Emotion Regulation To Salud y Adaptación

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 23

Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624

DOI 10.1007/s11031-016-9553-y

ORIGINAL PAPER

When context matters: Negative emotions predict psychological


health and adjustment
Karin G. Coifman1 • Jessica J. Flynn1 • Lavinia A. Pinto1

Published online: 2 April 2016


Ó Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Abstract Functional theories of emotion argue for the Introduction


adaptive function of negative emotions in response to
specific contextual or environmental demands. However, Functional models of emotion strongly emphasize the link
data supporting these theories in community samples is between emotions and adaptation, such that adaptive
limited and much research has suggested the opposite: emotional responses are those that meet the particular
negative emotions predict poor adjustment. To begin to demands of a given ‘‘incentive context’’ (Goldsmith and
address this discrepancy, we tested the functional associ- Davidson 2004, p. 363). This seems particularly true for
ation between negative emotion and psychological health certain negative emotions (e.g., anger, fear, sadness, dis-
and adjustment across three diverse samples: adults in gust) most tightly linked to contextual factors in order to
intimate-partnerships, patients with chronic illness, and serve clear survival functions (Ekman 1992). For example,
first-year college students. In each study we employed lab- fear is highly adaptive in the presence of a real threat (c.f.
based methods to elicit and index emotion as a multi-di- Ohman and Mineka 2001). Fear evokes physiological
mensional response system and considered contextual changes that augment threat responses (e.g., enhanced
factors and the theorized or demonstrated function of attention and perception: Phelps et al. 2006) as well as
negative emotions in that context and in relation to specific corresponding behavioral signals that are adaptive for the
outcomes. Data analysis revealed that contextually sensi- larger group (e.g., recognition of facial expressions of fear
tive negative emotion was adaptive, and associated with across cultures and species: Russell 1991; Bloom and
better relationship adjustment and related behaviors (Study Friedman 2013). However, patterns of contextually insen-
1), higher treatment adherence (Study 2), and adaptive sitive or over-generalized fear responses are maladaptive,
responses to peer rejection (Study 3). Across samples, and characteristic of certain psychiatric populations (e.g.,
circumstances, and outcomes, negative emotions were in anxiety disorders: Graham and Milad 2011). Indeed, the
positively associated with psychological health and assumption that negative emotional responses are adaptive
adjustment. in certain contexts and not in others underlies dominant
theories of emotion, emotion regulation (Cole et al. 1994;
Keywords Negative emotion  Adjustment  Stress  John and Gross 2004), and current models of psychiatric
Context  Health disease (Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins 2011).
Testing the link between negative emotion, context, and
indicators of psychological health has proven complicated.
First, the rapid production and resolution of emotional
responding demands in vivo measurement (c.f. Rosenberg
1998). Second, emotions are multi-dimensional with emo-
tion-related behavior, autonomic activity, and reported
& Karin G. Coifman
experience only ‘‘loosely coupled’’ (Bonanno and Keltner
kcoifman@kent.edu
2004). Indeed, emotion response modes may serve differ-
1
Kent State University, Kent, OH, USA ing functions, operate on very different time scales (Bulteel

123
Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624 603

et al. 2014), and be under varying influences often not beneficial and associated with health (see also Schwarz and
accessible to the individual. For example, self-reported Clore 1983; Salovey et al. 1995).
emotion is particularly vulnerable to influences that have Despite the broader evidence, there remains the chal-
been well-documented (e.g., demand characteristics: lenge that in most samples, negative emotions are largely
Dubitsky et al. 1993; dispositional differences: Coifman associated with poor outcomes. For example, those same
et al. 2007; environmental circumstances: Messner and psychiatric populations that are consistently low on indices
Wanke 2011; perceived stress: Zautra 2006; attachment: of emotion awareness often, in fact typically, report high
Zimmerman et al. 2001). Third, many factors influence levels of negative emotion (c.f., Barlow et al. 2014). This
the tie between emotional responses, context, and a given could be simply a measurement-related issue. For example,
outcome (e.g., some responses may have short-term ben- patients may confound different levels of affective phe-
efit but may be functionally maladaptive in relation to nomena, reporting on enduring negative mood rather than
longer-term indicators: Bonanno and Burton 2013). Fi- moment-level negative emotion. Affective phenomena are
nally, individuals differ considerably in their appraisal of highly complex, involving multiple levels and hierarchical
emotional contexts (c.f., Lerner and Keltner 2001) systems, and emotion episodes are but the briefest of these
including in particular negative emotional contexts. This phenomena, frequently passing outside of awareness
can translate to different negative emotional responses, (LeDoux 2012). Indeed, there is growing evidence of
such as the benefit of challenge rather than threat (or important variability in the capacity to attend to emotional
optimism versus pessimism) appraisals resulting in anger experiences (Kashdan et al. 2015). As such, negative
versus fear responses during aversive lab contexts (Lerner emotions are often poorly differentiated and other endur-
et al. 2007). ing, and likely more obvious phenomena, such as mood, is
Given these complexities, identifying a clear association readily available.
between a given negative emotional response and adjust- An additional, and perhaps more pressing, issue relates
ment requires careful tailoring of outcome indicators to the to the functional matching of negative emotions to their
emotions in question and their presumed population- and eliciting context, or the question of emotion context sen-
context-specific functions. Consequently, the evidence sitivity. Functional theories have been interpreted to posit
supporting functional theories of negative emotion has that negative emotions are functionally adaptive in their
been largely limited to highly controlled experimental associated contexts (e.g., fear in the presence of a real
designs, demonstrating the functionality of a negative threat; Ekman 1992). Obviously, when the context is nar-
emotion to proximal outcomes (e.g., performance on goal- row and discrete it may be easier to identify the function-
oriented tasks). In fact, there is scant evidence of the ally optimal emotional response. Less discrete contexts are
adaptive function of negative emotions in real-world or more likely to elicit a range of emotional responses,
community samples (for key exceptions: Lerner et al. 2003; modified by prior experiences and/or individual differences
McNulty and Russell 2010). (e.g. Lerner et al. 2003, 2007). Accordingly, there is an
increasing demand for research to focus on emotion and
What constitutes an adaptive negative emotion? emotion-related processing in relation to specific contex-
tual factors (Aldao 2013; McNulty and Fincham 2012).
Negative emotional responses are complex integrations of Indeed, there is a small but developing literature examining
biological signals that, when considered together, support emotion context sensitivity: the ability to generate emotion
the hypothesis that negative emotions are largely adaptive. responses that are adaptive within a given context as well
For example, there is convincing evidence demonstrating as flexibly shift responses across contexts (c.f., Coifman
the critical role of somatic manifestations of negative and Bonanno 2009). These studies have demonstrated that
emotion in effective decision making and risk management negative emotions expressed in positive or neutral contexts
(e.g., Bechara and Damasio 2005). Indeed, there is clear (contextually-insensitive negative emotion) predict poor
evidence of discrete functions for emotions such as fear long-term outcomes (e.g., Coifman and Bonanno 2010;
(Ohman and Mineka 2001), sadness (Bonanno et al. 2008), Buss et al. 2004). More importantly, there is also evidence
anger (Lerner and Keltner 2001), and disgust (Tybur et al. that weak contextually-sensitive negative emotional
2013) to facilitate survival as well as social living. More- responses may be equally problematic and predict the
over, low levels of negative emotion awareness or inability development and persistence of psychopathology (Rotten-
to discriminate emotion from bodily sensations (e.g., berg et al. 2002). Together, this evidence suggests that
alexithymia: Bagby et al. 1994) are particularly prevalent more contextually sensitive negative emotion is better than
in groups with psychiatric disorders (c.f., Kashdan et al. less.
2015). These findings support the concept that higher Indeed, recent research has demonstrated that contex-
negative emotion awareness could be psychologically tually sensitive negative emotion (or greater negative

123
604 Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624

emotion in explicit negative contexts) is associated with respect to their prediction of psychological health for that
adaptive health behavior, including higher treatment population.
adherence (Harvey et al. 2014) and greater physical In Study 1, we examined responses to films depicting
activity or exercise (Shields et al. 2015). However, largely, either discretely negative or positive (e.g., conflict or
the ECS literature has not tested these associations for reunion) relational themes in individuals engaged in long-
specific negative emotions. Indeed, evidence for the func- term, intimate-partnerships. Emotion responses were
tional connection between specific negative emotion, an examined in relation to established indicators of relation-
eliciting context, and broader psychological functioning in ship adjustment and behavior. In Study 2, we narrowed our
humans has been largely absent. focus on to the specific emotion sadness in chronically-ill
Here, we sought to narrow the gap between theories of adults, elicited during a film depicting loss which followed
negative emotion and existing evidence in community an interview about their illness. Sadness was selected
samples by testing the functional benefit of negative because of its relevance to this particular sample and its
emotions to psychological health and adjustment across association with factors that influence treatment adherence
three samples in distinct circumstances: intimate relation- (e.g., increased problem solving).
ships, chronic illness, and adjustment to college. Our In contrast, in Study 3 we employed a more naturalistic
overarching hypothesis was that negative contextually- emotion elicitation by simulating peer-rejection in first-
sensitive emotion would be functionally adaptive in rela- year college students via a within-subjects adaptation of the
tion to particular indicators of outcome. Specifically, that Cyberball paradigm (Williams et al. 2000). In this case,
negative emotions can be functionally adaptive if they are there were still pre-defined target emotions (e.g., fear,
contextually sensitive and they are adaptive with respect to anger, sadness, happiness) established in prior research but
a particular outcome that they can influence. As such, we the potential for individual variation in emotional respon-
strove to elicit and measure emotions linked to the unique ses due to differences in appraisal was substantially greater.
sample circumstances, the context, and the particular index As such, the target emotions would not necessarily all be
of psychological adjustment. functionally adaptive. Indeed, in Study 3, we not only
In all studies we assessed emotional responses in real- broadened our elicitation paradigm but also our test in
time, across response dimensions, and in relation to mul- relation to outcomes, considering both short-term or
tiple outcome indicators. Finally, we examined predicted proximal functions as well as long-term or distal functions.
emotion responses in concert with other possible emotions This final test constitutes a more ecologically valid and
to provide a more rigorous test of our hypotheses. For perhaps rigorous next step, as a wider range of emotion
example, our assessments always included positive emo- responses could be elicited that might be functionally
tion due to evidence demonstrating that positive emotions adaptive in a variety of ways. Indeed, given the importance
are both frequently evident and highly adaptive in negative of peer-relationships when adjusting to college, we antic-
emotional contexts. In particular, positive emotions have ipated that some negative emotion responses would be
been shown to both regulate negative emotional responses quite important and would have differing functionality or
as well as facilitate longer term goals (e.g., Papa and benefits depending on the indicator of outcome.
Bonanno 2008; c.f., Fredrickson 1998). Given the mea-
surement and elicitation paradigms used (i.e. indexing real-
time emotion for several minutes) we thought it likely that Study 1: Negative emotion and intimate-partner-
positive emotions would emerge and that mixed emotional relationships
responses could be adaptive (e.g., Larsen et al. 2001).
In the first two studies we focused on eliciting emotional Emotions arise between romantic partners and serve criti-
responses in discrete, unambiguous contexts defined using cal functions by communicating internal states and inten-
standardized film stimuli. Given the large literature tions (Keltner et al. 2006). Indeed, there are many that
demonstrating how individual differences in appraisal argue that intimate partnerships are both strongly influ-
influence emotion responses (c.f., Smith and Ellsworth enced by as well as do strongly influence emotional pro-
1985), we strove to limit individual variability in appraisal cessing (Shaver and Mikulincer 2008; Zaki and Williams
by using well-validated emotion eliciting film clips. Thus, 2013). There has been considerable research attention
in both Study 1 and 2, contextually sensitive negative focused on emotions in relationships, documenting the
emotion would be, by definition, the established target sometimes detrimental effect of negative emotions,
responses to each clip (e.g., sadness, in response to The including hostility, distress, and contempt (e.g., Gottman
Champ in Study 2). In order to evaluate if these were et al. 1998; c.f. Gottman 1994). More recently, however,
functionally adaptive negative emotions, we examined clip literature has begun to demonstrate not only the functional
target responses (as well as non-target responses) with relevance of negative emotions within relationships but

