Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Body Image 6 (2009) 75–82

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Body Image
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bodyimage

Is BodyThink an efficacious body image and self-esteem program?


A controlled evaluation with adolescents
Shanel M. Richardson a, Susan J. Paxton a,*, Julie S. Thomson b
a
School of Psychological Science, La Trobe University Bundoora, Victoria 3086, Australia
b
The Butterfly Foundation, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Article history: This study aimed to evaluate the efficacy of BodyThink, a widely disseminated body image and self-
Received 11 July 2008 esteem program. Participants were 277, grade 7 students from 4 secondary schools in Australia. The
Received in revised form 6 November 2008 intervention group (62 girls, 85 boys) participated in BodyThink during four 50-min lessons, while the
Accepted 10 November 2008
control group (65 girls, 65 boys) received their usual classes. All participants completed baseline,
postintervention and 3-month follow-up questionnaires. For girls, the intervention group reported
Keywords: higher media literacy and lower internalization of the thin ideal compared to the control group. For boys,
Body image
the intervention group reported higher media literacy and body satisfaction than the control group.
Body dissatisfaction
Although some positive outcomes were observed, it would be valuable to find ways to enhance the
Self-esteem
Prevention intervention impact of BodyThink, especially in light of its wide dissemination. Suggestions for improving BodyThink
Adolescent girls and boys are presented.
ß 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction in the delivery of BodyThink to over 900 professionals and as of June


2008, 40,000 young people had participated in the program.
The importance of addressing the prevention of body dis- A large proportion of the content of BodyThink relates to media
satisfaction as a public health issue is increasingly being literacy and self-esteem. Media literacy is the provision of
recognised (Paxton, 2000). Body dissatisfaction has been found education on the media’s promotion of unrealistic standards of
to affect up to 70 percent of adolescent girls (Levine & Smolak, ‘beauty’ so that people learn to critically analyse media messages
2002) and 23 percent of adolescent boys (Bearman, Presnell, (Wilksch, Tiggemann, & Wade, 2006). It can be argued that those
Martinez, & Stice, 2006). In a recent survey of over 28,000 who have the skills to analyse media messages are less likely to be
Australians aged 11–24 years, body image was identified as their adversely affected by such messages and therefore less susceptible
greatest issue of concern (Mission Australia, 2007). Further, body to body dissatisfaction. In regard to self-esteem, it has been
dissatisfaction has been shown to predict the development of proposed that low self-esteem is a causal risk factor for body
depressed mood (Paxton, Neumark-Sztainer, Hannan, & Eisenberg, dissatisfaction and therefore improving self-esteem will contri-
2006b) and to be a risk and maintenance factor for the bute to a reduction in body dissatisfaction (Furnham, Badmin, &
development of eating disorders (Stice, 2002). This study aimed Sneade, 2002). Research evidence supports self-esteem as a risk
to evaluate BodyThink, a body image and self-esteem program for factor for body dissatisfaction in adolescent girls (Paxton,
young people. It is especially important to evaluate this specific Einsenberg, & Neumark-Sztainer, 2006a).
program as it is being very widely disseminated. In conjunction with media literacy and self-esteem, BodyThink
BodyThink was developed by the United Kingdom Eating also aims to address additional likely causal risk factors for the
Disorders Association, with input from the Dove Self-Esteem development of body dissatisfaction, including internalization of
Fund, The Butterfly Foundation, psychotherapists and the Girls the media ideal, body comparison and appearance teasing.
Scouts of the USA (Eating Disorders Association & Lever Faberge Internalization of the media ideal occurs when an individual
Limited, 2006). The program has been disseminated throughout accepts the ideal promoted by the media (i.e., a thin ideal for
schools in the United Kingdom (where it is called BodyTalk) (Dove, females and a muscular ideal for males) and evaluates his or her
2006). In Australia, The Butterfly Foundation has provided training body in relation to this ideal (Thompson, van den Berg, Roehrig,
Guarda, & Heinberg, 2004). It is argued that body dissatisfaction
results when an individual does not live up to the ideal (Stice,
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +61 3 94791736; fax: +61 3 9479 1956. 2001) and research evidence supports this for girls and boys (Jones,
E-mail address: susan.paxton@latrobe.edu.au (S.J. Paxton). Vigfusdottir, & Lee, 2004; Smolak, Levine, & Thompson, 2001).

1740-1445/$ – see front matter ß 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2008.11.001
76 S.M. Richardson et al. / Body Image 6 (2009) 75–82

