The Transmission and Stability of Cultural Life Scripts

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

MEMORY, 2018

VOL. 26, NO. 1, 131–143


https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2017.1335327

The transmission and stability of cultural life scripts: a cross-cultural study


a,b
Steve M. J. Janssen and Shamsul Haquec
a
School of Psychology, University of Nottingham – Malaysia Campus, Semenyih, Malaysia; bSchool of Psychology, Flinders University,
Adelaide, Australia; cDepartment of Psychology, Jeffrey Cheah School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Monash University – Malaysia,
Bandar Sunway, Malaysia

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


Cultural life scripts are shared knowledge about the timing of important life events. In the Received 26 September 2016
present study, we examined whether cultural life scripts are transmitted through traditions Accepted 21 May 2017
and whether there are additional ways through which they can be attained by asking
KEYWORDS
Australian and Malaysian participants which information sources they had used to generate Autobiographical memory;
the life script of their culture. Participants hardly reported that they had used cultural and cultural life script; identity;
religious traditions. They more often reported that they had used their own experiences and social learning theory;
experiences of relatives and friends. They also reported the use of comments of relatives and
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:22 14 November 2017

vicarious memories
friends and the use of newspapers, books, movies and television programmes. Furthermore,
we examined the stability of life scripts and similarities and differences across cultures. We
found that life scripts are stable cognitive structures and that there are, besides cross-cultural
differences in the content, small cross-cultural differences in the valence and distribution of
life script events, with the Australian life script containing more positive events and more
events expected to occur before the age of 16.

Cultural life scripts are semantic knowledge about the idealised life course. Life scripts exist as shared cognitive
timing for important life events likely to occur in an ideal- structures influencing the retrieval of positive over nega-
ised life course (Berntsen & Rubin, 2002, 2004). It has been tive events and of events from early adulthood over
argued that this knowledge is used when people tell their those from other lifetime periods.
life story (Berntsen & Bohn, 2010; Bohn, 2010). Although 10 Berntsen and Rubin (2004) proposed 10 properties of
properties have been proposed, we will examine the prop- life scripts, six of which were derived from Schank and
erty that suggests that cultural life scripts are transmitted Abelson (1977), describing them as: (1) semantic knowl-
through traditions. Although there have been studies edge about expectations regarding; (2) a series of tem-
which have shown that life scripts are formed throughout porally ordered life events; (3) defined in terms of their
childhood (Bohn & Berntsen, 2008; Habermas, 2007), the time slots; (4) forming a hierarchical arrangement, with
present study is the first one – as far as we know – that transitional events forming a higher order (scene), in
has examined the transmission of life scripts. Besides which a series of subordinate actions or episodes are
examining the role of traditions, we will also explore if nested; (5) used to process life stories and (6) comprising
there are additional ways through which people can culturally important transitional events with culturally sanc-
learn about the life script of their culture and examine tioned timing. The remaining four properties were pro-
the stability of life scripts and similarities and differences posed by Berntsen and Rubin (2004): (7) life scripts do
in life scripts across two cultures (i.e., Australia and not represent an average but an idealised life, from
Malaysia). which many common and some important life events are
omitted; (8) life scripts are distorted from actual lives to
favour positive events and (9) events expected to occur
Cultural life scripts
in early adulthood; (10) and, because life scripts represent
Life scripts, which combine the concept of story scripts a normative life course, they are not based on personally
(Schank & Abelson, 1977) with that of age norms (Neugar- experienced events but transmitted through traditions.
ten, Moore, & Lowe, 1965), are defined as culturally shared Two methods have been used to examine life scripts (cf.,
representations of the order and timing of transitional Janssen & Haque, 2015). The first method requires partici-
events in a prototypical life course (Berntsen & Rubin, pants to estimate at what age highly emotional events,
2004). Cultural life scripts do not represent a list of remem- such as the happiest, saddest, most loving, most fearful,
bered experiences but events expected to occur in an most important and most traumatic event, might occur in

CONTACT Steve M. J. Janssen steve.janssen@nottingham.edu.my School of Psychology, University of Nottingham – Malaysia Campus, B1B21, Jalan
Broga, 43500 Semenyih, Selangor Darul Ehsan, Malaysia
© 2017 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
132 S. M. J. JANSSEN AND S. HAQUE

a prototypical life course (Berntsen & Rubin, 2004, Study 1; broad definition are the transmission of customs or beliefs
Haque & Hasking, 2010; Rubin & Berntsen, 2003, Study 2). from generation to generation. Using tradition in this
The second method asks participants to generate important context to explain the transmission of cultural life scripts
events – with no specific requests concerning emotional would be circular. A narrower definition for traditions is a
content – expected to occur in a prototypical life of a set of customs and rituals. This definition seems more
newborn child (Berntsen & Rubin, 2004, Study 2) or a cente- appropriate in the present context. These customs and
narian (Erdoğan, Baran, Avlar, Taş, & Tekcan, 2008). These rituals may have a religious connection and are then
events are subsequently categorised and rated on impor- regarded as religious. If they do not have a religious con-
tance, valence, prevalence and expected age at occurrence. nection, they are regarded as cultural. However, it is also
Studies on life scripts, mostly employing the second possible that they are attained in other ways. Social learn-
method, tend to investigate several properties simul- ing theory (Bandura, 1977) proposes three sources of infor-
taneously. The first property, indicating that life scripts mation from which people may learn about self-efficacy
are semantic knowledge, has been tested in four studies that could be applied to the transmission of life scripts.
(Bohn, 2010; Janssen & Rubin, 2011; Janssen, Uemiya, & People may learn which life events are regarded as
Naka, 2014; Tekcan, Kaya-Kızılöz, & Odaman, 2012), which highly important and at what ages these important
compared the proportions of life script events across events are supposed to occur by (a) personal experiences
young, middle-aged, and older adults in Denmark, the (called performance accomplishments in social learning
Netherlands, Japan and Turkey. Whereas two studies theory), (b) the observation of the lives of relatives and
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:22 14 November 2017

reported minor differences in the content of life scripts peers (called vicarious experiences) and (c) comments and
across age groups (Bohn, 2010; Tekcan et al., 2012), two suggestions from relatives and peers (called verbal persua-
studies with larger samples found no differences (Janssen sion). Furthermore, Janssen et al. (2014) speculated that
& Rubin, 2011; Janssen et al., 2014). The results of the reading books and watching television and movies about
latter studies indicate that life scripts are semantic knowl- other people’s lives may also influence which life events
edge, because, if they were not, the events would be are considered important and prevalent. Finally, laws and
increasingly person specific and thus diversified. regulations of a country may inform life scripts too. Most
Other properties have been investigated as well. Prop- countries have laws that stipulate from what age people
erty 7, indicating that many common and some important can obtain their driver’s licence or at what age they can
events are left out of the life script, has received significant retire.
support (Berntsen & Rubin, 2004, Study 2; Erdoğan et al., We examined how life scripts are transmitted by asking
2008; Janssen et al., 2014; Janssen & Rubin, 2011; Rubin, participants first to consider a newborn baby with a
Berntsen, & Hutson, 2009). Property 8, suggesting that life typical future ahead and list seven of the infant’s most
scripts contain more positive than negative events, and important events, with accompanying categorisation,
Property 9, indicating that the majority of events in the age estimation, and importance, valence and prevalence
life script occur in the bump period, have also received ratings for each event. After completing the standard
widespread support too (Berntsen & Rubin, 2004, Study life script questionnaire, we asked participants to
2; Bohn, 2010; Ece & Gülgöz, 2014; Erdoğan et al., 2008; provide the sources that they had used to generate the
Janssen et al., 2014; Janssen & Rubin, 2011; Ottsen & Bernt- seven events, determine their importance, and estimate
sen, 2014; Rubin et al., 2009). their expected age at occurrence. However, people may
not be fully aware of how they learned this semantic
knowledge. After answering the three open-ended ques-
Transmission of life scripts
tions about the sources on which they had based their
Receiving almost no attention is Property 10, which pro- answers, we therefore gave participants the seven
poses that life scripts are transmitted by tradition. Accord- events back one-by-one. For each event, we asked them
ing to the life script framework, nearly everyone in a given how they had determined the event’s importance and
society knows at what age a prototypical baby, with still its expected age at occurrence. Each question was followed
entire life ahead of it, is supposed to join high school, leave by a list with 10 information sources. By offering the list
home for higher education, fall in love, complete gradu- with possible information sources, we hoped to increase
ation, start first job, get married and have children. This their awareness of the range of sources.
knowledge is semantic and shared by almost everyone We measured which information options were selected
(Berntsen & Rubin, 2004), including adolescents and most frequently. We expected that participants would indi-
young adults (Bohn & Berntsen, 2008). Because adolescents cate that they had learned about life scripts, besides
and young adults have not personally experienced most through cultural and religious traditions as had been pre-
life script events, it is unclear how they have obtained dicted by Berntsen and Rubin (2004), also through
this knowledge. The first goal of the current study was reading about other people’s lives in newspapers and
therefore to examine this proposition. books and watching television programmes and movies
Berntsen and Rubin (2004) suggested that life scripts are as had been speculated by Janssen et al. (2014). Partici-
transmitted through traditions. Traditions when given their pants were also expected to have learned about life
MEMORY 133

