Automatic Craccoetal. - 2018 - PsychBul

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 151

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/323647498

Automatic Imitation: A Meta-Analysis

Article  in  Psychological Bulletin · March 2018


DOI: 10.1037/bul0000143

CITATIONS READS

151 4,667

9 authors, including:

Emiel Cracco Lara Bardi


Ghent University French National Centre for Scientific Research
78 PUBLICATIONS   946 CITATIONS    50 PUBLICATIONS   1,141 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Charlotte Desmet Oliver Genschow


City of Ghent, Belgium Leuphana University Lüneburg
17 PUBLICATIONS   392 CITATIONS    67 PUBLICATIONS   1,014 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

A Functional-Cognitive Perspective on Social Modulation of Imitation in Individuals With and Without Autism Spectrum Disorder View project

Validation study using fNIRS View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Emiel Cracco on 10 March 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Running head: AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 1

Automatic Imitation: A Meta-Analysis

Emiel Cracco, Lara Bardi, and Charlotte Desmet

Ghent University

Oliver Genschow

University of Cologne

Davide Rigoni

Ghent University

Lize De Coster

University of California, San Francisco

Ina Radkova, Eliane Deschrijver, and Marcel Brass

Ghent University
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 2

Author Note

Emiel Cracco, Lara Bardi, Charlotte Desmet, Davide Rigoni, Ina Radkova, Eliane

Deschrijver, and Marcel Brass, Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University;

Oliver Genschow, Social Cognition Center Cologne, University of Cologne; Lize De Coster,

Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco.

This work was supported by the Research Foundation Flanders Grant

FWO14/ASP/050 awarded to the first author. The second to eight author contributed

equally to the manuscript and the order of authorship for these authors was determined at

random. We are grateful to the researchers who provided us the required data for their

studies. We are especially thankful to the researchers who provided us their unpublished

data: Henryk Bukowski, Emily Butler, Tullio Liuzza, Barbara Müller, Lior Noy, Roland Pfister,

Birgit Rauchbauer, and Federica Riva. Finally, we would like to thank Richard Ramsey for his

insightful comments on a previous draft of this manuscript.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Emiel Cracco,

Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Henri Dunantlaan 2, Gent, B-

9000. E-mail: emiel.cracco@ugent.be.

Copyright Statement

The following manuscript is accepted for publication in Psychological Bulletin. ©American

Psychological Association, 2018. This paper is not the copy of record and may not exactly

replicate the final, authoritative version of the article. Please do not copy or cite without
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 3

authors permission. The final article will be available, upon publication, via its DOI:

10.1037/bul0000143.
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 4

Abstract

Automatic imitation is the finding that movement execution is facilitated by compatible and

impeded by incompatible observed movements. In the past fifteen years, automatic

imitation has been studied to understand the relation between perception and action in

social interaction. Although research on this topic started in cognitive science, interest

quickly spread to related disciplines such as social psychology, clinical psychology, and

neuroscience. However, important theoretical questions have remained unanswered.

Therefore, in the present meta-analysis, we evaluated seven key questions on automatic

imitation. The results, based on 161 studies containing 226 experiments, revealed an overall

effect size of gz = 0.95, 95% CI = [0.88, 1.02]. Moderator analyses identified automatic

imitation as a flexible, largely automatic process that is driven by movement and effector

compatibility, but is also influenced by spatial compatibility. Automatic imitation was found

to be stronger for forced choice tasks than for simple response tasks, for human agents than

for non-human agents, and for goalless actions than for goal-directed actions. However, it

was not modulated by more subtle factors such as animacy beliefs, motion profiles, or visual

perspective. Finally, there was no evidence for a relation between automatic imitation and

either empathy or autism. Among other things, these findings point towards actor-imitator

similarity as a crucial modulator of automatic imitation and challenge the view that imitative

tendencies are an indicator of social functioning. The current meta-analysis has important

theoretical implications and sheds light on longstanding controversies in the literature on

automatic imitation and related domains.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 5

Public Significance Statement

The current meta-analysis indicates that automatic imitation is a flexible, automatic process

that depends on how similar the actor is to the imitator. In contrast to popular belief, we

found no support for the hypothesis that automatic imitation is an indicator of social

functioning, which was studied in the form of empathy and autism spectrum disorder. These

results have important implications for our understanding of perception and action in the

context of social interaction.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 6

The mob, when they are gazing at a dancer on the slack rope, naturally writhe and

twist and balance their own bodies, as they see him do, and as they feel that they

themselves must do if in his situation (Smith, 1759, p. 5)

Why do we imitate others? Is it that we choose to imitate them, or rather that we

cannot control the urge to do so? Interest in these questions dates back to at least the 18th

century when Adam Smith (1759) argued that spontaneous imitation can be seen as a

primitive form of sympathy. In spite of the longstanding theoretical interest, however, it was

not until the 20th century that experimental evidence started to emerge. That is, in an

ingenious study, Hull (1933) was able to demonstrate that subjects had inadvertently copied

the body movements of an experimenter reaching forward and backward during a series of

psychological tests. This finding was later extended to mannerism (Chartrand & Bargh,

1999), postures (Sheflen, 1964), facial expressions (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999), and speech

patterns (Bock, 1986), and has subsequently become known as motor mimicry (Chartrand &

Lakin, 2013; Chartrand & van Baaren, 2009). More precisely, motor mimicry is the finding

that people tend to imitate each other during social interaction (Chartrand & Lakin, 2013;

Chartrand & van Baaren, 2009; Heyes, 2011).

However, the strongest evidence that imitative tendencies cannot be controlled

comes from research on automatic imitation. More specifically, this research has shown

that people imitate others even when it impairs task performance (Brass, Bekkering, &

Prinz, 2001; Brass, Bekkering, Wohlschlager, & Prinz, 2000; Kilner, Paulignan, & Blakemore,

2003; Stürmer et al., 2000). For example, in one of the first studies, Brass et al. (2000)

instructed participants to lift their index finger in response to the number “1” and their

middle finger in response to the number “2”. At the same time, a hand on the screen also
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 7

lifted its index finger, also lifted its middle finger, or did not move (Figure 1a). The results

showed that, compared with when the hand did not move, responses were faster and more

accurate when the observed movement matched the instructed response, but slower and

less accurate when the observed movement did not match the instructed response. Given

that participants copied the observed movements regardless of whether they facilitated or

impaired response selection, these results indicate that imitation could not be controlled

and therefore that it was involuntarily (Heyes, 2011; Moors & De Houwer, 2006; Proctor &

Vu, 2006). The finding that performance on a primary task is modulated by the observation

of actions that can either correspond or not correspond with the required response is now

widely known as automatic imitation (Heyes, 2011), and will be how the term is used in the

current paper.

As demonstrated in Figure 2, research on automatic imitation has grown

exponentially since the effect was first demonstrated in the early 2000s (Brass, Bekkering, et

al., 2001; Brass et al., 2000; Kilner et al., 2003; Stürmer et al., 2000). In contrast to motor

mimicry, which typically measures the frequency of imitation over an extended period of

time, automatic imitation measures reaction times (RTs), errors rates (ERs), or kinematics

(KMs) using stimulus-response compatibility paradigms (Heyes, 2011). The use of these

paradigms is rooted in a long tradition of psychological research on stimulus-response

compatibility (Simon, 1969; Stroop, 1935), and has been instrumental in understanding the

mechanisms that connect perception to action (Proctor & Vu, 2006). As part of this

tradition, the study of automatic imitation has provided crucial insights into the interplay

between perception and action in social settings (Heyes, 2011).

Although automatic imitation was first demonstrated in cognitive science, it is now

widely studied across a broad range of psychological disciplines. For example, it is thought
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 8

to rely on the same mechanisms as motor mimicry (Heyes, 2011). Moreover, these

mechanism are believed to facilitate social interaction (Stel, van Dijk, & van Baaren, 2016;

Wang & Hamilton, 2012) and to predict both empathy (Butler, Ward, & Ramsey, 2015;

Genschow et al., 2017) and autism spectrum disorder (Spengler, Bird, & Brass, 2010;

Williams, 2008; Williams, Whiten, Suddendorf, & Perrett, 2001). Furthermore, automatic

imitation is widely considered an index of mirror neuron activation (Bien, Roebroeck,

Goebel, & Sack, 2009; Catmur, Walsh, & Heyes, 2009; R. Cook, Bird, Catmur, Press, & Heyes,

2014; Heyes, 2010), serves as a model for social interaction in artificial intelligence

(Chaminade & Cheng, 2009), and has greatly advanced our understanding of the

mechanisms behind imitation in adults (Heyes, 2011), children (Heyes, 2016; Ray & Heyes,

2011), and non-human species (Heyes, 2012).

Following fifteen years of research, it is now well-established that automatic

imitation – the finding that movement execution is facilitated by compatible and impeded

by incompatible observed movements – exists (Heyes, 2011). However, important

theoretical questions remain. Some questions relate to the mechanisms underlying

automatic imitation: What processes contribute to automatic imitation? Is automatic

imitation flexible? When during action preparation does automatic imitation operate? Is

automatic imitation really automatic? Other questions relate to the factors that modulate

automatic imitation: What is the role of action goals in automatic imitation? Is automatic

imitation sensitive to the similarity between actor and imitator? Is automatic imitation

related to social abilities? Each of these questions is essential to understand the connection

between perception and action in social interaction. As such, answering them will not only

have important implications for the domain of automatic imitation but also for the broader

literature on how we represent others’ actions.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 9

Although several narrative reviews on automatic imitation already exist (e.g.,

Chaminade & Cheng, 2009; Gowen, Stanley, & Miall, 2008; Heyes, 2011; Press, 2011),

inconsistent evidence has made it difficult to provide conclusive answers to the

aforementioned questions. To address this issue, the current study, we will use a meta-

analytic approach. A meta-analysis is well-suited to evaluate inconsistent evidence because

it provides a quantitative estimate of the available evidence based on the combined data of

many independent studies (Hedges & Vevea, 1998; Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). Moreover, a

meta-analysis allows us to identify important methodological variables that should be taken

into account in research on automatic imitation to ensure that the effects are unbiased and

reliable. In the remainder of our introduction, we will first describe the paradigms and

theories of automatic imitation and will then provide a brief review of the different

questions that are addressed in the meta-analysis.

--- Insert Figure 1 and 2 about here ---

Paradigms of Automatic Imitation

Automatic imitation in the current meta-analysis will be operationalized as “a type of

stimulus-response compatibility effect in which the topographical features of task-irrelevant

action stimuli facilitate similar, and interfere with dissimilar, responses” (Heyes, 2011, p.

463). There are three paradigms that fit this description: a forced choice RT paradigm, a

simple response RT paradigm, and a simple response KM paradigm. All three paradigms

investigate the interference of an irrelevant observed movement on a relevant executed

movement. In the forced choice RT paradigm, participants are required to select a response
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 10

on the basis of a symbolic cue while a hand on the screen makes an irrelevant compatible or

incompatible movement (Brass et al., 2000; Stürmer et al., 2000). In the study by Brass et al.

(2000) described earlier, a compatible trial is when the stimulus hand lifts the same finger as

the finger that has to be lifted by the participant (e.g., see index finger, move index finger)

and an incompatible trial is when the stimulus hand lifts a different finger as the finger that

has to be lifted by the participant (e.g., see index finger, move middle finger). Automatic

imitation in this paradigm is the finding that RTs are slower and ERs higher on incompatible

trials than on compatible trials (e.g., Bertenthal, Longo, & Kosobud, 2006; Brass et al., 2000;

Catmur & Heyes, 2011; Gillmeister, Catmur, Liepelt, Brass, & Heyes, 2008; Otte, Habel,

Schulte-Ruther, Konrad, & Koch, 2011; Stürmer et al., 2000).

In the simple response RT paradigms, participants are no longer required to choose a

response on the basis of a cue but can instead prepare the relevant response before the

onset of the trial. For example, Brass et al. (2001) instructed participants to either lift or tap

their index finger as soon as the stimulus hand lifted or tapped its index finger. In line with

the forced choice RT paradigm, it was found that responses on compatible trials were faster

than responses on incompatible trials (e.g., Bird, Leighton, Press, & Heyes, 2007; Brass,

Bekkering, et al., 2001; Heyes, Bird, Johnson, & Haggard, 2005; Otte, Habel, et al., 2011;

Press, Bird, Walsh, & Heyes, 2008). Finally, the simple response KM paradigm follows a

procedure that is similar to the simple response RT paradigm but focuses on KMs rather

than RTs. In a prominent study, Kilner et al. (2003) asked participants to move their arm

back and forth in the horizontal or vertical plane while facing an experimenter who moved

his arm in the compatible (i.e., parallel) or incompatible (i.e., orthogonal) plane. In line with

the RT paradigms, participants’ movement trajectory contained more variability when the

experimenter performed an incompatible movement than when he performed a compatible


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 11

movement (e.g., Bouquet, Gaurier, Shipley, Toussaint, & Blandin, 2007; Kilner, Hamilton, &

Blakemore, 2007; Roberts, Bennett, & Hayes, 2015; Stanley, Gowen, & Miall, 2007).

Theories of Automatic Imitation

In contrast to early views assuming an innate imitation mechanism (Meltzoff &

Moore, 1983, 1989), the current dominant view is that automatic imitation develops as a

consequence of domain-general learning mechanisms (Brass & Heyes, 2005). In particular,

this view is substantiated by two distinct yet related theories. A first theory is the associative

sequence learning (ASL) theory (Brass & Heyes, 2005; Cook et al., 2014; Heyes, 2010; Ray &

Heyes, 2011). This theory argues that the visual representation of an action is initially

unconnected to its motor representation. Instead, these connections develop gradually

because action execution produces perceivable sensory consequences that over time

become associated with the motor command that produces them. For example, when we

grasp an object, we typically see how our hand grasps the object. Likewise, when we express

an emotion, we tend to see the same emotion expressed on the face of others. These

experiences lead to bidirectional connections between perceiving and executing an action,

and this is thought to cause automatic imitation.

A second theory, named ideomotor (IM) theory, extends ASL by assuming an

additional mechanism (Brass & Heyes, 2005; Greenwald, 1970; Hommel, Müsseler,

Aschersleben, & Prinz, 2001; Prinz, 1997; Shin, Proctor, & Capaldi, 2010). Like ASL, IM posits

that associative learning leads to connections between visual and motor representations.

However, IM further predicts that this learning process culminates in the development of

ideomotor representations that represent actions in terms of their anticipated sensory


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 12

consequences (Greenwald, 1970). Given that the visual image of an action is part of its

sensory consequences, seeing an action primes the ideomotor representation controlling

that action (Brass & Muhle-Karbe, 2014), and this leads to automatic imitation. In other

words, IM and ASL differ in whether or not they assume ideomotor representations, but

agree in their emphasis on the role of motor learning (ML) in automatic imitation (Brass &

Heyes, 2005). Therefore we will refer to these theories as ML theories of automatic

imitation.

At the neurophysiological level, automatic imitation has mostly been explained in

relation to the mirror neuron system (Bien et al., 2009; Brass & Heyes, 2005; Catmur et al.,

2009; R. Cook et al., 2014; Heyes, 2011; Simpson, Murray, Paukner, & Ferrari, 2014). The

mirror neuron system refers to a network of motor areas in the frontal and parietal cortex

that do not only respond to action execution but also to action observation (Rizzolatti &

Craighero, 2004; Rizzolatti & Sinigaglia, 2010). Although this system was initially discovered

in the monkey brain (Gallese, Fadiga, Fogassi, & Rizzolatti, 1996), there is now converging

evidence that a similar system exists in humans as well (Molenberghs, Cunnington, &

Mattingley, 2012). Importantly, the presence of a mirror neuron system is consistent with

both ML theories of automatic imitation (Brass & Heyes, 2005). More specifically, ASL

interprets mirror neurons in the context of associative networks (Cook et al., 2014; Heyes,

2010) and IM sees mirror neurons as the neurophysiological manifestation of ideomotor

representations (Brass & Muhle-Karbe, 2014).


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 13

The Present Meta-Analysis

The current meta-analysis is structured around seven key questions in the literature.

As outlined above, these questions concern the mechanisms underlying automatic imitation

and the conditions under which automatic imitation occurs. In the remainder of the

introduction, we will briefly review extant research on these questions and explain how they

will be addressed in the meta-analysis that follows. Our meta-analysis will focus primarily on

RT data, but will also report ERs as a secondary outcome measure. In general, we expect

that RTs will be more sensitive to modulatory influences than ERs because the number of

errors is typically low (i.e. < 5%) in automatic imitation studies (e.g., Brass et al., 2000;

Stürmer et al., 2000). For KM tasks of automatic imitation, we will restrict our analysis to

questions that have been considered relevant in these tasks because the number of KM

studies (n = 23) is insufficient to explore all moderators.

What Processes Contribute to Automatic Imitation?

Imitative Versus Spatial Compatibility. To understand automatic imitation, it is

important to delimit the processes that do and the processes that do not contribute. For

example, a longstanding discussion concerns the role of spatial processes in automatic

imitation (Aicken, Wilson, Williams, & Mon-Williams, 2007; Bertenthal et al., 2006; Boyer,

Longo, & Bertenthal, 2012; Catmur & Heyes, 2011; Heyes, 2011; Jansson, Wilson, Williams,

& Mon-Williams, 2007; Jimenez et al., 2012; Press et al., 2008). For example, in the study of

Brass et al. (2000), participants observed a left stimulus hand mirroring their right response

hand (Figure 1a). As a result, index finger stimulus movements were not only compatible

with index finger responses in terms of imitation (i.e., see index finger, move index finger)
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 14

but also in terms of location (i.e., see left finger, move left finger). A fundamental question is

therefore whether automatic imitation (i.e., imitative compatibility) can be explained as a

mere byproduct of spatial processing (i.e., spatial compatibility).

Three methods have been developed to investigate the extent to which automatic

imitation depends on spatial compatibility. The first method is to position the stimulus hand

orthogonal to the response hand (e.g., J. L. Cook & Bird, 2011, 2012; Heyes et al., 2005;

Press, Bird, Flach, & Heyes, 2005). For example, in the Brass et al. (2000) study, spatial

processes can be neutralized by rotating the stimulus hand 90° counterclockwise so that its

fingers point to the right instead of downwards (Figure 1b). In this case, an observed index

finger movement is still compatible with an index finger response in terms of imitation (i.e.,

see index finger, move index finger), but no longer in terms of location (i.e., see lower

finger, move left finger). However, a potential issue with this method is that there is a

documented tendency to associate “down” with “left” and “up” with “right” (Weeks &

Proctor, 1990). As a result, this method confounds automatic imitation with orthogonal

spatial compatibility (Heyes, 2011).

The second method offers a solution to orthogonal spatial confounds by separately

manipulating imitative and spatial compatibility (e.g., Bertenthal et al., 2006; Boyer et al.,

2012; Catmur & Heyes, 2011). For instance, using the stimuli of Brass et al. (2000), this can

be achieved by presenting a left stimulus hand in one half of the trials and a right stimulus

hand in the other half of the trials. This setup results in a positive relation between imitative

and spatial compatibility in left hand trials and a negative relation in right hand trials, which

makes it possible to calculate a main effect of imitative compatibility that is independent of

spatial compatibility (Figure 1c). Finally, the third method addresses the spatial compatibility

confound by using more complex stimuli such as symbolic gestures that cannot easily be
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 15

categorized on a simple spatial dimension (Bortoletto, Mattingley, & Cunnington, 2013;

Cracco, Genschow, Radkova, & Brass, 2018; Liepelt, Prinz, & Brass, 2010). Since all three

methods have been proven to elicit automatic imitation, it is now widely accepted that

imitative compatibility can be influenced but not be reduced to spatial compatibility (Heyes,

2011).

However, it is still an open question how much of automatic imitation can be

explained by spatial processes. To address this question, we will compare RT tasks that

confound imitative and spatial compatibility with RT tasks that control for simple (e.g., left-

right) spatial compatibility by using one of the three methods discussed above. In addition,

we will explore whether orthogonal spatial compatibility has an influence on automatic

imitation when orthogonal stimulus-response compositions are used. While preliminary

research suggests that orthogonal spatial compatibility poses no threat to automatic

imitation (Jimenez et al., 2012; Press et al., 2008), the small sample size of these studies (N =

17 and 18) has made it difficult to draw strong conclusions. In the current meta-analysis, we

will perform a stringent test of the orthogonal spatial compatibility hypothesis by comparing

studies that controlled for simple but not orthogonal spatial compatibility with studies that

controlled for both types of spatial compatibility.

Effector Versus Movement Compatibility. If automatic imitation is not a spatial

effect, then what is it? According to Heyes (2011), it is a combination of effector and

movement compatibility. The term effector in this context is used as a synonym of body

part. In other words, effector compatibility refers to the overlap between the body part

moved by the model and the body part that has to be moved by the participant (e.g., index

or middle finger). In contrast, movement compatibility refers to the overlap between the

type of movement made by the model and the type of movement that has to be made by
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 16

the participant (e.g., lifting or tapping). Studies on effector compatibility manipulate effector

overlap while keeping the movement constant (e.g., lift index finger or middle finger),

whereas studies on movement compatibility manipulate movement overlap while keeping

the effector constant (e.g., lift or tap index finger). Research has found reliable imitation in

both effector (e.g., Biermann-Ruben et al., 2008; Brass et al., 2000; Catmur & Heyes, 2011;

Obhi, Hogeveen, Giacomin, & Jordan, 2014) and movement compatibility tasks (e.g., Brass,

Bekkering, et al., 2001; Heyes et al., 2005; Obhi & Hogeveen, 2010; Stürmer et al., 2000).

Moreover, research has demonstrated that effector compatibility can be found in the

absence of a stimulus movement by highlighting the relevant effector (J. L. Cook & Bird,

2011, 2012), and two studies have now shown that movement and effector compatibility

are at least partially independent (Leighton & Heyes, 2010; Press, Gherri, Heyes, & Eimer,

2010).

However, it is currently unknown which type of compatibility contributes most to

automatic imitation. An important distinction between effector and movement

compatibility is that effector incompatible trials activate two movements that can be

executed at the same time (e.g., lift index and middle finger), whereas movement

incompatible trials activate two movements that cannot be executed at the same time (e.g.,

lift and tap index finger). If the mutually exclusive nature of the movements activated on

movement incompatible trials causes more response conflict, then it can be predicted that

movement compatibility should be stronger than effector compatibility. Nevertheless, the

opposite can also be predicted if the two movements activated on effector incompatible

trials are combined into a composite movement (e.g., lift both index and middle finger). In

this case, effector incompatible trials would require participants to inhibit not only the

stimulus movement (e.g., lift middle finger) but also the composite movement (e.g., lift both
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 17

index and middle finger) in order to select the correct response (e.g., lift index finger). To

distinguish between these two hypotheses, we will compare RT automatic imitation in

effector compatibility tasks with RT automatic imitation in movement compatibility tasks.

Is Automatic Imitation Flexible?

In addition to the processes that make up automatic imitation, research has also

started to investigate the extent to which these processes are susceptible to additional

learning (R. Cook et al., 2014; Heyes, 2010). That is, both ML theories assume that automatic

imitation develops through associative learning (Brass & Heyes, 2005). As a result, these

theories predict that the acquisition of incompatible sensorimotor associations can overrule

preexisting compatible associations (R. Cook et al., 2014; Heyes, 2010). In support, research

has shown that automatic imitation was reduced after short periods of sensorimotor

training where participants had to counterimitate the actions they observed (e.g., R. Cook,

Press, Dickinson, & Heyes, 2010; Gillmeister et al., 2008; Heyes et al., 2005; Wiggett,

Hudson, Tipper, & Downing, 2011). Furthermore, automatic imitation has been found to

reverse when it was embedded in an action context that required participants to

counterimitate the stimulus movements (e.g., Ocampo, Kritikos, & Cunnington, 2011; van

Schie, van Waterschoot, & Bekkering, 2008). However, not all studies have supported the

flexible nature of automatic imitation (Poljac, van Schie, & Bekkering, 2009). Therefore, we

will investigate whether automatic imitation in RT tasks is sensitive to (in)compatible

training and (in)compatible action contexts. In particular, we expect a reduction of the

imitation effect after incompatible associations are acquired and either an enhancement or

no changes after compatible associations are acquired.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 18

When During Action Preparation Does Automatic Imitation Operate?

A third question that will be addressed is the locus of automatic imitation. Action

preparation is a hierarchical process that entails two consecutive stages (Wong, Haith, &

Krakauer, 2015). The first stage consists of determining a motor goal (i.e., “what”) and the

second stage of developing a motor plan that achieves the established goal (i.e., “how”).

From this perspective, action observation can interfere either with the motor goal (e.g., “lift

the right index finger…”), with the motor plan (e.g., “… by activating the relevant muscles”),

or with both. To distinguish between these two possibilities, we will compare automatic

imitation in forced choice and simple response RT tasks. While observed movements in the

former task can interfere both with the motor goal and with the motor plan, observed

movements in the latter task can only interfere with the motor plan because the motor goal

is known before the stimulus movement is observed. Therefore, if automatic imitation is

restricted to forced choice RT tasks, then it influences the motor goal but not the motor

plan. In contrast, if automatic imitation is equally strong in both tasks, then it influences the

motor plan but not the motor goal. Finally, if automatic imitation occurs in both tasks but

more strongly in forced choice RT tasks, then it influences the motor goal as well as the

motor plan.

Is Automatic Imitation Really Automatic?

A fourth important question concerns the degree to which automatic imitation is

automatic. In the current meta-analysis, we will adopt a componential view on automaticity.

This is consistent with recent insights that automaticity is not a binary concept but rather a
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 19

multifaceted concept that encompasses a range of related features such as intentionality,

consciousness, speed, and attention (Moors & De Houwer, 2006). The word automatic in

automatic imitation therefore implies that it is a fast process that requires no intention, no

awareness, and no attention (Moors & De Houwer, 2006). Although it is well-established

that automatic imitation is unintentional (Heyes, 2011), less is known about its speed

(Catmur & Heyes, 2011; Cracco et al., 2018; Wiggett, Downing, & Tipper, 2013) and the role

of awareness (Mele, Mattiassi, & Urgesi, 2014) or attention (Bach, Peatfield, & Tipper, 2007;

Catmur, 2016; Chong, Cunnington, Williams, & Mattingley, 2009; Cracco & Brass, 2017). To

obtain a more informed view on the degree to which automatic imitation can be considered

automatic, we will focus on the features speed and attention.

Speed. A process can be considered automatic if it is a fast process (Moors & De

Houwer, 2006). A useful method to measure the speed of a process is to look at the

influence of stimulus-onset asynchronies (SOAs). Applied to automatic imitation, this can be

defined as the delay between the presentation of the stimulus movement and the

presentation of the imperative cue. Following this approach, research has discovered that

automatic imitation is a transient process that first increases with SOA but then decreases

again when the delay exceeds 80 to 150 ms (Catmur & Heyes, 2011; Cracco et al., 2018;

Wiggett et al., 2013). However, it is currently unknown how long after action observation

that automatic imitation can influence responses. Therefore, to further evaluate the extent

to which automatic imitation is a fast process, we will explore in detail how automatic

imitation develops as SOA increases.

