Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES

The Development of Academic Achievement and


Cognitive Abilities: A Bidirectional Perspective
Peng Peng,1 and Rogier A. Kievit2
1
The University of Texas at Austin and 2MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of
Cambridge

ABSTRACT—Developing academic skills and cognitive abili- mathematics, affect many outcomes, including educational
ties is critical for children’s development. In this article, attainment, performance and income at work, physical and men-
we review evidence from recent research on the bidirec- tal health, and longevity (Calvin et al., 2017; Kuncel & Hezlett,
tional relations between academic achievement and cog- 2010; Wrulich et al., 2014). Not surprisingly, much research in
nitive abilities. Our findings suggest that (a) reading/ the past several decades has explored factors associated with
mathematics and cognitive abilities (i.e., working memory, academic achievement and how to incorporate these factors into
reasoning, and executive function) predict each other in intervention and instruction to improve academic performance
development, (b) direct academic instruction positively and remediate learning difficulties (for reviews, see Gersten,
affects the development of reasoning, and (c) such bidi- Fuchs, Williams, & Baker, 2001; Stockard, Wood, Coughlin, &
rectional relations between cognitive abilities and aca- Khoury, 2018). This rich body of research has identified two
demic achievement seem weaker among children with major categories of factors important for academic development:
disadvantages (e.g., those with special needs or low One is a set of foundational domain-specific skills. For example,
socioeconomic status). Together, these findings are in line meta-linguistic skills (e.g., phonological processing, orthographic
with the theory of mutualism and the transactional model. knowledge, morphological awareness), fluency, and comprehen-
They suggest that sustained and high-quality schooling sion strategies are critical for word reading and reading compre-
and education directly foster children’s academic and hension (National Reading Panel, 2000), and numerical skills
cognitive development, and may indirectly affect aca- such as number sense and fact retrieval are often suggested as
demic and cognitive development by triggering cognitive- foundations for mathematics (Geary, 2004).
academic bidirectionality. The other set of factors important for academic performance
consists of cognitive abilities, including but not limited to work-
KEYWORDS—bidirectional; mutualism; transactional; aca- ing memory (simultaneous information storage and manipulation;
demic development; cognitive development Peng et al., 2018), reasoning (the capacity to solve novel and
complex problems; Sternberg, Kaufman, & Grigorenko, 2008),
Academic achievement plays an important role in child devel- and executive function (cognitive and social-emotional processes
opment because academic skills, especially in reading and that underlie goal-directed behavior such as flexible thinking,
self-control, and self-regulation; Best & Miller, 2010). In this
Peng Peng, University of Texas at Austin, Rogier A. Kievit, article, we focus on the relation between academic achievement
University of Cambridge. and cognitive abilities. We also consider a relatively new per-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to spective for this relation: mutualism or bidirectionality—the
Peng Peng, Department of Special Education, College of Education, facilitatory, reciprocal effects between individual skills that sup-
The University of Texas at Austin, 1912 Speedway, Stop D5300, port and amplify the development of academic achievement and
Austin, TX 78712; e-mail: kevpp2004@hotmail.com.
cognitive performance (van der Maas et al., 2006).
© 2020 The Authors In the following sections, we first describe the conventional
Child Development Perspectives published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. on behalf of Society
for Research in Child Development.
assumption that cognitive abilities lead to academic achieve-
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution- ment. Next, we present theories and hypotheses suggesting that
NonCommercial-NoDerivs License, which permits use and distribution in any medium, these are related bidirectionally, and we review recent research
provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non-commercial and no on these bidirectional effects with a focus on how cognitive abil-
modifications or adaptations are made.
DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12352
ities (working memory, reasoning, and executive function) are

Volume 14, Number 1, 2020, Pages 15–20


17508606, 2020, 1, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/cdep.12352 by Cochrane Malaysia, Wiley Online Library on [11/06/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
16 Peng and Kievit

related to reading and mathematics achievement. We also dis- achievement and these cognitive abilities should predict each
cuss mechanisms for bidirectional relations between cognitive other from a longitudinal perspective, and 3) interventions tar-
abilities and academic achievement, with a focus on a transac- geting these cognitive abilities should result in improved aca-
tional framework in the context of education. Finally, we make demic performance and vice versa. Next, we review evidence for
recommendations for studying these bidirectional relations. each of these hypotheses.

