Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

473146

mmunication Disorders QuarterlySuskind et al.


© Hammill Institute on Disabilities 2013

Reprints and permissions: http://www.


CDQ34410.1177/1525740112473146Co

sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Research
Communication Disorders Quarterly

An Exploratory Study of “Quantitative


34(4) 199­–209
© Hammill Institute on Disabilities 2013
Reprints and permissions:

Linguistic Feedback”: Effect of LENA sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav


DOI: 10.1177/1525740112473146
cdq.sagepub.com
Feedback on Adult Language Production

Dana Suskind, MD1, Kristin R. Leffel, BS1, Marc W. Hernandez, PhD1,


Shannon G. Sapolich, BA1, Elizabeth Suskind, BS1, Erin Kirkham, MD2,
and Patrick Meehan, MSW3

Abstract
A child’s early language environment is critical to his or her life-course trajectory. Quantitative linguistic feedback utilizes
the Language ENvironment Analysis (LENA) technology as a tool to analyze verbal interactions and reinforce behavior
change. This exploratory pilot study evaluates the feasibility and efficacy of a novel behavior-change strategy, quantitative
linguistic feedback, to influence adult linguistic behavior and, as a result, a child’s early language environment. Baseline LENA
outcome measures (i.e., adult word count [AWC] and conversational turn count [CTC]) were obtained from a diverse
sample of 17 nonparental caregivers and their typically developing children (charges) ages 10 to 40 months. Caregivers
participated in a one-time educational intervention focusing on enriching a child’s home language environment, interpreting
feedback from the baseline LENA recordings, and setting language goals for the following session. Post-intervention, six
additional LENA recordings were obtained weekly to measure linguistic behavior. Caregivers showed a significant and
prolonged increase from mean baseline to mean postintervention AWC and CTC as measured by LENA–AWC: mean
difference = 395 words per hour, 31.6% increase, t = 3.29, p < .01; CTC: mean difference = 14 turns per hour, 24.9% increase,
t = 3.54, p < .01. Preliminary results indicate that a one-time educational intervention combined with quantitative linguistic
feedback may have a positive effect on caregiver language output, thus enhancing the child’s language environment. This
study represents an initial step in the development and evaluation of a novel behavior-change strategy. We propose that
quantitative linguistic feedback will add significantly to the arsenal of clinical and research tools used to evaluate and enrich
a child’s early language environment.

Keywords
speech and language therapy, parent education, specific language impairment (SLI)

Background Selzer, & Lyons, 1991; Kaiser & Hancock, 2003a, 2003b;
Landry, Smith, Swank, & Guttentag, 2008; Landry, Smith,
A child’s early language environment is critical to his or her Swank, & Miller-Loncar, 2000; Reilly et al., 2010; Snow,
life-course trajectory (Forget-Dubois & Dionne, 2009; Law 1972). This research demonstrates clearly that a qualita-
& Rush, 2009; Phillips, 2011). Language exposure not only tively and quantitatively rich early language environment is
bears an obvious relationship to a child’s linguistic devel- paramount for a child to realize his or her linguistic poten-
opment but also significantly influences a child’s overall tial and, ultimately, life-course potential as well.
cognitive and educational achievement (Hart & Risley, Disparities in early language environments, along with
1992, 1995; Huttenlocher, Vasilyeva, Cymerman, & Levine, their profound lifelong consequences, have been
2002; Kashinath, Woods, & Goldstein, 2006). Hart & demonstrated in children with developmental disabilities
Risley’s landmark study demonstrated a significant correla-
tion between the number of words a child hears by age 3 1
University of Chicago, IL, USA
and his or her ultimate IQ and academic success. Equally 2
University of Washington, Seattle, USA
compelling is the literature linking the qualitative aspects 3
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, USA
of language exposure, including complexity of speech,
Corresponding author:
adult-to-child interaction and responsive caregiving, with a
Dana L. Suskind, Department of Pediatrics and Otolaryngology,
child’s ultimate educational outcome (Bornstein, Tamis- University of Chicago, 5841 S Maryland Ave., mc 1035, Chicago, IL, USA.
LeMonda, Hahn, & Haynes, 2008; Huttenlocher, Haight, Email: dsuskind@surgery.bsd.uchicago.edu

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at UNIV OF WISCONSIN OSHKOSH on June 8, 2015