Attention. A process is automatic in terms of attention if it requires little or no

attentional processes (Moors & De Houwer, 2006). Although initial research argued that

attention to the relevant features (Chong et al., 2009) of the relevant effector (Bach et al.,
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 20

2007) is a necessary condition of automatic imitation, a more recent study found that

directing attention away from the stimulus movements decreased but did not eliminate the

imitation effect (Cracco & Brass, 2017). Similarly, using a different technique, another study

showed that increasing the perceptual load did not make automatic imitation disappear but

instead made it reverse (Catmur, 2016). Since response speed was also slower at high

perceptual load, this reversal was interpreted as evidence that the processing of task-

irrelevant stimuli was inhibited after initially being facilitated (Eimer & Schlaghecken, 2003;

Jonas et al., 2007). In other words, it was argued that stimulus movements were still

processed when attentional load was high but that this led to a negative imitation effect

attributabel to inhibitory processes (Catmur, 2016). To further examine the role of attention

in automatic imitation, we will investigate whether the imitation effect in RT tasks depends

on the position of the imperative cue relative to the stimulus movement. That is, we expect

that automatic imitation will be stronger when the cue is positioned close to the stimulus

movement. In addition, we will also examine whether automatic imitation is stronger when

the cue is visual compared with auditory. This is because auditory cues do not force

participants to focus their attention on the screen while visual cues do.

What Is the Role of Action Goals in Automatic Imitation?

Another debate is whether automatic imitation is driven by action goals or by

movement parameters (Bouquet, Shipley, Capa, & Marshall, 2011; Chiavarino, Bugiani,

Grandi, & Colle, 2013; Liepelt et al., 2010; Liepelt, Von Cramon, & Brass, 2008; Press et al.,

2008). According to an influential view, imitation is a goal-directed process that involves

decomposing observed actions into a hierarchy of features where goals are more important
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 21

than how those goals are achieved (Wohlschläger, Gattis, & Bekkering, 2003). In line with

this view, research has shown that children (Bekkering, Wohlschläger, & Gattis, 2000) as

well as adults (Avikainen, Wohlschlager, Liuhanen, Hanninen, & Hari, 2003) prioritize the

ends over the means when asked to imitate (see also Gattis, Bekkering, & Wohlschläger,

2002; Gleissner, Meltzoff, & Bekkering, 2000; Want & Gattis, 2005). Furthermore, the notion

of goal-directed imitation is consistent with the observation that mirror neurons

preferentially respond to actions that are directed towards an object (Caspers, Zilles, Laird,

& Eickhoff, 2010; Cattaneo, Caruana, Jezzini, & Rizzolatti, 2009; Rizzolatti & Sinigaglia, 2010).

Applied to automatic imitation, goal-directed theories predict that imitative responses

should be stronger for goal-directed actions than for goalless actions (Bouquet et al., 2011;

Chiavarino et al., 2013). However, evidence for this theory is currently mixed with one study

supporting this pattern (Bouquet et al., 2011) and two other studies finding either the

opposite pattern (Chiavarino et al., 2013) or no difference (Liepelt et al., 2010).

Interestingly, the latter two studies suggest that movement parameters rather than

action goals might be what drives imitation (Chiavarino et al., 2013; Genschow, Florack, &

Waenke, 2013). In line with this view, it has been shown that the reason why goals are

imitated over movements is that goals tend to be more salient (Bird, Brindley, Leighton, &

Heyes, 2007; Leighton, Bird, & Heyes, 2010). Furthermore, strong evidence that movements

can guide imitation comes from research on cross-contextual imitation, which has

demonstrated that watching someone lift a barbell can trigger an action with similar

movement parameters but a different action goal such as lifting a cup to drink (Genschow &

Florack, 2014; Genschow et al., 2013; Genschow & Schindler, 2016). This is further

supported by the fact that automatic imitation occurs even when the stimulus movements

have no clear goal (e.g., Heyes et al., 2005; Press et al., 2008; Stürmer et al., 2000).
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 22

In sum, research on automatic imitation as well as related research has supported

both the view that imitation is driven by goals and the view that it is driven by movements.

Therefore, to further investigate the hypothesis that automatic imitation is a goal-directed

process, we will compare RT studies that used goalless stimulus movements such as finger

lifting (e.g., Brass et al., 2000; Stürmer et al., 2000) with RT studies that used object-directed

stimulus movements such as grasping (Chong et al., 2009; van Schie et al., 2008) or

communicative stimulus movements such as symbolic gestures (Bortoletto et al., 2013;

Cracco et al., 2018; Liepelt et al., 2010) and facial expressions (Otte, Jost, Habel, & Koch,

2011; Press, Richardson, & Bird, 2010). If goals have a stronger influence than movements,

then automatic imitation should be weaker when the stimulus movement is goalless

compared with when it is objected-directed or communicative (Bouquet et al., 2011;

Chiavarino et al., 2013). However, given that action goals are often more salient than

movement parameters (Bird, Brindley, et al., 2007; Leighton, Bird, & Heyes, 2010), an

alternative hypothesis is also that goal-related actions may drive away attention from the

observed movement towards the action goal. In this case, automatic imitation should be

weaker for goal-directed actions, and this should be especially true for object-directed

actions where attention can be drawn towards the object (Chiavarino et al., 2013).

Is Automatic Imitation Sensitive to the Similarity Between Actor and Imitator?

Apart from research on the mechanisms and conditions of automatic imitation, there

is also an extensive literature on whether automatic imitation is sensitive to the similarity

between the actor and the imitator (Chaminade & Cheng, 2009; Gowen & Poliakoff, 2012;

Press, 2011). In particular, this research builds on the assumption that automatic imitation is
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 23

a social process that should be moderated by the degree to which the imitator resembles

the actor in terms of perceived or actual appearance. At a theoretical level, both ML

theories consider similarity to be an important modulator of imitative behavior. For

example, according to ASL, imitative tendencies are learned responses that develop as a

result of self-observation and interaction with other, often similar (Efferson, Lalive, & Fehr,

2008), individuals (Brass & Heyes, 2005; Cook et al., 2014; Heyes, 2010; Ray & Heyes, 2011).

Although perception-action links are flexible in the sense that they allow generalization to

comparable stimuli, the strength of the imitative response is thought to depend on the

number of features that the observed movement shares with the stored action

representation (Shanks & Darby, 1998). Similarly, IM postulates that stimuli differ in terms

of ideomotor compatibility – the extent to which they resemble the sensory outcomes of an

action (Brass, Bekkering, et al., 2001; Greenwald, 1970; Shin et al., 2010). As a result, IM

assumes that stimuli with higher ideomotor compatibility can access the corresponding

ideomotor representation more directly and therefore that automatic imitation is stronger

when the model is similar to the participant.

Bottom-Up Animacy. If similarity modulates automatic imitation, then human agents

should elicit a stronger imitative response than nonhuman agents. In support of this

hypothesis, research has found stronger automatic imitation for human agents compared

with nonhuman agents in both RT (e.g., Bird et al., 2007; Brass, Bekkering, et al., 2001;

Gowen, Bolton, & Poliakoff, 2016; Press et al., 2005; Press, Gillmeister, & Heyes, 2006, 2007)

and KM (e.g., Chaminade, Franklin, Oztop, & Cheng, 2005; J. L. Cook, Swapp, Pan, Bianchi-

Berthouze, & Blakemore, 2014; Gowen et al., 2008; Kilner et al., 2003) tasks. However, not

all studies have supported a clear-cut human bias in automatic imitation (Cracco, De Coster,

Andres, & Brass, 2015; Jansson et al., 2007; Kilner et al., 2007; Klapper, Ramsey, Wigboldus,
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 24

& Cross, 2014; Kupferberg et al., 2012; Liepelt et al., 2010; Oztop, Franklin, & Chaminade,

2005; Press et al., 2006). A closer look at these studies shows that they mostly used

nonhuman agents with high (e.g., a wooden hand) as opposed to low (e.g., a moving dot)

human resemblance. To investigate whether automatic imitation is reduced for nonhuman

agents, we will compare RT and KM automatic imitation of human models with RT and KM

automatic imitation of nonhuman models such as robots (e.g., Press et al., 2006) and

geometrical models such as moving dots (e.g., Gowen et al., 2016). If automatic imitation is

sensitive to similarity, then imitative responses should become stronger as the stimuli

become more humanlike.

Top-Down Animacy. In addition to bottom-up animacy, beliefs about animacy may

also influence automatic imitation. For example, Liepelt and Brass (2010b) measured

automatic imitation of a gloved hand after showing participants that the hand was human

or wooden. The results revealed a stronger imitation effect in the human condition,

indicating that beliefs about animacy can influence automatic imitation. The influence of

animacy beliefs on automatic imitation has since been supported in some (Gowen et al.,

2016; Klapper et al., 2014; Longo & Bertenthal, 2009; Stanley et al., 2007) but not all studies

(Press et al., 2006). Therefore, we will investigate whether RT automatic imitation is weaker

for human and ambiguous agents when participants are told that the agent is nonhuman

and whether it is stronger for nonhuman and ambiguous agents when participants are told

that the agent is human.

Motion Profile. Apart from model characteristics, the degree to which the stimulus

movement follows a human motion profile might also play a role. For example, in a study on

KM automatic imitation, Kilner et al. (2007) found that participants did not imitate a human

model when its movements followed a non-biological rather than a biological motion
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 25

profile. Although some studies have obtained similar results (Chaminade et al., 2005;

Stanley, Gowen, & Miall, 2007), other studies found no difference between biological and

non-biological motion profiles (Bouquet et al., 2007; J. L. Cook et al., 2014; Gowen et al.,

2008). Therefore, we will compare KM automatic imitation of stimulus movements that

followed a biological motion profile with KM automatic imitation of stimulus movements

that did not follow a biological motion profile.

Although movement characteristics have often been studied in KM studies, this

factor has remained relatively unexplored in RT studies. To address this issue, we will look at

the presentation mode of the stimulus movements. In particular, we will compare stimulus

movements presented as short videos (e.g., Brass et al., 2000; Gowen et al., 2016; Heyes et

al., 2005) with stimulus movements presented as a two-frame sequence in which a picture

of the stimulus in neutral position is replaced with a picture of the stimulus in final position

(e.g., Catmur & Heyes, 2011; Cracco et al., 2015; Press et al., 2007) and stimulus movements

presented as a single picture of the stimulus in final position (e.g., Chong et al., 2009; Liepelt

et al., 2010; Otte, Jost, et al., 2011). Given that videos provide a more realistic

representation of human motion than a sequence of two pictures or a single picture, it can

be predicted that RT automatic imitation will be stronger when videos are used instead of

pictures.

Perspective. A more subtle aspect of similarity is the perspective from which the

stimulus movement is seen. That is, at least for non-facial actions, ML theories postulate

that self-observation is the main mechanism behind the formation of perception-action links

(Brass & Heyes, 2005). As a result, it can be predicted from these theories that automatic

imitation should be stronger when a movement is seen from a first-person perspective than

when it is seen from a third-person perspective. In line with this idea, research has found
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 26

stronger automatic imitation for actions presented in a first-person perspective than for

actions presented in a third-person perspective (Bortoletto et al., 2013; Genschow et al.,

2013; Vogt, Taylor, & Hopkins, 2003). However, some studies have also suggested that the

presence of a first-person bias might depend on contextual variables such as “cognitive

style” (Bortoletto et al., 2013). To evaluate the role of visual perspective, we will examine

whether automatic imitation in RT tasks is stronger when stimulus movements are

presented in a first-person perspective than when they are presented in a third-person

perspective.

Gender Overlap. Finally, we will look at two similarity features that have not yet

been explored in the literature. The first feature is gender overlap. Given that two persons

tend to be more similar when they are of the same gender, it can be predicted that

automatic imitation should be stronger when there is a match between the gender of the

model and the gender of the participant. In support of this idea, a recent study reported

stronger imitative responses for female than for male participants when a female stimulus

hand was used (Butler et al., 2015). However, this study did not manipulate the gender of

the stimulus hand. As a result, it is unclear whether this effect was a gender effect or a

gender overlap effect. To address this question, we will investigate whether automatic

imitation in RT tasks is stronger when the gender of the model corresponds to the gender of

the majority of the sample.

Response Overlap. The second feature of similarity that has remained unexplored is

response overlap, which refers to how similar the stimulus movement is to the response

movement. For example, studies with finger lifting/tapping stimuli have not always used

finger lifting/tapping responses (e.g., Brass, Bekkering, et al., 2001; Brass et al., 2000;

Liepelt, Stenzel, & Lappe, 2012), but have sometimes also used key release/press responses
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 27

(e.g., Butler et al., 2015; Longo & Bertenthal, 2009; Longo, Kosobud, & Bertenthal, 2008).

Although a key press looks similar to a finger tap, these two actions feel different, sound

different, and have a different outcome. Since ML theories assume that perception-action

associations are not only constructed from the visual but also from the sensory and auditory

outcomes of an action (Hommel et al., 2001; Shin et al., 2010), it can be predicted, to put it

in the words of IM, that a key press is represented with a different ideomotor

representation than a finger tap. To test this hypothesis, we will investigate whether

automatic imitation of finger lifting/tapping stimuli is stronger when the response is also

finger lifting/tapping than when the response is releasing/pressing a key.

Is Automatic Imitation Related to Social Abilities?

The finding that automatic imitation is sensitive to the similarity between the actor

and imitator has been interpreted as evidence that imitation is an inherently social process

(Wang & Hamilton, 2012). In extension, a prominent view is that automatic imitation

(Gallese, 2007; Wang & Hamilton, 2012) or how it is controlled (Brass, Ruby, & Spengler,

2009; Sowden & Shah, 2014) can provide insight into people’s social abilities. For example, it

has been proposed that automatic imitation might be related to social functioning because

it relies on embodied simulation, which is assumed to be at the basis of important social

skills such as mentalizing and empathy (Gallese, 2007). In the case of empathy, for example,

it is argued that simulation is necessary to feel the emotional state of someone in distress

(Carr, Iacoboni, Dubeau, Mazziotta, & Lenzi, 2003). In contrast, another view argues that it is

not self-other overlap but self-other control that is important for social cognition (Brass et

al., 2009; Sowden & Shah, 2014). For instance, empathy does not only require individuals to
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 28

share others’ emotional state but also to control these emotions in order to provide a

complementary empathic response (Bird & Viding, 2014; Decety & Lamm, 2006). From this

perspective, automatic imitation is an indicator of social abilities because participants have

to distinguish between self and other representations on incongruent trials to overcome the

urge to imitate (Brass et al., 2009; Sowden & Shah, 2014). Although these two theories

differ in how automatic imitation is related to social cognition, they agree that impairments

in automatic imitation should be accompanied by social difficulties, which should be visible

as impaired empathic abilities (Carr et al., 2003; Gallese, 2007; Sowden & Shah, 2014) and

might lead to autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (Spengler, Bird, et al., 2010; Williams, 2008;

Williams et al., 2001).

Empathy. The construct of empathy can be divided into cognitive and emotional

empathy, with the former referring to perspective taking and the latter to emotional

concern (Blair, 2005; Davis, 1983). However, in contrast to the two theories outlined earlier,

research on the relation between automatic imitation and empathy has yielded mixed

results. That is, one study found a relation with empathy only when the model was

attractive (Müller, Van Leeuwen, Van Baaren, Bekkering, & Dijksterhuis, 2013), a second

study only with the social modulation of automatic imitation (Haffey, Press, O’Connell, &

Chakrabarti, 2013), and two final studies found no relation (Butler et al., 2015; Genschow et

al., 2017). In the current meta-analysis, we will investigate the relation between automatic

imitation and both cognitive and emotional empathy across studies. Since multiple

instruments have been used in the literature, we will combine data from the Interpersonal

Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis, 1983; De Corte et al., 2007), the Empathy Assessment Index (EAI;

Gerdes, Lietz, & Segal, 2011; Inzunza, 2014), and the Empathy Quotient (EQ; Baron-cohen &

Wheelwright, 2004), for which validation studies have found high inter-correlations (De
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 29

Corte et al., 2007; Gerdes et al., 2011; Inzunza, 2014; Lawrence, Shaw, Baker, Baron-Cohen,

& David, 2004).

Autism Spectrum Disorder. If automatic imitation is a mirror into the social mind,

then it should be impaired in social disorders such as ASD (Spengler, Bird, et al., 2010;

Williams, 2008; Williams et al., 2001). This view is instantiated by two important theories on

ASD, each corresponding to one of the two social cognition accounts outlined earlier.

According to one theory, ASD is the result of a ‘broken mirror neuron system’ and this is

thought to cause hypo-imitation (Williams, 2008; Williams et al., 2001). In particular, it is

assumed that impairments in the perception-action system lead to difficulties in simulating

the mental states of others, which in turn is at the root of social problems in ASD. However,

it is important to note that hypo-imitation can also point to a different mechanism, namely

that participants with ASD do not attend to social stimuli or are less motivated to process

them (Blake, Turner, Smoski, Pozdol, & Stone, 2003; Chevallier, Kohls, Troiani, Brodkin, &

Schultz, 2012; Klin, Lin, Gorrindo, Ramsay, & Jones, 2009). Moreover, there is currently only

one study showing reduced automatic imitation in ASD (J. L. Cook et al., 2014). Therefore, a

second theory has argued that it is not the mirror system but the control over this system

that is impaired in ASD (Spengler, Bird, et al., 2010). More precisely, this theory argues that

individuals with ASD have difficulties to distinguish self from other representations. As a

result, they are slower to inhibit automatic imitation when the observed movement does

not match the instructed response and for that reason are expected to display hyper- rather

than hypo-imitation (Spengler, Bird, et al., 2010).

In support of the self-other control account, two studies have now reported hyper-

imitation in ASD (Deschrijver, Wiersema, & Brass, 2017; Spengler, Bird, et al., 2010), two

additional studies reported context-bound hyper-imitation (Bird, Leighton, et al., 2007;


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 30

Schunke et al., 2015), and one final study reported a positive relation between autism

symptom severity and automatic imitation (Sowden, Koehne, Catmur, & Dziobek, 2015).

Nevertheless, a substantial research body also failed to find evidence for hyper-imitation in

both children (Grecucci et al., 2013) and adults (Forbes, Wang, & Hamilton, 2016; Gowen et

al., 2008; Press, Richardson, et al., 2010; Sowden et al., 2015) with ASD. Taken together,

studies on ASD have provided mixed results. An important reason is presumably that

research on this topic has mostly used small samples (for an exception: Sowden et al.,

2015). As a stringent test of the hypo- and hyperimitation hypotheses of ASD, we will

therefore compare RT and KM automatic imitation between ASD and neurotypical samples.

Secondary Moderators

In addition to the moderators discussed above, we will also consider the influence of

participant age, number of trials, and response effector as moderators of secondary

interest.

Age. With regard to participant age, we will explore whether the mean age of the

sample is related to the strength of automatic imitation in RT tasks.

Trials. To investigate whether automatic imitation in RT tasks is influenced by the

length of the experiment, we will look at the total number of trials in the blocks on which

the effect size was based.

Effector. The effect of response effector will be investigated to explore whether

automatic imitation differs for finger movements, hand movements, foot movements, and

facial movements. To this end, we will examine if the imitation effect in RT tasks depends

on the effector that is involved.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 31

Method

Literature Search

The literature search was initiated in February 2015 and terminated in January 2016.

We searched for studies published between January 1 2000 and January 1 2016 based on

the publication date of two important papers (Brass et al., 2000; Stürmer et al., 2000). To

collect published studies, we entered the following search terms in Web of Science,

PubMed, and Google Scholar: “automatic imitation” OR “imitation inhibition” OR "imitative

compatibility" OR "imitative congruency" OR "movement compatibility" OR "movement

congruency" OR "action compatibility" OR "action congruency" OR "motor compatibility" OR

"motor congruency" OR "movement interference" OR "action interference" OR "motor

interference" OR "motor facilitation" OR "movement facilitation" OR "action facilitation" OR

"motor mimicry" OR "action mimicry" OR "movement mimicry" OR "motor priming" OR

“action priming" OR "movement priming" OR "visuomotor priming" OR "body part priming"

OR "effector priming". Most of these search terms were identified by Heyes (2011). The

Web of Science and PubMed search was performed on titles, abstracts, and keywords. A

similar strategy was initially used for Google Scholar, but was later restricted to a title search

to limit the number of initial results (i.e., 2870).

In addition to the Boolean search, we also looked for studies that cited at least one

out of four influential papers on automatic imitation (Brass, Bekkering, et al., 2001; Brass et

al., 2000; Heyes, 2011; Stürmer et al., 2000). Furthermore, to include doctoral dissertations

in the database we added AND “dissertation” to the Google Scholar title, abstract, and

keyword search. Finally, unpublished data was collected by sending requests to prominent
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 32

authors, the European Social and Affective Neuroscience mailing list, and the Belgian

Association for Psychological Science mailing list. The above search strategy resulted in 1374

unduplicated results consisting of 1326 published articles or books, 10 doctoral

dissertations, and 38 unpublished datasets.

Screening Process

The studies obtained from the literature search were divided among the different

authors and were evaluated on the basis of the criteria described below. At the end of this

process, the first author checked the screening of all authors for errors. We first screened

the abstracts to identify studies that were clearly not eligible. The remaining studies were

then evaluated in more detail, resulting in a database of 161 studies. Together, these

studies contained 251 experiments, of which 25 did not meet the criteria for inclusion.

Importantly, we only included experimental conditions that satisfied all inclusion criteria.

The criteria used to screen the 1374 search results will now be described in detail. The

number of excluded studies is denoted with n and the number of excluded experiments

within otherwise included studies is denoted with m (Figure 3).

1. The study should report original empirical data (n = 238).

2. The study should investigate RT, ER, or KM automatic imitation (n = 939, m = 19). A

task was considered to assess automatic imitation if it measured how task-related

motor responses were influenced by seeing compatible and incompatible

movements (e.g., Brass, Bekkering, et al., 2001; Brass et al., 2000; Kilner et al., 2003).

The dependent variable of these studies should be RTs, ERs, or KMs. However,

studies on KM automatic imitation were only considered if they measured


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 33

movement trajectories as in Kilner et al. (2003) to ensure that the effect sizes were

comparable (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). This means that we did not include studies that

measured alternative KM properties such as force (e.g., Salama, Turner, & Edwards,

2011) or movement duration (e.g., Pfister, Dignath, Hommel, & Kunde, 2013).

Furthermore, studies that measured automatic imitation of tool use were considered

beyond the scope of the meta-analysis and were therefore not included (e.g.,

Massen, 2009). Finally, studies on social attention were not included even if they

reported automatic imitation of movements such as eye gaze or pointing because

imitation in these paradigms cannot be distinguished from attentional processes

(e.g., Crostella, Carducci, & Aglioti, 2009).

3. The compatibility between the observed movement and the instructed response

should be manipulated within-subjects (n = 2). This criterion was included to ensure

that the effect sizes could be compared (Dunlap, Cortina, Vaslow, & Burke, 1996;

Lakens, 2013).

4. Studies should report a measure of imitative compatibility that is independent of

other types of compatibility with the exception of spatial compatibility (n = 10, m =

1). We did not exclude studies in which imitative compatibility was confounded with

spatial compatibility so we could investigate the influence of spatial processes on

automatic imitation. However, we did exclude studies in which imitative

compatibility and simple spatial compatibility were always negatively correlated

because it is impossible to extract a clean imitation effect from such studies (e.g.,

Müller et al., 2013).

5. Each response should occur equally often in the compatible condition as in the

incompatible condition (n = 1, m = 1). Studies that did not meet this criterion were
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 34

excluded to ensure that automatic imitation was not confounded with response

execution.

6. Studies reporting a reversed imitation effect were not included when this could be

explained as a complementary response (n = 1). This resulted in the exclusion of one

unpublished study in which it was found that participants were faster to take a

dominant pose when seeing a submissive pose and vice versa (Müller, de Graag, &

van Baaren, 2016). Moreover, studies that predicted a reversed automatic imitation

effect due to inhibitory processes were excluded as well (n = 2). For example, one

study was excluded because it predicted reversed automatic imitation due to social

inhibition of return (Jonas et al., 2007).

7. Stimuli should be presented for more than 100 ms to ensure that they were

processed consciously (n = 1).

8. The dependent measure should be recorded reliably (n = 3). For example, one study

was excluded because RT was measured by counting the number of video frames

with 40 ms duration each between the presentation of the stimulus movement and

the movement onset (Wild, Erb, Eyb, Bartels, & Grodd, 2003).

9. The sample should consist of human participants (n = 8). Moreover, a study was only

considered when the sample consisted of participants that (a) had no actual or

virtual brain damage, (b) had not been diagnosed with a psycho- or

neuropathological disorder except for ASD, and (c) had not been administered a

psychopharmaceutic substance (n = 1). An exception was made for ASD to test the

hypo- and hyper-imitation hypotheses of ASD. If an experiment contained an

ineligible clinical group together with a control group, then only the control group
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 35

was included in the analysis. Sham brain stimulation or brain stimulation at control

locations was not considered virtual brain damage.

10. The same dataset should not be included more than once in the meta-analysis (n = 2,

m = 2). If multiple studies used the same dataset, then only the first study was

included.

11. Sufficient statistical information should be available to calculate the necessary effect

sizes (n = 5, m = 2). The required information should be either reported in the paper

or obtained from the authors.

--- Insert Figure 3 about here ---

Coding and Reliability

The eligible studies were coded by the first author according to a coding manual that

was approved by all authors. The coded variables are described in Appendix A. Afterwards,

we added one additional variable that indicated whether studies that controlled for simple

spatial compatibility also controlled for orthogonal spatial compatibility. With regard to

perspective, a stimulus movement was coded as third-person when it was biomechanically

impossible for the observer to position the relevant body part in the same way as the

observed body part. Furthermore, because studies rarely report their results separately for

male and female participants, participant gender was coded as ‘female’ when > 50% of the

participants in the sample were female (MRT = 69%, MER = 69%) and as ‘male’ when ≤ 50% of

the participants were female (MRT = 39%, MER = 42%). Finally, we used the variable response

device to determine response overlap. Regular meetings were organized among eight
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 36

authors to discuss problems that were encountered during coding. For example, one

notable problem was that model gender was difficult to code objectively. Therefore, this

variable was coded according to a consensus method in which each of the eight authors

coded this variable individually by indicating whether they considered the model to be male,

female, or ambiguous. If ≥ 75% of the coders agreed on the gender, it was coded as such.

Otherwise, the gender was coded as ambiguous.

To evaluate the reliability of the coding, the ninth co-author was trained to use the

coding manual and subsequently recoded 28 randomly selected studies containing 47

experiments in total. Afterwards, to further identify errors, the same co-author also checked

the coding of the first author. Disagreements and inconsistencies that arose during coding

were resolved through discussion in group. Coding reliability was assessed with intraclass

correlation coefficients (ICC) for continuous variables and with kappa coefficients for

categorical variables. The average reliability for continuous variables (ICC = .98) was high

and varied between .94 (Trials) and 1.00 (Age and SOA). Similarly, high reliability was

observed for categorical variables (κ = .91) and varied between .70 (Action Goals) and 1.00

(Flexibility, Paradigm Type, Cue Modality, Top-Down Animacy, ASD).

Meta-Analytic Procedures

All analyses were performed in R using the metafor (Viechtbauer, 2010) and

robumeta (Fisher & Tipton, 2015) packages.