UNIDIRECTIONAL EFFECT: COGNITIVE AGE EFFECT


ABILITIES ? ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT
Meta-analyses are a major source of evidence for an age effect.
Conventional opinions and most research on the relation They include studies tapping different age groups, so they can
between cognitive abilities and academic achievement have provide a sufficiently wide age range and enough statistical
treated cognitive abilities as foundational constructs, presuppos- power to detect whether the relation between cognitive abili-
ing that cognitive abilities are primary and cause academic out- ties and academic performance increases with age. In a meta-
comes (e.g., Cattell, 1987; Sternberg et al., 2008). This view of analysis of 680 studies on the relations between nonverbal rea-
cognitive abilities ? academic achievement is best explained by soning and reading/mathematics across a wide range of age
two influential cognitive theories: investment theory and dual- groups (3–80 years old; Peng, Wang, Wang, & Lin, 2019),
process theory. According to the investment theory, the develop- these relations increased with age, even after controlling for
ment of cognitive abilities is influenced mostly by biological, confounding variables (i.e., different types of reading or mathe-
genetic, and health factors, not by education (Cattell, 1987; matics skills, socioeconomic status [SES], and sample types).
Deary, Penke, & Johnson, 2010). Thus, academic performance However, the age effect was relatively small (correlations
is a result of the investment of cognitive abilities and the envi- increased by about .03 for every decade). In another meta-
ronmental stimulation offered by, for example, educational set- analysis of 197 studies of typically developing individuals (4–
tings, and cognitive abilities are assumed to be the basis for the 80 years old; Peng et al., 2018), the relations between reading
development of academic performance (Cattell, 1987). and working memory increased with grade level, even after
Similarly, the dual-process theory of higher cognition claims controlling for confounding variables (i.e., publication type,
that individuals process familiar information in an autonomous types of tasks, working memory materials, and bilingual sta-
way that requires few cognitive resources, and they process tus). This meta-analysis also suggested that reading develop-
novel information in a controlled way that requires many cogni- ment might further strengthen the relations between working
tive resources (Evans & Stanovich, 2013). Thus, the involvement memory and verbal long-term memory, thus boosting verbal
of cognitive abilities in an academic task is determined mostly working memory and its role in reading development. Such
by how efficiently the academic task can be performed, which is bidirectional relations between reading and verbal working
closely associated with long-term memory of knowledge of the memory suggested that domain-specific features of working
task. Therefore, performing an academic task at an early stage memory can be attributed in part to academic development.
of learning should take more effort and be more demanding cog- Three other meta-analyses have explored the relations
nitively, whereas with further development and accumulated between executive function and academic achievement, with
knowledge, cognitive abilities may be less involved, so individu- findings showing a less clear age effect. One meta-analysis exam-
als are more likely to rely on direct retrieval of knowledge from ined the relation between executive function (including working
long-term memory. memory, inhibition, and flexibility) and reading in individuals
from age 6 years to adulthood (Follmer, 2018). The results did
BIDIRECTIONALITY: COGNITIVE not indicate that this relation varied with age or age group (ages
ABILITIES ↔ ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT 6–11 vs. ages 12–17 vs. adult), although the relation was smaller
for adults (r = .25) than for children and adolescents
In recent years, the unidirectional relation between cognitive (rs = .33~.38). In another meta-analysis of school-age children,
abilities and academic performance has been challenged by the the relation between executive function (including working mem-
theory of mutualism, which claims that different types of skills ory, attentional control, flexibility, and inhibition) and academic
and abilities become bidirectionally related during human achievement did not vary with age, but the authors claimed that
development as a consequence of mutually beneficial interac- this relation was somewhat stronger for children and youth aged
tions between originally uncorrelated cognitive processes (van 6–18 than for children aged 3–5 (Jacob & Parkinson, 2015).
der Maas et al., 2006). Therefore, cognitive abilities and aca- Finally, two meta-analyses studied the relation between flexibil-
demic achievement should influence each other through devel- ity and academic performance among elementary school children
opment, and 1) the relation between academic achievement and (Yeniad, Malda, Mesman, van IJzendoorn, & Pieper, 2013).
relevant important cognitive abilities (working memory, reason- Their findings also did not suggest that the relation between flexi-
ing, executive function) should increase with age, 2) academic bility and reading/mathematics varied with age.

