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
Baird, B. M., Le, K., & Lucas, R. E. (2006). On the nature of intraindividual personal- 47(5), 974–980. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.03.017
ity variability: Reliability, validity and associations with well-being. Journal of Lakey, C. E., Kernis, M. H., Heppner, W. L., & Lance, C. E. (2008). Individual
Personality and Social Psychology, 90(3), 512–527. differences in authenticity and mindfulness as predictors of verbal defen-
Biron, C., Karanika-Murray, M., & Cooper, C. (2012). Improving organizational siveness. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(1), 230–238. http://dx.doi.org/
interventions for stress and well-being: Addressing process and context. Hove: Rout- 10.1016/j.jrp.2007.05.002
ledge. Lenton, A. P., Slabu, L., & Sedikides, C. (2016). State authenticity in everyday life.
Bleidorn, W. (2009). Linking personality states, current social roles and major European Journal of Personality, 30(1), 64–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.2033
life goals. European Journal of Personality, 23(6), 509–530. http://dx.doi.org/ Linton, M.-J., Dieppe, P., & Medina-Lara, A. (2016). Review of 99 self-report
10.1002/per.731 measures for assessing well-being in adults: Exploring dimensions of well-
Bleidorn, W., & Ködding, C. (2013). The divided self and psychological (mal) being and developments over time. BMJ Open, 6(7), e010641. http://dx.doi.org/
adjustment – A meta-analytic review. Journal of Research in Personality, 47(5), 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-010641
547–552. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1990). Personality in adulthood. New York: Guilford Press.
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of mean-level
Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(2), 77–101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1191/ change in personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longi-
1478088706qp063oa tudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132(1), 1–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
Buchanan, T., Johnson, J. A., & Goldberg, L. R. (2005). Implementing a five-factor 0033-2909.132.1.1
personality inventory for use on the Internet. European Journal of Psychological Scollon, C. N., & Diener, E. (2006). Love, work and changes in extraversion and neu-
Assessment, 21(2), 115–127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.18.1.116 roticism over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(6), 1152–1165.
Butterfield, L. D., Borgen, W. A., Amundson, N. E., & Maglio, A.-S. T. (2005). Fifty years http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022.3514.91.6.1152
of the critical incident technique: 1954–2004 and beyond. Qualitative Research, Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Rawsthorne, L. J., & Ilardi, B. (1997). Trait self
5(4), 475–497. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1468794105056924 and true self: Cross-role variation in the Big-Five personality traits and its
Church, A. T., Katigbak, M. S., & del Prado, A. M. (2010). Cultural similar- relations with psychological authenticity and subjective well-being. Jour-
ities and differences in perceived affordances of situations for Big Five nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(6), 1380–1393. http://dx.doi.org/
behaviors. Journal of Research in Personality, 44(1), 78–90. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/0022-3514.73.6.1380
10.1016/j.jrp.2009.11.003 Shen, W., Kiger, T. B., Davies, S. E., Rasch, R. L., Simon, K. M., & Ones, D. S. (2011).
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived Samples in applied psychology: Over a decade of research in review. The Journal
stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24(4), 385–396. of Applied Psychology, 96(5), 1055–1064. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023322
Donahue, E. M., & Harary, K. (1998). The patterned inconsistency of traits: Map- Shoda, Y., & Mischel, W. (2000). Reconciling contextualism with the core assump-
ping the Differential effects of social roles on self-perceptions of the Big Five. tions of personality psychology. European Journal of Personality, 14(5), 407–428
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(6), 610–619. http://dx.doi.org/ (10.1002/1099-0984(200009/10)14:5<407::AID-PER391>3.0.CO;2-3).
10.1177/0146167298246005 Sutton, A., Williams, H. M., & Allinson, C. W. (2015). A longitudinal, mixed
Dunlop, W. L. (2015). Contextualized personality, beyond traits. European Journal of method evaluation of self-awareness training in the workplace. Euro-
Personality, 29(3), 310–325. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.1995 pean Journal of Training and Development, 39(7), 610–627. http://dx.doi.org/
Flanagan, J. C. (1954). The critical incident technique. Psychological Bulletin, 51(4), 10.1108/EJTD-04-2015-0031
327–358. Van Heck, G. L., Perugini, M., Caprara, G.-V., & Fröger, J. (1994). The big five as
Furr, R. M., & Bacharach, V. R. (2013). Psychometrics: An introduction (2nd Ed.). Lon- tendencies in situations. Personality and Individual Differences, 16(5), 715–731.
don: Sage. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(94)90213-5
Heller, D., Ferris, D. L., Brown, D., & Watson, D. (2009). The influence of Wood, A. M. L., Linley, P. A., Maltby, J., Baliousis, M., & Joseph, S. (2008). The authentic
work personality on job satisfaction: Incremental validity and mediation personality: A theoretical and empirical conceptualization and the develop-
effects. Journal of Personality, 77(4), 1051–1084. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ ment of the Authenticity Scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 55(3), 385–399.
j.1467-6494.2009.00574.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.55.3.385

Please cite this article in press as: Sutton, A. Distinguishing between authenticity and personality consistency in predicting well-being:
A mixed method approach. Rev. Eur. Psychol. Appl. (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2018.06.001

You might also like