Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Speed and Safety

Ezra Hauer

The speed at which people elect to travel is affected by vehicle and road EVOLUTION OF SPEED
design; by limits to speed and enforcement of those limits; by traffic con-
trol, signs, and markings; and so forth. The speed at which people travel, Figure 1 shows the time evolution of speed on the rural interstates in
in turn, affects road safety. In this context two questions arise: (a) How Montana. The horizontal axis is in quarterly units, except for the years
is the evolution of speed over time and space affected by what drivers 1982 to 1986 and for the “gap” years (1986 to 1995) where no speed
do? (b) How does speed affect safety? This paper reviews what is known, data were collected, and the increments are annual. The solid line is
notes the gaps in knowledge, and describes where opinions differ and for the 85th percentile and the dashed line for the median speed.
why. Unfortunately, despite decades of speed measurement and moni- Between 1979 and April 1987, the speed limit was 55 mph. From
toring, the evolution of speed over time is poorly documented, and the there until December 1995, the speed limit was raised to 65 mph.
understanding of what drives the evolution is largely missing. It is Whether there was a jump in April to May 1987 cannot be said
known that speeds evolve over time, but not why; it is known that there because data collection stopped until 1995. A backward extrapolation
is some spillover of the change from one road to another, but its size or of the later trend indicates that a jump in speed likely occurred. In
extent cannot be predicted. This is a neglected field of inquiry. More is December 1995 Montana adopted the “Basic Rule,” which prevailed
known in answer to Question b. There can be no reasonable doubt that until the end of May 1999. According to the Basic Rule, daytime
if speed increases while other conditions (vehicles, roads, medical ser- speeds should not exceed what is “reasonable and prudent” in a police
vices) remain unchanged, the accidents that occur will tend to be more officer’s judgment, while the nighttime speed limit remained at
severe. However, the prevalent and strongly held belief that the greater 65 mph. On May 28, 1999, Montana abandoned the Basic Rule and
the speed, the higher is the probability that accidents will occur is, at raised the speed limit to 70 mph.
present, not well supported by research. Even so, given a change in Figure 1 represents a phenomenon that is evolving over time. The
mean speed, one can predict the consequences in injuries and fatalities left half of the figure, in particular the period 1981 to 1999, shows a
and this paper discusses how to do so. steady upward creep in speed that, over the nearly two decades,
amounted to 10 to 15 mph. What was the cause of this creep which
continued even during times when the speed law and the road
The relationship between speed and safety is subject to much debate. remained the same? There must have been influences other than a
Opinions are strongly held and much heat is generated around the change in speed limit. Then, sometimes in 1998, even before the
dinner table, in the media, on the pages of professional and scien- Basic Rule gave way to the 70 mph speed limit, the upward creep
tific publications, and, of course, in politics. The essence of the sit- seems to have slowed down and stopped. What has changed? Is this
uation is a two-link causal chain in which the arrow represents the kind of speed evolution typical in other states or countries? Is it char-
word “affects”: acteristic of all road types or confined to rural access-controlled
roads? What drives the upward creep and what stops it? These are
Some of what people do (by vehicle design and advertising, by road weighty questions that do not seem to have received much attention.
design, by setting speed limits and enforcing them, etc.) → speed One can easily imagine various mechanisms that might contribute
choice → road safety. to the apparent upward drift in speeds.
One such mechanism could be the practice of setting the speed
These two causal links raise two questions: limit by the 85th percentile of the speed distribution. For example,
assume that collectively drivers elect speeds such that about half of
Question 1. How does what we do shape the evolution of speeds them drive faster than the speed limit. This behavior, if coupled with
on roads? a periodical application of the 85th percentile rule, would cause an
Question 2. How does speed affect safety? upward drift in speeds as illustrated in Figure 2.
The dashed curve depicts the speed distribution with the prevail-
These are the main questions discussed in this paper. The purpose ing speed limit of 70 km/h. When the speed limit is raised to 90 km/h
is to review what can be regarded as “known,” to note where the to be in line with the 85th percentile rationale, then, by the assumed
gaps in knowledge are, and to state where opinions differ and why. behavior, the solid curve will gradually form. This, in turn, will justify
the next round of speed limit adjustments, and so on.
Another behavioral mechanism affecting speed choice could be
rooted in the self-image of drivers. Research shows that the majority
35 Merton Street, Apartment 1706, Toronto, Ontario M4s 3G4, Canada. of drivers believe that they are better than average drivers. One way
Ezra.Hauer@utoronto.ca. to confirm this self-image is by driving faster than the average speed.
If the majority of drivers try to be faster than the average, speeds must
Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board,
No. 2103, Transportation Research Board of the National Academies, Washington,
continuously increase.
D.C., 2009, pp. 10–17. A third possible mechanism might be the building of wider
DOI: 10.3141/2103-02 roads. As lanes become wider, travel becomes faster, as was shown