Mixed Effects Model. The data was analyzed with a mixed effects model:
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 37

𝑦𝑖 = 𝛽0 + 𝛽1 𝑥𝑖1 + … + 𝛽𝑝 𝑥𝑖𝑝 + 𝑢𝑖 + 𝑒𝑖 (1)

where yi is the effect size of experiment i, β0 is the weighted average population effect, β1 …

βp are the regression coefficients for moderators xi1 … xip, ui ~ N(0, τ2) is the error between

experiments, and ei ~ N(0, vi) is the error within experiments. More precisely, mixed effects

models assume that variability among effect sizes can arise both within and between

experiments. First, variability within experiments vi is introduced through inaccurate

measurement of the underlying effect size. Second, variability between experiments or

heterogeneity τ2 is introduced through methodological or contextual differences between

experiments. Importantly, τ2 reflects the amount of heterogeneity that remains after

accounting for the moderator variables that are included in the model. Since mixed effects

models allow for heterogeneity among the effect sizes, these models can be used to make

inferences about the population rather than about the m experiments included in the meta-

analysis (Field & Gillett, 2010). However, a crucial assumption of mixed models is that the

effect sizes yi should be statistically independent (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). This assumption is

violated when multiple effect sizes from the same sample are included and such a violation

is known to inflate the false-positive rate (Tanner-Smith, Tipton, & Polanin, 2016). Since

some of the moderators have mostly been studied in repeated measures designs, we

decided to control for statistical dependency by using robust variance estimation (RVE)

(Fisher & Tipton, 2015; Hedges, Tipton, & Johnson, 2010; Tanner-Smith & Tipton, 2014;

Tanner-Smith et al., 2016).

Robust Variance Estimation. RVE accounts for dependency in the data by calculating

an empirical estimate of the standard error that does not assume independent effect sizes

(Fisher & Tipton, 2015; Hedges et al., 2010; Tanner-Smith & Tipton, 2014; Tanner-Smith et

al., 2016). Simulation studies have shown that RVE provides unbiased estimates of the true
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 38

standard error, does not assume a specific distribution, and is robust to misspecification of

the weights (Hedges et al., 2010). However, even though statistical inferences are valid for

any set of weights, inverse variance weights are preferred in terms of statistical efficiency

(Hedges et al., 2010). In particular, two types of weights have been proposed: hierarchical

effects weights and correlated effects weights. Hierarchical effects weights are most

appropriate when multiple experiments are nested within a larger cluster (e.g., research

group), while correlated effects weights are most appropriate when multiple effect sizes

from the same sample are included. Although both types of dependency typically co-occur,

the choice of weights only affects statistical efficiency. It has therefore been recommended

to pick a weighting method based on the main source of dependency (Tanner-Smith &

Tipton, 2014).

To optimally account for the inclusion of multiple effect sizes from the same sample,

the current meta-analysis used correlated effects weights (Fisher & Tipton, 2015):

1
𝑤𝑖 = (2)
𝑘𝑖 (𝑣∙𝑖 + 𝜏 2 )

where v∙i is the average variability within experiment i, τ2 is an estimate of the variability

between experiments, and ki is the number of effect sizes in experiment i. Note that the

total variability in each experiment v∙i + τ2 is divided equally among the ki effect sizes of

experiment i so that each effect size has the same weight. This strategy ensures that the

total weight assigned to experiment i does not depend on the number of effect sizes

provided by this experiment. Moreover, the calculation of τ2 depends on a parameter ρ that

represents the correlation among the effect sizes and is assumed to be the same for all

experiments (Fisher & Tipton, 2015). Although the value of ρ has to be set manually, varying

this parameter typically has little influence on the results. In the current study, the default
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 39

value ρ = 0.8 of the robumeta package was used (Fisher & Tipton, 2015). In addition,

sensitivity analyses will be reported to investigate whether the results depend on the value

of ρ.

Although RVE provides an unbiased estimate of the standard error, it is known to

underestimate the true variance when the number of experiments is small. To address this

issue, small sample corrections have been proposed (Tipton, 2015; Tipton & Pustejovsky,

2015). As these corrections also control for unbalanced or skewed moderators, it was

recently recommended that these corrections should always be implemented (Fisher &

Tipton, 2015; Tanner-Smith et al., 2016). In keeping with this recommendation, small

sample adjustments were applied to all tests in the current meta-analysis. Importantly, p

values from t tests with small sample correction are valid when df ≥ 4 but suffer from

inflated Type I error rates when df < 4 (Tanner-Smith et al., 2016). Therefore, we will only

report analyses where all t test contrasts have df ≥ 4. In addition, for continuous

moderators, we will remove outlier values exceeding the weighted mean value by > 3 SD

because including these values reduces the dfs and therefore reduces statistical power

(Tanner-Smith et al., 2016).

Effect Size Computation. Cohen’s d was used as a measure of effect size. In

particular, dz for the difference between incongruent (IC) and congruent (C) trials was

calculated with the following formula:

𝑀𝐼𝐶 − 𝑀𝐶 𝑀𝐼𝐶−𝐶
𝑑𝑧 = = . (3)
√𝑣𝑎𝑟𝐶 + 𝑣𝑎𝑟𝐼𝐶 − 2 × 𝑐𝑜𝑣𝐶,𝐼𝐶 𝑆𝐷𝐼𝐶−𝐶

This effect size controls for inter-individual differences by including the correlation between

C and IC in the denominator (Lakens, 2013). Since the value of dz depends on the strength of

the correlation, it has been argued that dz does not provide a good estimate of the true
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 40

effect size (Dunlap et al., 1996). However, this argument rests on the assumption that

between-subject designs are the default design in psychology (Lakens, 2013). Given that

automatic imitation is almost always assessed in a repeated measures design, this argument

does not hold for the current meta-analysis. Indeed, the natural unit of analysis in such

studies is the compatibility effect (IC - C). As this unit is calculated at the intra-individual

level, it is reasonable to rely on effect sizes that control for inter-individual differences

(Lakens, 2013). When dz could not be calculated on the basis of the raw data, it was

calculated on the basis of the relevant t or F test:

𝑡 𝐹
𝑑𝑧 = or √ . (4)
√𝑛 𝑛

Finally, because dz provides a slightly biased estimate of the population effect size, it was

corrected using the “escalc” function of the metafor package (Viechtbauer, 2010) to obtain

the unbiased effect size gz:

𝑛−1
𝛤( 2 )
𝑔𝑧 = 𝑑𝑧 × (5)
√(𝑛 − 1) × 𝛤 (𝑛 − 2)
2 2

With Γ(x) representing the gamma function and with the following variance:

1 𝑔𝑧2
𝑉𝑎𝑟𝑔𝑧 = + . (6)
𝑛 2𝑛

Seventy-three studies did not contain sufficient statistical information to calculate an

effect size in at least one experiment. To maximize the number of effect sizes that could be

included, we therefore sent data requests to the corresponding author of these studies

between November and December 2015. A subsequent reminder was sent to non-

responding authors between January and February 2016. We received all requested

statistics for 46 studies and part of the requested statistics for 1 study. For 22 studies, the
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 41

data was no longer available or could not be retrieved in time. Finally, we received no

response to our data requests for 4 studies.

For the 27 studies without sufficient statistical information, we obtained an effect

size estimate using the following procedure. First, we looked for studies where we could

extract MIC-C, SDC, and SDIC from the text (n = 6, m = 13, k = 139). To calculate an effect size

for these studies, we computed the correlation between C and IC for all experiments in the

meta-analysis where this was possible and then imputed the relevant RT (r = .94, n = 61, m =

95), ER (r = .47, n = 23, m = 36), or KM (r = .81, n = 11, m = 12) correlation coefficient in the

dz formula (Higgins, Deeks, & Altman, 2011). Second, we looked for studies where MIC-C and

either SDIC-C or both SDC and SDIC could be extracted from the provided figures using a

computerized measurement tool (n = 8, m = 12, k = 51). When SDC and SDIC were extracted,

we imputed the mean correlation coefficient in the dz formula following the procedure

described above (Higgins et al., 2011). Third, we looked for studies where we could extract

MIC-C but not SDC or SDIC from the text or figures (n = 6, m = 10, k = 27). To get an effect size

for these studies, we calculated SDIC-C for all experiments in the meta-analysis where this

was possible and then imputed the relevant RT (SDIC-C = 30.83 ms, n = 97, m = 138) or ER

(SDIC-C = 4.27%, n = 25, m = 38) SD in the dz formula (Higgins et al., 2011). Finally, we were

able to calculate an effect size for an additional 2 studies (m = 2) and 1 experiment when we

did not code one of the moderators. However, despite these efforts, we could not calculate

an effect size for 5 studies (m = 5) and 2 experiments of studies that were otherwise

included.

Outliers. RT, ER, and KM extreme values were identified by looking for effect sizes

that exceeded the weighted mean effect size by more than 3 SDs. Note that a weighted

mean was used to ensure that the influence of each experiment did not depend on the
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 42

number of provided effect sizes. To calculate the weighted mean, a weight of 1 was divided

equally among the different effect sizes belonging to the same experiment. The above

procedure identified 1 RT outlier (gz = 3.99), 2 ER outliers (gz = 2.19, 2.37), and no KM

outliers. These outliers were subsequently replaced by the 3 SD deviation value for RTs (gz =

3.41) and ERs (gz = 2.03) respectively.

Moderator Correlations. A potential issue in meta-analyses is confounded

moderators (Field & Gillett, 2010; Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). To identify such confounds, we

assessed the weighted association between the different RT and KM moderators with r for

continuous-continuous and continuous-dichotomous pairs, with multiple R for continuous-

polytomous pairs, and with Cramér’s V for dichotomous-dichotomous, dichotomous-

polytomous, and polytomous-polytomous pairs (Table 1). For all three statistics, a value of 0

indicates the absence of a relation and a value of 1 indicates a perfect relation. In the

current meta-analysis, an association ≥ .40 was considered substantial. According to Cohen

(1988), a value of .40 can be considered a medium-to-large association. When a substantial

relation between two moderators was identified, we examined the moderator effect while

controlling for the other moderator to rule out that the presence or absence of a moderator

effect was the result of confounding.

Publication Bias. To test for potential publication bias, we will plot each

1
experiment’s mean effect size against √ with a funnel plot. A negative association
𝑁

between the sample size and the effect size across studies is indicative of publication bias

because studies with small samples need large effect sizes to yield a significant result. We

used N rather than SE as a measure of precision because the latter depends on gz and can

therefore result in spurious funnel plot asymmetry, especially when the average effect size
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 43

is large (Sterne, Egger, & Moher, 2011; Zwetsloot et al., 2017). Funnel plots allow us to test

for publication bias at the level of the main effect (i.e., the congruency effect).

However, publication bias can also occur at the moderator level. That is, if studies

that did not find a significant moderation effect were not published, then this could lead to

publication bias that cannot be detected with funnel plots. Therefore, to address this issue,

we will report p-curve analyses for three moderators that have mostly been manipulated

within experiment: Flexibility, Bottom-Up Animacy, and Top-Down Animacy. A p-curve

analysis tests if the distribution of significant p-values is consistent with what can be

expected when the effect is true (Simonsohn, Nelson, & Simmons, 2014; Simonsohn,

Simmons, & Nelson, 2015). That is, true effects have a right-skewed distribution in which p <

.025 is more likely than .025 < p < .050. In contrast, nonexistent effects have a uniform

distribution in which each p-value is equally likely. Finally, p-hacked effects have a left-

skewed distribution in which .025 < p < .050 is more likely than p < .025. A p-curve analysis

investigates the evidential value in a set of studies by testing whether the observed

distribution of p-values is significantly right-skewed. Because it only looks at significant p-

values, this method is robust to publication bias and p-hacking (Simonsohn et al., 2014,

2015). Importantly, simulation studies have shown that p-curve analyses have a controlled

5% false positive rate even when the number of included p-values is low, and that their

statistical power is always higher than that of the individual studies included in the analysis

(Simonsohn et al., 2014).

--- Insert Table 1 about here ---


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 44

Results

Overall Analysis

The overall analysis revealed a large imitation effect, gz = 0.95, SE = 0.04, CI 95% =

[0.88, 1.02], m = 268, k = 7561. However, the I² coefficient indicated that 83% of the

variability was due to heterogeneity of the effect sizes2. Adding dependent variable to the

model as a moderator revealed a significant modulation, F(2, 69.3) = 20.51, p < .001, with a

larger effect size for RTs, gz = 1.03, SE = 0.04, CI 95% = [0.95, 1.12], m = 242, k = 546, than for

ERs, gz = 0.69, SE = 0.04, CI 95% = [0.60, 0.77], m = 96, k = 142, and KMs, gz = 0.77, SE = 0.10,

CI 95% = [0.56, 0.97], m = 26, k = 68.

Publication Bias

An inspection of the funnel plots revealed that the effect size distribution was

symmetrical for all three dependent variables (Figures 4-6). In line with this visual analysis,

Egger’s regression test found no relation between sample size and effect size for either RTs,

z = -1.55, p = .121, ERs, z = -1.86, p = .062, or KMs, z = 0.64, p = .521 (Sterne & Egger, 2005).

Although the ER regression test approached significance, the funnel plot suggested that this

was due to a single study with a large effect size and a large sample. Indeed, when this study

was removed, there was no longer evidence for ER funnel plot asymmetry, z = -1.00, p =

.317. In conclusion, the funnel plot analyses suggest it is unlikely that publication bias had a

strong influence on the main effect of automatic imitation. P-curve analyses testing for
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 45

publication bias at the moderator level will be reported together with the corresponding

moderator effects.

--- Insert Figures 4-6 about here ---

What Processes Contribute to Automatic Imitation?

Spatial Compatibility. We first explored the effect of simple spatial compatibility on

RT/ER automatic imitation by comparing studies that confounded imitative and simple

spatial compatibility with studies that controlled for simple spatial compatibility either by

using orthogonal stimulus-response compositions, by separately manipulating imitative and

spatial compatibility, or by using stimuli that could not be classified on a simple spatial

dimension3. This revealed smaller effect sizes for both RTs, t(231.0) = -4.15, p < .001, and

ERs, t(90.8) = -3.51, p < .001, when simple spatial compatibility was controlled. However,

robust automatic imitation was still present even when simple spatial compatibility did not

contribute (Table 2 and 3).

Second, we investigated the effect of orthogonal spatial compatibility on RT/ER

automatic imitation by comparing studies that used orthogonal stimulus-response

compositions (i.e., orthogonal) with studies that separately manipulated imitative and

spatial compatibility (i.e., independent). Importantly, this analysis was restricted to studies

using finger lifting/tapping or hand opening/closing stimuli because other studies have not

yet used orthogonal stimulus-response compositions (Heyes, 2011). The finger analysis

indicated smaller effect sizes for RTs, t(38.5) = -4.95, p < .001, gdiff = 0.48, and ERs, t(11.8) = -

2.23, p = .046, gdiff = 0.23, in independent studies than in orthogonal studies. However, even

in independent studies, there was robust RT, gz = 0.78, SE = 0.08, CI 95% = [0.63, 0.93], and
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 46

ER, gz = 0.48, SE = 0.09, CI 95% = [0.30, 0.66] automatic imitation. The RT hand analysis also

indicated smaller effect sizes in independent studies, t(24.7) = -4.07, p < .001, gdiff = 0.38.

However, this analysis should be interpreted with care because it included only a single

independent study (Press et al., 2008). The ER hand analysis did not have sufficient dfs and

could therefore not be interpreted. Taken together, the spatial compatibility analysis thus

indicates that both simple and orthogonal spatial compatibility inflated automatic imitation,

but could not explain it.

Compatibility Type. The difference between movement and effector compatibility

was explored for RTs and ERs. The RT analysis found a smaller imitation effect for

movement compatibility than for effector compatibility, t(227.1) = 6.35, p < .001 (Table 2).

However, compatibility type was confounded with paradigm type and response effector

(Table 1). We therefore controlled for these variables in a second analysis, but the effect of

compatibility type remained significant, both p ≤ 0.024. The ER analysis mirrored the RT

pattern but did not reach significance, t(31.3) = 1.73, p = 0.093 (Table 3). This indicates that

automatic imitation was stronger in effector compatibility tasks than in movement

compatibility tasks.

Is Automatic Imitation Flexible?

The flexibility of RT and ER automatic imitation was investigated by comparing

conditions in which compatible sensorimotor association were formed with conditions in

which incompatible sensorimotor association were formed, and conditions in which no or

non-contingent sensorimotor associations were formed. However, the ER analysis could not

be interpreted because it did not have sufficient dfs for all relevant t contrasts. The RT
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 47

analysis revealed a marginally significant main effect, F(2, 17.8) = 2.76, p = 0.090, with a

smaller effect size for incompatible associations than for compatible associations, t(20.0) = -

2.40, p = 0.027, and a trend towards a smaller effect size for incompatible associations than

for no associations, t(16.3) = -1.99, p = 0.064. No difference was found between compatible

associations and no associations, t(11.9) = 1.04, p = 0.317 (Table 2).

In addition to the main analysis, we also performed a p-curve analysis on the RT data

(Supplementary Material 1). The half p-curve test revealed a significant right-skewed

distribution, z = -4.64, p < .001 (Figure 7). The robustness of this analysis was checked in

three steps. In a first step, we excluded one experiment with unclear hypotheses and one

experiment that used non-standard analyses. In a second step, we additionally excluded two

experiments in which non-contingent incompatible associations were used as a baseline

instead of no associations. Finally, in a third step, we explored the influence of removing the

lowest p-values from each of the analyses reported above. The results revealed a significant

half p-curve for the first two robustness analyses, both z ≤ -4.2, both p < .001, that was

resistant to removal of the 5 lowest p-values. In other words, the p-curve analysis showed

robust evidence for the flexibility of automatic imitation and thus strengthened the

preliminary evidence provided by the main analysis.

--- Insert Figure 7 about here ---

When During Action Preparation Does Automatic Imitation Operate?

To investigate whether automatic imitation interferes with the motor goal or with

the motor plan, we compared forced choice and simple response tasks using the factor
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 48

paradigm type. This analysis was restricted to RTs because there are no meaningful errors in

simple response tasks. The RT analysis revealed a larger effect size in forced choice tasks

than in simple response tasks, t(148.6) = 7.31, p < .001, and this effect was still present

when the confounding influence of compatibility type was controlled, p < .001 (Table 1).

Importantly, however, a robust imitation effect was obtained in both tasks (Table 2). This

suggests that automatic imitation influenced both the motor goal and the motor plan.

Is Automatic Imitation Really Automatic?

Speed. To investigate the speed of automatic imitation, we investigated the

influence of SOA on automatic imitation. The SOA analysis was restricted to conditions

where the imperative cue was presented either together with the stimulus movement (i.e.,

SOA = 0) or after the stimulus movement (i.e., SOA > 0). Given that SOA varied not only

between but also within experiments, we furthermore tested both its between- and within-

experiment effect (Tanner-Smith & Tipton, 2014). The between-experiment analysis

revealed a negative effect of SOA on automatic imitation for both RTs, t(13.9) = -7.44, p <

.001, and ERs, t(7.0) = -3.83, p = .007. The within-experiment analysis confirmed this pattern

for RTs, t(13.2) = -2.55, p = .024, but not for ERs, t(4.9) = -0.91, p = .408. An inspection of

Figure 8 revealed that the imitation effect could be observed until SOAs of around 1000 to

1200 ms. Together, these results suggest that automatic imitation became weaker as SOA

increased but did not disappear unless very long SOAs were used.

--- Insert Figure 8 about here ---


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 49

Spatial Attention. The role of spatial attention on RT and ER automatic imitation was

investigated by looking at the effect of cue location and cue modality. The RT analysis

revealed a significant effect of cue location, t(39.8) = 3.81, p < .001, with a stronger

imitation effect when the cue was positioned near the stimulus movement compared to

when it was positioned away from the stimulus movement. The ER analysis did not find an

effect of cue location, t(21.4) = 1.18, p = .252, but the numerical pattern mirrored the RT

results (Table 3). The cue modality analysis was restricted to RTs because the ER analysis did

not have sufficient dfs to be interpreted. This revealed a marginally stronger effect size

when visual cues rather than auditory cues were used, t(5.3) = 2.32, p = 0.065 (Table 2).

However, there was also a substantial correlation between cue location and cue modality

(Table 1). When we included both moderators in the same model, a significant effect was

found for cue location, t(30.2) = 3.42, p = .002, but not for cue modality, t(7.5) = -0.64, p =

.542. Taken together, these analyses indicate that automatic imitation was weaker when

spatial attention was not directed to the stimulus movement.

What Is the Role of Action Goals in Automatic Imitation?

To investigate the effect of action goals on automatic imitation, we compared

goalless stimulus movements with object-directed and communicative stimulus movements

for RTs and ERs. The RT analysis revealed a significant main effect of action goal, F(2, 33.5) =

11.19, p < .001, with a stronger imitation effect for goalless stimulus movements than for

both communicative stimulus movements, t(17.3) = 2.60, p = .019, and objected-directed

stimulus movements, t(37.5) = 4.5, p < .001 (Table 2). There was no significant difference

between communicative and object-directed stimulus movements, t(30.6) = 1.29, p = 0.205.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 50

Although there was a substantial correlation between action goal and response effector, the

main effect of action goal did not change when response effector was included as a control

variable, F(2, 23.4) = 6.85, p = .005 (Table 1). In line with the RT analysis, the ER analysis also

demonstrated a main effect of action goal, F(2, 12.4) = 4.89, p = .027. This effect was

characterized by a stronger imitation effect for goalless stimulus movements than for

object-directed stimulus movements, t(8.0) = 3.12, p = .014, and a trend towards a stronger

imitation effect for goalless stimulus movements than for communicative stimulus

movements, t(11.1) = 2.14, p = .056. No difference was found between object-directed and

communicative stimulus movements, t(13.6) = 1.34, p = .202 (Table 3).

These findings speak against the hypothesis that goals have a stronger influence on

automatic imitation than movements. Furthermore, because the imitation effect was not

only weaker for object-directed stimulus movements but also for communicative stimulus

movements, they also speak against the hypothesis that objects may drive attention away

from the stimulus movement. Nevertheless, to further examine the attentional hypothesis,

we additionally tested whether RT automatic imitation differed between object-directed

stimulus movements that were performed towards an actual object and object-directed

stimulus movements that were performed towards an implied object (i.e., pantomimes). In

contrast with the attentional hypothesis, the effect sizes for pantomimes, gz = 0.66, SE =

0.09, CI 95% = [0.45, 0.87], and object-directed stimulus movements, gz = 0.71, SE = 0.13, CI

95% = [0.45, 0.97], were highly similar. Moreover, the action goal analysis confirmed the

significant main effect of action goal, F(3, 25.4) = 9.34, p < .001, with a stronger imitation

effect for goalless stimulus movements than for object-directed stimulus movements,

t(23.6) = 3.27, p = .003, pantomimes, t(8.8) = 4.71, p = .001, and communicative stimulus
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 51

movements, t(17.3) = 2.60, p = .019. None of the other comparisons reached significance, all

t(16.8) ≥ 1.52, all p ≥ .147.

Is Automatic Imitation Sensitive to the Similarity Between Actor and Imitator?

Bottom-Up Animacy. The role of bottom-up animacy was tested by comparing RT,

ER, and KM automatic imitation of human, nonhuman, and geometrical models. However,

the ER analysis could not be interpreted because it did not have sufficient dfs. The RT

analysis revealed a significant main effect of animacy, F(2, 23.2) = 26.90, p < .001. Follow-up

tests indicated the presence of a linear trend with stronger automatic imitation for human

models than for nonhuman models, t(11.9) = 2.89, p = .014, and stronger automatic

imitation for nonhuman models than for geometrical models, t(21.7) = 2.97, p = .007 (Table

2). The KM analysis likewise found a main effect of animacy, F(2, 12.5) = 7.23, p = .008.

However, in contrast to our expectations, a weaker imitation effect was obtained for

nonhuman models than for both human models, t(8.6) = -3.46, p = .008, and geometrical

models, t(10.7) = -3.48, p = .005, and no difference was observed between human models

and geometrical models, t(15.6) = 0.89, p = 0.386 (Table 4). Although bottom-up animacy

was highly correlated with motion profile in KM studies (V = .56), including both moderators

in the same model did not change the animacy main effect, F(2, 9.9) = 8.13, p = .008.

In addition to the main analysis, we also conducted a p-curve analysis on the

difference between human and nonhuman models. Comparisons between human and

geometrical models were not included. To maximize the number of data points in the

analysis, we included both eligible RT (n = 3) and KM (n = 1) studies (Supplementary

Material 2). The half p-curve analysis revealed a significantly right-skewed distribution, z = -

3.60, p < .001 (Figure 8). We then repeated the p-curve analysis including only RT studies.
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 52

However, this also indicated significant right skew, z = -4.22, p < .001. Taken together, the

RT analysis showed that automatic imitation became stronger when the similarity between

the model and the participant increased. In contrast, the KM analysis revealed an

unexpected pattern with reduced automatic imitation for nonhuman models but not for

geometrical models.

--- Insert Figure 9 about here ---

Top-Down Animacy. The influence of top-down animacy was investigated for RTs

and ERs. That is, we examined whether automatic imitation of human/ambiguous models

was reduced when participants were told that these models were nonhuman and whether

automatic imitation of nonhuman/ambiguous models was increased when participants were

told that these models were human. However, we will report only the RT analysis for

nonhuman/ambiguous models because the other analyses did not have sufficient dfs to be

interpreted. In contrast to our expectations, we found no increased automatic imitation in

the human belief condition, t(6.8) = 0.60, p = 0.566 (Table 2).

We followed up on these results with a p-curve analysis. Similar to bottom-up

animacy, both RT (n = 3) and KM (n = 1) studies were included to maximize the number of

data points in the analysis (Supplementary Material 3). In line with the main analysis, the

half p-curve analysis found no significant right skew, z = -0.9, p = .185. The flat test

furthermore indicated that the obtained curve was not significantly flatter than the curve

that is expected when studies are powered at 33%, z =-0.67, p =.251. This indicates that

there is currently no evidence for either the presence or absence of a top-down animacy
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 53

effect (Figure 9). Similar to bottom-up animacy, including only RT studies did not change the

results.

--- Insert Figure 10 about here ---

Motion Profile. To explore the influence of motion profile on KM automatic

imitation, we compared biological and non-biological motion profiles. However, there was

no difference in effect size between the two motion profiles, t(7.5) = 0.02, p = .986, and

controlling for the relation between motion profile and bottom-up animacy (V = .56) did not

change this, t(7.4) = -0.62, p = .554 (Table 4).