scripts from observing relatives and friends and from lis- Stability of life scripts
tening to the comments of relatives and friends as was pre-
The current study’s third goal was to examine the stability
dicted from social learning theory (Bandura, 1977).
of life scripts. In cross-cultural comparisons, responses on
life script questionnaires are aggregated into a single list
Similarities and differences across cultures of event categories and their proportion of mentions. To
examine whether two life scripts are different, one can
The second goal of the current study was to examine
examine which events reached the 4% threshold in one
whether there are similarities and differences in the life
group but not the other. Alternatively, one can examine
scripts across cultures. Several earlier studies have directly
which events were among the 10 most frequently men-
compared life scripts produced by participants with differ-
tioned events in one group but not the other. There are
ent cultural backgrounds. Janssen et al. (2014) compared
also more inferential approaches available. By calculating
Japanese and Dutch life scripts with a particular focus on
the correlation of the proportion of mentions, one can
the temporal distribution of positive, negative and
also examine whether two life scripts are different. If the
neutral events and the event content. The temporal distri-
groups mention the same set of events frequently, then
butions of the positive, neutral and negative events were
the correlation will be high. Besides correlations, one can
similar across the two samples. More positive than negative
also conduct chi-square tests to see whether the events
events were mentioned, with positive events mainly
are mentioned more often in one group.
expected to occur between 15 and 30 years of age and
However, like any measure, there is bound to be some
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:22 14 November 2017

the expected ages at occurrence of negative events


noise in the list of events and each event’s proportion of
equally distributed over the entire lifespan. While investi-
mentions. The amount of noise, or stability, of the life
gating the event content, it was found that 25 events
script questionnaire is not known, making it difficult to
were common to both groups. Some events, such as the
determine whether disparities in life scripts are due to
high-school entrance exam, Seijinshiki ceremony and
noise in the measurement or to genuine cultural differ-
Shichi-go-san festival, were present in the Japanese but
ences. To test the stability of life scripts, we compared
absent in the Dutch life script, whereas other events,
the results of the Australian participants of the present
such as leaving home, the first sexual experience and
study to the results of a previous study with Australian par-
death of grandparents, were part of the Dutch life script
ticipants (i.e., Janssen, 2015). Both samples consisted of
but not the Japanese. Rubin et al. (2009) compared
participants who were mostly first-year psychology stu-
Danish and American undergraduates, reporting similar
dents. Because we hypothesised that life scripts would
life scripts with minor but reasonable differences in
be stable, we predicted no substantial differences in the
regards to the types of events included. A positivity bias
content of life scripts and similar distributions of the
and a bias for events estimated to happen between 10
expected ages at occurrence and the positive, neutral
and 30 years of age was observed in both groups. Scher-
and negative events. The observed differences can be
man (2013) reanalysed data from 5 studies and demon-
used as a benchmark in cross-cultural comparisons. If the
strated that life scripts from all included cultures
differences between Malaysian and Australian participants
contained a greater allowance of positive events occurring
are similar to the differences between the two groups of
between the ages of 15 and 30 years, creating a distri-
Australian participants, then they should be attributed to
bution for life script events similar to the distribution
noise. If the differences are larger, then they can be attrib-
obtained from life story memories.
uted to cultural factors.
The present study compared the content, the distri-
bution of positive, neutral and negative events, and the dis-
tribution of the expected age at occurrence from Australian
and Malaysian life scripts. In line with the literature (cf., Method
Janssen & Haque, 2015), participants from both countries
Participants
were expected to mention a high number of positive
events that occur mainly in early adulthood and a There were two groups of participants in this study. We
reduced number of negative events that are equally dis- tested 124 Malaysian students who were enrolled at the
tributed across ages, resulting in the standard deviations Malaysian campus of Monash University. The results of
of expected age at occurrence of positive events being three students were omitted from the analyses (because
smaller than those of negative events. However, there the participant had entered events from their own life or
may be substantial differences in the content of the because the results were double entries), leaving the
events between both countries as their cultural fabrics results of 121 participants. The final group consisted of
are unique. Whereas Australia is a developed nation and 85 female and 36 male participants, and the mean age of
highly individualistic, Malaysia is a developing nation and the group was 18.99 (SD = 1.08, range = 15–25).
collectivistic (Hofstede, 2001), and thus their cultural We also tested 152 Australian students from Flinders
expectations concerning the content of life events were University. The results of 5 students were omitted
expected to be different. (because the participants had only included early
134 S. M. J. JANSSEN AND S. HAQUE

childhood events or because the results were double which information sources they had used to generate the
entries), leaving the results of 147 participants. The final seven events that will most likely occur in the prototypical
group consisted of 121 female and 26 male participants, person’s life, which information sources they had used to
and the mean age of the group was 22.68 (SD = 7.98, rate how important these events were, and which infor-
range = 17–55). mation sources they had used to estimate at what ages
Although the Australian participants were older (t(266) these events would occur.
= 5.04, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.593) and the Australian Subsequently, participants were again given the
sample consisted of more female participants (χ 2(1) = descriptions of the seven events back one-by-one. For
5.43, p = .020, Cramer’s V = 0.142), cultural life scripts are each event, participants had to answer two questions.
known for not being affected by age (Janssen & Rubin, We asked them which information sources they had used
2011) or gender (Janssen et al., 2014). to decide upon the importance of the event and the age
at which the event was expected to occur. Participants
could select multiple options among 10 information
Materials and procedure sources and one open-ended option, in which they could
After providing informed consent and their background enter their information source if it would not be among
information, participants were asked to imagine an ordin- the 10 options (also see Tables 1 and 2).
ary infant that had the same gender and cultural back- When participants had reported the information
ground as themselves. The infant could not be a specific sources they had used to decide upon the events’ impor-
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:22 14 November 2017

