Where Do Desires Come From?

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Motiv Emot (2017) 41:431–442

DOI 10.1007/s11031-017-9617-7

ORIGINAL PAPER

Where do desires come from? Positivity offset and negativity bias


predict implicit attitude toward temptations
Alethea H. Q. Koh1 · Lile Jia1   · Edward R. Hirt2 

Published online: 4 May 2017


© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2017

Abstract  Temptations elicit both appetitive and aversive Introduction


responses because they offer hedonic gratification on the
one hand and impede long-term goal pursuit on the other People struggle to resist various desires every day (Hof-
hand (Fujita, Personality and Social Psychology Review mann et  al. 2012, 2013). A majority of these desires are
15(4):352–366, 2011). In this paper, we investigate how temptations: short-term gains that conflict with one’s pur-
people’s affective responses toward temptations are regu- suit of long-term goals (Fujita 2011). Because tempta-
lated by the appetitive and aversive motivational systems. tions can elicit both appetitive and aversive responses, two
We employ the mini Motivated Action Measure (mini- fundamental motivational systems (Cacioppo et  al. 1999;
MAM; Lang et al., Communication Methods and Measures Gray 1987, 1994; Lang 1995)—the appetitive and aver-
5(2):146–162, 2011) to measure the signature patterns with sive systems—regulate people’s responses to temptations.
which the two systems regulate affective activation: positiv- We argue that past research investigating individual dif-
ity offset and negativity bias. We found that positivity offset ferences related to the two systems have focused primar-
and negativity bias predict unique variance (5.5%) of diet- ily on the behavioral regulation of the two systems (e.g.,
ers’ (N = 312) implicit attitude toward tempting foods, over BIS/BAS; Carver and White 1994). Here, we consider how
and above predictors related to behavioral regulation (BIS/ the two motivational systems interplay and regulate affec-
BAS: Carver, White, Journal of Personality and Social Psy- tive activation. Specifically, we aim to explore if the rela-
chology 67:319–333, 1994; BSC: Tangney et al., Journal of tive activation of one system over the other, captured by the
Personality 72(2):271–324, 2004). By contrast, positivity Motivated Action Measure (MAM: Lang et al. 2005, mini-
offset and negativity bias did not predict dieters’ behavioral MAM: 2011), may be a better predictor for people’s spon-
intentions for tempting foods. Investigating how the appeti- taneous affective reactions toward temptations than meas-
tive and aversive systems regulate affective activation apart ures related to behavioral regulation.
from behavioral responses offers unique insights into peo-
ple’s desires towards temptations.
Appetitive, aversive motivational systems,
Keywords  Temptation · Motivational systems · and temptations
Appetitive and aversive responses · Implicit attitude ·
Desire Two motivational systems underlie our interactions with
the social environment (Cacioppo et  al. 1999; Gray 1987,
1994; Lang 1995). The appetitive system determines how
* Lile Jia readily one would approach and engage with the stimulus,
psyjl@nus.edu.sg
deem it to be safe or appealing, and experience affective
1
Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, positivity towards it. The aversive system determines how
Singapore, Singapore defensively one would react and avoid the stimulus, deem it
2
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana to be harmful or threatening, and experience affective nega-
University, Bloomington, USA tivity towards it (Lee and Lang 2009; Potter et  al. 2006).

