Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Emotion Review http://emr.sagepub.

com/

Emotion Development in Infancy through the Lens of Culture


Amy G. Halberstadt and Fantasy T. Lozada
Emotion Review 2011 3: 158
DOI: 10.1177/1754073910387946

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://emr.sagepub.com/content/3/2/158

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

International Society for Research on Emotion

Additional services and information for Emotion Review can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://emr.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://emr.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Citations: http://emr.sagepub.com/content/3/2/158.refs.html

>> Version of Record - Apr 6, 2011

What is This?

Downloaded from emr.sagepub.com by INARLTF Oundation on April 6, 2012


Emotion Review
Vol. 3, No. 2 (April 2011) 158–168
© The Author(s) 2011
ISSN 1754-0739
Emotion Development in Infancy through DOI: 10.1177/1754073910387946
er.sagepub.com

the Lens of Culture

Amy G. Halberstadt
Fantasy T. Lozada
Department of Psychology, North Carolina State University, USA

Abstract
The goal of this review is to consider how culture impacts the socialization of emotion development in infancy, and infants’ and
young children’s subsequent outcomes. First, we argue that parents’ socialization decisions are embedded within cultural
structures, beliefs, and practices. Second, we identify five broad cultural frames (collectivism/individualism; power distance; chil-
dren’s place in family and culture; ways children learn; and value of emotional experience and expression) that help to organize
current and future research. For each frame, we discuss the impact on parents’ socialization practices and infants’ subsequent
outcomes relating to emotion-related experience, expression, and understanding. We also generate testable hypotheses to further
our understanding of the relationships between the five frames and emotion development in infancy.

Keywords
culture, emotion, infancy, parents, socialization

Socialization processes are central to infant emotional Barry, 2009; Mills-Koonce et al., 2007; Propper & Moore,
development. Despite the many ways our lives have changed 2006). Thus, how caregivers communicate with and comfort
over the millennia, parents remain as first sources of influence infants is likely to direct infants’ attention toward an interpreta-
during infancy and toddlerhood. Parents are highly effective in tion of various features of their social and emotional worlds,
scaffolding emotional behaviors of infants and young children and their own and others’ placement in those social and
(e.g., Campos, Thein, & Owen, 2003; Fernald, 1993), and emotional worlds.
emotion-related socialization is possible even in the first days We contend that socialization processes occur within the
of life, given newborns’ vocal and facial recognition abilities context of cultural systems that alter the very nature of infants’
(Mastropieri & Turkewitz, 1999) and infants’ early differential and young children’s emotional experience, expression, and
recognition and responsivity to caregivers’ facial expressions understanding. Culture is known to be important to individuals’
soon thereafter (e.g., Barrera & Maurer, 1981; Montague & overall psychological structure (Hofstede, 1980, 2001; Triandis,
Walker-Andrews, 2002; Muir, Humphrey, & Humphrey, 1994; 1995), the development of self (Markus & Kitayama, 1994),
Nielsen, 2006; Weir, Toland, King, & Martin, 2005). Further, individuals’ emotional lives (Markus & Kitayama, 1991), and
as infants’ cognitive, motoric, and physiological systems are the structure and character of parent–child relationships
developing and becoming coordinated (Lewis, Hitchcock, & (Bronfenbrenner, 1977; Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998;
Sullivan, 2004), parents’ role as external regulators of emotion Keller, Borke, Yovsi, Lohaus, & Jensen, 2005; Super &
shifts to that of cocreating regulatory systems (Calkins, 1994; Harkness, 1986, 1997).
Campos, Frankel, & Camras, 2004; Crockenberg & Leerkes, The role of culture in children’s emotional socialization has
2004; Kopp, 1982). These systems are influenced by infants’ been specifically described in four models (Cole & Tan, 2007;
genotypes modulated by parents’ skills, including maternal Dunsmore & Halberstadt, 1997; Eisenberg, Cumberland, &
sensitivity and supportive attachment (Kochanska, Philibert, & Spinrad, 1998; Garcia Coll et al., 1996), with culture providing

Authors’ note: We thank the FABB lab, especially Claire Blevins and Sable Watson, for their assistance in article retrieval, organization, and analysis. We thank Ashley Craig, Julie
Dunsmore, Michael Lewis, and Rebecca Stelter for their comments on this article.
Corresponding author: Amy G. Halberstadt, North Carolina State University, Department of Psychology, Box 7650, Raleigh, NC 27695-7650, USA. Email: amy_halberstadt@ncsu.edu

Downloaded from emr.sagepub.com by INARLTF Oundation on April 6, 2012


Halberstadt & Lozada  Emotion Development in Infancy   159

both direct and indirect influences on parents’ emotion sociali- dimension (Hofstede, 1980; Markus & Kitayama, 1991;
zation behaviors (Dunsmore & Halberstadt, 2009). Culture Mesquita, 2001), but recognition of within-culture variability
determines the type of environment in which infants develop (Triandis, 1995), connections to context (Kağitçibaşi, 1996),
(Super & Harkness, 1986), and has indirect influence via par- clusters of values within dimensions (Schwartz, 1994), and
ents’ own individual beliefs and socialization goals. Keller simultaneous endorsement of both values (Tamis-LeMonda
(2007) suggests that culture is infused in parent–child relations et al., 2008) have expanded the frame considerably.
through parents’ and infants’ cocreation of shared activity (e.g., More nuanced descriptions of this frame are being devel-
cultural practices) and shared meaning (e.g., cultural beliefs oped, and these may be promising for future research with
and interpretations). Further, infants absorb information pro- infants. For example, two forms of individualism have been
vided by culture through experience-expectant (species typical identified (Kusserow, 2004). “Soft” individualism, which is
neural preparedness to integrate information from the environ- more characteristic of upper class American life, emphasizes
ment) and experience-dependent (individual neural changes personal identity and the full exploration and development of
due to learning and experience) processes (Greenough, Black, one’s unique abilities. “Hard” individualism, which is more
& Wallace, 1987; Keller, 2007). With age, infants are also characteristic of less financially secure American life, empha-
increasingly able to integrate information across cognitive, sizes independence, assertion, and the ability to manage on
physiological, and motoric domains in the service of emotion one’s own. Although both fit under the rubric of individualism,
(e.g., Saarni, Campos, Camras, & Witherington, 2006). Although one can imagine how parents with these different beliefs might
we focus primarily on infancy, culture likely impacts the devel- engage in very different practices with their infants. Soft
oping individual differently throughout the lifespan, as differ- individualists, for example, might tenderly present toys and a
ent beliefs and practices are invoked for individuals and their variety of wonders to their infants, thus promoting their
families during different developmental stages, and indeed, children’s sense of the world as a place that is emotionally
because culture itself is dynamic and changing (Bronfenbrenner warm, secure, and theirs to explore. Hard protective individu-
& Morris, 1998). alists, however, might engage in more active, rougher kinds of
In this review we identify and describe five “cultural play and teasing activities, thus promoting children’s emo-
frames” (Markus & Kitayama, 1994) that we think are the most tional toughness and a sense of the world as demanding and not
important to consider with regard to emotion in infancy. Two of altogether benign. Collectivism may also have variants that
these frames are well known; three are introduced for the first create other unique clusters, and identifying them may be a
time in this article. Within each frame we discuss variation useful endeavor.
within cultures; how parent socialization practices may vary, Parents who are influenced by a culture that values interde-
both generally and within the context of emotion; and how pendence and shared family goals are likely to socialize their
infant outcomes vary in terms of the three domains of affec- children differently than parents who are influenced by a cul-
tive social competence: emotion experience, expression, and ture largely driven by values of independence and individuali-
understanding (Halberstadt, Denham, & Dunsmore, 2001). zation. For example, the encouragement an infant receives for
Throughout, we attempt to extend the boundaries of what we throwing a toy, reaching for a toy, sharing a toy, giving a toy to
know by generating specific ideas for future research. another, and, in fact, the very presence of a toy, may relate to
societal collectivism versus individualism. The first two acts of
self-assertion may be well supported in an individualistic cul-
Collectivism/Individualism ture; the second two acts of sharing and social harmony may be
better supported in a collectivist culture. The presence of toys
Perhaps the most well-known frame that varies across cultures may also support self-exploration and individuation; after all,
is that of collectivism and individualism. Collectivist cultures infants in collectivist cultures may not have much need for toys
emphasize interdependence, group identity, interpersonal har- when they have family members who may be of much greater
mony, and achievement of shared goals; individualist cultures importance. Finally, in individualistic cultures, toys might also
emphasize independence, individual identity, personal asser- be tools for distracting infants in emotionally arousing situa-
tion, and achievement of personal goals (Hofstede, 1980, 2001; tions; in collectivistic cultures, family members may be the
Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Triandis, Bontempo, Villareal, main source of emotion regulation. Work on translating cultural
Asai, & Lucca, 1988). In collectivist cultures, emotions reflect beliefs to parenting behaviors has just begun; for example,
who a person is in relation to other group members or the world compared to European American mothers, Chinese mothers of
at large; emotions are often characterized by self-control, so as 3-month-olds reported more allocentric values, and more
to promote interpersonal harmony and group goals; and emotions relational-related and less autonomy-related themes (Keller
may be identified on the basis of what other group members are et al., 2007). Additionally, the frequency of contingent respond-
feeling (Mesquita & Frijda, 1992). In individualistic cultures, ing by German mothers compared to Nso Cameroonian mothers
emotions are perceived to be unique to the individual, expres- with their 3-month-olds was significantly different (Keller,
siveness may be encouraged, and emotions are identified on the Kärtner, Borke, Yovsi, & Kleis, 2005), supporting one way in
basis of intrapersonal, subjective experience. Collectivism and which parental values for independence versus interdependence
individualism were initially posed as bipolar ends of a single might be inculcated in children.

