Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol.

166 (3rd series)


Cingulate Cortex
B.A. Vogt, Editor
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-64196-0.00003-0
Copyright © 2019 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved

Chapter 3

Cingulate cortex in the three limbic subsystems


BRENT A. VOGT*
Cingulum Neurosciences Institute, Manlius, NY, United States
Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, MA, United States

Abstract
Broca’s (1878) definition of the limbic lobe referred to its being located at the edge of the cerebral cortex,
and Papez (1937) and MacLean (1990) welded a series of medial surface structures into what we now know
as the limbic system. The last four decades of research have provided a wealth of detailed information on
the connectivity and functions of the limbic system and one can only conclude that it is not a uniform and
single system. The cingulate cortex itself has three major divisions: anterior primarily for emotion, middle
mainly for response selection and feedback-guided decision making, and posterior/retrosplenial cortices
for visuospatial orientation and assessing the self-relevance of objects and events. Each of these divisions
has a different cytoarchitecture and set of connections. The cingulate observations lead to a new framework
of limbic organization: three limbic subsystems that include the amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex, the insula,
the hippocampus, and, of course, the cingulate cortex. This concept is expanded in terms of connectivity
among them and the underlying functions of each subsystem. The three limbic subsystems considered here
are the “anterior emotional subsystem,” the “middle sensorimotor subsystem,” and the “posterior cognitive
spatial map subsystem” for localizing personally relevant objects and episodes. A defining characteristic of
the anterior emotional subsystem is its input from the amygdala. Another interesting outcome of this anal-
ysis is that the middle hippocampus and anterior midcingulate cortex share a role in approach-avoidance
decision making suggesting a potential for connectional synergy. Thus, the concept of “a” limbic system
needs radical revision to accommodate a minimum of three limbic subsystems. As this approach was
initiated by the three-part composition of the cingulate cortex, a finer-grain analysis of the cingulate region
shows that six limbic subsystems may be a more accurate reflection of limbic organization.

OVERVIEW
the limbic system concept, see Pessoa and Hof (2015).
After Broca’s (1878) observation of the Great Limbic It is important to note that as it stood during MacLean’s
Lobe, Papez (1937) conceptualized a limbic system as life, the limbic system, even with his wealth of data and
a series of interconnected structures mainly lying on modifications, was essentially a reverberating circuit
the medial surface. Based on large stroke and tumor that received no external input (even though he recorded
cases, he concluded that the mammillary bodies, anterior visual responses) and had no output for motor or auto-
thalamic nuclei, and cingulate cortex were intercon- nomic control; even though his electrical stimulation
nected and were involved in emotional experience. studies themselves evoked activity in both, including
Classic studies by MacLean (1990) added the amygdala vocalization. Without a structural/connection substrate,
and hypothalamus to the limbic system, which is now however, his views were not widely accepted and he
referred to as the Papez/MacLean model. For an excel- was an investigator working on problems that were
lent review of the transition from Broca’s concept to innovative before his time.

*Correspondence to: Brent A. Vogt, Ph.D., President, Cingulum Neurosciences Institute, 4435 Stephanie Drive, Manlius, NY,
13104, United States. Tel: +1-315-280-6847, Fax: +1-617-358-2105, E-mail: bavogt@bu.edu
40 B.A. VOGT
From our studies of the cingulate cortex, it has become can occur unconditionally to stimuli (e.g., autonomic
clear that we cannot simply adopt the single limbic system responses to stimuli that prepare the body for activity
concept. Indeed, Rolls (2015) proposed that there are and reflexes such as freezing or flight), reflexes for which
two limbic systems: an anterior “emotion limbic system” no intervening state of motivation or emotion is required.
including the amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and Value coding systems must be separate from purely sen-
anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and a posterior system or sory or motor systems, and while a goal is being sought
“hippocampal memory system” that receives information or if a goal is not obtained, the value-related representa-
from neocortical areas about spatial location and objects tion must remain to direct further goal-directed behavior,
and includes the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and ret- and it is these continuing goal-related states to which
rosplenial cortex (RSC). However, this view still does not emotion is related. Critical to this definition is the fact
accommodate cingulate heterogeneity and some of its key that they operate on a common value scale that leads
functions, nor those of other cortical parts of the limbic to the selection of different rewards with appropriately
systems. Most important is the fact that the anterior mid- balanced probabilities and often depending on modula-
cingulate cortex (aMCC) is an action-monitoring system tion by internal motivational signals. In this context,
that regulates somatomotor output (see Chapters 1 and 4) the OFC provides two major types of information: value
and should not be included with the two systems recog- assessment and value scaling such that options can be
nized in the two–limbic system model. Of course, the assessed and prioritized via the action-outcome and mon-
pMCC and dPCC subserve orientation of the body in itoring systems as in the midcingulate cortex (MCC).
space and are part of an attentional system. The purpose OFC itself receives sensory and premotor inputs that
of this chapter is to put forward the theoretical basis for at subserve these functions as noted later.
least three limbic subsystems. Each of the relevant struc-
tures is considered in terms of its primate connections
MACRO ORGANIZATION OF FOUR
and functions. In order to keep this review to a manage-
LIMBIC REGIONS DEFINE THREE
able length, this assessment does not include subcortical
LIMBIC SUBSYSTEMS
structures including the nucleus accumbens, cholinergic
septum/basal forebrain, thalamus, hypothalamus, mam- Early gross anatomy and cytoarchitectonic studies of
millary bodies, periaqueductal gray, or parabrachial the amygdala, cingulate cortex, OFC, the insula, and hip-
nuclei. These latter structures will need to be added to pocampus shows that each of these regions in primates is
the three–limbic subsystem networks at a future time. comprised of three major divisions as shown in Fig. 3.1.
We conclude with the proposition based on the organiza- (1) In OFC, Hof et al. (1995) identified three anteropos-
tion of the cingulate cortex that there may actually be six terior divisions of about equal volumes: an agranular
limbic subsystems. posterior division (periallocortex), an intermediate dys-
In brief the three limbic subsystems considered here are granular component, and an anteromedial/anterolateral
the “anterior emotional subsystem,” the “middle sensori- granular cortex (isocortex). (2) The cingulate cortex
motor subsystem,” and the “posterior cognitive spatial has three divisions with an agranular ACC, an MCC
map subsystem” for localizing personally relevant objects dysgranular division, and a granular PCC/RSC (see
and episodes. Due to the substantial connectivity of each Chapter 1). (3) The insula has long been known to have
subsystem with the hippocampus (HPC), it does not three major components (Mesulam and Muffson, 1982).
appear to be accurate to stipulate that any one subsystem These include a rostral agranular, a middle dysgranular,
has a predominant role in memory. Finally, I propose that and a caudal granular division (Fig. 3.1C.1). Fig. 3.1C.2
each subsystem has a connection substrate summarized in shows the regions of interest (ROIs) used by Taylor et al.
Figs.3.4 and 3.5 with the latter emphasizing directional (2009) in human studies to assess interactions between
flow in the limbic networks that guide these functions the insula and cingulate cortex. (4) While the amygdala
from sensory to skeletomotor and autonomic output. has as many as nine subnuclei, three divisions have been
If we are to propose multiple limbic systems, we proposed. Amunts et al. (2005) observed three primary
must consider the content of emotion before attempting divisions using probabilistic mapping (Fig. 3.1D). (5)
to understand their connections and functions. Rolls The HPC formation is comprised of the dentate gyrus,
(2014) defines emotion as states elicited by rewards CA1–4 sectors, prosubiculum, subiculum, and presubicu-
and punishers; i.e., by instrumental reinforcers, which lum. However, since the subiculum and CA1sector are the
are the goals for action. The goal must be motivating primary output structures of the HPC, their connectivities
and elicited by emotional states that are rewarding and are emphasized here. Demeter et al. (1985; Fig. 3.1E.3)
reinforce the action just made, and the goal must exist first recognized three anteroposterior (septotemporal ori-
to direct behavior independently of prespecified entation) components of the hippocampal formation in
responses or habits. Emotional and motivational states the monkey: anterior body, middle body, and posterior
may be contrasted with responses or reflexes, which body. Lia et al. (2013; Fig. 3.1E.1) recognized a similar
CINGULATE CORTEX IN THE THREE LIMBIC SUBSYSTEMS 41