Thus, it is proposed that reducing internalization of the media ideal months, SD = 5.58 months). One school was in a low socioeconomic
will also reduce body dissatisfaction. area, two in medium socioeconomic areas, and one in a medium–
Body comparison is the tendency for a person to evaluate his or high socioeconomic area. Classes of students were allocated by
her appearance by making comparisons with the appearance of teaching staff to either an intervention group (62 girls, 85 boys) or a
others (Schutz, Paxton, & Wertheim, 2002). Both girls and boys control group (65 girls, 65 boys). Class allocation was based on a
engage in body comparisons, which have been found to predict variety of reasons, which differed between schools. For instance, at
body dissatisfaction in adolescent girls (Jones, 2004). In light of one school the decision was based on teachers who volunteered
these findings, it is likely that reducing body comparisons could their class to participate in the intervention or control group,
assist in reducing body dissatisfaction. whereas at another schools class allocation was based on avai-
Appearance teasing has also been found to be a risk factor for lability of classes at particular times. Fourteen classes of students
body dissatisfaction in girls and boys (Jones et al., 2004). took part in the research and each school contributed at least one
BodyThink aims to reduce the incidence of adolescent appearance class to each group. For girls who reported their weight and height
teasing and also assist adolescents to cope with such teasing, (n = 79), 12.67% were below the 5th percentile for BMI for their age,
therefore reducing body dissatisfaction. Thus, BodyThink aims to 35.44% were between the 5th and 50th percentile, 41.77% were
reduce body dissatisfaction and prevent its development by between the 50th and 85th percentile, 8.86% were between the
intervening to reduce a number of different risk factors. 85th and 95th percentile and 1.27% were above the 95th
A number of considerations were taken into account when percentile. For boys who reported their weight and height
deciding on appropriate participants for this research. BodyThink (n = 114), 8.77% were below the 5th percentile for BMI for their
has been delivered in many different settings, including youth age, 37.72% were between the 5th and 50th percentile, 34.21%
groups and community health centres. However, schools are the were between the 50th and 85th percentile, 14.04% were between
most common location for its delivery in Australia and thus schools the 85th and 95th percentile and 5.26% were above the 95th
were selected as the best evaluation location. percentile. Participants were born in a variety of countries, with
BodyThink has been delivered to young people between the ages the most common being Australia (83.80%), China (1.4%), England
of 10–17 years, with the most common age group of participants (1.4%), New Zealand (1.4%) and Sudan (1.4%).
being 13–15 years. Grade 7 students (approximate range 11–13
years) were selected for this study for a number of reasons. First, Assessment instruments
grade 7 is generally the first year of secondary schooling in Victoria,
Australia and consequently it is likely that these adolescents are Media literacy
making new friendships and establishing group norms. It was Media literacy was assessed by evaluating knowledge about
hoped that addressing issues of body image and self-esteem during digital manipulation of images, lighting, camera shoots and the
this time would contribute to students developing attitudes and effect of media images on feelings. Participants were asked to
behaviors that foster positive body image and self-esteem amongst respond to four statements (‘Before pictures of models are
peer groups. Second, it has been argued that interventions should published in magazines they are digitally manipulated to make
be implemented before individuals are likely to develop eating the models look perfect’, ‘Lighting can be used to make a model
disorders (Paxton, 1993), which most often occurs in adolescents look scarier’, ‘The camera never lies’ and ‘There is no evidence to
between 16 and 17 years (Lewinsohn, Striegel-Moore, & Seeley, suggest that media images have an effect on the way people feel
2000). Finally, this age group is likely to be experiencing puberty about themselves’) on a 5-point scale from Strongly disagree (1) to
(Pinyerd & Zipf, 2005), which has been implicated in advancing the Strongly agree (5). Analysis of internal consistency indicated that
genetic influences on disordered eating (Klump, Perkins, Burt, the items were measuring different constructs so responses to each
McGue, & Iacono, 2007). item are presented separately. Items were scored so that higher
Both girls and boys participated in this evaluation as both scores corresponded with greater media literacy.
genders are affected by body dissatisfaction, they both contribute
to the social environments of the other gender and BodyThink was Risk factors for body dissatisfaction
specifically designed to address issues relevant to girls and boys. Self-esteem was assessed using the 10-item General Scale of the
BodyThink has been designed to be flexible in regard to the time Self-Description Questionnaire II (Marsh, 1992). Items (e.g., ‘Overall,
taken to deliver the program. In this study, BodyThink was I have a lot to be proud of’) are rated on a 6-point scale ranging from
delivered over four 50-min sessions. This decision was based on False (1) to True (6). Modelled on Rosenberg’s Self-esteem Inventory
practical limitations and previous research that indicates that (Rosenberg, 1965), this scale was designed specifically for use with
multiple session interventions are more effective than single adolescents and has been shown to produce reliable and valid scores
session interventions (Stice & Shaw, 2004). in adolescent samples (Marsh). In the current study internal
In summary, the current study aimed to evaluate the efficacy of consistency was high (Girls: a = .91; Boys a = .87).
BodyThink, a widely disseminated body image and self-esteem For girls, internalization of the thin ideal was assessed using the