10
Hauer 11

85th percentile
80
Cars 75 mph
Trucks 65 mph
75
Base Rule
(Reasonable and Prudent)
70 Median Speed
MPH

55 mph 65 mph

65

60

55
1979 1981 1986 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004 2005 2006

FIGURE 1 Median and 85th percentile speeds on rural Interstates in Montana. (Source: R. Retting of the Insurance Institute for
Highway Safety.)

by Fitzpatrick et al. (1). Therefore, as wider lanes become more als do shapes the evolution of precrash speeds. Unfortunately, in spite
prevalent, the average speed increases. of generations of measurement and monitoring, the evolution of
The time profile in Figure 1 is but one anecdote and the specula- speed over time is poorly documented and the understanding of what
tion it may engender has no natural end. To be on solid ground, the drives the evolution is largely missing. This seems to be a lacuna in
evolution of speed by road type, environment, and jurisdiction must extant knowledge, a neglected field of inquiry. The following section
first be documented and then explained by systematic inquiry. reviews the more commonly explored subject: how safety depends
In addition to questions about how speed evolves over time, there on speed.
are questions about how speed evolves over space. Is it true that if
the speed changes on one kind of road, some of the change will spill
to other roads too? Is the spillover limited to nearby roads or does it HOW SPEED AFFECTS SAFETY
change norms of behavior more generally? These questions were
investigated by Casey and Lund (2, 3). The authors found that Guided by the Haddon matrix (4), it is useful to think about the
“allowing higher speeds on some highways . . . causes higher speeds speed–safety relationship in terms of the “pre-event” and the “event”
on local, connecting roads through speed adaptation.” They also phases. In the pre-event phase one asks about how the probability of
claim that allowing higher speed on some highways “may cause an accident occurring depends on speed. In the event phase one asks
higher speeds on other, unconnected and distant roads through some how speed affects the severity of the accident. A comprehensive
indirect process of speed generalization.” This latter conclusion is review of what was known in 1998 about the effect of speed in the
somewhat tenuous because the general increase in speed over time pre-event and event phases can be found in Shinar (5).
may have been caused by many factors, not only the increase in
speed limits. At present not enough is known about the spillover and
adaptation phenomena to allow one to predict the size and extent of
Pre-Event Phase: How the Probability of
the effect.
Accident Involvement Depends on Speed
As will be argued later, one cannot reasonably believe that the pre-
crash speed has no effect on the severity of crashes. If precrash speed Does one’s chance of getting into an accident increase with speed?
matters to safety, then one ought to understand how what profession- Does it depend mostly on the deviation from the mean speed? These
questions have not yet received a satisfactory answer. Shinar states,

From a very simplistic point of view it appears that as speed increases,


the time to react to emerging dangers is shortened, and the likelihood
of successfully coping with the imminent crash situation decreases.
Also, even after the driver reacts by braking, the braking distance of
the vehicle is proportional to the square of pre-braking speed. . . . But
reality is much more complicated, both theoretically and empirically.
(5, p. 231)