Fig. 3.1. Macro layout of the three subsystems. (A) Hof et al. (1995). (B) Chapter 1. (C.1) Insula modified from Nieuwenhuys
(2012) and (C.2) from the three ROIs set by Taylor et al. (2009) for resting-state fMRI studies. (D) Three amygdala divisions
recognized by Amunts et al. (2005) in a probabilistic study. (E.1) Human hippocampus analyzed with arterial spin labeling by
Lia et al. (2013). (E.2) Human hippocampus nodes assessed for intrinsic functional networks by Qin et al. (2016). (E.3) Monkey
hippocampal divisions from Demeter et al. (1988).
42 B.A. VOGT
three divisions in humans using arterial spin labeling. Per- Fink-Heimer method in monkeys and concluded that all
fusion studies by this group indicated that anterior, mid- sensory systems contain areas that project to the amygda-
dle, and posterior HPC segments have unique transit loid complex. These sources included the visual area TE
times and regional cerebral blood flow values. Of these and ventral TG, auditory anterior TA and dorsal TG, taste
three, the middle HPC (mHPC) has the highest perfusion area in the anterior insula, and somesthesis SI. (Their
and the shortest transit time and the anterior HPC method did not show strong SI and SII connectivity,
(aHPC) has the lowest perfusion and the longest transit but Amaral and Price (1984) used autoradiography of
time. Finally, Qin et al. (2016) analyzed large-scale intrin- projections in the opposite direction and showed them
sic functional networks in three nodes of the HPC to be quite robust.) Also the parabrachial nucleus is a
(Fig. 3.1E.2). Thus, all the relevant players in the three source of visceral nociceptive input (Bernard and
limbic subsystems have already been recognized. The Besson, 1990; Bernard et al., 1994). Continuing with
amygdala is an important part of the anterior agranular the Turner study, within the visual system, progressively
and dysgranular subsystems and, with its extensive sen- heavier and more widespread efferents arose from suc-
sory afferents, may be a key starting point of functional cessively later stages of the amygdalar sources of input.
activity in the anterior limbic subsystem. The posterior half of TE sends a moderate projection to
the dorsal part of the lateral nucleus, the anterior half of
INTRALIMBIC CONNECTIONS AND TE sends a heavy projection to the dorsal parts of both the
EXTRINSIC POINTS OF CONVERGENCE lateral and basal nuclei, and the ventral part of TG sends a
heavy projection to the dorsal and medial parts of the lat-
The anterior emotional subsystem has robust interactions
eral and basal nuclei and to the dorsal part of the basal
with the amygdala, the middle subsystem engaged in
accessory nucleus. Thus, they drew the very important
decision making and movement in conjunction with neo-
conclusion that progressively greater influences on
cortical sensorimotor cortices, and a posterior subsystem
amygdaloid activity are made by successively more
that engages in spatial orientation to extract personally
highly processed sensory information.
relevant objects from the visual environment; i.e., per-
In terms of the OFC, the accessory basal and lateral
sons, objects, and episodes in their appropriate context
amygdaloid nuclei project most strongly to areas in the
(Bar and Aminoff, 2003). All of these general functions
posterior and medial orbital cortex. The medial, anterior
relate to limbic functions as they involve emotion and
cortical, and central amygdaloid nuclei and the peria-
motivation. Finally, the HPC contributes to memory in
mygdaloid cortex are connected with the posterior orbital
each subsystem and is unique only to the extent that it
areas (Vogt and Pandya, 1987; Carmichael and Price,
subserves different memories in each.
1995b; Fig. 3.2A and B). These nuclei project primarily
The four limbic regions are highly interconnected;
to each area of the ACC and less so to aMCC. This latter
however, connections often do not respect cytoarchitec-
connection is noteworthy because the ventral bank of the
tural borders, i.e., there are often overlaps in intrinsic and
cingulate gyrus in aMCC is engaged in fear and may be
extrinsic connectivity across each limbic region. To add
critical for somatomotor escape/avoidance responses
to the difficulty of summarizing connections, many dif-
(Vogt et al., 2003). In terms of the insula, once again these
ferent organization schemes have been reported for each
amygdaloid nuclei project primarily to the anterior, agra-
structure under consideration here, i.e., consensus is rare
nular sector. Finally, Aggleton (2012) also recognized
rather than common. As most corticocortical connections
three divisions of the HPC and noted projections that pre-
are reciprocal, we will begin by assuming this to be the
dominantly arise from the rostral HPC to include those to
case to reduce complexity except in Fig. 3.5 where the
the amygdala. Thus, the amygdala receives inputs from
direction of information flow is shown across the various
all sensory modalities and projects to all parts of the ante-
limbic subsystems.
rior limbic subsystem. Indeed, this latter fact is one of the
defining characteristics of this subsystem.
Amygdala
Damage to OFC has long been associated with defi-
The amygdala has been implicated in implicit and cits in reversal learning. OFC damage also causes inflex-
explicit emotional learning, it influences perception ible associative encoding in basolateral amygdala (BLA)
and facilitates attention to salient stimuli, and it is during reversal learning. Stalnaker et al. (2007) showed
involved in social behavior and extinction of emotional that rats with OFC lesions were slower to acquire rever-
learning (Phelps and LeDoux, 2005). It plays a key role sals than sham controls in a two-odor go/no-go discrim-
in setting emotional states that are important substrates ination task, followed by two serial reversals of the final
for the three limbic subsystems and projects to the ante- problem. This impairment was abolished by accompany-
rior component of each subsystem upon which the other ing BLA lesions, while BLA lesions alone had no effect
subsystems operate. Turner et al. (1980) used the lesion/ on reversal learning. These results are consistent with the
CINGULATE CORTEX IN THE THREE LIMBIC SUBSYSTEMS 43