program. It was hypothesised that those who participated in 14-item internalization subscale of the Sociocultural Attitudes
BodyThink, compared to participants in the control group, would Toward Appearance Questionnaire (Heinberg, Thompson, &
report more desirable outcomes on measures of media literacy, Stormer, 1995). Items (e.g., ‘I believe clothes look better on
self-esteem, internalization of the media ideal, body comparisons, thinner models’) are rated on a 5-point scale ranging from Strongly
appearance teasing and body image. Exploratory analyses were disagree (1) to Strongly agree (5). The subscale has good
also conducted to assess if the program had any impact on eating psychometric properties (Jones et al., 2004; Smolak et al., 2001)
disorder symptoms. and internal consistency in this study was high (a = .87). For boys,
to assess internalization of the muscular ideal, a modified version
Method of this subscale was used (Smolak et al.). The 14-items (e.g., ‘I
believe that clothes look better on muscular men’) are rated in the
Participants same manner as the original version. This subscale has been found
to have sound psychometric properties for adolescent boys (Jones
Participants were 277, grade 7 students from four public et al.; Smolak et al.) and in this study internal consistency was
secondary schools in Melbourne, Australia (M age = 12 years 8 adequate (a = .85).
S.M. Richardson et al. / Body Image 6 (2009) 75–82 77

Body comparison tendency in girls and boys was assessed using tion (e.g., ‘I think my biceps are too small’), as opposed to the
the 5-item Physical Appearance Comparison Scale (Thompson, original version that focuses on common areas of female body
Heinberg, & Tantleff, 1991), which has been shown to produce dissatisfaction (e.g., ‘I think my hips are too big’). For both versions,
reliable and valid scores in samples of adolescents (Durkin & items are rated on a 6-point scale ranging from Never (1) to Always
Paxton, 2002; Schutz et al., 2002). Items (e.g., ‘At parties or other (6). These subscales were scored based on the recommendations
social events, I compare my physical appearance to the physical for nonclinical samples (Schoemaker, van Strien, & van der Staak,
appearance of others’) are rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1994). Both measures have been shown to produce reliable and
Never (1) to Always (5). In this study, one item (i.e., ‘Comparing valid scores in adolescent samples (Jones, 2004; Jones et al., 2004)
your looks to the looks of others is a bad way to determine if you and internal consistency was adequate in this study (Girls: a = .91;
are attractive or unattractive’) produced a very low item total Boys: a = .79).
correlation at each time point (range .01 to .03) and was Dietary restraint was assessed with the Eating Disorder
therefore not included in the scale. The consequent internal Examination Questionnaire Restraint subscale (Fairburn & Beglin,
consistency was adequate (Girls: a = .87; Boys: a = .72). 1994). Items (e.g., ‘On how many days out of the past 28 days have
Appearance teasing was assessed using the 6-item Weight you been deliberately trying to limit the amount of food you eat to
Teasing subscale of the Perception of Teasing Scale (Thompson, influence your weight or shape’) are rated on a 7-point scale
Cattarin, Fowler, & Fisher, 1995). Items (e.g., ‘People made fun of ranging from No days to Every day. This 5-item subscale has been
you because you are heavy’) are rated on a 5-point scale ranging shown to provide reliable and valid scores in samples of
from Never (1) to Very often (5). This subscale has been shown to adolescents (Wade, Davidson, & O’Dea, 2003). Internal consistency
provide reliable and valid scores in samples of adolescents (Lunner was adequate in this study (Girls: a = .83; Boys: a = .85).
et al., 2000; Schutz et al., 2002). Internal consistency was high in Bulimic symptoms (e.g., binging, purging) were assessed with
this study (Girls: a = .93; Boys: a = .94). the Eating Disorder Inventory Bulimia subscale (Garner et al.,
1983). This 7-item subscale (e.g., ‘I stuff myself with food’) is rated
Body image and eating disorder symptoms on a 6-point scale ranging from Never (1) to Always (6). The
Body satisfaction was assessed using an adapted version of the untransformed scoring system was used, which has been shown to
Body Satisfaction Visual Analogue Scale (Durkin & Paxton, 2002). provide reliable and valid scores in samples of nonclinical
The original version has 1 item (‘Feel satisfied with your body’), adolescents (Schoemaker et al., 1994; Schutz et al., 2002). Internal
while the adapted scale has an additional 4-items (e.g., ‘Feel satisfied consistency was moderate in this study (Girls: a = .74; Boys:
with your weight’, ‘Feel comfortable with your body shape’, ‘Feel a = .89).
satisfied with your appearance’, ‘Feel like an attractive person’).
Participants respond ‘how they feel right now’ by marking a 100 mm Feedback on BodyThink
horizontal line anchored by Not at all and Very much so. Individual Intervention participants were asked to use a 5-point rating
responses were measured to the nearest millimetre producing a scale ranging from Strongly disagree (1) to Strongly agree (5), to
numerical score on a 100-point scale. Numerical scores from all provide feedback on relevance, interest, enjoyment, comfort and
items were summed to create a scale total. The original scale has honesty during the BodyThink sessions. Participants were also
been shown to produce reliable and valid scores with adolescent asked to comment on their favourite thing, least favourite thing,
girls (Durkin & Paxton) and internal consistency was found to be what they would change and to provide additional comments if
adequate in this study (Girls: a = .91; Boys: a = .83). desired.
For girls, body dissatisfaction was assessed using the 9-item
Body Dissatisfaction subscale of the Eating Disorder Inventory BodyThink program
(Garner, Olmstead, & Polivy, 1983). For boys, a modified version of
the EDI-Body Dissatisfaction subscale (Jones, 2004) was adminis- Table 1 provides an outline of BodyThink’s aims and activities.
tered, which focused on common areas of male body dissatisfac- BodyThink is an interactive, participant focused program. Topics