200 Communication Disorders Quarterly 34(4)

such as autism, children with intellectual disabilities, chil- varied approaches, philosophies, and target populations,
dren with hearing loss whose parents are hearing, and chil- overall Roberts and Kaiser found that, with relatively mod-
dren with specific language impairments (SLI), as well as est training, parents successfully implemented language
children of low socioeconomic status (SES; DesJardin & interventions, which in turn positively affected children’s
Eisenberg, 2007; Hammer, Tomblin, Zhang, & Weiss, 2001; receptive and expressive language. Most interventional
Hoff & Tian, 2005; Huttenlocher et al., 1991; Meadow- approaches that Roberts and Kaiser reviewed included a
Orlans, 1997; Reilly et al., 2010; Rowe, 2008; Spencer & broad “social communication focus,” with linguistic strate-
Gutfreund, 1990). These populations have been demon- gies designed to be integrated into daily routines (Roberts
strated to have significantly reduced quantitative and quali- & Kaiser, 2011). The two most common linguistic
tative parental linguistic input. As such, it has been estimated approaches aimed to affect “what the parents say” and
that children living in poverty will have heard 30 million “how much the parents say” (Roberts & Kaiser, 2011).
fewer words by age 3 compared with their more affluent Interventions also sought to enhance parent–child interac-
peers (Hart & Risley, 1995). This gap in linguistic input tion via increased parental responsiveness to the child’s
mirrors an academic achievement gap that is evident from attempts to communicate.
the first day of school and persists throughout adulthood Despite the generally positive outcomes, Roberts and
(Phillips, 2011). Children with hearing loss in particular, Kaiser (2011) identified a number of weaknesses in the
regardless of SES, have been shown to have significant dis- interventions reviewed. They found a lack of detailed
parities in language experiences when compared with nor- descriptions of the interventions, resulting in an inability to
mal-hearing peers. In low-SES children, language disparities pinpoint the intervention components that correlated with
are most often associated with less exposure to and facility specific language improvement (Roberts & Kaiser, 2011).
with academic language (Snow, 1972). For children with In addition, many studies failed to accurately assess care-
hearing loss, however, it is the fundamental development of giver intervention uptake, sustainability of caregiver lan-
listening and spoken language that is at risk. guage behavior change, or the necessary child intervention
Clinical interventions for children who have speech and dosage, for example, frequency and length of intervention
language delays often target the adult caregiver as the cata- program (Roberts & Kaiser, 2011). Furthermore, the pri-
lyst for improving the child’s language development (Kaiser marily middle-class study participants raised the question
& Hancock, 2003a; Kashinath et al., 2006; McWilliam, of external validity and generalizability of results.
2001; Pickston, Golbart, Marshall, Rees, & Roulstone, These shortcomings mirror a similar concern in the over-
2009; Reese, Sparks, & Leyva, 2010; Roberts & Kaiser, all health promotions literature, where studies of complex
2011). As the primary contributors to their child’s language behavioral interventions often lack clarity in descriptions of
environment, adult caregivers are able to enrich their child’s behavioral strategies used (Michie, Johnston, Francis,
home language environment throughout the child’s day. Hardeman, & Eccles, 2008). It has been suggested that the
This family-centered approach is fundamental to the feder- modest behavioral effects often found in interventions are
ally funded early intervention programs for young children linked to this lack of transparency and well-defined behavior-
with disabilities (Individuals With Disabilities Education change strategies used in these interventions (Michie,
Improvement Act of 2004, 2003). While the intervention Fixsen, Grimshaw, & Eccles, 2009). In response to these
literature focuses primarily on parental caregivers, the lit- limitations, the health promotions field has advanced a tax-
erature also supports the incorporation of nonparental care- onomy of theoretically based behavior-change techniques
givers (NPCs; Giurolametto, Weltzman, & Greenberg, (Michie et al., 2011). Michie et al. have proposed a compre-
2003, 2004; Milburn, 2011). Intervening with NPCs is an hensive group of 40 discrete evidence-based behavior-
important method for improving the child’s language envi- change techniques, which they term CALO-RE1 taxonomy.
ronment as NPCs care for the majority of 0- to 4-year-old The CALO-RE taxonomy of strategies is not specific to a
American children; 77% of children of employed mothers particular behavioral intervention type (e.g., smoking or
received care from NPCs in 2010 (Table FAM3.A. Child weight loss) but is, rather, a methodological tool that can be
care: Primary child care arrangements for children 0–4 with generalized to any health promotion intervention. The ulti-
employed mothers by selected characteristics, selected mate goal is to eliminate the ambiguities of interventional
years 1985–2010). strategies so the specific factors and their specific effects in
The literature strongly supports the potential for improv- an intervention are understood. The literature provides
ing the child’s language outcome via the adult caregiver compelling evidence that incorporating these theoretically
(Roberts & Kaiser, 2011). In a meta-analysis of 18 parent- based behavior change techniques into health promotion
implemented language interventions, Roberts and Kaiser interventions is essential to stimulating sustainable behavior-
found significant improvements in receptive and expres- change and advancing the science of behavior change
sive language skills of children 18 to 60 months of age (Abraham & Michie, 2008; Michie et al., 2005; Michie,
(Roberts & Kaiser, 2011). Although the interventions had Jochelson, Markham, & Bridle, 2009).

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at UNIV OF WISCONSIN OSHKOSH on June 8, 2015