Fig. 3.2. (A) Pronounced connections of medial OFC with both pACC (1) and vPCC (2). Reciprocal connections between these
cingulate subregions are apparent with cingulate injections (D.1). A remarkable overlap that has still not been explained that occurs
between OFC and PCC in pACC. (B) Tritiated amino acid injections mainly into pACC with spread into aMCC. (C) Injection into
pMCC. (D) Injection into dPCC with some spread into RSC. The middle panel of monkey brains was modified from Pandya et al.
(1991). (E) Human PET studies seeded in dPCC (1) and vPCC (2) modified from Vogt et al. (2006). Panel A: Modified from
Carmichael, S.T., Price, J.L. (1995b). Limbic connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys.
J Comp Neurol. 363, 515–641.

hypothesis that OFC facilitates cognitive flexibility by in translating them to the anteroposterior divisions
promoting updating of associative encoding in down- shown in Fig. 3.1A. Also note that the agranular area
stream areas. Therefore, it is likely that the amygdala is the most posterior region in contrast to that of the other
plays a key role in the emotional functions of the anterior three components of the limbic subsystems. All of the
limbic subsystem. limbic structures are substantially connected with the fol-
lowing posterior and medial orbital areas: the posterome-
dial, medial, intermediate, and lateral agranular insular
Orbitofrontal cortex
areas (Iapm, Iam, Iai, and Ial, respectively) and areas
The medial/rostral part of OFC has robust connections llm, 13a, 13b, 14c, and 14r. In contrast, lateral orbital
with ACC and PCC (Fig. 3.2A.1) in pACC and vPCC, areas 12o, 12 m, and 12 L and medial wall areas 24a,
respectively. The intrinsic cingulate connections 24b, and 32 are primarily connected with the amygdala
(Pandya et al., 1981; Fig. 3.2B.1) mirror this interaction. and the temporal pole. The projection from the HPC orig-
As the definition of emotion requires value scaling and inates from the rostral subiculum and terminates in the
action-outcome modulation, it is particularly important medial orbital areas. The parahippocampal cortical areas
that OFC projects to MCC. Carmichael and Price (including the temporal polar, entorhinal, perirhinal, and
(1995a) showed the sensory and premotor connections posterior parahippocampal cortices) are primarily con-
of individual areas in the OFC. Their results indicate that nected with posterior and medial orbital areas with some
these connections define a general organization such that projections to the dorsal part of the medial wall. The
lateral orbital areas receive most of the sensory- and ACC sends axons to the medial wall, the medial orbital
premotor-related inputs, and posterior and central orbital areas, and to lateral areas 12o, 12r, and Iai. The posterior
areas participate in several stages of convergent olfac- cingulate gyrus, including the caudomedial lobule,
tory, visceral, and gustatory processing as emphasized which is the most caudal part of the cingulate gyrus,
in Fig. 3.4. While OFC has premotor connections, it is was particularly strongly connected with area l and lm
surprising that it does not have pronounced projections as shown in Fig. 3.3A.1 and 2.
to MCC, which has key somatomotor outputs. Vogt et al. (2006) used PET in the human with seeds
Carmichael and Price (1995b) present their findings in in the dPCC and vPCC to identify correlated voxels of
a mediolateral orientation, so I have taken a few liberties interest (VOI; Fig. 3.2E). While the OFC did not show
44 B.A. VOGT

Fig. 3.3. Connections of the amygdala and insula. (A.1) Retrogradely labeled neurons in the amygdala after horseradish perox-
idase (HRP) injections into the lateral insula and anterior OFC (2 and 4). (B.2) A large tritiated amino acid injection into the
amygdala (2) showing the topography of the cingulate projection (1) and its laminar distribution in areas 24 and 25 (3; Vogt
and Pandya, 1987). (C) Retrogradely labeled neurons following HRP injections into the anterior (1), middle (2), and posterior
(3) insula; modified from Mufson and Mesulam (1982). (D) Resting state functional connectivity modified from Taylor et al.
(2009). (E) Organization of the insula proposed by Nieuwenhuys (2012). Abbreviations: AA, agranular anterior zone; GI and
GII, primary and secondary gustatory areas; ILC, insular limbic cortex; INTC, insular nociceptive and thermoreceptive cortex;
ISAC, insular somatic association cortex; IVSC, insular viscerosensory cortex; OCI, primary olfactory cortex. Panel A: Modified
from Carmichael, S.T., Price, J.L. (1995b). Limbic connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys.
J Comp Neurol. 363, 515–641.

correlations of activity in the dPCC, those of the vPCC


Insula
did. Correlated VOIs were mainly in the middle parts
of OFC. Interestingly, the dPCC had strong correlated For a thorough review of insular organization and func-
VOIs in MCC, while vPCC did not. As these VOIs tions, the reader is referred to Nieuwenhuys (2012)
entered the cingulate sulcus where the cingulate premotor whose anatomic designations and viewpoints are quite
areas reside, it suggests that the visuospatial orientation of useful. He recognized the following divisions: AA, agra-
dPCC plays an important role in somatomotor output. nular anterior zone; GI and GII, primary and secondary
CINGULATE CORTEX IN THE THREE LIMBIC SUBSYSTEMS 45

Fig. 3.4. Summary of most of the important interactions among the three limbic subsystems as discussed in the text. Of particular note
are the extensive sensory inputs to the amygdala and OFC, the centrality of MCC with is somatomotor connections, and the fact that
the anterior part of each anterior subsystem receives amygdalar input, which is a defining feature of the anterior subsystems. Finally,
autonomic output arises from the amygdala and ACC. Abbreviations: GI, GII, primary and secondary gustatory cortex; HPC, hip-
pocampal complex; noci, nociception; PB, parabrachial nucleus; PirCx, piriform cortex; PreSMA/SMA, presupplementary motor
area/supplementary motor area; SI and SII, primary and secondary somatosensory cortex; STG, superior temporal gyrus.