13
Vol.:(0123456789)

432 Motiv Emot (2017) 41:431–442

Both systems are evolutionarily adaptive, with the appe- more likely to resist the temptation (Dholakia et al. 2006).
titive system promoting exploration and foraging behavior Hofmann and colleagues (2012) conducted the most com-
and the aversive system enabling timely withdrawal from prehensive investigation of people’s daily experience and
danger (Ito and Cacioppo 2005; Lang and Bradley 2013). coping of desires (i.e., food, social, leisure) to date. Their
Given the importance of the two systems in human sur- findings suggest that BIS/BAS seemed to predict more of
vival, scholars have been interested in capturing individual how people regulate desires than how desires are origi-
differences in how they regulate behavior. The predominant nated. First, BAS predicted people’s self-reported strength,
measures in the literature are the behavioral inhibition and but not frequency, of desires. Put another way, BAS had no
activation system (BIS/BAS) scales (Carver and White bearing on people’s susceptibility in experiencing desires
1994). The BIS/BAS was designed to capture the regula- but only affected how intensely they experience it after the
tion of approach and avoidance behavior by the appetitive desire had occurred. Second, people’s self-reported desire
and aversive systems, respectively (Carver and White 1994; strength may inherently encompass conscious control, for it
Cooper et  al. 2007; Leone et  al. 2001). BIS measures the was also predicted, negatively, by trait self-control. Again,
degree to which the person will move away from (avoid) an BAS might have predicted self-reported desire strength by
undesirable, unpleasant goal or punishment; BAS measures virtue of its association with behavioral regulation rather
the degree to which a person will act towards (approach) a than affective origins of desires. Finally, BIS predicted
desirable goal or reward. BIS/BAS scales have been shown neither frequency nor strength of desires. Instead, BIS,
to predict impulsivity (Caseras et al. 2003; Dawe and Lox- together with trait self-control, negatively predicted subse-
ton 2004; Jorm et al. 1998), alcoholic tendencies (Franken quent behavioral enactment of desires. This again indicates
2002; Zisserson and Palfai 2007), as well as substance use that BIS/BAS mostly taps into the way appetitive and aver-
and risky health behaviors (Franken and Muris 2006a). In sive systems regulate people’s behavioral responses toward
general, BIS negatively and BAS positively predict impul- temptations or desires.
sive, detrimental behaviors.
Individual differences in the activations of the two sys-
tems should contribute to people’s desires toward temp-
tations because temptations can elicit both appetitive and Affective activation of the motivational systems
aversive responses. Temptations have two key features:
short-term gains that also conflict with one’s pursuit of The appetitive and aversive systems may, however, regulate
long-term goals (Fujita 2011). Partaking in temptation not only behavioral responses but also affective activation
offers immediate gratification (e.g., consuming alcohol), toward temptations. The appetitive system may activate
but may impede our long-term goals (e.g., being produc- the initial appetitive feeling toward temptations as well as
tive). Thus, temptations can activate two opposing affective regulate the subsequent approach behavior; similarly, the
responses. First, the hedonic and often visceral gratifica- aversive system may be in charge of both activating the
tion associated with temptations (Loewenstein 1996) can initial aversive feeling and regulating the subsequent avoid-
activate appetitive responses. Second, because temptations ant behavior toward temptations. Accordingly, because the
are instrumentally incongruent with long-term goal pursuit conceptualization of BIS/BAS scales focuses on behavioral
and succumbing to them also reflects negatively on one’s regulation (Carver and White 1994), it might not be captur-
self-efficacy (Rothman et al. 2011), they can activate aver- ing the way the two motivational systems regulate affective
sive responses in the forms of negative evaluation (Fergu- activation. Our distinction between affective activation and
son and Bargh 2004; Fishbach et  al. 2010) and negative behavioral regulation is consistent with recent models of
emotions such as regret (Abraham and Sheeran 2003) and desire and self-control. Most notably, Kotabe and Hofmann
shame (Patrick et  al. 2009). Consequently, temptations (2015) distinguish between desire and control, whereby the
should evoke a strong appetitive response from people with final behavioral outcome toward the desirable target results
high BAS and a strong aversive response from people with from the relative potency of desire and control. High con-
high BIS. How the appetitive and aversive systems regu- trol does not necessitate abstinence if the desire is over-
late responses, thus, should predict where people’s desire whelming. Conversely, low desire may be enacted if the
toward temptations comes from. control against it is weak. Here, we similarly argue that the
Evidence for this prediction, however, has been incon- appetitive and aversive systems are relevant to both affec-
sistent. Individual differences in the affective and aversive tive activation (or desire) and behavioral control. Under-
systems as captured by BIS/BAS have shown mixed influ- standing individual differences in how the interplay of the
ence on people’s responses to desires. Although individu- appetitive and aversive systems regulate affective activation
als with higher BAS experience desires with more intensity would thus complement past focus on behavioral regula-
(Franken 2002; Zisserson and Palfai 2007), they are also tion, painting a fuller picture of the relationship between

13
Motiv Emot (2017) 41:431–442 433

Fig. 1  Relative activation of
the appetitive and aversive moti-
vational systems (adapted from
Lang et al. 2005). Positivity

Activation Strength
offset is represented by a higher
activation strength of the appeti-
tive system over aversive system
at intercept (low arousal). Nega-
tivity bias is represented by the
steeper slope with which the
aversive system rises in activa- Appetitive system activation
tion than the appetitive system Aversive system activation
as arousal increases
Negativity Bias
Positivity (Steeper slope)
Offset

Arousal

the motivational systems and people’s desire toward arousal. Two indices, approach system activation (ASA)
temptations. and defensive system activation (DSA), can be computed
Past research suggests that the two motivational systems from participants’ feelings. The computation of ASA cap-
regulate affective activation through two signature activa- tures the signature activation pattern of the positivity off-
tion patterns (Ito and Cacioppo 2005; Lang et  al. 2005). set, while the computation of DSA captures the signature
The first signature pattern, termed the positivity offset, activation pattern of the negativity bias. Hence, individ-
focuses on the relative activation of the appetitive system ual differences in ASA would show how readily the indi-
over the aversive system. This pattern of activation emerges vidual’s appetitive system activates over the aversive sys-
when the person’s appetitive system is relatively more tem at low levels of arousal, and higher ASA predisposes
responsive (readily activated) than the aversive system in an individual with a greater likelihood of a resultant posi-
emotionally neutral situations (i.e., safe, peaceful and com- tive affective response. Conversely, individual differences
fortable contexts). The person thus assumes a positive out- in DSA would show how readily the individual’s aversive
look when entering a social environment, ready to engage system activates over the appetitive system as arousal
and explore the environment. The second signature pattern, increases, and higher DSA predisposes the individual
termed as the negativity bias, depicts the relative activation with a greater likelihood of a resultant negative affective
of the aversive system over the appetitive system. This pat- response (Lang et al. 2007; Wang et al. 2011).
tern emerges when the aversive system readily responds Research has found that ASA and DSA exert independ-
with more intensity than the appetitive system, when the ent and opposite effects on people’s affective responses.
situation increases in arousal. In other words, while the Whereby high ASA has been linked to high sensation
aversive system is less ready to fire up than the appetitive seeking and greater alcohol use, high DSA tends to be
system in a neutral state, as soon as a threatening stimulus found among low sensation seekers (Finn 2002; Joseph
appears in the environment, the activation of the aversive et  al. 2009; Rubenking and Lang 2015). In the context
system quickly overwhelms the appetitive system to moti- of substance abuse, people with high ASA demonstrated
vate avoidant responses (Fig.  1). The signature activation more positive physiological responses (i.e., startle reflex,
patterns of the appetitive and aversive systems—positivity facial muscle activity) towards images of the addictive
offset and negativity bias—allow a person to retain both substances, while individuals with high DSA displayed
curiosity and cautiousness about the surroundings. The per- stronger physiological responses of withdrawal from the
son is motivated to explore in safety and prepared to avoid images (Lang and Yegiyan 2011). Even in the context of
danger if the situation turns for the worse. media consumption, whereby strong physiological addic-
How can we measure these two signature activation tion is largely absent, ASA and DSA predict people’s
patterns? The Motivation Action Measure (MAM; Lang preferences. People with higher ASA prefer more risqué,
et al. 2005) has been utilized to capture individual differ- competitive, and violent content such as pornography and
ences in positivity offset and negativity bias. In MAM, violent video games, while those with DSA were more
participants view a range of positive and negative pic- inclined towards safer, traditional forms of entertainment
tures and report their feelings in terms of valence and