child they knew, but the child had to be a prototypical tance and expected age of occurrence, the questionnaire
infant from Malaysia (or Australia) with an ordinary life ended. They were then thanked for their participation
course ahead. Participants were then asked to describe and briefly explained the purpose of the study.
briefly the seven most important events that are most
likely to occur in this prototypical infant’s life. Results
When participants had entered the seven events, they
were given the descriptions back one-by-one. They were The results section consists of five parts. We examine first
asked to categorise the event, estimate its prevalence, whether the Malaysian sample contains a cultural life
rate its importance, estimate at what age the event most script and then whether there is a cultural life script in
likely will occur and rate its valence. While categorising the Australian sample. Subsequently, to examine the stab-
the events, participants could choose from 54 categories ility of life scripts, the results of the Australian sample are
that were taken from previous studies. The categories compared to the results of an Australian sample collected
were presented in alphabetic order (and not in order of in a previous study. The results of the Australian sample
expected age at occurrence). When there was no category are also compared to the results of the Malaysian sample
that suited the event, participants were instructed to select to examine the influence of culture on life scripts. Finally,
the “other” option, which was presented as the last option. we will test whether cultural life scripts are transmitted
Prevalence was rated on a scale that ranged from 0 to 100 through traditions and explore if there are additional
(out of 100) people, whereas expected age at occurrence ways through which cultural life scripts can be attained.
was rated on a scale ranging from 0 to 100 years. Impor-
tance and valence were rated on 7-point scales that
Malaysian life script
ranged from (1) “unimportant” to (7) “of greatest impor-
tance” and from (−3) “very negative” to (3) “very positive”. We first examined whether there was a cultural life script in
After answering the life script questionnaire, partici- the Malaysian sample. These participants were asked to
pants were asked to answer three open-ended questions name the seven most important events that will most
about the transmission of life scripts. We asked them likely take place in the life of an ordinary Malaysian

Table 1. The proportion of participants who selected the source or the kind of information on which they based their
decision regarding the importance of the event (multiple answers possible).
Information source Prop
I have personally experienced this event and I found that it was an important event 0.521
Comments that members of my family have made suggest to me that this event is important 0.491
Members of my family have experienced this event and it has changed their lives 0.533
Comments that my friends have made suggest to me that this event is important 0.368
Friends have experienced this event and it has changed their lives 0.392
Cultural traditions related to this event indicate to me that this event is important 0.260
Religious traditions related to this event indicate to me that this event is important 0.128
Laws and government policies related to this event indicate to me that this event is important 0.149
When reading about other people’s lives in newspapers and books, this event seemed important 0.386
When watching a television programme or a movie about another person’s life, this event seemed important 0.426
Other, namely … 0.007
Total 3.662
MEMORY 135

Table 2. The proportion of participants who selected the source or the kind of information on which they based their decision regarding the expected age at
occurrence of the event (multiple answers possible).
Information source Prop
I have personally experienced this event and it occurred to me when I was that age 0.468
Comments that members of my family have made suggest to me that this event is supposed to occur at that age 0.411
Members of my family have experienced this event and it occurred when they were that age 0.513
Comments that my friends have made suggest to me that this event is supposed to occur at that age 0.294
Friends have experienced this event and it occurred when they were that age 0.372
Cultural traditions related to this event indicate to me at what age this event is supposed to occur 0.183
Religious traditions related to this event indicate to me at what age this event is supposed to occur 0.073
Laws and government policies related to this event indicate to me at what age this event is supposed to occur 0.135
When reading about other people’s lives in newspapers and books, this event occurred when they were that age 0.335
When watching a television programme or a movie about another person’s life, this event occurred when they were that age 0.367
Other, namely … 0.029
Total 3.179

person. The mentioned events were accordingly rated as SD = 13.9) than negative categories (M = 76.8, SD = 16.2),
highly important (M = 5.80, SD = 1.24) and highly prevalent p = .891. The standard deviations of the age at which the
(M = 76.7, SD = 21.0). The participants were also asked to positive events were expected to occur (M = 4.81, SD =
rate the events on valence and to indicate at what age 5.40) were smaller than the standard deviations of the
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:22 14 November 2017

they expected the events to occur. The mentioned age at which the negative events were expected to occur
events were predominantly rated as positive, χ 2(2) = (M = 9.86, SD = 5.93), t(32) = 2.05, p = .049, Cohen’s d =
672.05, p < .001, Cramer’s V = 0.630, with 75.2% given posi- 1.121. Although this last difference was significant, these
tive, 9.7% given neutral and 15.1% given negative ratings. results need to be treated with caution due to the low
The expected ages at occurrence of positive, neutral and number of negative categories.
negative events were not distributed equally across the
lifespan (see top panel of Figure 1), χ 2(4) = 131.58, p
Australian life script
< .001, Cramer’s V = 0.394. About a quarter of the positive
events was expected to occur before the age of 16 We then examined whether the Australian sample con-
(27.2%), more than half of the positive events between tained a cultural life script. Similar to the Malaysian partici-
the ages of 16 and 30 (61.2%), and the remaining positive pants, the Australian participants were asked to name the
events after the age of 30 (11.6%). These three proportions seven most important events that will most likely take
were more equal for the neutral (30.5%, 42.7% and 26.8%) place in the life of an ordinary person from their culture.
and negative events (16.4%, 28.9% and 54.7%). Accordingly, the events were rated as highly important
To test whether the expected ages at occurrence of (M = 5.91, SD = 1.07) and highly prevalent (M = 81.6, SD =
positive event categories are smaller than those of nega- 19.7). The participants were also asked to rate the events
tive event categories, the Malaysian participants cate- on valence and to indicate at what age they expected
gorised the events. The number and proportion of the events to occur. The events were predominantly
mentions and the mean prevalence, importance, expected rated as positive, χ 2(2) = 1178.08, p < .001, Cramer’s V =
age at occurrence and valence per event category are 0.757, with 83.8% given positive, 8.6% given neutral and
given in the Appendix (Table A1). The participants classi- 7.7% given negative ratings. Again, the expected ages at
fied 8.5% of the total number of events as “other”. One occurrence of positive, neutral and negative events were
new category could be established from the descriptions not distributed equally across the lifespan (see bottom
of these “other” events (i.e., “first birthday”). Categories panel of Figure 1), χ 2(4) = 70.29, p < .001, Cramer’s V =
that were mentioned by at least 4% of the participants 0.185. About a third of the positive events was expected
(cf., Berntsen & Rubin, 2004) were regarded as part of the to occur before the age of 16 (31.3%), more than half of
Malaysian life script. The new category (4.1%) and 34 the positive events between the ages of 16 and 30
other categories passed the threshold. (60.7%), and the remaining positive events after the age
Under the assumption that the event categories are of 30 (8.0%). These three proportions were more equal
independent units, the majority of the categories in the for the neutral (51.1%, 31.8% and 17.0%) and negative
Malaysian life script were positive (N = 28), χ 2(2) = 35.37, events (43.0%, 27.8% and 29.1%).
p < .001, Cramer’s V = 0.711. Six categories were negative To test whether the expected ages at occurrence of
(i.e., death parents, own death, serious illness, first rejec- positive event categories are smaller than those of nega-
tion, accident, and death sibling or friend), whereas one tive event categories, the Australian participants cate-
category was considered as neither positive nor negative gorised the events. The number and proportion of
(i.e., first sexual experience). Positive categories were not mentions and the mean prevalence, importance, expected
rated as more important (M = 5.76, SD = 0.50) than nega- age at occurrence and valence per event category are
tive categories (M = 5.41, SD = 0.55), p = .143, and positive given in the Appendix (Table A2). The participants classi-
categories were not rated as more prevalent (M = 77.7, fied 6.6% of the total number of events as “other”. Three
136 S. M. J. JANSSEN AND S. HAQUE
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:22 14 November 2017

Figure 1. The distribution of the expected age at occurrence of positive (black bars), neutral (white bars) and negative events (grey bars) named by Malaysian
(top panel) and Australian (bottom panel) participants.

