Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Received: 22 November 2019 | Accepted: 29 March 2020

DOI: 10.1002/nur.22023

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Sleep health in young children living with socioeconomic


adversity

Monica R. Ordway1 | Lois S. Sadler2 | Sangchoon Jeon1 | Meghan O'Connell1 |


Nancy Banasiak1 | Ada M. Fenick3 | Angela A. Crowley1 | Craig Canapari3 |
Nancy S. Redeker1

1
Yale School of Nursing, West Haven,
Connecticut Abstract
2
Yale Child Study Center, Yale University
Racially and ethnically diverse young children who live with socioeconomic adversity
School of Nursing, West Haven, Connecticut
3
Department of Pediatrics, Yale School of are at high risk for sleep deficiency, but few behavioral sleep interventions (BSIs) are
Medicine, New Haven, Connecticut tailored to their needs. To support the future development of a feasible, acceptable,
Correspondence and culturally relevant sleep intervention, we conducted a community‐engaged,
Monica R. Ordway, Yale School of Nursing, mixed‐methods study with 40 low‐income, racially, and ethnically diverse parents to
West Haven 06516, CT.
Email: Monica.ordway@yale.edu and describe sleep characteristics, sleep habits, and parental sleep knowledge of their
monica.ordway@gmail.com 6–36‐month‐old children and to examine the associations between children's sleep

Funding information
characteristics and sleep habits. This report presents quantitative data from this
National Institute of Nursing Research, mixed‐methods study. We measured objective (actigraphy) and parent‐reported
Grant/Award Numbers: 5K23NR016277‐03,
P20NR014126, R21NR01690
sleep (Brief Infant Sleep Questionnaire) characteristics, sleep habits at bedtime,
sleep onset, and during night awakenings, parental sleep knowledge, psychological
function (Brief Symptom Inventory), and parenting stress (Parenting Stress Index).
Children had low sleep duration (537.2 ± 54.7 nighttime and 111.2 ± 29.8 nap
minutes), late bedtimes (22:36 ± 1.5 hr), and high bedtime variability (mean squared
successive difference = 3.68 ± 4.31 hr) based on actigraphy. Parental knowledge
about sleep recommendations was limited. Sleep habits before bedtime, at sleep
onset, and during night awakenings were varied. Sixty‐five percent of parents
reported co‐sleeping. Feeding near bedtime or during the night was associated with
later bedtimes, more fragmented sleep, and increased bedtime variability. These
findings suggest the need for BSIs to support earlier bedtimes and improve sleep
duration and continuity by addressing modifiable behaviors. Tailored BSIs that
consider socioecological influences on the development of sleep habits are needed.

KEYWORDS

actigraphy, community‐engaged research, health disparities, infants, sleep, toddlers

1 | INTRODUCTION performance, memory consolidation, and learning (Diekelmann &


Born, 2010; Walker & Stickgold, 2006; Xie et al., 2013). Yet, sleep
Sleep health and good sleep habits begin early in life (National deficiency, including insufficient sleep duration (Combs, Goodwin,
Scientific Council on the Developing Child & National Forum Quan, Morgan, & Parthasarathy, 2016; Koulouglioti et al., 2014;
on Early Childhood Policy and Programs, 2010) and are essential for Magee, Gordon, & Caputi, 2014), irregular bedtimes (Kelly, Kelly, &
children's wellbeing, growth and development, healthy metabolism, Sacker, 2013; Staples, Bates, & Petersen, 2015), and poor sleep

Res Nurs Health. 2020;1–12. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/nur © 2020 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. | 1


2 | ORDWAY ET AL.

quality (Gruber et al., 2014; Soffer‐Dudek, Sadeh, Dahl, & 2 | METHO DS


Rosenblat‐Stein, 2011) are common in young children. The occur-
rence of sleep problems in >25–40% of children by the preschool 2.1 | Design
years (2.5–5 years) suggests the importance of modifying con-
tributing behaviors early in life, while they are still modifiable We conducted a cross‐sectional convergent mixed methods study
(Byars, Yolton, Rausch, Lanphear, & Beebe, 2012; Mindell, (Redeker et al., 2018), framed by the socioecological model, to under-
Leichman, Puzino, Walters, & Bhullar, 2015; Owens & Jones, 2011; stand sleep within the layers of parenting, family, household, neigh-
Owens, Jones, & Nash, 2011). borhood, and cultural factors that may influence sleep in early
Racially and ethnically diverse families who live with socio- childhood, parent sleep behaviors, and parent knowledge about child
economic adversity are at especially high risk for sleep deficiency sleep (Owens & Ordway, 2019). The overall purpose of the mixed
and its negative consequences (El‐Sheikh et al., 2013; Guglielmo, methods study was to obtain qualitative and quantitative data to assess
Gazmararian, Chung, Rogers, & Hale, 2018; Smith, Hardy, Hale, & the need for and determine the focus of a possible future sleep health
Gazmararian, 2019). The reasons for this are multifactorial but intervention designed specifically for families living with socioeconomic
may include individual, family, home, neighborhood, cultural, and adversity. We previously described the protocol in‐depth, including a
societal factors, as characterized by the social ecological model detailed overview of the community‐engaged research approach we
(Bronfenbrenner, 1979, 1986; Grandner, Williams, Knutson, used to partner with community stakeholders and a community ad-
Roberts, & Jean‐Louis, 2016; Owens & Ordway, 2019). Family visory committee who helped to guide the design of the study, interview
members' behaviors and sleep habits (e.g., co‐sleeping; Caldwell, questions, and interpretation of data (Redeker et al., 2018)
Ordway, Sadler, & Redeker, 2019; Sadler et al., in press), parental (NCT03045874). The University Human Subjects Protection and pe-
knowledge about children's sleep, parental psychosocial func- diatric research committees and the research committee in the clinical
tioning (e.g., depression, anxiety, parenting stress; Caldwell & setting approved the study. All parents provided written informed
Redeker, 2015), and living in crowded or unsafe environments all consent and permission for their children's participation.
contribute to sleep habits among children and their parents
(Caldwell et al., 2019; Martinez & Thompson‐Lastad, 2015; Yip
et al., 2019). These behavioral and socioecological factors may also 2.2 | Setting and sample
interact with young children's sleep to influence the development
of future health problems that may be associated with health We conducted the study in a U.S. Northeastern city that has over
disparities (Doane et al., 2019). 130,000 residents who are racially and ethnically diverse and where
There is a critical need for effective strategies to address 26% of families with children under age 18 years living in poverty (US
sleep health in young children. While behavioral interventions for Census Bureau, 2019). We recruited families who received care at
young children are available and efficacious (Jones, Owens, & the local academic medical center's comprehensive pediatric primary
Pham, 2013; Mindell et al., 2011b; Paul et al., 2016), few have care center that serves >7,000 families living in the city and sur-
addressed the family, social, or ecological contexts of children rounding area. This center serves 40% Hispanic, 50% Black, 5% Asian
who live with socioeconomic adversity, many of whom are people, and 5% of people of other races/ethnicities. The primary care
members of multiethnic families (Schwichtenberg, Abel, Keys, & center predominantly serves families from lower socioeconomic
Honaker, 2019). To be culturally relevant, these interventions backgrounds who receive Medicaid health insurance and the Women,
must consider diverse child‐rearing traditions, expectations of Infants, and Children nutritional support program.
children's and parent's behaviors, and distinctive family stressors We previously reported eligibility criteria (Redeker
and resources (Lau, 2006). et al., 2018; Sadler et al., in press). In brief, we included English and
We conducted a community‐engaged mixed methods study Spanish‐speaking families with children between the ages of 6–36
to support the future development of a feasible, acceptable, and months. Families with children with developmental delays or ser-
culturally relevant sleep intervention for young children (Redeker ious medical conditions and children in the custody of child pro-
et al., 2018; Sadler et al., in press) (NCT03045874), and to learn tective services were excluded. We used stratified purposive
about sleep from parents who were raising infants and toddlers in a sampling to assure ethnic diversity and representation of families
racially and ethnically diverse, low‐income urban community. We with 6–18‐month and 19–36‐month‐old children. We included
previously reported our qualitative findings (Sadler et al., in press) these age ranges because sleep habits begin early in life and this is
and report the quantitative findings from that study in this paper. a formative time (Tierney & Nelson, 2009).
The purposes were to examine (a) parent‐reported and actigraph‐ We determined the need for a sample size of 40 based on the
measured characteristics of sleep and sleep habits; (b) parent mixed methods design in the larger study and estimated that 40
knowledge about child sleep; (c) associations among parents' participants would be sufficient to reach thematic saturation for
psychosocial health, knowledge about sleep, and child sleep char- the qualitative strand (Redeker et al., 2018). This sample size was
acteristics; and (d) associations between child sleep behaviors and adequate for quantitative descriptive purposes and to address the
parent‐reported and actigraphy‐measured sleep characteristics. goal of obtaining foundational information to support a possible
ORDWAY ET AL. | 3