Table 1
An outline of BodyThink’s aims and activities.

Aims Activities

Develop an understanding of body image and Class discussion—‘What is body image and self-esteem?’
self-esteem
Power-point presentation—definitions of body image and self-esteem
Develop awareness that people can have positive body Narrative presented by facilitator—the body satisfaction and self-esteem of identical twins
image and self-esteem regardless of appearance
Increase body satisfaction and self-esteem Individual worksheets & class discussion—Brainstorm ways to increase body satisfaction and self-esteem
Develop an understanding of factors that impact on Class brainstorm & power-point presentation—‘What factors impact on body image and self-esteem?’
body image and self-esteem
Increase media literacy, and reduce internalization of Class discussion—‘How do media images make people feel?’
the media ideal and body comparison
Small group activity & class discussion—‘How do models in magazines feel?’
DVD, power-point presentation, small group activity, individual worksheet & class discussion—the
fabrication of ‘beauty’ and its impact
Develop an understanding of the qualities people Class discussion—‘What makes people beautiful?’
appreciate about others
Increase self-esteem, decrease appearance teasing and Class discussion—‘What is the impact of appearance teasing?’
improve peer communications
Small group brainstorm—‘What positive things can you do if you are teased?’
Class discussion—the impact of compliments
Activity in pairs & class discussion—providing written compliments to partner and self
Class activity—participants provide ribbons to each other that represent positive qualities
Encourage the continuation of activities that have a Class brainstorm & individual worksheet—‘What can I do to continue to assist my body image and self-
positive impact on body image and self-esteem esteem?’
78 S.M. Richardson et al. / Body Image 6 (2009) 75–82

addressed include, ‘What is body image and self-esteem?’ ‘Factors of participants considered high risk and the final third of
that impact on body image and self-esteem’ and ‘Ways to increase participants considered low risk. Similarly, high and low risk
body satisfaction and self-esteem’. Through the use of materials groups for self-esteem were determined by conducting a tertial
such as power-point presentations, magazines, worksheets and a split using T1 self-esteem scores, with the first third considered
DVD, participants are encouraged to work together to develop low risk and the final third considered high risk.
strategies that empower them to improve their own body image Quantitative feedback was analysed by calculating percentages
and self-esteem. BodyThink content is relevant to both girls and for each response. To assess if there were any gender differences
boys, for instance magazine pictures utilised are of both female and for BodyThink feedback the quantitative scores were treated as a
male models. continuous scale and were examined using t-tests. For qualitative
feedback, the first author reviewed all responses and identified
Procedure common themes. Responses were then grouped according to these
themes. An independent rater was provided with the themes and a
Ethics approval was gained from La Trobe University and the random selection of responses and was asked to categorise the
Department of Education. The evaluation of BodyThink was responses according to the themes. Inter-rater reliability between
conducted by SR and SP with no financial or other support from the two raters was moderate to high (weighted kappa = .57).
The Butterfly Foundation or Dove, with complete independence
and no conflict of interest. JT and DW, who facilitated the program Results
in classrooms, are both employees of The Butterfly Foundation.
After active informed consent was received from both participants Participant characteristics
and a parent/guardian, participants completed baseline question-
naires (T1) in a classroom setting. Participants in the intervention All participants completed T1, 266 participants (96.03%)
group then participated in BodyThink. The BodyThink program was completed T2 and 258 participants (93.14%) completed T3
delivered during health classes at two schools, during physical questionnaires. Analyses indicated that there were no significant
education classes at another school and during a different class differences between participants who returned every question-
each time at the fourth school. Each of the four 50 min BodyThink naire compared to participants who did not in regard to gender,
sessions were generally delivered approximately 1 week apart. age, BMI, country of birth, group (intervention vs. control) or T1
Approximately a week after the completion of BodyThink all variables. The only significant difference found between the
participants completed the Time 2 (T2) questionnaires. All intervention and control groups at T1 was for girls on the media
participants were re-assessed approximately three months later literacy item ‘The camera never lies’, with the intervention group
(M = 93.6days, SD = 34.6days) at Time 3 (T3). reporting higher scores.