new categories (i.e., “learning to cycle or swim”, “learning to (31) = 2.55, p = .016, Cohen’s d = 1.436. Again, although
read and write” and “first tooth”) could be established from this difference was significant, these results need to be
the “other” descriptions. Thirty-five categories were men- treated with caution due to the low number of negative
tioned by at least 4% of the participants and were regarded categories.
as part of the Australian life script. “Learning to cycle or
swim” passed the threshold (5.4%), but the other two
new categories did not (2.7% and 2.0%, respectively).
Stability of life scripts
Under the assumption that the event categories are
independent units, the majority of the categories in the To examine the stability of life scripts, the results of the
Australian life script were positive (N = 30), χ 2(2) = 42.54, Australian sample were compared to the results of a pre-
p < .001, Cramer’s V = 0.758. Only three categories were vious study (Janssen, 2015), in which 107 Australian partici-
negative (i.e., death grandparents, death parents and first pants (M age = 18.87, SD age = 2.01, range 16–28 years,
rejection), whereas two categories were considered as 83.2% female) had answered a life script questionnaire
neither positive nor negative (i.e., own death and adoles- that was identical to the first part of the questionnaire in
cence). Positive categories were not rated as more impor- the present study. Although the participants from the pre-
tant (M = 5.95, SD = 0.44) than negative categories (M = vious study were younger (t(252) = 4.83, p < .001, Cohen’s
6.06, SD = 0.33), p = .678, and positive categories were d = 0.588), cultural life scripts are known for not being
not rated as more prevalent (M = 82.3, SD = 13.9) than affected by age (Janssen & Rubin, 2011). The two
negative categories (M = 94.2, SD = 5.2), p = .158. The stan- samples did not differ on the proportion of female and
dard deviations of the age at which the positive events male participants, p = .857. We compared the content of
were expected to occur (M = 2.77, SD = 3.27) were smaller the life scripts, the distribution of the expected age at
than the standard deviations of the age at which the nega- occurrence and the distribution of positive, neutral and
tive events were expected to occur (M = 7.93, SD = 4.24), t negative events.
MEMORY 137

There was considerable overlap in the content of the analyses reported small differences (9.4%): death grand-
two Australian life scripts. Of the 35 event categories of parents, χ 2(1) = 5.00, p = .025, Cramer’s V = 0.140, high-
the Australian life script in the present study, 29 event cat- school graduation, χ 2(1) = 10.49, p = .001, Cramer’s V =
egories (82.9%) also appeared in the Australian life script of 0.203, learning to cycle or swim, χ 2(1) = 6.01, p = .014,
Janssen (2015). The six categories that did not reach the Cramer’s V = 0.154, marriage, χ 2(1) = 8.47, p = .004,
threshold in the previous study were: death grandparents Cramer’s V = 0.183 and 21st birthday, χ 2(1) = 4.87, p = .027,
(11.6% vs. 3.7%), peer group (7.5% vs. 2.8%), first sexual Cramer’s V = 0.138. When a Bonferroni correction was
experience (6.8% vs. 3.7%), major achievement (5.4% vs. applied to these analyses, none of the differences was
3.7%), learning to cycle or swim (5.4% vs. 0.0%) and first significant.
rejection (4.8% vs. 3.7%). Furthermore, there were four cat- The distributions of the expected age at occurrence in
egories that reached the threshold in Janssen (2015) but the present sample and the sample of Janssen (2015)
not in the present study: end primary school (3.4% vs. were also similar (see Figure 3), χ 2(2) = 4.09, p = .130,
6.5%), 21st birthday (1.4% vs. 6.5%), birth younger sibling Cramer’s V = 0.048. In both samples, about a third of the
(2.7% vs. 5.6%) and death sibling or friend (3.4% vs. events were expected to occur before the age of 16
5.6%). When we narrowed the comparison to the 10 (33.9% vs. 30.2%), more than half the events between the
most mentioned categories, there was still much overlap ages of 16 and 30 (55.7% vs. 60.5%), and about a 10th of
in the content of the two Australian life scripts, with the events after the age of 30 (10.4% vs. 9.3%). The distri-
eight categories being the same. The 2 events of the butions of positive, neutral and negative events in both
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:22 14 November 2017

present study that were not among the 10 most men- samples were similar as well, χ 2(2) = 1.99, p = .369,
tioned categories in Janssen (2015) were: making a long Cramer’s V = 0.033. More than 80% of the events were
journey (18.4% vs. 14.0%) and retirement (18.4% vs. rated as positive (83.8% vs. 82.6%), about a tenth of the
10.3%), whereas the 2 events of Janssen (2015) that were events neutral (8.6% vs. 10.4%), and less than a 10th of
not among the 10 most mentioned categories in the the events negative (7.7% vs. 6.9%).
present study were: Begin university (17.0% vs. 27.1%) The comparison of the Australian samples suggests that
and 18th birthday (15.0% vs. 19.6%). life scripts are stable. There was much overlap between
The findings of the content analyses were supported by two samples regarding the content. Furthermore, the dis-
the extremely high correlation between the two samples tributions of the expected age at occurrence and the distri-
for the proportion of mentions per event category (see butions of positive, neutral and negative events were
Figure 2), r(53) = 0.960, p < .001. In the figure, categories similar across the two samples.
close to the diagonal line represent events that had
similar proportions of mentions in both samples. Further-
Similarities and differences across cultures
more, the findings of the content analyses were also sup-
ported by comparisons of the number of mentions for To examine the influence of culture on life scripts, the
each event category. We conducted 53 chi-square tests, results of the Malaysian sample were compared to the
and most analyses did not yield significant results (p > .05), results of the Australian sample. Again, we compared the
suggesting that most proportions were similar. Only five content of the life scripts, the distribution of the expected
age at occurrence and the distribution of positive, neutral
and negative events.
There was considerable overlap in the content of the
two life scripts. Of the 35 event categories of the Malaysian
life script in the present study, 29 event categories (82.9%)
also appeared in the Australian life script. The six categories
that did not reach the threshold in the Australian sample
were: serious illness (13.2% vs. 0.0%), children leave
home (9.1% vs. 1.4%), important promotion at work
(9.1% vs. 2.7%), accident (5.0% vs. 0.7%), death sibling or
friend (4.1% vs. 3.4%) and end primary school (4.1% vs.
3.4%). Furthermore, there were six categories that
reached the threshold in the Australian sample but not in
the Malaysian sample: 18th birthday (1.7% vs. 15.0%), first
part-time job (0.8% vs. 14.3%), death grandparents (1.7%
vs. 11.6%), own birth (3.3% vs. 7.5%), learning to swim or
cycle (0.0% vs. 5.4%) and family holiday (0.0% vs. 4.7%).
When we narrowed the comparison to the 10 most men-
tioned categories, there was some overlap in the content
Figure 2. Comparison of the proportion of mentions per event category for of the 2 life scripts. Six categories were the same. The 4
the Australian participants from the present study and from Janssen (2015). events of the Malaysian sample that were not among the
138 S. M. J. JANSSEN AND S. HAQUE

Figure 3. Comparison of the distributions of the expected age at occurrence of the Australian samples from the present study and from Janssen (2015).
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:22 14 November 2017