For RT and ER automatic imitation, we instead compared one-frame stimuli with

two-frame stimuli and multi-frame stimuli. The RT analysis revealed a main effect of number

of frames, F(2, 44.9) = 3.52, p = .038. This effect was characterized by a smaller effect size

for one-frame stimuli than for two-frame stimuli, t(19.3) = -2.68, p = .015, and a marginally

smaller effect size for one-frame stimuli than for multi-frame stimuli, t(18.6) = -1.79, p =

.090 (Table 2). No difference was found between two-frame stimuli and multi-frame stimuli,

t(207.4) = -1.10, p = .273. The ER analysis revealed a marginally significant main effect of

number of frames, F(2, 16.9) = 3.54, p = .052. Similar to RTs, this was due to a smaller effect

size for one-frame stimuli than for two-frame stimuli, t(7.3) = -2.72, p = .028. However,

there was no difference between multi-frame stimuli and one-frame stimuli, t(7.5) = -1.80, p

= .113, or two-frame stimuli, t(81.6) = -1.41, p = .162 (Table 3). In sum, the KM analysis

found no effect of motion profile and the RT/ER analysis found no evidence that automatic

imitation was stronger when the stimulus movement was presented with a short video

rather than with a sequence of two frames.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 54

Perspective. The role of perspective was examined for RTs and ERs. Studies in which

the perspective of the model could not be determined (e.g., a moving dot) were not

included in the analysis. Neither the RT analysis, t(113.3) = 1.53, p = .130, nor the ER

analysis, t(40.8) = 1.24, p = .220, found a difference between first- and third-person

perspective (Table 2 and 3). Importantly, as facial actions are assumed to be learned mainly

through social interaction rather than through self-observation, we also looked at the

influence of perspective while excluding studies on facial automatic imitation, but this

likewise revealed no difference in either the RT, t(110.3) = 1.48, p = .142, or ER, t(34.4) =

1.29, p = .207, analysis. However, Table 1 indicated a substantial correlation between

perspective and response effector for RTs. In a second step, we therefore repeated the RT

analysis while controlling for effector, but the effect of perspective remained non-

significant, t(75.4) = 0.01, p = .996. In other words, there was no support for the hypothesis

that automatic imitation depends on the perspective in which the stimulus movement is

presented.

Gender Overlap. To investigate the hypothesis that automatic imitation depends on

whether the gender of the participant and the model overlap, we looked at the interaction

between participant gender and model gender for RTs and ERs. However, prior to this

analysis, we first explored whether there was a main effect of these two variables. It is

important to note here that non-human agents were not included in any of the model

gender analyses because these models have no gender. The participant gender analysis

found no difference between predominantly male and predominantly female samples for

RTs, t(111.1) = 0.39, p = .695, but a stronger effect in predominantly female samples for ERs,

t(41.8) = 2.20, p = .033. The model gender analysis revealed a main effect for both RTs, F(2,

64.8) = 6.75, p = .002, and ERs, F(2, 28.4) = 3.79, p = .035. The RT main effect was
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 55

characterized by stronger automatic imitation for female models than for ambiguous

models, t(32.7) = 2.12, p = .042, and stronger automatic imitation for ambiguous models

than for male models, t(90.9) = 2.49, p = .015 (Table 2). The ER main effect followed a

similar pattern with stronger automatic imitation for female models than for male models,

t(17.3) = 2.75, p = .014, and marginally stronger automatic imitation for ambiguous models

than for male models, t(55.7) = 1.77, p = .082, but no significant difference between female

models and ambiguous models, t(14.3) = 1.30, p = .214 (Table 3).

Given that most of the participants in the dataset were female (RT: 60%, ER: 61%), an

interesting hypothesis is that the main effect of model gender was driven by a participant

gender (male vs. female) x model gender (male vs. ambiguous) interaction. Note that the

interaction analysis did not include female models because only 3 experiments in the

dataset had used a female model in a predominantly male sample. As a result, the analysis

had insufficient dfs to be interpreted if female models were included. In line with the gender

overlap hypothesis, the RT analysis revealed a significant participant gender x model gender

interaction, t(40.7) = -2.33, p = .025. Follow-up tests found no difference between

predominantly male and predominantly female samples when the model was ambiguous,

t(62.9) = -0.54, p = .592, but a larger effect size in predominantly male samples when the

model was male, t(21.8) = 2.33, p = .029 (Figure 10). The participant gender x model gender

interaction did not reach significance for ERs, t(31.9) = -0.56, p = .580, but the numerical

pattern mirrored the RT data.

Nevertheless, an important caveat is that we coded samples as “predominantly

female” even if only 51% of the participants were female. Instead, a more suitable analysis

may be to analyze the proportion of female participants as a continuous variable. However,

this analysis likewise revealed a significant participant gender x model gender interaction,
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 56

t(19.1) = 2.11, p = .048, with a significant negative relation between proportion of females

and automatic imitation when the model was male, t(13.2) = -2.63, p = .021, β = -0.95, CI

95% = [-1.72,- 0.17], but no relation when the model was ambiguous, t(28.7) = -0.40, p =

.696, β = -0.10, CI 95% = [-0.59, 0.40]. Taken together, the gender overlap analysis thus

indicates that automatic imitation was weaker when there was a discrepancy between the

gender of the model and the gender of the participant.

--- Insert Figure 11 about here ---

Response Overlap. The response overlap analysis was restricted to studies using

finger/lifting tapping stimuli because these were the only stimuli that differed systematically

with respect to response overlap. More precisely, we compared RTs and ERs of studies in

which the required response was a finger lift/tap (i.e., response overlap) with RTs and ERs of

studies in which the required response was a key release/press (i.e., no response overlap).

In support of the response overlap hypothesis, the RT analysis revealed stronger automatic

imitation, t(69.9) = 3.88, p < .001, and the ER analysis revealed marginally stronger

automatic imitation, t(43.5) = 1.77, p = .084, for finger lift/tap responses than for key

release/press responses (Table 2 and 3).

Is Automatic Imitation Related to Social Abilities?

Empathy. The relation between empathy and automatic imitation was assessed for

RTs and ERs, but the ER analysis did not have sufficient dfs to be interpreted. As a result, we

will only report the RT analysis (N = 975). As noted before, we included data from the IRI,

EAI, and EQ. The cognitive empathy analysis was conducted on the IRI Perspective Taking
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 57

and EAI Perspective Taking scales. The emotional empathy analysis was conducted on the

IRI Empathic Concern and EAI Affective Responses scales. Although our analysis focuses on

cognitive and emotional empathy, we also report correlations with IRI Fantasy and IRI

Personal Distress because the IRI has often been used in the automatic imitation literature.

In contrast to the IRI and EAI, the EQ is a unidimensional instrument that combines cognitive

and emotional empathy. Therefore, as a final analysis, we also investigated whether

automatic imitation was correlated with a composite measure of cognitive and emotional

empathy obtained from the IRI, EAI, and EQ data.

To estimate the relation between automatic imitation and empathy, we entered the

mean Fisher transformed correlation of each relevant experiment into the analysis. We then

used hierarchical effects weights instead of random effects weights to account for the fact

that some experiments were part of the same study. However, the results revealed no

significant correlation between automatic imitation and either cognitive empathy, t(9.6) =

0.71, p = 0.497, or emotional empathy, t(9.6) = 1.49, p = 0.168 (Figures 12-13). To

determine whether this non-significant correlation could be interpreted as evidence of

absence, we then tested whether the 90% confidence interval around the obtained

correlation was captured by the equivalence range of r = [-.20, .20]. More specifically, this

would show that the correlation was significantly smaller than r = .20 at α = .05 (Rogers,

Howard, & Vessey, 1993). A r = .20 explains 4% of the variance and can be considered a

small-to-medium effect size according to Cohen (1988). We chose r = .20 as the smallest

effect size of interest because an inspection of the literature revealed that this was the

smallest effect size that could be detected with reasonable power by the best powered

study (i.e., N = 193 for 80% power). The fact that none of the studies were powered to test

smaller effect sizes indicates that such effects were considered practically or theoretically
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 58

irrelevant. The 90% confidence interval of neither cognitive empathy, CI 90% = [-.06, .13],

nor emotional empathy, CI 90% = [-.01, .10], exceeded the equivalence range. This indicates

that the correlation was significantly smaller than r = .20.

In line with the above analyses, the analysis of IRI Fantasy, t(8.7) = 1.15, p = 0.279, CI

90% = [-.03, .12], and IRI Personal Distress, t(8.5) = 1.62, p = 0.141, CI 90% = [-.01, .20],

likewise showed that the correlations with automatic imitation did not differ significantly

from zero, and that they were significantly smaller than r = .20 (Table 2). Finally, no

significant correlation with automatic imitation was found when we calculated a composite

measure of cognitive and emotional empathy, t(10.0) = 1.29, p = 0.225, and this correlation

was again significantly smaller than r = .20, CI 90% = [-.02, .09].

--- Insert Figures 12-13 about here ---

Autism Spectrum Disorder. To test the hypothesis that automatic imitation is

impaired in individuals with ASD, we first tested for differences in RT automatic imitation

between control samples and ASD samples (Table 2). The results of this analysis revealed

that the imitation effect between the two groups was indistinguishable, t(9.7) = 0.24, p =

0.819. Similar to the empathy analyses, we then performed an equivalence test to

determine whether the non-significant result indicated that the effect was absent (Rogers et

al., 1993). The equivalence range was set to gz = [-0.30, 0.30], which corresponds to a small-

to-medium effect size according to Cohen (1988). The approach described for empathy

revealed that d = 0.50 was the smallest effect size that could be detected with reasonable

power by the best powered study (i.e., N = 64 per group for 80% power). However, because

we did not consider effect sizes below d = 0.50 to be theoretically irrelevant, we decided to
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 59

use a similar threshold as in the empathy analysis, namely a small-to-medium effect size.

The 90% confidence interval [-0.22, 0.28] was captured by the equivalence range, indicating

that the effect was significantly smaller than gz = 0.30.

Nevertheless, it could be argued that there were demographic differences between

the two groups (e.g. age, gender, IQ, …) that might have concealed a potential effect. To

address this issue, we performed a second analysis including only the studies that used both

a control group (N = 192) and an ASD group (N = 201). Given that the groups were matched

on demographic variables in all of these studies, this approach controls for a potential

influence of these variables. Notwithstanding, there was again no significant difference

between the two groups, t(16.5) = 0.07, p = .947, and the difference was significantly

smaller than gz = 0.30, CI 90% = [-0.27, 0.30]. Finally, because the previous analyses did not

include ER data or two KM studies on ASD, we repeated the second analysis using not only

RTs but also ERs and KMs. This resulted in N = 213 in the control sample and N = 220 in the

ASD sample. However, in line with the previous analyses, there was again no significant

difference, t(21.0) = -0.06, p = .952, and the difference was significantly smaller than gz =

0.30, CI 90% = [-0.28, 0.26]4. Taken together, the ASD analysis thus strongly suggests that

there was no difference in automatic imitation between the control samples and the ASD

samples.

Secondary Moderators

Age. The influence of age on automatic imitation was explored for RTs and ERs.

However, neither the RT analysis, t(34.7) = 0.85, p = .404, nor the ER analysis, t(16.1) = -1.64,

p = .121, revealed a significant effect (Table 2 and 3).


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 60

Trials. The relation between the number of trials and automatic imitation was

investigated for RTs and ERs. The RT analysis indicated that the imitation effect decreased

when the number of trials increased, t(53.6) = -2.01, p = .050. The ER analysis mirrored this

pattern but did not reach significance, t(22.7) = -1.70, p = .103.

Response Effector. The influence of effector was investigated for RTs and ERs.

However, the ER analysis did not have sufficient dfs to be interpreted. The RT analysis

revealed a significant main effect of effector, F(3, 13.8) = 4.00, p = .030 (Table 2). However,

there was a substantial correlation between effector and perspective, action goal, response

device, and compatibility type. Although the main effect of effector was still present when

controlling for response device, p = .003, it disappeared when controlling for perspective,

action goal, or compatibility type, p ≥ .141. Since the response effector is determined by the

stimulus movement, this suggests – all else being equal – that stimulus differences do not

have a discernible influence on automatic imitation. In line with this view, none of the

effects that were confounded with response effector disappeared when its influence was

controlled.

--- Insert Table 2, 3, and 4 about here ---

Sensitivity Analyses

To test the robustness of the RT and KM findings reported in Tables 2 and 4, we

performed four sensitivity analyses. In a first sensitivity analysis, we varied the ρ parameter

of the RVE models (Fisher & Tipton, 2015). However, none of the results changed when the

default ρ = .80 setting was replaced by ρ = .00, ρ = .20, ρ = .40, ρ = .60, ρ = .80, or ρ = 1.00. In
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 61

a second sensitivity analysis, we used a different method to estimate the effect size for

experiments that did not provide sufficient statistical information. More precisely, we

imputed the median correlation and standard deviation instead of the mean correlation and

standard deviation. This procedure resulted in a median correlation of r = .92 for RTs and r =

.71 for KMs and in a median standard deviation of SD = 26.72 for RTs. In line with the first

analysis, the second sensitivity analysis did not change any of the results.

In a third sensitivity analysis, we removed effect sizes corresponding to geometrical

models from the meta-analysis. Although geometrical objects have been interpreted as

inanimate models in the literature (Gowen & Poliakoff, 2012), it could be argued that these

studies can no longer be seen as automatic imitation studies. Removing geometrical models

caused the marginally significant effect of cue modality to disappear, t(5.3) = 1.54, p = .182,

but no other changes were observed. Finally, in a fourth sensitivity analysis, we added SOA

as a covariate to the RT analyses. Since we did not exclude studies on the basis of SOA in the

present meta-analysis, it could be argued that some of the studies used SOAs that were too

long to obtain a reliable imitation effect5. Even though Table 1 did not reveal substantial

correlations between SOA and the other moderators, we decided to explicitly address

potential SOA confounds by controlling for its influence in a sensitivity analysis. Adding SOA

to the model removed the marginally significant effect of cue modality, t(5.4) = 1.17, p =

.290, the main effect of number of frames, F(2, 45.9) = .69, p = .509, and the linear effect of

number of trials, t(47.8) = -1.20, p = .235.

In summary, the sensitivity analyses indicated that the results for cue modality,

number of frames, and number of trials were sensitive to methodological decisions.

However, an inspection of Table 2 shows that the evidence for these effects was already

relatively weak in the main analysis. Taken together, it can therefore be concluded that
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 62

three small effects proved to be unstable but that none of the other main findings were

sensitive to four important methodological decisions.

Discussion

Automatic imitation is widely studied in cognitive (Heyes, 2011), social (Wang &

Hamilton, 2012), and developmental psychology (Ray & Heyes, 2011), and in animal

cognition (Heyes, 2012), robotics (Chaminade & Cheng, 2009), and neuroscience (Bien et al.,

2009; Brass & Heyes, 2005; Hamilton, 2015). Although the moderators of automatic

imitation have already been reviewed extensively in previous work (Chaminade & Cheng,

2009; R. Cook et al., 2014; Gowen & Poliakoff, 2012; Heyes, 2011; Press, 2011; Sowden &

Shah, 2014; Wang & Hamilton, 2012), the evidence has often been inconsistent and

important questions remain. Therefore, the goal of the current study was to answer seven

key theoretical questions on automatic imitation: What processes contribute to automatic

imitation? Is automatic imitation flexible? When during action preparation does automatic

imitation operate? Is automatic imitation really automatic? What is the role of action goals

in automatic imitation? Is automatic imitation sensitive to the similarity between actor and

imitator? Is automatic imitation related to social abilities? In what follows, we will discuss

the answers obtained to each of these questions, and their implications for related research

fields. We will furthermore elaborate on whether KM automatic imitation is a valid measure

of automatic imitation, and go into the limitations of the present meta-analysis.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 63

What Processes Contribute to Automatic Imitation?

What Is The Role of Spatial Compatibility? In line with previous research, we found

that automatic imitation was weaker but not absent when spatial processes were controlled

(Heyes, 2011). The results furthermore showed that both simple and orthogonal spatial

compatibility influenced automatic imitation. This goes against earlier work in which

automatic imitation was found to be insensitive to orthogonal spatial effects (Jimenez et al.,

2012; Press et al., 2008) and suggests that researchers should opt for paradigms that allow

spatial compatibility to be separated from imitative compatibility (e.g., Bertenthal et al.,

2006; Catmur & Heyes, 2011) or use gestures that are matched on spatial properties

(Bortoletto et al., 2013; Cracco et al., 2018). The finding that spatial processes can interfere

with automatic imitation furthermore highlights the need to explore potential non-social

confounds in social paradigms. This issue is not only relevant for research on automatic

imitation (Heyes, 2011), but also for research on related topics such as joint action that

suffers from similar confounds (Dolk, Hommel, Prinz, & Liepelt, 2013). As shown in the

current meta-analysis, a valuable approach is to show that the explanatory power of social

processes exceeds that of non-social processes.

However, in addition to spatial compatibility, it is important to also consider other

non-social processes that might contribute to automatic imitation. One such process is

emulation, which is the phenomenon that task-irrelevant object properties modulate

responses to task-relevant stimuli (Edwards, Humphreys, & Castiello, 2003; Symes, Ellis, &

Tucker, 2005; Tucker & Ellis, 1998; Wilf, Holmes, Schwartz, & Makin, 2013). When automatic

imitation studies have an object in the stimulus display (e.g., Ocampo & Kritikos, 2010; van

Schie et al., 2008), emulation can act as a confound (Heyes, 2011). However, most studies

do not include objects (e.g., Brass, Bekkering, et al., 2001; Brass et al., 2000; Heyes et al.,
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 64

2005; Stürmer et al., 2000). Given that the current meta-analysis revealed a robust imitation

effect also in these studies, it is clear that emulation cannot explain automatic imitation.

Is There a Difference Between Effector and Movement Compatibility? The analysis

of effector and movement compatibility revealed that automatic imitation was stronger

when it was measured in terms of effector compatibility. A potential explanation for this

finding is that, on incompatible trials of effector compatibility paradigms, the stimulus

movement (e.g., lift index finger) is integrated with the response movement (e.g., lift middle

finger) into a composite movement (e.g., lift both index and middle finger). More

specifically, such a mechanism makes it more difficult to select the correct response on

effector incompatible trials because participants then have to inhibit not only the stimulus

movement but also the composite movement. Given that automatic imitation depends on

the degree of response conflict, such a mechanism can explain why effector compatibility

was stronger than movement compatibility.

The idea that multiple actions can be integrated into a composite action is consistent

with evidence that the actions of multiple agents can be co-represented in the motor

system, as shown in studies on automatic imitation (Cracco & Brass, 2017; Cracco et al.,

2015), the mirror system (Cracco, De Coster, Andres, & Brass, 2016), and joint action

(Ramenzoni, Sebanz, & Knoblich, 2014; Tsai, Sebanz, & Knoblich, 2011). Being able to

integrate planned actions with observed actions into a composite action may furthermore

be an important tool to achieve interpersonal coordination in joint action tasks that require

the cooperation of multiple persons (e.g., rowing). In particular, it allows each person to

represent the actions of the others as a “group action”. This, in turn, enables the use of

motor prediction mechanisms to achieve synchronized group behavior (Colling, Knoblich, &

Sebanz, 2013; Kourtis, Sebanz, & Knoblich, 2013).


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 65

Is Automatic Imitation Flexible?

Consistent with the notion that automatic imitation is a flexible process, we found

weaker imitative responses when incompatible sensorimotor associations were established

(Brass & Heyes, 2005; Brass & Muhle-Karbe, 2014; R. Cook et al., 2014). However, there was

no increase in automatic imitation when compatible associations were established instead.

This suggests that perception-action associations are overlearned due to years of experience

and for that reason are difficult to strengthen. The finding that imitative tendencies are

flexible accords well with neural evidence that mirror activation is likewise modulated by

incompatible sensorimotor training (Catmur, Walsh, & Heyes, 2007; R. Cook et al., 2014;

Heyes, 2010). From the perspective of ML theories, this shows that associations between

perception and action are not inborn but acquired through associative learning (R. Cook et

al., 2014; Heyes, 2010, 2016). Interestingly, the claim that imitation is learned was recently

supported by a comprehensive longitudinal study that followed 106 infants until the age of

9 weeks old and found no evidence of neonatal imitation (Oostenbroek et al., 2016) in

contrast with what has been reported before (Meltzoff & Moore, 1983, 1989; Simpson et

al., 2014). Together with our results, this indicates that “nature” does not provide humans

with an inborn capacity to imitate but rather with an inborn capacity to learn imitation

through “nurture”.

Furthermore, flexible perception-action links offer an elegant solution to the

paradoxical finding that action observation does not always trigger an imitative response

but sometimes also a complementary response (Ocampo & Kritikos, 2010; Ocampo et al.,

2011; Sartori, Betti, & Castiello, 2013; van Schie et al., 2008). For instance, when someone

throws you a ball, your initial response is not to imitate but to open your hands so that you
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 66

can catch the ball. In the associative learning framework, complementary actions can be

explained as contextual modulations of the dominant imitative association (R. Cook,

Dickinson, & Heyes, 2012). That is, it can be learned through experience or instruction that

in specific contexts certain actions should be followed by a complementary rather than an

imitative response (R. Cook et al., 2012; Ocampo & Kritikos, 2010; Ocampo et al., 2011; van

Schie et al., 2008). As such, associative learning mechanisms allow perception-action

associations to support social interaction well beyond mere imitation (Sebanz, Bekkering, &

Knoblich, 2006).

When During Action Preparation Does Automatic Imitation Operate?

The analysis of forced choice and simple response tasks revealed that both tasks

produced reliable automatic imitation, but also that imitative responses were stronger in

forced choice tasks. The finding that automatic imitation was stronger in forced choice tasks

shows that it interfered with the motor goal (“what finger should I lift?”), whereas the

finding that it persisted in simple response tasks shows that it influenced the motor plan

(“what muscles will achieve this?”) as well (Wong et al., 2015). This interpretation is

consistent with neuroscientific evidence that seeing an action with the intention to imitate

does not only activate brain regions involved in action planning (Bien et al., 2009;

Molenberghs et al., 2012) but also activates primary motor regions that execute planned

actions (Caspers et al., 2010), and is further supported by single cell evidence that mirror

neurons can also be found in the primary motor cortex of the macaque brain (Vigneswaran,

Philipp, Lemon, & Kraskov, 2013).


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 67

With regard to simple response tasks, it is interesting to note that stimulus-response

compatibility research outside the domain of automatic imitation has found little support

for the idea that task-irrelevant stimuli can interfere with prepared responses (Brass,

Bekkering, et al., 2001; Hommel, 1996). A potential explanation for this pattern can be

found in the IM assumption that observed and planned movements are represented in the

form of shared perception-action representations (Brass & Heyes, 2005; Prinz, 1997). In

particular, IM argues that the visual image of an action is part of its motor representation

and therefore that observed actions have direct access to the corresponding motor code. As

a consequence, observed actions can influence behavior even after the response has been

selected (Brass, Bekkering, et al., 2001). In contrast, abstract stimuli with low ideomotor

compatibility are not able to directly activate the motor code and are instead assumed to

rely on a translation process (Boyer et al., 2012; Sauser & Billard, 2006). Therefore, task-

irrelevant stimuli in non-imitative tasks can only access the motor code if the response has

not yet been selected (Brass, Bekkering, et al., 2001).

Is Automatic Imitation Really Automatic?

The extent to which automatic imitation is automatic was evaluated by looking at

two components of automaticity, namely speed and attention. The speed analysis revealed

that automatic imitation became weaker when the duration between the stimulus

movement and the imperative cue increased. This suggests that the processes involved in

automatic imitation are fast, and as such that it is an automatic process (Moors & De

Houwer, 2006). Nevertheless, it is of note that more subtle SOA analyses have shown that

automatic imitation is strongest when there is a short interval of 80 to 150 ms between the
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 68

presentation of the stimulus movement and the presentation of the imperative cue (Catmur

& Heyes, 2011; Cracco et al., 2018; Wiggett et al., 2013). Together with the results of the

current study, this suggests that automatic imitation is transient, but also that a short period

of time is needed before the influence of observed actions on behavior reaches its maximal

potential. Given that motor preparation processes are known to operate at a nearly

instantaneous time scale (Wong et al., 2015), a plausible hypothesis is that the delay reflects

the visual processing of observed actions rather than actual imitation.

With regard to attention, the results showed that automatic imitation was reduced

but not eliminated when participants’ attention was directed away from the stimulus

movement. This is consistent with evidence that automatic imitation is weaker (Cracco &

Brass, 2017) or absent (Bach et al., 2007; Chong et al., 2009) when attention is directed

away from the stimulus movement, but also with evidence that imitative responses do not

disappear under conditions of high attentional load (Catmur, 2016). In particular, it indicates

that automatic imitation is sensitive to variations in attention but persists in its absence.

Together with the fact that automatic imitation is an unintentional process (Heyes, 2011),

the present automaticity analysis thus indicates that its classification as “automatic” is

justified (Moors & De Houwer, 2006).

Nevertheless, a full automaticity investigation also requires an evaluation in terms

consciousness (Moors & De Houwer, 2006). Specifically, automatic imitation can be

considered subconscious at the input level if it does not require conscious processing of the

stimulus movement and subconscious at the process level if subjects are unaware of their

imitative behavior (Moors & De Houwer, 2006). An inspection of the literature indicates that

there is currently no evidence that automatic imitation is unconscious at the input level

because it was shown to be absent when the stimulus movement was presented
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 69

subliminally (Mele et al., 2014). The same is true at the process level because participants

typically “feel” the urge to imitate the stimulus movement. This is in sharp contrast with

studies on motor mimicry where subjects are often unaware of their imitative tendencies

(Chartrand & Bargh, 1999). Taken together, it can thus be concluded that automatic

imitation is automatic with respect to some but not all components of automaticity (Moors

& De Houwer, 2006). However, additional research will be needed to understand how

automatic imitation varies with different degrees of attention or awareness, whether the

role of attention depends on the type of attention under consideration (e.g., spatial vs. non-

spatial), and at what stage during processing the delay in automatic imitation occurs (i.e.,

visual, visuomotor, or motor).

What Is the Role of Action Goals in Automatic Imitation?

In contrast to the hypothesis that action goals have a stronger influence on

automatic imitation than movement parameters (Bouquet et al., 2011; Chiavarino et al.,

2013), goalless stimulus movements were found to produce stronger imitative responses

than object-directed or communicative stimulus movements. Furthermore, against the view

that attentional processes might drive attention away from the stimulus movement and

towards the action object (Bird, Brindley, et al., 2007; Leighton, Bird, & Heyes, 2010), no

difference was observed between object-directed movements, pantomimes, and

communicative movements. These findings go against previous research on intentional

imitation (Avikainen et al., 2003; Bekkering et al., 2000) and the mirror neuron system

(Caspers et al., 2010; Rizzolatti & Sinigaglia, 2010), and are instead more consistent with

reports of cross-contextual imitation (Genschow & Florack, 2014; Genschow et al., 2013;
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 70

Genschow & Schindler, 2016). However, does this mean that, despite popular belief (Bien et

al., 2009; Brass & Heyes, 2005; Catmur et al., 2009; R. Cook et al., 2014; Hamilton, 2015;

Heyes, 2011; Simpson et al., 2014), automatic imitation recruits different processes than

intentional imitation and does not activate the mirror neuron system?

To answer this question, it is interesting to consider the study of Chiavarino et al.