13

434 Motiv Emot (2017) 41:431–442

such as news and puzzle games (Alhabash et  al. 2014; affective responses (Fazio and Olson 2014), and implicit
Krcmar et al. 2015; Potter et al. 2011). attitude in particular may represent the computational
Despite the clear overlap in conceptualization and outcome from affective activations of the appetitive and
empirical evidence, the MAM and BIS/BAS have not been aversive systems. The dual-process models of self-reg-
investigated together. In the present paper, we aim to bridge ulation (e.g., Strack and Deutsch 2004) make the distinc-
the gap in the literature by considering affective activation tion between an association-based, resource-independent
in addition to behavioral regulation. We included the MAM impulsive system and a rule-based, resource-dependent
for additional predictive utility, as it directly captures indi- reflective system. The affective systems are thought to exert
viduals’ affective response to temptations. Specifically, direct influence in the impulsive system; the reflective sys-
ASA and DSA should contribute to the relative strength tem, when the person has adequate cognitive resources and
of the appetitive and aversive responses elicited by temp- motivation, may further modulate the behavioral outcome.
tations. As discussed, temptations embody features that Implicit attitude has been deemed to represent the evalu-
respectively activate the affective and aversive systems: ative summary in the impulsive system. Indeed, implicit
hedonic positivity from immediate, often visceral gratifi- attitude predicts behavior especially when one’s ability for
cation (Loewenstein 1996), versus instrumental negativity controlled processing is diminished (Hofmann et al. 2007,
from being a detriment to one’s efforts to fulfil beneficial 2008). Hence, we posit that people’s implicit attitudes
long-term goals (Abraham and Sheeran 2003; Ferguson reflect the outcome of their impulsive affective activations
and Bargh 2004; Fujita 2011; Patrick et al. 2009). The pro- of the two motivational systems. In addition, we measure
nounced pattern of positivity offset associated with high people’s behavioral responses toward temptations through
ASA would signify that the hedonic positivity of a tempta- a preference task, where they choose between tempting or
tion activates stronger appetitive responses than instrumen- non-tempting options. We argue that like previous studies
tal negativity activates aversive responses. Hence, individu- (Hofmann et al. 2012), people’s responses on such explicit
als with high, as compared to low, ASA should generate measures may be the result of behavioral regulation rather
a larger offset of appetitive over aversive responses from than affective activation.
temptations. This initial offset can be countered by the acti-
vation of the aversive system. The negativity bias allows
the instrumental negativity of temptations to activate an Overview of current research
intense aversive response. DSA indexes the effectiveness
of this negativity bias. People with high DSA should thus We test our predictions in a dieting context. Dieting
generate a stronger aversive response to temptations than revolves around achieving an ideal body appearance or
those with low DSA, and should be better able to refrain healthy body in the long run, but dieters often meet with
and withdraw from the temptation. temptations in the form of tasty, unhealthy food. The con-
Hence, the MAM and the BIS/BAS differ in that the sumption of such food gives immediate pleasure, but holds
MAM measures the trait affective reaction toward the negative consequences such as weight gain or health issues
temptation targets, while the BIS/BAS measures the trait contrary to the individual’s long-term goal. As such, we are
propensity to behaviorally consume those objects. Though interested in how dieters regulate their affective activation
they both deal with trait level motivational systems, MAM and behavioral responses toward tasty, unhealthy food.
is uniquely suited in determining if a temptation target We employed the miniMAM, a brief and validated
will elicit desire. It gauges the allure of the temptation version of the MAM (Lang et  al. 2011), and the BIS/
object through the combined activations of appetitive and BAS (Carver and White 1994) to index affective acti-
approach motivations to determine the resultant affective vation and behavioral regulation of the appetitive and
experience. In contrast, the BIS/BAS measures the propen- aversive systems, respectively. As another index of
sity to act on the temptation. In sum, we predict that ASA behavioral regulation to contrast with the MAM, we
and DSA guide people’s spontaneous desire towards temp- measured participants’ trait self-control using the Brief
tations, albeit in opposite directions. On the other hand, we Self-Control scale (BSC: Tangney et  al. 2004). Trait
expect the BIS/BAS to be less instrumental in predicting self-control has been construed as the ability to over-
such spontaneous responses, but more predictive of peo- come instinctual response tendencies for self-regulatory
ple’s behavioral tendency to indulge in temptations. purposes (Metcalfe and Mischel 1999; Baumeister and
To examine whether the MAM has any predictive utility Vohs 2004), focusing on the effortful promotion of desir-
over the BIS/BAS, we measure both affective and behav- able responses and inhibition of undesirable responses
ioral responses to temptation. We aim to capture people’s (Baumeister et al. 1994; Muraven and Baumeister 2000;
spontaneous affective responses to temptations in the form Tangney et  al. 2004). Past research has shown that the
of implicit attitude. Attitude is the evaluative summary of BSC, like the BIS/BAS, is strongly related to behavioral

