Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

LSHSS

Research Article

Spoken Persuasive Discourse Abilities


of Adolescents With Acquired
Brain Injury
Catherine Moran, a Cecilia Kirk, b and Emma Powell c

Purpose: The aim of this study was to examine the performance Results: There were no statistically significant group differences
of adolescents with acquired brain injury (ABI) during a spoken on measures of language productivity or syntactic complexity, with
persuasive discourse task. Persuasive discourse is frequently the exception of the use of mazes, which was more prevalent in the
used in social and academic settings and is of importance in discourse of the group with ABI. However, there were significant
the study of adolescent language. differences in language content, with age-matched peers produc-
Method: Participants included 8 adolescents with ABI and ing more than twice as many supporting reasons and far fewer
8 peers without ABI who were matched for age, gender, and tangentially related utterances than the adolescents with ABI.
education. A spoken persuasive discourse task requiring par- Conclusions: Persuasive discourse production was affected follow-
ticipants to express their opinion on a topic was administered, ing ABI. Given the importance of persuasive discourse in social and
and the 2 groups were compared on measures of language academic situations, further investigations into factors that influ-
productivity, syntactic complexity, and language content. ence discourse production in adolescents with ABI are warranted.
In addition, the relationship between working memory and
discourse production was explored. Key Words: adolescents, brain injury, discourse analysis

t is estimated that approximately half a million American communication deficits that are not only recognized at the

I children under the age of 14 sustain an acquired brain


injury (ABI) each year (Langlois, Rutland-Brown, &
Thomas, 2005). ABI refers to brain injury that can result
time of injury, but that may become evident or worsen as
the child progresses into adolescence. For instance, it has
been reported that children who demonstrate relative re-
from impact to the brain. It also includes diffuse damage that covery of language skills may present with difficulties with
arises from accidents that result in a severe lack of oxygen to later developing syntax and semantics (e.g., Ewing-Cobbs,
the brain such as near-drowning incidents (e.g., Blosser & Brookshire, Scott, & Fletcher, 1998; Jordan, Cannon, &
dePompeii, 2003). For the purposes of this paper, ABI refers Murdoch, 1992; Jordan & Murdoch, 1994; Moran & Gillon,
specifically to accidental injuries that are limited to closed 2004).
head injury. For children who suffer an ABI in childhood, One area of language that continues to develop through-
most will experience ongoing social and academic diffi- out childhood and adolescence is discourse production. Dis-
culties (Turkstra, 1999). Related to this is the presence of course impairments are often evident at the time of brain injury
and persist in children and adolescents regardless of the age
a
University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand of injury or stage of recovery (Biddle, McCabe, & Bliss,
b
University of Oregon, Eugene 1996; Chapman et al., 1992; Chapman et al., 1997; Ewing-
c
Accident Compensation Corporation of New Zealand, Cobbs et al., 1998).
Wellington
Correspondence to Catherine Moran: Discourse Production Following ABI
catherine.moran@canterbury.ac.nz
Editor: Marilyn Nippold The most widely studied type of discourse produced by
Associate Editor: Victoria Joffe children with ABI is narrative discourse (e.g., Coelho, 1995,
Received February 2, 2011 2002). Performance on narrative production tasks has typ-
Revision received June 14, 2011 ically been measured across three broad domains: language
Accepted November 25, 2011 productivity (e.g., number of words, number of T-units),
DOI: 10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0114) syntactic complexity (e.g., clause density), and language

264 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 43 • 264–275 • July 2012 * American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
content (e.g., essential story information, inclusion of epi- another person to share one’s point of view (e.g., Devito,
sodic structure elements). 2003; Nippold, 2007). Persuasive discourse is an important
Within the narrative genre, children and adolescents with genre to examine in adolescents with ABI for a variety of
ABI have been found to present with deficits predominantly reasons. First, persuasion is considered to be a more cogni-
in the domains of language content (Brookshire, Chapman, tively demanding task than narrative production (Crowhurst,
Song, & Levin, 2000; Chapman et al., 1992, 1997). For 1990; Felton & Kuhn, 2001; Knudson, 1992), and given
instance, Chapman et al. (1997) compared the performance of the cognitive deficits associated with ABI, it is likely to be
children with ABI to that of age-matched peers (AMPs) on impaired.
two narrative tasks involving a retell of an Aesop’s fable and Second, persuasive discourse is a form of discourse that is
generation of a story based on a five-picture card sequence. common in daily life. For adolescents, persuasive discourse
Each story contained 16 propositions and had distinctive may occur in both formal situations (e.g., school debates,
episodes and similar macrostructure or gist components. Re- school essays) and informal situations (e.g., convincing a
sults indicated highly significant differences between the friend to see a movie, urging parents to purchase the latest
two groups on measures of language content, with the ABI electronic gadget). The ability to produce efficient persua-
group producing less information and omitting essential story sive discourse is recognized as an important academic skill.
components and global story content, as well as producing For instance, the New Zealand Qualifications Authority (2007)
disruptions to story structure. However, there were no sta- stated that in order to achieve English standards for grad-
tistically significant group differences on measures of lan- uation, adolescents should be able to produce spoken lan-
guage productivity or syntactic complexity. guage that is persuasive by providing coherent arguments
In contrast, Hay and Moran (2005) found that children and clear points of view.
and adolescents with ABI had deficits across all three lan- Third, Nippold (2007) described ongoing development of
guage domains. Hay and Moran compared the performance persuasive discourse throughout the school-age and adolescent
of nine individuals with ABI and nine typically developing years, with improvements to such areas as the anticipation
AMPs on the retelling of narrative fables and expository of counterarguments, the substitution of “whining” and
passages. Their results showed that, in addition to reduced “begging” tactics for bargaining and continued politeness,
language content, the participants with ABI produced fewer providing a larger number of different arguments including
words and fewer T-units as well as less complex syntactic advantages for the speaker to comply, and changing the
structures. speaker’s persuasive style to cater to differing audience fea-
In a different type of narrative task, personal narrative tures. However, it appears that full maturation of persuasive
production, Biddle et al. (1996) found that although both discourse skills is not yet complete by 17 years of age, par-
adults and children with ABI produced equal amounts of ticularly in the area of pragmatics (e.g., Nippold, 2007). This
discourse as their peers, their productions were less efficient. makes the persuasive genre a later developing skill and there-
For example, critical, partial, or entire units of information fore may be affected by an ABI in childhood.
were left out of the narratives of the participants with ABI, Finally, adolescence is a time of significant social de-
making it difficult for listeners to understand the main points velopment. From pre-adolescence (9–12 years) through to
of the narrative. Productivity was affected in that the indi- older adolescence (17+ years), a number of developmental
viduals with ABI produced a greater number of repetitions, changes take place with regard to the construction of self-
filled pauses, and false starts. identity, including development of independent values,
Although the findings in the studies described are varied, morals, and opinions (Turkstra, 2000). Using a variety of
measures of language productivity, syntactic complexity, ecological data collection methods such as focus groups and
and language content may all be reduced in the discourse observation, Turkstra (2000) compiled a list of skills that are
production of individuals with ABI. For this reason, a broad considered to be required for social acceptance in this age
range of measures should be considered when evaluating group. Of significance to the application of persuasive dis-
discourse in this population. course were the ability to understand another person’s point
of view and to be flexible in thinking about an issue. Within
Persuasive Discourse adolescent conversation interactions, speaker characteris-
tics included asking questions and seeking opinions of others.
One factor that must be considered when evaluating dis- In summarizing the study, Turkstra stated that communica-
course production is the genre that is assessed. In nonclinical tion was not only the means for conveying information, but it
populations, genre has been shown to affect language produc- also provided a medium for developing social skills. View-
tivity and syntactic complexity (Nippold, Hesketh, Duthie, ing persuasive discourse as a means to develop one’s sense
& Mansfield, 2005; Scott & Windsor, 2000). Differences in of identity through communicating opinions, ideas, and
organization and content have also been noted. One type of beliefs signals that this genre is vitally important in the ado-
discourse that has received little attention in ABI research is lescent years. Given the challenges faced by adolescents with
persuasive discourse. Persuasive discourse aims to convince ABI, the more that is known about their ability to conduct