Downloaded from emr.sagepub.com by INARLTF Oundation on April 6, 2012


160   Emotion Review Vol. 3 No. 2

Although collectivism and individualism are not yet known children, and possibly older infants, in vertical compared to
to impact emotional experience, expression, and interpretations horizontal societies, particularly when power distance is not
of emotion in infancy, several studies have explored these moderated by other frames that include indulgent beliefs about
emotion-related domains with preschoolers. Using multiple children and their role in the family, as will be described below.
methodologies, consistent differences in these three domains Power-distant parents might also utilize more top-down
emerged between preschoolers in traditionally collectivist conversational strategies to direct infant or toddler actions,
versus traditionally individualistic cultures (Lewis, Takai- compared to egalitarian parents who might engage in more
Kawakami, Kawakami, & Sullivan, 2010; Masataka, 2002; dialogic, turn-taking strategies and invitations to infants to take
Zahn-Waxler, Friedman, Cole, Mizuta, & Hiruma, 1996). Thus, the conversational lead (Richman, Miller, & LeVine, 1992;
in concert with the parenting differences noted just above, these Super & Harkness, 1997). Although we know of no fine-
results invite further exploration of emerging differences of grained linguistic analyses comparing vertical societies versus
emotion-related behaviors in infancy as a consequence of cross- horizontal societies, hints of linguistic patterns differing by
and within-cultural variability in collectivism and individualism. culture are evident by parents and infants as early as the first
Further, because contingent responsiveness is also associated three months of life (Keller, Otto, Lamm, Yovsi, & Kärtner,
with early mirror self-recognition, one of the hallmarks of 2008; Richman et al., 1992) and continue to change during
understanding of self as an individual (Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, infants’ first year of life (Richman et al., 1992). In Germany, for
1979), toddlers’ sense of self may mediate relationships between example, parents encourage a great deal of face-to-face interac-
parent practices of contingent responding and their emotion- tion, with turn-taking pauses so that infants may respond and
related experiences (e.g., the early advent of independent iden- are strongly reinforced for doing so; this style of interaction is
tity may allow young children to secure their own emotional very limited among the more power-distant Gusii of southwest
experiences rather than the experiences of those around them). Kenya and the Nso of Cameroon (Dixon, Tronick, Keefer, &
Brazelton, 1981; Keller, 2003; Keller, Borke, et al., 2005;
Power Distance Richman et al., 1992).
Power distance may overlap somewhat with the dimension
Another classic dimension concerns how power is structured of collectivism/individualism, but it is also a distinctly differ-
within cultures or families. Hofstede (1980, 2001) describes this ent dimension. For example, many of the collectivist societies
dimension as power distance; other researchers refer to interper- that are power distant tend to discourage anger; however, other
sonal distance or verticality (Kağitçibaşi, 1996; Matsumoto, 1996; collectivist societies that are more egalitarian, including the
Singelis, Triandis, Bhawuk, & Gelfand, 1995; Triandis & Gelfand, Ifaluk and Kaluli, do value anger (Lutz, 1983; Schieffelin,
1998). Power distant/vertical societies accept and expect inequal- 1983, 1985). Furthermore, power distance becomes differently
ity in status relationships, and emphasize the values of obedience, integrated with other frames noted below, and may have its
respect for authority, and rule-following (Hofstede, 2001; own dynamic trajectories over historical time (Hofstede, 1980,
Matsumoto, 1996; Triandis & Gelfand, 1998). Less power distant/ 2001; Kağitçibaşi, 1996; Kağitçibaşi & Ataca, 2005; Lamm,
more horizontal societies affirm the idea of equality across status Keller, Yovsi, & Chaudhary, 2008; Triandis & Gelfand, 1998).
relationships, promote more democratic values, and allow more
discussion and greater permeability across hierarchical boundaries
(Baldwin, 1948; Hofstede, 2001; Matsumoto, 1996). Power dis- Children’s Place in Family and Culture
tance initially looks simpler in horizontal societies because of the
proclaimed value for sameness in power. In actuality, it is likely to The first two cultural frames (collectivism and power distance)
be more complex because of the narrowly circumscribed levels of were first considered within organizations and nations
status that exist within different domains but within the same (Hofstede, 1980). This third frame addresses family structures,
relationship, and complicit “blindness” to the evidence of vertical- and specifically the role of children within families. Whereas
ity, thereby retaining the principle of equality across status, even children seem to be valued in almost all cultures, they are val-
when it does not really exist (Hofstede, 2001; Much, 1997; ued for different reasons. The relationships that parents have
Triandis & Gelfand, 1998). with children also differ substantially by culture. These may
Power distance may create emotional as well as social impact how emotions are organized within families and may
structures that are likely to be reflected in parents’ socialization lead to very different goals and strategies regarding socialization
goals, for example, in valuing obedience and attending to of emotion, and thus different emotion-related outcomes for
parents’ rather than infants’ goals. In Latin American families, children.
the value of respeto encourages children to defer to, obey, and
honor family members (Cervantes, 2002; Delgado-Gaitan, The Value of Children
1994; Harwood, Schölmerich, Schulze, & González, 1999;
Zayas & Solari, 1994). Respeto requires that emotions such as Historically, children provided an important economic contri-
anger should not be expressed against adult family members, bution to their families, and, today in many present-day cul-
particularly male figures (Gonzalez-Ramos, Zayas, & Cohen, tures, particularly those which are rural and agriculturally
1998). Thus, emotion regulation may be greater for very young based; one purpose of having children is to widen the economic