future intervention within this community. Therefore, we did not approaches). The BIS‐Extended is valid for children ages 0–36 months
conduct a power analysis. and has a grade 5 reading level (Sadeh, 2004; Sadeh, Mindell, Luedtke, &
Wiegand, 2009) The items are correlated with a daily sleep diary and
caregiver reports of sleep. It has test–retest reliability of 0.82–0.95 for
2.3 | Procedures individual items (Sadeh, 2004).

Pediatric health care providers at the health care center screened


medical records to identify potentially eligible children between 6 and 36 2.4.3 | Actigraphy‐measured child sleep
months of age who were scheduled for routine clinic visits. Trained re-
search assistants (RAs) met families in the clinic to explain the study, We used the Actiwatch 2 (Philips Respironics, Murrysville, PA) to
obtain informed consent, and schedule the first study visit. Two 1‐hr measure sleep characteristics. Actigraph measures of sleep are valid
visits were held at the parents' convenience in their homes. During the and reliable beginning in infancy (see review Meltzer, Montgomery‐
first visit, we instructed parents on the use of the actigraph and ac- Downs, Insana, & Walsh, 2012). Reliability estimates for age 12
companying sleep diaries. We asked them to complete two parent‐report months range from 0.77 to 0.89 for sleep efficiency and 0.70–0.85
sleep questionnaires and participated in interviews for the qualitative for sleep duration based on 3–7 nights of data (Acebo et al., 1999).
strand of the study (Sadler et al., in press). After 7 days and nights during We used the medium sensitivity setting and a wake threshold value
which the children wore the actigraphs, the RAs returned to the families' of 40 activity counts, and immobile minutes for sleep equal to 10 min to
homes to retrieve the actigraphs, review the sleep diaries, and ask the evaluate the data that were collected in 30‐s epochs, consistent with
parents to complete two questionnaires on parental psychosocial health. the literature (Sadeh, Sharkey, & Carskadon, 1994). We used event
Parents received $25 at each of the two data collection visits. marker recordings and parent‐recorded sleep diary entries to indicate
Because of our interest in the sleep of children from diverse nap time onset and offset in preparing the data for analysis.
English and Spanish‐speaking families who often are not included in We requested that children wear the actigraph for 7 days to assure
research on behavioral sleep interventions, we used questionnaires that we would obtain at least 3 days of data (Acebo et al., 1999) and
available in Spanish (Brief Symptom Inventory [BSI] and Parenting instructed parents to keep the actigraph on their children for the entire
Stress Index [PSI], see descriptions below). Professional translators 7 days, including bathing unless the child was going to swim in water
translated the consent forms and surveys that were not available in over one meter in depth. To address safety recommendations (Bélanger,
Spanish with a translation‐back translation approach. A Spanish‐ Bernier, Paquet, Simard, & Carrier, 2013), the children wore the acti-
speaking RA collected data from Spanish‐speaking families and spoke graph on their ankle. We covered the black actigraph bands with col-
in their language of choice. orful child‐themed duct tape to make them more appealing to the child
and family. We instructed parents to press the actigraph event markers
at sleep and wake times, including nap times. Parents recorded bedtime,
2.4 | Variables and measures wake times, naps, and bed‐sharing in a sleep diary to assist with scoring
the actigraph data, consistent with standard actigraph protocols
2.4.1 | Demographic and health information (Bélanger et al., 2013). We sent text messages or called parents every 2
days to remind them to keep the actigraphs on the children.
We collected parent demographic and health information, including
age, gender, race and ethnicity, education, employment, shiftwork,
marital status, number of people in the household, type of dwelling 2.4.4 | Parent knowledge about sleep
(house, apartment, etc.), and type of health insurance. Health in-
surance (Medicaid, private, or no insurance) was used as a proxy for We measured parent knowledge and beliefs about sleep with the
income level. Connecticut's eligibility standards to qualify for Medi- 10‐item Owens and Jones questionnaire developed by experts with
caid insurance is 196 percent of the federal poverty level for children clinical experience with children's sleep (Owens et al., 2011). Each of
ages 0–18 years (Norris, 2018). Parents also reported their child's the 10 questions is a yes/no question and a total score between
age, gender, race and ethnicity, and their childcare arrangements. 0 and 10 is calculated, with 10 indicating the highest level of sleep
knowledge. While there are no reliability statistics published, this
questionnaire is widely used in pediatric sleep research (McDowall,
2.4.2 | Parent‐reported child sleep Galland, Campbell, & Elder, 2017).

We used the 26‐item Brief Infant Sleep Questionnaire (BISQ)‐Extended


(Sadeh, 2004), to elicit parent‐reported child sleep characteristics 2.4.5 | Parental psychosocial health
(bedtime, nighttime sleep duration, minutes wake after sleep onset
[WASO], nap duration, and total 24‐hr sleep duration) and sleep habits The BSI (Derogatis, 1993) is an 18‐item symptom inventory that
(bedtime routine activities, sleep onset behaviors, and night awakening measures parents' psychological distress. It has three subscales
4 | ORDWAY ET AL.