123
Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624 605

also the importance of considering contextual factors (e.g., at least six months. Informed consent was obtained for all
McNulty and Russell 2010). In particular, there is evidence participants and all research activity was approved by the
suggesting that contextually sensitive negative emotional Institutional Review Board overseeing human subjects
responses can be adaptive and highly relevant to relation- research. One participant was excluded because he was
ship functioning. For example, Waldinger et al. (2004) older than the age mean by seven standard deviations. The
demonstrated that male expressions of distress during mean relationship length was 29 months (SD = 38). Forty-
couple conflict discussions predicted better marital adjust- one percent of the sample were in a long distance rela-
ment. Whereas, Graber et al. (2011) found that expressions tionship, defined as greater than 30-min drive for in-person
of contempt during ‘‘love’’ interactions predicted the contact.
opposite. Participation included completion of questionnaires
Our goal here was to build upon this developing litera- followed by an emotion-modulation task in which partici-
ture by examining negative emotion generated during dis- pants were asked to engage emotionally with a series of
crete negative contexts created by films depicting core brief film clips. Participants were video-taped to obtain
relational themes (e.g., conflict, loss; Mikulincer et al. facial behavior and provided reports of emotional experi-
2002). This was specifically investigated in individuals in ence following each film. Following the lab-task, 52 par-
long-term, intimate-partnerships in relation to patterns of ticipants1 (66 %) also completed a relationship interview in
relational behavior and ratings of relationship adjustment. which they reported on the frequency of relationship-re-
Indeed, there is considerable theory suggesting the lated behaviors in the past 7 days. Upon completion of the
importance of negative emotional expression and related study, 60 participants received course credit and 19
responsiveness in intimate relationships, beginning in early received $25. Paid participants were older, in longer rela-
childhood and throughout life (Magai 1999). This includes tionships, and reported greater negative affect. As such,
both expressions of negative emotions that elicit responses, compensation type was examined in all analyses.
as well as responsiveness to those expressions (Keltner
et al. 2006; Magai and MacFadden 1995). Measures
Broadly, we expected that negative emotions elicited in
negative contexts (here films depicting negative relational Relationship adjustment Participants completed the
themes) would be positively associated with relationship Dyadic Adjustment Scale, a well-validated 32-item self-
adjustment indicators. Our study builds upon recent evi- report measure of relationship adjustment among couples
dence suggesting the relevance of emotion-context match- (DAS; Spanier 1976). An overall relationship adjustment
ing in adult relationships (e.g., Waldinger et al. 2004). We score was calculated according to Spanier (1976) and the
deliberately included positive films that might also activate mean for this sample was M = 2.30; SD = .90 (a = .92).
attachment-related emotion through representation of
reunion-related themes (e.g., Mikulincer et al. 2001) so as to Positive relationship behaviors Following the lab task,
test the importance of contextually-matched negative participants were seated across from a research assistant
emotion. Moreover, we considered other contextual factors and asked to think over their interactions and experiences
known to influence relationship adjustment, including the with their partner over the past 7 days. They were handed a
length and type of relationship (e.g., long-distance) as well calendar, and asked to report on the frequency of each in-
as the presence of psychopathology which can influence not person contact with their partner as well as three specific
only relationship adjustment but also negative emotional behaviors for each day, beginning with the previous day
responses (e.g., depression: Davila et al. 2003). Accord- and working back in time by day, to seven days prior.
ingly, our hypothesis was that negative emotional responses Participants reported the number of times they scheduled
generated discretely in response to negative films depicting activities with their partner, the number of times they
conflict and loss would be positively associated with rela- engaged in an activity with their partner, and the number of
tionship adjustment and related behaviors. times they laughed with their partner. Frequencies for each
behavior were summed by day and then averaged across
Methods the 7 days, yielding one score reflecting mean positive
relationship behavior per day, M = 3.11, SD = 2.51. The
Participants and procedure frequency of in-person contact was left as a control
1
Participants were 79 adult individuals (71 % female, age This interview was added to the study after data collection had
already commenced, as such it was not available to the first 27
M = 22, SD = 6 years, 82 % Caucasian, 94 % non-His-
participants. Analysis of the individuals who had and had not
panic), recruited from a Midwestern university community participated in the interview revealed no demographic, relationship or
in an intimate heterosexual or homosexual relationship for emotion-related differences.

123
606 Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624

variable for these analyses given that some partnerships degree of negative emotional expressiveness and degree of
were long-distance. positive emotional expressiveness. Overall coder agree-
ment was satisfactory (average ICC = .84) and scores
Depression symptoms Participants completed the Center were averaged across coders by participant to increase
for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) a reliability further. There was an equipment malfunction for
commonly used, well-validated assessment of depressive one participant, so facial behavior was only available for
symptomatology (a = 0.80; Radloff 1977). Mean CES-D 78 participants. Negative and positive affect and facial
was: 10.19 (SD = 7.43). responses were combined across the two negative films and
two positive films to create a more robust indicator of
Emotion assessment responses to negative or positive relational themes. This
yielded 4 indices of emotion to negative films and the same
Participants were asked to engage emotionally with a series 4 indices to positive films: negative affect, positive affect,
of brief film clips: one initial neutral film (‘‘Big Cat Dia- negative facial behavior, positive facial behavior.
ries’’; 2010, BBC Earth) to adjust to the viewing paradigm
and camera and four well-validated films for inducing Results
specific negative and positive emotions (Gross and
Levenson 1995; Gilman et al. 2016). We employed a Preliminary analyses
sequence of video stimuli that alternated negative with
positive film clips in order to maximize the discreteness of Emotion data were examined to ensure that participants
each negative context and place greater demands on par- responded to each film context. As expected, participants
ticipants to shift emotion responses accordingly. In addi- reported significantly greater negative affect, M = 2.81
tion, the clips were selected for their explicit interpersonal SD = 1.04, relative to positive affect, M = 1.65 SD = .58,
content and depiction of themes, including conflict and t(78) = 8.43 p \ .000, and showed greater negative facial
loss, central in relationships (i.e., conflict between inmates behavior, M = 3.13 SD = 1.00, relative to positive facial
and guards: ‘‘The Road to Guantanamo Bay’’ 2006 Revo- behavior, M = 1.08 SD = .17, t(77) = 17.87, p \ .000, in
lution Films; joy at being rescued by friends: ‘‘Alive’’ the negative films and greater positive affect, M = 3.57
1993, Paramount Pictures; sadness at the loss of a parent: SD = .94, relative to negative affect, M = 1.25 SD = .35,
‘‘The Champ’’ 1979, Metro-Goldwyn Mayer; and shared t(78) = -23.32, p \ .000, and showed greater positive
humor between peers: ‘‘Between Two Ferns’’ 2010, www. facial behavior, M = 3.02 SD = .76, relative to negative
funnyordie.com). Participants were videotaped and fol- facial behavior, M = 1.91 SD = .60, t(77) = -8.92,
lowing the viewing of each film, completed affect ratings p \ .000, in the positive films. We examined concordance
during a 2 min break. Film stimuli were displayed on a across dimensions of emotion using bivariate correlations.
23-inch monitor at a viewing distance of 2 feet. The task Negative affect was associated with negative facial behavior
was programmed in E-prime 2.0. during the negative films (r = .31, p = .003), and positive
affect was associated with positive facial behavior during the
Affect Participants rated negative (fear, sadness, distress, positive films (r = .40, p \ .000). See Table 1 for bi-variate
guilt, anger, disgust) and positive (interest, relief, enjoyment, correlations between all key variables.
happiness, amusement, affection) emotion words on a scale
from 1 (none) to 7 (strong) after each film. Self-ratings were Primary analyses: Negative emotions and relationship
averaged by film to derive overall negative affect (negative adjustment
films a = .85; positive films a = .76) and positive affect
scores (negative films a = .75; positive films a = .83). We conducted two OLS regression analyses with rela-
tionship adjustment (DAS) as the dependent variable. In
Facial behavior Negative and positive emotional facial Step 1 we entered factors likely to influence either emotion
behaviors were coded by five research assistants blind to and/or relationship functioning. These included age, gen-
hypotheses and all study details. Coders received limited der, depression, as well as length and type (long distance v.
training2 and each coded videos independently on a not) of relationship, and amount of in-person contact in the
23-inch monitor without sound, rating participant facial previous week. We did also initially control for compen-
expressions for every film on 7-point Likert scales for sation type, race and ethnicity but these variables did not
meaningfully impact the results, so they were excluded. In
2 Step 2, we entered expressed negative and positive facial
Increasing evidence suggests that facial coding by relatively naı̈ve
coders may be as valid as highly trained coders (e.g., Dondi et al. behavior (Table 2: Regression A) or reported negative and
2007; Waldinger et al. 2004). positive affect (Table 2: Regression B) from both negative

123
Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624 607

Table 1 Study 1 bi-variate correlations between key study variables


1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12.

1. Length of Relationship –
2. In-Person Contact .03 –
3. Depression Symptoms -.02 -.29 –
4. Neg. Expression—Neg. Films -.18 .01 .27 –
5. Pos. Expression—neg. Films .11 -.16 -.11 -.06 –
6. Neg. Expression—pos. Films -.09 .15 .15 2.50 -.13 –
7. Pos. Expression—pos. Films .02 2.44 .10 .28 .27 2.32 –
8. Neg. Reported Emotion—neg. Films -.00 .03 .13 .33 -.01 .03 .11 –
9. Pos. Reported Emotion—neg. Films .02 .08 -.09 -.21 .16 -.03 -.15 -.05 –
10. Neg. Reported Emotion—pos. Films .11 .36 -.07 .14 .20 .08 .16 .51 .23 –
11. Pos. Reported Emotion—pos. Films -.11 .14 -.12 .20 .10 -.16 .40 .51 .25 .44 –
12. Dyadic Adjustment Scale Score 2.56 -.07 2.35 .16 -.05 .08 .05 -.07 .08 -.07 .15
13. Positive Relationship Behaviors -.28 -.01 -.16 .17 -.07 .22 -.03 .14 -.03 -.07 .14 .47
Significant associations are indicated in bold

and positive film contexts. We entered negative and posi- results also supported our hypothesis. Higher negative
tive indicators of each dimension of emotion together in emotion facial behavior, exhibited only in the negative film
order to control for their shared variance. Moreover, we context, was significantly associated with higher mean
entered together responses from both emotional contexts frequency of daily positive relationship behaviors b = .39,
(negative and positive films) in order to provide a rigorous t = 2.23, p = .03, sr2 = 0.08; F(10, 40) = 2.58, p = .016,
test of our hypothesis and to test if negative emotions R2 = .39, when controlling for other emotional responding,
uniquely in negative contexts are adaptive. The results of relationship variables, and depression (which did margin-
these analyses supported our hypothesis. Indeed, higher ally predict fewer positive behaviors b = -.25, t = -1.78,
negative emotion facial behavior, exhibited only in the p = .08, sr2 = 0.05) (see Table 3: Regression A). More-
negative film context, was significantly associated with over, we found that higher reports of negative emotion in
higher relationship functioning b = .37, t = 2.40, p = .02, the negative film contexts also appeared to predict higher
sr2 = 0.06; F(10, 63) = 2.86, p = .01, R2 = .31, even daily positive relationship behaviors. However when we
when controlling for all other emotional responding, rela- corrected the threshold alpha for multiple tests, employing
tionship variables, and depression (which did also signifi- a False Discovery Rate Calculation (Benjamini and
cantly predict DAS scores b = -.47, t = -4.19, p = .00, Hochberg 1995, 2000) the results did not reach signifi-
sr2 = 0.19). Moreover, we also found that higher positive cance, b = .36, t = 1.77, p = .08, sr2 = 0.05; F(10,
emotion facial behavior exhibited only in the negative film 41) = 2.37, p = .03, R2 = .37 (see Table 3: Regression
context (a contextually in-sensitive response) was signifi- B). There were no effects for reported positive emotions or
cantly associated with lower relationship functioning coded facial positive emotion.
b = -.27, t = -2.45, p = .02, sr2 = 0.07. We did not
find any effects for reported emotion. Discussion