After reviewing the extant evidence, Shinar concludes that, “In sum-
mary, with the exception of one small study . . . , none of the obser-
vational/correlational studies that have been reviewed were able to
measure empirically or statistically control for all the potential factors
that mediate speed and crash probability” (5, p. 251). The purpose of
the discussion to follow is to explain some of the complications that
have hampered progress toward understanding.
FIGURE 2 Speed distributions before and after change in Figure 3 is from the patina-covered but still influential report by
speed limit. Solomon (6). Its most striking feature is that for speeds less than
12 Transportation Research Record 2103

FIGURE 3 Involvement rate by day and night. [Source: Figure 2 in Solomon (6).]

about 50 mph, the larger the speed of the accident-involved vehicles, rence and how much is due to increased accident severity cannot be
the less is their involvement rate. This is the opposite of what the said. To explain why, consider the invented example of Table 1.
stopping distance conjecture suggests. Only the right branch of the The story line is that at the end of the “before” period there was an
U-shaped curve could support the conjecture that the probability of increase in the speed limit, causing a shift in the speed distribution.
accident occurrence increases with speed. One can see in the unshaded numbers of Table 1 a before-to-after
In the same study Solomon shows (Figure 4) that accident severity increase in the number of reported crashes for each severity class. In
increases with speed. Thus, at least a part of the increasing tendency inventing these numbers, it was assumed that after the speed limit
of the right branch of the curve in Figure 3 reflects that the higher the change, the number of crashes did not change; only some crashes got
speed, the larger is the proportion of accidents that are reportable and more severe and this caused a transfer of 1% of the crashes to the
get reported. adjacent higher severity category. Thus, if in the before period there
How much of the increasing tendency of the right branch of the were 50,000 crashes that were not severe enough and did not get
curve in Figure 3 is due to the increased probability of accident occur- reported, then 500 would be shifted into the property damage only

FIGURE 4 Persons injured per 100 involvements and property damage per involvement.
[Source: Figure 3 in Solomon (6).]
Hauer 13

TABLE 1 Crashes Before and After Change in Speed Limit speed V on the road). The speed in the denominator is measured fairly
accurately; the speed in the numerator is less accurately determined
Not in Official because it comes from estimates by police, the crash-involved drivers,
Records In Official Records
or accident reconstruction. Therefore, even if the two true speed dis-
Unreported Property Damage Nonfatal Fatal tributions were equal (i.e., if the probability of a crash for all speeds
Crashes Only Injury Injury were the same), the distribution of estimated V in the numerator would
be wider than that in the denominator. To visualize the consequences
Before 50,000 6,000 4,000 40 of the two kinds of speeds being measured with differing accuracies,
After 49,500 6,440 4,020 80 assume that the speeds on the road are normally distributed with a
mean of 60 mph and a standard deviation of 3 mph and that the pre-
crash speeds of crash-involved vehicles have the same distribution.
Assume further that the process of precrash speed estimation for
(PDO) column in the after period. At the same time, 60 (1% of 6,000) crash-involved vehicles adds some uncertainty and therefore the esti-
of the PDO crashes would be transferred into the Nonfatal Injury col- mates of their precrash speeds are normally distributed with the same
umn and so forth The upshot is that what appears to be an increase in mean (60 mph) but a slightly larger standard deviation of 4 mph.
crash frequency could come about entirely as a result of increases in Dividing the ordinates of the wider normal probability density by the
severity and without any increase in the probability of crashes to narrower one shows that Solomon’s procedure is expected to yield a
occur. The general issue of frequency–severity indeterminacy and its curve like that in Figure 6, even though it should be a horizontal line.
repercussions is described in Hauer (7). A fuller explanation is provided by Hauer (10). That Solomon’s curve
The left branch of Figure 3 also suffers from critical shortcomings. is partly a statistical artifact was recognized and forcefully stated in
Foremost is the fact that many of the slow-vehicle accidents in 1970 by White and Nelson (11). However, in 2003 Solomon’s results
Solomon’s data involved turning vehicles. Therefore what appears were still being used as an argument against differential speed limits
to be due to low speed may be partly due to the maneuver that neces- for trucks and cars (12).
sitated it. This question was later explored in a study by the Research To explain his surprising results, Solomon showed his data in
Triangle Institute (RTI) that found that 44% of accidents and 56% another way. Instead of travel speed on the horizontal axis of Figure 3,
of accident involvements “had at least one vehicle directly involved he subtracted the mean speed, making the horizontal axis into varia-
in some turning maneuver or was directly influenced by another tion (deviation) from mean speed. This led to his interpretation that
vehicle . . . involved in some turning maneuver” (8, p. 18). The it is not speed that affects the probability of accident involvement but
impact of excluding such involvements from the analysis is shown that “the greater the variation in speed of any vehicle from the aver-
in Figure 5. age speed of all traffic, the greater its chance of being involved in an
Another problem affecting both the left and the right branches of accident” (5, p. 1). Inasmuch as this amounts to just a shift of the hor-
the U-shaped curve stems from a subtle statistical artifact. From the izontal axis, this interpretation suffers from all the shortcomings and
kind of data assembled by Solomon (6), RTI (8), and Kloeden et al. uncertainties already discussed.
(9), one would tend to find a U-shaped curve even if speed had noth- Attempts to replicate Solomon’s U curve were not successful.
ing to do with the probability of being in a crash. For example, each Munden (13) found that drivers observed once during his study did
data point in Figure 3 and Figure 5 represents the ratio: (proportion not yield a U-shaped curve. Drivers driving habitually at less than or
of vehicles with speed V in crashes)/(proportion of vehicles with more than the average speed did show a mild U-shaped curve. There