13
Motiv Emot (2017) 41:431–442 435

regulation (e.g., Hofmann et al. 2012). As such, showing Procedure


that MAM predicts implicit attitude toward temptations
over and above both the BIS/BAS and BSC, two trait Participants turned up for two sessions, separated by about
individual differences of behavioral regulation, would a week (5–9 days). During the first session, participants
lend strong support to our prediction that the MAM completed the mini Motivation Activation Measure (Lang
uniquely indexes affective activation. Conversely, if both et  al. 2011). Participants were presented with 35 pictures
the BIS/BAS and BSC are more predictive of partici- (none of the pictures are about food) of varying arousal
pants’ behavioural responses than the MAM, we would (categorized into 6 arousal levels), selected from the Inter-
be more confident that the MAM is much less involved national Affective Picture System (IAPS). For each picture,
in behavioural regulation. Hence, the BSC would allow they rated its arousal first, followed by positive and nega-
us to better elucidate how MAM accounts for the affec- tive content in random order using a 9-point scale, 1 (not at
tive origin of temptations. all) to 9 (extremely). The pictures were randomly presented
Participants’ implicit attitude towards tempting foods to prevent order-effects.
(e.g., cakes, burgers) was measured using an Affect We computed ASA and DSA according to Lang et  al.
Misattribution Procedure (Payne et  al. 2005). Partici- (2005, 2011). The computation of ASA follows the char-
pants’ behavioral intention was measured through a acterization of the positivity offset: the readiness of activa-
food choice task. They had to choose between receiv- tion of the appetitive over the aversive system in a neutral
ing healthy or unhealthy foods as possible parting gifts. setting. Two mean positivity ratings were calculated. First,
The choice of an unhealthy food indicates indulgence participants’ mean positivity rating for the seven positive
in temptation, against the dieting goal. We predict that images highest in arousal (level 6) was calculated. These
participants’ implicit attitude toward tempting foods is mean positivity ratings indicated the maximum activation
positively predicted by ASA and negatively predicted by of the appetitive system towards highly arousing pleasant
DSA, over and above BIS/BAS and BSC. ASA and DSA, stimuli. Second, participants’ mean positivity ratings for
however, should be less effective with regard to partici- all 14 positive and negative images lowest in arousal (level
pants’ behavioral intention towards temptation. 1) were calculated. These mean positivity ratings indicated
the resting activation of the appetitive system. The differ-
ence between the first and second mean positive ratings is
then taken as the readiness of activation of the appetitive
Method system.
The computation of DSA follows the characterization of
Participants the negativity bias: the readiness of activation of the aver-
sive over the appetitive system at rising levels of arousal.
From a mass survey during a university lecture, 358 stu- Two mean negativity ratings were calculated. First, partici-
dents who indicated they had a dieting goal were pants’ mean negativity rating for the seven negative images
recruited for the current study. Forty-two participants did of medium level of arousal (level 3/4) was calculated.
not complete the full study and four additional partici- These mean negativity ratings indicated the activation of
pants had computer errors. They were hence excluded the aversive system towards stimuli which are slightly more
from the analyses. Of the remaining 312 participants negative than neutral. Second, participants’ mean negativ-
(M = 18.9 years, SD  = 1.3 years), 213 were female. ity ratings for all 14 positive and negative images lowest
Because correlations between individual differences sta- in arousal (level 1) were calculated. These mean negativ-
bilize as the sample size approaches 250 (Schönbrodt and ity ratings indicated the resting activation of the aversive
Perugini 2013), our sample size is appropriate for our system. The difference between the first and second mean
investigation. Similarly, power analysis1 indicates that we negative rating for negative images is taken as the readiness
have high power to detect small to medium effect sizes of activation of the aversive system.
(Cohen 1988). Participants’ activations of the behavioral inhibition
and approach systems were measured using the BIS/BAS
scale (Carver and White 1994). For the current study, 13
1
  G*Power software (Faul et  al. 2009) was employed to investigate items (α  = 0.80) measuring BAS-related factors; drive,
the kind of power our study has in detecting the effects of ASA and fun seeking, and reward responsiveness were averaged for
DSA. The sensitivity analysis suggests that our study has 80% power each factor and summed together as participants’ overall
to detect an effect size of f2 = 0.032. The post hoc power analysis indi- BAS. The correlations for these subscales can be found in
cated an observed power of 0.974 (effect size, f2 = 0.058). Hence, the
study has high power to detect small to medium effect sizes (Cohen Table  1. Seven items were averaged as participants’ BIS
1988). (α = 0.72), following the normal procedure. Participants

13

436 Motiv Emot (2017) 41:431–442

Table 1  Correlations between ASA, DSA, other predictor variables and food primes

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

1. ASA
2. DSA 0.08
3. BIS −0.10 0.15**
4. BAS 0.16** 0.10 −0.053
5. Reward 0.07 0.15** 0.22** 0.65**
6. Drive 0.14* 0.08 −0.05 0.83** 0.45**
7. Fun 0.15** 0.02 −0.19** 0.77** 0.23** 0.39**
8. BSC −0.22** 0.03 −0.00 −0.24** −0.09 −0.07 −0.36**
9. Grey primes 0.02 −0.01 0.03 0.01 0.02 0.03 −0.03 −0.033
10. Neutral primes 0.10 0.02 −0.00 0.07 0.06 0.04 0.06 −0.06 0.44**
11. Food primes 0.24** −0.11* −0.06 0.11* 0.07 0.11 0.07 −0.02 0.19** 0.47**