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 14, Number 1, 2020, Pages 15–20


17508606, 2020, 1, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/cdep.12352 by Cochrane Malaysia, Wiley Online Library on [11/06/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
The bidirectional relationship between cognitive and academic development 17

LONGITUDINAL RELATIONS memory and executive function training. However, meta-analy-


ses have suggested that although such cognitive training has
In a recent meta-analysis (Peng et al., 2019), my colleagues and resulted in effects on performance of trained or similar cognitive
I explored bidirectional relations between cognitive abilities and tasks, evidence of transfer effects on nontrained academic per-
academic achievement from a longitudinal perspective. The formance has been weak or rare (e.g., Jacob & Parkinson,
findings suggest that nonverbal reasoning and reading/mathe- 2015). In contrast, reviews and meta-analyses have documented
matics predicted each other in development even after control- positive effects of academic instruction on cognitive abilities. In
ling for initial performance, although almost all these one meta-analysis of 328 intervention studies of direct academic
synthesized longitudinal relations were from studies of children skill instruction (including reading, math, language, spelling,
(ages 6–10). Several individual studies with more complicated and multiple or other academic subjects) among school-age chil-
research designs have shown that working memory, executive dren, academic instruction positively affected academic perfor-
function, and IQ are related longitudinally to academic achieve- mance, and those effects transferred to performance on
ment across childhood and early adulthood. Specifically, using measures of ability and IQ—although the authors did not
cross-lagged modeling on a large data sample from the Early describe these measures in detail (Stockard et al., 2018).
Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten, one study showed
that working memory and reading/mathematics significantly pre- MECHANISMS OF COGNITIVE-ACADEMIC
dicted each other from the beginning of kindergarten through BIDIRECTIONALITY
second grade (Miller-Cotto & Byrnes, 2019). Another study
showed a bidirectional relation between executive function (in- Researchers have proposed several mechanisms for cognitive-
cluding flexibility, working memory, and inhibition) and mathe- academic bidirectionality. For instance, Cattell’s investment the-
matics, and a significant relation between the growth of ory posits that reasoning ability underlies the acquisition of aca-
executive function and the growth of mathematics, over the pre- demic achievement because greater reasoning facilitates the use
school period (Schmitt, Geldhof, Purpura, Duncan, & McClel- of analogies and abstract schema that help organize and solidify
land, 2017). However, this study did not show such a academic knowledge (for an updated perspective on Cattell, see
bidirectional relation between executive function and reading. Schweizer & Koch, 2002). In contrast, concrete knowledge may
Other studies have examined the relation between IQ (indi- benefit more abstract cognitive abilities through mechanisms
cated as performance IQ and full-scale IQ from the Wechsler such as semantic bootstrapping; according to this hypothesis,
Intelligence Scale) and reading (based on a Woodcock–Johnson children with more advanced verbal skills (e.g., vocabulary) can
reading composite) from first through 12th grades based on decompose abstract cognitive problems into constituent rules
latent change score models (Ferrer et al., 2007; Ferrer, Shaywitz, more efficiently (e.g., Kievit et al., 2017). The availability of
Holahan, Marchione, & Shaywitz, 2010). They found a positive greater verbal resources may also lower demands on working
dynamic relation between reading and IQ. They also found that memory for maintaining and applying such rules (Gathercole,
the dynamics of reading and IQ appeared to be of stronger mag- Service, Hitch, Adams, & Martin, 1999).
nitude from first through third grade, less strong from fourth Transactional processes are another potentially important
through eighth grade, and weaker from ninth through 12th mechanism (Dickens & Flynn, 2001; Tucker-Drob, Briley, &
grade, and that such bidirectional effects were obvious only Harden, 2013); researchers claim that genetic influences on cog-
among typically developing children, not among children with nition increase from infancy to adulthood and are maximized in
learning disabilities. In a more recent study, latent change score more advantaged socioeconomic contexts. The net result is the
models were used to demonstrate that vocabulary and nonverbal mutual influence between academic achievement and cognitive
reasoning mutually influenced each other during development abilities, which is moderated largely by the environment, so this
(Kievit, Hofman, & Nation, 2019; Kievit et al., 2017); this bidi- mutual influence is more likely to occur in relatively richer as
rectional effect seemed stronger among 6- to 8-year-olds than opposed to lower-income environments (Tucker-Drob et al.,
among 14- to 25-year-olds. 2013). Two related but relatively independent environmental
factors are often considered: schooling and learning experiences
INTERVENTION EFFECTS or opportunities associated with families’ SES (Armor, Marks, &
Malatinszky, 2018).
In comparison with correlational studies, intervention studies Schooling, with its primary function of academic instruction,
with rigorous designs (e.g., randomized controlled trials) can can explain bidirectional relations between cognitive abilities
offer convincing evidence for bidirectional relations between and academic performance (Ceci & Williams, 1997; Jacob &
cognitive abilities and academic achievement if such relations Parkinson, 2015; Ritchie & Tucker-Drob, 2018). Early on, chil-
exist. In terms of the training effects of cognitive abilities on dren use cognitive abilities to learn academic skills, and per-
academic achievement, most intervention studies from the last forming most academic tasks involves the use of those cognitive
two decades have focused on short-term intensive working abilities (Evans & Stanovich, 2013; Peng et al., 2018). Thus,