Primary analyses: Negative emotions and positive Here we examined the functional utility of negative
relationship behaviors emotion generated in response to negative relational
themes depicted in films, in relation to adjustment and
Again, we used 2 OLS regressions, employing the identical behavior in intimate-partnerships. Our findings suggest
step-wise approach and control variables with positive that greater negative emotion generated discretely in
relationship behaviors as the dependent variable.3 The response to films depicting conflict and loss was associ-
ated with higher relationship adjustment (DAS) and
3
We tested all assumptions of regression in each analysis in all three greater daily positive relational behavior. Additionally we
studies, including the assumption of normality in the dependent found that positive facial emotion in response to negative
variable. Although behavioral indices including relationship behav-
films was maladaptive, predicting lower scores on the
iors indexed here and treatment adherence indexed in Study 2 can be
skewed, the distributions in our samples were within normal DAS. Together, these data demonstrate the importance of
parameters (Tibachnik and Fidell 2006). context sensitive emotion and in particular, suggest a

123
608 Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624

Table 2 Study 1—OLS regression examining emotional responses during relational contexts and relationship adjustment
Dependent variable: relationship adjustment—Dyadic Adjustment Scale B SE b sr2 R2 DR2

Regression A
Step 1 Age -.22 .43 -.08 .00 .13 –
Sex .28 4.30 .01 .00
Length of Relationship -.02 .06 -.06 .00
Relationship Typea -.02 3.80 -.00 .00
In-Person Contactb -.05 .11 -.06 .00
Depression Symptoms -.77** .26 -.36 .12
F(6, 67) = 1.69, p = .14
Step 2 Age -.29 .40 -.11 .01 .31 .18***
Sex -4.44 4.34 -.13 .01
Length of Relationship -.02 .06 -.04 .00
Relationship Type -.11 3.90 -.00 .00
In-Person Contact -.04 .01 -.05 .00
Depression Symptoms -1.02** .24 -.47 .19
Neg. Expression—Neg. Films 5.90* 2.46 .37 .06
Pos. Expression—Neg. Films -30.61* 12.51 -.27 .07
Neg. Expression—Pos. Films -.49 3.91 -.02 .00
Pos. Expression—Pos. Films 1.66 2.99 .08 .00
F(10, 63) = 2.86, p = .005
Regression B
Step 1 Age -.22 .42 -.08 .00 .14 –
Sex .35 4.26 .01 .00
Length of Relationship -.02 .06 -.06 .00
Relationship Type .05 3.95 .00 .00
In-Person Contact -.05 .11 -.06 .00
Depression Symptoms -.75** .24 -.37 .12
F(6, 68) = 1.77, p = .12
Step 2 Age -.08 .44 -.03 .00 .16 .02
Sex .61 4.79 .02 .00
Length of Relationship -.04 .06 -.09 .00
Relationship Type -1.12 4.22 -.04 .00
In-Person Contact -.05 .11 -.06 .00
Depression Symptoms -.69* .27 -.34 .09
Neg. Reported Emotion—Neg. Films -.55 2.63 -.04 .00
Pos. Reported Emotion—Neg. Films -.11 3.63 -.00 .00
Neg. Reported Emotion—Pos. Films -5.82 6.74 -.13 .01
Pos. Reported Emotion—Pos. Films 3.1 2.61 .19 .02
F(10, 64) = 1.24, p = .29
Neg. negative, Pos. positive
* p \ .05; ** p \ .001
a
Relationship Type describes if the relationships was long distance
b
In-Person Contact was the frequency of in-person contact in the prior week

functional benefit to contextually matched negative emo- In particular, there has been recent work demonstrating
tion to intimate partnerships. This finding is consistent the role of inter-personal emotion regulation and empathic
with emerging research on inter-personal emotion pro- accuracy in relationships (c.f., Zaki and Williams 2013).
cesses in relationships. Broadly, the literature has suggested a critical role for

123
Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624 609

Table 3 Study 1—OLS Regression examining emotional responses during relational contexts and positive relationship behaviors
Dependent variable: relationship adjustment —positive relationship behaviors B SE b sr2 R2 DR2

Regression A
Step 1 Age -.05 .07 -.13 .01 .27 –
Sex .01 .75 .00 .00
Length of Relationship -.00 .01 -.05 .00
Relationship Typea .88 .77 .17 .02
In-Person Contactb .06** .02 .49 .21
Depression Symptoms -.04 .05 -.12 .01
F(6, 44) = 2.75, p = .02
Step 2 Age -.10 .07 -.24 .03 .39 .12*
Sex -.75 .79 -.14 .01
Length of Relationship -.00 .01 -.07 .00
Relationship Type .88 .83 .17 .02
In-Person Contact .06** .02 .44 .14
Depression Symptoms -.09 .05 -.25 .05
Neg. Expression—Neg. Films 1.06* .48 .39 .08
Pos. Expression—Neg. Films -2.17 2.53 -.13 .01
Neg. Expression—Pos. Films -.12 .99 -.02 .00
Pos. Expression—Pos. Films -.55 .57 -.16 .01
F(10, 40) = 2.28, p = .02
Regression B
Step 1 Age -.05 .07 -.13 .01 .28 –
Sex -.01 .74 -.00 .00
Length of Relationship -.00 .01 -.05 .00
Relationship Type .86 .76 .17 .02
In-Person Contact -.06**** .02 .49 .21
Depression Symptoms -.05 .04 -.14 .02
F(6, 45) = 2.85, p = .02
Step 2 Age -.06 .07 -.15 .01 .37 .09
Sex -.59 .81 -.11 .01
Length of Relationship -.00 .01 -.03 .00
Relationship Type 1.14 .78 .22 .03
In-Person Contact .06** .02 .47 .18
Depression Symptoms -.07 .05 -.21 .03
Neg. Reported Emotion—Neg. Films .83 .47 .36 .05
Pos. Reported Emotion—Neg. Films -.05 .63 -.01 .00
Neg. Reported Emotion—Pos. Films -1.55 1.28 -.22 .02
Pos. Reported Emotion—Pos. Films .18 .45 .07 .00
F(10, 41) = 2.37, p = .03
* p \ .05; ** p \ .001

emotion attunement in relationships. As such, our data sensitive negative emotion, they do not attend to the like-
demonstrating the importance of context-sensitive emotion lihood that specific negative emotions (e.g., contempt) may
is well in line. Moreover, these data are largely consistent be more or less functionally adaptive in intimate-partner-
with recent evidence suggesting that in socially salient ships (c.f., Gottman 1994). Finally, our results suggested
situations, positive emotional expressions can be mal- that facial expressions of emotion were most predictive of
adaptive (e.g., Bonanno et al. 2007). However, although outcome indices of relationship functioning. This finding is
our results speak to the functional benefit of context- consistent with the longstanding emphasis in relationship

123
610 Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624

research on behavioral indicators of emotion, as well as the itself (i.e., fear of a mammography experience) then
clear role that facial emotion can play in social commu- increased fear predicts less screening (Consedine et al.
nication (Keltner et al. 2006). 2004).
There were several limitations. First, the sample was Much of the research examining emotions and health-
young and the nature of relationships varied. Also, we related behaviors has focused on negative emotions typi-
examined broader affective and facial responses, rather cally associated with avoidance, including the above work
than specific emotions which may be particularly important on fear. In contrast, there has been relatively little research
in the context of intimate-partnerships. Finally, given the on other negative emotions that are well-demonstrated to
cross-sectional design, we cannot point to a causal asso- facilitate other, perhaps more adaptive responses. For
ciation between negative emotion and relationship func- example, sadness has been shown to increase deliberation,
tioning (e.g., we cannot rule-out that individuals in higher problem-solving, and reflection (e.g., Bodenhausen et al.
functioning relationships are then more emotionally 2000; c.f., Bonanno et al. 2008). Typically, researchers
responsive to negative relational themes). Indeed, though have examined enduring sad-related emotion, such as grief
we could not test explicitly that it was the attachment or depression in relation to health or illness in community
system at work, our results are consistent with social- samples. However, there has been no prior research
functional and discrete emotion theories, as well as domi- examining sad emotion (brief in duration) in response to
nant attachment theories and new relationship-specific coping with real-life circumstances such as chronic illness.
emotion research. Indeed, although evidence for benefits in sadness have been
Our results in Study 1 suggest that context sensitive well-demonstrated in laboratory paradigms there have been
negative emotional responses to relational themes of con- few attempts to examine sadness in contexts where adap-
flict and loss were functionally beneficial in intimate- tive problem-solving and decision making are important,
partnerships. These benefits were restricted to negative such as chronic illness.
emotions in response to negative contexts. Further, we We built upon this developing literature by examining
found that context in-sensitive positive emotions could be sadness and treatment adherence in adults with transfusion-
problematic in relation to these particular outcomes. In our dependent forms of the congenital blood-disorder Tha-
next study, we strove to move beyond general valence- lassemia (TDT). TDTs are diagnosed in infancy and
based context sensitivity by increasing the precision of our associated with considerable life-long treatment demands,
elicitation, targeting one specific negative emotion that given the high risk of life-threatening comorbidities due to
would have bearing on one functional outcome for a iron toxicity (e.g., cardiovascular disease). These include
specific population. regular blood transfusions, frequent medical screenings,
and daily iron chelation treatment. Indeed, this disorder is
associated with shortened lifespan and deterioration in
Study 2: Sadness and treatment adherence major organ systems due to complications associated with
during chronic illness poor disease management (Rund and Rachmilewitz 2005).
Patients with TDT experience substantial losses over their
Theories of coping with illness suggest that emotion-re- lifespan, including frequent loss of peers and/or siblings
lated processes are highly relevant to health behaviors, from complications relating to the same disorder, as well as
influencing assessments of risk, decision-making, and potential losses of independence and normative develop-
related coping responses (e.g., Leventhal et al. 1998). mental progression (e.g., decreased fertility).
However, much research on chronic illness has implicated Given the frequency of experienced and anticipated
negative emotions in poor adherence (e.g., Leombruni et al. losses in TDT patients (Vardaki et al. 2004), we predicted
2009) and even increased mortality (e.g., Moussavi et al. that sad emotion could be functionally important in patient
2007). In contrast, recent evidence suggests that specific management of their disease. Compelling evidence has
negative emotions can be functionally adaptive when the demonstrated that sadness can promote deliberation,
source of the emotions are factors relating to the disease problem-solving, and other factors underlying adherence
itself. For instance, a recent review of the literature sug- with treatment and self-management of care (Hill-Briggs
gests that the functional benefit of fear and worry in rela- 2003). Moreover, sadness is associated with reflection and
tion to disease screening is constrained by contextual acceptance (c.f. Bonanno et al. 2008), both important given
factors. For example, if the fear is in response to the the challenging circumstances of this particular population.
probability of developing the disease, or fear of regret for As such, we predicted that sad responses within the context
not screening, then greater fear and worry appears to of their illness could facilitate better adherence in this
motivate greater screening (e.g., Sanberg and Conner population. Specifically, we predicted that following an
2008). However, if the fear is in response to the screening interview about their illness, expressions of sadness elicited