(depression, somatization, and anxiety symptoms) and contains a 3 | RE SU LTS


Global Severity Index that reflects symptoms across domains.
The BSI has good internal reliability showing an average rating 3.1 | Sample description
above 0.70 for each of the subscales and a test–retest reliability
range of 0.68–0.91 (Derogatis, 1993). The sample included 40 racially and ethnically diverse children between
The 36‐item Parenting Stress Index‐Short Form (PSI‐SF; the ages of 6–36 months and their parents (18 boys and 22 girls; see
Abdin, 1995) was used to measure the magnitude of stress in Table 1). All but one of the parents was the child's mother. All of the
the parent–child system. There are four subscales in the PSI‐SF: children were recruited from the primary care center, and 90% reported
Parent Distress, Parent–Child Difficulty, Difficult Child, and having Medicaid insurance or no insurance. We obtained at least three
Total Stress. The PSI‐SF subscales have concurrent validity nights of actigraphy data from 28 children (15 boys, 13 girls; M age =
with the full‐length PSI. Alpha coefficients ranged from .88 to .95 19.11 months, standard deviation [SD] = 8.54, range = 6–35 months). Six
across the subscales in similar samples (Reitman, Currier, & children did not wear the actigraphs (two parents refused and four
Stickle, 2002). parents never put the actigraph on the children), four actigraphs did not
collect data (actigraph error), and two actigraphs had fewer than 3 days/
nights of data and were excluded from the analyses. There were no
2.5 | Statistical analysis differences between the groups with and without actigraph data on age,
race, gender, parent knowledge of healthy sleep, or parent‐reported
We scored the actigraph data with Actiware v6 software sleep characteristics (sleep duration, regular bedtime).
(Philips Respironics Mini‐Mitter Inc.). For each day, we computed
bedtime and wake up time (time of sleep offset) for
the nighttime sleep period to compute time in bed. We used 3.2 | Sleep characteristics
custom intervals confirmed by review of the parent‐recorded
sleep diaries to determine the onset and offset of nap intervals Data on the actigraph‐measured and parent‐reported sleep character-
and computed nap duration and number of naps. As the istics are presented in Table 2. There was a difference between parent‐
number of nights available for analysis varied across individuals reported and actigraph‐measured bedtime (W = 175.5; p < .01) and
(ranging between 3 and 9 nights; mean = 7.0 ± 1.6 nights), WASO (W = 149; p < .01), with parents reporting earlier bedtimes and
weighted means were calculated for each child and used for the fewer WASO minutes than recorded by the actigraphs. Parent‐reported
overall group means. bedtime was 9 p.m. Actigraph‐measured bedtime was 10:30 p.m.
We calculated night‐to‐night variability in bedtimes and night- Parent‐reported WASO was a half hour. Actigraph‐measured WASO
time sleep duration by creating a series of successive differences was an hour and a quarter. There were no differences between parent‐
from the differences calculated between nights within the same reported and actigraph‐measured sleep duration. Nighttime sleep
toddler (e.g., Night 4 − Night 3, Night 3 − Night 2, etc.) and then duration was 8–9 hr, depending on parent or actigraph source, re-
squaring them to account for potential negative values. These spectively. Daytime naps were about 2 hr total. Thus, total sleep daily
squared successive differences were used to compute a mean, an was 10.2 hr (actigraphy) to 10.8 hr (parent‐reported).
index of variability/instability that is more sensitive to fluctuations The parent‐reported data on children's sleep (BISQ) are presented
across successive observations (Jahng, Wood, & Trull, 2008; Meltzer, in Table 3. Sleep onset latency (time to fall asleep) was 30 min or less for
Sanchez‐Ortuno, Edinger, & Avis, 2015). most children (n = 33; 82.5%). Slightly more than half (n = 21; 52.5%)
We computed descriptive statistics for the demographic, reported that their child had consistent bedtimes at least 5 nights a
sleep, parental sleep knowledge, and psychosocial variables and week. Twenty‐six (65%) children woke up at least one night per week,
evaluated these data for missing data and skewness. Nonpara- with eleven (27.5%) waking every night. The average number of awa-
metric tests (Wilcoxon rank‐sum and Kruskal–Wallis test) were kenings at night was 1.33 ± 1.02. None of the parent‐reported sleep
used to compare the children's sleep characteristics and parents' characteristics differed between infants and toddlers. Overall, 12 par-
sleep knowledge by categorical demographic characteristics of the ents (30%) perceived that their children had sleep problems.
children and parents. Pearson coefficients (r) were used to ex-
amine the associations between continuous measures of sleep
characteristics and sleep knowledge, and point‐biserial coefficient 3.3 | Sleep habits
(rpb) was used to examine the association between those con-
tinuous measures and binary items of BISQ for sleep behaviors. 3.3.1 | Bedtime routine activities
We also computed parametric and nonparametric bivariate cor-
relations to examine associations between clinical and demo- Parents reported that their children were engaged in an average of 5.8
graphic characteristics of the children and parents, parent (SD = 2.3) activities during the hour before bed, including bathing (n = 30;
psychosocial distress, actigraph and parent‐reported sleep char- 75%), drinking fluids (n = 28; 70%), playing (n = 27; 67.5%), watching TV
acteristics, and sleep habits and knowledge. (n = 23; 57.5%), reading (n = 19; 47.5%), and brushing teeth (n = 18; 45%).
ORDWAY ET AL. | 5

T A B L E 1 Demographic characteristics of parents and children and


parent psychosocial health characteristics

(69.0)
(186)
(SD)
Total 24‐hr sleep
Full sample (N = 40)

Mean/N (SD)/%

Children

Mean

613.5
648
Age (months) 18.4 (8.7)
Sex (Male) 18 45%
Race/ethnicity

(76.7)

(29.8)
Non‐Hispanic white 12 27.5%

Nap duration (min)

(SD)
Black 11 25%
Asian 3 7.5%
Multiracial 7 17.5%

Mean

134.4

111.2
Other/unknown 7 17.5%

Abbreviations: BISQ, Brief Infant Sleep Questionnaire; MSSD, mean squared successive differences; SD, standard deviation; WASO, wake after sleep onset.
Hispanic 22 55%
Attend childcare center 10 25%

Parents

Nighttime sleep duration (min)


Parent age (years) 28.8 (5.6)

(173.2)
Parent sex (male) 1 2.5%

(54.7)
(SD)
Education (years) 12.5 (3.3)
Race/ethnicity
Non‐Hispanic white 9 22.5%
Black 11 27.5%
Asian 3 7.5%
Mean

499.0

537.2
Multiracial 4 10%
Other/unknown 13 32.5%
Hispanic 18 45%
(45.57)

(25.3)
Marital status
(SD)
WASO (min)*

Single 28 70%
Married 12 30%
Mean

Number living in home 4.1 (1.8)


74.7
28

Dwelling
Apartment 31 77.5%
House 7 17.5%
(SD)
T A B L E 2 Parent‐reported and actigraph‐measured sleep characteristics

Other 2 5%
(4.31)

Employed 17 42.5%
MSSD bedtime

Full‐time 9 22.5%
Part‐time 8 20%
Mean

Shiftwork 10 25%
3.68
NA

Unemployed 23 57.5%
Health insurance
(1.1 hr)

(1.5 hr)

Medicaid 30 75%
(SD)

Private 4 10%
Bedtime*

None 6 15%
21:06

22:36
Mean

Global Severity Index 43.5 (10.9)


Parenting Stress Index‐Total Stress 69.2 (20.9)

Abbreviation: SD, standard deviation.


Parent‐reported (BISQ) N = 40

Actigraph‐measured N = 28

3.3.2 | Sleep onset behaviors

Parents reported that their children most commonly fall asleep “in
his/her own crib/bed and with a parent in the room” (30%), “in his/
her own crib/bed alone in the room” (40%), “in the parents' bed with
*p < .01.

a parent” (40%), and “in the parents' bed alone” (10%; parents could
select more than one option). Children also fell asleep while bottle
6 | ORDWAY ET AL.

T A B L E 3 Parent‐reported sleep habits (BISQ): Bedtime routine, TABLE 3 (Continued)