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 14, Number 1, 2020, Pages 15–20


17508606, 2020, 1, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/cdep.12352 by Cochrane Malaysia, Wiley Online Library on [11/06/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
18 Peng and Kievit

academic tasks practiced during schooling may offer long-term cognitive abilities (working memory, reasoning, and executive
training for cognitive abilities, too. Following this logic, the bidi- function) are moderate (rs = .30~.45, 10 ~ 20% variance over-
rectional relations between cognitive abilities and reading/math- lap; Jacob & Parkinson, 2015; Peng et al., 2019), and the effect
ematics may be most obvious during elementary and secondary of age on these relations is small though significant (Peng et al.,
schooling, when reading and mathematics are taught systemati- 2018, 2019). Thus, short-term cognitive training may be insuffi-
cally and practiced intensively (Peng et al., 2019). Indeed, this cient to improve academic performance. For most children,
hypothesis can help explain why bidirectional relations between long-lasting learning or the continued experiences of using read-
academic achievement and reasoning found in longitudinal data ing and mathematics skills at school may be the ideal approach
are stronger among children than among adolescents and adults to improving both cognitive abilities and academic skills (Ceci
(Ferrer et al., 2007, 2010; Kievit et al., 2019). In addition, & Williams, 1997).
schooling may promote teacher-student interactions that directly
improve executive function among young children (Vanden- CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS FOR
broucke, Spilt, Verschueren, Piccinin, & Baeyens, 2018), which FURTHER STUDY
may indirectly contribute to cognitive-academic bidirectionality
(e.g., schooling ? teacher–student interactions ? executive Meta-analyses have provided evidence that the relations
function ? academic achievement). between reading/mathematics and working memory/reasoning
Learning experiences or opportunities associated with SES increase with age, but age effects were unclear for relations
also influence bidirectional relations between cognitive abilities involving executive functions. Reading/mathematics and cogni-
and academic performance. In high-SES contexts, children have tive abilities (working memory, reasoning, and executive func-
abundant opportunities for positive learning experiences, tion) predicted each other’s development. Intensive short-term
whereas opportunities are much fewer in low-SES contexts cognitive training did not produce reliable transfer effects on
(Duncan & Murnane, 2011). Much research has documented academic achievement, but direct academic instruction posi-
the effectiveness of early academic intervention on academic tively affected cognitive development. Cognitive-academic bidi-
performance among children from low-SES backgrounds or with rectional relations seemed weaker among children with
special needs (Dietrichson, Bøg, Filges, & Klint Jørgensen, disadvantages. Together, these findings suggest a pattern of bidi-
2017; Jacob & Parkinson, 2015). However, the effects of such rectional relations between cognitive abilities and academic
early academic intervention generally fade after the intervention achievement. However, several issues need further investigation
(Jenkins et al., 2018), and fadeout is more likely among those to understand these relationships more thoroughly.
who had very low academic skills before treatment (Bailey Age effects may be quite nuanced. Specifically, effects of age
et al., 2016). These findings, together with the null findings were not consistently reported across studies, and to the extent
obtained in longitudinal data for bidirectional relations between that evidence in favor of age-related differences in correlations
cognitive skills and academic performance among children with have been shown, they are based mostly on meta-analyses rather
learning disabilities (Ferrer et al., 2010), further support the than on single large-scale cross-sectional empirical studies
transactional model. That is, children with advantages (e.g., with (although see Hofman et al., 2018). Evidence from large cross-
high SES and relatively strong cognitive abilities and founda- sectional studies would circumvent the potential limit of hetero-
tional academic skills) in early development are more likely to geneity of methods and measures that could affect findings from
trigger and benefit from cognitive-academic bidirectionality. meta-analyses and reviews. We need such studies, as well as
That said, researchers have also suggested that sustained high- large longitudinal studies (Molenaar, 2004), to investigate
quality schooling may offset the negative effects of low SES on whether age is indeed associated with changes in the association
academic achievement (Jenkins et al., 2018) and decrease the between cognitive abilities and academic achievement. How-
inhibitive effects of low SES on relations between cognitive abil- ever, although the relation between cognitive abilities and aca-
ities and academic achievement (Peng et al., 2019). demic achievement may vary with age, this does not necessarily
Together within the transactional model, these points have indicate a causal, bidirectional relation. A range of factors—in-
important implications for education and cognitive training. Sus- cluding but not limited to different age-related trajectories,
tained, high-quality schooling and education, especially in the changes in task demands and strategies, correlated and uncorre-
primary and secondary years, not only foster children’s aca- lated changes beyond the modeled effects, the magnitude of
demic and cognitive development directly but also contribute autoregressive paths, and time-varying disturbances—all can,
indirectly to academic and cognitive development by facilitating and do, affect cross-sectional and longitudinal correlations so
cognitive-academic bidirectionality. Such facilitation is espe- they can increase, decrease, or remain static depending on the
cially important for children with disadvantages, who often lack precise causal mechanisms at play.
the resources or foundational skills to trigger and benefit from We also need to consider measurement issues in explaining
the interaction between cognitive skills and academic perfor- bidirectional relations between cognitive abilities and academic
mance. But the relations between reading/mathematics and achievement. First, measures of cognitive abilities and academic

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 14, Number 1, 2020, Pages 15–20