**Sig. at the 0.01 level (2-tailed)


*Sig. at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)

also completed the brief self-control scale (α = 0.81, BSC: were thinking of giving presents to subsequent par-
Tangney et  al. 2004), and other unrelated measures that ticipants of future experiments and were asked to aid in
included demographic questions. choosing one out of two possible gifts for ten different
During the second session, participants completed the pairs of gifts. Participants were asked to indicate which
Affect Misattribution Procedure (Payne et  al. 2005) to gift they would take, should they be presented with each
examine their implicit attitudes towards tempting foods. pair at the end of the experimental session. Out of the ten
Participants were presented with pairs of images flashed pairs, three choices pitted unhealthy foods with healthy
sequentially, first with an image of the attitude object ones: apple versus chips, tea versus soda, chocolate ver-
(prime stimuli) followed by a Chinese pictograph. They sus granola bar (reverse scored).
were asked to do nothing with the first image and only Participants completed other unrelated tasks before
judge the visual pleasantness of the Chinese pictograph. going through funneled debriefing (Bargh and Chartrand
For each pictograph, they were instructed to press keys to 2000), and no one guessed the purpose of the experiment.
indicate if the pictograph was more or less visually pleas-
ant than average. Their judgments are assumed to reflect
the (misattributed) affective response to the prime (i.e.,
attitude object) presented before (Blaison et al. 2012). The Results
prime stimuli in the present study were images of tempting
foods (e.g., pizza, chocolate, cake), which have been shown The correlations between all other variables can be found
to be adequate in eliciting consumption responses (Bailey in Table  1.2 ASA and DSA were not significantly corre-
2014), and images of gray squares and neutral objects (e.g., lated, r(310) = 0.08, p = .17, suggesting that they were
buildings, trees) were used as controls. The inter-stimulus distinct and orthogonal constructs (Gable et  al. 2003).
interval (ISI) between the prime and the pictograph was ASA was positively correlated with BAS, r(310) = 0.15,
varied to be 100 and 1000  ms for each half of the stim- p = .008, and DSA was positively correlated with BIS,
uli respectively, as this ensured the spontaneity of each r(310) = 0.16, p = .005, consistent with the notion that
response (Davis 1970; Kornmeier et al. 2007). Participants’ ASA/DSA and BAS/BIS are conceptually related. The
proportion of “pleasant” responses to the pictographs fol- modest correlations also indicate that ASA/DSA and
lowing the food primes was taken to be their implicit atti- BAS/BIS are distinct constructs.
tude towards foods. Participants’ proportion of “pleasant”
responses to the pictographs following the gray and neutral
images was also measured. 2
  While a median-split approach have been used in the past to sepa-
Next, participants underwent a gift-choosing task as an rate participants into groups with high/low combinations of ASA/
explicit measure of participants’ behavioral intention to DSA (Lang et  al. 2005), the current results were analyzed and dis-
indulge or resist the temptation of unhealthy food. As a cussed in a regression approach. As we are focusing on the unique
contribution of ASA and DSA in predicting participants’ response to
cover story, participants were told that the experimenters temptations, maintaining them as continuous predictors should have
more validity (Irwin and McClelland 2003).

13
Motiv Emot (2017) 41:431–442 437

Impulsive desire et al. 2005, 2011), provide additional predictive utility over
and above measures related to behavioral regulation (i.e.,
In order to examine the unique effect of ASA and DSA on BIS/BAS, BSC) in people’s response towards temptation.
implicit attitudes toward the tempting food primes, a hier- We measured participants’ implicit attitudes towards
archical regression was performed. Participants’ ratings the temptation as an indication of their spontaneous affec-
for neutral and grey primes were first entered in Block 1, tive responses towards the tempting stimuli. As hypoth-
R2 = 0.22, F(2, 309) = 43.77, p < .001. Participants’ rat- esized, the individuals’ positivity offset, as captured by
ings of neutral primes positively predicted ratings of the ASA, positively predicted participants’ implicit attitudes
food primes, B = 0.18, t(309) = 8.55, p < .001, 95% CI towards tempting foods. At the default level of processing,
[0.137, 0.219], but the grey primes did not, B = −0.02, the hedonic gratification of indulgent foods (Loewenstein
t(309) = −0.39, p = .70, 95% CI [−0.114, 0.076]. In Block 1996) elicits a stronger appetitive response than the instru-
2, measures related to behavioral regulation: BIS, BAS, mental negativity (Ferguson and Bargh 2004; Fishbach
and trait self-control (BSC), were added, R2 change = 0.01, et al. 2010) does an aversive response. The more easily acti-
F(3, 306) = 1.39, p = .25. They did not yield any significant vated one’s appetitive over the aversive system is, the larger
effects, ps > 0.103. In Block 3, ASA and DSA were added, the positivity offset it generates, and the more positive the
R2 change = 0.055, F(2, 304) = 11.62, p < .001. Consistent person evaluates temptations. On the other hand, the indi-
with predictions, ASA positively predicted implicit atti- viduals’ negativity bias, captured by DSA, negatively pre-
tudes towards food primes, B = 0.03, t(304) = 4.09, p < .001, dicted participants’ implicit attitudes towards temptations.
95% CI [.014, 0.039]. Participants with higher affective As predicted, an easily activated aversive (than appetitive)
activation of the appetitive system found tempting food to system diminishes the appetitive responses towards tempt-
be more pleasant. DSA negatively predicted implicit atti- ing foods by activating an intense response to the instru-
tudes towards food primes, B = −0.02, t(304) = −2.91, mental negativity of unhealthy foods. The more activated
p = .004, 95% CI [−0.040, −0.008]. Participants with the aversive system is relative to the appetitive system,
higher affective activation of the aversive system found the greater the aversive response that is generated, and the
tempting food to be less pleasant. BIS, BAS, and BSC more negative the person views temptations. Thus, affective
measures remained non-significant, ps > 0.114. activation, involving the relative patterns of appetitive and
aversive activation, independently determine the strength of
Behavioral intent affective responses toward temptations.
More importantly, these relative patterns of activation
Hierarchical regression was also conducted on participants’ predicted implicit attitudes over and beyond the BIS/BAS
unhealthy food choices (M = 1.36, SD = 0.92). In Block and the BSC scale, testifying to the importance of measur-
1, the regression model containing BIS, BAS and BSC ing affective activation in addition to behavioral regulation.
achieved significance, R2 = 0.03, F(3, 307) = 2.89, p = .035. As predicted, the non-significant finding of the BIS/BAS
Specifically, BSC significantly predicted participants’ suggests that unlike the positivity offset and negativity
behavioral intention, B = −0.22, t(307) = −2.71, p = .007, bias, the BIS/BAS is less involved in the affective origin of
95% CI [−0.384, −0.061]. BIS and BAS did not yield any desires. This corroborates past research indicating its con-
significant effects, ps > 0.519. In Block 2, ASA and DSA ceptualization of and involvement in behavioral regulation
were added, R2 change = 0.001, F(2, 305) = 0.22, p = .80, instead (Carver and White 1994; Dholakia et al. 2006; Hof-
indicating that ASA and DSA offered little predictive util- mann et al. 2012). It concurs with the similarly non-signif-
ity with regard to participants’ behavioral intentions. Other icant findings for BSC, suggesting that unlike past research
than BSC, all predictors were non-significant, ps > 0.500. assessing desires in an explicit manner, implicit attitude
more purely captures the spontaneous affective responses
or instinctual desires toward temptations.
Discussion We also compared the predictive utility of consider-
ing affective activation with BIS/BAS and BSC towards
In this paper, we distinguish affective activation from explicit behavioral intention to indulge in temptation. We
behavioral regulation as a distinct way appetitive and aver- posited that MAM, which primarily indices spontaneous
sive systems regulate people’s responses. In accordance to affective activation, would have less predictive power over
past research, affective activation involves the relative acti- participants’ behavioral intention, compared to the BIS/
vation patterns of the two motivational systems, the posi- BAS and BSC. Our findings were consistent with expec-
tivity offset and negativity bias (Ito and Cacioppo 2005). tations in that individuals’ positivity offset and negativity
The key question is whether these relative patterns of acti- bias did not predict participants’ tempting food choices. On
vation, as measured by ASA and DSA in the MAM (Lang the other hand, BSC predicted their food choices, validating