Downloaded from emr.sagepub.com by INARLTF Oundation on April 6, 2012


Halberstadt & Lozada  Emotion Development in Infancy   161

base of the family (Kağitçibaşi & Ataca, 2005). However, other Ways Children Learn
reasons include the inherent value of children, in terms of the
psychological benefit derived by parents (e.g., the joy of chil- Our fourth cultural frame concerns myriad beliefs about learning
dren; Keller, 2003), or as gifts from, or, indeed, the very pres- as they pertain to children. We include four kinds of beliefs that
ence of the gods (Diener, 2000; Kağitçibaşi & Ataca, 2005). may be most related to emotion socialization and infants’ out-
Parents who think of children as continuing and enhancing the comes: “when,” “whether,” “who,” and “how”. We believe this
productivity of the family enterprise may socialize their chil- frame is distinctly different from the frames above; yet because
dren to persevere in the face of hard work and, thus, may mini- there is so little empirical work, future research will have to test
mize emotions that may distract a child from their work. During the veracity of our claim for its independence.
infancy, these parents may not engage in socialization practices Regarding the “when” of learning, the age at which infants
that encourage high emotional arousal. Alternatively, they may and young children are perceived as capable of learning various
ignore emotions as irrelevant events. However, if parents tasks varies widely across cultures. Regarding language learn-
believe that children are gifts from the gods, they might engage ing, for example, American parents scaffold words for babies as
in socialization behaviors that encourage positive affect as a early as 8 months of age, whereas Kipsigis mothers report that
means of giving thanks for the very existence of that child. infants don’t really learn language until they are about two years
old (Super & Harkness, 1986); British, Indian, and Japanese
Relations with Children mothers also report different ages at which infants become com-
petent (Joshi & MacLean, 1997). Kurdish and Yemenite Jewish
Some ways in which children are regarded fit well within the mothers living in Israel also had quite different timetables for
above frames, for example, the concept of children as obedient, the onset of social smiling and the acquisition of various emo-
as noted above under the frame of power distance. However, tions (Frankel, Roer-Bornstein, & LeVine, 1982).
other ways in which children are regarded do not fit either frame With regard to the timelines for emotional control, many
above. One component of Latino culture, familismo, encourages American parents believe that very young children are quite
strong familial bonds and concern for family members’ well- capable of learning about emotions, yet this viewpoint is also
being (Cervantes, 2002; Cristofaro & Tamis-LeMonda, 2008). held in conjunction with the belief that young children initially
At first glance, familismo may appear to be a subdimension of have little control over their emotions, and, thus, one of the jobs
collectivism, as it includes the importance of family interde- of parents is to scaffold the most supportive environment pos-
pendence. However, familismo is characterized by family affec- sible for their children’s well-being (Goodnow, Cashmore,
tion, and a great deal of care and indulgence for young children, Cotton, & Knight, 1984; Parker et al., in press). This is an inter-
values that can also be found in more individualistic societies. esting dialectic! The latter belief, that infants and young
In fact, American and Greek mothers of 3-month-old infants children have little control over their emotions, is shared with
report equivalent levels of familismo as Costa Rican and cultures which are considerably different from American culture
Mexican mothers (Keller et al., 2006). Further, Latin American in terms of the other cultural frames described above. The Utku,
culture encourages the concept of simpatía, which is a rela- Javanese, and Ifaluk, for example, do not spend much time try-
tional style among individuals that emphasizes the goal of social ing to teach infants or toddlers about emotion, because they
harmony (Triandis, Marín, Lisansky, & Betancourt, 1984). perceive them as not yet socializable (Briggs, 1970; Geertz,
Cultures that practice simpatía prioritize creating personable 1959; Lutz, 1983). Comparisons between American and Italian
environments for others. Thus, each of the three frames— mothers, Japanese and American mothers, and Lebanese and
collectivism (family interdependence), verticality/power distance Australian mothers indicate different beliefs regarding when
(children obeying parents), and child/familial devotion (family children are capable of emotion-regulation related skills (e.g.,
affection and indulgence of children) are necessary to create the controlling anger or expression of disappointment, resolving
unique cultural mix that is distinctive in Latin America. disagreements), with American, Japanese, and Lebanese mothers
Other non-Spanish-speaking cultures, for example, Italian, expecting earlier competency than their comparison counterparts
Chinese, and Japanese, also emphasize a very special place of (Edwards, Gandini, & Giovaninni, 1996; Goodnow et al., 1984;
children, characterized by love, devotion, and indulgence of Hess, 1980). This provides evidence that beliefs about learning
children, while also varying in the degree of their collectivist do not match onto the other frames above, in that American and
and power-distance inclinations (Bornstein et al., 2008; Camras Italian cultures have relatively similar beliefs regarding
et al., 1998). Implications for infants in these cultures are individualism and power distance (Hofstede, 2001).
numerous. In keeping with their tradition of devotion and indul- Cultural beliefs regarding timetables for developing compe-
gence for infants and young children, coupled with their rela- tency may impact parental socialization efforts, including struc-
tively low emphasis on power distance, Italian mothers seem turing ecological niches that foster the development of those
more emotionally sensitive than American or Argentinean competencies and expending effort with infants to scaffold
mothers in interactions with their infants. Further, Italian infants various skills. In the case of emotional control, when parents
responded in kind in those interactions, with greater responsive- believe infants are emotionally capable they may provide the
ness and interactivity toward their mothers, compared to the settings and scaffolding that allows infants to meet parental
American and Argentinean infants (Bornstein et al., 2008). goals, compared to when parents don’t believe infants are

Downloaded from emr.sagepub.com by INARLTF Oundation on April 6, 2012


162   Emotion Review Vol. 3 No. 2

emotionally capable. Thus, infants and young children might respective beliefs about maturation and learning may be reflected
initially achieve emotional control earlier in the United States in their children’s choices regarding emotional expression versus
than in other countries. It might also be that the task is very time masking in social situations (Cole & Tamang, 1998). Parental
consuming and/or becomes frustrating for American parents beliefs that children can learn about emotions on their own
when high expectations are not met (e.g., Donovan, Leavitt, & (rather than needing parental guidance) predicts relatively less
Taylor, 2005). If so, the lack of success early on might lead to skill in their children’s emotion understanding (Perez-Rivera &
lowered expectations in the United States at precisely the ages Dunsmore, in press). On the other hand, when parents believe
at which emotional socialization is activated in other countries. that they are in control and think that they are highly capable of
These conjectures have yet to be tested, and cross-cultural effecting change with their infants, they may actually have less
research including parents’ specific beliefs about control, their sensitivity to infants’ cries when confronted with frequent or
socialization behaviors, and their success as demonstrated in apparently inconsolable crying (Donovan et al., 2005; Donovan,
children’s behavior may be a fruitful avenue of future research. Leavitt, & Walsh, 1990). As Donovan et al. suggest, parents
A second set of beliefs relates to the underlying structure by with illusions of control may give up more easily when faced
which competencies are developed—whether children acquire with failure than parents who have more modest beliefs about
competencies via maturation or teaching. Again, cultures vary their own efficacy. Thus, curvilinear relations may emerge,
widely with regard to this issue, and across many different types such that moderate levels of the belief that parents are important
of competencies. Chinese mothers in Taiwan, Tamang mothers guides for children’s emotional development and that children
in Nepal, and Mayan mothers in Yucatan believe that develop- are somewhat in control of their own emotional destinies may
ment is largely maturational and that children grow up “natu- be ideal. Finally, as alluded to above, “who is in charge” is not
rally” (Chen & Luster, 2002; Cole & Tamang, 1998; Gaskins, restricted to parents; siblings, grandparents, peers, teachers, and
1996; Miller, Wang, Sandel, & Cho, 2002). In contrast, the other community members are integral components of infants’
Chhetri and Brahmin in Nepal, the Tallensi in Ghana and the social networks across the world (e.g., Gaskins, 1996; Lamb,
Nso of Cameroon believe learning is a process that requires 2005; Lewis, 2005; Lewis, Feiring, & Kotsonis, 1984; Super &
teaching (Cole & Tamang, 1998; Fortes, 1938; Keller, 2003). Harkness, 1982, 1986).
Historically, parents in the United States have shifted from a A fourth kind of belief, for cultures that do assume learning,
rather maturationist perspective, which suggests that children’s concerns how one learns. This topic spans a wide array of learn-
emotional lives develop over time and without need of scaffold- ing theories, including using discipline to instruct, telling
ing (Gesell & Ilg, 1943; Hall, 1907), to a greater focus on what children what to do and expecting that they will then do it,
can be done to further the emotional well-being of their infants providing children with opportunities to be active participants
and children (Brazelton & Sparrow, 2006). in their learning, scaffolding children’s next steps, and cocreat-
A third kind of belief, for cultures that do assume learning, ing knowledge through discussion. More than a superficial
is “who is in charge?” For the Tallensi people in Africa, parents naming of these beliefs is beyond the scope of this paper; nev-
are always on duty as teachers and guides (Fortes, 1938). ertheless it is important to note that cultures vary widely in their
Although the Javanese and the Ifaluk differ from the Tallensi beliefs about how individuals learn (see Keller, 2003).
regarding when infants and children are ready to learn, they do With regard to emotion-related learning, we note briefly a
perceive that it is the parents’ role to teach their children once few differences previously identified. For example, Chinese
children are mature enough for that process to commence parents in Taiwan emphasize that “training” is important for
(Geertz, 1959; Lutz, 1983). Yet other cultures, such as the learning and development (Chao, 1994; Chen & Luster, 2002).
Kipsigis people in Kenya, assume that when children are ready As noted above, discipline for these parents is an important
to learn, they will look to other children as guides to appropriate technique for guiding children (Chen & Luster, 2002; Miller
emotion-related behaviors (Super & Harkness, 1982, 1986). et al., 2002). Discussions about emotion, however, are not
Kipsigis practices include a lot of energy toward teaching chil- perceived to be a useful strategy; in one sample, 40% of
dren to sit and to walk, but neither emotions nor language are European American mothers reported processing emotions with
things to be socialized by parents, thus talk with infants or their preschoolers, whereas no Taiwanese Chinese mothers
children about emotions seldom occurs. There is also variabil- mentioned emotion-related conversations at all (Chao, 1995).
ity across class and ethnicity in the degree to which American We note that Hofstede (2001) suggests that collectivists
parents believe that children can be in charge of their own emo- assume that little needs to be said because information is read-
tional lives and also that parents need to guide and teach their ily available in the context, and individualists assume that noth-
children about emotions (Craig, Stelter, & Halberstadt, 2010; ing is clear because we are separate and distinct entities.
Parker et al., in press). With regard to infancy, Québécois and However, we consider this kind of belief to be conceptually and
Haitian immigrant mothers believe they play a significant role pragmatically distinct from the collectivist/individualist cultural
in the affective development of their young infants in contrast frame. Although Chinese parents in Taipei (thought to be
to Vietnamese immigrant mothers (Pomerleau, Malcuit, & collectivist) do not tend to discuss emotion-related topics with
Sabatier, 1991). their children, and American parents (thought to be individual-
These beliefs have obvious implications. For example, the istic) do, when it comes to moral transgressions Chinese parents
differences between Tamang and Chhetri and Brahmin mothers’ have a lot to say, building on events as they happen (Fung,