17508606, 2020, 1, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/cdep.12352 by Cochrane Malaysia, Wiley Online Library on [11/06/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
The bidirectional relationship between cognitive and academic development 19

achievement do not necessarily reflect two discrete entities. may be further explained and moderated by developmental
Measures of cognitive ability often tap aspects of achievement stages, domains of academic skills, types of cognitive skills, and
skills (e.g., measures of verbal and numerical working memory academically relevant social-emotional factors.
often heavily tap knowledge of language, reading, and numerals,
REFERENCES
especially for young children), and achievement tests often mea-
sure aspects of cognitive abilities (e.g., phonological awareness Armor, D. J., Marks, G. N., & Malatinszky, A. (2018). The impact of
and reading comprehension often tap working memory, reason- school SES on student achievement: Evidence from U.S. statewide
ing, and executive function). In this regard, pure measures of achievement data. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 40,
cognitive abilities or academic achievement do not exist. In 613–630. https://doi.org/10.3102/0162373718787917
other words, part of the associations in the studies reviewed here Bailey, D. H., Nguyen, T., Jenkins, J. M., Domina, T., Clements, D. H.,
may be explained by the inherent degree to which purportedly & Sarama, J. S. (2016). Fadeout in an early mathematics
intervention: Constraining content or preexisting differences?
task-pure measurements tap many different academic and cogni-
Developmental Psychology, 52, 1457–1469. https://doi.org/10.
tive domains. 1037/dev0000188
In addition, this issue may be especially pressing when con- Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on
sidering executive function (Jacob & Parkinson, 2015; Miyake executive function. Child Development, 81, 1641–1660. https://doi.
& Friedman, 2012). Different executive functions are often org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x
entangled, and it is difficult to establish that different execu- Calvin, C. M., Batty, G. D., Der, G., Brett, C. E., Taylor, A., Pattie, A.,
tive tests assess different components of executive function . . . Deary, I. J. (2017). Childhood intelligence in relation to major
(Miyake & Friedman, 2012). Moreover, executive function is a causes of death in 68 year follow-up: Prospective population study.
BMJ, 357, j2708. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.j2708
complex and developmentally changing construct (Best &
Cattell, R. B. (1987). Intelligence: Its structure, growth and action. Ams-
Miller, 2010). Different executive functions have different terdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier, North-Holland.
developmental trajectories. Among all executive functions, Ceci, S. J., & Williams, W. M. (1997). Schooling, intelligence, and
inhibition seems to develop first and mature in early child- income. American Psychologist, 52, 1051–1058. https://doi.org/10.
hood, whereas flexibility develops along a slower, longer trajec- 1037/0003-066X.52.10.1051
tory until late adolescence (Best & Miller, 2010; Zelazo & Deary, I. J., Penke, L., & Johnson, W. (2010). The neuroscience of
Carlson, 2012). Executive functions that do not include moti- human intelligence differences. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11,
201–211. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2793
