Herr

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Personality and Mental Health

(2012)
Published online in Wiley Online Library
(wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI 10.1002/pmh.1204

Difficulties with emotion regulation mediate


the relationship between borderline
personality disorder symptom severity and
interpersonal problems

NATHANIEL R. HERR1, M. ZACHARY ROSENTHAL1, PAUL J. GEIGER1 AND


KAREN ERIKSON2, 1Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC, USA; 2University of Nevada,
Reno, NV, USA

ABSTRACT
Problems with interpersonal functioning and difficulties with emotion regulation are core characteristics of borderline
personality disorder (BPD). Little is known, however, about the interrelationship between these areas of dysfunction
in accounting for BPD symptom severity. The present study examines a model of the relationship between difficulties
with emotion regulation and interpersonal dysfunction in a community sample of adults (n = 124) with the full
range of BPD symptoms. Results showed that difficulties with emotion regulation fully mediated the relationship
between BPD symptom severity and interpersonal dysfunction. An alternative model indicated that interpersonal
problems partially mediated the relationship between difficulties with emotion regulation and BPD symptom severity.
These findings support existing theories of BPD, which propose that difficulties with emotion regulation may account
for the types of interpersonal problems experienced by individuals with BPD and suggest further examination of
the possibility that interpersonal dysfunction may worsen these individuals’ difficulties with emotion regulation.
Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is charac- key latent variables, including emotional dysregula-
terized by the chronic presence of a heterogeneous tion, behavioural dyscontrol and interpersonal
constellation of symptoms that include emotional dysfunction (e.g. Sanislow, Grilo, & McGlashan,
(e.g. affective instability), cognitive (e.g. paranoia 2000; Silverman et al., 1991). Although recent
or dissociation when stressed), behavioural research has begun to examine the neurobiological
(e.g. self-injurious behaviour) and interpersonal and behavioural process that may underlie the
(e.g. idealization and devaluation of others) symp- aetiology and maintenance of these core BPD
toms (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). factors (e.g. emotional dysregulation, Donegan
Despite the diversity in symptom presentations et al., 2003; and impulsivity, Siever, Torgersen,
found among individuals with BPD, factor analytic Gunderson, Livesley, & Kendler, 2002), it is
and family aggregation studies have shown that not yet clear how these core factors relate to and
BPD symptoms can be organized around several influence each other in the expression of BPD

Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. (2012)


DOI: 10.1002/pmh
Herr et al.

symptoms. To address this gap in the empirical other personality disorders (Henry et al., 2001;
literature, in the present study, we examined Koenigsberg et al., 2002; Levine, Marziali, &
the relationships among problems with emotion Hood, 1997). Negative affect intensity is associ-
regulation, interpersonal dysfunction and symp- ated with higher BPD features (Cheavens et al.,
toms of BPD. 2005) and diagnostic symptoms (Rosenthal,
Cheavens, Lejuez, & Lynch, 2005; Yen, Zlotnick,
& Costello, 2002). In addition, studies using
BPD and emotion regulation
experience sampling methodologies have found
Emotion regulation is a multi-faceted construct that individuals with BPD report greater intensity
that includes both antecedent-focused (changing of negative emotions and greater fluctuations in
the environment) and response-focused (changing affective states compared with healthy controls and
emotional expression) modulation of emotional individuals with other psychiatric disorders (Cowdry,
experience (Gross & Thompson, 2007). Skilled Gardner, O’Leary, Leibenluft, & Rubinow, 1991;
emotion regulation does not imply the absence Ebner-Priemer et al., 2007; Stein, 1996; Stiglmayr
of negative emotions but rather implies that an et al., 2005). There is evidence that experiential
individual can maintain goal-directed behaviour avoidance (i.e. the lack of willingness to experi-
even in the presence of intense, negative emotional ence negative emotions) mediates the relationships
experiences (Fruzzetti, Crook, Erikson, Lee, & between anxiety sensitivity and BPD symptoms
Worrall, 2009). Emotion dysregulation, in turn, (Gratz, Tull, & Gunderson, 2008), and experiential
includes but does not equate with experiencing avoidance has been found experimentally to be
strong negative emotions. Although there is no greater in a group of outpatients with BPD com-
consensus definition of emotion dysregulation, one pared with outpatients with no personality disorders
recently proposed conceptualization of this multi- (Gratz, Rosenthal, Tull, Lejuez, & Gunderson,
dimensional construct includes the following: 2006). Likewise, there is growing empirical evi-
(1) lack of awareness, understanding and acceptance dence that self-injury, a behaviour that is charac-
of emotions; (2) lack of access to adaptive strategies teristic of individuals with BPD, functions as a
for modulating the intensity and/or duration of the problematic strategy to avoid intense emotional
emotional response; (3) unwillingness to experi- experiences (Chapman, Gratz, & Brown, 2006).
ence emotional distress as a part of pursuing desired A common self-report tool used to measure
goals; (4) difficulty controlling impulsive beha- specific problems with emotion regulation in BPD
viours when distressed; and (5) inability to engage is the Difficulties with Emotion Regulation Scale
in goal-directed behaviours when distressed (Gratz (DERS; Gratz & Roemer, 2004), which assesses
& Roemer, 2004). deficits in six emotion regulation skills, including
Influential contemporary conceptualizations of the following: lack of emotional awareness or clar-
BPD have suggested that problems with emotional ity, unwillingness to experience and accept negative
dysregulation are central to the disorder (Linehan, emotions, difficulty controlling impulsive beha-
1993; Paris, 1994; Siever et al., 2002). Self-report viours when distressed, difficulty accomplishing
studies examining emotion dysregulation in BPD goals when emotionally distressed and perceived
have used both cross-sectional and prospective lack of access to strategies needed to regulate
measures of emotion regulation constructs such as emotions when distressed. Findings suggest that
negative affective intensity, affective instability and problems with emotion regulation as measured
emotional reactivity (see Rosenthal et al., 2008, for by the DERS are related to higher levels of BPD
a review). Individuals with BPD report significantly features and symptoms both in undergraduate
higher baseline levels of negative affect compared and clinical samples (Bornovalova et al., 2008;
with non-clinical controls and individuals with Chapman, Leung, & Lynch, 2008; Glenn &

Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. (2012)


DOI: 10.1002/pmh
Emotion REG and interpersonal functioning in BPD

Klonsky, 2009). In addition, Kuo and Linehan with BPD were worse than healthy controls in their
(2009) reported that DERS scores were significantly ability to fix a broken cooperative relationship in a
higher among individuals with BPD compared with social exchange laboratory task (King-Casas et al.,
healthy controls and a clinical control group com- 2008). Data from this study suggest that, compared
posed of participants with social anxiety disorder. with healthy controls, individuals with BPD may
Across all of these studies, there is a great deal be less likely to perceive violations of social norms
of evidence that self-reported emotional intensity, in interpersonal contexts.
lability, experiential avoidance and emotion regula- Research using experience sampling methods
tion skill deficits are associated with higher BPD also has found evidence of interpersonal problem
symptom severity and may differentiate individuals in individuals with BPD. Russell, Moskowitz,
with BPD from other samples. As a next step, it is Zuroff, Sookman and Paris (2007) asked indivi-
important to investigate whether, as hypothesized duals with BPD and non-clinical controls to
by Linehan (1993), greater difficulties with emotion complete short surveys following any substantive
regulation may account for other BPD-related social interactions for 20 days. They found that
phenomena. Although the diagnostic symptoms individuals with BPD reported more submissive,
of BPD are topographically different, it is possible quarrelsome and extreme interpersonal behaviours
that emotional dysregulation may functionally than the non-clinical controls. Another study asked
link all of the BPD symptoms in that each symp- participants to track social interactions for 7 days
tom is either a maladaptive strategy for regulating using a hand-held computerized social interaction
emotions (e.g. self-injurious behaviour; Chapman diary (Stepp, Pilkonis, Yaggi, Morse, & Feske,
et al., 2006) or a natural consequence of dysregu- 2009). Although individuals with BPD reported
lated emotions. In particular, it is possible that a similar amount of social interaction per day
interpersonal dysfunction, another central char- compared with individuals with other personality
acteristic of BPD, falls into this latter category. disorders and normal controls, they reported inter-
acting with fewer people overall. The individuals
with BPD also reported having more disagreements,
BDP and interpersonal functioning
more negative emotional reactions to social interac-
Studies examining the interpersonal functioning tion and more overall ambivalence with respect to
of individuals with BPD have shown that inter- social interactions. Collectively, these studies use
personal dysfunction may be one of the key diverse methods of measurement, which converge
features that differentiates BPD from other psychi- on the conclusion that problems with interpersonal
atric disorders (Nurnberg, Hurt, Feldman, & Suh, functioning characterize BPD.
1988; Modestin, 1987; Skodol et al., 2002). In
addition, longitudinal studies suggest that impair-
The relationship between emotion regulation and
ment in social functioning in BPD is stable across
interpersonal functioning in BPD
time (Skodol et al., 2005; Zanarini, Frankenburg,
Hennen, Reich, & Silk, 2005). Interpersonal Although difficulties with emotion regulation and
dysfunction in BPD has also been examined interpersonal dysfunction are separate constructs,
experimentally, with Dougherty, Bjork, Huckabee, it is unlikely that they are orthogonally related. The-
Moeller, and Swann (1999) showing that indivi- oretical models of BPD posit that poor emotion
duals with BPD responded more aggressively than regulation leads to problems in interpersonal
normal controls on a competitive computer task, relatedness (e.g. Linehan, 1993; Putnam & Silk,
although the result was no longer significant after 2005), yet few of the interpersonal functioning stud-
controlling for depressive symptoms. A study using ies described previously assessed emotion regulation
a different lab-based task found that individuals difficulties. One that did (Stepp et al., 2009) found

Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. (2012)


DOI: 10.1002/pmh
Herr et al.