Downloaded from emr.sagepub.com by INARLTF Oundation on April 6, 2012


Halberstadt & Lozada  Emotion Development in Infancy   163

1999; Miller, Wang, Sandel, & Cho, 2002). Thus the domain to frequency of their vocalizations. With the onset of social smiling,
be considered (e.g., emotion, morality) is more predictive of the parents’ frequency and speed of contingent responsiveness to
strategies to be used in inculcating cultural norms than the infants’ positive expressions may be further magnified by
overarching framework of collectivism/individualism. culture. For cultures that particularly value both positive affect
and parental sensitivity expressed visually (rather than by
physical contact), infants’ developmental shift to social smiling
Value of Emotional Experience and Expression likely functions as a reward for parents that further facilitates
their goals, whereas for cultures that de-emphasize the value of
Many differences between collectivist and individualistic cul- positive affect, the onset of social smiling, while extremely
tures have been identified regarding emotional experience and pleasant, is not perceived as a critical milestone, and an
expression (e.g., Eid & Diener, 2001; Kitayama, Markus, & opportunity to achieve more relational, emotional-bonding
Kurokawa, 2000; Li, Wang, & Fischer, 2004; Markus & goals (see Yovsi, Kärtner, Keller, & Lohaus, 2009, for an exam-
Kitayama, 1991; Mesquita & Karasawa, 2004; Schimmack, ple of how German and Nso mothers develop different trajecto-
Oishi, & Diener, 2002; Scollon, Diener, Oishi, & Biswas- ries of sensitivity during early infant–parent interactions).
Diener, 2004; Stipek, 1998; van Hemert, Poortinga, & van der Second, differences in infant–parent play across cultures
Vijver, 2007). As noted above, the frequency and organizational may have implications for acculturation of positive affect. In
structure of emotions may vary across the collectivist/individu- American society, positive expression with associated arousal is
alist and the power-distance frames. And beliefs about children highly valued, as evidenced by “peek-a-boo,” a nationally played
(e.g., infants as gifts from the gods, or as part of the family that infant–parent game. Additionally, most adults in the United
requires indulgence and “spoiling”) and learning (by discipline, States exaggerate positive affect and intensify the contours of
scaffolding, self-exploration, etc.) have implications for their vocalizations when with young infants. In contrast, numer-
emotion-related socialization with infants and children. In these ous other cultures devalue the high positive arousal associated
cases, emotional experience and expression vary as a conse- with such games and parent–infant activity. For example, the
quence of achieving other goals (e.g., successful individuation, Gusii in southwest Kenya emphasize tremendous emotional
deference to authority, harmonious interpersonal relations), and restraint with their children. First, Gusii mothers engage in very
are appropriately embedded within those frames. little affect sharing and do not provide the big, open mouth smil-
Nevertheless, a fifth frame devoted to the value of emotion ing displays so commonly noted in the northeastern United
across cultures seems indicated, because emotion is sometimes States. Second, they avert eye gaze even with infants, which
valued (or not) in its own right, as part of what it means to be functions as another means of reducing infant arousal (Dixon
human and as part of life (Parker et al., in press), and so et al., 1981; Richman et al., 1992). From the American stand-
the value of emotion is not linked to the goals identified in the point, these behaviors suggest such low maternal responsive-
above frames. Further, there is substantial variability in the ness and engagement as to be considered possibly pathological;
values and frequency of emotional expression and experience yet by other indices (e.g., tactile engagement and responsive-
within collectivist and individualist cultures (e.g., Eid & Diener, ness to crying), the Gusii mothers are as responsive to their
2001; Parker et al., in press), and possibly within vertical and infants as Bostonian mothers, and appear to be preparing their
horizontal cultures, which provides additional evidence that infants as successfully as the Bostonian mothers for their own
emotional experience is best considered as its own cultural cultures. Nso Cameroonian mothers also value calm infants, and
frame (Eid & Diener, 2001; Mesquita & Frijda, 1992). As one respond with anticipatory coping (preventive) so as to avoid
brief example, anger is not accepted in many collectivist situations in which infants cry (Keller & Otto, 2009).
societies, such as the Utku, Chinese, Taiwanese, and Japanese Cultures in other parts of the world also emphasize restraint
(Briggs, 1970; Eid & Diener, 2001; Markus & Kitayama, 1991), in positive affective arousal. Javanese mothers in Indonesia
but is valued in others, such as the Ifaluk and Kaluli (Lutz, rarely express strong positive emotion with their young chil-
1988; Schieffelin, 1983, 1985). Likewise, happiness appears to dren, as emotional control is valued and strong positive emotion
be valued more in individualistic versus collectivistic cultures, is perceived as disruptive (Eisenberg, Pidada, & Liew, 2001).
and the preferred intensity level of happiness varies as well (Eid Chinese and Japanese families also emphasize positive emo-
& Diener, 2001; Tsai, Knutson, & Fung, 2006). In the broadest tional restraint (Bond & Hwang, 1986; Camras et al., 1998;
way, cultures may value or devalue emotional experience in Chen et al., 1998; Eid & Deiner, 2001; Tsai et al., 2006).
general, may value certain emotions more so than other emo- Restraint in expression of positive affect seems more likely in
tions, may value certain levels of emotional arousal more than collectivist than individualistic societies; however, anecdotal
other levels, or may value emotions contingent upon certain evidence suggests that upper class British society, which is
social contexts. We take as an example the development of strongly individualistic, also downplays high-intensity displays
positive affect across cultures. of positive affect (Super & Harkness, 1982).
First, cultural beliefs about the value and meaning of posi- Emotion-related values are also reflected in other cultural
tive affect are likely to impact parental responsiveness to new- manifestations, for example, preschool children’s picture books,
borns’ gaze, including the frequency and magnitude of their which then further support cultural messages being inculcated
smiling, as well as the warmth and energy in their voices and by caregivers. Happiness is depicted differently cross-culturally,