FIGURE 5 Impact of excluding involvements, based on Tables 3.1.1 and 3.1.3


from Research Triangle Institute project (8).
14 Transportation Research Record 2103

100

Source: CCIS Study 1983 - 1992

75

Probability MAIS ≥ 2
50

25

Male (n=61)
FIGURE 6 Curve expected from Solomon’s procedure (mean speed Female (n=34)
= 60 mph,  denominator = 3 mph,  numerator = 4 mph).
0

+70
0-4
5-9
10 - 14
15 - 19
20 - 24
25 - 29
30 - 34
35 - 39
40 - 44
45 - 49
50 - 54
55 - 59
60 - 64
65 - 69
are, of course, reasons why some drive habitually slowly and others
drive fast. Perhaps these are the same reasons and personal traits that Δv in km/h
make some of us cautious and others aggressive and it is these very FIGURE 7 Results of a 10-year study of crashes for restrained
reasons that are causally related to both speed choice and involve- front-seat occupants. [Source: Adapted from Mackay (15].]
ment in reported accidents. Thus, for example, old drivers who drive
slower and are more fragile may be found disproportionately on the
left branch, whereas the young who tend to drive faster and have a
high accident involvement may dominate the right branch. Fildes and to calculate the risk of injury to equal or exceed all six MAIS injury
Lee (14) also found no trace of a U-shaped relationship. levels. Thus, for example, for MAIS = 6 (unsurvivable injury), the
In summary, it is still reasonable to believe that the longer it takes probability of a passenger vehicle occupant to die is e0.1525ΔV − 8.2629/
to stop a vehicle, the larger is the probability of accident involvement. (1 + e0.1525ΔV − 8.2629). The models by Joksch and the NHTSA give sim-
However, the tortuous history of research on this subject counsels ilar results. The conclusion is inescapable; the severity of injury and
caution. There is tension between our inability to show that the higher the chance of dying increase dramatically with Δv.
the speed the larger the chance of an accident and the pivotal role that
the stopping distance plays in highway geometric design.
How Safety Changes When Mean Speed Changes