123
Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624 611

during a film about loss would be predictive of better open-ended prompts regarding the nature of their illness
adherence to treatment. experience (i.e., tell me about what it means to have
Thalassemia; tell me how you are coping). Each question
Methods was open-ended, the interviewer rarely commented, and
participants were allowed to speak broadly about each
Participants and procedure topic as they saw fit. At the conclusion of the interview
(approximately 10 min), participants were told they would
Adult patients diagnosed with transfusion-dependent Tha- next watch a brief film clip and were asked to engage with
lassemias (TDTs: e.g., Thalassemia Major, Intermedia, it as best they could. They were informed that the camera
Blackfan Diamond Anemia) were offered the opportunity would be recording their responses during the film and the
to participate in the study by their physician. Those interviewer left the room. Participants viewed the final
expressing interest were approached directly by researchers scenes of the film ‘‘The Champ’’ 1979, Metro-Goldwyn
and informed consent was obtained. Participants were not Mayer, on a 1700 screen. This brief clip has been very well-
offered compensation and all research activity was validated as an elicitor of sadness (Gross and Levenson
approved by the Institutional Review Board overseeing 1995) and depicts a son watching his father die, a theme
research involving human subjects. highly relevant to patients who have both experienced
Of fifty-four possible patients, 36 adult individuals were considerable losses, and anticipate more, including their
recruited, however, one individual did not provide suffi- own early death and its impact on their family. Immedi-
cient treatment diaries for inclusion, so the final study ately following the clip, participants were instructed to
sample was n = 35. The mean age of participants was make ratings of their emotional experience. Four partici-
32.33 years, SD = 8.50. The majority were female (70 %), pants elected not to be video-taped during the film and
Caucasian (83 %), and college educated (66 %). The mean therefore do not have scores for emotional expression.
number of comorbid physical disorders, or disease burden,
was 3.53, SD = 1.64 (range 1–7), these most commonly Emotional expression We employed two coders, certified
were osteoporosis, HIV, hyper/hypogonadism, heart fail- in the Facial Action Coding System (FACS: Ekman et al.
ure, and diabetes. 2002 and blind to the hypotheses and all study details to
Participation occurred exclusively in the hospital setting code specific muscle actions related to happiness and
(as part of a larger project examining emotional processes sadness (smiling and frowning). Unlike in Study 1, we
in health, see Harvey et al. 2014) where patients regularly employed FACS (rather than naı̈ve coders) and focused on
attended blood transfusion appointments and included a these particular muscle actions given recent evidence
packet of initial questionnaires assessing disease–related suggesting that they may most clearly differentiate these
and demographic factors. This was followed by a ten emotional signatures as compared to muscle action that is
minute interview about their experiences with Thalassemia common to both expressions (e.g., AU 6: Messinger 2002).
which was concluded with the presentation of a film clip, Each coder coded one minute of each participant’s video,
evoking sadness and loss, during which emotional facial corresponding to the peak minute in the film clip for
behavior was recorded and affective responses were muscle changes or Action Units (AUs) associated with
assessed. Following the interview, participants completed frowns (AUs: 15, 17) and smiles (AU: 12). Intercoder-
treatment diaries at the next six consecutive blood trans- reliability (0.84) was the mean ratio of agreement across 5
fusion appointments (approximately every 2–3 weeks; the randomly selected videos including onset/offset, duration
total period ranged from 15 to 18 weeks for all partici- and intensity (e.g., Keltner et al. 1995). Participant scores
pants) in the hospital. In each diary, participants reported reflect the AU (15 and/or 17 for sadness; 12 for happiness)
adherence in the previous 14 days with iron chelation frequency by duration (in seconds) and are weighted by
(prescribed 2–7 times weekly to all patients in the study to intensity. Mean expressed sadness was M = 5.53,
address iron toxicity associated with frequent blood SD = 11.66, range 0–48.50, and mean expressed happiness
transfusions). was M = 14.46, SD = 53.40, range 0–300.00. There was
one extreme score for expressed happiness, greater than 5
Interview and emotion assessment SDs above the mean. As such, we performed a truncation
procedure (Winsor method, Guttman 1973) reducing the
Following completion of the initial packet, participants one outlier to the next highest value and adjusted happiness
were brought to a 40 X 60 room, instructed to sit comfortably was M = 5.72, SD = 15.38, range 0–54.01.
in a chair facing a tripod-mounted video camera and told
they would be asked to speak about their illness and then Emotional experience Participants rated six emotion
would watch a brief video. Participants responded to two words (fear, sadness, guilt, interest, happiness, amusement)

123
612 Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624

on a scale from 1 (none) to 7 (strong) after the film. Ratings expression versus adjusted happiness expression was not
of sadness and happiness were used to comprise self-re- significant t(30) = 0.054, p = .96. We examined concor-
ported emotional experience that corresponded to the dance across dimensions of emotion and there was a sig-
specific emotions in question. Mean reported sadness was nificant correlation between sadness report and expression
M = 3.31, SD = 1.94, range 1–7, and mean reported r = .47, p = .007, but not between happiness report and
happiness was M = 2.14, SD = 1.42, range 1–6. expression r = -.15, p = .42. A review of all correlations
between key study variables was consistent with our pre-
Treatment diary dictions (e.g., both sadness expressions and reported sad-
ness were significantly positively correlated with
Participants reported adherence with physician prescribed adherence). See Table 4 for all correlations between key
iron chelation six times at each consecutive blood-trans- study variables.
fusion appointment following the interview. Daily iron
chelation is an essential component of treatment in TDT Primary analyses
and is one of the most commonly used indicators of
adherence in this population (e.g., Trachtenberg et al. We conducted two OLS regression models with adherence
2011). Prior to commencing the study, participants repor- as the dependent variable. In the first step, we controlled
ted the prescribed frequency of iron chelation (prescribed for variables that would likely influence adherence because
M = 5.69 times per week, SD = 1.60, range: 2–7) as well of increased treatment-related demands, these included the
as the technique used. Fifty percent of participants disease burden (i.e., number of comorbid physical condi-
employed subcutaneous/intravenous methods and the rest tions), chelation method, and prescribed rate of chelation
reported oral methods. These data were later confirmed by (i.e., times per week). We did test to see if other variables
reviewing the medical record. were meaningful, including age and gender, but found that
they did not impact the model. In the second step we
Treatment adherence During each diary, participants included coded sadness and happiness expressions
indicated which days, over the past fourteen, they had (Table 5: Regression A) or reported sadness and happiness
completed iron chelation so that together, the six diaries (Table 5: Regression B). As in Study 1, we included sad-
allowed for reporting over 12 weeks. Percentage of ness and happiness together in the analysis to control for
adherence was calculated by dividing the total patient shared variance as well as to provide a more rigorous test
reported chelation days by the total prescribed by their of our hypothesis. The results of these analyses supported
physician. Mean reported chelation adherence was high our hypothesis that greater sadness expression, b = .32,
M = 84.45, SD = 21.11 (range: 23.64–119.05). Some p = .049, sr2 = 0.09; F(5, 25) = 4.45, p = .01, R2 = .47
participants did choose to chelate more frequently than was associated with higher treatment adherence. Greater
prescribed; as such we used scores truncated to 100 % in reported sadness b = .24, p = .087, sr2 = 0.06; F(5,
the primary analysis.4 There was a marginal difference in 29) = 5.25, p = .001, R2 = .48, did not reach significance.
adherence based on the method employed (i.e., oral chela- There were no effects for happiness.
tors appeared to have higher adherence, t(33) = -1.71,
p = .10). Discussion

Results Here we examined the adaptive utility of sadness generated


in a loss-evoking context and treatment adherence in
Preliminary analyses patients with chronic illness. Prior research has broadly
implicated negative emotions in poor adherence. However,
Emotion data were examined to ensure that participants recent evidence has suggested that contextual factors are
responded as anticipated to the film. Participants reported quite important when evaluating these associations for
significantly greater sadness than happiness, t(34) = specific negative emotions (Consedine et al. 2004). Con-
-2.82, p = .008, but the difference between sadness sistent with this, our data suggest that sadness in response
to a loss context predicted higher rates of treatment
4
Although we are not aware of data suggesting that over-chelation in adherence (daily iron chelation) in patients with TDT. In
patients with TDT is problematic or beneficial, we did not want to prior research, sadness has been associated with factors that
assume that chelating more than prescribed was is indicative of could translate into greater patient self-management,
‘‘better’’ adherence. However, to be sure that we we did not
including improving problem solving, reflection, and
artificially influence results by truncating scores to 100 %, we
conducted analyses again using un-truncated scores and the results deliberation. We believe that of particular importance was
were unchanged. the disease-focused interview which may have primed

123
Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624 613

Table 4 Study 2 bi-variate


1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6.
correlations between key
variables 1. Disease Burden –
2. Prescribed Rate of Chelation .21 –
3. Sadness Expression 2.36 -.28 –
4. Happiness Expression -.06 -.33 .48 –
5. Sadness Report -.14 -.09 -.05 -.25 –
6. Happiness Report -.11 -.06 .09 -.07 -.04 –
7. Treatment Adherence (Chelation) 2.40 -.15 .40 .30 .36 -.27
Significant associations are indicated in bold

Table 5 Study 2—OLS


Dependent variable: treatment adherence B SE b sr2 R2 DR2
Regression examining sad and
happy responses and treatment Regression A
adherence during chronic illness
Step 1 Disease Burden -4.37* 2.06 -.33 .11 .35 –
Chelation Method 22.05** 6.79 .56 .25
Prescribed Rate of Chelation -3.26 2.20 -.26 .05
F(3, 27) = 4.84, p = .008
Step 2 Disease Burden -4.04 1.98 -.31 .09 .47 .12 
Chelation Method 19.59** 6.66 .50 .18
Prescribed Rate of Chelation -.98 2.35 -.08 .00
Sadness Expression .55* .27 .32 .09
Happiness Expression .26 .21 .20 .03
F(5, 25) = 4.48, p = .005
Regression B
Step 1 Disease Burden -5.01** 1.75 -.41 .16 .39 –
Chelation Method 21.97** 6.23 .57 .24
Prescribed Rate of Chelation -3.33 1.97 -.27 .06
F(3, 31) = 6.64, p = .001
Step 2 Disease Burden -4.54* 1.72 -.37 .13 .48 .09
Chelation Method 19.36** 6.24 .50 .17
Prescribed Rate of Chelation -2.92 1.91 -.24 .04
Sadness Report 2.48 1.40 .24 .06
Happiness Report -2.22 1.96 -.16 .02
F(5, 29) = 5.25, p = .001
* p \ .05; ** p \ .001

participant’s beliefs and feelings about their illness, prior to there are many reasons why this may be the case, including
investigating sadness in response to loss. Patients with the vulnerability of self-report to well-documented influ-
TDT experience and anticipate tremendous losses ences. Limitations in this study included the small sample
throughout their lifetime. As such, sadness when elicited in size. TDTs are rare in the US. As such it will be important
this context, for these patients, was functionally adaptive to examine these kinds of relationships in larger patient
and predicted better adherence with iron chelation over a groups. Also, in this investigation we relied only on single-
period of 4–6 months. ratings of sadness and happiness from participants. Finally,
Like Study 1, we found that the strongest association although we believe the method of employing the disease-
with outcome was for coded negative emotional expres- focused interview was important in eliciting relevant sad-
sions, here sadness, rather than reported emotional expe- ness responses, we cannot confirm this as we were unable
rience. Prior research examining in vivo emotion broadly in to include a control condition.
relation to psychological adjustment has similar findings The results of Study 1 and 2 suggest the adaptive utility
(e.g., Bonanno and Keltner 1997). As we describe earlier, of negative emotions generated during discrete and explicit