Downloaded from emr.sagepub.com by INARLTF Oundation on April 6, 2012


164   Emotion Review Vol. 3 No. 2

with relatively calm exemplars such as serenity found in best- mothers in Berlin report that their infants show basic emotions
selling picture books in Taiwan, compared to higher arousal a good bit earlier than Nso mothers of Cameroon (Keller &
exemplars such as joy found in picture books in the United Otto, 2009). What we don’t know is whether German mothers
States (Tsai, Louie, Chen, & Uchida, 2007). Thus, caregivers imbue cues with greater significance than supported by infants’
seem to increase or decrease infants’ positive affect in at least expressions or are relatively more sensitive to nascent expres-
three ways: by modulating their own affective communications, sions of emotions compared to Nso mothers. Either pathway
either by exaggeration or attenuation of affect; by modulating might then lead to greater affective matching by parents and
their infants’ arousal, for example, by increasing or decreasing constructing those emotions in infants. Infants’ experience,
eye gaze directed toward them; or by increasing or decreasing expression, and understanding may all be influenced regarding
involvement in activities that activate positive affect. In all type of emotions as well as intensity of experience. Previous
these cases, parents are engaging in socialization practices that work has included showing videotapes of parenting behaviors
reflect the values of their culture, and are teaching their infants to mothers for their assessment of other cultures’ parenting
and children culturally specific emotional meanings. In addi- styles (Keller, 2003). An important next step may be to show
tion, the tools of the culture (e.g., picture books) contribute to videotapes of infant emotions to parents from cultures that vary
parents’ socialization goals. regarding the value of emotional expression; this methodology
Even within cultures, individual differences in parental would allow for an assessment as to whether parents of differ-
socialization emerge over the course of infancy. For example, in ent cultures actually do interpret infants’ emotion expressions in
the United States, mothers respond differently over time (from keeping with their values regarding emotions.
4 to 24 weeks) to the emotional tones of their infants, with Further, capturing these processes in early infancy would be
approximately half increasing in their responsiveness, one quar- ideal in terms of identifying the transactional processes involved
ter reducing their responsiveness, and one quarter demonstrat- in the inculcation of emotion-related values. As infants shift
ing little change over time (Hsu & Fogel, 2003). This type of from primarily sleep to more waking states, they begin to
longitudinal study is ideal for exploring the relationship become more active participants in creating their affective envi-
between parents’ values and/or beliefs about infant emotionality ronments. And, as they become more coordinated and controlled
and their actual behavior with infants; we predict that beliefs in their reactions, there tends to be a pattern from global/diffuse
about the value of infant emotionality likely direct the trajectories behaviors to specific coordinated behaviors (Sroufe, 1996).
of parents’ responsiveness. These more coordinated behaviors are likely to increasingly
Because infants’ expression of emotion continues to retain reflect the socialization messages communicated by parents
some of its undifferentiated characteristics throughout the first within their cultures. Undoubtedly, infants’ increased participa-
year (Camras, Oster, Campos, & Bakeman, 2003), how infants tion is accompanied by increases in the cognitive components of
and young children learn the specific contours of emotions may memory (e.g., remembering what happens when the sheet comes
be largely dependent upon caregivers’ interpretations of contex- down during “peek-a-boo”) and anticipation (e.g., pulling away
tual cues and beliefs about what infants can and should be feel- that sheet and expecting to find one’s play partner). As external
ing. Thus, parents’ pre-existing belief systems and values stimulation elicits the coordination of these two systems, the
regarding emotion may lead to various degrees of contingent cognitive development results in more sophisticated affective
and noncontingent responses to infants’ emotional expressions. systems in which emotion is tied more to the context of the situ-
These, in turn, may provide important guides to infants’ experi- ation and not just independent stimuli (Sroufe, 1996).
ence, expression, and understanding of emotion, with infants’ Evidence for the influence of parents’ socialization
emotional experiences being shaped by caregivers’ interpretive practices is accumulating in work with toddlers and preschool-
mirroring of their expressions (Campos et al., 2004; Gergely & age children (Chen et al., 1998; Super & Harkness, 1986; Tsai
Watson, 1999), which subsequently leads to understanding et al., 2007). Some suggestions of differential outcomes are
more about self- and others’ emotional experience and later, emerging now from the work of Keller and Otto (2009) in an
appropriate expression thereof. attachment paradigm adapted for use with 1-year-old Nso
In cultures that emphasize knowing one’s own feelings and infants. The ability to adjust to care from others in the commu-
emotional differentiation, caregivers may provide a great deal nity is of some importance and is associated with mothers’
of emotion-related guidance, including affective matching, desire for calm infants. Perhaps because of the strong sociali-
naming feelings, and appropriate responses to perceived feel- zation emphasis on calm demeanor or because of maternal
ings. This type of information allows for substantial construc- preparation for infants to be cared by other adult women in the
tion of emotion because infants’ undifferentiated responses community, the predominant response of Nso infants was to be
invite parental interpretation as to what the infant should be calm with few indices of stress (e.g., cortisol output), suggest-
feeling. In cultures that de-emphasize emotions or support only ing that Nso mothers had successfully socialized their infants
the lowest levels of emotional intensity, caregivers may ignore for this highly important relational task (Keller & Otto, 2009).
infant emotion expression, or attempt to distract attention away And, that calmness was the predominant response for these
from emotions, thereby increasing the time of undifferentiated 1-year-olds, in contrast to the distress and comfort-seeking
emotional expression and reducing attention for the complexity strategies of secure European or American infants, suggests
of emotional experience. It is interesting to note that German that the cultural values inculcated by parents’ socialization

Downloaded from emr.sagepub.com by INARLTF Oundation on April 6, 2012


Halberstadt & Lozada  Emotion Development in Infancy   165

behaviors lead to very different emotion-related behavioral consciously chosen by parents on a day-to-day level (Papoušek &
patterns in infancy. Future longitudinal research beginning Papoušek, 1991). Finally, we acknowledge that there may be other
with newborns would be desirable. cultural frames that can either better classify or further differenti-
ate cross-cultural differences found in the literature. Thus, the
Conclusions cultural frames proposed here are offered as a starting point and as
part of a work in progress.
The five cultural frames described have been offered as organiza- In conclusion, our overarching theme is that cultural frames
tional tools for current and future research. It is likely that as affect parents’ beliefs systems and socialization behaviors;
research builds on these frames, some overlap will be uncovered. these, in turn, as well as the ways in which culture is infused in
Nevertheless, we propose exploring these frames first as inde- every aspect of life, lead to demonstrated effects on developing
pendent rather than overlapping structures so as capitalize on the infants’ emotional experience, expression, and understanding.
richness and diversity across cultures. Further, overlaps that occur In some ways, this message is not new, and, at the surface level,
may be psychologically interesting, particularly if they predict dif- is well recognized. However, it is not yet, in our opinion, well
ferent infant outcomes across cultures. These conceptual frames integrated into the scientific endeavor. Our more specific contri-
can also be considered in combination. For example, a power- butions are: (a) the delineation of five different frameworks,
distant culture that believes infants are capable of early learning three of which have not been previously named or described in
might, through simple instruction, expect infants to show a modi- any detail, (b) the connection of each frame to emotion develop-
cum of emotional control within the first year of life, compared to ment in infancy, and (c) the generation of ideas within each
a power-distant culture that does not believe infants can learn frame for future assessment. We hope these contributions serve
emotional control. In contrast, a more egalitarian society that to generate more systematic study of cultural socialization of
believes infants are capable of early learning might also expect emotional development.
early emotion control, but via verbal dialogues that impart large Infants’ emotion regulation, emotion communication, and
amounts of nuanced affective information. In the first society, emotion understanding are differentially shaped not only by
infants and young children might learn more about interpreting infant characteristics (i.e., temperament), but contextually, via
others’ emotional expression, but their emotional experience will transactions with their social environments—in particular, par-
be relatively simply structured. In the second society, infants and ents and family members, who are themselves recipients and
young children might develop greater emotional complexity to conveyors of the meanings, values, and goals of their culture.
their emotional experience, but will expect people to talk about These caregiver–infant relationships, embedded within the cul-
what they are feeling and may not be as skilled at surmising the tural context, lay the foundation on which socioemotional com-
feelings of others. petencies can be developed. As research continues to uncover
If another frame, such as children’s place in family and cul- the biological and physiological underpinnings of infants’ emo-
ture, is considered in conjunction with the previous two, another tional outcomes, the true meaning behind intra and interindi-
layer is added to the cultural lens. For example, power-distant vidual differences will continue to be intertwined within the
parents who believe that infants learn about emotion later and threads of cultural fabric.
are a gift from the gods may socialize their children with an
emphasis on more positive affect, as this would be pleasant for References
their child, and less negative affect, as this would be less disrup- Baldwin, A. L. (1948). Socialization and the parent–child relationship.
tive of relations with parents or other high-ranking family mem- Child Development, 19, 127–136.
Barrera, M. E., & Maurer, D. (1981). The perception of facial expres-
bers. Combining frames to focus on specific differences in
sions by the three-month-old. Child Development, 52, 203–206.
socialization across cultures can occur with all of the frames. doi: 10.2307/1129231
Although we assert that these frames are valuable for their Bond, M. H., & Hwang, K. K. (1986). The social psychology of Chinese
ability to categorize cross-cultural phenomena, there are some people. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The psychology of the Chinese people
limitations to what we have proposed. First, as some of these (pp. 213–266). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
frames have not been described previously, we have sometimes Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Heslington, M., Gini, M., Suwalsky, J.,
extrapolated widely. Therefore, we rely on future research to help Venuti, P., & Zingman de Galperín, C. (2008). Mother–child emotional
availability in ecological perspective: Three countries, two regions, two
validate and lend support to these frames. Second, we have not
genders. Developmental Psychology, 44, 666–680. doi: 10.1037/0012-
woven in much about genetic processes or physiological outcomes 1649.44.3.666
associated with emotional experience, expression, or understand- Brazelton, T. B., & Sparrow, J. D. (2006). Touchpoints birth to 3: Your
ing; although few studies relate these to culture, there are intrigu- child’s emotional and behavioral development (2nd ed.). Cambridge,
ing hints in the adult literature (e.g., relations between autonomic MA: Da Capo Press.
nervous system [ANS], facial expression, and self-report of Briggs, J. L. (1970). Never in anger: Portrait of an Eskimo family.
emotion; Levenson, Ekman, Heider, & Friesen, 1992). Third, we Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human
do not intend to suggest that many of these frames or the parenting development. American Psychologist, 32, 513–531. doi: 10.1037/0003-
practices associated with them are engaged in consciously or 066X.32.7.513
intentionally by parents; in many ways these frames are so well Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology of developmental
embedded within cultures that they are intuitively rather than processes. Handbook of child psychology: Volume 1: Theoretical models