gustatory areas, respectively; ILC, insular limbic cortex; Speech production was demonstrated in a series of
INTC, insular nociceptive and thermoreceptive cortex; aphasic patients and revealed that in patients with a dis-
ISAC, insular somatic association cortex; IVSC, insular order in coordinating the movements for speech articula-
viscerosensory cortex; and OCI, primary olfactory tion, an area in the anterosuperior insula was specifically
cortex. The distribution of these structure/function divi- damaged.
sions is shown in Fig. 3.2E and many of the functional A review of functional neuroimaging studies by Cole
summaries mentioned later are from this review. and Schneider (2007) showed that the AIC is involved in
Belin et al. (2000) discussed the auditory posterior a large variety of cognitive control tasks that are more
insula, as primates share the ability to reliably extract complex than the sensorimotor responses noted earlier.
affective- and identity-related cues from conspecific They provided evidence indicating that this insular
vocalizations as in many other species. This information region forms, together with other cortical regions includ-
arises from along the upper bank of the superior temporal ing the ACC, presupplementary motor area, dorsal
gyrus. Polymodal vestibular neurons are located in the premotor cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and the
PIVC. Physiologic studies in rhesus monkeys have posterior parietal cortex, a highly interconnected cogni-
shown that neurons in the granular part of the insula tive control network.
respond to innocuous cutaneous stimuli as well as A meta-analysis of imaging studies of emotion
nociceptive and thermal stimuli. The posterior insula was presented by Phan et al. (2002) that suggested the
receives projections from the viscera, which is relayed anterior insula is preferentially involved in the evaluative,
from the nucleus of the solitary tract and the parabrachial experiential, and expressive aspects of specific indivi-
complex. dual emotions such as “happiness,” “sadness,” “fear,”
The results of experimental neuroanatomic studies and “disgust.” These were also analyzed in the cingulate
and of stimulation and ablation experiments in monkeys cortex (Vogt et al., 2003) with ACC showing the highest
show a gustatory (taste) center in the anterior insula. degree of emotional activity for all emotions analyzed
Olfaction is also part of insular function as electrical including “sadness” in sACC and “happiness” in pACC.
stimulation of the anterior insula in humans evokes olfac- The cingulate gyral surface of aMCC expressed the high-
tory sensations. Finally, visceromotor control has been est density of studies reporting “fear” and the pMCC
demonstrated with electrical stimulation of the insula was almost devoid of emotional activity, while the PCC
in humans and may elicit a variety of visceromotor was engaged almost exclusively by “happiness” and con-
phenomena, including vomiting and other alterations trol nonemotional activity. Furthermore, social species
of the gastrointestinal tract, respiratory arrest, as well employ social emotions to form group organizations that
as changes in cardiac rate and rhythm control, and extend beyond the individual. Studies by Lamm and
blood pressure. This “sensorimotor” area is situated in Singer (2010) indicate that the anterior insula is consis-
the anterosuperior part of the posterior insular lobule. tently involved in social emotions, including empathy
46 B.A. VOGT
and compassion, and interpersonal interactions such as Overall, the posterior HPC (subiculum, presubicu-
fairness and cooperation and understanding the mental lum, parasubiculum) provides the majority of RSC
state of others through language (Theory of Mind). projections, which predominantly terminate in area
Neuroimaging has often shown that the AIC and ACC 29 (Kobayashi and Amaral, 2003). However, it is the
are jointly activated in subjects experiencing emotion. anterior half of the subicular cortex that projects to ven-
Because the ACC underlies motivation, it seems likely tral areas v23a/b and 29 (the caudomedial lobule). In con-
that the AIC and ACC serve as complementary limbic trast, the more posterior subiculum, presubiculum, and
sensory and motor systems that work together in a way parasubiculum project to the remainder of the RSC
similar to the somatosensory and somatomotor cortices. (Kobayashi and Amaral, 2003).
The AIC and ACC contain salient components of a core Just as the efferents from the primate HPC often show
control network that guides all mental activity and a rostrocaudal gradient, so do some of its afferents. The
behavior in adult humans. The proviso to these observa- more lateral and posterior portion of the entorhinal cortex
tions is that while monkey studies show connections projects to the posterior HPC, whereas the more medial
between the anterior insula and ACC (Fig. 3.2C.1), the and anterior portion of the entorhinal cortex projects to
human studies have still not resolved this connection the anterior HPC (Witter and Amaral, 1991). Other
(Fig. 3.2D). Thus, the work of Taylor et al. (2009) in examples include projections from the amygdala and
human show that the anterior and middle insula are con- the OFC, both of which preferentially terminate in anterior
nected with MCC, while the posterior insula is connected CA1 and adjacent subiculum (Aggleton, 1986; Saunders
mainly with pMCC and vPCC. and Rosene, 1988; Carmichael and Price, 1995b).
Functional connectivity in human HPC is being
resolved with resting-state fMRI, correlation studies dur-
Hippocampus
ing task challenges, and many other approaches. For
Blatt and Rosene (1998) were the first to show that three example, Libby et al. (2012) collected whole-brain
parts of the monkey HPC (Fig. 3.1E.3) had connections, resting-state fMRI, and in another study they used
organized in columns, with specific proximal/distal high-resolution fMRI to target the HPC in another sub-
distributions. Their study of the direct HPC projections ject sample. The first experiment showed that the peri-
to the temporal cortex revealed complementary, longitu- rhinal cortex has preferential connectivity with the
dinal strips of neurons in CA1 and subiculum that pro- anterior HPC (aHPC), while the parahippocampal cortex
jected to different temporal areas. In terms of afferents, has preferential connectivity with pHPC. The latter
the rostral and medial entorhinal cortex projects to the experiment indicated that this anterior–posterior func-
distal CA1 and proximal subiculum, while the caudal tional connectivity dissociation was more evident for
and lateral entorhinal cortex preferentially projects to subfields CA1 and subiculum than for a combined