Suskind et al. 201

Aims Bandura writes that a sense of self-efficacy is essential any


time a parent is to take action on behalf of a child’s educa-
It is with appreciation for the critical need for theoretically tion or health status, or act as a child’s teacher and advocate.
based behavior-change strategies in caregiver-directed lan- DesJardin and Eisenberg (2007) also found that caregiver
guage interventions that we describe our preliminary pilot self-efficacy is strongly related to the linguistic skills of
testing of a novel, multifaceted behavior-change strategy young children with hearing loss. Self-efficacy is also asso-
we call quantitative linguistic feedback. This strategy has ciated with more sophisticated language strategies, such as
been developed to facilitate changes in language behavior open-ended questioning and parallel talk, which are in turn
of adult caretakers and to quantify the impact and sustain- correlated with improved language outcomes (Hughes &
ability of intervention in adult linguistic behavior. It is Demo, 1989; Johnston-Brooks, Lewis, & Garg, 2002;
designed to be incorporated into and enhance any existing Raikes & Thompson, 2005; Wolf et al., 2007).
caregiver-directed interventions. Given these theoretical foundations, the parent-directed
The theoretical underpinnings of quantitative linguistic educational intervention was designed to (a) review the
feedback are Bandura’s social cognitive theory (SCT), the importance of a child’s early language environment in rela-
theory of planned behavior (TPB), self-determination the- tion to later language proficiency, (b) understand how the
ory (SDT), and the health belief model (HBM; Ajzen, LENA measures certain critical aspects of this environment
2001; Ajzen, Brown, & Carvajal, 2004; Bandura, 1997; (i.e., adult word count [AWC], conversational turn count
Vansteenkiste & Sheldon, 2006). These theories of behavior [CTC], TV Time [TVT]), and (c) discuss strategies to help
change must be factored into the development of successful parents enrich their child’s language environment.
behavioral intervention techniques (Michie et al., 2008). The technological centerpiece of quantitative linguistic
Guided by such theories, we developed quantitative linguis- feedback’s behavior-change intervention is the LENA
tic feedback with a multifaceted approach that includes a System. The LENA is an innovative recording device that,
mechanism by which adults can set and respond to goals along with its accompanying computer software, can be used
(TPB and goal setting), utilize cues for action or external to automatically capture and analyze a child’s home language
prompts for behavior change (HBM), recognize the environment (Oller, 2010). The LENA quantifies the number
improved language environment they are providing for of words to which a child is exposed and the number of con-
their child, and develop a sense of competency and auton- versational turns the child takes with adult(s) over a continu-
omy in facilitating their child’s development (SCT). ous 16-hr period (Gilkerson & Richards, 2008). The LENA’s
TPB describes “intention” as a critical determinant of quantitative outcome measures are then presented to caregiv-
behavior. Health intention is, itself, determined by beliefs ers, providing them with concrete feedback about where need
and attitudes about treatment (e.g., medication efficacy and for greater language exposure exists and where improvement
adverse effects), subjective social norms (e.g., concern with has been made. Thus, the LENA’s value is not simply for
the opinions of others), past experience (e.g., previous scientific quantification but for providing feedback to the
experience with similar programs), and perceived behav- adult caregivers. In collaboration with a therapist, caregivers
ioral control and self-efficacy (Hagger, Chatzisarantis, & can clearly see a snapshot of the home language environ-
Biddle, 2001). Attitudes, both favorable and unfavorable, ment, consider their role within it, discuss opportunities to
will affect intention and, ultimately, action and adherence, enrich that environment, and importantly, chart and track
or the lack thereof (Ajzen, 1996; Hagger, et al., 2001; White progress toward enrichment. In other words, the quantitative
& Wellington, 2009). Quantitative linguistic feedback outcome measures are utilized as a type of “biofeedback”
encompasses intention by incorporating goals to be set and used for goal setting and measuring progress toward those
accomplished, and, most critically, engages caregivers as goals. Thus, this quantitative linguistic feedback can be used
goal setters. to reinforce traditional approaches used in a wide variety of
HBM maintains that “readiness” to change behaviors is language-development focused interventions.
“activated” by motivational feedback and “cues to action” Quantitative linguistic feedback is a multifaceted tech-
(Janz, Champion, & Stretcher, 2002). External prompts are nique for behavior change that may work through a number
cues to action, which in turn stimulate behavior change. In of different mechanisms as described in the CALO-RE tax-
quantitative linguistic feedback, “cues to action” are the onomy (Michie et al., 2011). For adult caretakers, quantita-
Language ENvironment Analysis (LENA) data that provide tive linguistic feedback offers a mechanism by which they
concrete evidence of a child’s language environment and can set and respond to goals, utilize cues for action or exter-
patterns of change. nal prompts for behavior change (e.g., reviewing LENA
Finally, Bandura’s SCT recognizes self-efficacy―the outcome measures), and recognize the improved language
“belief in one’s capacity to organize and execute the courses environment they are providing for their child. It also instills
of action required to produce given attainments”―as criti- a sense of competency and autonomy in facilitating their
cal for the success of all interventions (Bandura, 1997). child’s development.

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at UNIV OF WISCONSIN OSHKOSH on June 8, 2015


202 Communication Disorders Quarterly 34(4)

Our initial work evaluating quantitative linguistic feed- Table 1.  Nonparental Caregiver Characteristics.
back’s feasibility and efficacy in improving adult quantitative
Nonparental caregiver n %
language behavior is presented here. We hypothesized that
quantitative linguistic feedback would result in a significant Education 17 –
increase in AWC and CTC as measured by the LENA.  GED 2 12
Subsequent efforts will focus on demonstrating a correspond-   High school diploma 2 12
ing qualitative improvement, sustained linguistic change, as   Some college 4 23
well as methods for incorporating this novel strategy into   Four year college degree 9 53
existing interventions. Ultimately, the technique of caregiver-   Nanny school 2 12
directed, quantitative linguistic feedback holds the promise Race/Ethnicity 17 –
for significantly improving child language outcomes.   African American 2 12
 Asian 1 6
 Hispanic 3 19
Method and Procedures  Caucasian 10 63
  Did not state 1 6
Design
Primary language 17 –
 English 12 71
The study used a prospective case-crossover design involv-
 Other 5 29
ing two baseline recordings followed by a single educa- Language spoken with child 17 –
tional intervention and six postintervention LENA feedback  English 12 71
recordings. This exploratory pilot study was conducted  Other 5 29
with a group of NPCs who were chosen because of their   Other language spoken 3 to 6 hours/day 1 20
extensive, focused, and consistent periods of time with the   Other language spoken 1 to 3 hours/day 2 40
children in their care.   Other language spoken <1 hour/day 2 40