Downloaded from emr.sagepub.com by INARLTF Oundation on April 6, 2012


166   Emotion Review Vol. 3 No. 2

of human development (5th ed., pp. 993–1028). Hoboken, NJ: John Dixon, S. D., Tronick, E. Z., Keefer, C., & Brazelton, T. B. (1981). Mother–
Wiley & Sons. infant interaction among the Gusii of Kenya. In T. M. Field, A. M. Sostek,
Calkins, S. D. (1994). Origins and outcomes of individual differences in P. Vietze, & P. H. Leiderman (Eds.), Culture and early interactions
emotional regulation. Emotion regulation: Behavioral and biological (pp. 149–170). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
considerations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Donovan, W. L., Leavitt, L. A., & Taylor, N. (2005). Maternal self-efficacy
Development, 2/3, 53–72. and experimentally manipulated infant difficulty effects on maternal
Campos, J. J., Frankel, C. B., & Camras, L. A. (2004). On the nature sensory sensitivity: A signal detection analysis. Developmental
of emotion regulation. Child Development, 75, 377–394. doi: 10.1111/ Psychology, 41, 784–798. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.5.784
j.1467-8624.2004.00681 Donovan, W. L., Leavitt, L. A., & Walsh, R. (1990). Maternal self-efficacy:
Campos, J. J., Thein, S., & Owen, D. (2003). A Darwinian legacy to Illusory control and its effect on susceptibility to learned helplessness.
understanding human infancy: Emotional expressions as behavior Child Development, 61, 1638–1647. doi: 10.2307/1130771
regulators. In P. Ekman, J. Campos, R. Davidson & F. de Waal (Eds.), Dunsmore, J. C., & Halberstadt, A. G. (1997). How does family emotional
Emotions inside out: 130 years after Darwin’s The expression of the expressiveness affect children’s schemas? New Directions for Child
emotions in man and animals (pp. 110–134). New York, NY: Development, 77, 45–66.
University Press. Dunsmore, J. C., & Halberstadt, A. G. (2009). The dynamic context
Camras, L. A., Oster, H., Campos, J. J., & Bakeman, R. (2003). Emotional of children’s emotions: Family and cultural system influences. In
facial expressions in European-American, Japanese, and Chinese J. A. Mancini & K. A. Roberto (Eds.), Pathways of human deve­
infants. In P. Ekman, J. Campos, R. Davidson & F. de Waal (Eds.), lopment: Explorations of change (pp. 171–190). Boulder, CO:
Emotions inside out: 130 years after Darwin’s The expression of the Lexington Books.
emotions in man and animals (pp. 1–17). New York, NY: New York Edwards, C. P., Gandini, L., & Giovaninni, D. (1996). The contrasting
Academy of Sciences. developmental timetables of parents and preschool teachers in two
Camras, L., Oster, H., Campos, J., Campos, R., Ujiie, T., Miyake, K., . . . cultural communities. In S. Harkness & C. M. Super (Eds.),  Parents’
Meng, Z. (1998). Production of emotional facial expressions in cultural belief systems: Their origins, expressions, and consequences 
European American, Japanese, and Chinese infants.  Developmental (pp. 270–288). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Psychology, 34, 616–628. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.4.616 Eid, M., & Diener, E. (2001). Norms for experiencing emotions in different
Cervantes, C. A. (2002). Explanatory emotion talk in Mexican immigrant cultures: Inter- and intranational differences. Journal of Personality and
and Mexican American families. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Social Psychology, 81, 869–885. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.869
Sciences, 24, 138–163. doi: 10.1177/0739986302024002003 Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socializa-
Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting tion of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 241–273. doi:10.1207/
style: Understanding Chinese parenting through the cultural notion s15327965pli0904_1
of training. Child Development, 65, 1111–1119. doi: 10.2307/1131308 Eisenberg, N., Pidada, S., & Liew, J. (2001). The relations of regulation and
Chao, R. K. (1995). Chinese and European American cultural models of the negative emotionality to Indonesian children’s social functioning. Child
self reflected in mothers’ childrearing beliefs. Ethos, 23, 328–354. doi: Development, 72, 1747–1763. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00376
10.1525/eth.1995.23.3.02a00030 Fernald, A. (1993). Approval and disapproval: Infant responsiveness to
Chen, F., & Luster, T. (2002). Factors related to parenting practices in vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Development,
Taiwan. Early Child Development and Care, 172, 413–430. doi: 64, 657–674. doi: 10.2307/1131209
10.1080/03004430214549 Fortes, M. (1938). Social and psychological aspects of education in
Chen, X., Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., Chen, H., Cen, G., & Stewart, S. L. Taleland. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
(1998). Child rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese and Frankel, D. G., Roer-Bornstein, D., & LeVine, R. A. (1982). Traditional and
Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology, modern contributions to changing infant-rearing ideologies of two
34, 677–686. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.4.677 ethnic communities. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Cole, P. M., & Tamang, B. L. (1998). Nepali children’s ideas about emo- Development, 47, 1–51. doi: 10.2307/1165998
tional displays in hypothetical challenges. Developmental Psychology, Fung, H. (1999). Becoming a moral child: The socialization of shame
34, 640–646. doi: 10.1037/00121649.34.4.640 among young Chinese children. Ethos, 27, 180–209. doi: 10.1525/
Cole, P. M., & Tan, P. Z. (2007). Emotion socialization from a cultural per- eth.1999.27.2.180
spective. In J. E. Grusec & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socializa- Garcia Coll, C. T., Halpern, L., Seifer, R., Meyer, E., Kilis, E., Lester, B. M.,
tion: Theory and research (pp. 516–542). New York, NY: Guilford. . . . Oh, W. (1996). Behavioral intervention and post-natal growth in
Craig, A. B., Stelter, R. L., & Halberstadt, A. G. (2010). Parents’ beliefs full-term intrauterine growth retarded (IUGR) infants. Early Human
about children’s emotions cluster by gender, ethnicity, education, and Development, 46, 105–116. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(96)017483
age. Unpublished manuscript. Gaskins, S. (1996). How Mayan parental theories come into play. In
Cristofaro, T. N., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2008). Lessons in mother–child S. Harkness & C. M. Super (Eds.),  Parents’ cultural belief systems:
and father–child personal narratives in Latino families. Spanish- Their origins, expressions, and consequences (pp. 345–363). New York,
language narration and literacy: Culture, cognition, and emotion NY: Guilford Press.
(pp. 54–91). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Geertz, H. (1959). The vocabulary of emotion: A study of Javanese social-
Crockenberg, S. C., & Leerkes, E. M. (2004). Infant and maternal behaviors ization processes. Psychiatry: Journal for the Study of Interpersonal
regulate infant reactivity to novelty at 6 months. Developmental Processes, 22, 225–237.
Psychology, 40, 1123–1132. Gergely, G., & Watson, J. S. (1999). Early socio-emotional development:
Delgado-Gaitan, C. (1994). Socializing young children in Mexican- Contingency perception and the social-biofeedback model. In P. Rochat
American families: An intergenerational perspective. In P. M. Greenfield (Ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months
& R. R. Cocking (Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child develop- of life (pp. 101–136). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
ment (pp. 55–86). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Gesell, A., & Ilg, F. (1943). Infant and child in the culture of today: The
Diener, M. (2000). Gift from the gods: A Balinese guide to early child rear- guidance of development in home and nursery school. New York, NY:
ing. In J. deLoache & A. Gottlieb (Eds.), A world of babies: Imagined Harper & Brothers.
childcare guides for seven societies (pp. 91–116). Cambridge, UK: Gonzalez-Ramos, G., Zayas, L., & Cohen, E. (1998). Child-rearing values
Cambridge University Press. of low-income, urban Puerto Rican mothers of preschool children.