13

438 Motiv Emot (2017) 41:431–442

the task as an indicator of participants’ behavioral intention presence of temptations. However, additional analyses
towards temptation. However, despite its theoretical rel- using the BAS subscales instead of the BAS composite
evance, the BIS/BAS was not significant in predicting regu- yielded no novel insights.3 Our present study design may
latory behavior towards temptations. not be sensitive enough to detect such differences, and
We offer possible explanations for why the BIS/BAS, certain BAS subscales may emerge important in a differ-
compared to BSC, was not predictive of the food choice ent context. Even so, it is clear from our findings that the
task. First, the BIS/BAS may not be as sensitive as BSC MAM remains the stronger predictor of implicit attitudes
when the temptation is not salient. A recent meta-analysis towards temptation beyond the BIS/BAS. Though the
suggests that BSC is uniquely related to long-term forma- diversity in BAS taps into different aspects of behavioural
tion of good habits (de Ridder et al. 2012), which may be activation, we reason that they still operate on a different
effectively activated even with minimal salience of the level than the MAM (behavioural and not affective acti-
temptation. The food choice task was fairly indirect in tap- vation). The associated impulsivity, motivations, and
ping into participants’ indulgence in tempting foods, as it neural correlates with these subscales, though operating
was designed to reduce demand characteristics regarding in an implicit manner, may still be directed more towards
the temptation. The cover story and filler options should regulating action than affect. Future research is encour-
have diluted the salience of tempting foods. Hence, tempt- aged to further differentiate the roles of the BAS sub-
ing food options might not have elicited strong responses scales in affective and behavioral regulation.
from the BIS/BAS. If the task had been framed such that Our paper demonstrates the predictive utility of indi-
participants were to make a choice using information pro- vidual differences in affective activation, as charted by
vided about the gifts, in this case, all the nutritional value the relative activation of the appetitive and aversive sys-
(etc., calories) of each food item, both the BSC and the tems. This has important implications. Past research has
behavioral regulatory measure of BIS/BAS may have focused on how implicit attitudes towards temptations,
emerged as significant predictors. Second, the present way once formed, are especially potent in guiding consump-
of indexing participants’ behavioral intention may be meth- tion behaviors (Hofmann et al. 2007, 2008). Less research
odologically suited to the BSC. The meta-analysis showed is devoted to what underlies implicit attitudes toward
that the self-control scale is particularly predictive when temptations in the first place. By showing that dieters’
behavior is measured in hypothetical terms and through implicit attitudes towards tempting foods are predicted by
self-report (de Ridder et  al. 2012). This is similar to the affective activation as fundamental as the appetitive and
current food choice task, where participants have to project aversive systems (Elliot and Covington 2001; Gable et al.
their gift preferences and list them down. 2003), we can begin to make novel predictions about
Although the BAS, as a unitary construct, was not pre- individuals’ reactions to temptations across domains. For
dictive of participants’ implicit attitudes and food choice, example, we can predict that individuals with an easily
we do not rule out the possibility that its subscales (drive, activated appetitive system relative to the aversive system
fun-seeking and reward responsiveness) may capture cer- (i.e. high trait positivity offset), are particularly prone to
tain aspects of behavioral regulatory activation toward experiencing strong desires for temptations. These strong
temptations. Past studies have employed BAS as a unitary desires would apply across multiple life domains such as
construct to investigate temptation and desires (Hoffman dieting, monetary spending, and indulging in leisure and
et  al. 2012; Zisserson and Palfai 2007) and we retained have behavioral consequences. Past research has shown
the same approach for the ease of comparison. However, that the more frequently people feel and inhibit unwanted
there can be meaningful distinctions between its sub- desires, the more likely they were to experience self-con-
scales (Voigt et al. 2009). Fun-seeking, for example, has trol failures in the later parts of the day (Hofmann et al.
been linked to impulsivity, while reward responsiveness 2012), perhaps due to a depletion of limited self-control
and drive have been tied to sensitivity for reward (Dawe resources (Baumeister et  al. 2007). An easily activated
et  al. 2004; Dawe and Loxton 2004; Franken and Muris appetitive system may thus dispose the individual to
2006b). In particular, the drive subscale has been shown exercise more self-control to handle the allure of tempta-
to correlate strongly with neural activity in response to tions and leave him/her too depleted to cope with other
pictures of appetitive food (Beaver et  al. 2006), which important tasks. On the other hand, individuals who are
suggests that it may be particularly sensitive to the