Moran et al.: Persuasive Discourse in Adolescents 265


persuasive discourse, the better equipped clinicians and edu- relationship between WM and the ability to produce expos-
cators will be in assisting them to reach their social com- itory and narrative discourse using a retelling task. They
municative potential. suggested that if WM is constrained, individuals are likely to
perform more poorly on a discourse retelling task as it is
difficult to store the information for later retelling. When
Discourse and Working Memory (WM)
the task is a generation task as opposed to retelling, how-
There are a number of reasons why individuals would ever, the storage demands may not be as great, so it is
demonstrate difficulties with discourse production following less clear whether WM would be taxed. Youse and Coelho
ABI. It is useful to consider the possible underlying causes of (2005) explored the contribution of WM on conversa-
the discourse difficulties in order to determine how to best tional discourse. Using a digit span task, they examined
proceed with intervention (Chapman et al., 1997). Although the relationship between performance on the memory task
communication deficits following ABI have typically been and measures of discourse production. Although they
found to be influenced by characteristics of the injury (e.g., did not find a significant correlation, Youse and Coelho
severity of injury, age at injury; Dennis, 1989; Ewing-Cobbs, suggested that the relationship be further explored using
Fletcher, Levin, & Eisenberg, 1987; Jordan & Murdoch, a more complex WM task that taxes both storage and
1994), it has been proposed that executive functions may be processing.
an important contributor to pragmatic deficits such as dis- To summarize, the practical applications of persuasive
course generation, regardless of the nature of the injury discourse skills are lifelong and are important in both aca-
(Douglas, 2010). Executive functions include but are not demic success and functioning in everyday life (Crowhurst,
limited to attentional control, inhibition of irrelevant infor- 1990). Furthermore, the ability to persuade and use argu-
mation and responses, and monitoring and updating of infor- ments effectively is considered a fundamental social inter-
mation over time (Miyake et al., 2000; Miyake & Shah, 1999). action skill (Bartsch, London, & Campbell, 2007; Felton &
Douglas (2010) examined the influence of a small subset Kuhn, 2001). Spoken discourse production is disrupted in
of executive functions on the ability to produce conversa- children and adolescents following ABI, with deficits oc-
tional discourse following ABI. She found associations curring in the domains of language productivity, syntactic
between executive functioning and conversational ability, complexity, and language content. Although performance on
whereas no such links were found between conversational measures of discourse has been found to vary across studies,
performance and injury characteristics such as length of it is collectively agreed that individuals with ABI have
posttraumatic amnesia or site of lesion. This is consistent with difficulty verbally organizing and constructing language for
earlier findings by Brookshire et al. (2000), who found that functional tasks such as narration and explanation.
although executive functions including memory were asso-
ciated with discourse performance on a narrative task, the Goals of the Study
site and extent of the lesion were not. Given the heterogeneity
of individuals with ABI, performance on executive function- Currently, there is limited research investigating either
ing may be a more important indicator of overall communi- written or spoken persuasive discourse in individuals with
cative functioning than injury characteristics. Executive ABI. The aim of this study was to address this gap in the
functions are part of a broader mechanism of cognitive literature by examining the performance of adolescents with
function referred to as WM (Baddeley, 1976). WM is typi- ABI on a task of spoken persuasive discourse. Specifically,
cally conceptualized as a part of memory that is involved three questions are addressed:
in the strategic processing and storage of information (e.g., & Do adolescents with ABI differ from AMPs on
Baddeley, 1976; Just & Carpenter, 1992). WM has been language productivity and syntactic complexity
linked extensively to the processing of discourse in popu- measures in a spoken discourse task?
lations with and without brain injury (e.g., Cain, Bryant, & & Do adolescents with ABI differ from AMPs on
Oakhill 2004; Chapman et al., 2006; Light & Anderson, 1985). language content measures in a spoken discourse task?
The contribution of WM to discourse production is less & Is WM associated with performance on spoken
clear, yet there is evidence to support a link. In an exam- discourse in adolescents with and without ABI?
ination of the neural correlates of discourse comprehension
and production, the frontal and temporal areas of the brain,
areas that support WM, are also active in discourse pro-
duction (Mar, 2004). In written text, WM has been found to
be a strong contributor to discourse production, associated
METHOD
with measures such as sentence length (Kellogg, 2004) and
Participants
goal-directed writing (McCutcheon, 1996). With regard to
WM and discourse production in clinical populations, results Study participants (N = 16) included eight adolescents with
have been equivocal. Hay and Moran (2005) noted a strong brain injury (Mage = 14;6 [years;months], range = 11;7–17;5)