(2013) in which automatic imitation was likewise found to be weaker for goalless stimulus

movements than for goal-directed stimulus movements. In line with goal-directed theories

of imitation, these authors argued that goalless actions were coded in terms of movement

characteristics and goal-directed actions in terms of action goals. This can lead to stronger

automatic imitation for goalless actions if movement coding is stronger than goal coding

(Chiavarino et al., 2013), but also if goal coding is slower than movement coding. The latter

is consistent with evidence that it is the motor goal and not the motor plan that accounts

for most of the response time in choice tasks (Wong et al., 2015). Constructing a motor goal

in terms of goal coding is more abstract and may therefore be more difficult than in terms of

movement coding. This, in turn, might make goal coding slower and could explain why it

does not have strong influence in fast-paced automatic imitation paradigms. In support of

this idea, increased automatic imitation for goal-directed stimulus movements was reported

in a KM study that measured automatic imitation over an extended period of time (Bouquet

et al., 2011). This suggests that variations in time course could explain why action goals have

a different effect on automatic imitation than on intentional imitation or mirror activation.

From this perspective, an interesting avenue for future work is to explore whether the

influence of action goals on automatic imitation depends on the time available to process

the stimulus movement (i.e., SOA).


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 71

Is Automatic Imitation Sensitive to the Similarity Between Actor and Imitator?

In line with the hypothesis that self-other overlap is an important modulator of

automatic imitation (Chaminade & Cheng, 2009; Gowen & Poliakoff, 2012; Press, 2011), the

results indicated that RT automatic imitation increased linearly from geometrical models via

nonhuman models to human models. This finding accords well with previous research that

likewise found stronger automatic imitation for human models than for nonhuman models

(e.g., Press et al., 2005, 2006, 2007) and suggests that the absence of a human bias in some

studies might have been caused by the fact that they used models that were too humanlike

to produce an effect (Cracco et al., 2015). In contrast to the RT analysis, the KM analysis

revealed an unexpected pattern with reduced automatic imitation for nonhuman models,

but not for geometrical models. While this pattern is consistent with evidence that dot

movements (e.g., Bouquet et al., 2007; Gowen et al., 2008; Jansson et al., 2007) but not

robotic movements (e.g., Kilner et al., 2003) produce KM automatic imitation, it is unclear

how these findings can be explained within the framework of ML theories. As discussed in

detail later on, a potential explanation might be that KM tasks of automatic imitation

measure something else than imitation.

In contrast to bottom-up animacy, we found no evidence that automatic imitation

was sensitive to animacy beliefs. This speaks against previous research reporting such

effects (Gowen et al., 2016; Klapper et al., 2014; Liepelt & Brass, 2010b; Stanley et al., 2007)

and instead is more consistent with the study of Press et al. (2006) where similar results

were obtained (see also Press, 2011). Notwithstanding, it is of note that the number of

studies investigating animacy beliefs is currently limited. For example, the p-curve analysis

indicated that there was no evidence either in support or against a top-down animacy
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 72

effect. It will therefore be important to further investigate this factor in future studies.

Indeed, evidence suggests that psychological beliefs on topics such as free will can have

pervasive effects on human performance (e.g., Rigoni, Kühn, Gaudino, Sartori, & Brass,

2012; Rigoni, Wilquin, Brass, & Burle, 2013). In addition to top-down animacy, there was

also no evidence that RT or KM automatic imitation was stronger when the stimulus

movements followed a biologically plausible motion profile (Kilner et al., 2007). However,

similar to top-down animacy, the KM motion profile analysis was based on just a limited set

of studies, highlighting the need for further research. Moreover, the difference between

biological and non-biological motion profiles is often subtle. It will thus be interesting to see

if an effect can be found when stronger manipulations are used. In support of this notion,

research has shown that automatic imitation is reduced when the stimulus movements are

impossible to execute (Liepelt & Brass, 2010a; Longo et al., 2008).

In addition to animacy, we also considered the visual perspective in which the

stimulus movement was presented. Given that ML theories assume that automatic

imitation, especially of non-facial actions, is learned through self-observation (Brass &

Heyes, 2005), it can be predicted from these theories that imitative responses should be

stronger when the stimulus movements are shown in a first-person perspective. However,

in contrast to this hypothesis, we found no influence of perspective on automatic imitation.

A potential explanation is that the effect of perspective depends on contextual moderators

(Bortoletto et al., 2013; Vogt et al., 2003), but there are currently no theories that describe

which variables should and which variables should not interact with visual perspective.

Therefore, a more parsimonious account is that perception-action links gradually generalize

over different perspectives as a result of continual exposure to the actions of others

(Burgess, van Baaren, Fitzgibbon, Fitzgerald, & Enticott, 2013). Indeed, in addition to self-
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 73

observation, social exposure is considered to be an important mechanism through which

automatic imitation develops (Brass & Heyes, 2005). Therefore, it is conceivable that with

sufficient social experience a third-person bias rather than a first-person bias starts to

emerge. In line with this view, a study using the rubber hand illusion found that the motor

system responds more strongly to observed actions that are attributed to another person

than to observed actions that are attributed to oneself (Schütz-Bosbach et al., 2006).

Finally, we looked at two factors that have so far not been studied in the literature,

namely gender overlap and response overlap. First, the gender analysis indicated that ER

automatic imitation was stronger in predominantly female samples than in predominantly

male samples and that RT automatic imitation was stronger when the model’s gender

matched the gender of the majority of the participants. This is consistent with the study of

Butler et al. (2015) in which automatic imitation of a female model was found to be stronger

for female participants than for male participants (see also Genschow et al., 2017).

However, this study did not include a male model and was thus unable to determine

whether the gender effect was driven by participant gender or by gender overlap. The

current results provide preliminary evidence that both factors may play a role.

Second, the response overlap analyses revealed that automatic imitation was

stronger when the response matched the stimulus movement not only in terms of visual

characteristics but also in terms of the other outcomes produced by these actions. This

finding supports the theoretical assumption that actions are represented in the motor

system as the combination of their sensory outcomes (Brass & Heyes, 2005). It furthermore

suggests that researchers should strive to maximize stimulus and response overlap in

automatic imitation studies. That is to say, our results indicate that finger tapping/lifting
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 74

stimuli may produce stronger automatic imitation in studies measuring finger tap/lift

responses than in studies measuring key press/release responses.

To conclude, the similarity analysis revealed that automatic imitation depends on

how similar the model is to the participant, but not on more subtle factors such as animacy

beliefs or motion profile. These results are consistent with the literature on mirror neurons

(Press, 2011) and motor mimicry (Chartrand & van Baaren, 2009). However, in contrast to

automatic imitation, mimicry research has focused more on overlap of opinions or social

groups than on overlap of physical characteristics (e.g., Castelli, Pavan, Ferrari, & Kashima,

2009; Yabar, Johnston, Miles, & Peace, 2006). Rather than feature overlap (Press, 2011), this

research has argued that similar others are mimicked more often because individuals use

mimicry to foster social relations and are more motivated to affiliate with persons that are

more like themselves (Lakin & Chartrand, 2003; Stel et al., 2016; Wang & Hamilton, 2012).

Interestingly, the idea that affiliation goals shape who we do and who we don’t imitate has

now also received support from studies on automatic imitation (e.g., J. L. Cook & Bird, 2011,

2012; Genschow & Schindler, 2016; Gleibs, Wilson, Reddy, & Catmur, 2016; Leighton, Bird,

Orsini, & Heyes, 2010; Rauchbauer, Majdandzic, Hummer, Windischberger, & Lamm, 2015).

However, the role of feature overlap in motor mimicry so far remains unclear.

Is Automatic Imitation Related to Social Abilities?

Is Automatic Imitation Related to Empathy? The final question addressed in our

meta-analysis was the relation between automatic imitation and social abilities. According

to prominent accounts of social cognition, automatic imitation should be related to

empathy because embodied simulation and self-other control are essential components of

both constructs (Bird & Viding, 2014; Brass et al., 2009; Decety & Lamm, 2006; Sowden &
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 75

Shah, 2014). However, in contrast to this prediction, we found no association between

automatic imitation and empathy in a combined sample of almost 1000 participants (Butler

et al., 2015; Genschow et al., 2017). This finding highlights the need for a more nuanced

view on the role of self-other processes in social cognition. For example, self-other theories

could be updated to take into account the representational content on which a process

operates. That is, automatic imitation might be unrelated to empathy because the former

operates on motor representations and the latter on emotional representations even

though the performed operations are functionally equivalent. As would be expected from

such a hypothesis, research that looked at facial feedback in response to emotional

expressions has so far found consistent relations with empathy (Sonnby-Borgström, 2002;

Sonnby-Borgström, Jönsson, & Svensson, 2003).

Alternatively, it could be that a correlational approach is not suitable to investigate

the role of self-other processes in empathy, and that experimental approaches might be

more appropriate instead. In line with this view, it was recently demonstrated that short

periods of imitation or counter-imitation training can improve empathic abilities in a

subsequent unrelated task (De Coster, Andres, & Brass, 2014; De Coster, Verschuere,

Goubert, Tsakiris, & Brass, 2013; De Coster, Wiersema, Deschrijver, & Brass, 2017; de

Guzman, Bird, Banissy, & Catmur, 2015; Santiesteban, White, et al., 2012). Taken together,

the absence of a relation with empathy indicates that theories of social cognition should

consider the content of self-other representations and suggests that experimental

approaches might be more sensitive than correlational approaches.

Finally, although a relation with empathy has been reported in the domain of motor

mimicry, it is notable that the support for this relation is restricted to a single study

(Chartrand & Bargh, 1999) and could not be replicated in more recent work with higher
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 76

power (Genschow et al., 2017). Together with the current meta-analysis, this strongly

suggests that the assumed relation between measures of imitation and measures of

empathy should be reconsidered. More generally, our results call for caution when

interpreting correlations with personality measures in small sample studies. For example,

while preliminary studies has also reported relations between automatic imitation and

measures such as narcissism (Hogeveen & Obhi, 2013; Obhi et al., 2014), recent research

with larger samples has revealed that automatic imitation, but also motor mimicry, is largely

uncorrelated with personality traits (Butler et al., 2015; Genschow et al., 2017).

Is Automatic Imitation Impaired in Autism? Theories of embodied simulation and

self-other control furthermore have a prominent position in the literature on ASD (Spengler,

Bird, et al., 2010; Williams, 2008; Williams et al., 2001). The central tenet of simulation

theory is that social difficulties in ASD stem from a broken mirror neuron system (Williams,

2008; Williams et al., 2001). This hypothesis is challenged by proponents of the self-other

control theory who argue that it is not the mirror neuron system but the control over this

system that is abnormal in ASD (Spengler, Bird, et al., 2010). Although both theories assume

that automatic imitation is impaired in ASD, the former predicts hypo-imitation (Williams,

2008; Williams et al., 2001) and the latter predicts hyper-imitation (Spengler, Bird, et al.,

2010). However, In contrast to both hypotheses, we found no difference in automatic

imitation between ASD samples and control samples even after we controlled for

demographic variables. Given that our analysis contained 220 participants with ASD, this

clearly indicates that ASD is not associated with deficits in imitation or imitative control and

as such casts doubt on the idea that self-other processes are impaired in autism (Spengler,

Bird, et al., 2010; Williams, 2008; Williams et al., 2001).


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 77

Nevertheless, the absence of hypo- and hyper-imitation does not necessarily indicate

that imitation is normal in ASD. For example, the social top-down response modulation

(STORM) theory proposes that it is not imitation as such but the ability to adapt imitation to

the social context that is impaired in individuals with ASD (Forbes et al., 2016; Hamilton,

2013). This theory originated from evidence that healthy participants do while ASD

participants do not display stronger automatic imitation in prosocial contexts compared

with antisocial contexts (J. L. Cook & Bird, 2012) and is supported by related evidence that

the influence of eye contact on automatic imitation, which is present in control participants,

is absent in ASD participants (Forbes et al., 2016). Importantly, however, our results suggest

that STORM deficits should not be interpreted in terms of imitative control because such

problems should be visible as hyper-imitation (Spengler, Bird, et al., 2010). It instead seems

more likely that individuals with ASD fail to act on social cues because they process or

interpret these cues in an atypical manner (Chevallier et al., 2012; Hamilton, 2013).

However, from this perspective, deficits in imitation should no longer be considered a key

component of ASD and should instead be seen as a byproduct of abnormal social processing

(Blake et al., 2003; Chevallier et al., 2012; Klin et al., 2009).

To conclude, we found little evidence for the hypothesis that automatic imitation is

impaired in ASD. This finding is in strong disagreement with self-other theories of autism

(Spengler, Bird, et al., 2010; Williams, 2008; Williams et al., 2001). In particular, it suggests

that self-other processes are intact in individuals with ASD. Nevertheless, similar to

empathy, it remains possible that self-other impairments in ASD do not generalize across

domains but are instead restricted to a specific set of representations. In support of this

hypothesis, there is preliminary evidence that difficulties to distinguish self from other in

ASD might be restricted to the somatosensory domain (Deschrijver, Wiersema, & Brass,
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 78

2016; Deschrijver et al., 2017). Moreover, the STORM hypothesis that social regulation of

imitation is impaired in ASD is a promising avenue for further research (J. L. Cook & Bird,

2012; Forbes et al., 2016; Hamilton, 2013). Nevertheless, research on both theories is only

just developing and further investigation will be required to evaluate the predictions they

put forward. For example, recent work suggests that the effect of social cues on automatic

imitation may be limited to its spatial component (Marsh, Bird, & Catmur, 2016),

highlighting the need to differentiate between spatial and imitative compatibility in studies

of STORM. Finally, it is important to consider the implications of our results for imitative

behavior in related psychiatric disorders. In particular, considering that schizophrenia can be

seen as part of the autism spectrum (King & Lord, 2011), an interesting question is how

intact automatic imitation in ASD can be reconciled with recent reports of disturbed

automatic imitation in schizophrenic samples (Dankinsas, Melynyte, Siurkute, & Dapsys,

2017; Nakagawa & Hoshiyama, 2015).

Is Kinematic Imitation Really Imitation?

An important question that arises from our results is whether KM automatic

imitation actually measures imitation. In contrast to RT automatic imitation, we found no

evidence that KM automatic imitation became stronger when the model looked more like a

human. Instead, we found a reduction of KM automatic imitation when the model was a

robot but not when it was a moving dot. This is consistent with a large number of studies

that likewise found reliable KM automatic imitation in response to moving dots (Bouquet et

al., 2007; Gowen et al., 2008; Jansson et al., 2007; Roberts, Hayes, Uji, & Bennett, 2015;

Romero, Coey, Schmidt, & Richardson, 2012; Stanley et al., 2007). There are at least two
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 79

hypotheses that could explain the KM animacy pattern. A first hypothesis is that participants

interpret moving dots as human movements because moving dots, as opposed to robots,

can be ambiguous in terms of animacy. In line with this view, research has found that

human participants are skilled at recognizing biological motion from humanlike point-light

displays (e.g., Ulloa & Pineda, 2007). However, this hypothesis assumes that automatic

imitation is modulated by animacy beliefs, an assumption that could not be supported by

the data. Moreover, it cannot explain why KM automatic imitation was not reduced when

the stimulus movements followed a non-biological motion profile. Therefore, a second

hypothesis is that KM automatic imitation does not measure imitative processes, but

instead measures something else such as spatial processes.

The spatial hypothesis was recently tested by Hardwick and Edwards (2012) who

separately manipulated imitative and spatial compatibility. Participants in their task had to

make forward-to-backward (i.e., ↕) or left-to-right (i.e., ↔) arm movements while seeing

compatible or incompatible movements of an actor who either looked at the participant’s

face (i.e., ↑↓) or at the participant’s right side (i.e., ↑←). The manipulation of position

caused the relation between imitative and spatial compatibility to change from positive in

the face-to-face condition to negative in the face-to-side condition. This, in turn, made it

possible to calculate a main effect of imitative compatibility that was independent of spatial

compatibility. However, in contrast to RT automatic imitation (Heyes, 2011), the results

revealed that spatial compatibility did while imitative compatibility did not contribute to KM

automatic imitation. Although it is too soon to conclude that KM automatic imitation is

driven solely by spatial processes, these results clearly indicate that additional work is

needed to understand the different processes that are involved. Therefore, future research

may want to consider alternative KM measures that are typically independent of spatial
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 80

processes such as force (Salama et al., 2011), movement duration (Pfister et al., 2013) or

movement speed (Eaves, Haythornthwaite, & Vogt, 2014; Eaves, Turgeon, & Vogt, 2012).

Limitations of the Current Meta-Analysis

To finish, the present meta-analysis also has a number of limitations. A first

limitation is that we focused on automatic imitation and as such did not consider the closely

related phenomenon of motor mimicry. As noted throughout the paper, research on motor

mimicry has often addressed similar questions as research on automatic imitation

(Chartrand & Lakin, 2013; Chartrand & van Baaren, 2009; Heyes, 2011). There is

furthermore a strong overlap between the assumed mechanisms of the two constructs

(Genschow et al., 2017; Heyes, 2011). In consequence, it could be argued that studies on

motor mimicry should have been included in the meta-analysis. However, there are two

important distinctions between automatic imitation and motor mimicry that make this

difficult. First, automatic imitation differs from motor mimicry in the sense that it measures

imitative responses in situations where imitation can interfere with task performance

(Heyes, 2011). Second, there are substantial differences in the experimental procedure of

both constructs that lead to large differences in effect size (Genschow et al., 2017). A meta-

analysis combining multiple constructs is impossible to interpret because it compares

‘apples with oranges’ (Field & Gillett, 2010; Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). Therefore, even though

the present meta-analysis has implications for motor mimicry, it is important to keep in

mind that more research is needed to understand how automatic imitation and motor

mimicry are related (Genschow et al., 2017), and that the results obtained here might but

do not necessarily generalize to the field of motor mimicry.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 81

A second limitation is that most data points in the meta-analysis were obtained from

published studies. As a consequence, it could be argued that our results were at least partly

driven by publication bias. However, it is notable that we also included a substantial number

of unpublished studies. Moreover, there was no evidence for publication bias in the funnel

plots of RT, ER, or KM automatic imitation. This should not be surprising considering that

stimulus-response compatibility effects such as automatic imitation are known to be highly

reliable (Borgmann, Risko, Stolz, & Besner, 2007; Genschow et al., 2017). In addition to the

funnel plots, we also conducted p-curve analyses of three moderators that have mostly

been manipulated within experiments, but these analyses likewise showed no sign of

publication bias (Simonsohn et al., 2014, 2015). Taken together, it thus seems unlikely that

our results can be explained by publication bias.

A final limitation is that not all the effects reported in the meta-analysis can be

interpreted with the same level of confidence. As a rule of thumb, readers should treat the

analyses based on a larger number of studies (e.g., spatial compatibility) with more

confidence than the analyses based on a smaller number of studies (e.g., animacy beliefs).

For example, the fact that we found no significant effect of animacy beliefs on automatic

imitation does not necessarily mean that there is no effect, but could also mean that there

is insufficient data to detect an effect. This interpretation is consistent with the p-curve

analysis, which, due to the low number of studies, was unable to determine whether the

effect of top-down animacy was supported by the data or not. As a result, it will be

important for future research to follow up on the hypotheses that, according to the present

meta-analysis, have been understudied.

In the same vein, further research will be needed to confirm or disprove the effects

of gender overlap and response overlap. That is, neither effect has so far been studied in the
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 82

literature. While the present work provides preliminary evidence for their existence, this

evidence should be interpreted with care. For example, the proportion of female

participants was taken as a proxy for participant gender in the gender overlap analysis.

Likewise, the response overlap effect could also reflect weaker automatic imitation in

studies using key release/press responses regardless of the stimulus movement because

there are currently almost no studies with key release/press stimuli. In other words, we urge

not to interpret the data on gender overlap and response overlap as strong evidence for

these effects but rather as a first step towards a consistent research program that

investigates their influence by manipulating both participant gender and model gender, and

by manipulating the correspondence between the stimulus movement and the response

movement.

Conclusion

Despite the more than 200 experiments on automatic imitation, important

theoretical questions regarding its mechanisms and moderators have proven difficult to

answer. In the current meta-analysis, we evaluated seven such questions. The results

revealed that automatic imitation is a flexible, largely automatic process that is driven

mainly by movement and effector compatibility, but is also modulated by spatial

compatibility. They furthermore identified actor-imitator similarity as an important

modulator and showed that automatic imitation is related to neither empathy nor autism.

Overall, this indicates that automatic imitation is best explained in terms of domain-general

mechanisms such as movement, effector, and spatial compatibility (Heyes, 2011) that

develop through associative learning (Brass & Heyes, 2005; R. Cook et al., 2014).
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 83

More generally, our results provide important insights on the relation between

perception and action in social settings. In addition to automatic imitation, this has

important implications for research on motor mimicry (Chartrand & Lakin, 2013), mirror

neurons (Rizzolatti & Sinigaglia, 2010), and joint action (Sebanz et al., 2006). The current

work furthermore sheds light on the mechanisms through which imitation develops (Ray &

Heyes, 2011) and can be used to build robot models of human social behavior (Chaminade

& Cheng, 2009). Finally, our findings highlight the need to reconsider current theories on

self-other processing in empathy and autism, as well as in social cognition as a whole (Brass

et al., 2009; Sowden & Shah, 2014; Spengler, Bird, et al., 2010; Williams, 2008; Williams et

al., 2001).

References

*Aicken, M. D., Wilson, A. D., Williams, J. H. G., & Mon-Williams, M. (2007). Methodological

issues in measures of imitative reaction times. Brain and Cognition, 63(3), 304–308.

*Ainley, V., Brass, M., & Tsakiris, M. (2014). Heartfelt imitation: High interoceptive

awareness is linked to greater automatic imitation. Neuropsychologia, 60, 21–28.

*Alegre, M., Lazaro, D., Valencia, M., Iriarte, J., & Artieda, J. (2006). Imitating versus non-

imitating movements: Differences in frontal electroencephalographic oscillatory

activity. Neuroscience Letters, 398(3), 201–205.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2006.01.030

Avikainen, S., Wohlschlager, A., Liuhanen, S., Hanninen, R., & Hari, R. (2003). Impaired

mirror-image imitation in Asperger and high-functioning autistic subjects. Current

Biology, 13(4), 339–341. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00087-3


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 84

Bach, P., Peatfield, N. A., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Focusing on body sites: the role of spatial

attention in action perception. Experimental Brain Research, 178(4), 509–517.

*Badets, A., Toussaint, L., Blandin, Y., & Bidet-Ildei, C. (2013). Interference effect of body

shadow in action control. Perception, 42(8), 873–883.

*Bardi , L., & Desmet, C. (2016). Untitled. Unpublished Manuscript.

*Bardi, L., Desmet, C., & Brass, M. (2016). Untitled. Unpublished Manuscript.

Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults

with Asperger’s syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), 163–175.

Bekkering, H., Wohlschläger, A., & Gattis, M. (2000). Imitation of gestures in children is goal-

directed. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 53(1), 153–164.

http://doi.org/10.1080/713755872

*Bertenthal, B. I., Longo, M. R., & Kosobud, A. (2006). Imitative response tendencies

following observation of intransitive actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology-

Human Perception and Performance, 32(2), 210–225.

Bien, N., Roebroeck, A., Goebel, R., & Sack, A. T. (2009). The Brain’s Intention to Imitate: The

Neurobiology of Intentional versus Automatic Imitation. Cerebral Cortex, 19(10), 2338–

2351.

*Biermann-Ruben, K., Jonas, M., Kessler, K., Siebner, H. R., Baeumer, T., Schnitzler, A., &

Muenchau, A. (2008). Observing repetitive finger movements modulates response

times of auditorily cued finger movements. Brain and Cognition, 68(1), 107–113.

Bird, G., Brindley, R., Leighton, J., & Heyes, C. (2007). General processes, rather than “goals,”

explain imitation errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and

Performance, 33(5), 1158–1169.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 85

*Bird, G., Leighton, J., Press, C., & Heyes, C. (2007). Intact automatic imitation of human and

robot actions in autism spectrum disorders. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-

Biological Sciences, 274(1628), 3027–3031.

Bird, G., & Viding, E. (2014). The self to other model of empathy: Providing a new framework

for understanding empathy impairments in psychopathy, autism, and alexithymia.

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, 520–532.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.021

Blair, R. J. R. (2005). Responding to the emotions of others: Dissociating forms of empathy

through the study of typical and psychiatric populations. Consciousness and Cognition,

14(4), 698–718. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.06.004

Blake, R., Turner, L. M., Smoski, M. J., Pozdol, S. L., & Stone, W. L. (2003). Visual recognition

of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychological Science, 14(2),

151–157. http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.01434

Bock, J. K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18(3),

355–387. http://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(86)90004-6

Borgmann, K. W. U., Risko, E. F., Stolz, J. A., & Besner, D. (2007). Simon says: Reliability and

the role of working memory and attentional control in the Simon task. Psychonomic

Bulletin & Review, 14(2), 313–319. http://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194070

*Bortoletto, M., Mattingley, J. B., & Cunnington, R. (2013). Effects of Context on Visuomotor

Interference Depends on the Perspective of Observed Actions. Plos One, 8(1), e53248–

e53248.

*Bouquet, C. A., Gaurier, V., Shipley, T., Toussaint, L., & Blandin, Y. (2007). Influence of the

perception of biological or non-biological motion on movement execution. Journal of

Sports Sciences, 25(5), 519–530.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 86

*Bouquet, C. A., Shipley, T. F., Capa, R. L., & Marshall, P. J. (2011). Motor Contagion Goal-

Directed Actions Are More Contagious than Non-Goal-Directed Actions. Experimental

Psychology, 58(1), 71–78.

*Boyer, T. W., Longo, M. R., & Bertenthal, B. I. (2012). Is automatic imitation a specialized

form of stimulus-response compatibility? Dissociating imitative and spatial

compatibilities. Acta Psychologica, 139(3), 440–448.

*Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement

execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologica, 106(1–2), 3–22.

*Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Wohlschlager, A., & Prinz, W. (2000). Compatibility between

observed and executed finger movements: Comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative

cues. Brain and Cognition, 44(2), 124–143.

*Brass, M., Derrfuss, J., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2005). The inhibition of imitative and

overlearned responses: a functional double dissociation. Neuropsychologia, 43(1), 89–

98.

Brass, M., & Heyes, C. (2005). Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the

correspondence problem? Trends In Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 489–495.

Brass, M., & Muhle-Karbe, P. S. (2014). More than associations: An ideomotor perspective

on mirror neurons. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(2), 195–196.

*Brass, M., Ruby, P., & Spengler, S. (2009). Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social

cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences,

364(1528), 2359–2367.

*Brass, M., Zysset, S., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2001). The inhibition of imitative response

tendencies. Neuroimage, 14(6), 1416–1423.

*Bukowski, H. (2016). Untitled. Unpublished Manuscript.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 87

Burgess, J. D., van Baaren, R. B., Fitzgibbon, B. M., Fitzgerald, P. B., & Enticott, P. G. (2013). A

transcranial magnetic stimulation study of the effect of visual orientation on the

putative human mirror neuron system. Frontiers In Human Neuroscience, 7, Article

679.

*Butler, E. E., & Ramsey, R. (2016). Sex differences in the inhibition of automatic imitation.

Unpublished Manuscript.

*Butler, E. E., Robert, R., & Ramsey, R. (2016). To what extent do expressions and stable

personality traits impact on automatic imitation? Unpublished Manuscript.

*Butler, E. E., Ward, R., & Ramsey, R. (2015). Investigating the Relationship between Stable

Personality Characteristics and Automatic Imitation. Plos One, 10(6), e0129651.