Participants
NPCs. Nineteen English-speaking NPCs caring for typi- Eight children (47%) had one sibling, with four of the sib-
cally developing children were recruited for this study. Of lings younger and one older. One child (6%) had two sib-
the 19 recruited NPCs, 17 (90%) completed the study and lings, with one younger and one older (see Table 2). Five
are included in the present analysis. One NPC dropped out children received primary care from their mothers (9%),
secondary to pregnancy, and another withdrew due to 1 child from his or her NPC (6%), 10 from their mothers and
scheduling difficulties prior to obtaining baseline record- NPCs (9%), and 1 from his or her mother and father (6%).
ings. A table of NPC demographics is shown in Table 1. All All children were from two-parent households with
NPCs were women and ranged in age from 23 to 53 years annual incomes of US$100,000 or more. One parent had
(M = 33.1, SD = 10.1). Their education levels ranged from an associate’s degree, whereas the rest of the parents in
GED to 4-year college degree, with approximately half the study had a 4-year college degree or higher. Forty-one
having a 4-year degree (n = 9; 53%). Two NPCs (12%) percent of parents (n = 14) had attained doctorate degrees.
received specialized child care training (i.e., nanny school). Eleven families responded as Caucasian (65%), three as
Ten NPCs (59%) were Caucasian, with the other 6 repre- Asian (18%), two as multiethnic (12%), and one family
senting a range of races and ethnicities, including African declined to state (6%). All families spoke English as the
American (n = 2; 12%), Asian (n = 1; 6%), and Hispanic (n primary language at home. Five were bilingual, but
= 3; 18%). Nearly one third of the NPCs (n = 5; 29%) reported that they spoke their second language less than
spoke English as a second language. On average, NPCs 1 hr per day.
had 9 years of experience caring for children (SD = 7,
range = 2–28) and a range of 2 months to 3 years caring for
the child in this study (M = 1 year, SD = 9 months). Five of Materials and Procedures
the NPCs (n = 5/17, 29%) cared for at least one other child Recruitment. English-speaking NPCs caring for typically
simultaneously with the child in the study. developing children were recruited through professionals at
the University of Chicago and the surrounding area via
Children and families. Seventeen families participated in this email, online postings, and word of mouth. NPCs were paid
study: 10 girls (n = 10; 59%) and 7 boys (n = 7; 41%). Ages US$100 for completion of the study. Informed consent was
ranged from 10.5 to 40.3 months (M = 20.4 months). In obtained from parents and NPCs. The consent forms
families with multiple children, 1 child between the ages of included an agreement that parents would not know or ask
10 and 42 months was eligible for inclusion in the study. for their NPC’s results or feedback.

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at UNIV OF WISCONSIN OSHKOSH on June 8, 2015


Suskind et al. 203

Table 2.  Family Characteristics. the shirt containing the LENA device. The LENA device
records up to 16 hr of audio, and requires a minimum of
Child n %
12 hr of data for complete and accurate processing. Because
Sex 17   the NPCs all worked less than 12 hr per day, they were
 Male 7 41 instructed to keep the device turned on for the maximum
 Female 10 59 recording duration and to write down the timestamp present
Number of siblings 17   on the digital display when their interaction with the child
 0 8 47 ended for the day (i.e., when they left the child’s home or
 1 8 47 when the child was put to bed). NPCs each made a total of
 2 1 6 eight recordings during the course of the study: two base-
Level of education   line recordings prior to intervention and six postbaseline
Mother 16 – recordings. We used the average of the two baseline record-
  Four-year college degree 6 37.5 ings as their baseline measure.
  Master’s degree 4 25
  Doctorate degree 6 37.5
Educational intervention. After baseline recordings were
Father 16 –
completed, each NPC participated in a language-focused
  Associate’s degree 1 6
educational intervention lasting approximately 1 hr at the
  Four-year college degree 5 31
  Master’s degree 2 13
child’s home. This session included information on child
  Doctorate degree 8 50 language development, along with tips for increasing their
Race/Ethnicity 17 – talk and conversational turn-taking with the child. The edu-
 Asian 3 18 cational session was conveyed via a short PowerPoint pre-
 Caucasian 11 65 sentation, and included the following core components: (a)
 Multiethnic 2 11 review of the importance of a child’s early language envi-
  Did not state 1 6 ronment (i.e., connection between caregiver linguistic input
Mother employed 16 – and a child’s language and educational success), (b) discus-
  At home 1 6 sion of LENA and its target measures (i.e., AWC and CTC),
  Outside home 15 94 (c) review of caregiver baseline LENA recordings, and (d)
Primary caregiver 17 – discussion of strategies to enrich a child’s early language
 Mother 5 29 environment and raise LENA measures through talking
  Nonparental caregiver 1 6 more and increasing conversational turns with a child.
  Mother and nonparental caregiver 10 59
  Mother and father 1 6 Quantitative linguistic feedback. The research assistant then
provided the NPCs with their individual LENA linguistic
feedback from the baseline recordings, and facilitated the
Demographic and home language survey. NPCs and one par- NPCs’ understanding of the LENA feedback reports and
ent from each family completed a demographic survey, goal setting for the following recordings. LENA feedback
which included items about race, ethnicity, education, and reports included raw numbers, percentiles, and graphic rep-
household income. The instrument also included a home resentations of mean hourly AWC and CTC. Intervention-
language survey, which included questions about typical ists discussed with NPCs specific results, methods for
daily schedules, the amount that the child talked, the amount increasing AWC and CTC, and goal setting for upcoming
that NPCs and parents spoke to the child, and the amount of recordings. In addition, NPCs demonstrated ability to accu-
time children were exposed to TV during a typical day. rately interpret and understand feedback at the end of the
interventional session.
Interventionists. Two graduate-level research assistants After the educational intervention, NPCs made record-
involved in the development of the 1-hr educational inter- ings once weekly for 6 weeks and received their LENA
vention performed all interventions. Training and practice feedback results halfway between each recording session.
occurred with the principal investigator (Suskind) prior to No active discussion or goal setting was performed by the
commencement of interventions. interventionist beyond providing the results in paper form.