3
 Replacing BAS with its three subscales (drive, fun-seeking and
reward responsiveness) yielded the same pattern of results. Only ASA
(p < .001) and DSA (p = .004) predicted participants’ implicit atti-
tudes, and BSC (p = .006) predicted participants’ behavioral intent.
All other predictors were non-significant, ps > 0.185.

13
Motiv Emot (2017) 41:431–442 439

equipped with an easily activated aversive system relative influence implicit attitudes. This is in line with the view
to the appetitive system (i.e. high trait negativity bias) that while certain attitudes may be relatively stable and
may experience less unwanted desires. Without the need unchanging across contexts (Fazio 1995), many attitudes
to expand much effort to resist temptations, they could are constructed on the spot based on relevant input from
focus their cognitive resources on important pursuits in the surrounding circumstances (Schwarz 2007). Accord-
life. ing to the constructionist view, how the relative activa-
In addition, although previous research using fMRI has tion of the appetitive and aversive systems contribute to
examined the process underlying the desire for temptations, one’s construction of attitude toward temptations depends
they focused on the neural correlates of self-control fail- on the specific context.
ures as the basis of how people fall to temptation (Lopez This perspective sheds new light on how people’s
et al. 2014). The study found that greater reactivity of brain implicit attitude towards temptations is altered by situ-
regions associated with rewards predicted the strength and ational factors. For example, past research finds that when
enactment of desire, while activation of the brain region an abstract mindset is induced (Fujita et  al. 2006) or the
associated with self-regulation predicted less susceptibil- long-term goal is made salient (Fishbach et al. 2010), peo-
ity to the temptation. However, unlike the current study, ple’s implicit attitude towards temptations become more
their findings shed light on people’s differential response in negative. Our findings suggest that this common outcome
handling the desire, and not the origin of desire in the first of devalued temptations from the two situational factors
place. Hence, our findings offer unique contribution in that may work through different mechanisms. When an indi-
the MAM captures the trait-level reactivity of the appeti- vidual focuses on abstract, high-level concepts, the hedonic
tive and aversive systems that predispose individuals to the positivity of temptations may become less salient (Mischel
extent of desires they will feel, independent of their ability et al. 1996). Consequently, the temptations are less able to
to exert conscious self-control. activate the appetitive system to generate a large positiv-
Our research also contributes to existent literature, by ity offset, hence reducing the allure of temptations. On the
focusing on the conceptualization of the positivity offset other hand, a long-term goal (e.g., academic achievement)
and negativity bias as the relative activation of one motiva- may increase the salience of the negative instrumentality
tion system over another, rather than a linear relationship of temptations and upregulate the activation of the aversive
between each system and response. Past studies concerning system. The intense aversive response from the negativity
the neural correlates of BIS/BAS have discovered a consist- bias then undermines or overwhelms any initial appetitive
ent link of BAS with brain areas associated with appetitive response to reduce the overall attitude towards temptations.
responses and BIS with brain areas involved with aversive In sum, our findings highlight that the addition of affec-
responses (Balconi et  al. 2009; Herwig et  al. 2006; Reu- tive activation to behavioral regulation of the two motiva-
ter et  al. 2004), signalling a biological predisposition in tional systems provides a fuller understanding of people’s
approach and withdrawal behavioral activations. However, responses towards temptations. The relative activation pat-
these activations of specific brain structures in isolation do terns of the appetitive and aversive systems, in the form of
not shed light on how the systems interplay, and how the positivity offset and negativity bias, may allow better pre-
interaction guides one’s experience and response to temp- dictions on how desires originate, rather than based on the
tation. Our findings suggest that the relative activation of activation strength of each system alone. We suggest that
the motivational systems may be more relevant in under- a more inclusive and nuanced conceptualization of the
standing one’s affective response to temptations, as the appetitive and aversive systems is in order and future stud-
joint influence of the hedonic gratification and instrumental ies should consider including MAM as an index for affec-
negativity will activate both appetitive and aversive motiva- tive activation in addition to BIS/BAS measures in their
tions. Hence, the relative degree each motivational system investigations.
is activated will ultimately guide the individual’s eventual
affective response towards the temptation. This concept Funding  This research is partly supported by Grant R-581-000-
165-133 from National University of Singapore, awarded to the sec-
of relative activation has not been applied to temptation ond author.
in past research, nor captured in the traditional BIS/BAS
measure. Compliance with ethical standards 
We also encourage future research to explore the ways
in which situational factors modulate the relative activa- Conflict of interest  All authors declare that they have no conflict
tion of the appetitive and aversive systems. Although the of interest.
present paper focuses on individual differences in positiv- Ethical approval  All  procedures performed in studies involving
ity offset and negativity bias, we believe that situational human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of
factors can differentially activate the two systems to the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964