sleep onset, nighttime awakening, and sleep characteristics
Full sample (N = 40)
Full sample (N = 40)
Sleep latency >30 min 33 (82.5%)
Bedtime routine: Which of the following usually N (%) Sleeps through the night 10 (25%)
occurs on most nights for your child in the
Wakes up at least one night per week 26 (65%)
hour before bedtime? (Check all that apply)
Wakes up every night 11 (27.5%)
Bath 30 (75)
Number of night awakening 1.33 (1.2)
Massage 4 (10)
Child's sleep considered a problem by 12 (30%)
Read books/being read to 19 (47.5)
parent
Watch TV 23 (57.5)
Child sleeps poorly 2 (5%)
Have dinner or a snack 26 (65)
Number of parent–child bedtime 5.8 (2.3)
Have a bottle, nurse, drink 28 (70) interactions
Run around 11 (27.5) Excessive bedtime interactions (>5 20 (50%)
Brush teeth 18 (45) activities)
Play 27 (67.5)
Sleep location
Cuddle 19 (47.5)
Bed share with parents 11 (27.5%)
Say prayers 8 (20)
Room share with parents 15 (37.5%)
Sing songs 9 (22.5)
Independent sleep 14 (35.0%)
Listen to music 9 (22.5)
How many nights child has the same bedtime
Other 1 (2.5)
Never 5 (12.5%)
Sleep onset: How does your child fall asleep N (%) 1–2 nights per week 3 (7.5%)
most of the time? (Check all that apply)
3–4 nights per week 11 (27.5%)
Bottle‐fed 11 (27.5)
5–6 nights per week 9 (22.5%)
Breast‐fed 3 (7.5)
Every night 12 (30.0%)
In crib/bed alone 16 (40)
Abbreviations: BISQ, Brief Infant Sleep Questionnaire; SD, standard
In parent's bed alone 4 (10)
deviation.
Being rocked 10 (25)
In crib with parent present 1 (12)
Being held 9 (22.5) feeding (n = 1; 27.5%), being rocked to sleep (n = 10; 25%), being held
In parent's bed with parent present 16 (40)
(n = 9; 22.5%), and watching TV (n = 12; 30%).
Watching TV 12 (30)
In another room 2 (5)
In a swing 3 (7.5)
3.3.3 | Approaches to night awakenings
Other 3 (7.5)

Night awakening: When your child wakes up N (%)


Twenty‐three (57.5%) parents reported that they offered a feeding to get
during the night, what do you do? (Check
their child back to sleep when awakened at night. Most parents attended
all that apply)
Picked up and rocked 12 (30) to their children as soon as they woke up at night; some parents (n = 17;
Put down awake 6 (15) 42.5%) reported that they waited a few minutes to see if their children
Pat but not picked up 10 (25) would go back to sleep unattended. Sixty percent (n = 24) reported
Bottle‐fed 19 (47.5) changing their child's diapers, 12 (30%) picked their child up and rocked
Breast‐fed 4 (10) back to sleep, and 6 (5%) patted them on the back but did not pick them
Pacifier 6 (15) up. Fewer than 15% of parents reported the other options for activities
Change diaper 24 (60) asked on the BISQ (e.g., offer a pacifier, sing, play) during nighttime
Comfort verbally 6 (15) awakenings. Four (10%) parents reported that their children were diffi-
Bring to parent's bed 5 (12.5) cult to put to bed. Twenty‐six (65%) parents reported co‐sleeping. Eleven
Let cry 4 (10) (28%) shared beds with their children, and fifteen (38%) slept in the same
Give a few minutes to fall back to sleep 17 (42.5)
room as their children. Fourteen (35%) children slept in separate rooms
Play with child 4 (10)
from their parents.
Watch TV with child 3 (7.5)
Sing to child 4 (10)
Other 2 (5)
3.4 | Knowledge about sleep
Sleep characteristics N/Mean (%/SD)
Child snores 25 (62.5%)
Overall, parents scored <50% (48.9 ± 16.2%) on the Sleep Knowledge
Child is difficult to put to sleep 4 (10%)
Questionnaire. Only about half answered correctly on children's
ORDWAY ET AL. | 7

normal sleep duration, and less than a fifth responded correctly on measured sleep characteristics. Being bottle‐fed to fall asleep was
the need for consistent bedtimes and sleep routines (Table 4). The associated with later bedtimes (rpb = .51; p = .006) and breastfeeding
sleep knowledge scores were not associated with self‐reported or was associated with more variability in bedtime (rpb = .54; p = .003).
actigraph‐recorded sleep characteristics. Parents with higher sleep Children who fell asleep in their parents' beds with a parent in the
knowledge were less likely to report that their children watch TV room had more fragmented sleep (rpb = .41; p = .03), but earlier
before bedtime (rpb = −.32; p = .04), but sleep knowledge was not median bedtimes (rpb = .40; p = .04) than other children. Rocking
associated with any other child's sleep habits. children to sleep was associated with more WASO according to
parent report (rpb = .51; p < .001), but higher total sleepover 24 hr as
measured by actigraphy (rpb = .40; p = .03).
3.5 | Associations between sleep habits and sleep
characteristics
3.5.3 | Night awakening approaches and sleep
3.5.1 | Bedtime routine activities and sleep characteristics
characteristics
When children woke up at night, bottle‐fed children had more
Having something to drink before bed was associated with later fragmented sleep (rpb = .48; p = .01) and breast‐fed children had more
bedtimes (rpb = −.40; p = .04), lower sleep efficiency (rpb = −.51; variable bedtimes (rpb = .41; p = .03). Children left to cry when they
p = .005) and more fragmented sleep (rpb = .44; p = .02) as measured woke at night had lower nighttime (rpb = −.54; p = .003) and 24‐hr
by actigraphy. Children who cuddled in the hour before bedtime (rpb = −.45; p = .01) sleep durations. Children who had diaper changes
(rpb = −.40; p = .04) had less bedtime variability and longer total if they woke at night had lower sleep efficiency (rpb = −.40; p = .03),
nighttime sleep (rpb = .33; p = .03). Listening to music was associated longer nap duration (rpb = .50; p < .01) and shorter parent‐reported
with more variability in bedtimes (rpb = .41; p = .03) as measured longest period of sleep (rpb = −.41; p = .008). Sleep location (bed‐
by actigraphy. Watching TV in the hour before bedtime was asso- sharing, rooming‐in, independent sleeping) was not significantly as-
ciated with poorer sleep quality (“how well your child usually sleeps sociated with any of the parent‐reported or actigraph‐measured
at night”; rpb = .40; p = .01) and parent‐reported sleep problems sleep characteristics.
(rpb = −.34; p = .03).

3.6 | Parent psychosocial function and children's


3.5.2 | Sleep onset behaviors and sleep sleep, sleep habits, and sleep knowledge
characteristics
We evaluated the associations between parents' self‐reported psy-
The types of methods parents reported helping their children fall chosocial functioning and children's sleep characteristics. Total
asleep were associated with both parent‐reported and actigraph‐ parenting stress was moderately associated with actigraph‐recorded

T A B L E 4 Parental Knowledge of Sleep Questionnaire responses


Full sample (N = 40)

Parent Knowledge of Sleep Questionnaire items Correct answers (N) (%)

Children who do not get enough sleep are more likely to be underweight than overweight 10 (25)

Snoring in a child indicates that he or she is sleeping well 9 (22.5)

Being under‐ or overactive can both be warning signs that a child is not getting enough sleep 12 (30)

Watching TV in their bedroom makes it more difficult for children to fall asleep 9 (22.5)

Children should have the same bedtime and waketime on weekdays and weekends 8 (20)

Children only need a bedtime routine if they are having trouble falling asleep 6 (15)

Well‐rested children do not need an alarm clock to wake up 19 (47.5)

The average preschooler (3–5 years) needs about 10 hr of sleep/24 hr 21 (52.5)

Being overweight can increase a child's risk of sleep problems 3 (7.5)

The average school‐aged child (6–12 years) needs about 8 hr of sleep 29 (72.5)

Total number of correct answers (mean (SD)) 4.9 (1.6)

Abbreviation: SD, standard deviation.