Event Phase: How Accident Severity The main question is, how does the number of accidents, by sever-
ity, depend on the distribution of speeds? For some reason most of
Depends on Speed
past research is not about this question but about (a) how the prob-
Damage to vehicle and to occupant depends on pressure, decelera- ability of accidents depends on speed and (b) how the severity of
tion, change in velocity, and the kinetic energy dissipated by defor- outcome once an accident has occurred depends on speed. The
mation. All of these are increasing functions of precrash velocity. hope was that the answer to the main question would emerge from
What is predicted by Newtonian physics is amply supported by data. answering questions a and b. However, for reasons already explained,
Figure 7 shows the results of a 10-year study of crashes for restrained there is no consensus on the answer to question a. In addition,
front-seat occupants. while it is clear that severity increases with speed, it is not easy to
In this figure injury severity on the vertical axis is represented by estimate Δv or to know how it depends on the distribution of
MAIS, the maximum Abbreviated Injury Scale (AIS) scores of six speeds; Δv depends not only on the precrash speed of one vehicle
body regions. The AIS represents the “threat to life” associated with but also on the speed and direction of the other vehicle(s) in the
an injury. The horizontal axis is Δv, “the change in velocity of a vehi- crash, on their relative masses, and on other factors. In short, based
cle’s occupant compartment during the collision phase of a motor on the information in the earlier sections, the main question of
vehicle crash” (16). Figure 7 shows that the proportion of occupants interest cannot be answered.
sustaining a moderate or more severe injury increases with Δv. Thus, Some researchers did not succumb to the analyze-first-and-
for example, in a crash in which Δv is 30 to 34 km/h, about 40% of synthesize-later habit and strove to answer the question of interest
female restrained front-seat occupants will sustain an injury for which directly. Using results from several studies in which mean speed
MAIS ≥ 2, but when Δv is in the 50 to 55 km/h range, about 75% changed from before to after, Nilsson (19) found that if the mean
sustain such injury. speed of a road changed from –v0 to –v1, the ratio of accidents (N1:N0) of
Using National Accident Sampling System data about passenger a given severity was proportional to the ratio (v–1:v–0)α with α = 4
cars involved in accidents from 1980 through 1986, Joksch (17) found for fatal accidents, α = 3 for fatal plus serious injury accidents, and
that the probability of the driver being killed in a reported motor vehi- α = 2 for all injury accidents. This “power model” was later refined
cle accident was (Δv/70.6)3.88. The NHTSA (18) provides equations in Nilsson’s doctoral dissertation (20). Elvik et al. (21) assembled
Hauer 15

α
⎛ v⎞
N = N0 ⎜ ⎟
⎝ v0 ⎠

if f(v– ) equals 1 + βv–, then

N = N 0e
(
α ( v − v0 )+β v 2 − v 20 )2

and so forth. It follows that the power model is the right choice if the
data indicate that f(v– ) equals 1/ –v . Whether this is true can be exam-
ined by plotting (1/N0)(dN/dv– ) against –v 0. No such dependence was
found, certainly not one indicating that f(v) is halved for a doubling
FIGURE 8 Change in mean speed versus
of v, as implied by the power model. The plots were completely flat
relative change in fatal accidents.
and the most that could be assumed is that f(v– ) might change linearly
with –v . For this reason, f(v– ) = 1 + βv– was chosen.
The parameters α and β were estimated by weighed least-squares
a large data set from 97 published studies containing 460 results such that the weight of each study result takes into account both the
about –v 0, –v 1, N0, and N1. Their data about fatal accidents are shown number of accidents and the method used. Their estimates for two
in Figure 8. alternative models (A and B) when d –v is measured in mph are listed
Each point in the figure is one study result. Most results are in the in Table 3. If d –v is measured in km/h, multiply α and β by 1.609.
“expected” regions. That is, when the mean speed increased, so did These parameters are to be used in differential relationships:
the number of fatal accidents and vice versa. Similar results were
obtained for injury accidents. Elvik et al. (21) also used Nilsson’s dN = αN (1 + βv ) dv (1)
power model and their estimates of α are listed in Table 2.
The power model implies, for example, that a 3-mph reduction and
from a mean speed of 30 mph will cause the same reduction in the pro-
portion of crashes as a 6-mph reduction from a mean speed of 60 mph. N + dN
Because this implication was thought to be counterintuitive by some, AMF = = 1 + α (1 + βv ) dv (2)
N
Hauer and Bonneson (22) were asked to examine whether the data
prepared and used by Elvik et al. provide empirical support for the where AMF is the accident modification function. To illustrate
power model. the application of these equations, consider the following numerical
The relationships in Figure 8 can be represented by the separable example: A measure is expected to reduce the mean speed by 1.2 mph.
differential equation The mean speed “before” was 60 mph and the number of fatal accidents
was 500. The task is to compute dN and AMF for fatal accidents.
dN
= af ( v ) dv
N 1. Compute dN. Model A predicts dN = 0.1046 × 500 × (−1.2)
= −62.8 fatal accidents. Model B predicts dN = 0.2666 × 500 ×
the solution of which depends on what f (v– ) is. Thus, if f (v– ) is (1 − 0.0098 × 60) × (−1.2) = −65.9 fatal accidents. For fatal acci-
constant, then dents the power model uses α = 3.6 from Table 2 and predicts