*Butler, E. E., Ward, R., & Ramsey, R. (2016). The Influence of Facial Signals on the

Automatic Imitation of Hand Actions. Frontiers In Psychology, 7, Article 1653.

http://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2016.00512

Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M.-C., Mazziotta, J. C., & Lenzi, G. L. (2003). Neural

mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic

areas. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of

America, 100(9), 5497–502. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0935845100

Caspers, S., Zilles, K., Laird, A. R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). ALE meta-analysis of action

observation and imitation in the human brain. NeuroImage, 50(3), 1148–1167.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.112

Castelli, L., Pavan, G., Ferrari, E., & Kashima, Y. (2009). The stereotyper and the chameleon:

The effects of stereotype use on perceivers’ mimicry. Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology, 45(4), 835–839. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.012


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 88

*Catmur, C. (2016). Automatic imitation? Imitative compatibility affects responses at high

perceptual load. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and

Performance, 42(4), 530–539.

*Catmur, C., & Heyes, C. (2011). Time course analyses confirm independence of imitative

and spatial compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and

Performance Human Perception and Performance, 37(2), 409–421.

Catmur, C., Walsh, V., & Heyes, C. (2007). Sensorimotor learning configures the human

mirror system. Current Biology, 17(17), 1527–1531.

*Catmur, C., Walsh, V., & Heyes, C. (2009). Associative sequence learning: the role of

experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system. Philosophical

Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2369–2380.

Cattaneo, L., Caruana, F., Jezzini, A., & Rizzolatti, G. (2009). Representation of goal and

movements without overt motor behavior in the human motor cortex: a transcranial

magnetic stimulation study. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(36), 11134–11138.

http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2605-09.2009

Chaminade, T., & Cheng, G. (2009). Social cognitive neuroscience and humanoid robotics.

Journal of Physiology-Paris, 103(3–5), 286–295.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2009.08.011

Chaminade, T., Franklin, D. W., Oztop, E., & Cheng, G. (2005). Motor interference between

humans and humanoid robots: Effect of biological and artificial motion. In Proceedings

of 2005 4th IEEE International Conference On Development and Learning (pp. 96–101).

Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The Chameleon Effect: The Perception-Behavior Link

and Social Interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910.
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 89

Chartrand, T. L., & Lakin, J. L. (2013). The Antecedents and Consequences of Human

Behavioral Mimicry. Annual Review of Psychology, Vol 64, 64, 285–308.

Chartrand, T. L., & van Baaren, R. (2009). Human Mimicry. Advances In Experimental Social

Psychology, 41, 219–274.

Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2012). The social

motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 231–238.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007

*Chiavarino, C., Bugiani, S., Grandi, E., & Colle, L. (2013). Is Automatic Imitation Based on

Goal Coding or Movement Coding? A Comparison of Goal-Directed and Goal-Less

Actions. Experimental Psychology, 60(3), 213–225.

*Chong, T. T. J., Cunnington, R., Williams, M. A., & Mattingley, J. B. (2009). The role of

selective attention in matching observed and executed actions. Neuropsychologia,

47(3), 786–795.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,

NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Colling, L. J., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2013). How does “mirroring” support joint action?

Cortex, 49(10), 2964–2965. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.06.006

*Cook, J. L., & Bird, G. (2011). Social attitudes differentially modulate imitation in

adolescents and adults. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3–4), 601–612.

*Cook, J. L., & Bird, G. (2012). Atypical Social Modulation of Imitation in Autism Spectrum

Conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 1045–1051.

*Cook, J. L., Swapp, D., Pan, X., Bianchi-Berthouze, N., & Blakemore, S. J. (2014). Atypical

interference effect of action observation in autism spectrum conditions. Psychological

Medicine, 44(4), 731–740.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 90

Cook, R., Bird, G., Catmur, C., Press, C., & Heyes, C. (2014). Mirror neurons: from origin to

function. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(2), 177–192.

http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X13000903

*Cook, R., Dickinson, A., & Heyes, C. (2012). Contextual Modulation of Mirror and

Countermirror Sensorimotor Associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General,

141(4), 774–787.

*Cook, R., Press, C., Dickinson, A., & Heyes, C. (2010). Acquisition of Automatic Imitation Is

Sensitive to Sensorimotor Contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human

Perception and Performance, 36(4), 840–852.

*Cracco, E., & Brass, M. (2016a). Untitled. Unpublished Manuscript.

*Cracco, E., & Brass, M. (2016b). Untitled. Unpublished Manuscript.

Cracco, E., & Brass, M. (2017). Automatic Imitation of Multiple Agents: Simultaneous or

Random Representation? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and

Performance. http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000489

*Cracco, E., Clauwaert, A., Van den Broeck, Y., Van Damme, S., & Brass, M. (2016). Untitled.

Unpublished Manuscript.

*Cracco, E., De Coster, L., Andres, M., & Brass, M. (2015). Motor simulation beyond the

dyad: Automatic imitation of multiple actors. Journal of Experimental Psychology-

Human Perception and Performance, 41(6), 1488–1501. Empathy data available at:

https://osf.io/eas4m/

Cracco, E., De Coster, L., Andres, M., & Brass, M. (2016). Mirroring multiple agents: Motor

resonance during action observation is modulated by the number of agents. Social

Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(9), 1422–1427.

http://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw059
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 91

*Cracco, E., Genschow, O., Radkova, I., & Brass, M. (2016). Untitled. Unpublished

Manuscript.

*Cracco, E., Genschow, O., Radkova, I., & Brass, M. (2018). Automatic imitation of pro- and

antisocial gestures: Is implicit social behavior censored? Cognition, 170, 179–189.

*Craighero, L., Bello, A., Fadiga, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hand action preparation

influences the responses to hand pictures. Neuropsychologia, 40(5), 492–502.

*Crescentini, C., Mengotti, P., Grecucci, A., & Rumiati, R. I. (2011). The effect of observed

biological and non biological movements on action imitation: An fMRI study. Brain

Research, 1420, 80–92.

*Cross, K. A., & Iacoboni, M. (2013). Optimized neural coding? control mechanisms in large

cortical networks implemented by connectivity changes. Human Brain Mapping, 34(1),

213–225. http://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21428

*Cross, K. A., & Iacoboni, M. (2014a). Neural systems for preparatory control of imitation.

Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 369(1644),

20130176.

*Cross, K. A., & Iacoboni, M. (2014b). To imitate or not: Avoiding imitation involves

preparatory inhibition of motor resonance. Neuroimage, 91, 228–236.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.027

*Cross, K. A., Torrisi, S., Losin, E. A. R., & Iacoboni, M. (2013). Controlling automatic imitative

tendencies: Interactions between mirror neuron and cognitive control systems.

Neuroimage, 83, 493–504.

Crostella, F., Carducci, F., & Aglioti, S. M. (2009). Reflexive social attention is mapped

according to effector-specific reference systems. Experimental Brain Research, 197(2),

143–151.
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 92

Dankinsas, D., Melynyte, S., Siurkute, A., & Dapsys, K. (2017). Pathological Imitative Behavior

and Response Preparation in Schizophrenia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,

32(5), 533–540.

Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a

multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 113–

126. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113

De Corte, K., Buysse, A., Verhofstadt, L. L., Roeyers, H., Ponnet, K., & Davis, M. H. (2007).

Measuring Empathic Tendencies: Reliability And Validity of the Dutch Version of the

Interpersonal Reactivity Index. Psychologica Belgica, 47(4), 235.

http://doi.org/10.5334/pb-47-4-235

De Coster, L., Andres, M., & Brass, M. (2014). Effects of being imitated on motor responses

evoked by pain observation: exerting control determines action tendencies when

perceiving pain in others. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34(20), 6952–6957.

http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5044-13.2014

*De Coster, L., Mueller, S. C., Sjoen, G. T., Saedeleer, L. De, & Brass, M. (2014). The influence

of Oxytocin on automatic motor simulation. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 50, 220–226.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.08.021

De Coster, L., Verschuere, B., Goubert, L., Tsakiris, M., & Brass, M. (2013). I suffer more from

your pain when you act like me: Being imitated enhances affective responses to seeing

someone else in pain. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 13(3), 519–532.

http://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-013-0168-4

De Coster, L., Wiersema, J. R., Deschrijver, E., & Brass, M. (2017). The effect of being

imitated on empathy for pain in adults with high-functioning autism: Disturbed self-
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 93

other distinction leads to altered empathic responding. Autism.

http://doi.org/10.1177/1362361317701268

*de Guzman, M., Bird, G., Banissy, M. J., & Catmur, C. (2015). Self-other control processes in

social cognition: From imitation to empathy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal

Society B-Biological Sciences, 371(1686), 20150079.

http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0079

Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2006). Human Empathy Through the Lens of Social Neuroscience.

The Scientific World Journal, 6, 1146–1163. http://doi.org/10.1100/tsw.2006.221

Deschrijver, E., Wiersema, J. R., & Brass, M. (2016). Action-based touch observation in adults

with high functioning autism: Can compromised self-other distinction abilities link

social and sensory everyday problems? Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience.

*Deschrijver, E., Wiersema, J. R., & Brass, M. (2017). Disentangling neural sources of the

motor interference effect in high-functioning autism: An EEG-study. Journal of Autism

and Developmental Disorders, 47(3), 690–700. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-

2991-2

*Desmet, C., & Brass, M. (2016). Untitled. Unpublished Manuscript.

Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2013). The (not so) social Simon effect: a

referential coding account. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and

Performance, 39(5), 1248–60. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0031031

Dunlap, W. P., Cortina, J. M., Vaslow, J. B., & Burke, M. J. (1996). Meta-analysis of

experiments with matched groups or repeated measures designs. Psychological

Methods, 1(2), 170–177. http://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.170


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 94

Eaves, D. L., Haythornthwaite, L., & Vogt, S. (2014). Motor imagery during action

observation modulates automatic imitation effects in rhythmical actions. Frontiers in

Human Neuroscience, 8, Article 28. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00028

Eaves, D. L., Turgeon, M., & Vogt, S. (2012). Automatic Imitation in Rhythmical Actions:

Kinematic Fidelity and the Effects of Compatibility, Delay, and Visual Monitoring. Plos

One, 7(10), e46728.

Edwards, M. G., Humphreys, G. W., & Castiello, U. (2003). Motor facilitation following action

observation: A behavioural study in prehensile action. Brain and Cognition, 53(3), 495–

502.

Efferson, C., Lalive, R., & Fehr, E. (2008). The Coevolution of Cultural Groups and Ingroup

Favoritism. Science, 321, 1844–1849. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1155805

Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (2003). Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal

priming. Biological Psychology, 64(1–2), 7–26. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-

0511(03)00100-5

*Evans, E. (2014). Is There a Role for Top- Down Factors in “Automatic Imitation”? Retrieved

from https://www.escholar.manchester.ac.uk/uk-ac-man-scw:231750

Field, A. P., & Gillett, R. (2010). How to do a meta-analysis. British Journal of Mathematical &

Statistical Psychology, 63(3), 665–694. http://doi.org/10.1348/000711010x502733

*Fine, J. M., Gibbons, C. T., & Amazeen, E. L. (2013). Congruency Effects in Interpersonal

Coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance,

39(6), 1541–1556.

Fisher, Z., & Tipton, E. (2015). robumeta: An R-package for robust variance estimation in

meta-analysis. Retrieved from http://arxiv.org/abs/1503.02220


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 95

Forbes, P. A. G., Wang, Y., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2016). STORMy Interactions: Gaze and the

Modulation of Mimicry in Adults on the Autism Spectrum. Psychonomic Bulletin &

Review. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1136-0

Gallese, V. (2007). Before and below “theory of mind”: embodied simulation and the neural

correlates of social cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-

Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 659–669. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.2002

Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor

cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609. http://doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.2.593

Gattis, M., Bekkering, H., & Wohlschläger, A. (2002). Goal-directed imitation. In A. Meltzoff

& W. Prinz (Eds.), The Imitative Mind: Development, Evolution and Brain Bases (pp.

183–205). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511489969.011

*Genschow, O. (2016a). Untitled. Unpublished Manuscript.

*Genschow, O. (2016b). Untitled. Unpublished Manuscript.

Genschow, O., & Florack, A. (2014). Attention on the Source of Influence Reverses the

Impact of Cross-Contextual Imitation. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human

Perception and Performance, 40(3), 904–907. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0035430

Genschow, O., Florack, A., & Waenke, M. (2013). The Power of Movement: Evidence for

Context-Independent Movement Imitation. Journal of Experimental Psychology-

General, 142(3), 763–773.

Genschow, O., & Schindler, S. (2016). The influence of group membership on cross-

contextual imitation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1257–1265.

http://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0983-4
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 96

Genschow, O., Van Den Bossche, S., Cracco, E., Bardi, L., Rigoni, D., & Brass, M. (2017).

Mimicry and automatic imitation are not correlated. PLoS ONE, 12(9), 1–21.

http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183784

Gerdes, K. E., Lietz, C. A., & Segal, E. A. (2011). Measuring Empathy in the 21st Century:

Development of an Empathy Index Rooted in Social Cognitive Neuroscience and Social

Justice. Social Work Research, 35(2), 83–94. http://doi.org/10.1093/swr/35.2.83

*Gillmeister, H., Catmur, C., Liepelt, R., Brass, M., & Heyes, C. (2008). Experience-based

priming of body parts: A study of action imitation. Brain Research, 1217, 157–170.

Gleibs, I. H., Wilson, N., Reddy, G., & Catmur, C. (2016). Group dynamics in automatic

imitation. PLoS ONE, 11(9). http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0162880

Gleissner, B., Meltzoff, A. N., & Bekkering, H. (2000). Children’ s coding of human action:

cognitive factors influencing imitation in 3-year-olds. Developmental Science, 3, 405–

414. http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00135

*Gowen, E., Bolton, E., & Poliakoff, E. (2016). Believe it or not: Moving non-biological stimuli

believed to have human origin can be represented as human movement. Cognition,

146, 431–438. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.10.010

*Gowen, E., Bradshaw, C., Galpin, A., Lawrence, A., & Poliakoff, E. (2010). Exploring

visuomotor priming following biological and non-biological stimuli. Brain and Cognition,

74(3), 288–297.

Gowen, E., & Poliakoff, E. (2012). How does visuomotor priming differ for biological and

non-biological stimuli? A review of the evidence. Psychological Research, 76(4), 407–

420.

*Gowen, E., Stanley, J., & Miall, R. C. (2008). Movement interference in autism-spectrum

disorder. Neuropsychologia, 46(4), 1060–1068.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 97

*Grecucci, A., Brambilla, P., Siugzdaite, R., Londero, D., Fabbro, F., & Rumiati, R. I. (2013).

Emotional resonance deficits in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders, 43(3), 616–628. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1603-z

*Grecucci, A., Koch, I., & Rumiati, R. I. (2011). The role of emotional context in facilitating

imitative actions. Acta Psychologica, 138(2), 311–315.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.07.005

Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: with special

reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77(2), 73–99.

*Haffey, A., Press, C., O’Connell, G., & Chakrabarti, B. (2013). Autistic Traits Modulate

Mimicry of Social but not Nonsocial Rewards. Autism Research, 6(6), 614–620.

Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2013). Reflecting on the mirror neuron system in autism: A systematic

review of current theories. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 91–105.

Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2015). The neurocognitive mechanisms of imitation. Current Opinion

in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 63–67. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.01.011

*Hardwick, R. M., & Edwards, M. G. (2011). Observed reach trajectory influences executed

reach kinematics in prehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(6),

1082–1093.

*Hardwick, R. M., & Edwards, M. G. (2012). Motor interference and facilitation arising from

observed movement kinematics. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(5),

840–847.

Hedges, L. V., Tipton, E., & Johnson, M. C. (2010). Robust variance estimation in meta-

regression with dependent effect size estimates. Research Synthesis Methods, 1(1),

39–65. http://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.5
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 98

Hedges, L. V, & Vevea, J. L. (1998). Fixed-and Random-Effects Models in Meta-Analysis.

Psychological Methods, 3(4), 486–504. http://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.3.4.486

Heyes, C. (2010). Where do mirror neurons come from? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral

Reviews, 34(4), 575–583.

Heyes, C. (2011). Automatic Imitation. Psychological Bulletin, 137(3), 463–483.

Heyes, C. (2012). What’s Social About Social Learning? Journal of Comparative Psychology,

126(2), 193–202.

Heyes, C. (2016). Imitation: Not in our genes. Current Biology, 26(10), R412–R414.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.060

*Heyes, C., Bird, G., Johnson, H., & Haggard, P. (2005). Experience modulates automatic

imitation. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(2), 233–240.

Higgins, J. P. T., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2011). Chapter 16: Special topics in statistics. In

J. P. T. Higgins & S. Green (Eds.), Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of

Interventions. The Cochrane Collaboration. Retrieved from

www.handbook.cochrane.org

*Hogeveen, J., Hogeveen, J., & Obhi, S. S. (2011). Altogether now: Activating interdependent

self-construal induces hypermotor resonance. Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(2), 74–82.

http://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2010.533164

*Hogeveen, J., & Obhi, S. S. (2013). Automatic imitation is automatic, but less so for

narcissists. Experimental Brain Research, 224(4), 613–621.

*Hogeveen, J., Obhi, S. S., Banissy, M. J., Santiesteban, I., Press, C., Catmur, C., & Bird, G.

(2014). Task-dependent and distinct roles of the temporoparietal junction and inferior

frontal cortex in the control of imitation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,

10(7), 1003–1009.
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 99

Hommel, B. (1996). S-R Compatibility Effects Without Response Uncertainty. The Quarterly

Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A(3), 546–571.

Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event-coding

(TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioural and Brain Sciences,

24, 849–937. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X01000103

Hull, C. L. (1933). Hypnosis and suggestibility. New York: Appleton-Century.

Inzunza, M. (2014). Adaptation and development of the Empathy Assessment Index (EAI).

International Journal of Comparative and Applied Criminal Justice, 39(3), 239–255.

http://doi.org/10.1080/01924036.2014.989245

Jackson, S., Brady, N., Cummins, F., & Monaghan, K. (2006). Interaction effects in

simultaneous motor control and movement perception tasks. Artificial Intelligence

Review, 26(1–2), 141–154.

*Jansson, E., Wilson, A. D., Williams, J. H. G., & Mon-Williams, M. (2007). Methodological

problems undermine tests of the ideo-motor conjecture. Experimental Brain Research,

182(4), 549–558.

*Jarick, M., & Jones, J. A. (2008). Observation of static gestures influences speech

production. Experimental Brain Research, 189(2), 221–228.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1416-7

*Jarick, M., & Jones, J. A. (2009). Effects of seeing and hearing speech on speech production:

a response time study. Experimental Brain Research, 195(2), 175–182.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-1765-x

*Jimenez, L., Recio, S., Mendez, A., Lorda, M. J., Permuy, B., & Mendez, C. (2012). Automatic

imitation and spatial compatibility in a key-pressing task. Acta Psychologica, 141(1), 96–

103.
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 100

Jonas, M., Biermann-Ruben, K., Kessler, K., Lange, R., Baumer, T., Siebner, H. R., … Munchau,

A. (2007). Observation of a finger or an object movement primes imitative responses

differentially. Experimental Brain Research, 177(2), 255–265.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0660-y

*Jonas, M., Thomalla, G., Biermann-Ruben, K., Siebner, H. R., Mueller-Vahl, K., Baeumer, T.,

… Muenchau, A. (2010). Imitation in Patients with Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome-A

Behavioral Study. Movement Disorders, 25(8), 991–999.

*Killingsworth, S. (2012). Object-Relevant Kinematics Influence Imitative Compatibility.

Retrieved from http://etd.library.vanderbilt.edu/available/etd-07292012-144954/

*Kilner, J. M., Hamilton, A. F. de C., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2007). Interference effect of

observed human movement on action is due to velocity profile of biological motion.

Social Neuroscience, 2(3–4), 158–166.

*Kilner, J. M., Paulignan, Y., & Blakemore, S. J. (2003). An interference effect of observed

biological movement on action. Current Biology, 13(6), 522–525.

King, B. H., & Lord, C. (2011). Is schizophrenia on the autism spectrum? Brain Research,

1380, 34–41.

*Klapper, A., Ramsey, R., Wigboldus, D., & Cross, E. S. (2014). The Control of Automatic

Imitation Based on Bottom-Up and Top-Down Cues to Animacy: Insights from Brain and

Behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(11), 2503–2513.

Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism

orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature, 459, 257–261.

http://doi.org/10.1038/nature07868
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 101

Kourtis, D., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2013). Predictive representation of other people’s

actions in joint action planning: An EEG study. Social Neuroscience, 8(1), 31–42.

http://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2012.694823

*Kupferberg, A., Glasauer, S., Huber, M., Rickert, M., & Knoll, A. (2009). Video observation

of humanoid robot movements elicits motor interference. In Adaptive and Emergent

Behaviour and Complex Systems Convention, Symposium on New Frontiers in Human-

Robot Interaction.

*Kupferberg, A., Huber, M., Helfer, B., Lenz, C., Knoll, A., & Glasauer, S. (2012). Moving Just

Like You: Motor Interference Depends on Similar Motility of Agent and Observer. Plos

One, 7(6), e39637.

Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A

practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 863.

http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863

Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create

affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14(4), 334–339.

Lawrence, E. J., Shaw, P., Baker, D., Baron-Cohen, S., & David, A. S. (2004). Measuring

empathy: reliability and validity of the Empathy Quotient. Psychological Medicine,

34(5), 911–919. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291703001624

Leighton, J., Bird, G., & Heyes, C. (2010). “Goals” are not an integral component of imitation.

Cognition, 114(3), 423–435. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.11.001

*Leighton, J., Bird, G., Orsini, C., & Heyes, C. (2010). Social attitudes modulate automatic

imitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(6), 905–910.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 102

*Leighton, J., & Heyes, C. (2010). Hand to Mouth: Automatic Imitation Across Effector

Systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance,

36(5), 1174–1183.

*Liepelt, R., & Brass, M. (2010a). Automatic Imitation of Physically Impossible Movements.

Social Cognition, 28(1), 59–73.

*Liepelt, R., & Brass, M. (2010b). Top-down modulation of motor priming by belief about

animacy. Experimental Psychology, 57(3), 221–227.

*Liepelt, R., Prinz, W., & Brass, M. (2010). When do we simulate non-human agents?

Dissociating communicative and non-communicative actions. Cognition, 115(3), 426–

434.

*Liepelt, R., Stenzel, A., & Lappe, M. (2012). Specifying social cognitive processes with a

social dual-task paradigm. Frontiers In Human Neuroscience, 6, Article 86.

*Liepelt, R., Von Cramon, D. Y., & Brass, M. (2008). What is matched in direct matching?

Intention attribution modulates motor priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology-

Human Perception and Performance, 34(3), 578–591.

Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical Meta-Analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

*Longo, M. R., & Bertenthal, B. I. (2009). Attention modulates the specificity of automatic

imitation to human actors. Experimental Brain Research, 192(4), 739–744.

*Longo, M. R., Kosobud, A., & Bertenthal, B. I. (2008). Automatic imitation of

biomechanically possible and impossible actions: Effects of priming movements versus

goals. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 34(2),

489–501.
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 103

Marsh, L. E., Bird, G., & Catmur, C. (2016). The imitation game: Effects of social cues on

“imitation” are domain-general in nature. NeuroImage, 139, 368–375.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.050

*Marshall, P. J., Bouquet, C. A., Thomas, A. L., & Shipley, T. F. (2010). Motor contagion in

young children: Exploring social influences on perception-action coupling. Neural

Networks, 23(8–9), 1017–1025. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2010.07.007

Massen, C. (2009). Observing human interaction with physical devices. Experimental Brain

Research, 199(1), 49–58.

Mele, S., Mattiassi, A. D. A., & Urgesi, C. (2014). Unconscious Processing of Body Actions

Primes Subsequent Action Perception but Not Motor Execution. Journal of

Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 40(5), 1940–1962.

Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn Infants Imitate Adult Facial Gestures. Child

Development, 54(3), 702–709.

Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1989). Imitation in newborn infants: Exploring the range of

gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 25(6),

954–962. http://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.25.6.954

*Mengotti, P., Corradi-Dell’Acqua, C., & Rumiati, R. I. (2012). Imitation components in the

human brain: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 59(2), 1622–1630.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.004

*Mengotti, P., Ticini, L. F., Waszak, F., Schuetz-Bosbach, S., & Rumiati, R. I. (2013). Imitating

others’ actions: transcranial magnetic stimulation of the parietal opercula reveals the

processes underlying automatic imitation. European Journal of Neuroscience, 37(2),

316–322.
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 104

Molenberghs, P., Cunnington, R., & Mattingley, J. B. (2012). Brain regions with mirror

properties: A meta-analysis of 125 human fMRI studies. Neuroscience and

Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(1), 341–349.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.07.004

Moors, A., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: A Theoretical and Conceptual Analysis.

Psychological Bulletin, 132(2), 297–326. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.297

Müller, B. C. N., de Graag, J., & van Baaren, R. B. (2016). Monkey see, monkey do:

Complementarity responses in reaction to dominant and submissive movements.

Unpublished Manuscript.

Müller, B. C. N., Van Leeuwen, M. L., Van Baaren, R. B., Bekkering, H., & Dijksterhuis, A.

(2013). Empathy is a beautiful thing: Empathy predicts imitation only for attractive

others. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 54(5), 401–406.

http://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12060

Nakagawa, Y., & Hoshiyama, M. (2015). Influence of observing another person’s action on

self-generated performance in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 20(4), 349–

360.

*Newman-Norlund, R. D., Ondobaka, S., van Schie, H. T., van Elswijk, G., & Bekkering, H.

(2010). Virtual lesions of the IFG abolish response facilitation for biological and non-

biological cues. Frontiers In Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, Article 5.

http://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.08.005.2010

*Obhi, S. S., & Hogeveen, J. (2010). Incidental action observation modulates muscle activity.

Experimental Brain Research, 203(2), 427–435.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 105

*Obhi, S. S., Hogeveen, J., Giacomin, M., & Jordan, C. H. (2014). Automatic imitation is

reduced in narcissists. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and

Performance, 40(3), 920–928. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0034056

*Ocampo, B., & Kritikos, A. (2010). Placing actions in context: motor facilitation following

observation of identical and non-identical manual acts. Experimental Brain Research,

201(4), 743–751.

*Ocampo, B., Kritikos, A., & Cunnington, R. (2011). How Frontoparietal Brain Regions

Mediate Imitative and Complementary Actions: An fMRI Study. Plos One, 6(10),

e26945. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026945

Oostenbroek, J., Suddendorf, T., Nielsen, M., Redshaw, J., Kennedy-Costantini, S., Davis, J., …

Slaughter, V. (2016). Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of

neonatal imitation in humans. Current Biology, 26(10), 1334–1338.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.047

*Otte, E., Habel, U., Schulte-Ruther, M., Konrad, K., & Koch, I. (2011). Interference in

simultaneously perceiving and producing facial expressions-Evidence from

electromyography. Neuropsychologia, 49(1), 124–130.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.11.005

*Otte, E., Jost, K., Habel, U., & Koch, I. (2011). Exploring cross-task compatibility in

perceiving and producing facial expressions using electromyography. Acta

Psychologica, 138(1), 187–192. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.06.003

*Oztop, E., Franklin, D. W., & Chaminade, T. (2005). Human – Humanoid Interaction: Is a

Humanoid Robot Perceived as a Human? International Journal of Humanoid Robotics,

2(4), 537–559.