Statistical analyses Gender differences at baseline

Gender differences at baseline were examined using t-tests. As Analyses revealed that girls reported significantly higher body
previous research has found that girls and boys respond differently comparison (t(219) = 4.47, p < .001, h2 = .069) (girls: M = 9.63,
to intervention programs (Wilksch et al., 2006), analyses were SD = 4.06; boys: M = 7.69, SD = 2.87), lower body satisfaction
conducted separately for girls and boys to examine intervention (t(231) = 3.49, p = .001, h2 = .043) (girls: M = 291.46, SD = 121.11;
effects. Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) was utilised for such boys: M = 337.40, SD = 92.12), higher body dissatisfaction
analyses because Stice and Shaw (2004) recommend ANCOVA as (t(218) = 3.74, p < .001, h2 = .051) (girls: M = 27.88, SD = 9.91;
an appropriate inferential test of intervention effects. Partial eta boys: M = 23.81, SD = 7.16), and higher dietary restraint scores
squared (hp2) is the effect size reported for all ANCOVAs. Most (t(205) = 3.51, p = .001, h2 = .044) (girls: M = 5.27, SD = 6.78; boys:
variables met assumptions of ANCOVA, although transformations M = 2.79, SD = 4.36) compared to the boys. No significant gender
were required to improve the normality of self-esteem, appearance differences were found for the media literacy items, self-esteem,
teasing and dietary restraint scores. internalization of media ideals, appearance teasing or bulimic
In order to compare intervention and the control groups T2 symptoms.
scores after controlling for T1 scores, ANCOVAs were conducted in
which the T1 scores for the relevant variable was entered as the Examination of the effects of the intervention
covariate. Similarly, in order to compare intervention and control
group T3 scores ANCOVAs were conducted on T3 scores in which Analyses were conducted to assess if there were significant
T1 of the relevant variable was entered as the covariate. The alpha differences in scores on media literacy, risk factors for body
level was set at p < .01 for all analyses. Analyses reported in this dissatisfaction, body image and eating disorder symptoms
paper were conducted using the intention to treat method, as this between intervention and control participants at T2 and T3, after
technique is a conservative way to analyse the results helping to controlling for T1 (see Tables 2 and 3).
prevent the over-estimation of the efficacy of the intervention. For girls, the intervention group reported significantly higher
The ANCOVA homogeneity of the regression slope assumption media literacy scores than the control group on three of the four
was violated for a small number of variables (Girls: T3 ‘Digital media literacy items at T2 and T3. At T2 the effect sizes were large,
manipulation’ and T3 ‘Effect of media images on feelings’; Boys: T2 while at T3 they were moderate-large. Furthermore, the inter-
self-esteem). In these cases an ANOVA was performed using vention group reported significantly lower internalization of the
difference scores (i.e., T1–T2 or T1–T3) because such an approach is thin ideal scores at T2, compared to the control group. This effect
recommended as an alternative to ANCOVA (Tabachnick & Fidell, size was moderate. Although the differences were not significant
2001). When ANOVAs or t-tests were used the effect sizes reported there was a trend towards the intervention group reporting higher
are eta squared (h2). self-esteem scores at T2 and T3, with small-moderate effect sizes.
ANCOVAs were also conducted to assess if the program had Also, there was a trend towards the intervention group reporting
differential effects depending on participants level of risk. High and lower body comparisons scores at T2 and T3, with small-moderate
low risk groups for body image were determined by conducting a effect sizes evident. No significant differences were found between
tertial split using T1 body dissatisfaction scores, with the first third the groups at either T2 or T3 on the other variables assessed (i.e.,
S.M. Richardson et al. / Body Image 6 (2009) 75–82 79

Table 2
Means (standard deviations), univariate F values and effect sizes for girls.

Scale T1 T2 T3
range
Intervention M Control M Intervention M Control M F ES Intervention M Control M F ES
(SD) (SD) (SD) (SD) (SD) (SD)

Digital manipulationa 1–5 3.90 (.95) 3.68 (.94) 4.56 (.84) 3.63 (1.14) 22.39*** .160 4.53 (.80) 3.79 (1.18) 8.20** .068
Lightinga 1–5 3.73 (.93) 3.66 (.80) 4.42 (.89) 3.73 (.97) 22.70*** .160 4.26 (1.00) 3.55 (.98) 7.13** .060
Camera shoota 1–5 3.82 (.93) 3.44 (1.01) 4.36 (.91) 3.35 (1.12) 19.85** .140 4.33 (.80) 3.57 (1.11) 11.74** .095
Effect of media images 1–5 3.19 (.94) 3.44 (1.01) 3.54 (1.06) 3.38 (.83) 2.84 .023 3.50 (1.06) 3.40 (.79) 1.68 .015
on feelingsa
Self-esteema 11–66 47.17 (8.89) 47.89 (8.11) 50.11 (7.48) 47.93 (8.90) 6.92y .053 50.66 (9.05) 48.69 (7.73) 5.98y .046
Internalization of 14–70 37.84 (10.18) 38.98 (10.85) 33.77 (8.33) 37.80 (9.76) 8.62** .065 34.31 (10.79) 37.54 (10.88) 3.42 .027
thin idealb
Body comparisonb 5–20 9.50 (3.96) 9.75 (4.18) 7.90 (3.34) 9.38 (4.30) 6.41y .050 8.15 (3.93) 9.49 (3.98) 4.23y .034
Appearance teasingb 6–30 7.80 (2.80) 7.42 (2.98) 7.96 (3.62) 8.12 (4.59) .84 .007 7.63 (3.01) 7.64 (3.18) .80 .006
Body satisfactiona 0–500 278.91 (128.20) 303.23 (113.80) 326.16 (114.81) 309.75 (116.87) .08 .025 319.81 (114.37) 329.89 (116.14) .07 .001
Body dissatisfactionb 9–54 28.27 (11.06) 27.51 (8.79) 26.62 (9.66) 27.67 (9.84) 1.47 .013 26.35 (8.94) 26.31(9.56) <.001 <.001
Dietary restraintb 0–30 4.93 (5.64) 5.58 (7.72) 4.03 (6.60) 5.03 (6.83) 2.54 .020 4.00 (6.55) 4.63 (6.44) 1.11 .009
Bulimic symptomsb 7–42 11.33 (4.09) 11.59 (4.77) 10.48 (4.77) 11.66 (5.80) 1.31 .012 10.09 (4.90) 12.02 (5.82) 2.70 .026

Note. Effect sizes of .01 = small, .06 = moderate, .14 = large (Cohen, 1988).
a
Higher scores are more desirable.
b
Lower scores are more desirable.
**
p < .01
***
p < .001.
y
p < .05

Table 3
Means (standard deviations), univariate F values and effect sizes for boys.