10 most mentioned categories in the Australian sample yielded significant differences when we compared the
were: Begin university (29.8% vs. 17.0%), death parents Malaysian and Australian samples. When Bonferroni correc-
(25.6% vs. 7.5%), begin childcare (20.7% vs. 10.9%) and tions were applied, eight differences were still significant
own death (17.4% vs. 9.5%), whereas the 4 events of the for the cross-cultural comparison whereas there were no
Australian sample that were not among the 10 most men- differences for the comparison of the two Australian
tioned categories in the Malaysian sample were: high- samples.
school graduation (15.7% vs. 44.9%), begin high school The distributions of the expected age at occurrence of
(5.8% vs. 28.6%), making a long journey (8.3% vs. 18.4%) the Malaysian and Australian samples were not similar
and retirement (9.1% vs. 18.4%). (see Figure 5), χ 2(2) = 37.10, p < .001, Cramer’s V = 0.141.
We then calculated the correlation for the proportion of There were more events expected to occur before the
mentions per event category and compared the number of age of 16 in the Australian sample (25.9% vs. 33.9%), a
mentions of each event category. Although the correlation similar proportion of events expected to occur between
was strong (see Figure 4), r(54) = 0.775, p < .001, it was the ages of 16 and 30 (54.5% vs. 55.7%), and more
smaller than the corresponding correlation between the events expected to occur after the age of 30 in the Malay-
two Australian samples, Z = 4.53, p < .001. Furthermore, sian sample (19.6% vs. 10.4%). The distributions of positive,
whereas only 5 of the 53 chi-square tests (9.4%) yielded sig- neutral and negative events in the Malaysian and Austra-
nificant differences (p < .05) when we compared the lian sample were also not similar, χ 2(2) = 28.19, p < .001,
number of mentions per event category of the 2 Australian Cramer’s V = 0.123. There were more positive events in
samples, nearly half of the 54 chi-square tests (46.3%) the Australian sample (75.2% vs. 83.8%), a similar pro-
portion of neutral events (9.7% vs. 8.6%), and more nega-
tive events in the Malaysian sample (15.1% vs. 7.7%).
The comparison of the Australian and Malaysian
samples suggests that, although there is much overlap
regarding the content when one looks at descriptive
measures, there are more differences when one looks
at inferential measures. Furthermore, although both
samples displayed the hallmarks of life scripts (i.e., positiv-
ity bias and bias for events from early adulthood), there
were significant differences in the valence and temporal
distribution.

Transmission of life scripts


After answering the life script questionnaire, participants
indicated which information sources they had used to
answer the questionnaire. Both samples selected for each
of the seven mentioned events what kinds of information
Figure 4. Comparison of the proportion of mentions per event category for they had used to decide upon the importance of the
the Malaysian and the Australian participants. events (Table 1) and the age at which the events were
MEMORY 139

Figure 5. Comparison of the distributions of the expected age at occurrence of the Malaysian and the Australian sample.
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:22 14 November 2017

expected to occur (Table 2). The open-ended options were Due to the high number of observations (n = 1876), many
hardly used to answer these two questions (0.7% and correlations reached significance. However, correlations
2.9%). Because the responses to this option included state- were only for personal experiences higher than 0.200. For
ments that it was “an educated guess”, “general knowl- both questions (i.e., importance and expected age at occur-
edge”, “common sense”, “statistics” or “social trends”, no rence), participants more often reported using personal
additional information sources could be established. experiences when the event was rated as highly prevalent,
Participants used more information sources to rate how r(1876) = 0.225, p < .001 and r(1876) = 0.245, p < .001, and
important the events were (M = 3.66, SD = 2.53) than they when the event was expected to occur at an early age,
used to estimate when the events were expected to r(1876) = −0.512, p < .001 and r(1876) = −0.513, p < .001.
occur (M = 3.18, SD = 2.34), t(1875) = 12.14, p < .001, Six correlations were between 0.200 and 0.100, and all
Cohen’s d = 0.280. Furthermore, although the Australian remaining correlations were lower than 0.100.
participants (M = 4.01, SD = 2.73) tended to use more infor- Before participants had been given the options, they
mation sources than the Malaysian participants (M = 3.23, had been asked which information sources had been
SD = 2.19) to decide upon the events’ importance, used to generate the seven events, rate how important
t(1874) = 6.72, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.308, they did not these events were, and estimate at what ages these
use significantly more information sources (M = 3.26, SD events would occur. The answers to these questions were
= 2.48) than their Malaysian counterparts (M = 3.08, SD = categorised by two coders with the scheme given in
2.15) to estimate the events’ expected age at occurrence, Tables 1 and 2. The two coders showed good agreement
p = .093. (Landis & Koch, 1977) for the three open-ended questions,
Despite the differences in the number of selected infor- with Cohen’s κ = 0.842, 0.750 and 0.804, respectively. No
mation sources between questions (i.e., importance and new categories could be formed from the answers that
expected age at occurrence) and samples (i.e., Malaysian could not be coded by the scheme. In these answers, par-
and Australian), the patterns across the information ticipants often indicated that they had used their personal
sources were similar, r(11) = 0.985, p < .001 and r(11) = opinion or rephrased the instructions (“cultural
0.901, p < .001. For example, experiences of relatives were expectations”).
selected the most frequently for both questions (53.3%
and 51.3%), closely followed personal experiences (52.1%
Discussion
and 46.8%) and comments from relatives (49.1% and
41.1%). Television programmes and movies (42.6% and The present study had three goals. We examined whether
36.7%) and experiences of friends (39.2% and 37.2%) life scripts are stable, whether there are similarities and
were also often selected. However, cultural traditions differences in life scripts across cultures, and how life
(26.0% and 18.3%), laws and government policies (14.9% scripts are transmitted. To address these goals, participants
and 13.5%) and religious traditions (12.8% and 7.3%) from Malaysia and Australia completed the life script ques-
were only selected occasionally as information sources tionnaire. Both samples displayed the hallmarks of life
for deciding upon the importance of the events and the scripts (cf., Janssen & Haque, 2015). The temporal distri-
age at which the events were expected to occur. bution of positive, neutral and negative events showed
Finally, we calculated the correlations between the use that participants mentioned more positive than negative
of each information source and the events’ prevalence, events, that positive events were expected to occur
importance, expected age at occurrence and valence. mainly between the ages of 16 and 30, that the expected
140 S. M. J. JANSSEN AND S. HAQUE

ages at occurrence of negative events were distributed comparing different cultures. We would therefore like
equally over the lifespan, and that the standard deviations to recommend comparisons to employ inferential
of the expected age at occurrence of positive events were statistics.
smaller than those of negative events. The present study and many previous studies have
To test the stability of life scripts, the results of the Aus- treated culture and nationality as being identical.
tralian sample were compared to the results from an earlier However, studies like Coleman (2014) and Hatiboğlu and
study with a similar sample (i.e., Janssen, 2015). There were Habermas (2016) have shown that the life script of subcul-
only minor differences between these two samples. The tures may differ. Coleman (2014) asked African-American
correlation of the proportion of mentions was extremely participants to imagine a prototypical child or a prototypi-
high, which showed that the two samples agreed on cal child from their own race and found that some events
which events should and which events should not be were specific to the African-American life script. Hatiboğlu
included in the Australian life script. This finding was sup- and Habermas (2016) examined the life script of Turkish
ported by the results of the chi-square tests, which students from urban and rural areas and second-gener-
showed that the two samples differed on the proportion ation Turkish and German students from a major German
of mentions for only five events. When a stricter criterion city and found differences between them. Future research
was applied, the two samples did not differ on any event. should take subcultures into account.
Furthermore, both the proportions of positive, neutral Finally, we examined the transmission of life scripts. As
and negative events and the proportions of events far as we know, the present study is the first one that has
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:22 14 November 2017