more conflictual and emotionally intense day-to-day the relationship between emotion regulation and
social interactions in individuals with BPD, as interpersonal dysfunction, we tested an atheoretical
compared with those without BPD. Among the model specifying that deficits in interpersonal func-
few additional studies that have examined the rela- tioning may account for the relationship between
tionship between elements of emotion regulation BPD symptoms and difficulties with emotion regu-
and interpersonal dysfunction, results have been lation. On the basis of Linehan’s theoretical model
mixed. Support for the model that affective instabil- of emotion regulation, we did not expect this model
ity leads to poorer interpersonal functioning was would be supported; however, we examined it to
found in a 2-year follow-up of undergraduates with either strengthen arguments for the primary model
BPD features (Bagge et al., 2004). Alternatively, or to generate questions for subsequent research.
Koenigsberg and colleagues (2001) found that Despite the limitations of using cross-sectional data,
affective problems were related to some of the diag- the present study is the first that we are aware of that
nostic criteria of BPD (e.g. identity disturbance and attempted to provide support for either or both of
suicidality) but were not related to interpersonal these models by comparing them in a single sample
dysfunction. Although affective instability is one of individuals with the full range of BPD symptoms.
component of emotional dysregulation, additional
research is needed to help clarify the nature of
Method
the relationship between problems with emotion
regulation and interpersonal dysfunction in BPD.
Participants
In sum, recent studies indicate that BPD is char-
acterized by difficulties with emotion regulation and As part of a larger project examining emotional
interpersonal dysfunction. However, research that functioning in BPD, 124 participants were recruited
examines the extent to which difficulties with emo- for the present study through advertisements in a
tion regulation account for the relationship between local free newspaper, on the Duke University
BPD symptoms and interpersonal dysfunction is Medical Center website and through outpatient
needed. In the present study, we sought to test the clinics at Duke University Hospital System. Sepa-
hypothesized model (e.g. Linehan, 1993; Putnam rate recruiting advertisements targeted individuals
& Silk, 2005) that emotion regulation difficulties with BPD (e.g. ‘Have you ever been diagnosed with
underlie the relationship between BPD symptoms borderline personality disorder?’) and a general
and interpersonal dysfunction. Difficulties with community sample (e.g. ‘Are you interested in a
emotion regulation were measured using the DERS study about emotion?’). Participants completed a
to include aspects of emotion regulation that extend brief telephone screen to exclude individuals with
beyond just intense negative affect. On the basis current mania and psychosis. Participants who
of the existing theoretical and empirical research were between the ages of 18 and 60 years were
(e.g. Bagge et al., 2004; Linehan, 1993; Stepp eligible and were scheduled to complete the
et al., 2009), we hypothesized that difficulties with study within 2 weeks. Participants received $150
emotion regulation would fully account for the compensation for completion of the study.
direct relationship between BPD symptoms and The final sample included 35 participants
interpersonal problems. Specific subtypes of inter- (28.2%) who met full criteria for BPD (see the
personal functioning (i.e. interpersonal sensitivity, following paragraphs for diagnostic interview
interpersonal ambivalence, interpersonal aggression, description). The sample had a mean age of
need for social approval and lack of sociability) were 36.5 years (standard deviation (SD) = 11.7 years;
also examined to determine if difficulties with emo- range. 18–58 years) and included more women
tion regulation was more associated with certain (n = 83; 66.9%) than men (n = 41; 33.1%). More
kinds of interpersonal problems. To further explore than half of the participants were identified as

Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. (2012)


DOI: 10.1002/pmh
Emotion REG and interpersonal functioning in BPD

African-American (n = 65, 52.4%), whereas Questionnaire (SCID-II-PQ), a questionnaire with


48 (38.1%) were Caucasian, 3 (2.4%) were Asian, 119 items assessing the presence (yes) or absence
3 were Native American, 2 (1.6%) were Hispanic (no) of specific personality disorder symptoms. For
and 3 indicated other race/ethnicity. The sample the current study, items endorsed on the SCID-II-
included 37 (29.8%) participants who were married PQ were further evaluated using the standard
or living with a partner, 18 (14.5%) who were in an SCID-II interview. This two-stage assessment pro-
intimate relationship and living separately, 18 who cess is commonly conducted, with studies suggesting
were separated or divorced, 4 (3.2%) who were a low false-negative rate for non-endorsed SCID-II-
widowed, and 44 (35.5%) who were never married. PQ items (Jacobsberg, Perry, & Frances, 1995).
Most participants (89%) had completed at least Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV
some college. The sample was predominantly Personality Disorders, Axis II interviews were
low income, with 47 (37.9%) making less than conducted by bachelor and master’s level assessors
$10 000 a year and 51 (41.1%) making between trained by the second author and an expert clinical
$10 000 and $40 000 a year. assessor in the second author’s lab to reliability.
With the use of digitally videotaped recordings,
Measures approximately 10% of the SCID-II interviews were
randomly re-assessed by a different trained rater.
Demographics. A self-report measure was used to Inter-rater reliability on total BPD symptoms was
obtain demographic and descriptive information, evaluated using intraclass correlation coefficients
including age, ethnicity, marital status and annual (ICCs). The ICC for BPD symptoms was 0.98, indi-
household income. cating excellent reliability. The ICCs for all person-
ality disorders ranged from 0.66 (schizotypal) to 1.0
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, Axis I. (histrionic). To obtain a continuous score for BPD
Diagnostic exclusions and current prevalence of symptom severity that included sub-diagnostic
Axis I diagnoses were determined by the Struc- threshold of symptoms, interviewer-rated symptoms
tured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, Axis I were summed, which resulted in a possible range of
(SCID-I; patient version; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, BPD symptom severity from 0 (no BPD criterion
& Williams, 1995), a measure with demonstrated behaviours present) to 9 (all BPD criterion beha-
reliability (Lobbestael, Leurgans, & Arntz, 2011). viours present beyond threshold). The sample had
The SCID-I was administered by bachelor and a mean score of 2.6 (SD = 3.1) symptoms, and 35
master’s level assessors trained by the second author participants (28.2%) met full criteria for a diagnosis
and an expert clinical assessor in the second of BPD.
author’s lab to reliability. Inter-rater reliability was The DERS (Gratz & Roemer, 2004) is a
assessed by a blind rater randomly rating 20% of 36-item measure that assesses individuals’ typical
video recorded SCID-I interviews. Kappas ranged levels of emotion dysregulation across six domains:
from 0.63 to 1.0, reflecting acceptable inter-rater non-acceptance of negative emotions, inability
reliability. to engage in goal-directed behaviours when dis-
tressed, difficulties controlling impulsive behaviours
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Personality when distressed, limited access to emotion regula-
Disorders, Axis II. The Structured Clinical Inter- tion strategies perceived as effective, lack of emo-
view for DSM-IV Personality Disorders, Axis II tional awareness and lack of emotional clarity.
(SCID-II; First, Gibbon, Spitzer, Williams, & The DERS has been found to have high internal
Benjamin, 1997) was used to assess diagnostic consistency (a = 0.93), good test–retest reliability
symptoms of personality disorders, including BPD. (rI = 0.88, p < 0.01) and adequate construct
Participants first completed the SCID-II Personality and predictive validity (Gratz & Roemer, 2004).

Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. (2012)


DOI: 10.1002/pmh
Herr et al.

Internal consistency in the present sample was (1986) in which we sought to establish (1) that the
also high (a = 0.89). direct pathway from BPD symptoms to interper-
sonal dysfunction was significant; (2) that the
Inventory of Interpersonal Problems. The original pathway from BPD symptoms to emotion regula-
Inventory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP; Horowitz, tion problems was significant; (3) that the pathway
Rosenberg, Baer, Ureno, & Villasenor, 1988) is a from emotion regulation problems to interpersonal
127-item measure of problems with interpersonal dysfunction was significant after controlling for
functioning relevant to samples of individuals with BPD symptoms; and (4) that the direct effect of
personality disorders (e.g. Pilkonis, Kim, Proietti, BPD symptoms predicting interpersonal dysfunc-
& Barkham, 1996; Scarpa et al., 1999). Pilkonis tion is reduced, accounting for emotion regulation
et al. (1996) developed a shortened version (47 items), problems. In addition, the indirect pathway
which delineated five subscales: (1) inter- from BPD symptoms to emotion regulation pro-
personal sensitivity, (2) interpersonal ambivalence, blems to interpersonal dysfunction was tested for
(3) interpersonal aggression, (4) need for social significance by using the Sobel test (Preacher &
approval and (5) lack of sociability. These sub- Hayes, 2004). These mediation procedures can
scales were created using factor analysis with items establish whether or not the DERS is measuring a
that were associated with personality disorders and critical component of BPD that fully accounts
cross-validated in three separate datasets, demon- for the interpersonal problems associated with
strating adequate ability to distinguish between BPD symptoms.
individuals with and without personality disorders.
In the present sample, internal consistency was high
for the full scale IIP (a = 0.97), as well as for each Results
subscale (a’s range from 0.89 to 0.93).
Zero-order correlations between the number of BPD
symptoms endorsed, the IIP total score and the IIP
Procedure subscales are presented in Table 1. As shown, there
This study was approved by the Institutional Review was a significant direct relationship between BPD
Board at Duke University Medical Center. All symptoms and IIP total score (p < 0.001), as well
participants provided written informed consent as with each IIP subscale (all p’s < 0.05). The rela-
prior to participating in the study. Participants tion between several non-BPD psychopathology
completed diagnostic interviews and self-report variables and DERS total score was examined using
instruments after completing lab tasks that were univariate ANOVA in which the presence or
part of a larger study of emotion processing. Partici- absence of each diagnostic category was the pre-
pants were given the self-report packet and then dictor variable and DERS total score was the
debriefed about the experiment and compensated. outcome variable. DERS total score was found to
be significantly related to lifetime history of
depressive disorder (F(1, 118) = 43.18, p < 0.001),
Data analysis
anxiety disorder (F(1, 118) = 51.15, p < 0.001), and
We tested two models in which (1) emotion regula- non-BPD personality disorder (F(1, 119) = 51.22,
tion problems account for the relationship between p < 0.001). All analyses, therefore, statistically
BPD symptoms and interpersonal problems and controlled for the effect of these psychopathology
(2) interpersonal problems account for the relation- variables. Gender, ethnicity and marital status
ship between BPD symptoms and emotion regulation were also examined using univariate ANOVA
problems. These models were tested using the and were found to be unrelated to DERS or IIP total
mediation procedures outlined by Baron and Kenny scores. Zero-order correlations also showed that

Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. (2012)


DOI: 10.1002/pmh
Emotion REG and interpersonal functioning in BPD

15.8 (9.3)
neither DERS nor IIP total score was related to
8 participant’s income or age; thus, no demographic
variables were controlled for in the analyses. Alpha
was set at 0.05 for all analyses.
First, a direct path between the number of

15.6 (10.7)
0.80***
endorsed SCID-II BPD symptoms and the IIP total
7

score was established using hierarchical regression


(b = 0.42, t(118) = 4.5, p < 0.001). To examine
whether or not this path was mediated by DERS
17.7 (10.5)

total score, first, the path from BPD symptoms to


0.82***
0.87***
DERS total score was examined and found to be
6

significant (b = 0.57, t(117) = 7.14, p < 0.001).


SD, standard deviation; DERS, Difficulties with Emotion Regulation Scale; IIP, Inventory of Interpersonal Problems.

Next, BPD symptoms and DERS total score were


entered simultaneously with IIP total score as the
7.5 (6.6)
0.76***
0.62***
0.61***

criterion variable. Results show that the path from


5

DERS total score to IIP total score was significant


(b = 0.73, t(117) = 8.52, p < 0.001), whereas the
path from BPD symptoms to IIP total score was
13.3 (9.0)
0.62***
0.63***
0.70***
0.54***

no longer significant (b = 0.00, t(117) = 0.006,


4

p = 0.99). Thus, the requirements of Baron and


Kenny (1986) for full mediation were met. The
Table 1: Correlations between study variables; means (SD) presented on the diagonal