vation and emotion processing tend to mature faster than exec-
Dickens, W. T., & Flynn, J. R. (2001). Heritability estimates versus
utive functions that do (e.g., self-regulation and control; Zelazo large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved. Psychologi-
& Carlson, 2012). Thus, researchers may be able to more thor- cal Review, 108, 346–369. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.
oughly explain and further investigate inconclusive effects of 108.2.346
age on the relation between executive functions and academic Dietrichson, J., Bøg, M., Filges, T., & Klint Jørgensen, A.-M. (2017).
performance by considering the measurement and development Academic interventions for elementary and middle school students
of executive function. with low socioeconomic status: A systematic review and meta-anal-
ysis. Review of Educational Research, 87, 243–282. https://doi.org/
We should also consider moderators on the bidirectional rela-
10.3102/0034654316687036
tions. Studies (Ferrer et al., 2007, 2010) show that although IQ Duncan, G. J., & Murnane, R. J. (Eds.). (2011). Whither opportunity?
is related bidirectionally to reading during development, IQ Rising inequality, schools, and children’s life chances. New York,
seems to drive this relation, partly in line with investment theory NY: Russell Sage Foundation.
(academic skills develop through the investment of cognitive Evans, J. S. B. T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of
abilities). Similarly, although another study (Schmitt et al., higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychologi-
2017) showed a bidirectional relation between mathematics and cal Science, 8, 223–241. https://doi.org/10.1177/
executive function during the preschool years, it also found that, 1745691612460685
Ferrer, E., McArdle, J. J., Shaywitz, B. A., Holahan, J. M., Marchione,
starting in kindergarten, only executive function predicted math-
K., & Shaywitz, S. E. (2007). Longitudinal models of developmental
ematics performance. Moreover, evidence from longitudinal dynamics between reading and cognition from childhood to adoles-
studies suggests that bidirectionality may exist within an aca- cence. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1460–1473. https://doi.org/
demic domain (e.g., different types of mathematics promote each 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1460
other’s development; Hofman et al., 2018), across academic Ferrer, E., Shaywitz, B. A., Holahan, J. M., Marchione, K., & Shaywitz,
domains (e.g., reading and mathematics mutually promote each S. E. (2010). Uncoupling of reading and IQ over time: Empirical
other’s development; Schmitt et al., 2017), and between aca- evidence for a definition of dyslexia. Psychological Science, 21, 93–
101. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609354084
demic and social-emotional domains (e.g., bidirectional relations
Follmer, D. J. (2018). Executive function and reading comprehension: A
between self-concept/motivation and academic performance; meta-analytic review. Educational Psychologist, 53, 42–60.
Sewasew & Schroeders, 2019). These findings suggest that the https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2017.1309295
directionality of the relation between cognitive abilities and aca- Gathercole, S. E., Service, E., Hitch, G. J., Adams, A.-M., & Martin, A.
demic achievement, and the magnitude of this directionality J. (1999). Phonological short-term memory and vocabulary