Recording procedure. At the start of the study, NPCs were


each given a LENA digital language processor designed to Data Processing
be worn by the child in the front pocket of a specially made Data were uploaded from the LENA recorders into a study
t-shirt. At the beginning of each recording day, NPCs wrote computer. All data were then processed into counts by
down the recording start time and then placed on the child LENA ADEX software. Audio recordings were discarded

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at UNIV OF WISCONSIN OSHKOSH on June 8, 2015


204 Communication Disorders Quarterly 34(4)

to protect the privacy of the participants per IRB-approved Table 3.  Family and Nonparental Caregiver Language
research protocol. Language-related outcomes were Characteristics.
assessed on three main measures: AWC, CTC, and child
Self-reported language behaviors n %
vocalization count (CVC). Naptime as well as NPC-child
interaction start and end time parameters were set using the Estimation of time spent conversing with child
information NPCs provided in their initial surveys and on  Parent 15 –
their daily recording time sheets. A computer script was    Less than average 0 0
written to filter recordings during nap periods. Once fil-    Average 1 7
tered, all counts were then standardized by total relevant    More than average 11 73
interaction time and were expressed as either words per    Much more than average 3 20
hour (wph) or turns per hour (tph).   Nonparental caregiver 17 –
   Less than average 0 0
   Average 7 41
Statistical Analysis    More than average 5 29
   Much more than average 5 29
Repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted for each of
Estimation of volume of child speech  
the four LENA measures (i.e., AWC, CTC, CVC, and TVT),
 Parent 16 –
with Time (postintervention Sessions 1, 2, 3 to 6) as the
   Less than average 2 12
within-subject variable, to determine whether there were
   Average 3 19
significant differences among postintervention recording    More than average 7 44
time points. No significant differences were found for any of    Much more than average 4 25
the four LENA measures. As such, the counts for each of the   Nonparental caregiver 17 –
six postintervention time points were averaged to create a    Less than average 0 0
single postintervention score for each of the four LENA    Average 7 41
measures. Paired t tests were then carried out to compare the    More than average 5 29
pre- and postintervention mean standardized counts.    Much more than average 5 29
Estimation of 50 word vocabulary  
 Parent 16 –
Outcomes and Results    Yes 9 56
Home Language Survey    No 7 44
  Nonparental caregiver 17 –
NPCs and parents were asked to rate the amount that the    Yes 12 71
child talked on a 5-point scale from much less than average    No 5 29
to much more than average. Their responses are shown in
Table 3, with 10 NPCs (59%) and 11 parents (65%) rating
their children as above-average talkers. When asked how
much they conversed with their child, a majority of parents recordings were made 4.8 days apart on average (SD = 7.5,
(n = 11; 73%) stated they talked to their child more than range = 1–33). Because of the Hawthorne effect, the aver-
average. One parent (7%) rated herself as average. Three age of the two baseline measures was to make pre- and
parents (20%) rated themselves as talking to their child postintervention comparisons (Wolfe & Michaud, 2010).
much more than average. In response to the same question, Indeed, AWC was marginally significantly higher for the
7 NPCs (41%) responded that they talked to the child an first baseline session than for the second (M difference =
average amount, whereas 5 (29%) thought they talked more 413 wph, 28.4%, t = 2.25, p < .04). In contrast, the CTC
than average, and the remaining 5 (29%) reported talking baselines did not differ significantly (M difference = 12
much more than average. tph, 19%, t = 1.64, p < .12) nor did the CVC baselines (M
Overall, NPCs and parents reported low amounts of child difference = 26 vph, 10.4%, t = 1.71, p < .11). The six
television exposure. NPCs reported that the television was on postbaseline recordings were made from 1 to 89 days post-
from 0 to 3 hr per day while they were with the child (M = 0.8 intervention (M = 25 days, SD = 19 days). The average
hr, SD = 0.9 hr). Similarly, parents reported watching 0 to 3 interval between postintervention recordings was 8 days
hr of television per day at home (M = 1.24, SD = 1.02). (SD = 6 days).

TVT overall. The TVT recorded in the child’s aural environ-


LENA Outcome Measures ment during waking hours was low for all children in the
Caregivers contributed two recordings at baseline prior to study, with an average of 22.8 min at baseline (range =
intervention and six postbaseline recordings. Baseline 1–63 min, SD = 16.1 min). Mean amount of TVT did not

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at UNIV OF WISCONSIN OSHKOSH on June 8, 2015


Suskind et al. 205

Figure 1.  Adult word count baseline and postintervention Figure 3.  Conversational turn count baseline and
averages in words per hour. postintervention averages in turns per hour.

Figure 2.  Adult word count in words per hour at baseline and Figure 4.  Conversational turn count in turns per hour at
6 weeks post-intervention. baseline and 6 weeks post-intervention.

differ significantly between baseline and postintervention Conclusion and Implications


recordings (M difference = 0.57 min, 2.5% decrease, t =
0.14, p < .89).
Discussion
The primary goal of our exploratory pilot was to evaluate
AWC overall. As seen in Figure 1, mean AWC from postint- the novel behavior-change strategy of quantitative lin-
ervention recordings was significantly higher than mean guistic feedback and its effect on adult linguistic behav-
AWC at baseline (M difference = 395 wph, 31.6% increase, ior. Our study comprised an initial 1-hr educational
t = 3.29, p < .01). Figure 2 shows AWCs at baseline and at session, six weekly LENA recordings, and the use of
each of the six postintervention sessions. quantitative feedback from those recordings to improve
language outcomes. The significant increases seen in
CTC overall. The mean postintervention CTC was signifi- adult quantitative language output for AWC and CTC
cantly greater than the baseline average, shown in Figure 3 indicate the potential of the quantitative linguistic feed-
(M difference = 14 tph, 24.9% increase, t = 3.54, p < .01). back technique to inform adults to motivate them to
Figure 4 graphs CTCs at baseline and at each of the six modify their interactions with children and provide a
postintervention sessions. richer language environment.