123
614 Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624

negative contexts. Both studies benefited from increased adaptive. Positive emotions have almost uniformly been
internal validity (e.g., use of standardized stimuli to elicit associated with approach-related behavior as well as broad
targeted emotion in well-defined contexts). However, typ- benefits in social contexts (c.f., Fredrickson 1998). How-
ical encounters with emotional contexts in daily life elicit a ever, prior research and theory has suggested that of the
range of emotions with differing functional utility (i.e., negative emotions, anger is the only emotion clearly
depending on the individual’s appraisal, circumstances, associated with ‘‘approach-related’’ behavior (c.f., Carver
and short-term versus long-term needs). As such, in Study and Harmon-Jones 2009) including adaptive responses to
3, we broadened the emotion-elicitation context by goal-blockage (Lerner and Keltner 2001). Though fear and
employing a highly salient and more naturalistic emotion sadness have functional benefits in responses to survival
induction to elicit multiple negative emotions, to further threats and losses, they are largely associated with avoid-
test our broader hypothesis regarding negative emotions ance (Ohman and Mineka 2001) and withdrawal (Bonanno
and adjustment. et al. 2008). Given the importance of peer-relations during
adjustment to college, avoidance and withdrawal responses
could be problematic as they would be least likely to fulfill
Study 3: Anger, sadness, fear and on-line peer- the broader goal.
rejection during adjustment to college Prior research on negative emotions during peer-rejec-
tion has had mixed findings. The conflicting evidence may
Adjustment to college is marked by considerable variabil- be due in part to the likelihood that functional benefits of
ity in responses and outcomes (e.g., completion rates: negative emotion vary depending on whether immediate
National Center for Educational Statistics 2013). A key versus long-term needs or outcomes are in question. For
factor may be the development of peer-support networks example, as compared to sadness or fear, anger could
(Fass and Tubman 2002) making peer rejection particularly activate resources most effectively in the moment because
salient during this transition (Williams 2007). Here, we anger is associated with appraisals of certainty, control, and
broadened our emotion elicitation context to test the predicts optimism and greater decisiveness (Lerner and
functional benefit of negative emotions using a more nat- Keltner 2001; Lerner et al. 2003). Moreover, there is clear
uralistic provocation. Given the association between psy- behavioral evidence that anger is predictive of less sym-
chological adjustment and success in college with peer- pathetic arousal and less HPA axis reactivity (e.g.,
relations, we investigated emotional responses to simulated Stemmler et al. 2001; Lerner et al. 2007) relative to fear.
online peer-rejection during the first year of college in Indeed, there is some evidence suggesting that anger may
relation to multiple outcome indicators. We employed a be relatively adaptive (Miller and Olson 2000) and that fear
well-validated paradigm, Cyberball (Williams et al. 2000) is particularly problematic during social rejection (Sethi
to simulate rejection naturalistically, thereby allowing for et al. 2013). Finally, there is very convincing evidence in
the elicitation of multiple emotions at once. Indeed, prior prior use of this paradigm that positive emotions are
research with this paradigm has demonstrated that peer- functionally important responses during peer-rejection.
rejection typically elicits anger, sadness, fear (Williams However, they appear to occur largely outside of the
2007), as well as positive emotions (DeWall et al. 2011). awareness and impact secondary or broader goals (DeWall
The variability in emotion responses is understood to be et al. 2011).
driven by individual differences in appraisals of the situa- In this study, we tested the functional relevance of the
tional context (Lerner and Keltner 2001) resulting in negative emotions anger, sadness, and fear, as well as
varying emotions that may have different consequences or positive emotion elicited during simulated-peer rejection in
functionality. For example, some emotions may have first year college students. To do so, we examined them in
functional utility in the short term but not in the long term. relation to an immediate functional indicator of outcome
The deliberate inclusion of this more naturalistic paradigm (sympathetic nervous system activity) and a distal indicator
to further answer our overall hypothesis provided a broader of adjustment (reports of psychological distress). We
context in which to more rigorously test the function of hypothesized that relative to fear or sadness, anger would
negative emotions in relation to adjustment. be most adaptive in the moment. Anger is associated with
Given the range of possible emotions elicited in this optimistic appraisals, more adaptive coping, and, in par-
context, we considered the functional utility of each emo- ticular, reduced sympathetic arousal. Increased sympa-
tion both in relation to the specific elicitation of peer-re- thetic arousal (in this case skin conductance) can be highly
jection and the broader framework of adjustment to adaptive in response to survival threats, but in a social
college. In general, we hypothesized that emotions that context would be incongruent with approach-related
have been demonstrated to facilitate approach responses behaviors and flexible responding. However, in relation to
(versus withdrawal or avoidance) would likely be most distal outcome, we considered that anger and positive

123
Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624 615

emotions could potentially both be adaptive given their Rejection task and emotion assessment and autonomic
associations with approach-related responses and goal activity
achievement. Indeed, prior work has demonstrated that
positive emotions are critical to relationship development, Participants engaged in an adapted version of the Cyberball
and are therefore likely adaptive in the broader framework paradigm, a well-established computer simulation of
of adjustment to college (Fredrickson 1998), though not rejection or ostracism (Williams et al. 2000). In this within-
necessarily during the task. Given the clear association of subjects version, participants were told they would be
sadness to social withdrawal and fear to avoidance, we playing a series of ‘‘online’’ ball games with three other
predicted that neither would provide functional benefit. students who were in other rooms around the university
Indeed, we anticipated that as in prior work, fear might be and that they would be evaluated on their performance.
most problematic, predictive of high sympathetic arousal Several features were integrated into the protocol to aid the
during rejection and higher reported psychological deception (e.g., online ‘‘profile’’ creation; the experimenter
distress. made several ‘‘checks’’ on the status of other online players
prior to each game, etc.). Participants played two games, in
Methods the first they experienced no rejection and were tossed the
ball 25 % of the time. In the second game, participants
Participants and procedure were rejected and were tossed the ball only 7 % of the
time, and only in the first 30 s of the game. Participant
Seventy participants who identified as first-year students at reported emotional experience after each game, facial
a Midwest public university were recruited for a study on behavior and autonomic activity were recorded in real
emotion and attention and compensated with course credit. time, throughout.
Informed consent was obtained for all participants and all
research activity was approved by the Institutional Review Reported emotional experience
Board overseeing research involving human subjects. After
consenting, participants were photographed and completed Participants rated their experience of Anger, Sadness, Fear,
an ‘‘online profile’’ to aid in the deception during the and Happiness, each on a 1–7 Likert scale immediately
simulated rejection task. Next, participants completed after each game. The pattern of means conformed to
questionnaires indexing demographics and psychological expectations from Game 1 (no rejection) to Game 2 (re-
distress. Finally, participants were seated in front of a jection) such that there was an increase in Anger and
desktop computer and played two ‘‘online’’ ball games Sadness and a decrease in Happiness: Game 1—Anger
during which rejection was simulated (Cyberball: Williams M = 1.10, SD = 1.04, Game 2—Anger M = 1.59,
et al. 2000). Emotion was indexed via self-reported emo- SD = 1.25, t(69) = -3.49, p = .001; Game 1—Sadness
tion responses as well as coded facial behavior (high-def- M = 1.14, SD = 57, Game 2—Sadness M = 1.40,
inition video of facial expressions was captured during SD = 1.04 t(69) = -2.21, p = .03; Game 1—Happiness
each game). Sympathetic arousal (skin conductance) was M = 3.57, SD = 1.57, Game 2—Happiness M = 2.80,
indexed throughout. SD = 1.54, t(69) = 7.02, p = .000. However, there was
Participant mean age was M = 19.27 years, SD = 3.71. no significant difference in reported Fear across the two
Four participants were older than traditional college age games: Game 1 Fear M = 1.20, SD = 0.63, Game 2 Fear
(i.e., greater than 22 years) and there was variability in age M = 1. 14, SD = 0.52, t(69) = 1.00, p = .32.
that predicted some marginal differences in emotional
responses, as such, we routinely controlled for age in all Coded emotional expression Participant facial behavior
analysis. Participants were primarily female (60 %), Cau- was recorded using a customized high-definition web-cam.
casian (84 %), and non-Hispanic (93 %). Nine participants did not have usable video data because of
camera malfunction (n = 5) and experimenter error
Questionnaire measures (n = 4), so facial scores are available for n = 61 partici-
pants only. Four coders, blind to the hypotheses and all
Psychological distress The Global Symptom Index is a study details, viewed each participants videos from each
29-item self-report measure of psychological distress game without sound on a 23-inch computer monitor.
measured using a combination of Depression, Anxiety, and Coders coded facial behavior indicative of Anger, Sadness,
Hostility sub-scales from the Symptom Check List (SCL- Fear or Happiness, each on 1–7 Likert scales for each
90-R; Derogatis 1983). Items were averaged to form a participant for each of the two games. Coders received
psychological distress score. Mean distress reported was limited training (as in Study 1) and relied on global indi-
M = .67; SD = .57 (a = .94). cators of each emotion in a manner consistent with prior

123
616 Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624

research (e.g., Waldinger et al. 2004). Coder reliability was were largely not. This suggests that participants rated
assessed (average ICC = .67, range: .41–.86) and scores emotional experiences less distinctly than their faces dis-
was averaged across all four coders by participant to played emotions, consistent with recent evidence on indi-
increase reliability. The result was each participant had one vidual differences in emotion differentiation and/or clarity
score for each coded emotion for each game. (Kashdan et al. 2015).
The pattern of means across games conformed to
expectations for Sadness and Happiness: Game 1—Sadness Emotion responses and proximal outcome—sympathetic
M = 1.15, SD = 0.27, Game 2–Sadness M = 1.28, arousal
SD = 0.34 t(60) = -2.91, p = .005 Game 1—Happiness
M = 2.06, SD = 1.16, Game 2—Happiness M = 1.49, In this analysis we focused on proximal responses within
SD = 0.92, t(60) = 4.86, p = .000. However, there was the rejection game. We conducted two OLS regressions
no significant difference in coded expressions of Fear or (one for each index of emotion). In each analysis, mean
Anger across the two games: Game 1 Fear M = 1.13, skin conductance during rejection was the dependent
SD = 0.20, Game 2 Fear M = 1. 12, SD = 0.22, variable. In the first step we controlled for mean skin
t(60) = .44, p = .67; Game 1—Anger M = 1.14, conductance from the prior neutral game, age and gender.
SD = 0.29, Game 2—Anger M = 1.15 SD = 0.29, In the second step we entered either expressed (Table 7—
t(60) = -.55, p = .59. Regression A) or reported (Table 7—Regression B) fear,
sadness, anger, and happiness during game 2. The results of
Sympathetic Arousal—skin conductance response After these analyses were consistent with our hypothesis and
completing the player ‘‘profile’’ participants were fitted revealed that greater expressed anger was significantly
with an Affectiva Q-SensorTM to monitor skin conductance predictive of lower skin conductance during rejection,
(SC) on the inner wrist or ventral forearm, of their non- although the effect was quite small, b = -.10, p = .03,
dominant arm, one inch below the wrist bone. The sr2 = .01; F(7, 48) = 65.23, p \ .000, R2 = .91. How-
Q-Sensor uses two, 1-cm diameter Ag–AgCl dry electrodes ever, we did not find effects consistent with our predictions
and samples skin conductance at a rate of 32 Hz. Data were for fear (expressed or reported) and found no other
cleaned and artifacts removed using customized software associations.
(Affectiva) and visual inspection. One SC value for each
game, in uSiemens, was calculated by averaging SC across Emotion responses and distal outcome—psychological
that game period. Mean SC for game 1 M = .18, SD = .35 distress
was not significantly different from mean SC for game 2
(rejection) M = .19, SD = .40, t(61) = -1.33, p = .19. In this analysis we focused broadly on responses to both
Eight individuals lacked usable SC data because of Cyberball games, and were particularly interested in
equipment malfunction (n = 4) and experimenter error emotions generated during the rejection game. Again we
(n = 4). conducted two OLS regressions. In each analysis, psy-
chological distress was the dependent variable. In the first
Results step of each model we controlled for age and gender, and
included emotion responses (either reported or expressed)
Preliminary analysis during Game 1 (no rejection). In the second step, entered
either expressed (Table 8: Regression A) or reported
We examined bi-variate correlations between key study (Table 8: Regression B) anger, fear, sadness and happiness
variables (see Table 6). The results were largely consistent from Game 2.5 In the final model examining facial
with our hypotheses. For example, fear expressions were behavior, expressed fear during rejection was significantly
associated with increased psychological distress and gen- associated with higher distress b = .28, p = .05,
erally reported emotions during the rejection game were sr2 = .06, and expressed happiness during rejection was
associate reported psychological distress (i.e., happy significantly associated with lower distress, Game 2 hap-
reports had an inverse association, whereas sad, anger, and piness: b = -.43, p = .01, sr2 = .10 F(10, 50) = 2.09,
fear reports had a positive association). However, notably
there was relatively little concordance across response 5
Given the correlations among emotion variables we did rerun these
modes by emotion. This may be due to differences in the analyses in various combinations (e.g., without all emotions included;
differentiation of reported emotional experience as com- without baseline emotion) to test the possibility that the effects
reported were dependent on the presence of the covariates. Across
pared to facial behavior. For example, ratings of negative
analyses the nature of the relationships between IVs and distress were
emotions (anger, fear, sadness) were all correlated signifi- relatively consistent and there were not meaningful differences when
cantly whereas facial expressions of those same emotions covariates were or were not included.