the proximal CA1 and distal subiculum (Witter and CA2/CA3/dentate gyrus region as would be predicted
Amaral, 1991). Aggleton (2012) summarized projections from monkey connection studies. The former experiment
that predominantly arise from the anterior HPC and revealed preferential perirhinal cortex connectivity with
include those to the amygdala, prefrontal cortex, temporal an anterior temporal and frontal cortical network and
pole, and nucleus accumbens (Aggleton, 1986; preferential parahippocampal connectivity with a poste-
Carmichael and Price, 1995b; Blatt and Rosene, 1998). rior medial temporal, parietal, and occipital network.
In contrast, projections to RSC, ACC area 24, and Thus, in the human medial temporal lobes, perirhinal
area TE predominantly arise from the posterior HPC and parahippocampal cortices are associated with
(Aggleton and Brown, 2005; Kobayashi and distinct neocortical pathways that may differentially
Amaral, 2003). interact with regions along the anterior–posterior axis
Projections to the medial prefrontal cortex originate in of the HPC.
areas 14, 25, and 32 and OFC areas 11 and 13 (Saunders The two-segment organization of the HPC, however,
and Rosene, 1988). The hippocampal projections to area has been extended to three segments that fit with the
TE also arise from the distal CA1 (Yukie, 2000) and prox- three–limbic subsystem concept. Qin et al. (2016)
imal subiculum (Insausti and Munoz, 2001). The inputs to explored the large-scale intrinsic functional networks
the amygdala arise from the proximal subiculum and from of three levels of the hippocampus (also in parahippo-
CA1, which is found in the medial uncus and is separated campal cortex but not discussed here or shown in
from the CA1 sector (Saunders et al., 1988). In contrast, Fig. 3.1E.2). Comparisons between the pHPC, mHPC,
the HPC projections to the RSC preferentially arise from and aHPC nodes showed that the pHPC, compared to
the distal subiculum, along with efferents from the adja- the aHPC, had stronger connectivity with multiple corti-
cent presubiculum and parasubiculum (Kobayashi and cal and subcortical regions. Cortical regions included
Amaral, 2003). PCC, RSC, precuneus, ACC and MCC, ventromedial
CINGULATE CORTEX IN THE THREE LIMBIC SUBSYSTEMS 47
prefrontal cortex, middle frontal gyrus, and visual asso- memory representations (i.e., pattern separation). The
ciation cortex. A similar but relatively weaker pattern CA1 and CA3 sectors have been implicated to varying
was in contrasts of the mHPC with the aHPC. Compared degrees in pattern separation and completion (e.g.,
to the mHPC, the aHPC showed significantly greater retrieval of a complete mnemonic representation based
connectivity with the amygdala, insula, temporal pole, on a partial input) depending on the nature of information
fusiform gyrus, lingual gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, and/or degree of dissimilarity between mnemonic repre-
and motor cortex. ROI analyses for three hippocampal sentations and incoming sensory information. In addition
nodes confirmed significant differences in connectivity to this, these regions may also play a role in novelty/
between anterior, middle, and posterior HPC nodes mismatch detection. As this is a function of aMCC, we
and the amygdala, postcentral gyrus, temporal pole, predict that this is also a function of the mHPC and there
medial prefrontal cortex, fusiform gyrus, and PCC/ is likely a connection between these two subregions.
RSC. Thus, the functional connectivity of the hippocam- Rolls (2015) was the first to differentiate the limbic
pus is characterized by decreasing connectivity strength system into two parts as already noted. In his formula-
along the posterior-to-anterior gradient with multiple tion, the anterior limbic system and related structures
posterior midline structures. Compared to posterior and including the OFC and amygdala are involved in emo-
middle subdivisions, the aHPC showed stronger connec- tion, reward valuation, and reward-related decision mak-
tivity only with the bilateral anterior medial temporal ing (but not memory), with the value representations
lobe and the left temporal pole. Thus, the anterior node transmitted to the ACC and aMCC for action-outcome
showed stronger connectivity with the anterior medial learning or an “emotion limbic system.” These are based
temporal lobe, and the posterior node showed stronger on feedforward projections that learn associations from
connectivity with widely distributed cortical and subcor- visual, olfactory, and auditory stimuli to primary rein-
tical regions including those involved in sensory and forcers such as taste, touch, and noxious stimuli. The
reward processing. OFC overshadows the amygdala in learning that is
important for social and emotional behavior. The aHPC
(Fig. 3.4) is likely part of this circuitry given its heavy
AHPC
interactions with the amygdala and OFC. Emotion can
Ito and Lee (2016) reviewed the role of the aHPC in enter this memory system from the OFC and be recalled
approach-avoidance learning and highlight a few studies to the OFC during memory recall, but the emotional and
that examined a role for the human aHPC in conflict pro- HPC networks or “limbic systems” operate independently
cessing. Bach et al. (2014) reported that subjects were of each other except as an emotional state or reward value
required to forage for yellow virtual tokens within a attribute that may be part of an episodic memory.
two-dimensional grid in the presence of a predator that
was asleep in one corner and would awake to take the
MHPC
tokens unless the participant retreated to a safe corner.
Healthy subjects adapted their behavior when the prob- This region of the HPC has received the least attention
ability of threat increased, showing greater avoidance from human connection and functional studies compared
behavior and behavioral inhibition. This adaptive behav- to the aHPC and pHPC. However, as noted earlier, it has
ior was accompanied by changes in greater aHPC activ- some striking properties. Lia et al. (2013) showed that the
ity and there was no evidence that this difference was mHPC has the highest perfusion and the shortest transit
driven by a primary impairment in memory and/or spatial time and the aHPC has the lowest perfusion and the lon-
cognition. Additionally, O’Neil et al. (2015) found signif- gest transit time. Using fMRI, K€ohler et al. (2005)
icantly greater aHPC involvement during high- compared reported that the right mHPC responded to the novelty
to no-conflict face-scene pairs. Since perceptual and mem- of spatial and nonspatial relationships but not to the nov-