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at UNIV OF WISCONSIN OSHKOSH on June 8, 2015


206 Communication Disorders Quarterly 34(4)

Pedometers offer a useful analogy for understanding the therapists to gain greater understanding of a family’s base-
quantitative linguistic feedback technique and clarifying its line linguistic style so they can personalize their clinical
impact. Pedometers provide a quantified, easily grasped approach. In addition, the LENA provides ongoing infor-
conceptual framework for behavior change; hence, the mation to therapists with regard to their clients’ therapeutic
health promotion concept of “10,000 steps a day” (Shape uptake. Just as personalized medicine lets oncologists indi-
Up America, Get Up and Go! 10,000 Steps a Day, 1994). vidually adapt therapeutic regimens to specific cancer char-
Behavior-change literature evaluating the pedometer as a acteristics, so also does quantitative linguistic feedback
form of quantitative feedback shows that pedometers have provide insights that help therapists provide the appropriate
been very successful in encouraging sustained physical therapy to each patient.
exercise and movement in all populations, including low- Quantitative linguistic feedback may also be a valuable
income individuals (Clarke et al., 2007; Duru, Sarkisian, tool for the greater scientific community. As Roberts and
Leng, & Mangione, 2010; Strath et al., 2011). Adults are Kaiser (2011) noted, advancing the science of evidence-
rarely conscious of their own “word count,” even if they based behavioral interventions requires transparency of
understand its impact on their children’s development. With strategies used and quantifiable outcome measures of
LENA feedback technology, as with pedometers, they can parental behavioral uptake to quantify intervention dosage.
see the language environment they provide for their child, Quantitative linguistic feedback is a concrete behavioral
begin to understand their critical role, and make appropriate strategy that corroborates fidelity of implementation of
behavioral changes. Although the LENA feedback is not interventional strategies as well as gauges parental behav-
instantaneous like the pedometer, it gives adults unprece- ioral uptake. Although a current limitation of the linguistic
dented insight into and concrete appreciation of their child’s feedback is its lack of qualitative measurement, in future
language environment and overall development. studies, the LENA’s audio recordings may be analyzed for
However, an enriched early language environment qualitative changes as well. This pilot represents only the
requires more than a purely quantitative measure. It is a beginning in the iterative development and testing of this
complex interplay of rich qualitative language and respon- novel multifaceted approach. The results of this study sug-
sive behaviors. Linguistic feedback is designed to signifi- gest that the LENA has the potential to be a valuable addi-
cantly increase adult language quantity to provide a tion to the world of speech and language intervention.
foundation on which qualitative behavioral strategies may
be layered. It is the combination of quantity and quality in a
child’s early language environment that will lead to optimal Considerations
cognitive and educational outcomes. The study was limited in three areas: participant sample,
We view quantitative linguistic feedback not as a “silver potential Hawthorne effect, and variance accounted by the
bullet” but rather as a strategy to increase the effectiveness educational intervention. Although our sample size was
of behavioral interventions. As an effective addition to clin- small, we were encouraged by the ability to demonstrate a
ical therapy sessions, it can provide parents and therapists statistically significant increase in our selected adult lan-
additional proof of behavioral change. Analyses of LENA guage parameters. In addition, caregivers with and without
audio recordings can also provide greater insight into the a college education appeared responsive to the behavioral
qualitative changes. The LENA technology’s ease of use strategy. We plan to investigate with a larger sample size
and automated software processing make it ideal for inclu- the issues of external validity related to the inclusion of
sion in all caregiver-directed interventions involving lan- NPCs as opposed to parents. We hypothesize that parents
guage feedback. Although privacy protections maintained will be more invested and motivated for behavioral change
under IRB protocols for this pilot precluded qualitative when it is their own child who benefits.
analysis by requiring the deletion of audio recordings, An important issue to consider is that of overall linguis-
future projects will include such evaluation. tic implications, in terms of the sustainability of the linguis-
This pilot study has demonstrated the potential of quan- tic behavior change and the true nature of this effect on a
titative linguistic feedback, laying the groundwork for its child’s early language environment. There is the issue of the
continued iterative development as an evidence-based “Hawthorne effect,” which states that individuals positively
behavioral approach. Quantitative linguistic feedback may modify their behavior when they are knowingly being
also have an even greater role in areas such as individual observed. An individual’s knowledge that one is being
therapy. Because of the wide diversity of client populations recorded and will be receiving feedback may result in care-
and the limited insight therapists may have into individual givers increasing linguistic input only during the recording
home environments, they are often forced to use a general- times. Quantitative linguistic feedback will never entirely
ized therapeutic approach rather than one tailored to the avoid this issue, although sustaining increased talk for a
specific family. This is especially true regarding the child’s 10-hr recording day is much less likely than being on best
early home language environment. The LENA may allow behavior during the 1-hr videotaped session typical in most

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at UNIV OF WISCONSIN OSHKOSH on June 8, 2015