123
Table 6 Study 3 bi-variate correlations between key variables
Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624

1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18.

1. Game 1 Fear Expression –


2. Game 1 Sad Expression .43 –
3. Game 1 Anger Expression -.15 .06 –
4. Game 1 Happy Expression -.06 2.35 -.20 –
5. Game 1 Fear Report -.08 -.12 -.11 .13 –
6. Game 1 Sad Report .05 -.07 -.11 .24 .58 –
7. Game 1 Anger Report -.12 -.05 -.09 .32 .74 .56 –
8. Game 1 Happy Report .27 .10 -.03 .08 .10 .11 .09 –
9. Game 2 Fear Expression .48 .14 -.08 -.02 -.07 -.04 -.06 .06 –
10. Game 2 Sad Expression -.00 .35 .05 -.04 -.17 -.13 -.08 -.01 .09 –
11. Game 2 Anger Expression .07 .17 .68 2.26 -.13 -.13 -.11 .09 -.03 .11 –
12. Game 2 Happy Expression -.08 2.27 -.20 .64 .03 .33 .16 .06 -.04 -.07 -.25 –
13. Game 2 Fear Report -.10 -.13 -.07 .30 .67 .48 .66 .07 -.04 -.08 -.09 .17
14. Game 2 Sad Report .05 -.17 -.15 .18 .48 .38 .31 .15 -.01 -.15 -.09 .09 .81 –
15. Game 2 Anger Report .07 -.04 -.08 -.04 .54 .24 .37 .07 .08 .07 -.08 -.12 .59 .67 –
16. Game 2 Happy Report .26 -.12 -.03 .08 -.04 .13 .07 .83 .02 -.01 .07 .15 .03 .01 -.14 –
17. Game 1 (neutral) SC -.12 -.07 -.02 -.02 -.02 -.07 -.04 .06 -.08 -.13 -.02 .04 -.09 -.09 -.12 .17 –
18. Game 2 SC -.10 -.04 -.07 .07 .04 -.04 .02 .13 -.05 -.16 -.11 -.03 -.04 -.02 -.05 .17 .94 –
19. Psychological Distress .02 -.01 -.10 .22 .19 -.04 .05 -.10 .27 .16 -.13 -.13 .36 .40 .32 2.25 -.12 -.09
Significant associations are indicated in bold
617

123
618 Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624

Table 7 Study 3—OLS regression examining emotional responses to simulated rejection during adjustment to college—proximal outcome:
sympathetic arousal
Dependent variable: mean skin conductance during rejection (Game 2) B SE b sr2 R2 DR2

Regression A
Step 1 Age .01 .01 .05 .00 .89 –
Gender .01 .04 .02 .00
Game 1 (neutral) SC 1.14*** .06 .94 .85
F(3, 52) = 146.64, p \ .000
Step 2 Age .01 .01 .04 .00 .91 .02
Gender .01 .04 .01 .00
Game 1 (neutral) SC 1.15*** .06 .95 .84
Game 2 Fear Expression .00 .09 .00 .00
Game 2 Sad Expression .02 .06 .01 .00
Game 2 Anger Expression -.15* .07 -.10 .01
Game 2 Happy Expression -.00 .02 -.00 .00
F(7, 48) = 65.23, p \ .000
Regression B
Step 1 Age .01 .01 .05 .00 .88 –
Gender .03 .04 .03 .00
Game 1 (neutral) SC 1.14*** .06 .94 .84
F(3, 58) = 146.86, p \ .000
Step 2 Age .01 .01 .07 .00 .89 .01
Gender .04 .04 .04 .00
Game 1 (neutral) SC 1.14*** .06 .95 .81
Game 2 Fear Report -.00 .06 -.01 .00
Game 2 Sad Report .01 .03 .02 .00
Game 2 Anger Report .02 .02 .06 .00
Game 2 Happy Report .01 .01 .03 .00
F(7, 54) = 61.48, p \ .000
* p \ .05; ** p \ .001

p = .04, R2 = .30. Neither expressed anger nor sadness approach the question of negative emotion context sensi-
was significantly associated with distress. Interestingly, tivity more naturalistically. Accordingly, we tested the
expressed happiness during Game 1 (no rejection) was functional importance of multiple negative emotions (i.e.,
associated with greater distress b = .46, p = .01, fear, anger, sadness) and positive emotion in relation to
sr2 = .12. In the final model, neither reported fear during proximal and distal outcomes.
rejection, b = .48, p = .10, sr2 = .03, nor reported hap- As predicted, our results largely depended on the out-
piness, b = -.41, p = .07, sr2 = .04; F(10, 59) = 2.85, come indicator. For example, anger expressed during peer-
p = .006, R2 = .33 reached significance. Again, neither rejection was adaptive in the moment, predicting lower
reported anger nor reported sadness was significantly sympathetic arousal during the task itself. Prior research
associated with distress. has clearly linked anger responses with optimism and
challenge (versus threat) appraisals (Lerner and Keltner
Discussion 2001). Both are predictive of parasympathetic nervous
system enhancement during lab stressors (e.g., higher,
The goal of this study was to examine the utility of nega- high-frequency heart rate variability: Laborde et al. 2015)
tive emotion responses in relation to proximal and distal an indicator of regulatory resources and psychological
outcomes when generated during simulated peer-rejection health (c.f., Beauchaine and Thayer 2015), although not
in first-year college students. Our approach in this final measured here. In contrast, although anger responses had
study was to broaden our elicitation paradigm, so as to proximal benefit, we found that only positive emotional

123
Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624 619

Table 8 Study 3—OLS


Dependent variable: psychological distress B SE b sr2 R2 DR2
regression examining emotional
responses to simulated rejection Regression A
during adjustment to college—
distal outcome: psychological Step 1 Age -.02 .01 -.21 .04 .11 –
distress Gender .08 .12 .09 .01
Game 1 Fear Expression -.05 .32 -.02 .00
Game 1 Sadness Expression .06 .24 .04 .00
Game 1 Happy Expression .09 .05 .24 .05
Game 1 Anger Expression -.10 .19 -.07 .00
F(6, 54) = 1.06, p = .40
Step 2 Age -.02 .01 -.17 .03 .30 19*
Gender -.01 .11 -.01 .00
Game 1 Fear Expression -.29 .34 -.14 .01
Game 1 Sadness Expression .00 .24 .00 .00
Game 1 Happy Expression .17** .06 .46 .12
Game 1 Anger Expression -.08 .25 -.06 .00
Game 2 Fear Expression .53* .26 .28 .06
Game 2 Sadness Expression .18 .17 .14 .02
Game 2 Happy Expression -.20** .07 -.43 .10
Game 2 Anger Expression -.13 .25 -.09 .00
F(10, 50) = 2.10, p = .04
Regression B
Step 1 Age -.03 .02 -.19 .03 .12 –
Gender .03 .14 .03 .00
Game 1 Fear Report .36* .17 .40 .07
Game 1 Sadness Report -.18 .15 -.18 .02
Game 1 Happy Report -.05 .04 -.14 .02
Game 1 Anger Report -.19 .25 -.14 .01
F(6, 63) = 1.45, p = .21
Step 2 Age -.02 .02 -.16 .02 .33 21**
Gender -.09 .14 -.08 .01
Game 1 Fear Report .05 .18 .06 .00
Game 1 Sadness Report -.22 .15 -.21 .02
Game 1 Happy Report .07 .08 .19 .01
Game 1 Anger Report -.29 .29 -.22 .01
Game 2 Fear Report .53 .32 .48 .03
Game 2 Sadness Report .07 .15 .12 .00
Game 2 Happy Report -.15 .08 -.41 .04
Game 2 Anger Report -.02 .08 -.04 .00
F(10, 59) = 2.84, p = .01
* p \ .05; ** p \ .001

responses during peer-rejection were functionally adaptive reported distress. We did not, despite our prediction, find
in the broader framework of adjustment to college. This is any association between fear and sympathetic arousal. This
largely consistent with prior research demonstrating the may be due to the restricted range in fear responses and the
relevance of positive emotions elicited during peer-rejec- limited change in skin conductance, perhaps because the
tion (that are largely out of awareness) on broader perhaps threat was social and not survival-related. Sadness was
secondary outcomes (e.g. DeWall et al. 2011). unrelated to our indices of outcome. Together these find-
Also consistent with prior work and our prediction, we ings suggest short-term functional benefits for anger but no
found that fear during rejection was predictive of more long term benefit. As with the prior two studies, we saw the

123
620 Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624

strongest associations for coded facial expressions of of general tendencies. The strengths and weaknesses of self-
emotion in our regression analyses. We address this pattern report indices aside, our data demonstrate the importance of
in detail in the general discussion below. capturing in-the-moment emotion responses with explicit
Limitations included reliance on single-item ratings of attention to in-lab contextual factors.
emotional experience which, particularly in a more Indeed, had we indexed negative emotions more gen-
ambiguous emotional context, may have made participant erally, we may have only captured affective phenomena
reports less reliable and contributed in part to the lack of more consistent with enduring negative mood (see
concordance across response dimensions. Indeed, there Rosenberg 1998) which has often been implicated in poor
were relatively high correlations among negative emotion psychological functioning. However, here we employed a
ratings, or evidence of poor emotion differentiation. This discrete or basic emotions framework, and sought to test
phenomenon is increasingly demonstrated to be less an the functional utility of discrete negative emotional
indicator of emotion responding and more an indicator of responses on carefully selected indices of adjustment.
differences in the conceptualization of emotional experi- Moreover, because we indexed responses across multiple
ence (c.f., Kashdan et al. 2015). Moreover, because of dimensions, we were able to explore how particular
equipment and other malfunctions we lost data and the response modes were more or less predictive of outcome.
sample was smaller than intended. Finally, the overall Overall, our findings suggested that behavioral indices of
change in skin conductance was quite small. Although we emotion were most predictive of outcome across samples
are confident in our findings, and they are consistent with and contexts (a point we return to later). Finally, matching
the extant literature, it will be important to replicate this of the outcome indicator to the sample’s unique circum-
particular effect in order to better understand the degree of stances, contextual features, and emotion(s) in question
benefit that anger provides during social threat in relation was important. Prior investigations of emotions and
to decreases in sympathetic arousal. Despite these limita- adjustment in community samples have often relied on
tions, we still provided evidence of a relatively adaptive broad indicators of outcome (e.g., symptoms) that are
(i.e., anger) negative emotion within the naturalistic con- unlikely to be functionally associated with specific emo-
text of peer-rejection during adjustment to college. tions elicited. Indeed, there are few examples of research
testing the influence of discrete negative emotions in real
world samples (see Lerner et al. 2003 for a key exception).
General discussion Assessing the theorized tight association between emo-
tion, context, and adaptive function is particularly chal-
In this investigation we examined associations between lenging in community samples, given the potential for
specific negative emotions and psychological health and intervening factors not measured in the investigation. Here,
adjustment across a range of samples, methods, and out- we attempted to address this important challenge by
comes. Although each of the studies varied in terms of employing established paradigms with demonstrated
population, emotions in question, emotion elicitation effectiveness in eliciting the target emotions in question,
paradigm, and indicator of adjustment, overall the data and we strove to include outcome indicators that would be
were consistently supportive of our broader hypothesis that likely influenced by factors or processes enhanced by the
negative emotions can be functionally adaptive. Taken target emotion(s). For example, sadness, indexed in Study
together, these data broadly support functional theories of 2, increases deliberation, acceptance, and problem solving.
negative emotions in a range of community samples. These are all factors demonstrated to underlie treatment
Moreover, these three studies take an important step adherence in chronic illness, and sadness was indexed
beyond largely basic experimental research examining the following an interview focused on experiences with their
functions of discrete negative emotions, to demonstrating illness. Overall, these three studies present an important
their broader role in psychological health and adjustment in step forward in the broader application of discrete emotions
real-world samples and circumstances. theory to furthering the understanding of negative emotion
A key strength of this investigation was use of lab para- and adjustment in community samples.
digms with well-defined contexts in which emotions were In this investigation we focused on examining context-
elicited and measured in real-time, across response dimen- sensitive negative emotion in relation to current adjustment
sions. Although there is considerable evidence demonstrat- broadly defined. As such, we focused specifically on con-
ing a link between negative emotions and poor adjustment in text-emotion matching, basing our determination of a
community samples, much of this work has relied on par- ‘‘match’’ on prior research, and testing the degree to which
ticipant reports of typical responding. Indeed, if one reviews context-sensitive emotion predicted selected outcomes.
the extant literature on negative emotion and adjustment, a Across all three studies, there was clear evidence that more
considerable portion has relied on self-reported assessment matched or context-sensitive negative emotion predicted a