Scale T1 T2 T3
Range
Intervention M Control M Intervention M Control M F ES Intervention M Control M F ES
(SD) (SD) (SD) (SD) (SD) (SD)

Digital manipulationa 1–5 3.48 (.92) 3.63 (1.01) 4.24 (1.12) 3.78 (1.05) 9.75** .064 4.28 (.89) 3.69 (1.08) 18.23*** .120
Lightinga 1–5 3.48 (.88) 3.82 (.95) 3.98 (1.06) 3.81 (1.15) 3.23 .022 3.93 (1.05) 3.74 (1.11) 10.79** .074
Camera shoota 1–5 3.49 (1.18) 3.62 (1.25) 4.13 (1.22) 3.92 (1.15) 1.90 .013 4.06 (1.04) 3.67 (1.30) 4.68y .034
Effect of media images 1–5 3.16 (.92) 3.09 (1.04) 3.78 (1.08) 3.35 (1.05) 5.84y .039 3.32 (1.08) 3.28 (.90) .028 <.001
on feelingsa
Self-esteema 11–66 46.95 (7.68) 48.80 (7.12) 48.98 (8.38) 50.85 (6.43) .020 <.001 48.59 (8.42) 49.14 (7.70) 1.50 .010
Internalization of 14–70 35.69 (8.82) 35.14 (7.79) 31.52 (9.43) 31.55 (9.42) .16 .001 29.92 (10.31) 32.20 (10.23) 3.41 .023
muscular idealb
Body comparisonb 5–20 7.88 (3.03) 7.45 (2.68) 7.84 (3.58) 7.49 (3.22) 2.09 .014 7.84 (3.65) 7.23 (2.74) 2.07 .014
Appearance teasingb 6–30 8.01 (3.46) 7.60 (3.32) 7.84 (3.58) 7.49 (3.22) <.001 <.001 7.84 (3.65) 7.23 (2.74) .43 .003
Body satisfactiona 0–500 332.96 (93.23) 343.30 (91.02) 377.82 (95.63) 352.38 (89.20) 7.91** .051 379.04 (99.02) 353.57 (83.95) 7.40** .048
Body dissatisfactionb 9–54 24.30 (6.95) 23.17 (7.45) 21.74 (7.69) 22.48 (7.28) 1.02 .008 21.50 (7.85) 22.69 (7.50) .99 .008
Dietary restraintb 0–30 2.88 (4.44) 2.69 (4.27) 2.11 (3.47) 2.58 (5.02) .18 .001 1.46 (2.91) 2.42 (4.39) 3.81 .026
Bulimic symptomsb 7–42 12.20 (6.36) 11.33 (5.38) 11.44 (5.80) 10.53 (5.26) .86 .006 10.70 (4.87) 10.41 (5.21) .031 <.001

Note. Effect sizes of .01 = small, .06 = moderate, .14 = large (Cohen, 1988).
a
Higher scores are more desirable.
b
Lower scores are more desirable.
**
p < .01
***
p < .001.
y
p < .05

effect of media images on feelings, appearance teasing, body internalization of the muscular ideal, body comparison, appear-
satisfaction, body dissatisfaction, dietary restraint and bulimic ance teasing, body dissatisfaction, dietary restraint and bulimic
symptoms). Also, no significant differences were found between symptoms). As with the girls, no significant differences were found
the intervention and control groups for high risk or low risk (when between the intervention and control groups for high risk or low
risk status was classified according to T1 body dissatisfaction or T1 risk participants on body dissatisfaction at T2 or T3. Notably at no
self-esteem) participants on body dissatisfaction at T2 or T3. point, for girls or boys, did control participants report significantly
For boys, the intervention compared to the control group more desirable outcomes than the intervention participants.
reported significantly higher media literacy for ‘digital manipula-
tion’ at T2, and for ‘digital manipulation’ and ‘lighting’ at T3. These Feedback on BodyThink
effects were moderate-large. The intervention group reported
higher body satisfaction scores, than the control group at both T2 Table 4 presents intervention participants’ feedback on
and T3, with small-moderate effects demonstrated at both time- relevance, interest, enjoyment, comfort and honesty during the
points. Although the differences were not significant there was a sessions. The majority of girls agreed or strongly agreed that they
trend towards the intervention group reporting greater knowledge found BodyThink interesting and enjoyable and felt that they could
on ‘effect of media images on feelings’ at T2 with a small-moderate be honest discussing opinions during the sessions. The majority of
effect size and on ‘camera shoot’ at T3 with a small-moderate effect boys reported that they found BodyThink interesting. No significant
size. No significant differences were found between the groups at differences were found between the feedback provided by girls and
either T2 or T3 on the other variables assessed (i.e., self-esteem, boys for these quantitative items.
80 S.M. Richardson et al. / Body Image 6 (2009) 75–82

Table 4
Intervention participants’ feedback on BodyThink reported in percentages.

Girls (n = 59) Boys (n = 82)

Strongly Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly Strongly Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly
disagree agree disagree agree

I felt comfortable discussing the issues in a group 3.39 5.08 44.07 30.51 16.95 10.97 9.76 43.90 24.40 10.97
with my classmates
I felt that I could be honest about my opinions 1.70 11.87 28.81 32.20 25.42 6.10 8.54 40.24 29.27 15.85
I felt the sessions were relevant to me 13.56 6.78 44.07 23.73 11.86 10.98 18.29 41.46 18.29 10.98
I felt that the sessions were interesting 5.09 6.78 25.42 38.98 23.73 7.32 9.76 32.92 29.27 20.73
I felt that the sessions were enjoyable 5.09 5.09 32.20 27.12 30.50 6.10 4.88 42.68 26.83 19.51

Table 5
Frequencies (percentages) and examples of, the thee most common themes reported by participants who responded to the questions on their favourite thing (Girls n = 59;
Boys n = 72), least favourite thing (Girls n = 58; Boys n = 68), what they would change (Girls n = 59; Boys n = 75) and any additional comments (Girls n = 32; Boys n = 30)
related to BodyThink.

Girls Boys Total


n (%) n (%) n (%)

Favourite thing
The ribbon activity. e.g., ‘‘Getting the coloured ribbons from your classmates. It makes you understand 24 (40.68) 8 (11.11) 32 (24.43)
just how special you are.’’