N = N 0 e α ( v − v0 ) ( N + dN ) = ⎛ 58.8 ⎞ 3.6 = 0.93


1 N ⎜⎝ 60 ⎟⎠
if f(v– ) equals , then
v
or dN = 500 × (0.93 − 1) = −35. The power model estimates, in this
circumstance, a substantially smaller reduction in fatal accidents.
TABLE 2 Estimates of  Elvik et al.
2. Compute AMF. Model A predicts AMF = 1 + 0.1046 × (−1.2)
Severity Estimate of α 95% Confidence Interval = 0.874. Model B predicts AMF = 1 + 0.2666 × (1 − 0.0098 × 60) ×
(−1.2) = 0.868. The power model predicts 0.93.
Fatalities 4.5 4.1–4.9
Seriously injured road users 2.4 1.6–3.2 –
TABLE 3 Estimated Parameters When dv
Slightly injured road users 1.5 1.0–2.0 Is in mph
All injured road users 1.9 1.0–2.8
(including fatally) α̂ β̂
Fatal accidents 3.6 2.4–4.8
Fatal accidents
Serious injury accidents 2.0 0.7–3.3
A 0.1046 Set to 0
Slight injury accidents 1.1 0.0–2.4 B 0.2666 −0.0098
All injury accidents 1.5 0.8–2.2 Nonfatal injury accidents
(including fatal) A 0.0670 Set to 0
PDO accidents 1.0 0.0–2.0 B 0.0838 −0.0051
16 Transportation Research Record 2103