Downloaded from emr.sagepub.com by INARLTF Oundation on April 6, 2012


Halberstadt & Lozada  Emotion Development in Infancy   167

Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 29, 377–382. doi: Kochanska, G., Philibert, R. A., & Barry, R. A. (2009). Interplay of genes
10.1037/0735-7028.29.4.377 and early mother–child relationship in the development of self-regulation
Goodnow, J., Cashmore, J., Cotton, S., & Knight, R. (1984). Mothers’ from toddler to preschool age. Journal of Child Psychology and
developmental timetables in two cultural groups. International Journal Psychiatry, 50, 1331–1338. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02050.x
of Psychology, 19, 193–205. doi: 10.1080/00207598408247526 Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental per-
Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E., & Wallace, C. S. (1987). Experience and brain spective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 199–214. doi: 10.1037/0012-
development. Child Development, 58, 539–559. doi: 10.2307/1130197 1649.18.2.199
Halberstadt, A. G., Denham, S. A., & Dunsmore, J. C. (2001). Affective Kusserow, A. (2004). American individualisms: Child rearing and social
social competence. Social Development, 10, 79–119. class in three neighborhoods. New York, NY: Global Publishing at
Hall, G. S. (1907). Aspects of child life and education. New York, NY: St. Martin’s Press.
D. Appleton & Co. Lamb, M. E. (2005). Attachments, social networks, and developmental
Harwood, R. L., Schölmerich, A., Schulze, P. A., & González, Z. (1999). contexts. Human Development, 48, 108–112. doi: 10.1159/000083222
Cultural differences in maternal beliefs and behaviors: A study of Lamm, B., Keller, H., Yovsi, R., & Chaudhary, N. (2008). Grandmaternal
middle-class Anglo and Puerto Rican mother–infant pairs in four and maternal ethnotheories about early child care. Journal of Family
everyday situations. Child Development, 70, 1005–1016. doi: 10.1111/ Psychology, 22, 80–88. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.22.1.80
14678624.00073 Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., Heider, K., & Friesen, W. V. (1992). Emotion
Hess, R. (1980). Maternal expectations for mastery of developmental tasks and autonomic nervous system activity in the Minangkabau of West
in Japan and the United States. International Journal of Psychology, 15, Sumatra. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 972–988.
259–271. doi: 10.1080/00207598008246996 doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.62.6.972
Hofstede, G. (1980). Cultures consequences: International differences in Lewis, M. (2005). The child and its family: The social network model.
work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Human Development, 48, 8–27. doi: 10.1159.000083213
Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acquisition
institutions and organizations across nations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. of self. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Hsu, H., & Fogel, A. (2003). Social regulatory effects of infant nondistress Lewis, M., Feiring, C., & Kotsonis, M. (1984). The social networks of the
vocalization on maternal behavior. Developmental Psychology, 39, young child: A developmental perspective. In M. Lewis (Ed.), Beyond
976–991. doi: 10.1037/00121649.39.6.976 the dyad (pp. 129–160). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Joshi, M. S., & MacLean, M. (1997). Maternal expectations of child devel- Lewis, M., Hitchcock, D. F. A., & Sullivan, M. W. (2004). Physiological and
opment in India, Japan, and England. Journal of Cross-Cultural emotional reactivity to learning and frustration. Infancy, 6, 121–143.
Psychology, 28, 219–234. doi: 10.1177/0022022197282005 doi: 10.1207/s15327078in0601_6
Kagitçibasi, Ç. (1996). The autonomous-relational self: A new synthesis. Lewis, M., Takai-Kawakami, K., Kawakami, K., & Sullivan, M. (2010).
European Psychologist, 1, 180–186. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040.1.3.180 Cultural differences in emotional responses to success and fail-
Kagitçibasi, Ç., & Ataca, B. (2005). Value of children and family change: ure. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34, 53–61. doi:
A three-decade portrait from Turkey. Applied Psychology: An 10.1177/0165025409348559
International Review, 54, 317–337. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2005.00213 Li, J., Wang, L., & Fischer, K. W. (2004). The organization of Chinese
Keller, H. (2003). Socialization for competence: Cultural models of infancy. shame concepts. Cognition & Emotion, 18, 767–797. doi: 10.1080/
Human Development, 46, 288–311. doi: 10.1159/000071937 02699930341000202
Keller, H. (2007).  Cultures of infancy. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Lutz, C. (1983). Parental goals, ethnopsychology, and the development of emo-
Associates. tional meaning. Ethos, 11, 246–262. doi: 10.1525/eth.1983.11.4.02a00040
Keller, H., Abels, M., Borke, J., Lamm, B., Lo, W., Su, Y., & Wang, Y. Lutz, C. (1988). Unnatural emotions: Everyday sentiments on a Micronesian
(2007). Socialization environments of Chinese and Euro-American atoll & their challenge to Western theory. Chicago, IL: University of
middle-class babies: Parenting behaviors, verbal discourses and ethno- Chicago Press.
theories. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31, 210–217. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for
doi: 10.1177/0165025407074633 cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.
Keller, H., Borke, J., Yovsi, R. D., Lohaus, A., & Jensen, H. (2005). Cultural doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224
orientations and historical changes as predictors of parenting behavior. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1994). A collective fear of the collective:
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 229–237. Implications for selves and theories of selves.  Personality and Social
Keller, H., Kärtner, J., Borke, J., Yovsi, R., & Kleis, A. (2005). Parenting Psychology Bulletin, 20, 568–579.
styles and the development of the categorical self: A longitudinal study Masataka, N. (2002). Low anger-aggression and anxiety-withdrawal character-
on mirror self-recognition in Cameroonian Nso and German families. istic to preschoolers in Japanese society where “hikikomori” is becoming a
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 496–504. major social problem. Early Education & Development, 13, 187–199.
doi: 10.1080/01650250500147485 Mastropieri, D., & Turkewitz, G. (1999). Prenatal experience and neonatal
Keller, H., Lamm, B., Abels, M., Yovsi, R. D., Borke, J., Jensen, H., . . . responsiveness to vocal expressions of emotion. Developmental
Chaudhary, N. (2006). Cultural models, socialization goals, and parent- Psychobiology,  35, 204–214. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199911)
ing ethnotheories: A multi-cultural analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural 35:3<204::AID-DEV5>3.0.CO;2-V
Psychology, 37, 155–172. Matsumoto, D. (1996). Culture & psychology. London, UK: Brooks/Cole
Keller, H., & Otto, H. (2009). The cultural socialization of emotion regula- Publishing.
tion during infancy. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40, 996–1011. Mesquita, B. (2001). Emotions in collectivist and individualistic contexts.
doi: 10.1177/0022022109348576. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 68–74. doi: 10.1037//
Keller, H., Otto, H., Lamm, B., Yovsi, R., & Kärtner, J. (2008). The timing O022-3514.80.1.68
of verbal/vocal communications between mothers and their infants: Mesquita, B., & Frijda, N. (1992). Cultural variations in emotions: A review.
A longitudinal cross-cultural comparison. Infant Behavior & Psychological Bulletin, 112, 179–204. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.2.179
Development, 31, 217–226. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.10.001 Mesquita, B., & Karasawa, M. (2004). Self-conscious emotions as dynamic
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Kurokawa, M. (2000). Culture, emotion, cultural processes. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 161–166.
and well-being: Good feelings in Japan and the United States. Cognition Miller, P. J., Wang, S., Sandel, T., & Cho, G. E. (2002). Self-esteem as folk
& Emotion, 14, 93–124. doi: 10.1080/026999300379003 theory: A comparison of European American and Taiwanese mothers’