Activities that related to the manipulation of media images


e.g., ‘‘I like seeing just how much models get changed for a photo. It proved what I thought happened.’’ 6 (10.17) 26 (36.11) 32 (24.43)

Expressing opinions.
e.g., ‘‘My favourite thing about the session was that you could express your opinion freely.’’ 5 (8.47) 8 (11.11) 13 (9.92)

Least favourite thing


I didn’t have a least favourite thing. e.g., ‘‘Sorry I don’t have a least favourite thing I liked it all!’’ 22 (37.93) 23 (33.82) 45 (35.71)

Completing work.
e.g., ‘‘My least favourite thing was the worksheets.’’ 6 (10.34) 18 (26.47) 24 (19.05)

Too much talking or sitting


e.g., ‘‘My least favourite part of the lessons were just sitting down and having to listen.’’ 7 (12.07) 3 (4.41) 10 (7.94)

Change
I wouldn’t change anything. e.g., ‘‘Nothing, it is great as it is.’’ 18 (30.51) 30 (40.00) 48 (35.82)

Make BodyThink more interactive, for instance increase the number of activities or games.
e.g., ‘‘I would have more dvd’s and a bigger slide show. I would get them involved a lot more.’’ 15 (25.42) 20 (26.67) 35 (26.12)

Increase the duration


e.g., ‘‘Making more sessions because that would help us more.’’ 5 (8.47) 3 (4.00) 8 (5.97)

Additional comments
The program was a positive experience. e.g., ‘‘It was good because it helped me see that I’m as pretty as anyone else.’’ 9 (28.13) 16 (53.33) 25 (40.32)
I’d change the program in some way. e.g., ‘‘I’d wished it could have been longer.’’ 8 (25.00) 10 (33.33) 18 (29.03)
I learnt something new and/or enjoyed myself. e.g., ‘‘BodyThink was fun, to learn about how the camera always 10 (31.25) 3 (10.00) 13 (20.97)
lies and how thinking positive is the best thinking’’

Table 5 presents the three most common themes reported by cents. At post-test, girls in the intervention group, compared to the
participants related to their favourite thing, least favourite thing, control group, had more desirable media literacy and internaliza-
what they would change and any additional comments. The ribbon tion of the thin ideal scores. At follow-up the difference between
activity, activities that involved the manipulation of media images the intervention and control groups was still evident for the media
and expressing opinions were the most commonly reported literacy items. Although not significant, trends suggest that
favourite themes. Participants reporting that they did not have a differences may have emerged between the intervention and
least favourite thing and that they would not change the program control groups if there was more statistical power on the measures
were the most common themes for the next two questions of self-esteem and body comparison as small-moderate effects
respectively. The other two least favourite themes were complet- were evident for these variables at both time-points. The large-
ing work and too much talking or sitting, while suggested changes moderate effects demonstrated for the media literacy items
included increasing the interactivity and the program’s duration. suggests that they represent a meaningful change in the girls’
The most common theme for additional comments was that the understanding about the ways in which images in the media are
program was a positive experience. Participants also used this manipulated. Although the findings suggest that the program may
opportunity to provide information on how the program could be have had an impact on self-esteem, internalization of the thin ideal
improved or to indicate that they learnt something or enjoyed the and body comparison, these effects were not as strong and thus
program. require further investigation. For girls, the results did not support
the program having any impact on body image or eating disorder
Discussion symptoms.
For boys, at post-test the intervention compared to control
This study aimed to evaluate the efficacy of BodyThink, a widely group had more desirable outcomes for ‘digital manipulation’ and
disseminated body image and self-esteem program for adoles- body satisfaction. At follow-up the intervention group reported
S.M. Richardson et al. / Body Image 6 (2009) 75–82 81

more desirable outcomes for ‘digital manipulation’ ‘lighting’ and to lose weight, BMI and depression as risk factors for body
body satisfaction. Trends suggest that differences may have dissatisfaction, while at other times findings do not support such
emerged between the groups if there was more statistical power factors (Barker & Galambos, 2003; Paxton et al., 2006a; Presnell,
for ‘effect of media images on feelings’ at T2 and ‘camera shoot’ at Bearman, & Stice, 2004; Ricciardelli, McCabe, Ball, & Mellor, 2004).
T3. The effect sizes for the media literacy items ranged from small- The current evaluation of BodyThink and the limited findings of
large and were inconsistent across the time-points suggesting that other curriculum-based co-educational interventions delivered in
the results were not as reliable for the boys as they were for the schools highlights the need for greater research in the area and
girls. The difference between the groups for body satisfaction was contrasts with the recent success of female only interventions (e.g.,
small-moderate, but a corresponding difference in body dissatis- Richardson & Paxton, 2008; Stice, Chase, Stromer, & Appel, 2001;
faction was not observed. This discrepancy in body image findings Stice, Trost, & Chase, 2003; Stice, Shaw, Burton, & Wade, 2006;
may have been because the measure of body satisfaction was more Stice, Marti, Spoor, Presnell, & Shaw, 2008). A broader school-wide
general (e.g., weight, body, and appearance) while the body ecological approach for co-educational settings may be more
dissatisfaction measure asked about specific body parts (e.g., desirable and research supporting such a suggestion is beginning
biceps, shoulders, and chest). It is possible that participation in to emerge (e.g., McVey, Tweed, & Blackmore, 2007).
BodyThink impacted on boys on a macro level, helping improve It is very encouraging to see dissemination of appealing
overall feelings about appearance (i.e., I feel better about my body), interventions designed to prevent the serious problem of body
but did not impact on a micro level (i.e., I am still unhappy with my dissatisfaction in adolescents. However, while BodyThink may play
biceps). Such results require further investigation. For boys, no a role in improving media literacy, in light of limited resources for
support was found for the program having any impact on risk prevention it is important to explore further ways to maximize its
factors for body dissatisfaction or eating disorder symptoms. impact. Enhancements to BodyThink which address a wider range
The program did not appear to have any differential effects of risk factors for body dissatisfaction may be valuable to explore
depending on participants’ levels of risk for girls or boys, whether and requires further research.
risk was classified according to T1 body dissatisfaction or T1 self- A strength of the current study was that it involved girls and boys,
esteem. Finally, there was no evidence to suggest that BodyThink as few studies have evaluated body image interventions with both
had any iatrogenic effects on girls or boys. genders (e.g., Buddeberg-Fischer et al., 1998; Dalle Grave et al., 2001;
Outcomes for this study are similar to other evaluations of Kater et al., 2000; McVey et al., 2007; O’Dea & Abraham, 2000; Wade
school-based co-educational body image intervention studies. It et al., 2003; Wilksch et al., 2006). Wilksch and colleagues have
appears that studies that have included measures of intervention suggested that educating boys about the appearance pressures
topic knowledge have found effects (Dalle Grave, De Luca, & placed on girls may lead to a reduction in the amount of appearance-
Campello, 2001; Kater, Rohwer, & Levine, 2000). However, an effect based comments they make to girls. Although the results of this
on risk factors for body dissatisfaction, body image and eating study provided no evidence to suggest that girls perceived a
disorder symptoms occurs less consistently. For instance, Dalle reduction in appearance teasing, it is possible that participation in
Grave and colleagues’ intervention appeared to have an impact on the program may result in boys communicating more appropriately
eating disorder symptoms at post-intervention, but this effect was with girls on appearance related issues.
not maintained at follow-up and the program did not have any Another strength of the study was the inclusion of a follow-up
impact on self-esteem. Similarly, while Wade et al. (2003) media period which has been absent from some similar evaluations (e.g.,
literacy intervention appeared to have an impact on weight Kater et al., 2000; Wilksch et al., 2006) and allows some
concern at post-intervention, this effect was not evident at follow- investigation of the medium-term impact of the program. This
up and the results indicated that the program did not have any research could be further strengthened by an extended follow-up
impact on body satisfaction or dietary restraint. In addition, period as it is not clear for how long the differences between
Buddeberg-Fischer, Klaghofer, Gnam, and Buddeberg (1998) three groups would have been evident, or indeed if greater differences
session intervention did not appear to have an impact on any of the may have emerged over time.
dependant variables assessed, which all related to eating disorder Our findings need to be considered in light of methodological
symptoms. While Kater et al. (2000) and Wilksch et al. (2006) limitations. A notable limitation was non-random allocation of
found some favourable outcomes when evaluating their interven- classes to groups by teachers, which may have introduced a
tions, they did not include control groups in their research, making sampling bias. Efforts were made to control for any pre-
it difficult to interpret the results. intervention group differences by using T1 variables as covariates,
The results of this research and other evaluations of curriculum- but any replication of this research would benefit from random
based co-educational interventions indicate that there is scope for allocation. Also, having intervention and control participants from
improving such programs and a need for more research. In regard the same school can be viewed as a limitation because such a
to BodyThink, a smarter application of the research literature may design provides the opportunity for intervention participants to
be beneficial. For instance, although it is advantageous that share what they learnt with the control participants and may lead
BodyThink attempts to address internalization of the media ideal, to the intervention participants interacting differently (e.g., teasing
using magazine images of media ideals to address this issue may be peers less about appearance) in a way that could influence the
counterproductive given that experimental studies have found control group’s results. The relatively small sample size is another
that such exposure contributes to body dissatisfaction (e.g., Durkin shortfall of this research, resulting in limited power to detect
& Paxton, 2002; Durkin, Paxton, & Sorbello, 2007; Farquhar & difference between groups. Finally, a further limitation was that
Wasylkiw, 2007; Hargreaves & Tiggemann, 2003). Also, while some of the assessment instruments asked participants to report
internalization of media ideals and appearance teasing are well over a time period that exceeded the 1 week delay between the
supported as risk factors for body dissatisfaction in girls and boys, completion of BodyThink and administration of the T2 question-
research has less consistently found such support for self-esteem naire (e.g., the Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire
and body comparison as risk factors in boys (Jones, 2004; Paxton Restraint subscale asks participants to report on the past 28 days).
et al., 2006a, 2006b). Perhaps, more content addressing risk factors Future research would benefit from evaluating BodyThink using
specific to boys would be beneficial. However, this may be difficult, more rigorous scientific methods.
as research findings in relation to risk factors in boys are mixed, at In practice, when BodyThink is delivered in Australian class-
times identifying factors such as pressure from parents and friends rooms there is variety in the gender and profession of the
82 S.M. Richardson et al. / Body Image 6 (2009) 75–82