Sobel test (Preacher & Hayes, 2004) also confirmed


that the indirect path was significant (Sobel = 5.47,
70.0 (40.5)
0.80***
0.80***
0.94***
0.92***
0.89***

p < 0.001). This model accounted for 68% of


3

the variance in IIP score (R2 = 0.68, F = 47.15,


p < 0.001).
To further examine which specific IIP subscales
86.4 (31.5)

the DERS may be accounting for, each IIP subscale


0.81***
0.57***
0.66***
0.80***
0.72***
0.75***

was entered separately into the first model described


2

previously. After the control variables were entered,


a significant direct path from BPD to each subscale
was established for all subscales: interpersonal
2.6 (3.1)

ambivalence (b = 0.29, t(118) = 2.63, p < 0.01);


0.64***
0.75***
0.62***
0.37***
0.53***
0.67***
0.50***
1

interpersonal aggression (b = 0.32, t(118) = 3.27,


p < 0.001); interpersonal sensitivity (b = 0.46,
t(118) = 5.01, p < 0.001); lack of sociability
IIP—need for social approval

(b = 0.26, t(118) = 2.53, p < 0.05); and


need for social approval (b = 0.49, t(118) = 5.16,
IIP—lack of sociability

p < 0.001). The DERS total score mediator was


IIP—ambivalence

then entered simultaneously with BPD symptoms


IIP—aggression
IIP—sensitivity
BPD symptoms

IIP total score

predicting each of these significant criterion IIP


***p < 0.001.

subscale variables. Full mediation, as indicated


**p < 0.01.
*p < 0.05.

by a significant path from the DERS score to


DERS

each IIP subscale, a now insignificant path between


BPD symptoms and the IIP subscale, and a
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.

Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. (2012)


DOI: 10.1002/pmh
Herr et al.

Table 2: Indirect path analyses for BPD symptoms predicting interpersonal problems, mediated by emotion regulation

Dependent Independent(s) b t Sobel R2 (full model) F (full model)

Emotion regulation (DERS)† BPD symptoms (SCID-II) 0.57 7.14***

IIP—interpersonal ambivalence BPD symptoms 0.14 1.15


Emotion regulation 0.68 5.55*** 4.23*** 0.35 12.31***

IIP—interpersonal aggression BPD symptoms 0.00 0.04


Emotion regulation 0.52 4.87*** 3.87*** 0.51 23.32***

IIP—interpersonal sensitivity BPD symptoms 0.07 0.80


Emotion regulation 0.67 7.80*** 5.22*** 0.68 46.93***

IIP—lack of sociability BPD symptoms 0.20 1.97


Emotion regulation 0.76 7.58*** 5.11*** 0.56 28.57***

IIP—need for social approval BPD symptoms 0.12 1.22


Emotion regulation 0.63 6.60*** 4.76*** 0.60 33.74***

DERS, Difficulties with Emotion Regulation Scale; IIP, Inventory of Interpersonal Problems; SCID-II, Structured Clinical
Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders, Axis II.

Note that this analysis was used to determine the Sobel statistic in all subsequent analyses.
Models include lifetime history of depressive or anxiety disorder and lifetime history of a non-BPD personality disorder as
control variables.
*p < 0.05.
**p < 0.01.
***p < 0.001.

significant Sobel test, was found for all subscales Discussion


(Table 2).
To examine a competing hypothesis, an alter- Difficulties with emotion regulation and interper-
native mediational model was tested in which sonal dysfunction are core problems in BPD. How-
IIP total score was the mediator between BPD ever, research has yet to clearly elucidate how
symptoms and DERS total score. The paths from these constructs relate to one another in the
BPD symptoms to DERS total score (criterion manifestation of BPD symptoms. Accordingly,
variable) and to IIP total score (mediator) were the present study sought to examine the relation-
significant, as reported in the analyses given ships among difficulties with emotion regulation,
previously. When BPD symptoms and IIP total interpersonal problems and symptoms of BPD in
score were entered simultaneously predicting a community sample of ethnically diverse adults.
DERS total score, IIP was a significant predictor On the basis of Linehan’s biosocial model of
(b = 0.54, t(117) = 8.52, p < 0.001), but the BPD (Linehan, 1993), we hypothesized that
path from BPD symptoms remained significant problems with emotion regulation would have a
(b = 0.35, t(117) = 5.10, p < 0.001). The Sobel central role underlying the expression of interper-
test of the indirect path was significant, however, sonal problems in BPD. As such, a model in which
indicating partial mediation (Sobel = 3.98, p < 0.001). difficulties with emotion regulation (measured by
This model accounted for 77% of the the DERS) mediated the relationship between
variance in DERS score (R2 = 0.77, F = 75.42, BPD symptoms and interpersonal functioning
p < 0.001). (measured by the IIP) was tested. We also tested

Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. (2012)