ory retrieval demands were matched across tasks with no- elty of individual objects vs familiar objects. This finding
and high-conflict trials, these findings provided further did not hold for either of the other two divisions.
evidence that the human aHPC plays an important role A similar finding has been reported for the MCC for
in approach-avoidance conflict processing, a function novel lists of words (Raichle et al., 1994) where verb
not usually associated with the HPC. generation for a list of nouns for which verbs were to
Ito and Lee (2016) also reviewed the more traditional be stated was practiced. The PET response declined
views of HPC functions but in its subfields. Different with practice but increased again with a new list of
subfields of the HPC subserve differential (but also over- nouns. The interaction of mHPC with aMCC must be
lapping) mnemonic functions. For instance, the dentate supportive of each other. The hippocampus has also
gyrus has been implicated in mismatch detection, mem- been implicated in approach-avoidance conflict monitor-
ory encoding, and the ability to discriminate two similar ing (Ito and Lee, 2016) as well as in pattern separation
48 B.A. VOGT
and completion (e.g., retrieval of a complete memory part of aMCC in addition to the striatum. These functional
representation based on a partial input) depending on connectivity results suggest that the pHPC may play a role
the nature of information and/or degree of dissimilarity in decision making by predicting the possibilities of
between memory representations and incoming sensory what might come next, allowing the OFC and striatum
information. Thus, the mHPC and aMCC may both play to evaluate the expected choice options to make the correct
a role in novelty/mismatch detection. We predict that action at a choice point.
given the similar functions of aMCC and mHPC there Finally, a substrate for recollection memory in the
is likely a connection between these two subregions, i.e., pHPC may be determined by the volume of its posterior
the mid-CA1 sector synergizes with the aMCC. vs anterior segments. Poppenk and Moscovitch (2011)
showed that volumetric measurements can reliably pre-
dict recollection where better memory is associated
PHPC
with a larger pHPC than aHPC. They also observed that
Rolls (2015) considered evidence that the pHPC and con- functional connectivity with a neocortical network
nected limbic structures including the PCC, mammillary was identified during a postencoding resting-state scan,
body, and anterior thalamus are involved in episodic or a relationship that was weakest when there was no
event memory, but not emotion. This view is supported appreciable study-test interval.
by the organization and functions of PCC. This
“hippocampal system” receives information from neo-
Cingulate cortex
cortical areas about spatial location and objects, as is
the case for PCC (Bar and Aminoff, 2003), and can rap- Chapter 1 summarizes the key functions of each cingu-
idly associate this information to engage in feedback late subregion and will not be repeated. Fig. 3.4 shows
mechanisms. The system can later recall the whole of this the complexity of interactions among the components
information in the CA3 sector from any component as a of the three limbic subsystems but does not show direc-
feedback process and recall the information to neocortical tionality that is crucial to determining the transition
areas by a feedback recall process including the PCC/RSC. from sensory input to the limbic subsystems engaged
The argument for three limbic subsystems, however, does in value scaling of potential options for action outcomes
not depend solely on a “hippocampocentric” viewpoint and monitoring. Thus, Fig. 3.5 depicts cingulate con-
and the HPC has connections that subserve more than epi- nectivity in a somewhat different light by providing
sodic memory and spatial orientation including novelty directionality to how the overall limbic system may
detection and feedback-mediated decision making that operate and includes MCC as a central hub for motor
links it to other parts of the three limbic subsystems includ- output and, along with the amygadala, the ACC auto-
ing the OFC, insula, MCC, and PCC. nomic projections. In this diagram, the pivotal nature
The HPC may use temporal information to retrieve of amygdalar projections, mainly from the BLA and
specific prior experiences, particularly when a represen- accessory basal nuclei with their sensory afferents
tation shares common elements with a similar prior expe- and efferents to the anterior limbic subsystems, is
rience. A defining characteristic of episodic memory is shown, particularly those to the pACC and less so those
that events comprising an episode are linked together to aMCC. Value scaling is performed in the OFC with its
in time (Tulving, 1984). It may be the role of the pHPC full range of sensory afferents and precise understand-
to link events comprising an episodic memory together in ing of the organism’s internal needs. The anterior
a sequential manner across time (Howard et al., 2005). insular cortex is thought to form a global representation
Ross et al. (2009) used fMRI to examine the role of of the bodily state, because it is situated to integrate
the HPC during the learning and retrieval of sequences. homeostatic afferent activity from the dorsal posterior
Participants learned four sequences comprised of six insula with emotional salience (Craig, 2002).
faces each. An overlapping condition, where sequences Resting-state functional connectivity analysis iden-
shared common elements, was comprised of two tifies cortical areas that have strong temporally correlated
sequences in which two identical faces were shown as low-frequency (<0.1 Hz) activity in a nontask at rest.
the middle images of both sequences. A nonoverlapping Using this approach, the relationship between anatomi-
condition contained two sequences that did not share cally distinct, but functionally connected, brain regions
any faces between them. The data were split into learning can be investigated. These low-frequency fluctuations
and experienced phases. The pHPC was activated during may be functionally relevant indices of connectivity
sequence learning and during the retrieval of learned between brain regions subserving similar or related brain
sequences. At the critical choice point of overlapping functions (Birn, 2007). Taylor et al. (2009) used this
stimuli, Ross et al. (2011) showed the left HPC had stron- methodology and observed that activity in both the left
ger functional connectivity with the OFC and a small and right anterior insula was strongly correlated with
CINGULATE CORTEX IN THE THREE LIMBIC SUBSYSTEMS 49