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 14, Number 1, 2020, Pages 15–20


17508606, 2020, 1, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/cdep.12352 by Cochrane Malaysia, Wiley Online Library on [11/06/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
20 Peng and Kievit

development: Further evidence on the nature of the relationship. and working memory. Psychological Bulletin, 144, 48–76. https://d
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 65–77. https://doi.org/10.1002/ oi.org/10.1037/bul0000124
(SICI)1099-0720(199902)13:1<65:AID-ACP548>3.0.CO;2-O Peng, P., Wang, T., Wang, C., & Lin, X. (2019). A meta-analysis on the
Geary, D. C. (2004). Mathematics and learning disabilities. Journal of relation between fluid intelligence and reading/mathematics:
Learning Disabilities, 37, 4–15. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Effects of tasks, age, and social economics status. Psychological
00222194040370010201. Bulletin, 145, 189–236. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000182
Gersten, R., Fuchs, L. S., Williams, J. P., & Baker, S. (2001). Teaching Ritchie, S. J., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2018). How much does education
reading comprehension strategies to students with learning disabili- improve intelligence? A meta-analysis. Psychological Science, 29,
ties: A review of research. Review of Educational Research, 71, 1358–1369. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797618774253
279–320. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543071002279 Schmitt, S. A., Geldhof, G. J., Purpura, D. J., Duncan, R., & McClelland,
Hofman, A. D., Kievit, R., Stevenson, C., Molenaar, D., Visser, I., & van M. M..(2017). Examining the relations between executive function,
der Maas, H. (2018). The dynamics of the development of mathe- math, and literacy during the transition to kindergarten: A multi-
matics skills: A comparison of theories of developing intelligence. analytic approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109, 1120–
Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/xa2ft 1140. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000193
Jacob, R., & Parkinson, J. (2015). The potential for school-based inter- Schweizer, K., & Koch, W. (2002). A revision of Cattell’s investment
ventions that target executive function to improve academic theory: Cognitive properties influencing learning. Learning and
achievement: A review. Review of Educational Research, 85, 512– Individual Differences, 13, 57–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1041-
552. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654314561338 6080(02)00062-6
Jenkins, J. M., Watts, T. W., Magnuson, K., Gershoff, E. T., Clements, Sewasew, D., & Schroeders, U. (2019). The developmental interplay of
D. H., Sarama, J., & Duncan, G. J. (2018). Do high-quality kinder- academic self-concept and achievement within and across domains
garten and first-grade classrooms mitigate preschool fadeout? Jour- among primary school students. Contemporary Educational Psy-
nal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 11, 339–374. chology, 58, 204–212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.03.
https://doi.org/10.1080/19345747.2018.1441347 009
Kievit, R. A., Hofman, A. D., & Nation, K. (2019). Mutualistic cou- Sternberg, R. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2008). Applied
pling between vocabulary and reasoning in young children: A intelligence. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Replication and Extension of the Study by Kievit et al (2017). Stockard, J., Wood, T. W., Coughlin, C., & Khoury, C. R. (2018). The