DOI: 10.1002/pmh
Emotion REG and interpersonal functioning in BPD

variations of this model with specific interpersonal mediator of the relationship between BPD symp-
functioning subtypes as the outcome variables. In toms and difficulties with emotion regulation. This
addition, we examined an atheoretical model in finding suggests that some of the difficulties with
which interpersonal functioning mediated the rela- emotion regulation that are found among indivi-
tionship between BPD symptoms and difficulties duals with many BPD symptoms may be accounted
with emotion regulation. for by interpersonal functioning problems these
The primary finding of this study supported our individuals also experience. To fully interpret this
hypothesis that difficulties with emotion regulation finding, however, we must take into account the test
would fully mediate the relationship between BPD of the first model and the limitations inherent in
symptoms and interpersonal functioning. In other using cross-sectional data for these analyses. When
words, although BPD symptoms were found to be one uses cross-sectional data to examine mediation,
directly associated with interpersonal problems, dif- the predictor, mediator and outcome variables are
ficulties with emotion regulation fully accounted for determined by a theoretical model rather than
this relationship. Furthermore, this was true for all temporal sequencing. Because we compared cross-
subtypes of interpersonal functioning measured by sectional models, which differ only by exchanging
the IIP. These findings lend empirical support to the mediator and outcome variables, the direction
the notion that emotion regulation difficulties are of effect between difficulties with emotion regula-
a key mechanism through which individuals with tion and interpersonal functioning could not be
BPD may experience more interpersonal problems. established; thus, some of the observed strength
Our findings are supported by previous studies that of the indirect pathway in each model may be
found interpersonal problems to be highly related inflated by any portion of the association between
to emotion regulation or affective instability. For these variables that is due to the outcome causing
instance, Bagge and colleagues (2004) showed that the mediator (i.e. the reverse of what a mediation
the BPD feature of affective instability, one aspect model specifies). The data show that BPD symp-
of emotion regulation, was related to social malad- toms were directly related to difficulties with
justment even after controlling for other Axis I emotion regulation, which, in turn, fully accounted
and Axis II disorders, which led the authors to for the poorer interpersonal functioning associated
suggest that affective instability may be one cause with more BPD symptoms. On the other hand,
of social problems for individuals with many BPD BPD symptoms were also directly related to pro-
characteristics. Our findings also support theoretical blems in interpersonal functioning, but these
models of emotion regulation broadly (e.g. Keltner problems only accounted for a portion of the
& Kring, 1998) and in BPD specifically (e.g. greater difficulties in emotion regulation among
Putnam & Silk, 2005), which suggest that difficulty these individuals. As such, the test of the first
regulating intense emotional experiences account model yields both a stronger result and supports
for problems with interpersonal functioning. Fur- existing theoretical and empirical research (Bagge
thermore, our subsequent analyses with specific et al., 2004; Linehan, 1993; Stepp et al., 2009).
subtypes of interpersonal problems showed that The results of the second model, however, suggest
difficulties with emotion regulation could fully the need for further study to determine whether
account for the relationship between BPD symp- the indirect effect of interpersonal functioning was
toms and each IIP subscale, lending further the result of statistical inflation or if there is, in fact,
support to the notion that difficulties with emotion a bidirectional causal relationship between inter-
regulation underlie a broad range of interpersonal personal functioning and difficulties with emotion
problems that are associated with BPD. regulation that leads to high levels of these con-
Our test of the atheoretical alternative model structs among individuals with many symptoms
showed that interpersonal functioning was a partial of BPD.

Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. (2012)


DOI: 10.1002/pmh
Herr et al.

There are several additional limitations that must interpersonal functioning and emotion regulation
be considered in the present study. First, self-report difficulties in BPD individuals; thus, further studies
measures of problems with emotion regulation and using longitudinal designs are warranted.
interpersonal functioning were used. It is possible Clinically, the findings of the present study
that this resulted in statistical relationships between suggest that the interpersonal problems found in
measures due to method overlap and raises the ques- individuals with a high number of BPD symptoms
tion of whether or not individuals can accurately may be a function of the emotion regulation pro-
rate their own problems with emotion regulation blems that these individuals experience. Addressing
and interpersonal functioning. However, this con- these emotion regulation problems in behavioural
cern is somewhat mitigated by the fact that the mea- treatments may, therefore, lead to improvement in
sures chosen for the present study are well validated the interpersonal functioning of individuals with
and commonly used to study these constructs as they BPD symptoms. For instance, there are a range of
relate to BPD symptoms in community samples emotion regulation skills in dialectical behaviour
similar to the present study. A related limitation therapy (Linehan, 1993) that could be provided to
is that the questionnaires chosen may not compre- patients with many dialectical behaviour therapy
hensively measure difficulties with emotion regula- symptoms. Although results from the present study
tion or interpersonal functioning and instead only should not be interpreted definitively, the findings
assess some aspects of these complex and multi- point to the possibility of exploring the sequencing
dimensional constructs. Future studies designed to of emotion regulation skill training before interper-
replicate and extend the present findings should sonal skill training to reduce BPD symptoms. Before
include more objective behavioural and biological clinical interventions are tested in this manner,
measures of emotion regulation (e.g. Herpertz, however, future research must be performed to
Kunert, Schwenger, & Sass, 1999; Herpertz et al., better understand the temporal or causal relation-
2000) and interpersonal functioning to better ship between difficulties with emotion regulation
understand the relationship between these two core and interpersonal functioning in BPD.
areas of impairment in BPD.
As noted previously, the study was also limited
by its cross-sectional design, precluding causal infer- Acknowledgements
ences about the temporal nature of the relationship
between difficulties with emotion regulation, inter- The work was supported by funding from the Brout
personal problems and BPD symptoms. Despite Family Foundation, SENetwork and departmental
our results showing that difficulties with emotion funds that accrued from clinical revenue. We thank
regulation completely accounted for the relationship Dr Jennifer Brout for her support of this research.
between interpersonal problems and BPD symptoms,
it is possible that emotion regulation difficulties References
developed, at least in part, from attachment-based
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and
or other relationship problems that occurred earlier statistical manual of mental disorders, (4th ed., text version).
in life. Alternatively, Linehan’s (1993) biosocial Washington, DC: Author.
model of BPD posits that the disorder stems from Bagge, C., Nickell, A., Stepp, S., Durrett, C., Jackson, K., &
the interaction between a biologically mediated Trull, T. J. (2004). Borderline personality disorder
predisposition to emotion regulation problems and features predict negative outcomes 2 years later. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 279–288.
invalidation from interpersonal (mostly familial)
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator
relationships. The present data cannot be used variable distinction in social psychological research:
to comprehensively evaluate the developmental, Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal
interactive or transactional relationship between of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. (2012)