facilitator, the location of the program, the number of sessions eating disturbance in Swedish and Australian samples. International Journal of
Eating Disorders, 28, 430–435.
delivered, the number and age of participants and many other Marsh, H. W. (1992). Self-Description Questionnaire (SDQ) II—A theoretical and empirical
factors. Thus, future research evaluating BodyThink is warranted to basis for the measurement of multiple dimensions of adolescent self-concept. A test
ascertain the extent to which findings of this study may be manual and research monograph. Macarthur, Australia: University of Western
Sydney, Faculty of Education.
generalised. It is important to note that this study did not observe McVey, G. L., Tweed, S., & Blackmore, E. (2007). Healthy schools—healthy kids: A
any harmful effects of BodyThink. Rather, BodyThink appears to controlled evaluation of a comprehensive eating disorder prevention program.
have an impact on media literacy when implemented in a co- Body Image, 4, 115–136.
Mission Australia. (2007). National survey of young Australians 2007: Key and emerging
educational setting. issues (ISBN 978-0-9804550-0-7). Sydney: Author.
O’Dea, J., & Abraham, S. (2000). Improving the body image, eating attitudes, and
Acknowledgements behaviors of young male and female adolescents: A new educational
approach that focuses on self-esteem. International Journal of Eating Disorders,
28, 43–57.
Special thanks to all the school staff and students that were Paxton, S. (1993). A prevention program for disturbed eating and body dissatisfaction
involved in this research. Thank you also to Danni Watts from The in adolescent girls: A 1 year follow-up. Health Education Research, 8, 43–51.
Butterfly Foundation for her role in the facilitation of BodyThink. Paxton, S. J. (2000). Body image dissatisfaction, extreme weight loss behaviors: Suitable
targets for public health concern? Health Promotion Journal of Australia, 10, 15–19.
Paxton, S. J., Einsenberg, M. E., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2006a). Prospective predictors
References of body dissatisfaction in adolescent girls and boys: A five-year longitudinal study.
Developmental Psychology, 42, 888–899.
Barker, E. T., & Galambos, N. L. (2003). Body dissatisfaction of adolescent girls and boys: Paxton, S. J., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Hannan, P. J., & Eisenberg, M. (2006b). Body
Risk and resource factors. Journal of Early Adolescence, 23, 141–165. dissatisfaction prospectively predicts depressive mood and low self-esteem in
Bearman, S. K., Presnell, K., Martinez, E., & Stice, E. (2006). The skinny on body adolescent girls and boys. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 35,
dissatisfaction: A longitudinal study of adolescent girls and boys. Journal of Youth 539–549.
and Adolescence, 35, 229–241. Pinyerd, B., & Zipf, W. (2005). Puberty: Timing is everything. Journal of Paediatric
Buddeberg-Fischer, B., Klaghofer, R., Gnam, G., & Buddeberg, C. (1998). Prevention of Nursing, 20, 75–82.
disturbed eating behavior: A prospective intervention study in 14–19-year-old Presnell, K., Bearman, S. K., & Stice, E. (2004). Risk factors for body dissatisfaction in
Swiss students. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 98, 146–155. adolescent boys and girls: A prospective study. International Journal of Eating
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Disorders, 36, 389–401.
Erlbaum. Ricciardelli, L. A., McCabe, M. P., Ball, K., & Mellor, D. (2004). Sociocultural influences on
Dalle Grave, R., De Luca, L., & Campello, G. (2001). Middle school primary prevention body image concerns and body change strategies among indigenous and non-
program for eating disorders: A controlled study with a twelve-month follow-up. indigenous Australian adolescent girls and boys. Sex Roles, 51, 731–741.
Eating Disorder Review, 9, 327–337. Richardson, S. M., & Paxton, S. J. (2008). An evaluation of a body image intervention based
Dove. (2006). BodyThink: Helping children build positive self-esteem. Retrieved June on risk factors for body dissatisfaction: A controlled study with adolescent girls,
27, 2008, from http://www.campaignforrealbeauty.com.au/dove-self-esteem- Manuscript submitted for publication.
fund/bodythink.asp. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
Durkin, S. J., & Paxton, S. J. (2002). Predictors of vulnerability to reduced body image University Press.
satisfaction and psychological wellbeing in response to exposure to idealized Schoemaker, C., van Strien, T., & van der Staak, C. (1994). Validation of the Eating
female media images in adolescent girls. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53, Disorder Inventory in a nonclinical population using transformed and untrans-
995–1005. formed responses. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 15, 387–393.
Durkin, S. J., Paxton, S. J., & Sorbello, M. (2007). An integrative model of the impact of Schutz, H. K., Paxton, S. J., & Wertheim, E. H. (2002). Investigation of body comparison
exposure to idealized female images on adolescent girls’ body satisfaction. Journal among adolescent girls. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 1906–1937.
of Applied Social Psychology, 37, 1092–1117. Smolak, L., Levine, M. P., & Thompson, J. K. (2001). The use of sociocultural attitudes
Eating Disorders Association and Lever Faberge Limited. (2006). BodyThink building towards appearance questionnaire with middle school boys and girls. International
body confidence: Self-esteem and body image workshop guide. Melbourne: Author. Journal of Eating Disorders, 29, 216–223.
Fairburn, C. G., & Beglin, S. J. (1994). The assessment of eating disorders: Interview or Stice, E. (2001). A prospective test of the dual pathway model of bulimic pathology:
self-report questionnaire? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 16, 363–370. Mediating effects of dieting and negative affect. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
Farquhar, J. C., & Wasylkiw, L. (2007). Media images of men: Trends and consequences 110, 124–135.
of body conceptualization. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 8, 145–160. Stice, E. (2002). Risk and maintenance factors for eating pathology: A meta-analytic
Furnham, A., Badmin, N., & Sneade, I. (2002). Body image dissatisfaction: Gender review. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 825–848.
differences in eating attitudes, self-esteem, and reasons for exercise. The Journal Stice, E., Chase, A., Stromer, S., & Appel, A. (2001). A randomised trial of a dissonance-
of Psychology, 136, 581–596. based eating disorder prevention program. International Journal of Eating Disorders,
Garner, D. M., Olmstead, M. A., & Polivy, J. (1983). Development and validation of a 29, 247–262.
multidimensional eating disorder inventory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia. Stice, E., Marti, C. N., Spoor, S., Presnell, K., & Shaw, S. (2008). Dissonance and healthy
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 2, 15–33. weight eating disorder prevention programs: Long term effects from a randomized
Hargreaves, D., & Tiggemann, M. (2003). Longer-term implications of responsiveness to efficacy trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 329–340.
‘thin-ideal’ television: Support for a cumulative hypothesis of body image dis- Stice, E., & Shaw, H. (2004). Eating disorder prevention programs: A meta-analytic
turbance? European Eating Disorders Review, 11, 465–477. review. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 206–227.
Heinberg, L. J., Thompson, J. K., & Stormer, S. (1995). Development and validation of the Stice, E., Shaw, H., Burton, E., & Wade, E. (2006). Dissonance and healthy weight eating
Socio-cultural Attitudes Toward Appearance Questionnaire. International Journal of disorder prevention programs: A randomised efficacy trial. Journal of Consulting
Eating Disorders, 17, 81–89. and Clinical Psychology, 74, 263–275.
Jones, D. C. (2004). Body image among adolescent girls and boys: A longitudinal study. Stice, E., Trost, A., & Chase, A. (2003). Healthy weight control and dissonance-based
Developmental Psychology, 40, 823–835. eating disorder prevention programs: Results from a controlled trial. International
Jones, D. C., Vigfusdottir, T. H., & Lee, Y. (2004). Body image and the appearance culture Journal of Eating Disorders, 33, 10–21.
among adolescent girls and boys: An examination of friend conversations, peer Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Boston:
criticism, appearance magazines, and the internalization of appearance ideals. Allyn and Bacon.
Journal of Adolescent Research, 19, 323–339. Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L., & Tantleff, S. (1991). The Physical Appearance Comparison
Kater, K. J., Rohwer, J., & Levine, M. P. (2000). An elementary school project for Scale (PACS). Behavioral Assessment Review, 14, 174.
developing healthy body image and reducing risk factors for unhealthy and Thompson, J. K., Cattarin, J., Fowler, B., & Fisher, E. (1995). The Perception of Teasing
disordered eating. Eating Disorders, 8, 3–16. Scale (POTS): A revision and extension of the Physical Appearance Related Teasing
Klump, K. L., Perkins, P. S., Burt, A., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2007). Puberty Scale (PARTS). Journal of Personality Assessment, 65, 146–157.
moderates genetic influence on disordered eating. Psychological Medicine, 37, Thompson, J. K., van den Berg, P., Roehrig, M., Guarda, A. S., & Heinberg, L. J. (2004). The
627–634. Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Scale-3 (SATAQ): Development and
Levine, M. P., & Smolak, L. (2002). Body image development in adolescence. In T. F. Cash validation. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 35, 293–304.
& T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body image: Handbook of theory, research & clinical practice Wade, T. D., Davidson, S., & O’Dea, J. (2003). A preliminary controlled evaluation of a
(pp. 74–82). New York: The Guilford Press. school-based media literacy program and self-esteem program for reducing eating
Lewinsohn, P. M., Striegel-Moore, R. H., & Seeley, J. R. (2000). Epidemiology and natural disorder risk factors. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 33, 371–383.
course of eating disorders in young women from adolescence to adulthood. Journal Wilksch, S. M., Tiggemann, M., & Wade, T. D. (2006). Impact of interactive school-based
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 1284–1292. media literacy lessons for reducing internalization of media ideals in
Lunner, K., Wertheim, E. H., Thompson, J. K., Paxton, S. J., McDonald, F., & Halvaarson, E. young adolescent girls and boys. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 39,
S. (2000). A cross-cultural examination of weight related teasing, body image, and 385–393.

You might also like