8 | ORDWAY ET AL.

variability in bedtime (r = .38; p = .04). Parent anxiety was negatively Sleep habits and behaviors can be organized into three distinct
associated with consistency in the bedtime routine (r = −.33; p = .04), categories based on their timing relative to the sleep interval
but there were no statistically significant associations between (bedtime/the hour before bedtime; sleep onset; and the time after
parental distress, difficult child, parents' report of depression or sleep onset) and their potential influence on bedtime, sleep onset,
somatization and children's sleep characteristics. and sleep continuity (Hager et al., 2016). Behaviors related to each of
these time frames are discussed below.
The most common parent‐reported bedtime routine activities
4 | D I S C U S SI O N were bathing, drinking milk or juice, playing, watching TV, reading,
and brushing teeth. Some behaviors are likely to be adaptive (e.g.,
This study adds new information to explain sleep and sleep habits brushing teeth, praying, reading a story), while others may be ma-
among multiethnic infants and toddlers living in socioeconomically dis- ladaptive or arousing (e.g., watching TV, engaging in play). However,
advantaged homes and confirms the presence of sleep deficiency in these specific behaviors may be less important than the ways in which
children. The children had shorter sleep durations than clinical re- parents cluster prebedtime activities (Henderson & Jordan, 2010).
commendations (a minimum of 11–12 hr per 24 hr; Paruthi et al., 2016). Identifying and choosing adaptive activities from the broad domains
Bedtimes were much later than recommended (Mindell, Meltzer, Cars- of nutrition, hygiene, communication, and physical contact that last
kadon, & Chervin, 2009) and were more variable (Paruthi et al., 2016). no more than 30 min may help to improve bedtime routines (Mindell
Parents had limited knowledge about sleep health recommendations and & Williamson, 2018). These activities may be tailored to specific
interacted with their children in a variety of ways during the time before cultural or social practices (e.g., prayer in families with religious
and at bedtime and during the night. Some of these behaviors may be traditions or healthy snacks of specific cultural foods). The strate-
adaptive; others may have a negative impact on sleep. gies could also be tailored for families that experience high levels of
Our finding that close to half of the children did not have regular psychosocial and/or economic adversity and use different strategies
bedtime routines is consistent with previous reports among low‐ (Cronin, Halligan, & Murray, 2008; Sheridan et al., 2013). Best
income families. (Hale, Berger, Lebourgeois, & Brooks‐Gunn, 2009; approaches to BSIs for these families should likely be flexible to
Henderson & Jordan, 2010; Yoo, Slack, & Holl, 2010). Sleep accommodate family schedules (e.g., parent work schedules), en-
deficiency may result from increased household stress, less struc- vironments (crowded and noisy living space and neighborhoods),
tured routines, and/or lack of awareness of the benefits of children's and cultural traditions, while promoting the importance of bedtime
bedtime routines, as evident in the literature (e.g., Hale et al., 2009), routines. For example, suggestions for simple types of bedtime
and in the previously reported qualitative findings collected from activities that consider the families’ traditions and resources (i.e.,
these parents (Sadler et al., in press). Although some of these social access to specific kinds of books, parental literacy level, availability
and environmental influences may not be readily modifiable (e.g., of pajamas) with instructions to choose four to five activities for the
mother's work schedule, noise in the neighborhood), sleep inter- same time at night might be helpful. These approaches could be
ventions (behavioral sleep interventions) that address modifiable enhanced with the use of a visual bedtime routine chart that is
factors (e.g., bedtime, bedtime behaviors) and are tailored to families' commonly used in interventions for children with autism (Vriend,
needs may support parents to improve children's sleep. Corkum, Moon, & Smith, 2011), and inclusion of free online re-
Our findings suggest a need to focus on earlier and more regular sources, but they should be selected to be consistent with the
bedtimes and improve sleep efficiency. Because we found low levels characteristics of individual families (e.g., depictions of children that
of parental sleep knowledge, education about the importance of early look like the target group).
and regular bedtimes may be a first step to raise awareness. Moving Parental presence at bedtime including rocking, holding or
bedtimes to the recommended time of 9:00 p.m. or earlier may in- feeding children, was associated with more variable sleep and poorer
crease sleep opportunity. Even a 45‐min increase in sleep opportu- sleep continuity in this study. Caregiver presence at sleep onset
nity can make a meaningful difference in children's physical, can create a sleep‐onset association and/or limit‐setting problems
cognitive, and behavioral health (Gruber, Cassoff, Frenette, Wiebe, & that have a negative influence on sleep continuity (Allen, Howlett,
Carrier, 2012; Sadeh, Gruber, & Raviv, 2003). Helping parents Coulombe, & Corkum, 2016; Mindell, Kuhn, Lewin, Meltzer, &
recognize improvements in children's daytime behavior after Sadeh, 2006). These behavioral sleep problems are most often as-
lengthening and regularizing sleep may help to reinforce this beha- sociated with difficulty maintaining sleep, as we found in more than
vior. It may be helpful to share the importance of quality sleep and half of the children in our study who were attended by parents
recommended sleep duration by emphasizing the active role of sleep during night awakenings. The negative impact of difficulty main-
on children's brain maturation, information processing of early taining sleep on children's emotional, behavioral, and cognitive
childhood experiences, and memory consolidation (Sadeh, 2007). The function (Sadeh et al., 2003; Vriend et al., 2013) supports the im-
result of quality sleep and meeting sleep duration recommendations portance of our findings and the need to help parents assist their
is likely to favor children's optimal cognitive function and lead to children to settle to sleep without the parental presence (e.g., self‐
favorable dispositions for learning, which is critical for the developing soothing; Allen et al., 2016). This is an important focus for education,
brains of infants, toddlers, and preschoolers (Sadeh, 2007). counseling, and feedback to assist with sustained behavior.
ORDWAY ET AL. | 9

Studies of BSIs in early childhood demonstrate modest improve- consistent with that literature. However, the lack of an association
ments in sleep duration and quality (Hall et al., 2015; Meltzer & between parent depression and other aspects of child sleep conflicts
Mindell, 2014; Mindell et al., 2011a, 2011b; Paul et al., 2016). Few with these findings. Although the reason for this difference is not
studies, however, included children during toddlerhood or racially and clear, these findings might be explained by the small sample size in
ethnically diverse families who live with socioeconomic adversity— this study. Future study is needed to expand on these results.
factors that may limit the feasibility, acceptability, and efficacy of these
interventions because of environmental differences or lack of re-
sources (Schwichtenberg et al., 2019). One study of a brief sleep 4.1 | Study strengths and limitations
education program provided through the head start for preschool‐
aged children (3–5 years) improved parent‐reported preschoolers' The strengths of this study include its community‐engaged approach
sleep, parent knowledge, and parent self‐efficacy at the immediate and the racial and ethnic diversity of the sample of very young
conclusion of the program, but these improvements were not sus- children that enabled an improved understanding of sleep from the
tained at 1 month, and beliefs about sleep did not change (Wilson, perspectives of these families. This is especially important, given that
Miller, Bonuck, Lumeng, & Chervin, 2014). Reasons for this are not most published sleep research has been limited to older children
known, but it is possible that the intervention was not sufficiently from primarily white families with higher incomes and education
personalized and tailored with strategies to address the practice of (Schwichtenberg et al., 2019). However, because our goal was to
skills (Arlinghaus & Johnston, 2017) or the specific needs and stressors conduct research within the population of interest and was not de-
of families living with socioeconomic adversity. signed to compare the data from these families with other groups, we
Another potential shortcoming of existing BSIs is the focus cannot quantify differences from those groups. We used health in-
on a singular aspect of sleep health such as sleep duration surance type as a proxy for income; and 90% of our sample were
(Santos et al., 2019) or parent‐identified bedtime problems (Mindell among people with the lowest incomes in the state based on their
et al., 2006). Our findings highlight the multidimensional nature of sleep enrollment with Medicaid insurance or having no health insurance,
health by identifying the variation in sleep duration, timing, regularity, thus limiting our ability to stratify for income. A measure of socio-
and quality within our sample of low‐income children. The idea that economic status that we did not collect was a measure of food
sleep quality may be more important to health than sleep duration is insecurity that would allow us to examine the impact of hunger on
gaining increasing interest among sleep researchers (Bin, 2016). sleep in early childhood. While food insecurity may be related to the
A Lancet series on child development included a review of the need to feed children during the night, most of these families parti-
best strategies to promote child development and concluded that cipated in the Women, Infants, and Children program and therefore,
approaches should focus on early intervention targeted to dis- this may have been less of a concern. The potential role of food
advantaged children; inclusion of high quality, direct learning ex- insecurity should be considered in future studies.
periences for the child and family; and integration with family The sample size was adequate for the overall aim of the larger
support, health (e.g., dental), nutrition, and educational services mixed‐methods study designed to obtain foundational data to in-
(Engle et al., 2007). Our findings of the low prevalence of tooth form the development of an intervention that would be sensitive
brushing at bedtime (45%) and high prevalence of drinking juice or to sociocultural factors (Redeker et al., 2018; Sadler et al., in
milk before bed (70%) and/or when waking at night (57.5%) are press). However, the sample size was small and under‐powered for
concerning because of the association between nighttime feedings the correlational analyses that we conducted. Therefore, the
and early childhood caries (American Academy of Pediatric Den- results of the correlational analyses should be interpreted
tistry, 2017). Considering the higher rates of dental caries among cautiously. It is possible that we missed correlations that would
children who live in socioeconomically disadvantaged communities have been detected in a larger sample, and it is also possible that
(Kim Seow, 2012) and the arguable synergy between sleep, nutri- conducting multiple correlations increased the potential for type I
tion, and dental hygiene at bedtime, it makes sense to integrate error. Nevertheless, our data provide an important foundation for
these topics as part of the bedtime routine, similar to the American future larger‐scale studies.
Academy of Pediatrics (2017) recommendations. However, recent We included both infants and children (6–36 months) because of
reviews of efficacy trials of children's sleep interventions have our interest in developing future interventions to begin healthy sleep
identified possible diluted effects from including too many habits at an early age. Sleep health recommendations and behavioral
behavior‐change messages that consequently stretch caregivers' sleep interventions are very similar in this age group, so we did not
capacity to actually use them on a daily basis (Hurley, Yousafzai, & expect to see many differences in sleep health characteristics or
Lopez‐Boo, 2016). bedtime sleep habits between infants and toddlers. However, there
There is considerable evidence that parental stress, depression, may be differences that could be addressed in a larger study in which
and anxiety contribute to poor sleep health in early childhood those groups are considered separately.
(Caldwell & Redeker, 2015; Cortesi, Giannotti, Sebastiani, Vagnoni, & The use of actigraphy to provide objective data in addition to
Marioni, 2008; Meltzer & Mindell, 2007). Our findings of associations self‐report was a strength of this study. However, the results need to
between parenting stress and anxiety and inconsistent bedtimes are be interpreted cautiously given the limited number of days and
10 | ORDWAY ET AL.