DOI: 10.1002/pmh
Emotion REG and interpersonal functioning in BPD

Bornovalova, M. A., Gratz, K. L., Daughters, S. B., Nick, B., Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment
Delany-Brumsey, A., Lynch, T. R., Kosson, D., Lejuez, C. W. of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development,
(2008). A multimodal assessment of the relationship factor structure, and initial validation of the Difficulties in
between emotion dysregulation and borderline personality Emotion Regulation Scale. Journal of Psychopathology and
disorder among inner-city substance users in residential Behavioral Assessment, 26, 41–54.
treatment. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 42, 717–726. Gratz, K. L., Rosenthal, M. Z., Tull, M. T., Lejuez, C. W., &
Chapman, A. L., Gratz, K. L., & Brown, M. Z. (2006). Solving Gunderson, J. G. (2006). An experimental investigation
the puzzle of deliberate self-harm: The experiential avoid- of emotion dysregulation in borderline personality
ance model. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 371–394. disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 850–855.
Chapman, A. L., Leung, D. W., & Lynch, T. R. (2008). Impul- Gratz, K. L., Tull, M. T., & Gunderson, J. G. (2008). Preliminary
sivity and emotion dysregulation in borderline personality data on the relationship between anxiety sensitivity and bor-
disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 22, 148–164. derline personality disorder: The role of experiential avoid-
Cheavens, J. S., Rosenthal, M. Z., Daughters, S. D., Novak, J., ance. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 42, 550–559.
Kosson, D., Lynch, T. R., & Lejeuz, C. W. (2005). An Gross, J., & Thompson, R. (2007). Emotion regulation:
analogue investigation of the relationships among Conceptual foundations. In J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of
perceived parental criticism, negative affect, and borderline Emotion Regulation. New York: Guilford Press.
personality disorder features: The role of thought Henry, C., Mitropoulou, V., New, A. S., Koenigsberg, H. W.,
suppression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 257–268. Silverman, J., & Siever, L. J. (2001). Affective instability
Cowdry, R. W., Gardner, D. L., O’Leary, K. M., Leibenluft, E., & and impulsivity in borderline personality and bipolar II
Rubinow, D. R. (1991). Mood variability: A study of four disorders: Similarities and differences. Journal of Psychiatric
groups. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1505–1511. Research, 35, 307–312.
Donegan, N. H., Sanislow, C. A., Blumberg, H. P., Fulbright, Herpertz, S. C., Kunert, H. J., Schwenger, U. B., & Sass, H.
R. K., Lacadie, C., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J. C., Olson, I. R., (1999). Affective responsiveness in borderline personality
McGlashan, T. H., & Wexler, B. E. (2003). Amygdala disorder: A psychophysiological approach. The American
hyperactivity in borderline personality disorder: Implica- Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 1550–1556.
tions for emotional dysregulation. Society of Biological Herpertz, S. C., Schwenger, U. B., Kunert, H. J., Lukas, G.,
Psychiatry, 54, 1284–1293. Gretzer, U., Nutzmann, J., Schuerkens, A., & Sass, H.
Dougherty, D. M., Bjork, J. M., Huckabee, H. C. G., Moeller, (2000). Emotional responses in patients with borderline
F. G., & Swann, A. C. (1999). Laboratory measures of as compared with avoidant personality disorder. Journal
aggression and impulsivity in women with borderline of Personality Disorders, 14, 339–351.
personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 85, 315–326. Horowitz, L. M., Rosenberg, S. E., Baer, B. A., Ureno, G.,
Ebner-Priemer, U. W., Shaw-Welch, S., Grossman, P., Villasenor, V. S. (1988). Inventory of interpersonal pro-
Reisch, T., Linehan, M. M., & Bohus, M. (2007). blems: Psychometric properties and clinical applications.
Psychophysiological ambulatory assessment of affective Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 885–92.
dysregulation in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Jacobsberg, L., Perry, S., & Frances, A. (1995). Diagnostic
Research, 150, 265–275. agreement between the SCID-II Screening Questionnaire
First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., & and the Personality Disorder Examination. Journal of
Benjamin, L. S. (1997). User’s Guide for the Structured Personality Assessment, 65, 428–433.
Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M. (1998). Emotion, social function,
(SCID-II). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. and psychopathology. Review of General Psychology, 2,
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. 320–342.
(1995). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I King-Casas, B., Sharp, C., Lomax-Bream, L., Lohrenz, T.,
disorders—Patient edition (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0). Fonagy, P., & Montague, P. (2008). The rupture and
Biometrics Research Department. New York: State Psychiatric repair of cooperation in borderline personality disorder.
Institute. Science, 321, 806–810.
Fruzzetti, A. E., Crook, W., Erikson, K. M., Lee, J. E., & Koenigsberg, H. W., Harvey, P. D., Mitropoulou, V., New, A. S.,
Worrall, J. M. (2009). Emotion regulation. In W. T. Goodman, M., Silverman, J., Serby, M., Schopick, F., &
O’Donohue, & J. E. Fisher (Eds.), Cognitive behavior Siever, L. J. (2001). Are the interpersonal and identity distur-
therapy, applying empirically supported techniques in your bances in the borderline personality disorder criteria linked
practice (2nd ed., pp. 272–284). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. to the traits of affective instability and impulsivity? Journal
Glenn, C. R., & Klonsky, E. D. (2009). Emotion dysregula- of Personality Disorders, 15, 358–370.
tion as a core feature of borderline personality disorder. Koenigsberg, H. W., Harvey, P. D., Mitropoulou, V., Schmeidler,
Journal of Personality Disorders, 23, 20–28. J., New, A. S., Goodman, M., Silverman, J. M., Serby, M.,

Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. (2012)


DOI: 10.1002/pmh
Herr et al.