expected to occur before the age of 16, between 16 and examined this issue. Besides answering open-ended ques-
30 and after the age of 30 were similar across the two tions, participants selected which information sources they
samples. These results do not only indicate that life had used to decide upon the importance of the event and
scripts are stable, but they can also be used to benchmark the age at which the event was expected to occur. The cat-
cross-cultural comparisons. egories from which they could choose were taken from
The differences between the Malaysian and Australian predictions by Berntsen and Rubin (2004), who proposed
sample were larger than the differences between the two that life scripts are transmitted through cultural and reli-
Australian samples. Although there was some overlap gious traditions (customs and rituals), and from conjecture
between the content of the Malaysian and Australian life by Janssen et al. (2014), who had speculated that life scripts
scripts, there was disagreement about each event’s promi- are transmitted through reading newspapers and books
nence within the life scripts. Event categories mentioned and watching television and movies. In line with the
by at least 4% in both samples were not necessarily men- social learning theory (Bandura, 1977), it was also predicted
tioned with similar frequency. Some events were fre- that life scripts could be transmitted through personal
quently mentioned in one sample and only occasionally experiences (performance accomplishments), observation
mentioned in the other sample and vice versa. Further- of other people’s lives (vicarious experiences) and com-
more, whereas both samples showed a higher prevalence ments and suggestions from others (verbal persuasion).
of events that are expected to occur between the ages of The results of the present study suggested that life
16 and 30 and positive events, the distributions of the scripts are transmitted in multiple ways. There was con-
expected age at occurrence and the distributions of posi- siderable support for responses derived from the social
tive, neutral and negative events were not similar across learning theory. Participants frequently selected the
the two samples, with the Australian life script containing options of experiences of relatives and friends, personal
more positive events and more events expected to occur experiences and comments made by relatives and
before the age of 16. friends. Personal experiences were especially used for
These results are similar to results of other cross-cul- prevalent events and events that were expected to occur
tural comparisons (Janssen et al., 2014; Ottsen & Bernt- at an early age. There was also support for the speculation
sen, 2014; Rubin et al., 2009; Scherman, 2013), which that life scripts are attained through watching movies and
have shown that, although the content might differ television programmes and reading books and newspa-
across cultures, life scripts from every culture display a pers. These options were often selected too. However, cul-
predominance of positive events and events expected tural and religious traditions were hardly selected, which
to occur in early adulthood. However, the present went against the prediction that life scripts would be trans-
study elaborates on these previous studies by showing mitted through traditions. These findings suggest that the
that descriptive statistics, such as which events reached tenth property of life scripts needs to be adjusted to
the 4% threshold or were among the 10 most mentioned include additional ways of transmission.
events in one sample but not the other, are less sensitive The additional way of transmission that seems the most
to differences than inferential statistics, such as corre- promising is the relatives’ and friends’ experiences. There is
lations and chi-square tests. When using comparisons increasing evidence that people know other people’s life
of samples from the same culture as a benchmark, stories (Pillemer, Steiner, Kuwabara, Thomsen, & Svob,
descriptive measures did not give larger differences 2015; Thomsen & Pillemer, 2016), either from witnessing
but inferential measures gave larger differences when them or being told about them. Other people’s life
MEMORY 141

stories are not personally experienced memories, but books, movies and television programmes. The results
people remember what has happened to their relatives seem to suggest that cultural life scripts are the abstraction
and friends. These events are therefore called vicarious of many vicarious memories about other people’s life
memories. Moreover, what people read or watch about stories, but this interpretation has to be examined further.
events happening to existing and fictional people in news-
papers, books, television programmes and movies might Acknowledgements
also be considered a form of vicarious memories. The
The authors would like to thank Jessie Khor for her help coding the
present results seem to suggest that cultural life scripts answers to the open-ended questions regarding the transmission of
seem to be the abstraction from witnessing and hearing life scripts and Prof. Martin Conway, Prof. Tillman Habermas and one
the life stories of relatives, friends and other people. anonymous reviewer for the helpful comments.
A limitation of the present study is that cultural life
scripts appear to be learned implicitly (Schacter, 1987; Disclosure statement
Seger, 1994). Because participants may not have explicitly No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
learned which events were important and prevalent and
when these important and prevalent events were expected
to occur, it may be difficult for them to report how they ORCID
have attained the life script. The implicit learning of life Steve M. J. Janssen http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3100-128X
scripts might explain the surprisingly high use of personal
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:22 14 November 2017

experiences. This finding seems to go against the idea that


cultural life scripts are semantic knowledge (Berntsen & References
Rubin, 2004). Many studies have shown that cultural life Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behav-
scripts are not affected by age (e.g., Janssen & Rubin, ioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215. doi:10.1037/0033-
2011; Janssen et al., 2014), suggesting that experiencing 295X.84.2.191
the events personally does not change which events are Berntsen, D., & Bohn, A. (2010). Remembering and forecasting: The
relation between autobiographical memory and episodic future
regarded as important and prevalent and at what age thinking. Memory & Cognition, 38, 265–278. doi:10.3758/MC.38.3.
these events are expected to occur. It is possible that par- 265
ticipants may have interpreted this item as simply a ques- Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2002). Emotionally charged autobiographi-
tion about whether they had personally experienced the cal memories across the life span: The recall of happy, sad, trau-
event rather than a question about whether they had matic, and involuntary memories. Psychology and Aging, 17, 636–
652. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.636
used personal experiences to attain the life script. Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2004). Cultural life scripts structure recall
We attempted to address the implicit learning of life from autobiographical memory. Memory & Cognition, 32, 427–442.
scripts by offering participants a list of options derived doi:10.3758/BF03195836
from the literature. However, this list might be missing Bohn, A. (2010). Generational differences in cultural life scripts and life
one or more ways in which life scripts can be transmitted. story memories of younger and older adults. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 24, 1324–1345. doi:10.1002/acp.1641
The list should therefore not be regarded as definitive. It is Bohn, A., & Berntsen, D. (2008). Life story development in childhood:
rather a starting point for further research. Furthermore, The development of life story abilities and the acquisition of cul-
some items on the list could benefit from refinement in tural life scripts from late middle childhood to adolescence.
future research. The item asking about family members’ Developmental Psychology, 44, 1135–1147. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.
experiences, for example, could be separated in two 44.4.1135
Coleman, J. T. (2014). Examining the life script of African-Americans: A
items, one asking about whether they had been told test of the cultural life script. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 419–
about the family members’ experiences (e.g., their 426. doi:10.1002/acp.3000
parents’ wedding) and another asking about whether Ece, B., & Gülgöz, S. (2014). The impact of suppressing the typical life
they had witnessed the family members’ experiences events on the reminiscence bump. Applied Cognitive Psychology,
(e.g., their parents’ 25th wedding anniversary). 28, 702–710. doi:10.1002/acp.3056
Erdoğan, A., Baran, B., Avlar, B., Taş, A. Ç, & Tekcan, A. İ. (2008). On the
persistence of positive events in life scripts. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 22, 95–111. doi:10.1002/acp.1363
Conclusions
Habermas, T. (2007). How to tell a life: The development of the cultural
The present study not only replicated findings regarding concept of biography. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8, 1–
31. doi:10.1080/15248370709336991
similarities and differences between life scripts from differ-
Haque, S., & Hasking, P. A. (2010). Life scripts for emotionally charged
ent cultures, it also demonstrated that cultural life scripts autobiographical memories: A cultural explanation of the reminis-
are stable when the results of the same culture were com- cence bump. Memory, 18, 712–729. doi:10.1080/09658211.2010.
pared. Furthermore, the study expanded our understand- 506442
ing of life scripts by showing that they are not Hatiboğlu, N., & Habermas, T. (2016). The normativity of life scripts and
transmitted through traditions as initially proposed but its relation with life story events across cultures and subcultures.
Memory, 24, 1369–1381. doi:10.1080/09658211.2015.1111389
through vicarious memories about the experiences of rela- Hofstede, G. H. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, beha-
tives and friends, comments of relatives and friends, and viors, institutions and organizations across nations (2nd ed.).
information about other people’s lives from newspapers, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
142 S. M. J. JANSSEN AND S. HAQUE