As is obvious from Figure 8, there is considerable noise in the to severity of outcome. Third, past studies neglected to account for
data. This noise reflects in part the randomness of accident counts, the fact that the speed on the road and the speed in a crash are esti-
in part the variety of circumstances under which the data were mated to a different accuracy. The result is that the relative rate pre-
obtained, and in part the variety of causes of change in mean speed. sents itself in the form of a U-shaped curve, even when in reality it
The question of whether these results would apply regardless of the is a straight horizontal line. In short, all can still believe what they
cause of the change in mean speed cannot be well answered at this choose; at this point the answers from research are inadequate.
time. If the observed change in the number of accidents reflects The question of how severity depends on speed can be answered
mainly the associated change in severity, then the results may apply with clarity. The mechanism of damage to vehicle and occupant is
generally. well understood; it is clear that damage increases with precrash
In the numerical example, the introduction of some measure was velocity, and what physics predict is amply supported by data. As a
expected to reduce the mean speed by 1.2 mph. Although all com- result, one can show how the probability of being killed or sustain-
putations depend on this expected d –v , it was not made clear where ing an injury of a given severity in an accident increases as a func-
such an expectation originates. The most common measure by tion of the change in velocity. That is, if one can estimate Δv, the
which speed is influenced is a change in the posted speed limit. The change in velocity of the passenger compartment at the time of the
relationship between changes in posted speed limit was examined crash, it is possible to associate with it a probability of injury of any
by Elvik et al. (21). They found that, on the average, “the change in given severity.
the mean speed of traffic induced by a change in speed limit appears The hope may have been that, by establishing (a) how the proba-
to be around 25% of the change in speed limit” (21, p. 90). This bility of accident involvement depends on speed and (b) how accident
probably varies by setting. Thus, for example, in rural settings, the severity depends on speed, one could answer the question of interest:
change in mean speed was found to be, on average, 42% of the How does the number of accidents by severity depend on speed? This
change in speed limit. hope turned out to be vain for two reasons. First, the answer to ques-
tion a is not clear. Second, outcome severity depends not on speed but
on the change in speed Δv, which, in turn, depends on many factors,
SUMMARY not only the speed of the crashing vehicle. Therefore, the question of
interest had to be answered directly.
Two questions have been discussed: “How does what professionals There are many research studies about the safety effect of counter-
do affect the evolution of speeds?” and “How does speed affect measures that changed the mean speed on roads. Summarizing
safety?” That more was said about the second question than about
their results, Nilsson (19, 20) and Elvik et al. (21) suggested using
the first does not reflect their relative importance. It reflects only the
the power model. The implications of the power model are some-
amount of attention they received in research. Clearly one cannot
what counterintuitive and, on closer examination, are not in accord
anticipate how safety is affected by what we do without answering
with their data. Hauer and Bonneson used Elvik’s data to estimate
both questions.
a different model (22), one that is largely free of counterintuitive
The discussion of the first question is anecdotal and speculative
implications. The results are presented in Equations 1 and 2 and in
in tone. This paper has shown how speed has evolved over time in
Table 3.
one specific case. This allowed the author to ask, If it evolves, what
In conclusion, there can be no reasonable doubt that if speed
drives the change? The question could be answered only in gener-
increases while other conditions (vehicles, roads, medical services)
alities: perhaps it is the smoother ride, the wider lanes, more travel
on freeways, and so forth. There might even be mechanisms that remain unchanged, accidents will be more severe and therefore more
would drive an upward drift in speed. But speculation is of little use. accidents will be reported. Given a change in mean speed, one can
The extent and universality of the speed drift are not even known. predict the consequences in injuries and fatalities. The link between
What is missing is knowledge of fact. what we do (by vehicle design, advertising, speed limits, infrastruc-
Similar to the question of how speed evolves over time is the ques- ture standards, etc.) and the speeds on the road is less clear. We know
tion of how it spreads over distance (space). It has been established that speeds evolve over time but do not know exactly why. We know
that there is some speed inertia, a carryover from a faster to a slower that there is some spillover of the change from one road to another but
road. However, it is not well known how this decays with distance cannot well predict its size or extent.
and, more importantly, whether and to what extent the increase in
speed on one set of roads spills over to other, more distant roads.
Three questions were addressed more extensively: (a) How does REFERENCES
the probability of accident involvement depend on speed?; (b) How
1. Fitzpatrick, K., P. Carlson, M. Brewer, and M. Wooldridge. Design
does the severity of the outcome depend on speed?; and (c) How Factors That Affect Driver Speed on Suburban Streets. In Transporta-
does safety change when the mean speed changes? On the first ques- tion Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board,
tion there are strong beliefs supported by weak evidence. To many No. 1751, TRB, National Research Council, Washington, D.C., 2001,
it seems obvious that the faster one drives the more likely one is to pp. 18–25.
2. Casey, S. M., and A. K. Lund. Three Field Studies of Driver Speed
crash. This is why the dependence of the stopping distance on speed
Adaptation. Human Factors, Vol. 29, No. 5, 1987, pp. 541–550.
plays such an important role in geometric design. Others believe that 3. Casey, S. M., and A. K. Lund. Changes in Speed and Speed Adaptation
the likelihood of a crash has more to do with deviation from mean Following the Increase in National Maximum Speed Limit. Journal of
speed. The paper explains why what seems obvious is not easy to Safety Research, Vol. 23, 1992, pp. 135–146.
demonstrate. First, the early research did not distinguish between 4. Haddon, W. A Logical Framework for Categorizing Highway Safety
Phenomena and Activity. Journal of Trauma, Vol. 12, 1972, pp. 193–207.
slow and turning vehicles. Second, from the kind of data used one 5. Shinar, D. Speed and Crashes: A Controversial Topic and an Elusive
cannot possibly know whether the observed differences in relative Relationship. Appendix B. In Special Report 254: Managing Speed,
accident rate are due to probability of accident involvement or due TRB, National Research Council, Washington D.C., 1998, pp. 221–276.
Hauer 17