123
Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624 621

better outcome. Indeed, this has largely been the finding in Taken together, this may mean that facial expressions of
the broader literature on emotion context sensitivity (e.g., emotion are more objective indicators of responding than
Rottenberg et al. 2002). However, it is possible that it may reports. Indeed emotion facial behavior is not only less
not simply be that higher intensity, context-sensitive emo- likely to be influenced by dispositional or circumstantial
tion is always adaptive. Indeed, there may be optimal levels, biases, but may be most indicative of shifts in emotion that
above or below which become problematic. Recent theo- are outside of awareness, including seemingly divergent,
rizing on emotion flexibility has argued that emotion con- conflicting, or mixed responses, as compared to resolved
text-sensitivity can not only be defined by the generation of responses reported after the fact. However, there are some
matched emotion to contexts, but also the ability to disen- that would argue that internal conceptualizations of emo-
gage that emotion when the context shifts (Coifman and tion define emotion responding and that facial expressions
Almahmoud in press). Relatively few studies have investi- are only secondary (Barrett 2006). We would argue that
gated this idea (see Coifman and Bonanno 2010 and here, both are important and would caution against approaches
our Study 1) and have not fully explored the possibility that that only consider one response mode.
too much context-sensitive emotion could also be prob- As described in each individual study discussion, there
lematic. This is clearly an important area for future research. were limitations to these data relating to the specifics of
Across the three studies, we consistently found evidence each study. First, some might argue that the variability in
that greater context-sensitive negative emotional expres- methodology and independent/dependent variables across
sions were associated with specific outcomes as predicted. the three studies was a limitation. Indeed, it will be
Surprisingly, we did not find similar effects for context- important to both replicate and further explicate these
sensitive reports of negative emotion. We believe it findings in similar samples. In particular, our method of
important that although reports of emotion did not reach coding facial expressions varied based on the research
significance, the pattern of associations was largely con- question and, in some cases, resources. Despite the con-
sistent with those relating to facial expressions. Indeed, sistency of our results, given the importance of facial
because this was so often the case, it may very well be that behavior in our findings, the variability in coding methods
measurement limitations (reporting after each elicitation is an important consideration and limitation. Moreover, we
context) and individual difference factors relating to how used highly controlled designs, yet differing paradigms for
individuals organize, conceptualize, and even value their Studies 1 and 2 (i.e., using standardized stimuli), versus a
emotional experiences, diluted the sensitivity of reports so broader, more naturalistic design in Study 3. In addition, in
that they only loosely matched behavioral indicators, in each study the hypotheses and at times methodology were
this case, facial expression of emotion. tailored to the specific population, in order to test how a
Over the last several decades of emotion research the particular emotion would be associated with adjustment in
issue of concordance or coherence across emotion response that context (hence the variability in dependent variables).
modes (e.g., facial expressions, self-report, and/or auto- We would argue that in order to be sensitive to contextual
nomic activity) has largely shifted. In place of previous and sample-specific factors that influence the role of neg-
assumptions that coherence should be ever-present, more ative emotions in relation to different outcomes, the
recent work characterizes response modes as ‘‘loosely methodology had to vary accordingly. Moreover, despite
coupled’’ (Bonanno and Keltner 2004) and demonstrations these differences, each study helped to address the question
of concordance occur only under certain circumstances of the functional utility of negative emotion in adjustment
(e.g., high intensity: Mauss et al. 2005). Moreover, there is and together the three studies are a convincing translation
a burgeoning literature demonstrating the vulnerability of of functional and discrete emotion theories into diverse
self-reports of emotion to internal influences. Indeed, real-world circumstances.
recent work has de-emphasized the role of bias or demand
characteristics (although these are likely still important)
and instead has focused on individual differences in pat- Conclusion
terns of emotion conceptualization (Kashdan et al. 2015)
and attention to emotional experience (e.g., Kashdan et al. Overall, these three studies are an important step in
2006; Kross and Ayuduk 2011). These have been demon- understanding the link between negative emotions and
strated to be highly influential on in-the-moment reporting. better psychological health and adjustment. Taken toge-
For example, considerable evidence suggests that even ther, this research provides support for the theorized
relatively simple associations between reported emotions adaptive function of negative emotions and illustrates how
of differing valence (negative versus positive) can shift negative emotions may have considerable utility across
within an individual, depending on their subjective per- samples and circumstances when contextually sensitive. In
ception of stress (c.f. Zautra 2006). particular, these studies illustrate the substantial relevance

123
622 Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624

of context as well as careful selection of outcome indica- Bonanno, G. A., & Burton, C. L. (2013). Regulatory flexibility: An
tors and real-time assessment, when evaluating negative individual differences perspective on coping and emotion
regulation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(6),
emotions in relation to psychological health and adjustment 591–612.
in community samples. Based on these findings, and the Bonanno, G. A., Colak, D. M., Keltner, D., Shiota, L., Papa, A., Noll,
broader theoretical background, we would argue for J. G., et al. (2007). Context matters: The benefits and costs of
increased attention to negative emotions in general and expressing positive emotion among survivors of childhood
sexual abuse. Emotion, 7, 824–837.
certainly in research specifically focused on emotion pro- Bonanno, G. A., Goorin, L., & Coifman, K. G. (2008). Sadness and
cesses in relation to psychological and physical health. grief. In M. Lewis, J. Haviland-Jones, & L. FeldmanBarrett
(Eds.), The handbook of emotion (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford.
Acknowledgments The authors would like to express their grati- Bonanno, G. A., & Keltner, D. (1997). Facial expressions of emotion
tude to the numerous research assistants and staff involved in the data and the course of conjugal bereavement. Journal of Abnormal
collection described here. In addition, we would like to thank Dr. Psychology, 106(1), 126–137.
T.L.Gilman for her comments on the final versions of the manuscript. Bonanno, G. A., & Keltner, D. (2004). The coherence of emotion
Finally, this work was funded in part by a Farris Family Innovation systems: Comparing ‘‘on-line’’ measures of appraisal and facial
Award, Farris Family Foundation to Dr. Coifman. expression, and self-report. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 431–444.
Bulteel, K., Ceulemans, E., Thompsom, R. J., Waugh, C. E., Gotlib, I.
Compliance with ethical standards E., Tuerlinckx, F., & Kuppens, P. (2014). DeCon: A tool to
detect emotional concordance in multivariate time series data of
Conflict of interest All authors have no conflicts of interest to emotional responding. Biological Psychology, 98, 29–42.
disclose. Buss, K. A., Davidson, R. J., Kalin, N. H., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2004).
Context-specific freezing and associated physiological reactivity
Human and animal rights All procedures performed in research as a dysregulated fear response. Developmental Psychology,
involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical 40(4), 583.
standards of the relevant institutional review boards governing Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-
research involving human subjects and with the 1964 Helsinki dec- related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bul-
laration and it later amendments or comparable ethical standards. letin, 135(2), 183–204.
Moreover, written informed consent was obtained from all individual Coifman, K. G., & Almahmoud, S. Y. (2016). Emotion flexibility in
participants included in this research. psychological risk and resilience. In U. Kumar (Ed.), Handbook
of resilience: A psychosocial perspective. New York: Routledge.
Coifman, K. G., & Bonanno, G. A. (2009). Emotion context
sensitivity in adaptation and recovery. In A. Kring Sloan (Ed.),
References Emotion regulation and psychotherapy. New York: Guilford.
Coifman, K. G., & Bonanno, G. A. (2010). When distress does not
Aldao, A. (2013). The future of emotion regulation research: become depression; Emotion context sensitivity and adjustment
Capturing context. Perspectives on Psychological Science, to bereavement. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(3),
8(2), 155–172. 479–490.
Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D. A., & Taylor, G. J. (1994). The twenty- Coifman, K. G., Bonanno, G. A., Ray, R., & Gross, J. (2007). Does
item Toronto Alexithymia Scale-I. Item selection and cross- repression lead to recovery and resilience? Affective-autonomic
validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic response discrepancy in bereavement. Journal of Personality
Research, 38, 23–32. and Social Psychology, 92, 745–758.
Barlow, D. H., Ellard, K. K., Sauer-Zavala, S., Bullis, J. R., & Carl, J. Cole, P. M., Michel, M. K., & Teti, L. O. (1994). The development of
R. (2014). The origins of neuroticism. Perspectives on Psycho- emotion regulation: Biological and behavioral considerations.
logical Science, 9(5), 481–496. doi:10.1177/1745691614544528. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
Barrett, L. F. (2006). Emotions as natural kinds? Perspectives on 59, 73–100.
Psychological Science, 1, 28–58. Consedine, N. S., Magai, C., Krivoshekova, Y. S., Ryzewicz, L., &
Beauchaine, T. P., & Thayer, J. F. (2015). Heart rate variability as a Neugut, A. I. (2004). Fear, anxiety, worry, and breast cancer
transdiagnostic biomarker of psychopathology. International screening behavior: A critical review. Cancer Epidemiology,
Journal of Psychophysiology. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.08. Biomarkers and Prevention, 13, 501–510.
004. Davila, J., Karney, B. R., Hall, T. W., & Bradbury, T. N. (2003).
Bechara, A., & Damasio, A. F. (2005). The somatic marker Depressive symptoms and marital satisfaction: Within-subject
hypothesis: A neural theory of economic decision. Games and associations and the moderating effects of gender and neuroti-
Economic Behavior, 52, 336–372. cism. Journal of Family Psychology, 17, 557–570.
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false Derogatis, L. R. (1983). SCL-90-R administration, scoring and
discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple procedures manual-II for the R (revised) version. Towson, MD:
testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, B, 57, 289–300. Clinical Psychometric Research.
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (2000). On the adaptive control of the DeWall, N. C., Twenge, J. M., Koole, S. L., Baumeister, R. F., et al.
false discovery rate in multiple testing with independent statis- (2011). Automatic emotion regulation after social exclusion:
tics. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 25, 60–83. Turning to positivity. Emotion, 11, 623–636.
Bloom, T., & Friedman, H. (2013). Classifying dogs’ (Canis Dondi, M., Messinger, D., Colle, M., Tabasso, A., Simion, F., Dalla
familiaris) facial expressions from photographs. Behavioural Barba, B., & Fogel, A. (2007). A new perspective on neonatal
Processes, 96, 1–10. smiling: Differences between the judgments of expert coders and
Bodenhausen, G. V., Gabriel, S., & Lineberger, M. (2000). Sadness naive observers. Infancy, 12, 235–255.
and susceptibility to judgmental bias: The case of anchoring. Dubitsky, S., Weber, R., & Rotton, J. (1993). Heat, hostility, and
Psychological Science, 11, 320–323. immune function: The moderating effects of gender and demand