266 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 43 • 264–275 • July 2012
and eight age-, gender-, and education-matched peers with- criteria defined by the Accident Compensation Corpora-
out brain injury (Mage = 14;6, age range = 11;7–17;11). All tion Serious Injury Profiles (Accident Compensation
participants were monolingual English speakers and attended Corporation, 2010).
middle or secondary school education institutions. No par- All participants exhibited some disability in daily func-
ticipants had a reported hearing or visual impairment. The tioning as measured by the LCOF and the DRS. The LCOF
following demographic information is provided for each describes stages of functioning following ABI that range
particpant in Table 1: gender, age at injury, age at testing, from I to VIII, with I being no response and VIII being
educational level, mechanism of injury, and scores on the purposeful and appropriate. The scores of the participants
Los Ranchos Amigos Scale of Cognitive Functioning with ABI ranged from VI to VIII on the LCOF. The DRS is
(LCOF; Hagen, 1998) and the Disability Rating Scale a valid, reliable tool that measures current functioning of
(Rappaport, 2005). individuals with ABI and other acquired brain injuries such
Adolescents with ABI. The participants with ABI were as stroke (Douglas, 2010; Rappaport, 2005). It is particularly
recruited from the existing and previous clinical caseload of useful when a long period of time has passed post injury
one of the authors and were deemed to be brain injured if as individuals with brain injury can demonstrate differing
they had a claim that had been accepted by the Accident levels of recovery over time, regardless of the severity of
Compensation Corporation of New Zealand. The partici- the injury (Blosser & de Pompei, 2003; Rappaport, 2005).
pants with ABI sustained their injuries a minimum of 7 years The DRS measures function across four areas: arousability,
before administration of the persuasive discourse task re- awareness, and responsiveness; cognition for self-care;
ported in this paper. Severity levels of injuries were unable to dependence on others; and employability (or alternatively,
be obtained for all participants due to the time since injury. functioning at school). The severity levels range from 0
However, all participants with ABI presented with language (no disability) to 29 (vegetative state). The scores of the
difficulties as identified by a combination of standardized participants in this study ranged from 2 (partial disability) to
testing and a practicing speech-language pathologist 4 (moderate disability). See Table 1 for details. Participants
(SLP). Specifically, the participants with ABI (a) had were excluded from the study if (a) English was not the
been diagnosed with language difficulties and had re- first language spoken in the home; (b) there was a premorbid
ceived or were receiving speech-language pathology history of special education, including treatment for lan-
services focusing on language, literacy, and/or cognitive- guage deficits for those who received their injury subsequent
communication disorders; (b) had scored >1 SD below the to the first year of life; (c) there was a prior history of psy-
mean on at least two subtests of the Clinical Evaluation of chological disorder; or (d) child abuse was suspected.
Language Fundamentals—Fourth Edition (CELF–4; Semel, AMPs. Once the participants with ABI had been identi-
Wiig, & Secord, 2003); (c) had scored >1 SD below the mean fied, typically developing participants were recruited to
on at least one subtest of the Test of Adolescent and Adult match the adolescents with ABI based on gender, age (matched
Language—Third Edition (TOAL–3; Hammill, Brown, within 6 months), and educational level. Students in the
Larsen, & Wiederholt, 1994); and (d) were receiving teacher AMP group were all recruited from a single high school and
aide support in the classroom. It was confirmed by the ser- were selected from an initial pool of students put forward
vice provider that all participants had sustained injuries by the school’s special needs coordinator. Students in the
that ranged from moderate-to-severe to severe according to initial pool were recommended because they were deemed

Table 1. Demographics of the participants with acquired brain injury (ABI).

Age at injury
Participant Gender (years;months) Age at testing Educational level Mechanism of injury LCOF DRS

1 Male 1;4 11;7 Year 7 Fall VI 3


2 Male 3;4 12;8 Year 8 Collision VIII 2
3 Male 7;4 14;4 Year 9 Fall VI 4
4 Male 5;6 14;6 Year 9 Fall VII 2
5 Female 2;7 14;11 Year 10 MVA VIII 1
6 Male 7;6 15;1 Year 10 Car vs. pedestrian VII 2
7 Male 8;8 16;1 Year 11 Medical mishap VI 4
8 Male 5;7 17;5 Year 13 Bike vs. car VIII 3

Note. LCOF = Los Ranchos Amigos Scale of Cognitive Functioning (Hagen, 1998); DRS = Disability Rating Scale (Rappaport, 2005), MVA =
motor vehicle accident.

Moran et al.: Persuasive Discourse in Adolescents 267


to have no existing learning or language problems and would Table 3. Independent t test results comparing performance of
likely be willing participants in the project. From this pool, the two groups on language and working memory tests.
the AMPs who best matched the participants with ABI were then
selected to participate in the study. All of the AMPs performed
Test t p d
within or above normal limits on the CELF–4 and TOAL–3.