6. Solomon, D. Accidents on Main Rural Highways Related to Speed, and Occupant Protection (J. A. C. Ambrosio, M. F. O. S. Pereira, and
Driver, and Vehicle. FHWA, U.S. Department of Transportation, 1964. F. P. da Silva, eds.), Kluwer Academic Publishers, Norwell, Mass., 1997.
7. Hauer, E. The Frequency-Severity Indeterminacy. Accident Analysis 16. Day, T. D., and R. L. Hargens. Differences Between EDCRASH and
and Prevention, Vol. 38, 2006, pp. 78–83. CRASH3. SAE Paper 850253, Feb. 1985.
8. Research Triangle Institute. Speed and Accidents, Vol. II. RTI Project 17. Joksch, H. C. Velocity Change and Fatality Risk in a Crash—A Rule
SU-409. National Highway Safety Bureau, June 1970. of Thumb. Accident Analysis and Prevention, Vol. 25, No. 1, 1993,
9. Kloeden, C. N., G. Ponte, and A. J. McLean. Report CR 204: Traveling pp. 103–104.
Speed and the Risk of Crash Involvement on Rural Roads. Road Accident 18. Tire Pressure Monitoring System. FMVSS 138. Final Regulatory
Research Unit, Adelaide University, July 2001. Impact Analysis. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration,
10. Hauer, E. Speed and Crash Risk: An Opinion. Report 04/02. Public Washington, D.C., March 2005.
Policy Department, Royal Automobile Club of Victoria, May 2004. 19. Nilsson, G. Hastigheter, Olycksrisker och Personskadekonsekvenser I
11. White, S. B., and A. C. Nelson, Jr. Some Effects of Measurement Olika Vägmiljöer. VTIrapport 277. Väg och Trafic Institutet, Linköping,
Errors in Estimating Involvement Rate as a Function of Deviation from Sweden, 1984.
Mean Traffic Speed. Journal of Safety Research, Vol. 2, No. 2, June 20. Nilsson, G. Traffic Safety Dimensions and the Power Model to Describe
1970, pp. 67–72. the Effect of Speed in Safety. Bulletin 221, Lund Institute of Technol-
12. Julie Anna Cirillo, Former Assistant Administrator and Chief Safety ogy, Department of Technology and Society, Traffic Engineering, Lund,
Officer for the Federal Motor Carrier Safety Administration. Testimony Sweden, 2004.
Before the Senate Highways and Transportation Committee, Senate Bill 21. Elvik, R., P. Christensen, and A. Amundsen. Speed and Road Accidents.
94, June 10, 2003. TØI Report 740. Institute of Transport Economics, Oslo, Norway, 2004.
13. Munden, J. W. The Relation Between Driver’s Speed and His Accident 22. Hauer, E., and J. Bonneson. An Empirical Examination of the Relation-
Rate. Report LR 88. Road Research Laboratory, Crowthorne, England, ship Between Speed and Road Accidents Based on Data by Elvik,
1967. Christensen and Amundsen. Report prepared for Project NCHRP 17-25,
14. Fildes, B., and S. Lee. The Speed Review: Road Environment, Behaviour, Sept. 20, 2006.
Speed Limits, Enforcement and Crashes. Monash University Accident
Research Centre, Sept. 1993.
15. Mackay, M. A Review of the Biomechanics of Impacts in Road Acci- The Safety Data, Analysis, and Evaluation Committee sponsored publication of
dents. In Crashworthiness of Transportation Systems: Structural Impact this paper.

You might also like