*Pfister, R. (2016). Untitled. Unpublished Manuscript.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 106

Pfister, R., Dignath, D., Hommel, B., & Kunde, W. (2013). It takes two to imitate: anticipation

and imitation in social interaction. Psychological Science, 24(10), 2117–21.

http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613489139

*Poliakoff, E., Galpin, A., Dick, J., Moore, P., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). The effect of viewing

graspable objects and actions in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroreport, 18(5), 483–487.

http://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e32805867a1

*Poljac, E., van Schie, H. T., & Bekkering, H. (2009). Understanding the flexibility of action-

perception coupling. Psychological Research, 73(4), 578–586.

Press, C. (2011). Action observation and robotic agents: Learning and anthropomorphism.

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(6), 1410–1418.

*Press, C., Bird, G., Flach, R., & Heyes, C. (2005). Robotic movement elicits automatic

imitation. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(3), 632–640.

*Press, C., Bird, G., Walsh, E., & Heyes, C. (2008). Automatic imitation of intransitive actions.

Brain and Cognition, 67(1), 44–50.

*Press, C., Gherri, E., Heyes, C., & Eimer, M. (2010). Action Preparation Helps and Hinders

Perception of Action. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(10), 2198–2211.

http://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21409

*Press, C., Gillmeister, H., & Heyes, C. (2006). Bottom-up, not top-down, modulation of

imitation by human and robotic models. European Journal of Neuroscience, 24(8),

2415–2419.

*Press, C., Gillmeister, H., & Heyes, C. (2007). Sensorimotor experience enhances automatic

imitation of robotic action. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences,

274(1625), 2509–2514.
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 107

*Press, C., Richardson, D., & Bird, G. (2010). Intact imitation of emotional facial actions in

autism spectrum conditions. Neuropsychologia, 48(11), 3291–3297.

Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and Action Planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology,

9(2), 129–154.

Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2006). Stimulus-response compatibility principles: Data, theory

and application. Boca Raton, FL: Taylor & Francis.

Ramenzoni, V. C., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2014). Scaling Up Perception-Action Links:

Evidence From Synchronization With Individual and Joint Action. Journal of

Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 40(4), 1551–1565.

http://doi.org/10.1037/a0036925

*Rauchbauer, B., Majdandzic, J., Hummer, A., Windischberger, C., & Lamm, C. (2015).

Distinct neural processes are engaged in the modulation of mimicry by social group-

membership and emotional expressions. Cortex, 70, 49–67.

*Rauchbauer, B., Majdandžić, J., Stieger, S., & Lamm, C. (2016). The Modulation of Mimicry

by Ethnic Group-Membership and Emotional Expressions. Plos One, 11(8), e0161064.

http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0161064

*Rauchbauer, B., Pfabigan, D., & Lamm, C. (2016). Temporal neural dynamics of automatic

imitation and its modulation by ethnically diverse stimuli - an event-related potentials

study. Unpublished Manuscript.

Ray, E., & Heyes, C. (2011). Imitation in infancy: the wealth of the stimulus. Developmental

Science, 14(1), 92–105.

*Richardson, M. J., Campbell, W. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2009). Movement interference during

action observation as emergent coordination. Neuroscience Letters, 449(2), 117–122.

*Riva, F. (2016). Untitled. Unpublished Manuscript.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 108

Rigoni, D., Kühn, S., Gaudino, G., Sartori, G., & Brass, M. (2012). Reducing self-control by

weakening belief in free will. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(3), 1482–1490.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.04.004

Rigoni, D., Wilquin, H., Brass, M., & Burle, B. (2013). When errors do not matter: Weakening

belief in intentional control impairs cognitive reaction to errors. Cognition, 127(2), 264–

269. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.01.009

Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of

Neuroscience, 27, 169–192. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230

Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit:

interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(4), 264–274.

http://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2805

*Roberts, J. W., Bennett, S. J., & Hayes, S. J. (2015). Top-down social modulation of

interpersonal observation-execution. Psychological Research, 80(4), 487–495.

*Roberts, J. W., Hayes, S. J., Uji, M., & Bennett, S. J. (2015). Motor contagion: the

contribution of trajectory and end-points. Psychological Research, 79(4), 621–629.

Rogers, J. L., Howard, K. I., & Vessey, J. T. (1993). Using Significance Tests to Evaluate

Equivalence Between Two Experimental Groups. Psychological Bulletin, 113(3), 553–

565.

*Romero, V., Coey, C., Schmidt, R. C., & Richardson, M. J. (2012). Movement Coordination or

Movement Interference: Visual Tracking and Spontaneous Coordination Modulate

Rhythmic Movement Interference. Plos One, 7(9), e44761.

Salama, I. M., Turner, S., & Edwards, M. G. (2011). Automatic priming of grip force following

action observation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(5), 833–838.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 109

*Santiesteban, I., Banissy, M. J., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2012). Enhancing Social Ability by

Stimulating Right Temporoparietal Junction. Current Biology, 22(23), 2274–2277.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.10.018

*Santiesteban, I., Banissy, M. J., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2015). Functional lateralization of

temporoparietal junction - imitation inhibition, visual perspective-taking and theory of

mind. European Journal of Neuroscience, 42(8), 2527–2533.

http://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13036

*Santiesteban, I., Bird, G., Tew, O., Cioffi, M. C., & Banissy, M. J. (2015). Mirror-touch

synaesthesia: Difficulties inhibiting the other. Cortex, 71, 116–121.

*Santiesteban, I., White, S., Cook, J. L., Gilbert, S. J., Heyes, C., & Bird, G. (2012). Training

social cognition: From imitation to Theory of Mind. Cognition, 122(2), 228–235.

Sartori, L., Bucchioni, G., & Castiello, U. (2013). When emulation becomes reciprocity. Social

Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(6), 662–669.

http://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss044

Sauser, E. L., & Billard, A. G. (2006). Parallel and distributed neural models of the ideomotor

principle: An investigation of imitative cortical pathways. Neural Networks, 19(3), 285–

298. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2006.02.003

*Schroeter, M. L., Ettrich, B., Schwier, C., Scheid, R., Guthke, T., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2007).

Diffuse axonal injury due to traumatic brain injury alters inhibition of imitative

response tendencies. Neuropsychologia, 45(14), 3149–3156.

*Schunke, O., Schottle, D., Vettorazzi, E., Brandt, V., Kahl, U., Baumer, T., … Munchau, A.

(2015). Mirror me: Imitative responses in adults with autism. Autism, 20(2), 134–144.

http://doi.org/10.1177/1362361315571757
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 110

Schütz-Bosbach, S., Mancini, B., Aglioti, S. M., Haggard, P., Schutz-Bosbach, S., Mancini, B., …

Haggard, P. (2006). Self and other in the human motor system. Curr Biol, 16(18), 1830–

1834. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.048

Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint action: Bodies and minds moving

together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 70–76.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.009

Shanks, D. R., & Darby, R. J. (1998). Feature- and rule-based generalization in human

associative learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Behavior Processes,

24(4), 405–415. http://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.24.4.405

*Shaw, D. J., Czekoova, K., Chromec, J., Marecek, R., & Brazdil, M. (2013). Copying You

Copying Me: Interpersonal Motor Co-Ordination Influences Automatic Imitation. Plos

One, 8(12), e84820.

Sheflen, A. E. (1964). The Significance of Posture in Communication Systems. Psychiatry,

27(4), 316–331.

*Shen, Q., Kose-Bagci, H., Saunders, J., & Dautenhahn, K. (2011). The Impact of Participants’

Beliefs on Motor Interference and Motor Coordination in Human-Humanoid

Interactions. Ieee Transactions On Autonomous Mental Development, 3(1), 6–16.

Shin, Y. K., Proctor, R. W., & Capaldi, E. J. (2010). A Review of Contemporary Ideomotor

Theory. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 943–974.

*Silas, J., Levy, J. P., & Holmes, A. (2012). Sensitivity of “mu” rhythm modulation to the

relevance of an observed movement but not to goal congruency. International Journal

of Psychophysiology, 85(2), 168–173.

Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental

Psychology, 81(1), 174–176.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 111

Simonsohn, U., Nelson, L. D., & Simmons, J. P. (2014). P-curve: a key to the file-drawer.

Journal of Experimental Psychology- General, 143(2), 534–547.

http://doi.org/10.1037/a0033242

Simonsohn, U., Simmons, J. P., & Nelson, L. D. (2015). Better P-Curves: Making P-Curve

Analysis More Robust To Errors, Fraud, and Ambitious P-Hacking, A Reply To Ulrich and

Miller (2015). Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 144(6), 1146–1152.

http://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000104

Simpson, E. A., Murray, L., Paukner, A., & Ferrari, P. F. (2014). The mirror neuron system as

revealed through neonatal imitation: presence from birth, predictive power and

evidence of plasticity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological

Sciences, 369(1644), 20130289. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0289

Smith, A. (1759). The theory of moral sentiments. London: A. Miller.

Sonnby-Borgström, M. (2002). Automatic mimicry reactions as related to differences in

emotional empathy. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43(5), 433–443.

http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9450.00312

Sonnby-Borgström, M., Jönsson, P., & Svensson, O. (2003). Emotional Empathy As Related

To Mimicry Reactions At Different Levels of Information Processing. Journal of

Nonverbal Behavior, 27(1), 3–23.

*Sowden, S., & Catmur, C. (2015). The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in the

Control of Imitation. Cereb Cortex, 25(4), 1107–1113.

*Sowden, S., Koehne, S., Catmur, C., & Dziobek, I. (2015). Intact automatic imitation and

typical spatial compatibility in autism spectrum disorder: Challenging the broken mirror

theory. Autism Research.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 112

Sowden, S., & Shah, P. (2014). Self-other control: a candidate mechanism for social cognitive

function. Frontiers In Human Neuroscience, 8, Article 789.

*Spengler, S., Bird, G., & Brass, M. (2010). Hyperimitation of Actions Is Related to Reduced

Understanding of Others’ Minds in Autism Spectrum Conditions. Biological Psychiatry,

68(12), 1148–1155.

*Spengler, S., Brass, M., Kuhn, S., & Schutz-Bosbach, S. (2010). Minimizing motor mimicry by

myself: Self-focus enhances online action-control mechanisms during motor contagion.

Consciousness and Cognition, 19(1), 98–106.

*Spengler, S., von Cramon, D. Y., & Brass, M. (2009). Control of Shared Representations

Relies on Key Processes Involved in Mental State Attribution. Human Brain Mapping,

30(11), 3704–3718. http://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20800

*Spengler, S., von Cramon, D. Y., & Brass, M. (2010). Resisting motor mimicry: Control of

imitation involves processes central to social cognition in patients with frontal and

temporo-parietal lesions. Social Neuroscience, 5(4), 401–416.

*Stanley, J., Gowen, E., & Miall, R. C. (2007). Effects of agency on movement interference

during observation of a moving dot stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology-

Human Perception and Performance, 33(4), 915–926.

Stel, M., van Dijk, E., & van Baaren, R. B. (2016). When and Why Mimicry is Facilitated and

Attenuated. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10(10), 561–574.

http://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12269

Sterne, J. A. C., & Egger, M. (2005). Regression methods to detect publication and other bias

in meta-analysis. In H. R. Rothstein, A. J. Sutton, & M. Borenstein (Eds.), Publication

bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment, and adjustments (pp. 99–110).

Chichester, England: Wiley.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 113

Sterne, J. A. C., Egger, M., & Moher, D. (2011). Chapter 10: Addressing reporting biases. In J.

P. T. Higgins & S. Green (Eds.), Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of

Intervention. The Cochrane Collaboration. Retrieved from

www.handbook.cochrane.org

Stroop, J. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental

Psychology, 18, 643–662.

*Stürmer, B., Aschersleben, G., Prinz, W., Sturmer, B., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2000).

Correspondence effects with manual gestures and postures: A study of imitation.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(6), 1746–

1759. http://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.26.6.1746

Symes, E., Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2005). Dissociating object-based and space-based

affordances. Visual Cognition, 12(7), 1337–1361.

http://doi.org/10.1080/13506280444000445

Tanner-Smith, E. E., & Tipton, E. (2014). Robust variance estimation with dependent effect

sizes: Practical considerations including a software tutorial in Stata and SPSS. Research

Synthesis Methods, 5(1), 13–30. http://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1091

Tanner-Smith, E. E., Tipton, E., & Polanin, J. R. (2016). Handling complex meta-analytic data

structures using robust variance estimates: a tutorial in R. Journal of Developmental

and Life-Course Criminology, 2(1), 85–112. http://doi.org/10.1007/s40865-016-0026-5

Tipton, E. (2015). Small sample adjustments for robust variance estimation with meta-

regression. Psychological Methods, 20(3), 375–393.

http://doi.org/10.1037/met0000011

Tipton, E., & Pustejovsky, J. E. (2015). Small-Sample Adjustments for Tests of Moderators

and Model Fit Using Robust Variance Estimation in Meta-Regression. Journal of


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 114

Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 40(6), 604–634.

http://doi.org/10.3102/1076998615606099

Tsai, J. C.-C., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2011). The GROOP effect: Groups mimic group

actions. Cognition, 118(1), 135–140.

Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of

potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and

Performance, 24(3), 830–846. http://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.830

Ulloa, E. R., & Pineda, J. A. (2007). Recognition of point-light biological motion: Mu rhythms

and mirror neuron activity. Behavioural Brain Research, 183(2), 188–194.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2007.06.007

*Vainio, L., Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (2007). Precision and power grip priming by observed

grasping. Brain and Cognition, 65(2), 195–207.

*van Elk, M., van Schie, H. T., & Bekkering, H. (2011). Imitation of hand and tool actions is

effector-independent. Experimental Brain Research, 214(4), 539–547.

*van Leeuwen, M. L., van Baaren, R. B., Martin, D., Dijksterhuis, A., & Bekkering, H. (2009).

Executive functioning and imitation: Increasing working memory load facilitates

behavioural imitation. Neuropsychologia, 47(14), 3265–3270.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.005

*van Schie, H. T., van Waterschoot, B. M., & Bekkering, H. (2008). Understanding Action

Beyond Imitation: Reversed Compatibility Effects of Action Observation in Imitation

and Joint Action. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and

Performance, 34(6), 1493–1500.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 115

*Verrel, J., Lisofsky, N., & Kühn, S. (2014). Balancing cognitive control: How observed

movements influence motor performance in a task with balance constraints. Acta

Psychologica, 150, 129–135. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.05.010

*Verrel, J., Lisofsky, N., Kühn, S., & Lindenberger, U. (2016). Normal aging increases postural

preparation errors: Evidence from a two-choice response task with balance constraints.

Gait & Posture, 44, 143–148. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2015.12.002

Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting Meta-Analyses in R with the metafor Package. Journal

of Statistical Software, 36(3), 1–48.

Vigneswaran, G., Philipp, R., Lemon, R. N., & Kraskov, A. (2013). M1 corticospinal mirror

neurons and their role in movement suppression during action observation. Current

Biology, 23(3), 236–243. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.12.006

*Vlaskamp, B. N. S., & Schubo, A. (2012). Eye movements during action preparation.

Experimental Brain Research, 216(3), 463–472. http://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-

2949-8

*Vogt, S., Taylor, P., & Hopkins, B. (2003). Visuomotor priming by pictures of hand postures:

perspective matters. Neuropsychologia, 41(8), 941–951.

Wang, Y., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2012). Social top-down response modulation (STORM): a

model of the control of mimicry in social interaction. Frontiers In Human Neuroscience,

6, Article 153.

*Wang, Y., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2013). Understanding the Role of the “Self” in the Social

Priming of Mimicry. Plos One, 8(4), e60249.

*Wang, Y., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2014). Why does gaze enhance mimicry? Placing gaze-

mimicry effects in relation to other gaze phenomena. Quarterly Journal of

Experimental Psychology, 67(4), 747–762.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 116

*Wang, Y., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2015). Anterior medial prefrontal cortex implements

social priming of mimicry. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(4), 486–493.

*Wang, Y., Newport, R., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2011). Eye contact enhances mimicry of

intransitive hand movements. Biology Letters, 7(1), 7–10.

Want, S. C., & Gattis, M. (2005). Are “late-signing” deaf children “mindblind”?

Understanding goal directedness in imitation. Cognitive Development, 20(2), 159–172.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.12.003

Weeks, D. J., & Proctor, R. W. (1990). Salient-features coding in the translation between

orthogonal stimulus and response dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology-

General General, 119(4), 355–366. http://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.119.4.355

*Wiggett, A. J., Downing, P. E., & Tipper, S. P. (2013). Facilitation and interference in spatial

and body reference frames. Experimental Brain Research, 225(1), 119–131.

*Wiggett, A. J., Hudson, M., Tipper, S. S. P., & Downing, P. E. P. (2011). Learning associations

between action and perception: Effects of incompatible training on body part and

spatial priming. Brain and Cognition, 76(1), 87–96.

Wild, B., Erb, M., Eyb, M., Bartels, M., & Grodd, W. (2003). Why are smiles contagious? An

fMRI study of the interaction between perception of facial affect and facial

movements. Psychiatry Research-Neuroimaging, 123(1), 17–36.

http://doi.org/10.1016/S0925-4927(03)00006-4

Wilf, M., Holmes, N. P., Schwartz, I., & Makin, T. R. (2013). Dissociating between object

affordances and spatial compatibility effects using early response components.

Frontiers in Psychology, 4. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00591

Williams, J. H. G. (2008). Self-Other Relations in Social Development and Autism: Multiple

Roles for Mirror Neurons and Other Brain Bases. Autism Research, 1(2), 73–90.
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 117

Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror

neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 287–295.

Wohlschläger, A., Gattis, M., & Bekkering, H. (2003). Action generation and action

perception in imitation: an instance of the ideomotor principle. Philosophical

Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 358(1431),

501–15. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1257

Wong, A. L., Haith, A. M., & Krakauer, J. W. (2015). Motor Planning. The Neuroscientist,

21(4), 385–398. http://doi.org/10.1177/1073858414541484

Yabar, Y., Johnston, L., Miles, L., & Peace, V. (2006). Implicit behavioral mimicry:

Investigating the impact of group membership. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30(3),

97–113. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-006-0010-6

Zwetsloot, P.-P., Van Der Naald, M., Sena, E. S., Howells, D. W., IntHout, J., De Groot, J. A., …

Wever, K. E. (2017). Standardized mean differences cause funnel plot distortion in

publication bias assessments. eLife, 6, e24260. http://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.24260


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 118

Footnotes

1
Note that m does not correspond to the number of experiments as stated in figure 2

because between-subject groups were coded as different experiments for the statistical

analysis. The reasoning behind this is that effect sizes corresponding to different between-

subject groups are not statistically dependent.


2
It is noteworthy that RVE models use a simplistic method to estimate heterogeneity.

Consequently, the I² parameter should not be seen as an accurate representation of the

true heterogeneity but rather as a rough indicator (Tanner-Smith et al., 2016).


3
Studies using facial stimuli were coded as controlled in terms of spatial compatibility

because we considered faces too complex to be categorized on a simple spatial dimension.

Nevertheless, it could be argued that some facial expressions do have a clear spatial

component (e.g., smile vs. frown). To address this issue, we also performed the spatial

compatibility analysis without the studies using facial stimuli. In line with the main analysis,

this revealed that automatic imitation was weaker for both RTs, t(222.0) = -4.43, p < .001,

and ERs, t(85.2) = -3.53, p < .001, when simple spatial compatibility was controlled.
4
There is one additional ASD study that was published after data collection had

already been completed (Forbes et al., 2016). Although this study is not included in the

meta-analysis, we did explore whether the results of the ASD analysis changed if this study

was included but this was not the case.


5
The reason why we did not exclude studies on the basis of SOA is that such a

selection would necessarily be arbitrary. That is, it is difficult to determine an objective

criterion to differentiate between SOAs that are acceptable and SOAs that are too long. This

is further complicated by the fact that a cut-off criterion for SOA should take into account
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 119

whether the stimulus movement is presented as a video or not because a positive SOA may

coincide with the development of the stimulus movement when it is presented as a video

but always occurs after the stimulus movement if it is presented as a single frame or as a

sequence of two frames. Therefore, we decided to take into account SOA in a sensitivity

analysis rather than using it to exclude studies.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 120

Figures and Tables

Figure 1. Examples of the stimuli used in RT tasks of automatic imitation. In all three panels,

participants have to lift their right index finger when the number 1 is presented and their

right middle finger when the number 2 is presented. The first panel presents a case where

imitative compatibility is confounded with spatial compatibility. The second panel presents

a case where imitative compatibility is orthogonal to spatial compatibility. The third panel

presents a case where the average performance across both trials provides a measure of

imitative compatibility that is independent of spatial compatibility. See text for additional

detail.
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 121

Figure 2. The evolution of Google Scholar hits for “automatic imitation”. The y-axis displays

the percentage of hits relative to the total number of entries in Google Scholar for the

respective period. The absolute number of hits is displayed above each bar.
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 122

Figure 3. Flowchart of the screening process. The number of studies (n) and experiments (m)

that are included or excluded are denoted. See text for additional detail.
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 123

Table 1

Weighted Correlations Among Moderator Variables

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19
1. Spatial Compatibility –
**
2. Compatibility Type .20 –
3. Flexibility .14 .10 –
** *** *
4. Paradigm Type .18 .69 .20 –
***
5. SOA -.06 .20 .07 .02 –
* ***
6. Cue Location .14 .04 .05 .05 -.20 –
* ** ***
7. Cue Modality .11 .09 .05 .11 -.13 .42 –
*** *** ***
8. Action Goal .27 .39 .12 .08 .21 .11 .09 –
*** *** *** *
9. Bottom-Up Animacy .15 .15 .06 .27 .19 .31 .25 .09 –
***
10. Top-Down Animacy .03 .05 .04 .08 .04 .05 .03 .06 .23 –
** *** * *** **
11. Frames .09 .22 .12 .13 .26 .07 .14 .32 .17 .04 –
*** *** ** ***
12. Perspective .27 .33 .13 .20 -.09 .03 .08 .07 .09 .13 .27 –
*
13. Participant Gender .07 .02 .15 .01 .08 .05 .08 .04 .06 .09 .18 .07 –
* *** * ***
14. Model Gender .13 .14 .14 .11 .20 .09 .20 .33 NA .06 .11 .06 .12 –
*** ** ** ** *** **
15. Response Device .13 .22 .07 .05 .01 .06 .15 .22 .19 .03 .11 .31 .09 .23 –
**
16. ASD .03 .06 .07 .01 -.04 .02 .10 .08 .03 .04 .09 .03 .23 .13 .00 –
* ** ** *** *** ***
17. Age .00 -.10 .05 -.04 .07 -.01 -.13 .11 .06 .03 .15 -.03 .34 .08 .18 .27 –
*** * *** *** *** *** *** * *** ** *
18. Trials -.03 .20 .11 .01 .27 -27 -27 .09 .19 .02 .02 -.27 -.12 .25 -.12 -.03 -.09 –
*** *** ** *** ** *** * *** *** *** *** **
19. Effector .32 .67 .22 .36 .17 .20 .16 .58 .17 .03 .37 .46 .05 .16 .43 .12 .23 .17 –
Note. Relations between continuous-continuous variables and between continuous-dichotomous variables were assessed with Pearson’s r.

Relations between continuous-polytomous variables were assessed with multiple R. Relations between dichotomous-dichotomous,

dichotomous-polytomous, and polytomous-polytomous variables were assessed with Cramér’s V. Correlations that could not be calculated are

indicated with NA. We controlled for correlations > .40 (in bold) by analyzing both moderators together in the same model. ASD = autism

spectrum disorder. SOA = stimulus-onset asynchrony. * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 124

Figure 4. Funnel plot for RTs.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 125

Figure 5. Funnel plot for ERs.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 126

Figure 6. Funnel plot for KMs.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 127

Figure 7. P-curve on the flexibility of RT automatic imitation. See supplementary material 1


for the p-curve disclosure table.
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 128

Figure 8. The relation between stimulus-onset asynchrony and automatic imitation. Effect

sizes from the same experiment are represented with the same symbol in the same color.

The fit line of a RVE mixed effects model with stimulus-onset asynchrony as predictor is

shown.
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 129

Figure 9. P-curve representing the influence of bottom-up animacy on automatic imitation.

See supplementary material 2 for the p-curve disclosure table.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 130

Figure 10. P-curve representing the influence of top-down animacy on automatic imitation.

See supplementary material 3 for the p-curve disclosure table.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 131

Figure 11. Model gender x participant gender interaction. Values on the y-axis represent the

weighted effect size. The errors bars are standard errors.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 132

Figure 12. Forest plot for the relation between RT automatic imitation and cognitive

empathy. Published studies are annotated with “+” and unpublished studies with “-“.

Genschow et al. (2017) was included in the meta-analysis as an unpublished study but was

published later after data-analysis had already been completed.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 133

Figure 13. Forest plot for the relation between RT automatic imitation and emotional

empathy. Published studies are annotated with “+” and unpublished studies with “-“.