samples. Despite our concerted efforts to provide reminders and practice recommendations. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 29, 1–14. https://
instruction, we were unable to score 30% of the actigraphs due to doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2015.08.006
American Academy of Pediatric Dentistry. (2017). Periodicity of examination,
technical failure, nonadherence, or having fewer than 3 nights of
preventive dental services, anticipatory guidance/counseling, and oral
data. Future researchers should carefully consider approaches to treatment for infants, children, and adolescents. Pediatric Dentistry, 39,
increase adherence to actigraph use with parents of young children 188–196.
and increase the number of days/nights to at least five for a more American Academy of Pediatrics. (2017). Brush, book, bed: A program of
the American Academy of Pediatrics. Retrieved from https://www.
comprehensive assessment of sleep habits.
aap.org/en‐us/advocacy‐and‐policy/aap‐health‐initiatives/Oral‐
We selected well known and often used questionnaires for this Health/Pages/Brush‐Book‐Bed.aspx
study, such as the BISQ, to measure sleep behaviors. However, there Arlinghaus, K. R., & Johnston, C. A. (2017). Advocating for behavior
may be other sleep behaviors, including variations in cultural habits change with education. American Journal of Lifestyle Medicine, 12,
113–116. https://doi.org/10.1177/1559827617745479
and practices, which are not addressed by this instrument. Future
Bin, Y. S. (2016). Is sleep quality more important than sleep duration for
studies should be conducted to further evaluate the cultural public health? Sleep, 39(9), 1629–1630. https://doi.org/10.5665/
relevance of sleep measurements. sleep.6078
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments
by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human
5 | C O NC LUSION development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22,
723–742. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012‐1649.22.6.723
Our findings and the extant literature suggest the need for further Byars, K. C., Yolton, K., Rausch, J., Lanphear, B., & Beebe, D. W. (2012).
research and studies of family‐friendly and tailored BSIs that focus Prevalence, patterns, and persistence of sleep problems in the first 3
years of life. Pediatrics, 129, e276–e284. https://doi.org/10.1542/
on potentially modifiable behaviors that are relevant to the needs of
peds.2011‐0372
families who live with socioeconomic adversity and may also be Bélanger, M. È., Bernier, A., Paquet, J., Simard, V., & Carrier, J. (2013).
culturally diverse. Children from racially and ethnically diverse urban Validating actigraphy as a measure of sleep for preschool children.
families had short sleep duration, late and highly variable bedtimes, Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 9, 701–706.
Caldwell, B. A., Ordway, M. R., Sadler, L. S., & Redeker, N. S. (2019). Parent
and parents had limited knowledge about sleep. Families used
perspectives on sleep and sleep habits among young children living
a variety of behaviors that may have both positive and negative with economic adversity. Journal of Pediatric Health Care, 34(1), 10–22.
influences on sleep. Future studies are needed to test the sustained https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedhc.2019.06.006
effects of BSIs tailored to the specific needs of these families. These Caldwell, B. A., & Redeker, N. S. (2015). Maternal stress and psychological
status and sleep in minority preschool children. Public Health Nursing,
interventions should address the social, economic, and environmental
32(2), 101–111. https://doi.org/10.1111/phn.12104
contexts in which these families live. Combs, D., Goodwin, J. L., Quan, S. F., Morgan, W. J., & Parthasarathy, S.
(2016). Longitudinal differences in sleep duration in Hispanic and
A C K N O W L E D GM E N T S Caucasian children. Sleep Medicine, 18, 61–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
sleep.2015.06.008
The authors are grateful to Barbara Caldwell for her advice and
Cortesi, F., Giannotti, F., Sebastiani, T., Vagnoni, C., & Marioni, P. (2008). Co‐
help with research design and to the student research assistants, sleeping versus solitary sleeping in children with bedtime problems: Child
community families and advisory committee members who made the emotional problems and parental distress. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 6,
project possible. This study was supported by the National Institutes 89–105. https://doi.org/10.1080/15402000801952922
of Health (R21NR01690; P20NR014126) Monica Ordway received Cronin, A., Halligan, S. L., & Murray, L. (2008). Maternal psychosocial
adversity and the longitudinal development of infant sleep. Infancy,
support from the National Institute of Nursing Research (K23
13, 469–495. https://doi.org/10.1080/15250000802329404
5K23NR016277‐03). The project is registered in clinicaltrials.gov Derogatis, L. R. (1993). BSI brief symptom inventory: Administration, scoring,
(Clinicaltrials.gov #NCT03045874). and procedures manual. Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems.
Diekelmann, S., & Born, J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nature
Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 114–126. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2762
OR CID
Doane, L. D., Breitenstein, R. S., Beekman, C., Clifford, S., Smith, T. J., &
Monica R. Ordway http://orcid.org/0000-0002-9765-8962 Lemery‐Chalfant, K. (2019). Early life socioeconomic disparities in
Nancy S. Redeker http://orcid.org/0000-0001-7817-2708 children's sleep: The mediating role of the current home environment.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 48, 56–70. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s10964‐018‐0917‐3
REFERENC ES
El‐Sheikh, M., Bagley, E. J., Keiley, M., Elmore‐Staton, L., Chen, E., &
Abdin, R. R. (1995). The parenting stress index: Short form. Charlottesville, Buckhalt, J. A. (2013). Economic adversity and children's sleep
VA: Pediatric Psychology Press. problems: Multiple indicators and moderation of effects. Health
Acebo, C., Sadeh, A., Seifer, R., Tzischinsky, O., Wolfson, A. R., Hafer, A., & Psychology, 32, 849–859.
Carskadon, M. A. (1999). Estimating sleep patterns with activity Engle, P. L., Black, M. M., Behrman, J. R., Cabral de Mello, M., Gertler, P. J.,
monitoring in children and adolescents: How many nights are Kapiriri, L., … Young, M. E. (2007). Strategies to avoid the loss of
necessary for reliable measures? Sleep, 22, 95–103. developmental potential in more than 200 million children in the
Allen, S. L., Howlett, M. D., Coulombe, J. A., & Corkum, P. V. (2016). ABCs developing world. Lancet, 369(9557), 229–242. https://doi.org/10.
of SLEEPING: A review of the evidence behind pediatric sleep 1016/s0140‐6736(07)60112‐3
ORDWAY ET AL. | 11