Downloaded from emr.sagepub.com by INARLTF Oundation on April 6, 2012


168   Emotion Review Vol. 3 No. 2

beliefs. Parenting: Science and Practice, 2, 209–239. doi: 10.1207/ Singelis, T. M., Triandis, H. C., Bhawuk, D., & Gelfand, M. J. (1995).
S15327922PAR0203_02 Horizontal and vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism:
Mills-Koonce, W., Propper, C., Gariepy, J., Blair, C., Garrett-Peters, P., & A theoretical and measurement refinement. Cross-Cultural Research:
Cox, M. (2007). Bidirectional genetic and environmental influences of The Journal of Comparative Social Science, 29, 240–275.
mother and child behavior: The family system as the unit of analyses. doi: 10.1177/106939719502900302
Development and Psychopathology, 19, 1073–1087. doi: 10.1017/ Sroufe, A. L. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of
S0954579407000545 emotional life in the early years. New York, NY: Cambridge University
Montague, D., & Walker-Andrews, A. (2002). Mothers, fathers, and infants: Press.
The role of person familiarity and parental involvement in infants’ per- Stipek, D. (1998). Differences between Americans and Chinese in the cir-
ception of emotion expressions.  Child Development,  73, 1339–1352. cumstances evoking pride, shame, and guilt. Journal of Cross-Cultural
doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00475 Psychology, 29, 616–629. doi: 10.1177/0022022198295002
Much, N. C. (1997). A semiotic view of socialization, lifespan development Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (1982). The development of affect in infancy
and cultural psychology: With vignettes from the moral culture of tradi- and early childhood. In D. A. Wagner & H. W. Stevenson (Eds.),
tional Hindu households. Psychology and Developing Societies, 9, Cultural perspectives on child development (pp. 1–19). San Francisco,
65–106. doi: 10.1177/097133369700900104 CA: Freeman.
Muir, D. W., Humphrey, D. E., & Humphrey, K. G. (1994). Pattern and Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: A concep-
space perception in young infants. Spatial Vision, 8, 141–165. tualization at the interface of child and culture. International Journal of
Nielsen, M. (2006). Copying actions and copying outcomes: Social learning Behavioral Development, 9, 545–569.
through the second year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 555–565. doi: Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (1997). The cultural structuring of child
10.1037/0012-1649.42.3.555 development. In J. W. Berry, P. R. Dasen, & T. S. Saraswathi
Papoušek, H., & Papoušek, M. (1991). Innate and cultural guidance of (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, Vol. 2: Basic processes
infants’ integrative competencies: China, the United States, and and human development (pp. 1–39). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn &
Germany. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Cultural approaches to parenting Bacon.
(pp. 23–44). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Way, N., Hughes, D., Yoshikawa, H., Kalman, R. K.,
Parker, A. E., Halberstadt, A. G., Dunsmore, J. C., Townley, G. E., Bryant, A., & Niwa, E. Y. (2008). Parents’ goals for children: The dynamic coexis-
Jr., Thompson, J. A., & Beale, K. S. (in press). “Emotions are a window tence of individualism and collectivism in cultures and individuals.
into one’s heart”: A qualitative analysis of parental beliefs about chil- Social Development, 17, 183–209.
dren’s emotions across three ethnic groups. Monographs of the Society Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO:
for Research in Child Development. Westview Press.
Perez-Rivera, M. B., & Dunsmore, J. C. (in press). Mothers’ acculturation and Triandis, H. C., Bontempo, R., Villareal, M. J., Asai, M., & Lucca, N.
beliefs about emotions, mother–child emotion discourse, and children’s (1988). Individualism and collectivism: Cross-cultural perspectives
emotion understanding in Latino families. Early Education & Development. on self–ingroup relationships. Journal of Personality and Social
Pomerleau, A., Malcuit, G., & Sabatier, C. (1991). Child-rearing practices Psychology, 54, 323–338. doi: 0022-3514/88
and parental beliefs in three cultural groups of Montréal: Québécois, Triandis, H. C., & Gelfand, M. (1998). Converging measurement of
Vietnamese, Haitian. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Cultural approaches to horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. Journal of
parenting (pp. 45–68). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 118–128. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.
Propper, C., & Moore, G. A. (2006). The influence of parenting on infant 74.1.118
emotionality: A multi-level psychobiological perspective. Developmental Triandis, H. C., Marín, G., Lisansky, J., & Betancourt, H. (1984). Simpatía
Review, 26, 427–460. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2006.06.003 as a cultural script of Hispanics. Journal of Personality and Social
Richman, A. L., Miller, P. M., & LeVine, R. A. (1992). Cultural and Psychology, 47, 1363–1375. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.47.6.1363
educational variations in maternal responsiveness. Developmental Tsai, J. L., Knutson, B., & Fung, H. H. (2006). Cultural variation in affect
Psychology, 28, 614–621. doi: 10.1037/00121649.28.4.614 valuation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 288–307.
Saarni, C., Campos, J., Camras, L., & Witherington, D. (2006). Emotional doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.288
development: Action, communication, and understanding.  In Tsai, J. L., Louie, J. Y., Chen, E. E., & Uchida, Y. (2007). Learning what
W. Damon, R. M. Lerner & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child feelings to desire: Socialization of ideal affect through children’s story-
psychology: Vol. 3, Social, emotional, and personality development (6th books. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 17–30.
ed., pp. 226–299). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. doi: 10.1177/0146167206292749
Schieffelin, E. L. (1983). Anger and shame in the tropical forest: On van Hemert, D. A., Poortinga, Y. H., & van der Vijver, F. J. R. (2007).
affect as a cultural system in Papua New Guinea. Ethos, 11, 181–191. Emotion and culture: A meta-analysis. Cognition & Emotion, 21, 913–
doi: 10.1525/eth.1983.11.3.02a00060 943. doi: 10.1080/02699930701339293
Schieffelin, E. (1985). Anger, grief, and shame: Toward a Kaluli ethnopsy- Weir, C., Toland, C., King, R. A., & Martin, L. M. (2005). Infant contingency/
chology. In G. M. White & J. Kirkpatrick (Eds.), Person, self, and extinction performance after observing partial reinforcement. Infancy,
experience: Exploring Pacific ethnopsychologies (pp. 168–182). 8, 63–80. doi: 10.1207/s15327078in0801
Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Yovsi, R., Kärtner, J., Keller, H., & Lohaus, A. (2009). Maternal interac-
Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2002). Cultural influences on the tional quality in two cultural environments: German middle class and
relation between pleasant emotions and unpleasant emotions: Asian Cameroonian rural mothers. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40,
dialectic philosophies or individualism-collectivism? Cognition & 701–707. doi: 10.1177/0022022109335065
Emotion, 16, 705–719. doi: 10.1080/02699930143000590 Zahn-Waxler, C., Friedman, R. J., Cole, P. M., Mizuta, I., & Hiruma, N.
Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Beyond individualism/collectivism: New cultural (1996). Japanese and United States preschool children’s responses to
dimensions of values. In U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, C. Kagitcibasi, S. C. Choi conflict and distress. Child Development, 67, 2462–2477. doi: 10.2307/
& G. Yoon (Eds.), Individualism and collectivism: Theory, method, and 1131634
applications (pp. 85–119). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Zayas, L. H., & Solari, F. (1994). Early childhood socialization in Hispanic
Scollon, C. N., Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2004). Emotions families: Context, culture, and practice implications. Professional
across cultures and methods. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, Psychology: Research and Practice, 25, 200–206. doi: 10.1037/0735-
304–326. doi: 10.1177/0022022104264124 7028.25.3.200

Downloaded from emr.sagepub.com by INARLTF Oundation on April 6, 2012

You might also like