CELF–4 5.88 <.001 2.94


Procedure TOAL–3: WV 6.95 <.001 3.47
All sessions followed the same format. Following a brief TOAL–3: WG 3.53 .003 1.76
CLPT 3.62 .003 1.81
introduction and explanation of the session, two standard-
ized language tests (CELF–4 and TOAL–3) and a WM test
(Competing Language Processing Test [CLPT]; Gaulin &
Campbell, 1994) were administered. The language tests were fatigue. Sessions were conducted at each participant’s school
administered in order to determine the language status of or home (all of the AMPs were seen at school, and six of
the adolescents with and without ABI. The WM test was the eight students with ABI were seen at home) in a quiet
administered in order to examine the relationship between room with minimal distraction. Participants were offered the
WM and discourse production. Results of the language and option of having a significant other present, and one of the
WM tests for the two groups of participants are presented eight students with ABI opted for this.
in Table 2. Results of independent t tests comparing perfor- WM. The CLPT was selected to measure WM as it is a
mance between the two groups are provided in Table 3. dual processing and storage task that is used to evaluate WM in
This testing was followed by a 10-min break, and finally, children. The CLPT is a complex WM task that is similar to
the persuasive discourse task was administered. Sessions Daneman and Carpenter’s (1980) Reading Span task for adults.
lasted between 1 hr and 15 min and 2 hr. Breaks were of- It has been suggested that complex WM tasks like the CLPT are
fered throughout the session to help minimize the effects of more likely to be linked to discourse generation than simple
span tasks (Youse & Coelho, 2005). The CLPT was selected for
use in this study because the test items are constructed of
Table 2. Performance of the two groups of participants on sentences that are short and simple to comprehend. Although
language and working memory tests. standardized on children ages 6 to 12, this test was deemed
particularly useful for children who sustained their ABI at a
young age as the sentence length and complexity meant that it
Test ABI AMP should be achievable by the youngest participants with ABI.
Despite being designed for younger children, no participants in
CELF–4 the current study, including both those with and without ABI,
M 93.63 122.25 achieved a perfect score. To complete the CLPT, participants
SD 11.69 7.27 were required to listen to groups of sentences increasing in num-
Range 79–115 114–134
ber from two to six. Participants were instructed to answer true
TOAL–3: WV subtest
M 4.25 12.88 or false after each sentence (processing component) and then
SD 3.01 1.81 to repeat the last word of each sentence once the whole group
Range 1–8 10–15 had been read aloud (storage component). The percentage cor-
TOAL–3: WG subtest rect of true/false responses and words recalled were recorded.
M 6.38 12.38
Spoken persuasive discourse task. Next, participants
SD 4.07 2.56
Range 1–14 8–15 were asked to offer an opinion as to whether they thought
CLPT team sports or individual sports were better and to provide
M 24.63 29.13 reasons for their claim. This topic was chosen because sports
SD 2.92 1.96 have a widespread appeal, thus increasing the likelihood that
Range 20–28 27–33
the participants would have personal experience with vari-
ous types of sports because topic knowledge is likely to af-
Note. CELF–4 = Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals— fect the quality of persuasive argumentation. Each participant
Fourth Edition (Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 2003; results shown are core
language scores where M = 100 and SD = 15); TOAL–3 = Test of was prompted to look at a photomontage depicting a vari-
Adolescent and Adult Language—Third Edition (Hammill, Brown, ety of team and individual sports. The examiner then read
Larsen, & Wiederholt, 1994; results shown are subtest standard scores aloud an introductory passage to encourage the participants
where M = 10 and SD = 3); WV = Writing Vocabulary; WG = Writing to consider all aspects of the topic:
Grammar; CLPT = Competing Language Processing Test (Gaulin
& Campbell, 1994; results shown are raw scores representing the People have different views on which are the best types of
number of words recalled; higher scores represent greater working sport to play, individual sports or team sports. For example,
memory capacity). some people think team sports like rugby, netball, and soccer

268 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 43 • 264–275 • July 2012
are great because you get to share the work and different that they had been identified correctly. The level of agree-
team members get to do different jobs like goal shoot. ment between the two investigators for number of T-units
However, other people think that individual sports like
swimming, snowboarding, and some athletics are the best was 99.5%. The level of agreement between the two inves-
because you get to challenge yourself and when you win tigators for total number of mazed words was 98.5%. Any
you’ve done it all on your own. I am interested in learning disagreement was resolved through discussion until 100%
whether you think team sports or individual sports are better agreement was attained. From this information, the follow-
and why. I want you to spend the next 10 minutes telling ing measures of language productivity were calculated:
me what you think and make sure you give me lots of good
reasons for your opinion. I will raise my hand when you’ve (a) total number of T-units, (b) mean length of T-unit in
got 1 minute to go. Do you have any questions? words, (c) total number of unmazed words, and (d) num-
ber of mazed words as a proportion of total words.
Syntactic complexity. Next, each sample was coded so
that a variety of syntactic variables could be identified. This
Data Transcription and Analyses
included identification of independent clauses and three
Each spoken sample was keyboarded into the Systematic types of subordinate clauses (i.e., nominal, adverbial, and
Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) computer pro- relative). Subordinate clauses that contained nonfinite verbs
gram (Miller & Chapman, 2010). Coding of the variables were included in the analysis if they contained a subject
of interest (i.e., language productivity, syntactic complexity, that was different than the subject in the matrix clause (e.g.,
and language content) was conducted by researchers who You have got [no-one to help you] ). A second researcher
were blinded to participant group membership. checked the coding of subordinate clauses to verify that they
Language productivity. One of the researchers divided had been identified correctly. The level of agreement between
each spoken sample into T-units. T-units were defined as an the two investigators on the coding of subordinate clauses
independent clause with any associated dependent clauses. was 99%. The identification of the different clause types was
Definitions and examples of independent and dependent used to determine the following: (a) use of nominal, adver-
clauses provided by the participants are provided in Table 4. bial, and relative clauses as a percentage of the T-units per
Maze behaviors that consisted of filled pauses (e.g., and they sample; (b) clause density, which is defined as the total num-
say [um] well done), revisions (e.g., you have more varia- ber of independent and subordinate clauses divided by the
tion [in the] in the sports you can play), and repetitions (e.g., total number of T-units in each sample; and (c) proportion
but [teams] teams are good to play in) were parenthesized of T-units with subordinate clauses that were themselves
and were excluded from the calculation of the mean length embedded inside a subordinate clause (e.g., I think [team
of T-units in words. Reliability of coding for this measure sports are better [because [if you injure yourself] then you’ve
and all other variables of interest followed the protocol set got other people to take your place]].)
out in Nippold, Ward-Lonergan, and Fanning (2005). A Language content. Each language sample was examined
second researcher checked the T-units and mazes to verify for essential elements of persuasive argument, including

Table 4. Definitions and examples of independent and dependent clauses provided by the study participants.

Clause type Definition Example

Independent clause Contains a subject and a main verb and can stand Team sports are more fun.
on its own to express a complete thought.
Subordinate clause Contains a subject and a main verb but must be linked
to an independent clause to express a complete thought.
There are three main types of subordinate clauses:
nominal, adverbial, and relative.
Nominal clause Acts as the subject or more commonly as the object I think (that they are both just the same).
of an independent clause.
Nonfinite clauses were included in the analysis if they You have got to encourage (them to do better).
contained a subject that was different to the subject
in the matrix clause.
Adverbial clause Expresses condition, time, or reason. Team sports are good (because you learn
social ways of doing stuff ).
Relative clause Describes the preceding noun. And then there are sports (that are sort of
individual and team) like car racing.
Describes the preceding sentence. You get to bond with other people (which is
good for your interacting skills).