Grandner, M. A., Williams, N. J., Knutson, K. L., Roberts, D., & Jean‐Louis, Martinez, S. M., & Thompson‐Lastad, A. (2015). Latino parents' insight on
G. (2016). Sleep disparity, race/ethnicity, and socioeconomic position. optimal sleep for their preschool‐age child: Does context matter? Academic
Sleep Medicine, 18, 7–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2015.01.020 Pediatrics, 15, 636–643. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2015.07.003
Gruber, R., Cassoff, J., Frenette, S., Wiebe, S., & Carrier, J. (2012). Impact McDowall, P. S., Galland, B. C., Campbell, A. J., & Elder, D. E. (2017). Parent
of sleep extension and restriction on children's emotional lability and knowledge of children's sleep: A systematic review. Sleep Medicine Reviews,
impulsivity. Pediatrics, 130, e1155–e1161. https://doi.org/10.1542/ 31, 39–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2016.01.002
peds.2012‐0564 Meltzer, L. J., & Mindell, J. A. (2007). Relationship between child sleep
Gruber, R., Somerville, G., Enros, P., Paquin, S., Kestler, M., & Gillies‐ disturbances and maternal sleep, mood, and parenting stress: A pilot
Poitras, E. (2014). Sleep efficiency (but not sleep duration) of healthy study. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 67–73. https://doi.org/10.
school‐age children is associated with grades in math and languages. 1037/0893‐3200.21.1.67
Sleep Medicine, 15, 1517–1525. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2014. Meltzer, L. J., & Mindell, J. A. (2014). Systematic review and meta‐analysis
08.009 of behavioral interventions for pediatric insomnia. Journal of Pediatric
Guglielmo, D., Gazmararian, J. A., Chung, J., Rogers, A. E., & Hale, L. (2018). Psychology, 39, 932–948. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsu041
Racial/ethnic sleep disparities in US school‐aged children and Meltzer, L. J., Montgomery‐Downs, H. E., Insana, S. P., & Walsh, C. M.
adolescents: A review of the literature. Sleep Health, 4, 68–80. (2012). Use of actigraphy for assessment in pediatric sleep research.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleh.2017.09.005 Sleep Medicine Reviews, 16, 463–475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.
Hale, L., Berger, L. M., Lebourgeois, M. K., & Brooks‐Gunn, J. (2009). Social 2011.10.002
and demographic predictors of preschoolers' bedtime routines. Meltzer, L. J., Sanchez‐Ortuno, M. M., Edinger, J. D., & Avis, K. T. (2015).
Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 30, 394–402. Sleep patterns, sleep instability, and health related quality of life in
https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181ba0e64 parents of ventilator‐assisted children. Journal of Clinical Sleep
Hall, W. A., Hutton, E., Brant, R. F., Collet, J. P., Gregg, K., Saunders, R., … Medicine, 11, 251–258. https://doi.org/10.5664/jcsm.4538
Wooldridge, J. (2015). A randomized controlled trial of an Mindell, J. A., Kuhn, B., Lewin, D. S., Meltzer, L. J., & Sadeh, A. (2006).
intervention for infants' behavioral sleep problems. BMC Pediatrics, Behavioral treatment of bedtime problems and night wakings in
15, 181. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887‐015‐0492‐7 infants and young children. Sleep, 29, 1263–1276.
Hager, E. R., Calamaro, C. J., Bentley, L. M., Hurley, K. M., Wang, Y., & Mindell, J. A., Leichman, E. S., Puzino, K., Walters, R., & Bhullar, B. (2015).
Black, M. M. (2016). Nighttime sleep duration and sleep behaviors Parental concerns about infant and toddler sleep assessed by a mobile
among toddlers from low‐income families: Associations with app. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 13(5), 359–374. https://doi.org/10.
obesogenic behaviors and obesity and the role of parenting. 1080/15402002.2014.905475
Childhood Obesity, 12(5), 392–400. https://doi.org/10.1089/chi. Mindell, J. A., Meltzer, L. J., Carskadon, M. A., & Chervin, R. D. (2009).
2015.0252 Developmental aspects of sleep hygiene: Findings from the 2004
Henderson, J. A., & Jordan, S. S. (2010). Development and preliminary National Sleep Foundation Sleep in America Poll. Sleep Medicine, 10,
evaluation of the bedtime routines questionnaire. Journal of 771–779. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2008.07.016
Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32, 271–280. https://doi. Mindell, J. A., Du Mond, C. E., Sadeh, A., Telofski, L. S., Kulkarni, N., &
org/10.1007/s10862‐009‐9143‐3 Gunn, E. (2011a). Efficacy of an internet‐based intervention for infant
Hurley, K. M., Yousafzai, A. K., & Lopez‐Boo, F. (2016). Early child and toddler sleep disturbances. Sleep, 34, 451–458.
development and nutrition: A review of the benefits and challenges of Mindell, J. A., Du Mond, C. E., Sadeh, A., Telofski, L. S., Kulkarni, N., &
implementing integrated interventions. Advances in Nutrition, 7(2), Gunn, E. (2011b). Long‐term efficacy of an internet‐based
357–363. https://doi.org/10.3945/an.115.010363 intervention for infant and toddler sleep disturbances: One year
Jahng, S., Wood, P. K., & Trull, T. J. (2008). Analysis of affective instability in follow‐up. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 7, 507–511. https://doi.
ecological momentary assessment: Indices using successive difference and org/10.5664/JCSM.1320
group comparison via multilevel modeling. Psychological Methods, 13, Mindell, J. A., & Williamson, A. A. (2018). Benefits of a bedtime routine in
354–375. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014173 young children: Sleep, development, and beyond. Sleep Medicine
Jones, C. H. D., Owens, J. A., & Pham, B. (2013). Can a brief educational Reviews, 40, 93–108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2017.10.007
intervention improve parents' knowledge of healthy children's sleep? National Scientific Council on the Developing Child, & National Forum on
A pilot‐test. Health Education Journal, 72, 601–610. https://doi.org/10. Early Childhood Policy and Programs. (2010). The foundations of
1177/0017896912464606 lifelong health are built in early childhood. Center on the Developing
Kelly, Y., Kelly, J., & Sacker, A. (2013). Time for bed: Associations with Child Harvard University.
cognitive performance in 7‐year‐old children: A longitudinal Norris, L. (2018). Connecticut and the ACA's Medicaid expansion. Retrieved
population‐based study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community from https://www.healthinsurance.org/connecticut‐medicaid/
Health, 67, 926–931. Owens, J. A., & Jones, C. (2011). Parental knowledge of healthy sleep in
Kim Seow, W. (2012). Environmental, maternal, and child factors which young children: Results of a primary care clinic survey. Journal of
contribute to early childhood caries: A unifying conceptual model. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 32, 447–453. https://doi.org/
International Journal of Paediatric Dentistry, 22, 157–168. https://doi. 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31821bd20b
org/10.1111/j.1365‐263X.2011.01186.x Owens, J. A., Jones, C., & Nash, R. (2011). Caregivers' knowledge,
Koulouglioti, C., Cole, R., Moskow, M., McQuillan, B., Carno, M.‐A., & behavior, and attitudes regarding healthy sleep in young children.
Grape, A. (2014). The longitudinal association of young children's Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 7, 345–350. https://doi.org/10.5664/
everyday routines to sleep duration. Journal of Pediatric Health Care, JCSM.1186
28, 80–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedhc.2012.12.006 Owens, J., & Ordway, M. R. (2019). Sleep among children. In D. T. Ducan, I.
Lau, A. S. (2006). Making the case for selective and directed cultural Kawachi & S. Redline (Eds.), The social epidemiology of sleep. New York,
adaptations of evidence‐based treatments: Examples from parent NY: Oxford University Press.
training. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 13, 295–310. https:// Paruthi, S., Brooks, L. J., D'Ambrosio, C., Hall, W. A., Kotagal, S., Lloyd,
doi.org/10.1111/j.1468‐2850.2006.00042.x R. M., … Wise, M. S. (2016). Recommended amount of sleep for
Magee, C. A., Gordon, R., & Caputi, P. (2014). Distinct developmental pediatric populations: A consensus statement of the American
trends in sleep duration during early childhood. Pediatrics, 133, Academy of Sleep Medicine. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 12,
e1561–e1567. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013‐3806 785–786. https://doi.org/10.5664/jcsm.5866
12 | ORDWAY ET AL.