Suskind et al. 207

intervention studies. In addition, the NPCs in our study Ajzen, I. (1996). The directive influence of attitudes on behav-
knew that their employers would not have access to their ior. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychol-
results. To address this limitation, future studies will include ogy of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior
greater number of LENA recordings to habituate caregiver (pp. 385–403). New York, NY: Guilford.
response as well as LENA recordings without feedback to Ajzen, I. (2001). Nature and operation of attitudes. Annual Review
assess sustainability of behavioral change. of Psychology, 52, 27–58.
Finally, the 1-hr educational intervention and initial Ajzen, I., Brown, T. C., & Carvajal, F. (2004). Explaining the dis-
baseline LENA feedback were inseparable in this study. We crepancy between intentions and actions: The case of hypo-
are unable to isolate the impact of the LENA feedback from thetical bias in contingent valuation. Personality and Social
the educational session on NPC behavior recorded over the Psychology bulletin, 30, 1108–1121. doi:10.1177/014616720
six remaining recordings. However, the behavioral litera- 426407930/9/1108[pii]
ture has shown that lack of continued feedback results in Bandura, A. (1997). The anatomy of stages of changes. American
attenuation of effect. We suspect that quantitative linguistic Journal of Health Promotion, 12, 8–10.
feedback was responsible for the sustained changes in the Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Hahn, C. S., & Haynes,
AWC and CTC. To address this limitation, in future studies, O. M. (2008). Maternal responsiveness to young children at
we will experimentally manipulate the number of times three ages: Longitudinal analysis of a multidimensional, mod-
caregivers are provided with quantitative linguistic feed- ular, and specific parenting construct. Developmental Psychol-
back after the educational intervention. Future embedding ogy, 44, 867–874. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.867
of quantitative linguistic feedback into established interven- Clarke, K. K., Freeland-Graves, J., Klohe-Lehman, D. M., Milani,
tions, inclusion of a control group and analyses of child out- T. J., Nuss, H. J., & Laffrey, S. (2007). Promotion of physi-
comes will also help address these limitations and confirm cal activity in low-income mothers using pedometers. Jour-
the effects of intervention. nal of the American Dietetic Association, 107, 962–967.
doi:10.1016/j.jada.2007.03.010
DesJardin, J., & Eisenberg, L. S. (2007). Maternal contributions:
Conclusion Supporting language development in young children with
Enhancing a child’s early language environment represents cochlear implants. Ear and Hearing, 28, 456–469. doi:10.1097/
an extraordinary opportunity to affect the life trajectory of AUD.0b013e31806dc1ab00003446-200708000-00004[pii]
a child. Pilot testing of the innovative behavioral strategy— Duru, O. K., Sarkisian, C. A., Leng, M., & Mangione, C. M.
quantitative linguistic feedback—demonstrates its potential (2010). Sisters in motion: A randomized controlled trial of
for stimulating changes in adult language behavior. Because a faith-based physical activity intervention. Journal of the
of these encouraging results, we will continue the develop- American Geriatrics Society, 58, 1863–1869. doi:10.1111/
ment and study of this promising strategy. j.1532-5415.2010.03082.x
Forget-Dubois, N., & Dionne, G. (2009). Early child language
Declaration of Conflicting Interests mediates the relationship between home environment and
The author(s) declared the following potential conflict of interest school readiness. Child Development, 80, 736–749.
with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this Gilkerson, J., & Richards, J. A. (2008). The LENA foundation
article: The LENA recorders used in this study were on loan from natural language study (Tech. Rep). Boulder, CO: The LENA
the LENA Foundation. Foundation.
Giurolametto, L., Weltzman, E., & Greenberg, J. (2003). Training
Funding daycare staff to facilitate children’s language. American Jour-
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support nal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 299–311.
for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: The Giurolametto, L., Weltzman, E., & Greenberg, J. (2004). The effects
authors received internal funding from the Department of Surgery of verbal support strategies on small-group peer interactions. Lan-
at the University of Chicago as well as funding from the Hemera guage, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 35, 254–268.
Foundation. Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N., & Biddle, S. J. (2001). The
influence of self-efficacy and past behaviour on the physical
Note activity intentions of young people. Journal of Sports Sci-
1.  CALO-RE is an acronym for “Coventry, Aberdeen and London ences, 19, 711–725. doi:10.1080/02640410152475847
Refined.” Hammer, J., Tomblin, B., Zhang, X., & Weiss, A. (2001). Rela-
tionship between parenting behaviours and specific language
References impairment in children. International Journal of Language &
Abraham, C., & Michie, S. (2008). A taxonomy of behavior change Communication Disorders, 36, 185–205.
techniques used in interventions. Health Psychology, 27, 379–387. Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1992). American parenting of language
doi:2008-08834-010[pii]10.1037/0278-6133.27.3.379 learning children: Persisting differences in family-child inter-

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at UNIV OF WISCONSIN OSHKOSH on June 8, 2015