Moran et al.: Persuasive Discourse in Adolescents 269


number of supporting reasons, number of tangentially related you have a tennis racket and a net). Tangentially related
utterances, and the speaker’s attitude. The definition of utterances also included statements that did not make sense
supporting reasons was adapted from previous research (e.g., pragmatically (e.g., And with a team, it is quite hard to learn
Clark & Delia, 1976; Nippold et al., 2005). Supporting from a team because you don’t know what they are doing),
reasons had to be distinct and were counted only once re- as well as utterances where the stated advantage/disadvantage
gardless of whether they were repeated or not. Furthermore, could be attributed to either team or individual sports (e.g.,
the supporting reason had to serve a persuasive function. That Individual sports like swimming can be good because you
is, it had to state an advantage about individual versus team have to strive at it.)
sports and not simply a fact or statement about sports. For Finally, attitude was scored for whether the discourse
instance, Team sports are better because you have other team expressed a single attitude or a mixed attitude. Adapted from
mates to rely on if you make a mistake would count as a Nippold et al. (2005), attitude was based on whether the
supporting reason. On the other hand, an utterance such as supporting reasons indicated a preference of one type of sport
Rugby is a team sport would not count as a supporting rea- over another or whether the reasons supported both types
son because it does not support an opinion either way. Like- of sport. Although some individuals stated a mixed claim
wise, utterances that stated advantages that could be used to (e.g., I like both), credit was only given to a mixed claim if
argue for either individual or team sports were not credited. the reasons supported both arguments.
For instance, Individual sports like swimming can be good After examining all of the transcripts according to the
because you have to strive at it was not counted as a sup- coding criteria for language content, a second investigator
porting reason because the argument could hold for either double checked each transcript and flagged any disagree-
team or individual sports. Supporting reasons that served a ments with the first investigator. The following levels of
persuasive function were credited regardless of whether or agreement were reached: number of supporting reasons
not they supported the original claim. Examples of different (91%), number of tangentially related utterances (97%),
types of supporting reasons proposed by the participants in and speaker’s attitude (89%). Any disagreements were then
this study are provided in Table 5. resolved to 100% agreement through discussion.
An utterance was coded as tangentially related if it was
related to the topic (i.e., sports) but did not serve a persuasive Statistical Analysis
function. Utterances that were coded as tangentially related
included those that simply described a sport or provided To examine group effects, a t test for independent means
some facts about sports but did nothing to further the speak- (2-tailed) was performed on each factor of interest. When
er’s point of view (e.g., One sport is tennis and with tennis the significance level for Levene’s Test for Equality of

Table 5. Examples of supporting reasons for individual versus team sports provided by the study participants.

Reason Example of reason

Work together to accomplish things You accomplish something that you might not be able to accomplish yourself.
Push each other to do better You don’t want to disappoint your team members.
Share the victory When you are playing cricket, if you hit the ball far enough they pat you on
the back and say well done.
Social activities associated with team sports After the game if you’re older you can have a few drinks.
Interaction with others You get to work with other people.
Variety of roles within a team Get to do different jobs.
Make friends I really like the team because you get to know people and you make lots of
new friends.
Help each other out You can help each other in the game.
Learn things from others Learn skills like working in a team that can make you better in the workforce.
Less pressure In a team sport if you decide not to have the ball anymore you can pass it to
someone else.
Shared responsibility for losing/winning If you fail you know you haven’t worked as hard as the others whereas in a team
it’s not all the blame that falls just on you.
Loneliness In individual sport you practice by yourself so you can end up feeling lonely.
Time on your own Individual is better because I like doing things on my own.
Do it on your own schedule Bike riding you can go as fast as you want to.
Don’t have to listen to anyone else Don’t have to listen to anyone.
Self-reliance You did it by yourself with no one’s help.

270 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 43 • 264–275 • July 2012
Variances was ≤.05, then the version of the t test that does Table 6. Measures of language productivity, syntactic
not assume equal variances was used. An alpha level of .05 complexity, language content, and working memory with
independent t test results.
was used for all statistical tests. In addition, the practical
significance of the results was assessed using Cohen’s
d statistic (Cohen, 1988). Cohen proposed the following Measure ABI AMP t p d
guidelines for the interpretation of effect sizes: small
d = .2, medium d = .5, and large d = .8.
Total number of T-units
M 21.88 23.25 .25 .80 .12
SD 12.90 8.89
RESULTS Range 9–45 11–31