Genschow et al. (2017) was included in the meta-analysis as an unpublished study but was

published later after data-analysis had already been completed.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 134

Table 2

RT Moderator Statistics

Moderator β SE 95% CI m K F/t (df) p


Spatial Compatibility 4.15 (231.0) <.001
Controlled 0.91a 0.05 [0.82, 1.00] 126 258
Confounded 1.26b 0.07 [1.12, 1.40] 116 286
Compatibility Type 6.35 (227.1) <.001
Movement 0.80a 0.05 [0.70, 0.90] 112 353
Effector 1.30b 0.06 [1.18, 1.41] 134 193
Flexibility 2.76 (2, 17.8) .090
Compatible 1.26a 0.16 [0.91, 1.61] 13 16
No 1.09a,b 0.05 [1.00, 1.18] 221 510
Incompatible 0.69b 0.20 [0.26, 1.11] 17 20
Paradigm Type 7.31 (148.6) <.001
Simple 0.69a 0.06 [0.57, 0.80] 78 290
Choice 1.25b 0.05 [1.15, 1.35] 164 256
SOA
Between -0.34 0.05 [-0.44, -0.24] 231 527 -7.44 (13.9) <.001
Within -0.06 0.02 [-0.10, -0.01] 231 523 -2.55 (13.2) .024
Cue Location 3.81 (39.8) <.001
Off 0.74a 0.09 [0.55, 0.93] 31 170
On 1.13b 0.05 [1.04, 1.23] 211 373
Cue Modality 2.32 (5.3) .065
Auditory 0.82a 0.11 [0.55, 1.09] 6 21
Visual 1.08a 0.04 [1.00, 1.17] 236 525
Action Goal 11.19 (2, 33.5) <.001
No Goal 1.15a 0.05 [1.06, 1.25] 198 452
Communicative 0.87b 0.10 [0.66, 1.07] 16 37
Object-Directed 0.70b 0.09 [0.51, 0.88] 30 57
Bottom-up Animacy 26.90 (2, 23.2) <.001
Geometrical 0.40a 0.10 [0.20, 0.60] 25 117
Nonhuman 0.81b 0.10 [0.59, 1.03] 13 18
Human 1.12c 0.04 [1.04, 1.21] 238 409
Top-down Animacy (Nonhuman/Neutral Models) 0.60 (6.8) .566
No 0.56a 0.08 [0.40, 0.71] 38 118
Human 0.78a 0.39 [-0.17, 1.74] 7 18
Frames 3.52 (2, 44.9) .038
One 0.84a 0.10 [0.64, 1.05] 16 37
Two 1.15b 0.06 [1.03, 1.28] 101 166
Multiple 1.05a,b 0.07 [0.93, 1.18] 119 334
Perspective 1.53 (113.3) .130
First 1.00a 0.07 [0.86, 1.14] 68 128
Third 1.13a 0.05 [1.03, 1.24] 169 282
Participant Gender 0.39 (111.1) .695
Male 1.05a 0.08 [0.88, 1.21] 61 151
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 135

Moderator β SE 95% CI m K F/t (df) p


Female 1.09a 0.06 [0.97, 1.20] 142 346
Model Gender 6.75 (2, 64.8) .002
Male 0.87a 0.08 [0.71, 1.02] 52 93
Ambiguous 1.11b 0.06 [1.00, 1.22] 139 238
Female 1.43c 0.14 [1.14, 1.72] 25 35
Response Overlap 3.88 (69.9) <.001
No 0.99a 0.07 [0.85, 1.13] 97 287
Yes 1.46b 0.10 [1.26, 1.66] 41 66
Empathy
Cognitive 0.04 0.05 [-0.08, 0.16] 12 15 0.71 (9.6) .497
Emotional 0.05 0.03 [-0.02, 0.12] 12 15 1.49 (9.6) .168
Fantasy 0.05 0.04 [-0.04, 0.13] 11 14 1.15 (8.7) .279
Personal Distress 0.09 0.06 [-0.04, 0.22] 11 14 1.62 (8.5) .141
ASD 0.24 (9.7) .819
Control 1.08a 0.04 [0.99, 1.16] 232 532
ASD 1.11a 0.13 [0.82, 1.40] 10 14
Age 0.13 0.15 [-0.18, 0.44] 198 477 0.85 (34.7) .404
Trials -0.20 0.10 [-0.40, 0.00] 232 525 -2.01 (53.6) .050
Effector 4.00 (3, 13.8) .030
Finger 1.16a 0.06 [1.05, 1.28] 158 380
Face 1.11a,c 0.17 [0.70, 1.53] 7 12
Foot 0.96a,c 0.29 [0.21, 1.71] 6 8
Hand 0.87b,c 0.06 [0.76, 0.98] 72 138
Note. Different subscripts indicate p < .05. For non-continuous variables, the beta

coefficients correspond to Hedge’s g. For empathy, the beta coefficients correspond to

Fisher transformed correlation values. β = beta coefficient; SE = standard error; 95% CI =

95% confidence interval; m = number of experiments; k = number of effect sizes.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 136

Table 3

ER Moderator Statistics

Moderator β SE 95% CI m k F/t (df) p


Spatial Compatibility 3.51 (90.8) <.001
Controlled 0.55a 0.04 [0.46, 0.63] 47 77
Confounded 0.83b 0.07 [0.69, 0.96] 49 65
Compatibility Type 1.73 (31.3) .093
Movement 0.59a 0.05 [0.49, 0.69] 21 41
Effector 0.71a 0.05 [0.61, 0.82] 75 101
SOA
Between -0.15 0.04 [-0.24, -0.06] 92 136 -3.83 (7.0) .007
Within -0.02 0.03 [-0.09, 0.04] 92 136 -0.91 (4.9) .408
Cue Location 1.18 (21.4) .252
Off 0.59a 0.08 [0.42, 0.77] 16 31
On 0.71a 0.05 [0.61, 0.80] 80 109
Action Goal 4.89 (2, 12.4) .027
No Goal 0.73a 0.05 [0.63, 0.82] 80 108
Communicative 0.56a,b 0.06 [0.43, 0.70] 10 23
Object-Directed 0.44b 0.08 [0.25, 0.63] 8 11
Frames 3.54 (2, 16.9) .052
One 0.43a 0.10 [0.18, 0.68] 7 18
Two 0.76b 0.07 [0.63, 0.90] 43 65
Multiple 0.64a,b 0.06 [0.52, 0.75] 43 56
Perspective 1.24 (40.8) .220
First 0.62a 0.06 [0.50, 0.74] 26 48
Third 0.72a 0.05 [0.61, 0.83] 68 82
Participant Gender 2.20 (41.8) .033
Male 0.54a 0.07 [0.40, 0.68] 24 40
Female 0.74b 0.06 [0.62, 0.85] 56 85
Model Gender 3.79 (2, 28.4) .035
Male 0.58a 0.07 [0.43, 0.73] 28 43
Ambiguous 0.75a,b 0.06 [0.62, 0.88] 46 51
Female 0.89b 0.09 [0.69, 1.08] 11 15
Response Overlap 1.77 (43.5) .084
No 0.66a 0.09 [0.49, 0.84] 35 40
Yes 0.86a 0.07 [0.72, 1.01] 22 30
Age -0.43 0.26 [-0.98, 0.12] 77 116 -1.64 (16.1) .121
Trials -0.15 0.09 [-0.33, 0.03] 93 139 -1.70 (22.7) .103
Note. Different subscripts indicate p < .05. For non-continuous variables, the beta

coefficients correspond to Hedge’s g. β = beta coefficient; SE = standard error; 95% CI = 95%

confidence interval; m = number of experiments; k = number of effect sizes.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 137

Table 4

KM Moderator Statistics

Moderator β SE 95% CI M k F/t (df) p


Bottom-up Animacy 7.23 (12.5) .008
Geometrical 0.92a 0.13 [0.64, 1.21] 10 19
Nonhuman 0.24b 0.14 [-0.09, 0.57] 8 13
Human 0.77a 0.12 [0.51, 1.03] 21 36
Motion Profile -0.02 (7.5) .986
Non-biological 0.72a 0.19 [0.23, 1.21] 9 12
Biological 0.72a 0.11 [0.48, 0.95] 23 53
Note. Different subscripts indicate p < .05. Beta coefficients correspond to Hedge’s g. β =

beta coefficient; SE = standard error; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval; m = number of

experiments; k = number of effect sizes.


AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 138

Appendix A
Description of the Moderator Variables

Variable Description Options


1. Spatial Is spatial compatibility controlled or 1: controlled, 2: confounded
Compatibility confounded with imitative compatibility?
2. Compatibility Does the task manipulate movement 1: movement, 2: effector
Type compatibility or effector compatibility?
3. Flexibility Is there a compatible or incompatible action 1: no or non-contingent, 2: compatible,
context or training phase? 3: incompatible
4. Paradigm Type Do participants perform a simple response 1: simple, 2: choice
task or a forced choice task?
5. SOA What is the SOA between the imperative continuous
cue and the stimulus movement?
6. Cue Location What is the position of the cue with respect 1: on, 2: off
to the stimulus movement?
7. Cue Modality Is the imperative cue visual or auditory? 1: visual, 2: auditory
8. Action Goal Does the stimulus movement have a goal? If 1: no goal, 2: communicative, 3:
so, what kind of goal? object-directed
9. Bottom-Up Is the model human? If not, what type of 1: human, 2: nonhuman, 3:
Animacy nonhuman model is used? geometrical
10. Top-Down Is there a belief manipulation? If so, what 1: no, 2: human, 3: nonhuman
Animacy type of belief manipulation?
11. Motion Profile Is the motion profile of the stimulus 1: biological, 2: non-biological
movement biologically plausible?
12. Frames How many frames are used to display the 1: one, 2: two, 3: multiple
stimulus movement?
13. Perspective From what perspective is the model 1: first, 2: third
observed?
14. Participant What is the percentage of females in the continuous
Gender sample?
15. Model Gender What is the gender of the model? 1: ambiguous, 2: male, 3: female
16. Response Device Does the response device have keys? 1: keys, 2: no keys
17. Disorder Do the participants suffer from a clinical 1: no, 2: ASD, 3: other
disorder?
18. Age What is the mean age of the sample? continuous
19. Trials On how many trials (RT/ER) or seconds (KM) continuous
is the effect size based?
20. Effector What effector do participants use to 1: finger, 2: hand, 3: arm, 4: face, 5:
respond? foot
21. Lesion Do subjects suffer from a virtual or actual 1: no, 2: tdcs anodal, 3: tdcs cathodal,
brain lesion? 4: tms, 5: brain damage
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 139

Appendix B

Evidence Table for the Published Studies in the Meta-Analysis

Study Exp N gz Age Sample DV Animacy Motion Belief Persp Goal Paradigm Spatial Type
Brass et al. (2000) 1 8 1.58 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Brass et al. (2000) 2 8 2.60 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Stürmer et al. (2000) 1 10 0.84 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice CFD MOV
Stürmer et al. (2000) 2 8 1.18 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice CFD MOV
Stürmer et al. (2000) 3 7 1.70 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice CFD MOV
Stürmer et al. (2000) 5 8 0.72 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice CFD MOV
Stürmer et al. (2000) 6 10 1.04 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice CFD MOV
Brass, Bekkering, et al. 1 8 0.90 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
(2001)
Brass, Bekkering, et al. 2 10 0.85 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
(2001) GM NA
Brass, Bekkering, et al. 3 8 1.66 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple IND MOV
(2001)
Brass, Zysset, et al. 1 10 1.72 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
(2001)
Craighero et al. (2002) 1 12 0.59 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Simple CFD MOV
Craighero et al. (2002) 2 15 1.06 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Simple CFD MOV
Kilner et al. (2003) 1 8 0.83 Adult Control KM HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
NHM NBP
Vogt et al. (2003) 1 24 0.71 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First OBJ Simple CFD MOV
Third
Vogt et al. (2003) 2 23 1.07 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First OBJ Simple CFD MOV
Third
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 140

Study Exp N gz Age Sample DV Animacy Motion Belief Persp Goal Paradigm Spatial Type
Brass et al. (2005) 1 10 2.03 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Heyes et al. (2005) 1 10 1.41 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
Heyes et al. (2005) 2 20 1.03 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
Oztop et al. (2005) 1 8 0.27 Adult Control KM HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
NHM
Press et al. (2005) 1 16 1.10 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
NHM NA
Alegre et al. (2006) 1 7 1.24 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD MOV
Bertenthal et al. (2006) 1 12 1.66 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Bertenthal et al. (2006) 3b 12 0.78 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice IND EFF
Jackson et al. (2006) 1 8 -0.51 Adult Control KM HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
NHM NA
Press et al. (2006) 1 12 1.52 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
NHM
Press et al. (2006) 2 24 0.90 Adult Control RT HM BP HB First GL Simple ORT MOV
NHM NHB NA
Aicken et al. (2007) 1 8 1.35 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Choice CFD EFF
Aicken et al. (2007) 2 8 0.44 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Choice CFD EFF
Bouquet et al. (2007) 1 16 1.16 Adult Control KM HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
Bouquet et al. (2007) 2 12 0.79 Adult Control KM GM BP NB NA GL Simple CFD MOV
NBP
Bird et al. (2007) 1 32 1.53 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
ASD NHM
Jansson et al. (2007) 1 8 0.87 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple CFD MOV
NHM
Jansson et al. (2007) 2 16 0.57 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
GM NA
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 141

Study Exp N gz Age Sample DV Animacy Motion Belief Persp Goal Paradigm Spatial Type
Jansson et al. (2007) 3 8 1.46 Adult Control KM GM BP NB NA GL Simple CFD MOV
NBP
Kilner et al. (2007) 1 15 0.87 Adult Control KM HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
GM NBP NA
Press et al. (2007) 1 16 1.06 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
NHM NA
Poliakoff et al. (2007) 2 24 0.69 Senior Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
PD GM NA
Stanley et al. (2007) 1 10 1.13 Adult Control KM HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
GM NBP NA
Stanley et al. (2007) 2 20 0.81 Adult Control KM HM BP NHB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
GM NBP HB NA
Schroeter et al. (2007) 1 10 2.23 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Lesion
Vaino et al. (2007) 1 31 1.01 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First OBJ Choice IND MOV
Vaino et al. (2007) 2 25 0.83 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First OBJ Choice IND MOV
Vaino et al. (2007) 3 27 0.67 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First OBJ Choice IND MOV
Biermann-Ruben et al. 1 15 0.57 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
(2008) GM NA
Biermann-Ruben et al. 2 16 1.94 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
(2008) GM NA
Gowen et al. (2008) 1 18 0.98 Adult Control KM HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
ASD GM NBP NA
Gillmeister et al. (2008) 1 25 0.56 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice IND EFF
Third
Gillmeister et al. (2008) 2 32 0.89 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice IND EFF
Third
Jarick & Jones (2008) 1 29 1.45 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third COM Choice IND MOV
Longo et al. (2008) 1 24 0.62 Adult Control RT HM NBP NB Third GL Choice IND EFF
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 142

Study Exp N gz Age Sample DV Animacy Motion Belief Persp Goal Paradigm Spatial Type
Longo et al. (2008) 2 24 0.45 Adult Control RT HM NBP NB Third GL Choice IND EFF
Liepelt et al. (2008) 1 18 1.84 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice CFD EFF
Liepelt et al. (2008) 2 17 1.03 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice CFD EFF
Liepelt et al. (2008) 3 20 2.49 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice CFD EFF
Liepelt et al. (2008) 4 26 3.03 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice CFD EFF
Press et al. (2008) 1 16 0.78 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple IND MOV
Van Schie et al. (2008) 1 16 0.70 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Simple CFD MOV
Van Schie et al. (2008) 2 16 0.28 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Simple CFD MOV
Van Schie et al. (2008) 3 16 0.57 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Simple CFD MOV
Brass et al. (2009) 1 20 0.54 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Catmur et al. (2009) 1 8 1.06 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice IND EFF
TMS
Catmur et al. (2009) 2 8 1.34 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice IND EFF
Chong et al. (2009) 1 16 0.80 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Simple IND MOV
Chong et al. (2009) 2 16 0.65 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Simple IND MOV
Chong et al. (2009) 3 20 0.6 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Simple IND MOV
Jarick & Jones (2009) 1 42 0.86 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third COM Choice IND MOV
Kupferberg et al. (2009) 1 10 1.35 Adult Control KM HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
NHM
Longo & Bertenthal 1 120 0.59 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice IND EFF
(2009) NHB
Van Leeuwen et al. 1 48 1.61 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
(2009)
Poljac et al. (2009) 1 20 1.00 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Choice CFD MOV
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 143

Study Exp N gz Age Sample DV Animacy Motion Belief Persp Goal Paradigm Spatial Type
Richardson et al. (2009) 1 9 1.24 Adult Control KM HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
Spengler et al. (2009) 1 17 1.94 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
R. Cook et al. (2010) 1 24 2.50 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
R. Cook et al. (2010) 2 36 1.73 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
Gowen et al. (2010) 1 29 0.44 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
GM NA
Gowen et al. (2010) 2a 20 0.71 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
GM NA
Gowen et al. (2010) 2b 20 0.20 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
GM NA
Jonas et al. (2010) 2 16 1.38 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
GTS GM NA
Leighton et al. (2010) 1 36 0.83 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
Leighton & Heyes (2010) 1 12 2.15 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice CFD MOV
Third IND EFF
Leighton & Heyes (2010) 2 12 1.86 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice ORT MOV
Third IND EFF
Leighton & Heyes (2010) 3 12 0.81 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice ORT MOV
Third IND EFF
Liepelt & Brass (2010a) 1 20 2.82 Adult Control RT HM NBP NB Third GL Choice CFD MOV
Liepelt & Brass (2010a) 2 25 2.89 Adult Control RT HM NBP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Liepelt & Brass (2010b) 1 19 3.12 Adult Control RT NA BP HB First GL Choice CFD EFF
NHB
Liepelt et al. (2010) 1 27 0.68 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice IND EFF
NHM COM
OBJ
Liepelt et al. (2010) 2 32 0.95 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third COM Choice IND EFF
NHM OBJ
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 144

Study Exp N gz Age Sample DV Animacy Motion Belief Persp Goal Paradigm Spatial Type
Marshall et al. (2010) 1 25 0.75 Child Control KM HM BP NB Third OBJ Simple CFD MOV
Newman-Norlund et al. 1 16 0.55 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Choice CFD MOV
(2010) TMS
Ocampo & Kritikos 1 26 -0.16 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Simple CFD MOV
(2010)
Obhi & Hogeveen (2010) 1 16 0.36 Adult Control RT HM BP NB NA OBJ Choice IND MOV
Press, Gherri, et al. 1 12 1.25 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple CFD EFF
(2010) ORT MOV
Press, Gherri, et al. 2 12 1.39 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
(2010)
Press, Richardson, et al. 1 28 1.21 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third COM Simple IND MOV
(2010) ASD
Spengler, Bird, et al. 1 36 1.84 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
(2010) ASD
Spengler, Brass, et al. 1 20 2.48 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
(2010)
Spengler, Brass, et al. 2 19 1.58 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
(2010)
Spengler, von Cramon, et 1 28 2.67 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
al. (2010) Lesion
Bouquet et al. (2011) 1 24 0.75 Adult Control KM HM BP NB Third OBJ Simple CFD MOV
GL
Catmur & Heyes (2011) 1 16 0.86 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice IND EFF
Catmur & Heyes (2011) 2 8 1.56 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice IND EFF
Crescentini et al. (2011) 1 19 0.77 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
GM NA
J. L. Cook & Bird (2011) 1 45 1.02 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice ORT EFF
Child
Grecucci et al. (2011) 1 21 0.87 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 145

Study Exp N gz Age Sample DV Animacy Motion Belief Persp Goal Paradigm Spatial Type
Grecucci et al. (2011) 2 21 0.75 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
Hardwick & Edwards 1 8 0.95 Adult Control KM HM BP NB Third OBJ Simple CFD MOV
(2011)
Hogeveen & Obhi (2011) 1 12 1.45 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First OBJ Choice IND MOV
Ocampo et al. (2011) 1 12 -0.04 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Simple CFD MOV
Otte, Habel, et al. (2011) 1 16 1.98 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third COM Choice IND MOV
Otte, Habel, et al. (2011) 2 16 0.72 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third COM Simple IND MOV
Otte, Jost, et al. (2011) 1 14 0.62 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third COM Choice IND MOV
Shen et al. (2011) 1 24 0.57 Adult Control KM NHM NBP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
GM NA
Van Elk et al. (2011) 1 19 0.54 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Choice CFD MOV
Wiggett et al. (2011) 1 96 1.25 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice IND EFF
Wang et al. (2011) 1 20 1.37 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple ORT MOV
Wang et al. (2011) 2 23 1.09 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple ORT MOV
Boyer et al. (2012) 2 24 0.59 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice IND EFF
J. L. Cook & Bird (2012) 1 41 1.21 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice ORT EFF
ASD
R. Cook et al. (2012) 1 16 2.35 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
Hardwick et al. (2012) 1 12 0.25 Adult Control KM HM BP NB Third GL Simple IND MOV
Jiménez et al. (2012) 1 18 -0.19 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice IND EFF
Jiménez et al. (2012) 2 17 0.97 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice IND EFF
Third
Kupferberg et al. (2012) 1 43 0.67 Adult Control KM HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
NHM
Killingsworth (2012) 1 40 0.44 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple IND MOV
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 146

Study Exp N gz Age Sample DV Animacy Motion Belief Persp Goal Paradigm Spatial Type
Killingsworth (2012) 2 33 0.31 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple IND MOV
Killingsworth (2012) 3 23 0.26 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple IND MOV
Killingsworth (2012) 4 24 0.30 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple IND MOV
Killingsworth (2012) 5 24 0.94 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple IND MOV
Liepelt et al. (2012) 1 32 1.39 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Mengotti et al. (2012) 1 22 0.64 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice IND EFF
Romero et al. (2012) 1 12 1.19 Adult Control KM GM NBP NB NA GL Simple CFD MOV
Santiesteban, Banissy, et 1 15 0.97 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice ORT EFF
al. (2012) TDCS
Santiesteban, White, et 1 36 1.78 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice ORT EFF
al. (2012)
Silas et al. (2012) 1 22 1.90 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First OBJ Choice CFD MOV
Vlaskamp & Schübo 1b 8 0.81 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Choice CFD MOV
(2012)
Vlaskamp & Schübo 2 9 0.20 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third OBJ Choice CFD MOV
(2012)
Badets et al. (2013) 1 36 1.68 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice NA MOV
Bortoletto et al. (2013) 1 28 0.70 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First COM Simple IND MOV
Third
Chiavarino et al. (2013) 1 48 0.84 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice IND EFF
OBJ
Chiavarino et al. (2013) 2 40 1.16 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice IND EFF
OBJ
Cross & Iacoboni (2013) 1 24 1.55 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
GM NA
Cross et al. (2013) 1 20 1.32 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD EFF
GM NA
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 147

Study Exp N gz Age Sample DV Animacy Motion Belief Persp Goal Paradigm Spatial Type
Fine et al. (2013) 1 8 1.86 Adult Control KM HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
Grecucci et al. (2013) 1 30 0.77 Child Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
ASD
Haffey et al. (2013) 1 36 0.81 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
NHM
Hogeveen & Obhi (2013) 1 18 1.69 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice ORT EFF
Mengotti et al. (2013) 1 21 0.85 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice IND EFF
TMS
Shaw et al. (2013) 1 68 1.86 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice ORT MOV
Wiggett et al. (2013) 1 27 1.45 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice IND EFF
Wang & Hamilton (2013) 1 19 1.10 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Wang & Hamilton (2013) 2 32 1.56 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Wang & Hamilton (2013) 3 18 1.16 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Ainley et al. (2014) 1 43 2.41 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
J. L. Cook et al. (2014) 1 22 0.04 Adult Control KM HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
ASD NHM NBP
Cross & Iacoboni (2014a) 1 32 2.47 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
GM NA
Cross & Iacoboni (2014b) 1 10 2.15 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD MOV
De Coster et al. (2014) 1 24 1.05 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Oxytocin
Evans (2014) 1a 20 0.04 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
GM NA
Evans (2014) 1b 20 0.16 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
GM NA
Evans (2014) 2 20 0.16 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
GM NA
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 148

Study Exp N gz Age Sample DV Animacy Motion Belief Persp Goal Paradigm Spatial Type
Evans (2014) 3 24 0.19 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
GM NA
Evans (2014) 4 48 0.03 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
GM NA
Evans (2014) 5 23 0.00 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
GM HB NA
Evans (2014) 6 30 0.15 Adult Control RT GM BP NB NA GL Simple ORT MOV
HB
Evans (2014) 8 20 0.11 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
GM HB NA
Hogeveen et al. (2014) 1 16 1.33 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice ORT EFF
TDCS
Klapper et al. (2014) 1 19 2.45 Adult Control RT HM BP HB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
NHM NHB
Obhi et al. (2014) 1 24 3.41 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Verrel et al. (2014) 1 16 1.46 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Wang & Hamilton (2014) 1 20 1.83 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple ORT MOV
Wang & Hamilton (2014) 2 19 1.13 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple ORT MOV
Butler et al. (2015) 1 230 1.11 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Cracco et al. (2015) 1 38 1.29 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice IND EFF
Cracco et al. (2015) 2 40 1.51 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice IND EFF
NHM
de Guzman et al. (2015) 1 25 1.98 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice ORT EFF
de Guzman et al. (2015) 2 38 -0.08 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple ORT EFF
Rauchbauer et al. (2015) 1 27 2.75 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Roberts, Bennett, et al. 1 18 0.74 Adult Control KM HM BP NB Third GL Simple CFD MOV
(2015)
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 149

Study Exp N gz Age Sample DV Animacy Motion Belief Persp Goal Paradigm Spatial Type
Roberts, Hayes, et al. 1 17 0.97 Adult Control KM GM BP NB NA GL Simple CFD MOV
(2015)
Santiesteban, Bird, et al. 1 16 1.47 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice ORT EFF
(2015) MTS
Santiesteban, Banissy, et 1 14 0.44 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice ORT EFF
al. (2015) TDCS
Schunke et al. (2015) 3 40 1.41 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
ASD GM NA
Sowden & Catmur (2015) 1 16 0.31 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice IND EFF
TMS
Sowden et al. (2015) 1 105 1.06 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice ORT EFF
ASD
Sowden et al. (2015) 2 36 0.65 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Choice IND EFF
ASD
Wang & Hamilton (2015) 1 20 1.12 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Butler et al. (2016)* 1 31 2.45 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Butler et al. (2016)* 2 52 1.91 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Butler et al. (2016)* 3 27 2.41 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
*
Butler et al. (2016) 4 45 1.66 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Catmur (2016) 1 22 0.67 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Catmur (2016) 4 71 0.37 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Gowen et al. (2016) 1 18 -0.04 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Simple ORT MOV
GM HB NA
Gowen et al. (2016) 2 19 0.76 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First GL Simple ORT MOV
Rauchbauer et al. (2016)* 1 62 1.82 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Rauchbauer et al. (2016)* 2 61 1.80 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Rauchbauer et al. (2016)* 3 58 2.48 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
AUTOMATIC IMITATION: A META-ANALYSIS 150

Study Exp N gz Age Sample DV Animacy Motion Belief Persp Goal Paradigm Spatial Type
Verrel et al. (2016) 1 16 1.65 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
Senior
Deschrijver et al. (2017)* 1 45 1.46 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice CFD EFF
ASD
Genschow et al. (2017)* 1 199 1.37 Adult Control RT HM BP NB Third GL Choice ORT EFF
Cracco et al. (2018)* 1 40 0.99 Adult Control RT HM BP NB First COM Choice IND MOV
Note. The evidence table includes all published research that was used in the meta-analysis. Unpublished studies that were eventually published are also

included and are annotated with *. If a moderator was manipulated in a particular experiment, then all moderator levels that were present in the experiment

are listed. Participant groups that were not included in the meta-analysis are in italics. If this was reported, gz is the effect size corresponding to the main

effect of congruency. Otherwise, it is the mean of all effect sizes included in the meta-analysis. N: number of participants included in the meta-analysis; gz:

overall effect size; NA: not available or not applicable; Child: < 18 years old; Adult: 18-60 years old; Senior: > 60 years old; ASD: autism spectrum disorder; PD:

Parkinson disease; Lesion: brain lesion; GTS: Gilles de la Tourette syndrome; MTS: mirror-touch synaesthesia; tDCS: non-sham tDCS stimulation; TMS: non-

sham TMS stimulation; RT: response time task; KM: kinematics task; HM: human model; NHM: nonhuman model; GM: geometrical model; BP: biological

motion profile; NBP: non-biological motion profile; NB: no belief; HB: human belief; NHB: nonhuman belief; GL: goalless stimulus movement; OBJ: object-

directed stimulus movement; COM: communicative stimulus movement; Choice: forced choice task; Simple: simple response task; CFD: spatial compatibility

is confounded with imitative compatibility; ORTH: spatial compatibility is orthogonal to imitative compatibility; IND: spatial compatibility is independent of

imitative compatibility; EFF: effector compatibility paradigm; MOV: movement compatibility paradigm.

View publication stats

You might also like