208 Communication Disorders Quarterly 34(4)

actions observed in natural home environments. Developmen- Michie, S., Ashford, S., Sniehotta, F. F., Dombrowski, S. U.,
tal Psychology, 28, 1096–1105. Bishop, A., & French, D. P. (2011). A refined taxonomy of
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the behaviour change techniques to help people change their phys-
everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, ical activity and healthy eating behaviours: The CALO-RE
MD: Paul H. Brookes. taxonomy. Psychology & Health, 26, 1479–1498. doi:938640
Hoff, E., & Tian, C. (2005). Socioeconomic status and cultural 058[pii]10.1080/08870446.2010.540664
influences on language. Journal of Communication Disorders, Michie, S., Fixsen, D., Grimshaw, J. M., & Eccles, M. P. (2009).
38, 271–278. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2005.02.003 Specifying and reporting complex behaviour change interven-
Hughes, M., & Demo, D. (1989). Self perceptions of black Ameri- tions: The need for a scientific method. Implementation Sci-
cans: Self-esteem and personal efficacy. American Journal of ence, 4, 40. doi:10.1186/1748-5908-4-40
Sociology, 95, 132–159. Michie, S., Jochelson, K., Markham, W. A., & Bridle, C. (2009).
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Selzer, B. A., & Lyons, T. (1991). Low-income groups and behaviour change interventions: A
Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gen- review of intervention content, effectiveness and theoretical
der. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236–248. frameworks. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health,
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S. 63, 610–622.
(2002). Language input and child syntax (Research Support, Michie, S., Johnston, M., Abraham, C., Lawton, R., Parker, D., &
Non-U.S. Gov’t). Cognitive Psychololgy, 45, 337–374. Walker, A. (2005). Making psychological theory useful for
Individuals With Disabilities Education Improvement Act of 2004, implementing evidence based practice: A consensus approach.
H.R. 1350, 108th Cong. (2003). BMJ Quality & Safety, 14, 26–33. doi:10.1136/qshc.2004.011155
Janz, N. K., Champion, V. L., & Stretcher, V. J. (2002). The health Michie, S., Johnston, M., Francis, J., Hardeman, W., & Eccles,
belief model. In K. Glanz, B. K. Rimer, & F. M. Lewis (Eds.), M. (2008). From theory to intervention: Mapping theoretically
Health behavior and health education: Theory, research, and derived behavioural determinants to behaviour change tech-
practice (pp. 45–66). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. niques. Applied Psychology, 57, 660–680. doi:10.1111/j.1464-
Johnston-Brooks, C. H., Lewis, M. A., & Garg, S. (2002). Self- 0597.2008.00341.x
efficacy impacts self-care and HbA1c in young adults with Milburn, T. (2011). The efficacy of a professional development
Type I diabetes. Psychosomatic Medicine Journals, 64, 43–51. program to enhance preschool educators’ ability to facili-
Kaiser, A., & Hancock, T. (2003a). Teaching parents new skills to tate conversation during shared book reading (Master’s the-
support their young children’s development. Infant & young sis). University of Toronto. Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.
children, 16, 9–21. net/1807/31341
Kaiser, A., & Hancock, T. (2003b). Teaching parents new skills to Oller, D. K. (2010). All-day recordings to investigate vocabu-
support their young children’s development. Infants & Young lary development: A case study of a trilingual toddler. Com-
Children, 16, 9–21. munication Disorders Quarterly, 31, 213–222. doi:10.1177/
Kashinath, S., Woods, J., & Goldstein, H. (2006). Enhancing gen- 1525740109358628
eralized teaching strategy use in daily routines by parents of Phillips, M. (2011). Parenting, time use, and disparities in aca-
children with autism. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hear- demic outcomes. In G. Duncan & R. Murnane (Eds.), Whither
ing Research, 49, 466–485. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2006/036) opportunity?: Rising inequality schools, and children’s life
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., & Guttentag, C. chances (pp. 207–228). New York, NY: Russell Sage.
(2008). A responsive parenting intervention: The opti- Pickston, C., Golbart, J., Marshall, J., Rees, A., & Roulstone, S.
mal timing across early childhood for impacting maternal (2009). A systematic review of environmental interventions to
behaviors and child outcomes. Developmental Psychology, improve child language for children with or at risk of primary
44, 1335–1353. language impairment. Journal of Research in Special Educa-
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., & Miller-Loncar, C. L. tional Needs, 9, 66–79.
(2000). Early maternal and child influences on children’s later Raikes, H., & Thompson, R. A. (2005). Efficacy and social sup-
independent cognitive and social functioning. Child Develop- port as predictors of parenting stress among families in pov-
ment, 71, 358–375. erty. Infant Mental Health Journal, 26, 177–190.
Law, J., & Rush, R. (2009). Modeling developmental language Reese, E., Sparks, A., & Leyva, D. (2010). A review of parent
difficulties from school entry into adulthood: Literacy, mental interventions for preschool children’s language and emergent
health, and employment outcomes. Journal of Speech, Lan- literacy. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 10, 97–117.
guage, and Hearing Research, 52, 1044–1092. Reilly, S., Wake, M., Ukoumunne, O. C., Bavin, E., Prior, M.,
McWilliam, R. A. (2001). Functional intervention planning: The Cini, E., & Bretherton, L. (2010). Predicting language out-
routines-based interview. Retrieved from http://www.wais- comes at 4 years of age: Findings from early language in Victo-
man.wisc.edu/birthto3/WPDP/rbi.pdf ria study. Pediatrics, 126, e1530–e1537. doi:peds.2010-0254
Meadow-Orlans, K. (1997). Effects of mother and infant hearing [pii]10.1542/peds.2010-0254
status on interactions at twelve and eighteen months. Journal Roberts, M. Y., & Kaiser, A. P. (2011). The effectiveness of parent-
of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2, 26–36. implemented language interventions: A meta-analysis. Ameri-

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at UNIV OF WISCONSIN OSHKOSH on June 8, 2015


Suskind et al. 209

can Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 20, 180–199. characteristics, selected years 1985-2010. In American’s Chil-
doi:10.1044/1058-0360(2011/10-0055)1058-0360_2011_10- dren: Key National Indicators of Well-Being 2011. Retrieved
0055[pii] May 3, 2012 from http://childstats.gov/americaschildren/
Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: Relation to socio- tables/fam3a.asp
economic status, knowledge of child development and child Vansteenkiste, M., & Sheldon, K. M. (2006). There’s nothing
vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35, 185–205. more practical than a good theory: Integrating motivational
Shape up America, Get up and go! 10,000 steps a day. (1994). interviewing and self-determination theory. British Journal of
Retrieved from http://www.shapeup.org/resources/10ksteps.html Clinical Psychology, 45, 63–82.
Snow, C. (1972). Mothers’ speech to children learning language. White, K. M., & Wellington, L. (2009). Predicting participation in
Child Development, 43, 549–565. group parenting education in an Australian sample: The role
Spencer, P., & Gutfreund, M. (1990). Oxford handbook of deaf of attitudes, norms, and control factors. Journal of Primary
studies, language, and education. Washington, DC: Gallaudet Prevention, 30, 173–189.
University Press. Wolf, M., Davis, T., Osborn, C., Skripkauskas, S., Bennett, C.,
Strath, S. J., Swartz, A. M., Parker, S. J., Miller, N. E., Grimm, E. K., & Makoul, G. (2007). Literacy, self-efficacy, and HIV medi-
& Cashin, S. E. (2011). A pilot randomized controlled trial cation adherence. Patient Education and Counseling, 65,
evaluating motivationally matched pedometer feedback to 253–260.
increase physical activity behavior in older adults. Journal of Wolfe, F., & Michaud, K. (2010). The Hawthorne effect, spon-
Physical Activity & Health, 8(Suppl. 2), S267–S274. sored trials, and the overestimation of treatment effectiveness.
Table FAM3.A. Child care: Primary child care arrange- Journal of Rheumatology, 37, 2216–2220. doi:10.3899/jrh
ments for children 0-4 with employed mothers by selected eum.100497

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at UNIV OF WISCONSIN OSHKOSH on June 8, 2015

You might also like