This study compared the performance of adolescents Total length of T-unit in words
M 15.09 13.19 1.11 .30 .55
with ABI and their AMPs on a spoken persuasive discourse SD 4.72 1.16
task. The results for group comparisons on measures of Range 9.6–24.2 11.5–15.0
language productivity, syntactic complexity, language con- Total number of unmazed words
tent, and WM are reported in Table 6. For the measures M 287.75 303.75 .26 .80 .17
of language productivity and syntactic complexity, there SD 135.22 114.28
were no statistically significant group differences except Range 154–580 157–432
for maze words as a proportion of total words. The effect Maze words as proportion of total words
size for this variable was very large, with the group with M 15.63 7.50 2.93 .02 1.47
ABI producing almost twice as many maze words as a SD 7.09 3.34
Range 6–27 2–19
proportion of total words than the AMPs. In addition, very
large effects sizes were obtained for clause density and total Nominal clause usea
M 37.00 22.25 1.14 .27 .56
number of multiple embeddings. Thus, although these re-
SD 36.05 6.06
sults were not statistically significant, there were clinically Range 10–122 14–35
significant group differences for both of these variables.
Adverbial clause usea
Surprisingly, the group with ABI outperformed the AMP M 57.00 46.13 .70 .50 .35
group on measures of clause density and total number of SD 38.0 22.0
multiple embeddings. Thus, as a group, the adolescents Range 27–133 21–82
with ABI produced a greater number of embedded clauses Relative clause usea
per T-unit, and they also produced more T-units that con- M 17.87 9.25 1.41 .19 .71
tained an embedded clause that was itself embedded in SD 15.87 6.84
another clause. Range 0–44 0–24
For the measures of language content, the following Clause density
variables were both statistically and clinically significant: M 2.12 1.77 1.96 .08 1.01
SD .43 .24
total number of supporting reasons and total number of Range 1.59–2.89 1.45–2.18
tangentially related utterances. On average, the AMP
Total number of multiple embeddingsa
group produced more than twice as many supporting
M 19.63 7.75 2.21 .06 1.10
reasons as the group with ABI. Furthermore, the group SD 14.38 4.95
with ABI produced more than twice as many utterances Range 5–38 0–17
that were only tangentially related to the topic. Group Total number of supporting reasons
differences in speaker attitude (mixed vs. single) were M 2.25 5.88 5.09 <.001 2.54
determined using the Fisher Exact Test. The Fisher Exact SD 1.58 1.25
test was chosen because of the small sample size. Al- Range 1–5 4–8
though the AMP group produced more arguments with Total number of tangentially related utterances
a mixed attitude, this difference was not statistically M 12.38 5.25 2.37 .033 1.19
significant (p = .25). SD 6.84 5.06
Range 3–21 1–18
For WM, there was a statistically significant difference
between the two groups on the CLPT, with the AMP group CLPTb
M 24.63 29.13 3.62 .003 1.81
scoring significantly higher than the group with ABI. A large SD 2.92 1.96
effect size was also noted. To determine whether WM was Range 20–28 27–33
related to measures of language productivity, syntactic com-
plexity, and language content, Pearson product–moment a
Reported as percentage of T-units per sample. bCLPT results are
correlation coefficients were calculated using participants’ number of words recalled. No norms were >12;0. Higher scores
scores on the CLPT and their scores on each of the dependent represent greater working memory capacity.

Moran et al.: Persuasive Discourse in Adolescents 271


variables. None of the correlations was statistically signifi- Given that persuasion is a later developing form of
cant at the .05 alpha level (see Table 7). discourse, we might expect that individuals with ABI
would perform more poorly on measures of syntactic
complexity than their typically developing peers. How-
ever, this is not what we found. In fact, the group with ABI
DISCUSSION actually produced greater clause density and more multi-
Persuasive discourse is frequently used in social and ple embeddings than the AMP group, and although these
academic settings and so is of great importance in the study group differences were not statistically significant, the
of adolescent language. The aim of this study was to exam- effect sizes were large. Closer inspection of the data re-
ine the performance of adolescents with ABI on spoken vealed that the group differences in syntactic complexity
persuasive discourse. Specifically, the authors examined were almost entirely due to the responses of a single par-
whether adolescents with ABI differed from AMPs on ticipant who produced multiple productions of one partic-
language productivity, syntactic complexity, and language ular syntactic structure that consisted of a relative clause
content measures when performing a spoken persuasive embedded under an adverbial clause of reason. Some exam-
discourse task. In addition, the relationship between WM ples of this construction produced by this participant in-
and discourse production was explored. clude, You can’t feel left out [cos you’re the one [who’s
doing the paddling]] and You might not get passed the ball
[because they might want to pass it to somebody [who’s
Language Productivity and
better at playing]]. Thus, although one participant with
Syntactic Complexity ABI produced many repetitions of one particular complex
The first question addressed by the study was whether syntactic construction, in general, there was little differ-
adolescents with ABI differed from AMPs on language ence between the two groups on measures of syntactic
productivity and syntactic complexity. As in the majority complexity. The lack of a group difference on this measure
of studies looking at narrative discourse and ABI, we found was particularly perplexing given the low performance
no statistically significant group differences on any of the of the group with ABI on the Written Grammar subtest of
measures of language productivity except for number of the TOAL–3. One explanation for the disparity between
mazes. Consistent with Biddle et al.’s (1996) description of performance on the standardized test and the persuasive
personal narratives following ABI, the group with ABI was task is that standardized tests like the TOAL–3 put a par-
much less efficient at producing persuasive discourse than ticularly large load on memory, and the memory demands
the AMP group. This was evidenced by a significantly greater of these tests can result in reduced performance (Moran
use of mazes, despite having sentences that were of similar & Gillon, 2004; Turkstra & Holland, 1998).
length.
Language Content
Table 7. Pearson product–moment correlation coefficients
calculated between working memory and measures of The second question addressed by this study was whether
language productivity, syntactic complexity, and language adolescents with ABI differed from AMPs on language
content (N = 16). content measures in a spoken discourse task. In order to
investigate this research question, participants’ discourse
was coded for the number of supporting reasons as well as
Correlation the number of tangentially related reasons produced. The
Dependent variable coefficient p value
speaker’s attitude as to whether team sports or individual
sports were better was also evaluated. The findings revealed
Total number of T-units –.160 .553 significant differences between the two groups in the number of
Total length of T-unit in words –.365 .165 supporting reasons and the number of tangentially related
Total number of unmazed words –.262 .327
reasons. There were no group differences as to whether a single
Maze words as proportion –.212 .431
of total words or mixed attitude was expressed.
Nominal clause usea –.478 .061 The results for the language content measures are con-
Adverbial clause usea –.192 .476 sistent with other discourse production studies that have
Relative clause usea –.023 .934 shown that individuals with ABI generally produce less
Clause density –.478 .061
content than typically developing peers (e.g., Chapman,
Total number of multiple embeddingsa –.373 .155
Total number of supporting reasons .387 .138 1997; Coelho, 2002). The current study extends previous
Total number of tangentially –.152 .575 findings to show that individuals with ABI attempt to
related utterances provide sufficient information when producing a persua-
sive argument, but the information put forward is often
a
Reported as percentage of T-units per sample. irrelevant. Hence, the individuals with ABI attempt to fulfil