Paul, I. M., Savage, J. S., Anzman‐Frasca, S., Marini, M. E., Mindell, J. A., & Soffer‐Dudek, N., Sadeh, A., Dahl, R. E., & Rosenblat‐Stein, S. (2011). Poor
Birch, L. L. (2016). INSIGHT responsive parenting intervention and sleep quality predicts deficient emotion information processing over
infant sleep. Pediatrics, 138, e20160762. https://doi.org/10.1542/ time in early adolescence. Sleep, 34, 1499–1508. https://doi.org/10.
peds.2016‐0762. 5665/sleep.1386
Redeker, N. S., Ordway, M. R., Banasiak, N., Caldwell, B., Canapari, C., Staples, A. D., Bates, J. E., & Petersen, I. T. (2015). Bedtime routines in
Crowley, A., … Sadler, L. S. (2018). Community partnership for healthy early childhood: Prevalence, consistency, and associations with
sleep: Research protocol. Research in Nursing and Health, 41(1), 19–29. nighttime sleep. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
https://doi.org/10.1002/nur.21840 Development, 80, 141–159. https://doi.org/10.1111/mono.12149
Reitman, D., Currier, R. O., & Stickle, T. R. (2002). A critical evaluation of Tierney, A. L., & Nelson, C. A., III (2009). Brain development and the role
the Parenting Stress Index‐Short Form (PSI‐SF) in a head start of experience in the early years. Zero to Three, 30, 9–13.
population. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31, US Census Bureau. (2019). Quick facts. Retrieved from https://www.
384–392. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3103_10 census.gov/quickfacts/newhavencityconnecticut
Sadeh, A. (2004). A brief screening questionnaire for infant sleep Vriend, J. L., Corkum, P. V., Moon, E. C., & Smith, I. M. (2011).
problems: Validation and findings for an Internet sample. Pediatrics, Behavioral Interventions for sleep problems in children with autism
113, e570–e577. spectrum disorders: Current findings and future directions. Journal
Sadeh, A. (2007). Consequences of sleep loss or sleep disruption of Pediatric Psychology, 36, 1017–1029. https://doi.org/10.1093/
in children. Sleep Medicine Clinics, 2(3), 513–520. https://doi.org/10. jpepsy/jsr044
1016/j.jsmc.2007.05.012 Vriend, J. L., Davidson, F. D., Corkum, P. V., Rusak, B., Chambers, C. T., &
Sadeh, A., Gruber, R., & Raviv, A. (2003). The effects of sleep restriction and McLaughlin, E. N. (2013). Manipulating sleep duration alters
extension on school‐age children: What a difference an hour makes. Child emotional functioning and cognitive performance in children. Journal
Development, 74, 444–455. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467‐8624.7402008 of Pediatric Psychology, 38, 1058–1069.
Sadeh, A., Mindell, J. A., Luedtke, K., & Wiegand, B. (2009). Sleep and sleep Walker, M. P., & Stickgold, R. (2006). Sleep, memory, and plasticity. Annual
ecology in the first 3 years: A web‐based study. Journal of Sleep Research, Review of Psychology, 57, 139–166.
18, 60–73. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365‐2869.2008.00699.x Wilson, K. E., Miller, A. L., Bonuck, K., Lumeng, J. C., & Chervin, R. D.
Sadeh, A., Sharkey, K. M., & Carskadon, M. A. (1994). Activity‐based sleep‐ (2014). Evaluation of a sleep education program for low‐income
wake identification: An empirical test of methodological issues. Sleep, preschool children and their families. Sleep, 37, 1117–1125. https://
17, 201–207. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/17.3.201 doi.org/10.5665/sleep.3774
Sadler, L. S., Banasiak, N., Canapari, C., Crowley, A. A., Fenick, A., Xie, L., Kang, H., Xu, Q., Chen, M. J., Liao, Y., Thiyagarajan, M., …
O'Connell, M., … Redeker, N. S. (in press). “If mommy's not cranky, Nedergaard, M. (2013). Sleep drives metabolite clearance from the
everybody else survives another day.” Perspectives on sleep from adult brain. Science, 342, 373–377. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.
multiethnic community parents, pediatric providers and childcare 1241224
providers. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. Yip, T., Cheon, Y. M., Wang, Y., Cham, H., Tryon, W., & El‐Sheikh, M.
Santos, I. S., Del‐Ponte, B., Tovo‐Rodrigues, L., Halal, C. S., Matijasevich, A., (2019). Racial disparities in sleep: Associations with discrimination
Cruz, S., … Bassani, D. G. (2019). Effect of parental counseling on among ethnic/racial minority adolescents. Child Development, https://
infants' healthy sleep habits in Brazil: A randomized clinical trial. doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13234
JAMA Network Open, 2(12):e1918062. https://doi.org/10.1001/ Yoo, J., Slack, K. S., & Holl, J. L. (2010). The impact of health‐promoting
jamanetworkopen.2019.18062 behaviors on low‐income children's health: A risk and resilience
Schwichtenberg, A. J., Abel, E. A., Keys, E., & Honaker, S. M. (2019). perspective. Health and Social Work, 35, 133–143. https://doi.org/10.
Diversity in pediatric behavioral sleep intervention studies. 1093/hsw/35.2.133
Sleep Medicine Reviews, 47, 103–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
smrv.2019.07.004
Sheridan, A., Murray, L., Cooper, P. J., Evangeli, M., Byram, V., &
Halligan, S. L. (2013). A longitudinal study of child sleep in high and
low risk families: Relationship to early maternal settling strategies and How to cite this article: Ordway MR, Sadler LS, Jeon S, et al.
child psychological functioning. Sleep Medicine, 14, 266–273. https:// Sleep health in young children living with socioeconomic
doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2012.11.006 adversity. Res Nurs Health. 2020;1–12.
Smith, J. P., Hardy, S. T., Hale, L. E., & Gazmararian, J. A. (2019). Racial
https://doi.org/10.1002/nur.22023
disparities and sleep among preschool aged children: A systematic review.
Sleep Health, 5, 49–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleh.2018.09.010

You might also like