Psychological Science, 30(8), 1245–1252. doi: 10.1177/ effectiveness of direct instruction curricula: A meta-analysis of a
0956797619841265 half century of research. Review of Educational Research, 88, 479–
Kievit, R. A., Lindenberger, U., Goodyer, I. M., Jones, P. B., Fonagy, P., 507. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654317751919
Bullmore, E. T., . . . Dolan, R. J. (2017). Mutualistic coupling Tucker-Drob, E. M., Briley, D. A., & Harden, K. P. (2013). Genetic and
between vocabulary and reasoning supports cognitive development environmental influences on cognition across development and
during late adolescence and early adulthood. Psychological Science, context. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 349–355.
28, 1419–1431. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617710785 https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413485087.
Kuncel, N. R., & Hezlett, S. A. (2010). Fact and fiction in cognitive abil- van der Maas, H. L. J., Dolan, C. V., Grasman, R. P. P. P., Wicherts, J.
ity testing for admissions and hiring decisions. Current Directions M., Huizenga, H. M., & Raijmakers, M. E. J. (2006). A dynamical
in Psychological Science, 19, 339–345. https://doi.org/10.1177/ model of general intelligence: The positive manifold of intelligence
0963721410389459 by mutualism. Psychological Review, 113, 842–861. https://doi.org/
Miller-Cotto, D., & Byrnes, J. P. (2019). What’s the best way to charac- 10.1037/0033-295X.113.4.842
terize the relationship between working memory and achievement? Vandenbroucke, L., Spilt, J., Verschueren, K., Piccinin, C., & Baeyens,
An initial examination of competing theories. Journal of Educa- D. (2018). The classroom as a developmental context for cognitive
tional Psychology. Advance online publication, https://doi.org/10. development: A meta-analysis on the importance of teacher–stu-
1037/edu0000395 dent interactions for children’s executive functions. Review of Edu-
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of cational Research, 88, 125–164. https://doi.org/10.3102/
individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclu- 0034654317743200
sions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 8–14. Wrulich, M., Brunner, M., Stadler, G., Schalke, D., Keller, U., & Martin,
https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458 R. (2014). Forty years on: Childhood intelligence predicts health in
Molenaar, P. C. (2004). A manifesto on psychology as idiographic middle adulthood. Health Psychology, 33, 292–296. https://doi.org/
science: Bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this 10.1037/a0030727
time forever. Measurement, 2, 201–218. https://doi.org/10.1207/ Yeniad, N., Malda, M., Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Pieper, S.
s15366359mea0204_1 (2013). Shifting ability predicts math and reading performance in
National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence- children: A meta-analytical study. Learning and Individual Differ-
based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and ences, 23, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2012.10.004
its implications for reading instruction. Bethesda, MD: National Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function
Institute of Child Health and Human Development in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child
Peng, P., Barnes, M., Wang, C., Wang, W., Li, S., Swanson, H. L., . . . Development Perspectives, 6, 354–360. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
Tao, S. (2018). A meta-analysis on the relation between reading 1750-8606.2012.00246.x

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 14, Number 1, 2020, Pages 15–20

You might also like