Janssen, S. M. J. (2015). Is there a cultural life script for public events? negative, events. Memory & Cognition, 31, 1–14. doi:10.3758/
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29, 61–68. doi:10.1002/acp.3022 BF03196077
Janssen, S. M. J., & Haque, S. (2015). Cultural life scripts in autobiogra- Rubin, D. C., Berntsen, D., & Hutson, M. (2009). The normative and the
phical memory. In E. Sheppard & S. Haque (Eds.), Culture and cogni- personal life: Individual differences in life scripts and life story
tion: A collection of critical essays (pp. 27–44). Bern: Peter Lang. events among USA and Danish undergraduates. Memory, 17, 54–
Janssen, S. M. J., & Rubin, D. C. (2011). Age effects in cultural life scripts. 68. doi:10.1080/09658210802541442
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 291–298. doi:10.1002/acp.1690 Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status.
Janssen, S. M. J., Uemiya, A., & Naka, M. (2014). Age and gender effects Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
in the cultural life script of Japanese adults. Journal of Cognitive Cognition, 13, 501–518. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.13.3.501
Psychology, 26, 307–321. doi:10.1080/20445911.2014.892493 Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, and knowledge. In
Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agree- P. N. Johnson-Laird & P. C. Wason (Eds.), Thinking: Readings in cog-
ment for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159–174. doi:10.2307/ nitive science (pp. 421–432). Cambridge: Cambridge University
2529310 Press.
Neugarten, B. L., Moore, J. W., & Lowe, J. C. (1965). Age norms, age con- Scherman, A. Z. (2013). Cultural life script theory and the reminiscence
straints, and adult socialization. American Journal of Sociology, 70, bump: A reanalysis of seven studies across cultures. Nordic
710–717. doi:10.1086/223965 Psychology, 65, 103–119. doi:10.1080/19012276.2013.807667
Ottsen, C. L., & Berntsen, D. (2014). The cultural life script of Qatar and Seger, C. A. (1994). Implicit learning. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 163–
across cultures: Effects of gender and religion. Memory, 22, 390–407. 196. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.163
doi:10.1080/09658211.2013.795598 Tekcan, A. İ, Kaya-Kızılöz, B., & Odaman, H. (2012). Life scripts across age
Pillemer, D. B., Steiner, K. L., Kuwabara, K. J., Thomsen, D. K., & Svob, C. groups: A comparison of adolescents, young adults, and older
(2015). Vicarious memories. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 233– adults. Memory, 20, 836–847. doi:10.1080/09658211.2012.710431
245. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2015.06.010 Thomsen, D. K., & Pillemer, D. B. (2016). I know my story and I know
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:22 14 November 2017

Rubin, D. C., & Berntsen, D. (2003). Life scripts help to maintain auto- your story: Developing a conceptual framework for vicarious life
biographical memories of highly positive, but not highly stories. Journal of Personality. doi:10.1111/jopy.12253

Appendix

Table A1. The description of the categories, including the number and the percentage of mentions, the average (and the standard deviation) of expected age
at occurrence, the average (and standard deviation) of prevalence, the average (and standard deviation) of importance and the average (and standard
deviation) of valence of the Malaysian sample.
Description Number Percentage Age Prevalence Importance Valence
Marriage 108 89.3 27.70 (2.39) 75.3 (15.3) 5.99 (1.12) 2.32 (1.03)
Having children 82 67.8 30.46 (8.09) 67.1 (17.5) 5.87 (1.14) 1.96 (1.24)
University graduation 60 49.6 23.43 (17.68) 65.6 (17.0) 6.05 (0.87) 2.47 (0.79)
First full-time job 54 44.6 24.17 (1.95) 82.7 (18.6) 6.15 (1.02) 1.76 (1.23)
Fall in love, first kiss 48 39.7 16.33 (3.57) 87.0 (12.9) 5.27 (1.30) 1.65 (1.26)
Begin university 36 29.8 19.03 (0.85) 66.4 (15.2) 5.92 (0.94) 1.86 (1.07)
Begin primary school 32 26.4 6.41 (1.04) 91.7 (8.4) 6.09 (0.96) 1.72 (1.11)
Death parents 31 25.6 50.94 (10.18) 92.5 (8.8) 6.42 (0.99) −2.55 (1.06)
Begin childcare, kindergarten 25 20.7 3.84 (0.90) 89.6 (13.7) 5.84 (1.43) 1.76 (1.39)
Death (own death) 21 17.4 81.62 (8.94) 100.0 (0.0) 5.33 (1.88) −1.05 (1.56)
High-school graduation 19 15.7 17.79 (2.32) 84.4 (12.7) 5.63 (1.01) 1.63 (1.26)
Learning to walk, or first steps 17 14.0 1.18 (0.95) 90.8 (23.7) 6.71 (0.47) 2.76 (0.56)
Major achievement 16 13.2 30.00 (12.88) 44.3 (23.3) 5.56 (1.26) 1.88 (1.36)
Serious illness 16 13.2 56.50 (7.22) 67.3 (30.1) 5.50 (1.32) −2.31 (0.79)
Learning to talk, or first words 15 12.4 1.47 (0.74) 96.9 (4.5) 6.47 (0.74) 2.67 (0.62)
Adolescence, puberty 14 11.6 12.50 (1.79) 95.9 (5.9) 5.93 (1.44) 1.00 (1.36)
Having grandchildren 12 9.9 65.00 (6.16) 53.2 (16.0) 5.75 (1.36) 2.08 (1.24)
Children leave house 11 9.1 55.18 (8.39) 63.3 (16.5) 5.09 (1.30) 1.55 (1.29)
First play friend 11 9.1 5.09 (1.51) 93.7 (6.8) 6.82 (0.41) 2.36 (0.81)
First rejection 11 9.1 18.00 (3.13) 68.6 (26.0) 5.00 (1.10) −1.64 (1.29)
Important promotion at work 11 9.1 34.36 (4.48) 64.3 (15.1) 5.82 (0.98) 2.18 (1.08)
Retirement 11 9.1 63.27 (17.17) 73.8 (28.2) 5.55 (1.04) 1.45 (1.29)
Settle on career 11 9.1 27.45 (1.56) 75.2 (26.7) 5.91 (0.94) 2.18 (0.98)
Making a long journey 10 8.3 28.70 (18.32) 62.8 (23.3) 4.50 (1.27) 1.80 (1.03)
Meeting future partner 10 8.3 24.60 (3.34) 68.1 (11.2) 5.10 (1.37) 1.70 (1.34)
Obtaining driver’s licence 8 6.6 17.88 (0.64) 76.5 (22.0) 5.13 (1.64) 1.50 (0.93)
Peer group, friendships 8 6.6 11.38 (4.24) 79.1 (30.0) 6.00 (1.41) 1.63 (1.30)
Begin high school 7 5.8 13.00 (0.00) 83.7 (11.9) 6.14 (0.90) 2.29 (0.76)
Accident 6 5.0 13.00 (8.81) 75.0 (25.9) 4.83 (1.17) −1.83 (0.41)
Buying house or other property 6 5.0 29.17 (4.79) 74.5 (14.7) 5.33 (1.21) 2.00 (1.67)
First sexual experience 6 5.0 23.67 (5.79) 87.5 (10.7) 5.17 (2.40) 0.83 (2.23)
Death sibling or friend 5 4.1 38.20 (20.87) 57.6 (37.7) 5.40 (1.67) −2.40 (0.89)
End primary school 5 4.1 15.20 (3.90) 92.4 (9.9) 5.40 (0.89) 2.00 (1.00)
Leave home 5 4.1 22.60 (5.08) 77.8 (16.4) 5.60 (1.14) 1.00 (1.87)
First birthday* 5 4.1 1.00 (0.00) 100.0 (0.0) 5.60 (1.95) 2.20 (1.30)

(Continued)
MEMORY 143

Table A1. Continued.


Description Number Percentage Age Prevalence Importance Valence
Adulthood 4 3.3 20.25 (2.87) 76.0 (24.9) 4.75 (1.50) 1.50 (0.58)
Birth (own birth) 4 3.3 0.00 (0.00) 100.0 (0.0) 5.75 (0.96) 2.00 (1.41)
Birth younger sibling 4 3.3 6.50 (1.73) 62.8 (22.8) 5.00 (1.41) 2.00 (1.16)
Death partner 4 3.3 70.25 (7.32) 47.5 (5.1) 6.00 (0.82) −3.00 (0.00)
Death grandparents 2 1.7 20.50 (10.61) 92.5 (3.5) 6.50 (0.71) −2.50 (0.71)
Divorce 2 1.7 41.50 (4.95) 82.5 (2.1) 5.50 (0.71) −2.50 (0.71)
Eighteen birthday 2 1.7 18.00 (0.00) 100.0 (0.0) 5.00 (0.00) 1.00 (0.00)
First part-time job 1 0.8 20.00 (0.00) 100.0 (0.0) 7.00 (0.00) 3.00 (0.00)
Problems at work 1 0.8 22.00 (0.00) 58.0 (0.0) 6.00 (0.00) 0.00 (0.00)
Other 70 57.9 19.59 (17.68) 71.3 (24.5) 5.51 (1.57) 1.06 (1.91)
*New event category.