Fig. 3.5. Directionality of information flow from sensory afferents (amygdala) to value scaling (OFC) and action outcome and
monitoring MCC). Modified from Fig. 3.4. Of particular note is the wide distribution of amygdalar (BLA and accessory basal
nuclei) throughout the anterior limbic subsystems including projections of the sACC, and central nucleus of the amygdala to
the autonomic nervous system (ANS; nuc. Ambiguous, hypothalamus, periaqueductal gray, etc.) and that of the cingulate premotor
areas to the supplementary motor area/presupplementary motor area and spinal cord (SC). Note also that the amygdala has an
influence on this system as well via projections to aMCC.

the bilateral pACC/aMCC and bilateral pMCC. How- in the mOFC that projects to MCC, which is the critical
ever, the fMRI signal in the middle and posterior insula node for skeletomotor output from the three limbic sub-
was strongly correlated with bilateral pMCC but not systems. As discussed in Chapter 1, aMCC is critical for
with the pACC and aMCC. The anterior insula bilaterally feedback-mediated decision making and has key outputs
showed stronger resting-state connectivity with the to the supplementary motor area/presupplementary motor
pACC/aMCC than either the middle or posterior insula. area and spinal cord. It is an interesting fact that MCC has
However, the mid-insula was more strongly connected virtually no emotional function except for “fear” that likely
with the pMCC than the posterior insula. Based on these drives avoidance behaviors. The pACC and vPCC that are
observations, Taylor et al. (2009) proposed that the sys- engaged in emotion are reciprocally connected and bypass
tem linking the anterior insula with the pACC/aMCC MCC, which is the last stage for action and feedback
may be involved in emotional salience monitoring, while monitoring. Fig. 3.3B.1 emphasizes this arrangement with
the system linking the entire insula with the pMCC could MCC having extensive dorsal projections to sensorimotor
be a general salience and action system. cortices that pACC and vPCC do not have.
Seeley et al. (2007) suggested that a task-positive net- This is not to say, however, that PCC has no role in
work is composed of a “salience network” that includes choice among behavioral options. Hayden et al. (2008)
aMCC, OFC and insular cortices, and subcortical limbic recorded from neurons in dPCC in areas 23 and 31 in
structures in an “executive-control network,” connecting the cingulate gyrus and ventral bank of the cingulate
the dorsolateral frontal cortex with the parietal cortex. sulcus, anterior to the intersection of the marginal and
They proposed that the “salience network” responds to horizontal rami. Single neurons in monkeys during per-
the amount of personal salience and not to the stimuli’s formance of a gambling task in which the reward outcome
specific nature (i.e., homeostatic, emotional, or cogni- of each choice strongly influenced subsequent choices.
tive). A key conclusion from this work is that there is PCC neurons were found to signal reward outcomes in
a general salience and action system that links the entire a nonlinear fashion, and outcome-contingent modulations
insula and MCC, a system involved in environmental in firing rate persisted into subsequent trials. Moreover,
monitoring, response selection, and somatomotor body firing rate on any one trial predicted switching to the alter-
orientation. This is likely the reason that cingulate and native option on the next trial. Finally, microstimulation in
insular cortices are so frequently activated in tandem. dPCC following risky choices promoted a preference
The functions and connectivity of OFC have already reversal for the safe option on the following trial. Collec-
been discussed; however, here we will put it in the context tively, these results demonstrate that PCC directly contrib-
of the three limbic subsystems. Value scaling for selection utes to the evaluative processes that support dynamic
among the most effective (internally relevant) choices changes in decision making in volatile environments.
requires a full set of sensory inputs that converge either Finally, sACC has substantial projections into auto-
directly or via intrinsic circuitry. This I propose to happen nomic motor structures. One set of connections arise
50 B.A. VOGT
from area 25 and terminate directly in the medial part of Blatt GJ, Rosene DL (1998). Organization of direct hippo-
the central nucleus (CeM; Yasui et al., 1985). Chiba et al. campal efferent projections to the cerebral cortex of the
(2001) reported projections to the BLA and accessory rhesus monkey: projections from CA1, prosubiculum,
basal nuclei, ventromedial hypothalamus, dorsolateral and subiculum to the temporal lobe. J Comp Neurol 393:
92–114.
periaqueductal gray, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis,
Broca P (1878). Anatomie compareedes circonvolutions
medial and lateral parabrachial nuclei, and locus coeru-
cerebrales: le grande lobe limbique et la scissure limbique
leus. It may be that this projection system operates in tan- dans la serie des mammifères. Rev d’Anthropol 1:
dem temporally to coordinate somatomotor and 385–498.
autonomic responses. Carmichael ST, Price JL (1995a). Sensory and premotor con-
nections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of
macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 363: 642–664.
SIX LIMBIC SUBSYSTEMS ARE LIKELY Carmichael ST, Price JL (1995b). Limbic connections of the
RATHER THAN ONLY THREE orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys.
J Comp Neurol 363: 515–641.
As these studies began with the cingulate cortex, we have Chiba T, Kayahara T, Nakano K (2001). Efferent projections
to revisit this region to determine limbic system organi- of infralimbic and prelimbic areas of the medial prefrontal
zation. This region is comprised of ACC, vaMCC, cortex in the Japanese monkey, Macaca fuscata. Brain Res
daMCC, pMCC, dPCC/RSC, and vPCC/RSC. Thus it 888: 83–101.
is likely that there are at least six limbic systems. As Cole MW, Schneider W (2007). The cognitive control net-
ACC has two components, there could be further subsys- work: integrated cortical regions with dissociable func-
tems. These subsystems will eventually be culled from tions. Neuroimage 37: 343–360.
Craig AD (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of
the three limbic systems designated in this chapter.
the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci
3: 655–666.
REFERENCES Demeter S, Rosene DL, Van Hoesen GW (1985).
Aggleton JP (1986). Adescription of the amygdalo- Interhemispheric pathways of the hippocampal formation,
hippocampal interconnections in the macaque monkey. presubiculum, and entorhinal and posterior parahippocam-
Exp Brain Res 64: 515–526. pal cortices in the rhesus monkey: the structure and organi-
Aggleton JP (2012). Multiple anatomical systems embedded zation of the hippocampal commissures. J Comp Neurol
within the primate medial temporal lobe: implications forhip- 233: 30–47.
pocampal function. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36: 1579–1596. Hayden BY, Nair AC, McCoy AN et al. (2008). Posterior cin-
Aggleton JP, Brown MW (2005). Contrasting hippocampal gulate cortex mediates outcome-contingent allocation of
and perirhinal cortex function using immediate early gene behavior. Neuron 60: 19–25.
imaging. Q J Exp Psychol B 58: 218–233. Hof PR, Mufson EJ, Morrison JH (1995). Human orbitofrontal
Amaral DG, Price JL (1984). Amygdalo-cortical projections in cortex: Cytoarchitecture and quantitative immunohisto-
the monkey (Macaca fascicularis). J Comp Neurol 230: chemical parcellation. J Comp Neurol 359: 48–68.
465–496. Howard MW, Fotedar MS, Datey AV et al. (2005). The tem-