Schopick, F., & Siever, L. J. (2002). Characterizing affec- Scarpa, A., Luscher, K. A., Smalley, K. J., Pilkonis, P. A.,
tive instability in borderline personality disorder. The Amer- Kim, Y., & Williams, W. C. (1999). Screening for
ican Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 784–788. personality disorders in a nonclinical population. Journal
Kuo, J. R., & Linehan, M. M. (2009). Disentangling emotion of Personality Disorders, 13, 345–360.
processes in borderline personality disorder: Physiological Siever, L. J., Torgersen, S., Gunderson, J. G., Livesley, W. J.,
and self-reported assessment of biological vulnerability, & Kendler, K. S. (2002). The borderline diagnosis III:
baseline intensity, and reactivity to emotionally evocative Identifying endophenotypes for genetic studies. Society of
stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 531–544. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 964–968.
Levine, D., Marziali, E., & Hood, J. (1997). Emotion processing Silverman, J. M., Pinkham, L., Horvath, T. B., & Coccaro, E. F.,
in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Nervous and Klar, H., Schear, S., Apter, S., Davidson, M., Mohs, R. C., &
Mental Diseases, 185, 240–246. Siever, L. J. (1991). Affective and impulsive personality
Linehan, M. (1993). Cognitive–behavioral treatment of borderline disorder traits in the relatives of patients with borderline
personality disorder. New York: Guilford Press. personality disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry,
Lobbestael, J., Leurgans M., & Arntz A. (2011). Inter-rater 148, 1378–1385.
reliability of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM- Skodol, A. E., Gunderson, J. G., McGlashan, T. H., Dyck, I.
IV Axis I Disorders (SCID I) and Axis II Disorders (SCID R., Stout, R. L., Bender, D. S., Grilo, C. M., Shea, M. T.,
II). Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 18, 75–79. Zanarini, M. C., Morey, L. C., Sanislow, C. A., &
Modestin, J. (1987). Quality of interpersonal relationships: Oldham, J. M. (2002). Functional impairment in patients
The most characteristic DSM-III BPD criterion. Compre- with schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, or obsessive–
hensive Psychiatry, 28, 397–402. compulsive personality disorder. The American Journal of
Nurnberg, H. G., Hurt, S. W., Feldman, A., & Suh, R. (1988). Psychiatry, 159, 276–283.
Evaluation of diagnostic criteria for borderline personality Skodol, A. E., Pagano, M. E., Bender, D. S., Shea, M. T.,
disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 1280–1284. Gunderson, J. G., Yen, S., Stout, R. L, Morey, L. C., Sanislow,
Paris, J. (1994). Borderline personality disorder: A multidimen- C. A., Grilo, C. M., Zanarini, M. C., & McGlashan, T. H.
sional approach. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric (2005). Stability of functional impairment in patients with
Association. schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, or obsessive–compulsive
Pilkonis, P. A., Kim, Y., Proietti, J. M., & Barkham, M. personality disorder over two years. Psychological Medicine, 35,
(1996). Scales for personality disorders developed from the 443–451.
inventory of interpersonal problems. Journal of Personality Stein, K. F. (1996). Affect instability in adults with a borderline
Disorders, 10, 355–369. personality disorder. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 10, 32–40.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS Stepp, S. D., Pilkonis, P. A., Yaggi, K. E., Morse, J. Q., & Feske, U.
procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple (2009). Interpersonal and emotional experiences of social
mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, interactions in borderline personality disorder. The Journal of
& Computers, 36, 717–731. Nervous and Mental Disease, 197, 484–491.
Putnam, K. M., & Silk, K. R. (2005). Emotion dysregulation Stiglmayr, C., Grathwol, T., Linehan, M., Ihorst, G., Fahrenberg,
and the development of borderline personality disorder. J., & Bohus, M. (2005). Aversive tension in patients with
Development and Psychopathology, 17, 899–925. borderline personality disorder: A computer-based controlled
Rosenthal, M. Z., Cheavens, J. S., Lejuez, C. W., & Lynch, T. field study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 111, 372–379.
R. (2005). Thought suppression mediates the relationship Yen, S., Zlotnick, C., & Costello, E. (2002). Affect regulation
between negative affect and borderline personality disorder in women with borderline personality disorder traits. The
symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1173–1185. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 190, 693–696.
Rosenthal, M. Z., Gratz, K. L., Kosson, D. S., Cheavens, J. S., Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Hennen, J., Reich D. B., &
Lejuez, C. W., & Lynch, T. R. (2008). Borderline person- Silk, K. R. (2005). Psychosocial functioning of borderline
ality disorder and emotional responding: A review of the patients and Axis II comparison subjects followed prospec-
research literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 75–91. tively for six years. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19, 19–29.
Russell, J. J., Moskowitz, D. S., Zuroff, D. C., Sookman, D., &
Paris, J. (2007). Stability and variability of affective experi-
ence and interpersonal behavior in borderline personality Address correspondence to: Nathaniel R. Herr,
disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 578–588.
Department of Psychology, American University,
Sanislow, C. A., Grilo, C. M., & McGlashan, T. H. (2000).
Factor analysis of the DSM-III-R borderline personality Asbury 321A, 4400 Massachusetts Ave NW,
disorder criteria in psychiatric inpatients. The American Washington, D.C. 20016. E-mail: nherr@american.
Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1629–1633. edu.

Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. (2012)


DOI: 10.1002/pmh

You might also like