Table A2. The description of the categories, including the number and the percentage of mentions, the average (and the standard deviation) of expected age
at occurrence, the average (and standard deviation) of prevalence, the average (and standard deviation) of importance and the average (and standard
deviation) of valence of the Australian sample.
Description Number Percentage Age Prevalence Importance Valence
Having children 99 67.3 28.24 (2.62) 70.94 (15.39) 6.00 (1.08) 2.42 (0.89)
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:22 14 November 2017

Begin primary school 89 60.5 5.11 (0.35) 92.85 (13.55) 6.36 (0.79) 1.76 (1.05)
Marriage 87 59.2 27.16 (2.29) 73.82 (14.11) 5.46 (1.24) 2.66 (0.68)
High-school graduation 66 44.9 17.65 (0.57) 77.58 (16.17) 5.89 (0.84) 2.26 (0.85)
Fall in love, first kiss 54 36.7 17.69 (3.90) 90.26 (10.71) 5.91 (1.12) 2.33 (0.95)
Begin high school 42 28.6 13.19 (0.86) 91.62 (13.95) 6.19 (0.77) 1.05 (1.23)
First full-time job 38 25.9 22.95 (2.63) 84.03 (13.53) 6.18 (0.77) 1.66 (1.21)
University graduation 32 21.8 22.84 (1.61) 54.09 (17.59) 5.69 (0.97) 2.66 (0.55)
Making a long journey 27 18.4 23.81(10.41) 66.04 (22.75) 5.04 (1.63) 2.78 (0.42)
Retirement 27 18.4 67.67 (5.26) 86.48 (13.22) 5.63 (0.79) 1.93 (1.00)
Begin university 25 17.0 18.52 (0.77) 61.40 (14.89) 5.60 (0.96) 1.72 (0.94)
Obtaining driver’s licence 23 15.6 16.74 (1.14) 83.00 (13.05) 5.48 (1.12) 1.96 (1.07)
Eighteen birthday 22 15.0 18.00 (0.00) 97.00 (5.77) 5.91 (0.92) 1.86 (1.13)
First part-time job 21 14.3 15.52 (1.40) 83.67 (17.41) 5.81 (0.93) 1.43 (1.12)
Buy house or other property 20 13.6 28.80 (1.94) 65.90 (19.64) 5.85 (0.75) 2.20 (0.89)
First play friend 19 12.9 4.79 (1.08) 96.37 (6.18) 6.47 (0.77) 2.58 (0.84)
Death grandparents 17 11.6 15.47 (7.76) 99.65 (0.79) 6.12 (1.11) −2.00 (1.66)
Learning to walk, or first steps 17 11.6 1.06 (0.43) 97.76 (3.33) 6.82 (0.39) 2.76 (0.56)
Leave home 17 11.6 21.22 (2.64) 86.59 (14.65) 5.76 (0.83) 1.71 (1.16)
Settle on career 17 11.6 26.47 (6.06) 73.71 (19.17) 5.88 (0.78) 1.82 (0.88)
Begin childcare, kindergarten 16 10.9 3.69 (0.70) 90.94 (13.36) 5.94 (1.06) 1.19 (1.22)
Death (own death) 14 9.5 85.79 (7.34) 100.00 (0.00) 6.07 (1.54) −0.86 (1.70)
Having grandchildren 14 9.5 59.29 (7.18) 66.21 (17.00) 5.43 (1.02) 2.57 (0.65)
First birthday 14 9.5 1.00 (0.00) 99.79 (0.58) 6.00 (1.11) 2.50 (0.86)
Adolescence, puberty 13 8.8 12.46 (1.13) 92.54 (23.98) 6.00 (1.08) −0.23 (1.09)
Learning to talk, or first words 13 8.8 1.23 (0.60) 98.85 (1.82) 6.85 (0.38) 2.77 (0.44)
Meeting future partner 13 8.8 24.77 (3.40) 78.46 (17.49) 5.85 (0.99) 2.92 (0.28)
Birth (own birth) 11 7.5 0.00 (0.00) 100.00 (0.00) 6.91 (0.30) 1.36 (1.43)
Death parents 11 7.5 46.91 (12.26) 89.27 (22.23) 6.36 (0.67) −2.55 (0.93)
Peer group, friendships 11 7.5 14.55 (2.73) 87.91 (16.87) 6.18 (0.87) 1.64 (1.75)
First sexual experience 10 6.8 17.20 (2.39) 92.50 (7.71) 5.60 (1.17) 1.30 (0.95)
Learning to cycle or swim 8 5.4 6.25 (1.75) 84.63 (11.11) 5.75 (1.04) 2.25 (1.04)
Major achievement 8 5.4 23.88 (14.88) 48.13 (33.05) 6.50 (0.76) 2.75 (0.46)
First rejection 7 4.8 17.57 (3.78) 93.57 (7.93) 5.71 (1.11) −2.00 (1.00)
Family holiday 6 4.1 3.33 (3.39) 89.00 (14.14) 5.67 (0.82) 2.83 (0.41)
Death sibling or friend 5 3.4 55.20 (30.09) 97.60 (4.34) 5.60 (0.89) −3.00 (0.00)
End primary school 5 3.4 13.00 (0.71) 91.80 (8.87) 6.60 (0.89) 1.80 (1.10)
Birth of younger sibling 4 2.7 5.00 (1.41) 74.50 (11.73) 6.50 (0.58) 2.25 (0.50)
Important promotion at work 4 2.7 30.75 (7.14) 42.00 (20.80) 4.50 (1.92) 2.00 (0.82)
Learning to read and write* 4 2.7 4.75 (0.96) 87.75 (10.21) 6.75 (0.50) 2.00 (0.82)
Adulthood 3 2.0 19.67 (1.53) 99.33 (0.58) 6.00 (1.73) 1.67 (1.53)
Baptism 3 2.0 15.00 (12.29) 65.67 (30.11) 6.33 (1.16) 1.67 (1.53)
Divorce 3 2.0 39.00 (1.73) 53.33 (5.77) 3.67 (0.58) −1.67 (1.53)
Divorce parents 3 2.0 11.67 (4.51) 53.33 (20.21) 5.33 (1.16) −2.33 (0.58)
Moving to another house 3 2.0 22.33 (0.58) 83.33 (10.41) 5.33 (0.58) 2.67 (0.58)
First tooth, teething* 3 2.0 3.67 (3.22) 100.00 (0.00) 5.33 (1.16) 2.00 (1.73)
Children leave house 2 1.4 47.50 (3.54) 82.50 (3.54) 5.50 (0.71) −0.50 (2.12)
Twenty-first birthday 2 1.4 21.00 (0.00) 96.50 (4.95) 4.00 (1.41) 1.00 (0.00)
Fiftieth birthday 2 1.4 50.00 (0.00) 75.00 (35.36) 4.00 (0.00) 0.00 (0.00)
Accident 1 0.7 6.00 (0.00) 75.00 (0.00) 4.00 (0.00) −3.00 (0.00)
Other 54 36.2 19.67 (20.18) 76.46 (28.72) 5.81 (1.29) 1.41 (1.90)
*New event categories.
Copyright of Memory is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may not be
copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for
individual use.

You might also like