Amunts K, Kedo O, Kindler M et al. (2005). Cytoarchitectonic poral context model in spatial navigation and relational
mapping of the human amygdala, hippocampal region and learning: toward a common explanation of medial temporal
entorhinal cortex: intersubject variability and probability lobe function across domains. Psychol Rev 112: 75–116.
maps. Anat Embryol 210: 343–352. (Berl). Insausti R, Munoz M (2001). Cortical projections of the non-
Bach DR, Guitart-Masip M, Packard PA et al. (2014). Human entorhinal hippocampal formation in the cynomolgus mon-
hippocampus arbitrates approach-avoidance conflict. Curr key (Macaca fascicularis). Eur J Neurosci 14: 435–451.
Biol 24: 541–547. Ito R, Lee ACH (2016). The role of the hippocampus in approach-
Bar M, Aminoff E (2003). Cortical analysis of visual context. avoidance conflict decision-making: evidence from rodent
Neuron 38: 347–358. and human studies. Behav Brain Res 313: 345–357.
Belin P, Zatorre RJ, Lafaille P et al. (2000). Voice-selective Kobayashi Y, Amaral DG (2003). Macaque monkey retro-
areas in human auditory cortex. Nature 403: 310–312. splenial cortex: II. Cortical afferents. J Comp Neurol
Bernard JF, Besson JM (1990). The spino(trigemino)ponto- 466: 48–79.
amygdaloid pathway: electrophysiological evidence for an K€ohler S, Danckert S, Gati JS et al. (2005). Novelty responses
involvement in pain processes. J Neurophysiol 63: 473–490. to relational and non-relational information in the hippo-
Bernard JF, Huang GF, Besson JM (1994). The parabrachial campus and the parahippocampal region: a comparison
area: electrophysiological evidence for an involvement based on event-related fMRI. Hippocampus 15: 763–774.
in visceral nociceptive processes. J Neurophysiol 71: Lamm C, Singer T (2010). The role of anterior insular cortex in
1646–1660. social emotions. Brain Struct Funct 214: 579–591.
Birn RM (2007). The behavioral significance of spontaneous Lia X, Sarkara SN, Purdy DE et al. (2013). Anteroposterior
fluctuations in brain activity. Neuron 56: 8–9. perfusion heterogeneity in human hippocampus measured
CINGULATE CORTEX IN THE THREE LIMBIC SUBSYSTEMS 51
by arterial spin labeling MRI. NMR Biomed 26: 613–621. Ross RS, Sherrill KR, Stern CE (2011). The hippocampus is
https://doi.org/10.1002/nbm.2898. functionally connected to the striatum and orbitofrontal
Libby LA, Ekstrom AD, Ragland JD et al. (2012). Differential cortex during context dependent decision making. Brain
connectivity of perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices Res 1423: 53–66.
within human hippocampal subregions revealed by high- Saunders RC, Rosene DL (1988). A comparison of the effer-
resolution functional imaging. J Neurosci 32 (19): ents of the amygdala and the hippocampal formation in
6550–6560. the rhesus monkey: I. Convergence in the entorhinal, pro-
MacLean PD (1990). The triune brain in evolution: role in rhinal, and perirhinal cortices. J Comp Neurol 271:
paleocerebral functions, Plenum Press, New York. 153–184.
Mesulam M-M, Muffson EJ (1982). Insula of the old world Saunders RC, Rosene DL, Van Hoesen GW (1988).
monkey. I. Architectonics in the insulo-orbitotemporal com- Comparison of the efferents of the amygdala and the
ponent of the paralimbic brain. J Comp Neurol 2l2: 1–22. hippocampal formation in the rhesus monkey: II.
Mufson EJ, Mesulam M-M (1982). Insula of the old world Reciprocal and non-reciprocal connections. J Comp
monkey. 11: afferent cortical input and comments on the Neurol 271: 185–207.
claustrum. J Comp Neurol 2l2: 23–37. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF et al. (2007). Dissociable
Nieuwenhuys R (2012). The insular cortex: a review. In: intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and
MA Hofman, D Falk (Eds.), Progress in brain research. executive control. J Neurosci 27: 2349–2356.
vol. 195. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp. 123–163. https://doi. Stalnaker TA, Franz TM, Singh T et al. (2007). Basolateral
org/10.1016/B978-0-444-53860-4.00007-6. amygdala lesions abolish orbitofrontal-dependent reversal
O’Neil EB, Newsome RN, Li IHN et al. (2015). Examining the impairments. Neuron 54: 51–58.
role of the human hippocampus in approach-avoidance Taylor KS, Seminowicz DA, Davis KD (2009). Two systems
decision making using a novel conflict paradigm and of resting state connectivity between the insula and cingu-
multivariate functional magnetic resonance imaging. late cortex. Hum Brain Mapp 30: 2731–2745.
J Neurosci 35: 15039–15049. https://doi.org/10.1523/ Tulving E (1984). Precis of elements of episodic memory.
JNEUROSCI.1915-15.2015. Behav Brain Sci 7: 223–268.
Pandya DN, Van Hosen GW, Mesulam M-M (1981). Efferent Turner BH, Mishkin M, Knapp M (1980). Organization of
connections of the cingulate gyrus in the rhesus monkey. the amygdalopetal projections from modality-specific
Exper Brain Res 42: 319–330. cortical association areas in the monkey. J Comp Neurol
Papez JW (1937). Aproposed mechanism of emotion. Arch 191: 515–543.
Neurol Psychiatry 38: 725–743. Vogt BA, Pandya DN (1987). Cingulate cortex of the rhesus
Pessoa L, Hof PR (2015). From Paul Broca’s great limbic lobe monkey: II. Cortical afferents. J Comp Neurol 262(2):
to the limbic system. J Comp Neurol 523: 2495–2500. 271–289. PubMed PMID: 3624555.
Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF et al. (2002). Functional neuro- Vogt BA, Berger GR, Derbyshire SW (2003). Structural and
anatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation functional dichotomy of human midcingulate cortex.
studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage 16: 331–348. Eur J Neurosci 18 (11): 3134–3144. PubMed PMID:
Phelps EA, LeDoux JE (2005). Contributions of the amygdala 14656310; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2548277.
to emotion processing: from animal models to human Vogt BA, Vogt L, Laureys S (2006). Cytology and functionally
behavior. Neuron 48: 175–187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. correlated circuits of human posterior cingulate areas.
neuron.2005.09.025. Neuroimage 29 (2): 452–466. PubMed PMID:
Poppenk J, Moscovitch M (2011). A hippocampal marker of 16140550; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2649771.
recollection memory ability among healthy young adults: Witter MP, Amaral DG (1991). Entorhinal cortex of the mon-
contributions of posterior and anterior segments. Neuron key: V. Projections to the dentate gyrus, hippocampus, and
72: 931–937. subicular complex. J Comp Neurol 307: 437–459.
Qin S, Duan X, Supekar K et al. (2016). Large-scale intrinsic Yasui Y, Itoh K, Takada M et al. (1985). Direct cortical pro-
functional network organization along the long axis of the jections to the parabrachial nucleus in the cat. J Comp
human medial temporal lobe. Brain Struct Funct 221: Neurol 234: 77–86.
3237–3258. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-015-1098-4. Yukie M (2000). Connections between the medial temporal
Raichle ME, FrRiez JA, Videen TO et al. (1994). Practice- cortex and the CA1 subfield of the hippocampal formation
related changes in human brain functional anatomy during in the Japanese monkey (Macaca fuscata). J Comp Neurol
nonmotor learning. Cereb Cortex 4: 8–26. 423: 282–298.
Rolls ET (2014). Emotion and decision-making explained:
Precis, Synopsis of book published by Oxford University
Press, pp. 1–9. ISBN 978-0-19-965989-0. Cortex. FURTHER READING
Rolls ET (2015). Limbic systems for emotion and for memory,
but no single limbic system. Cortex 62: 119 e157. Petrovich GD, Canteras NS, Swanson LW (2001).
Ross RS, Brown TI, Stern CE (2009). The retrieval of learned Combinatorial amygdalar inputs to hippocampal domains
sequences engages the hippocampus: evidence from fMRI. and hypothalamic behavior systems. Brain Res Rev 38:
Hippocampus 19: 790–799. 247–289.

You might also like