272 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 43 • 264–275 • July 2012
the expectations of a persuasive argument by providing rea- limits with respect to their spoken persuasive discourse
sons, but these reasons do not always support their point of view. skills. Therefore, a normative sample of spoken persuasive
discourse across the age ranges is warranted.
WM and Discourse Topic choice has also been shown to affect the quantity
and quality of discourse that is produced (e.g., Kroll, 1984).
The third question addressed whether WM was associ- It is thought that topics with a clear purpose that are
ated with performance on a spoken discourse task in adoles- of interest to the producer may be more likely to produce
cents with and without ABI. The group with ABI scored output of high quality. It is possible that participants did not
significantly more poorly on the CLPT, a measure of WM, perceive a clear purpose or interest in the topic of whether
suggesting that they have reduced WM capacity. Despite they preferred individual or team sports. In fact, most par-
the group differences in overall WM capacity, however, WM ticipants provided reasons why both were good, so it would
did not appear to influence performance on the spoken dis- appear that the participants had no strong feelings either way.
course task. This is in contrast to Hay and Moran (2005), Alternatively, a topic such as “cruelty to animals” might
where WM was significantly correlated with expository be more likely to elicit strong opinions and a greater variety
discourse production. The differences may be due in part to of arguments.
the different type of discourse being evaluated (expository Another aspect of task elicitation worth considering is the
vs. persuasive). However, it is more likely that task elicita- specific modality. Written discourse often results in the use
tion factors affected the outcome. In the Hay and Moran of more formal, sophisticated syntactic structures than is
study, the task was an expository retelling task, whereas in typically warranted for spoken discourse (Scott, 2004). For
the current study, it was a generation task. example, Scott (2004) reported that children used more
Another potential explanation for the lack of correlation adverbial clauses in a written version of discourse than in
between WM and production is that the demands of the task a spoken version. Therefore, the written modality may be
may not have exceeded the WM capacity of the participants more effective in finding differences across populations.
with ABI. One reason that the task may have been “easy” for Written discourse also tends to be a more challenging modal-
participants with ABI is that the topic was highly familiar ity in terms of processing demands and so may better high-
(individual vs. team sports) and therefore did not challenge light any influence of WM (Scott, 2004).
the participants to draw on other knowledge. With regard Finally, examination of a broader range of cognitive and
to language processing, it has been shown that topics that executive functions and discourse production is warranted.
are highly familiar are less taxing on WM. For instance, In a study of cognitive influences on persuasive discourse,
Yekovick, Walker, Ogle, and Thompson (1990) demon- Nippold and Ward-Lonergan (2010) found that verbal ana-
strated that domain knowledge was related to the quick- logical reason was associated with number of reasons, length
ness and ease of generating inferences in spoken discourse. of utterance in words, and a flexible attitude. In addition,
Finally, it is possible that the lack of correlation could be Douglas (2010) described a range of executive functions that
due to the small sample size. Large population studies that were associated with conversational abilities, including topic
examine both WM and persuasion are warranted. maintenance and relevance.
Persuasive discourse is a widely employed genre, and
an individual’s ability to use it well may determine whether
Future Research
or not he or she succeeds in school, employment, and/or
The purpose of this study was to compare the spoken socially. Future research is required in this important area
persuasive discourse of adolescents with ABI and their so that professionals who work with individuals with ABI
typically developing peers. There are a number of influences can better help their students to express themselves more
that require consideration both in interpreting the findings effectively.
of the current study and in directing future research. Factors
that have already been shown to influence persuasive dis-
course production include age of production, topic, moti-
vation of the speaker, and modality. In this study, age of
REFERENCES
production was restricted to adolescence. Although there
have been a few studies that looked at how persuasion Accident Compensation Corporation. (2010). Active rehabil-
changes over time (e.g., Nippold et al., 2005), a larger body itation services for traumatic brain injury. Retrieved from
of normative data across the age range would provide a http:/www.acc.co.nz.
stronger base for determining the performance of clinical Baddeley, A. D. (1976). The psychology of memory. New York,
populations. In the current study, a closely matched peer NY: Basic Books.
group was sought so that educational level and socio- Bartsch, K., London, K., & Campbell, M. D. (2007). Children’s
economic status were similar. However, it is unknown attention to beliefs in interactive persuasion tasks. Developmental
whether the peer group would be considered within normal Psychology, 43, 111–120.