123
Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624 623

characteristics. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31, Laborde, S., Lautenbach, F., & Allen, M. S. (2015). The contribution
534–536. of coping-related variables and heart rate variability to visual
Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and search performance under pressure. Physiology & Behavior.
Emotion, 6, 169–200. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.12.003.
Ekman, P., Friesen, W., & Hager, J. (2002). Facial action coding Larsen, J. T., McGraw, A. P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Can people
system—Revised. Instructional manual and investigator guide. feel happy and sad at the same time? Journal of Personality and
San Francisco, CA: A Human Face. Social Psychology, 81, 684–696.
Fass, M. E., & Tubman, J. G. (2002). The influence of parental and LeDoux, J. (2012). A neuroscientist’s perspective on debates about
peer attachment of college students academic achievement. the nature of emotion. Emotion Review, 4, 375–379.
Psychology in the Schools, 39, 561–573. Leombruni, P., Fassino, S., Lavagnino, L., et al. (2009). The role of
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review anger in adherence to highly active antiretroviral treatment in
of General Psychology, 2, 300–319. patients infected with HIV. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics,
Gilman, T. L., Shaheen, R., Nylocks, K. M., Halachoff, D., Chapman, 78(4), 254–257.
J., Flynn, J. J., et al. (2016). A film set for the elicitation of Lerner, J. S., Dahl, R. E., Hariri, A. R., & Taylor, S. E. (2007). Facial
emotion in research: A comprehensive catalog derived from four expressions of emotion reveal neuroendocrine and cardiovascu-
decades of investigation (under review). lar stress responses. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 253–260.
Goldsmith, H. H., & Davidson, R. J. (2004). Disambiguating the Lerner, J. S., Gonzalez, R. M., Small, D. A., & Fischhoff, B. (2003).
components of emotion regulation. Child Development, 75(2), Effects of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: A
361–365. national field experiment. Psychological Science, 14(2), 144–150.
Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? The relationship Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of
between marital process and marital outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ: Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 146–159.
Erlbaum. Leventhal, H., Leventhal, E. A., & Contrada, R. J. (1998). Self-
Gottman, J. M., Coan, J., Carrere, S., & Swanson, C. (1998). regulation, health, and behavior: A perceptual-cognitive
Predicting marital happiness and stability from newlywed approach. Psychology and Health, 13, 717–733.
interactions. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 5–22. Magai, C. (1999). Affect, imagery, and attachment; Working models
Graber, E. C., Laurenceau, J. P., Miga, E., Chango, J., & Coan, J. of interpersonal affect and the socialization of emotion. In J.
(2011). Conflict and love: Predicting newlywed marital out- Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory,
comes from two interaction contexts. Journal of Family research, and clinical applications (pp. 787–802). New York:
Psychology, 25, 541–550. Guilford.
Graham, B. M., & Milad, M. R. (2011). The study of fear extinction: Magai, C., & MacFadden, S. (1995). The role of emotion in social and
Implications for anxiety disorders. American Journal of Psychi- personality development. New York: Plenum Press.
atry, 168(12), 1255–1264. Mauss, I. B., Levenson, R. W., McCarter, L., Wilhelm, F. H., &
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1995). Emotion-elicitation using Gross, J. J. (2005). The tie that binds? Coherence among emotion
films. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 87–108. experience, behavior and physiology. Emotion, 5, 175–190.
Guttman, I. (1973). Premium and protection of several procedures for McNulty, J. K., & Fincham, F. D. (2012). Beyond positive
dealing with outliers when sample sizes are moderate to large. psychology? Toward a contextual view of psychological pro-
Technometrics, 15(2), 385–404. cesses and well-being. American Psychologist, 67, 101–110.
Harvey, M. M., Coifman, K. G., Ross, G., Kleinert, D., & Giardina, P. McNulty, J. K., & Russell, V. M. (2010). When ‘‘negative’’ behaviors
(2014). Contextually appropriate emotion-word use predicts are positive: A contextual analysis of the long-term effects of
adaptive health behavior: Emotion context sensitivity and problem-solving behaviors on changes in relationship satisfac-
treatment adherence. Journal of Health Psychology. doi:10. tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 587–604.
1177/1359105314532152. Messinger, D. S. (2002). Positive and negative: Infant facial
Hill-Briggs, F. (2003). Problem-solving in diabetes self-management: expressions and emotions. Current Directions in Psychological
A model of chronic-illness self-management behavior. Annals of Science, 11(1), 1–6.
Behavioral Medicine, 25, 182–193. Messner, C., & Wanke, M. (2011). Good weather for Schwarz and
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion Clore. Emotion, 11, 436–437.
regulation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., & Shaver, P. R. (2002). Activation of the
span development. Journal of Personality, 72(6), 1301–1333. attachment system in adulthood: Threat-related primes increase
Kashdan, T. B., Barrett, L. F., & McKnight, P. E. (2015). Unpacking the accessibility of mental representations of attachment figures.
emotion differentiation; transforming unpleasant experience by Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 881–895.
perceiving distinctions in negativity. Current Directions in Mikulincer, M., Hirschberger, G., Nachmias, O., & Gillath, O. (2001).
Psychological Science, 24(1), 10–16. The affective component of the secure base schema: Affective
Kashdan, T. B., Barrios, V., Forsyth, J. P., & Steger, M. F. (2006). priming with representations of attachment security. Journal of
Experiential avoidance as a generalized psychological vulnera- Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 305–321.
bility: Comparisons with coping and emotion regulation strate- Miller, A. L., & Olson, S. L. (2000). Emotional expressiveness during
gies. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 9, 1301–1320. peer conflicts: A predictor of social maladjustment among high-
Keltner, D., Haidt, J., & Shiota, M. N. (2006). Social functionalism risk preschoolers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28,
and the evolution of emotions. In M. Schaller, J. A. Simpson, & 339–352.
D. T. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolution and social psychology (pp. Moussavi, S., Chatteriji, S., Verdes, E., Tandon, A., Vikram, P., &
115–142). Madison, CT: Psychosocial Press. Ustun, B. (2007). Depression, chronic diseases, and decrements
Keltner, D., Moffitt, T. E., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1995). Facial in health: Results from the World Health Surveys. Lancet, 370,
expressions of emotion and psychopathology in adolescent boys. 851–858.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(4), 644–652. National Center for Education Statistics, U.S. Department of Educa-
Kross, E., & Ayuduk, O. (2011). Making meaning out of negative tion. (2013). The condition of education 2013 (NCES 2013-037).
experiences by self-distancing. Current Directions in Psycho- Institutional Retention and Graduation Rates for Undergraduate
logical Science, 20(3), 187–191. Students.

123
624 Motiv Emot (2016) 40:602–624

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Watkins, E. R. (2011). A heuristic for Shields, M. C., Matt, L. M., & Coifman, K. G. (2015). Physical
developing transdiagnostic models of psychopathology: Explain- activity and negative emotion during peer-rejection: Evidence
ing multifinality and divergent trajectories. Perspectives on for emotion context sensitivity. Journal of Health Psychology.
Psychological Science, 6(6), 589–609. doi:10.1177/1359105315587139.
Ohman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive
Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psycho- appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social
logical Review, 108, 483–522. Psychology, 48, 813–838.
Papa, A., & Bonanno, G. A. (2008). Smiling in the face of adversity: Spanier, G. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for
The interpersonal and intrapersonal functions of smiling. Emo- assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of
tion, 8(1), 1–12. Marriage and the Family, 38, 15–28.
Phelps, E. A., Ling, S., & Carrasco, M. (2006). Emotion facilitates Stemmler, G., Heldmann, M., Pauls, C. A., & Scherer, T. (2001).
perception and potentiates the perceptual benefits of attention. Constraints for emotion specificity in fear and anger: the context
Psychological Science, 17(4), 292–299. counts. Psychophysiology, 38, 275–291.
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale a self-report depression scale Tibachnik, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2006). Using multivariate statistics
for research in the general population. Applied Psychological (5th ed.). New York: Pearson.
Measurement, 1(3), 385–401. Trachtenberg, F., Vichinsky, E., Haines, D., Pakbaz, Z., Mednick, L.,
Rosenberg, E. (1998). Levels of analysis and the organization of et al. (2011). Iron chelation adherence to deferoxamine and
affect. Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 247–270. deferasirox. American Journal of Hematology, 11, 433–436.
Rottenberg, J., Kasch, K. L., Gross, J. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2002). Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R., & DeScioli, P. (2013).
Sadness and amusement reactivity differentially predict concur- Disgust: Evolved function and structure. Psychological Review,
rent and prospective functioning in major depressive disorder. 120(1), 65.
Emotion, 2(2), 135–146. Vardaki, M. A., Philalithis, A. E., & Vlachonikolis, I. (2004). Factors
Rund, D. R., & Rachmilewitz, E. (2005). B-Thalassemia. New associated with attitudes and expectations of patients suffering
England Journal of Medicine, 353, 1135–1146. from B-Thalassaemia: A cross-sectional study. Scandinavian
Russell, J. A. (1991). Culture and the categorization of emotions. Journal of Caring Science, 18, 177–187.
Psychological Bulletin, 110, 426–450. Waldinger, R. J., Hauser, S. T., Schulz, M. S., Allen, J. P., & Crowell,
Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S. L., Turvey, C., & Palfai, T. P. J. A. (2004). Reading other emotions: The role of intuitive
(1995). Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: Exploring judgements in predicting marital satisfaction, quality and
emotional intelligence using the trait meta-mood scale. In J. stability. Journal of Family Psychology, 18(1), 58–71.
Pennebaker (Ed.), Emotion, disclosure, and health (pp. 125–154). Williams, K. D. (2007). Ostracism: The kiss of social death. Social
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. and Personality Psychology Compass, 1(1), 236–247.
Sanberg, T., & Conner, M. T. (2008). A mere measurement effect for Williams, K. D., Cheung, C., & Choi, W. (2000). Cyberostracism:
anticipated regret: Impacts on cervical screening attendance. Effects of being ignored over the internet. Journal of Personality
British Journal of Social Psychology, 48(2), 221–236. and Social Psychology, 79, 748–762.
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and Zaki, J., & Williams, W. C. (2013). Interpersonal emotion regulation.
judgements of well-being: informative an directive functions of Emotion, 13(5), 803–810.
affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Zautra, A. P. (2006). Emotions, stress & health. New York: Oxford
45(3), 513–523. University Press.
Sethi, N., Moulds, M. L., & Richardson, R. (2013). The role of focus Zimmerman, P., Maier, M., Winter, M., & Grossman, K. (2001).
of attention and reappraisal in prolonging the negative effects of Attachment and adolescents emotion regulation during a joint
ostracism. Group Dynamics, Theory, Research, and Practice, 17, problem-solving task with a friend. International Journal of
110–123. Behavioral Development, 25(4), 331–343.
Shaver, P. R., & Mikulincer, M. (2008). Adult attachment and
cognitive and affective reactions to positive and negative events.
Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(5), 1844–1865.

123

You might also like