Moran et al.: Persuasive Discourse in Adolescents 273


Biddle, K. R., McCabe, A., & Bliss, L. S. (1996). Narrative Douglas, J. (2010). Relation of executive functioning to pragmatic
skills following traumatic brain injury in children and adults. outcome following severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of
Journal of Communication Disorders, 29(6), 446–469. Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 365–382.
Blosser, J. L., & dePompeii, R. (2003). Pediatric traumatic Ewing-Cobbs, L., Brookshire, B., Scott, M. A., & Fletcher,
brain injury: Proactive intervention. New York, NY: Delmar J. M. (1998). Children’s narratives following traumatic brain
Learning. injury: Linguistic structure, cohesion, and thematic recall.
Brookshire, B. L., Chapman, S. B., Song, J., & Levin, H. S. Brain and Language, 61, 395–419.
(2000). Cognitive and linguistic correlates of children’s dis- Ewing-Cobbs, L., Fletcher, J. M., Levin, H. S., & Eisenberg,
course after closed head injury: A three-year follow-up. H. M. (1987). Language functions following closed head injury
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical and Experi-
741–751. mental Neuropsychology, 9, 575–592.
Cain, K., Bryant, P., & Oakhill, J. (2004). Children’s reading Felton, M., & Kuhn, D. (2001). The development of argumentive
comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working discourse skill. Discourse Processes, 32, 135–153.
memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Gaulin, C. A., & Campbell, T. F. (1994). Procedure for assessing
Educational Psychology, 96, 31–42. verbal working memory in normal school-age children: Some
Chapman, S. B. (1997). Cognitive-communication abilities in preliminary data. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79, 55–64.
children with closed head injury. American Journal of Speech- Hagen, C. (1998). The Rancho Levels of Cognitive Functioning:
Language Pathology, 6(2), 50–58. The Revised Levels—Third Edition. Downy, CA: Rancho Los
Chapman, S. B., Culhane, K. A., Levin, H. S., Harward, H., Amigos Medical Center.
Mendelsohn, D., Ewing-Cobbs, L., . . . Bruce, D. (1992). Hammill, D. D., Brown, V. L., Larsen, S. C., & Wiederholt, J. L.
Narrative discourse after closed head-injury in children and (1994). Test of Adolescent and Adult Language—Third Edition.
adolescents. Brain and Language, 43(1), 42–65. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Chapman, S. B., Gamino, J. F., Cook, L. G., Hanten, G., Li, X., Hay, E., & Moran, C. (2005). Discourse formulation in children
& Levin, H. S. (2006). Impaired discourse gist and working with closed head injury. American Journal of Speech-Language
memory in children after brain injury. Brain and Language, Pathology, 14, 324–336.
97, 178–188. Jordan, F. M., Cannon, A., & Murdoch, B. E. (1992). Lan-
Chapman, S. B., Watkins, R., Gustafoson, C., Moore, S., guage abilities of mildly closed head injured (CHI) children
Levin, H. S., & Kufera, J. A. (1997). Narrative discourse 10 years post-injury. Brain Injury, 6(1), 39–44.
in children with closed head injury: Children with language Jordan, F. M., & Murdoch, B. E. (1994). Severe closed head
impairment and typically developing children. American Journal injury in childhood: Linguistic outcomes into adulthood.
of Speech-Language Pathology, 6(2), 66–76. Brain Injury, 8(6), 501–508.
Clark, R. A., & Delia, J. G. (1976). The development of func- Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of
tional persuasive skill in childhood and early adolescence. comprehension: Individual differences in working memory.
Child Development, 1, 1008–1014. Psychological Review, 99(1), 122–149.
Coelho, C. A. (1995). Discourse production deficits following Kellogg, R. T. (2004). Working memory components in written
traumatic brain injury: A critical review of the recent literature. sentence generation. American Journal of Psychology, 117(3),
Aphasiology, 9(5), 409–429. 341–361.
Coelho, C. A. (2002). Story narratives of adults with closed head Knudson, R. E. (1992). The development of written argumenta-
injury and non-brain-injured adults: Influence of socioeconomic tion: An analysis and comparison of argumentative writing at
status, elicitation task, and executive functioning. Journal of four grade levels. Child Study Journal, 22, 167–184.
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 1232–1248. Kroll, B. W. (1984). Audience adaptation in children’s persuasive
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural letters. Written Communication, 1, 407–427.
sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Langlois, J. A., Rutland-Brown, W., & Thomas, K. E. (2005).
Crowhurst, M. (1990). Teaching and learning the writing of The incidence of traumatic brain injury among children in the
persuasive/argumentative discourse. Canadian Journal of United States: Differences by race. Journal of Head Trauma
Education/Revue canadienne de l’éducation, 15, 348–359. Rehabilitation, 20(3), 229–238.
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences Light, L., & Anderson, P. A. (1985). Working memory capacity,
in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning age, and memory for discourse. Journal of Gerontology, 40,
and Verbal Behaviour, 19, 450–466. 737–747.
Dennis, M. (1989). Language and the young damaged brain. In Mar, R. A. (2004). The neuropsychology of narrative: Story
T. Boll & B. K. Bryant (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology and comprehension, story production and their interrelation.
brain function: Research, measurement and practice (pp. 89–123). Neuropsychologia, 42, 1414–1434.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. McCutcheon, D. (1996). A capacity theory of writing: Working
Devito, J. A. (2003). The essential elements of public speaking. memory in text composition. Educational Psychology Review,
Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. 8, 299–335.

274 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 43 • 264–275 • July 2012
Miller, J. F., & Chapman, R. S. (2010). SALT: Systematic Rappaport, M. (2005). The Disability Rating and Coma/Near-
Analysis of Language Transcripts [Computer software]. Madison, Coma Scales in evaluating severe head injury. Neuropsycholog-
WI: University of Wisconsin–Madison, Waisman Center, ical Rehabilitation, 15, 442–453.
Language Analysis Laboratory. Scott, C. M. (2004). Syntactic ability in children and adolescents
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., with language and learning disabilities. In R. Berman (Ed.),
Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity Language development across childhood and adolescence
of executive functions and their contributions to complex (Vol. 3, pp. 111–133). Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.
“frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Scott, C. M., & Windsor, J. (2000). General language perfor-
Psychology, 41, 49–100. mance measures in spoken and written narrative and expository
Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (1999). Models of working memory: discourse of school-age children with language learning dis-
Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. abilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press. 43, 324–339.
Moran, C., & Gillon, G. T. (2004). Language and memory Semel, W., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2003). Clinical
profiles of adolescents with traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, Fourth Edition.
18(3), 273–288. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
New Zealand Qualifications Authority. (2007). Level 1 Turkstra, L. S. (1999). Language testing in adolescents with brain
English examination: Produce formal writing. Retrieved from injury: A consideration of the CELF–3. Language, Speech,
http://www.nzqa.govt.nz/ncea/assessment. and Hearing Services in Schools, 30, 132–140.
Nippold, M. A. (2007). Later language development: School-age Turkstra, L. (2000). Should my shirt be tucked in or left out?
children, adolescents, and young adults (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: The communication context of adolescence. Aphasiology, 14,
Pro-Ed. 349–364.
Nippold, M. A., Hesketh, L. J., Duthie, J. K., & Mansfield, T. C. Turkstra, L. S., & Holland, A. (1998). Assessment of syntax
(2005). Conversational versus expository discourse: A study after adolescent brain injury: Effects of memory on test per-
of syntactic development in children, adolescents, and adults. formance. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 41, 137–149.
1048–1064. Yekovich, F. R., Walker, C. H., Ogle, L. T., & Thompson, M. A.
Nippold, M. A., & Ward-Lonergan, J. M. (2010). Argumenta- (1990). The influence of domain knowledge on inferencing in
tive writing in pre-adolescents: The role of verbal reasoning. low-aptitude individuals. In A. C. Grasser & G. H. Bower (Eds.),
Child Language Teaching & Therapy, 26, 238–248. Inferences and text comprehension (pp. 259–277). San Diego,
Nippold, M. A., Ward-Lonergan, J. M., & Fanning, J. L. CA: Academic Press.
(2005). Persuasive writing in children, adolescents, and adults: Youse, K. M., & Coelho, C. A. (2005). Working memory and
A study of syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic development. discourse production abilities following closed-head injury.
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 125–138. Brain Injury, 19, 1001–1009.

Moran et al.: Persuasive Discourse in Adolescents 275


Copyright of Language, Speech & Hearing Services in Schools is the property of American Speech-Language-
Hearing Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

You might also like