13

440 Motiv Emot (2017) 41:431–442

Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(2), 295–
standards. 305. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.023.
Davis, M. (1970). Effects of interstimulus interval length and vari-
ability on startle-response habituation in the rat. Journal of
Informed consent  Informed consent was obtained from all individ-
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 72(2), 177–192.
ual participants included in the two studies.
doi:10.1037/h0029472.
Dawe, S., Gullo, M. J., & Loxton, N. J. (2004). Reward drive and
rash impulsiveness as dimensions of impulsivity: Implications
for substance misuse. Addictive Behaviors, 29(7), 1389–1405.
doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.06.004.
References Dawe, S., & Loxton, N. J. (2004). The role of impulsivity in the
development of substance use and eating disorders. Neurosci-
Abraham, C., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Acting on intentions: The role of ence & Biobehavioral Reviews, 28(3), 343–351. doi:10.1016/j.
anticipated regret. British Journal of Social Psychology, 42(4), neubiorev.2004.03.007.
495–511. doi:10.1348/014466603322595248. de Ridder, D. T., Lensvelt-Mulders, G., Finkenauer, C., Stok, F.
Alhabash, S., Chiang, Y.-h., & Huang, K. (2014). MAM & U&G in M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2012). Taking stock of self-control a
Taiwan: Differences in the uses and gratifications of Facebook meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range
as a function of motivational reactivity. Computers in Human of behaviors. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(1),
Behavior, 35, 423–430. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.033. 76–99. doi:10.1177/1088868311418749.
Bailey, R. L. (2014). Processing food advertisements: Initial biologi- Dholakia, U. M., Gopinath, M., Bagozzi, R. P., & Nataraajan, R.
cal responses matter. Communication Monographs, 82(1), 163– (2006). The role of regulatory focus in the experience and self-
178. doi:10.1080/03637751.2014.971417. control of desire for temptations. Journal of Consumer Psychol-
Balconi, M., Brambilla, E., & Falbo, L. (2009). BIS/BAS, corti- ogy, 16(2), 163–175. doi:10.1207/s15327663jcp1602_7.
cal oscillations and coherence in response to emotional cues. Elliot, A. J., & Covington, M. V. (2001). Approach and avoidance
Brain Research Bulletin, 80(3), 151–157. doi:10.1016/j. motivation. Educational Psychology Review, 13(2), 73–92. doi:
brainresbull.2009.07.001. 10.1023/A:1009009018235.
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2000). The mind in the middle: A Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statisti-
practical guide to priming and automaticity research. In H. T. cal power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and
Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149–
social and personality psychology (pp.  253–285). New York: 1160. doi:10.3758/brm.41.4.1149.
Cambridge University Press. Fazio, R. H. (1995). Attitudes as object-evaluation associations:
Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. (1994). Losing Determinants, consequences, and correlates of attitude accessi-
control: How and why people fail at self-regulation. San Diego: bility. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength:
Academic Press. Antecedents and consequences (pp.  247–282). Hillsdale, NJ:
Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (Eds.). (2004). Handbook of self- Erlbaum.
regulation: Research, theory, and applications. New York: Guil- Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2014). The MODE model: Attitude-
ford Press. behavior processes as a function of motivation and opportunity.
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength In J. W. Sherman, B. Gawronski & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process
model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Sci- theories of the social mind (pp.  155–171). New York: Guilford
ence, 16(6), 351–355. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x. Press.
Beaver, J. D., Lawrence, A. D., van Ditzhuijzen, J., Davis, M. H., Ferguson, M. J., & Bargh, J. A. (2004). Liking is for doing:
Woods, A., & Calder, A. J. (2006). Individual differences The effects of goal pursuit on automatic evaluation. Jour-
in reward drive predict neural responses to images of food. nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(5), 557–572.
The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 5160–5166. doi:10.1523/ doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.557.
JNEUROSCI.0350-06.2006. Finn, P. R. (2002). Motivation, working memory, and decision mak-
Blaison, C., Imhoff, R., Hühnel, I., Hess, U., & Banse, R. (2012). ing: A cognitive-motivational theory of personality vulnerability
The affect misattribution procedure: Hot or not? Emotion, 12(2), to alcoholism. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews,
403–412. doi:10.1037/a0026907. 1(3), 183–205. doi:10.1177/1534582302001003001.
Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G. (1999). The affect Fishbach, A., Zhang, Y., & Trope, Y. (2010). Counteractive evalu-
system has parallel and integrative processing components form ation: Asymmetric shifts in the implicit value of conflicting
follows function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, motivations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(1),
76(5), 839–855. 29–38. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.09.008.
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavio- Franken, I. H. (2002). Behavioral approach system (BAS) sensitivity
ral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and predicts alcohol craving. Personality and Individual Differences,
punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and 32(2), 349–355. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00030-7.
Social Psychology, 67, 319–333. Franken, I. H., & Muris, P. (2006a). BIS/BAS personality char-
Caseras, X., Avila, C., & Torrubia, R. (2003). The measurement of acteristics and college students’ substance use. Personality
individual differences in behavioural inhibition and behavioural and Individual Differences, 40(7), 1497–1503. doi:10.1016/j.
activation systems: A comparison of personality scales. Person- paid.2005.12.005.
ality and Individual Differences, 34(6), 999–1013. doi:10.1016/ Franken, I. H., & Muris, P. (2006b). Gray’s impulsivity dimension:
S0191-8869(02)00084-3. A distinction between reward sensitivity versus rash impulsive-
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sci- ness. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 1337–1347.
ences (2nd edn.). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.016.
Cooper, A., Gomez, R., & Aucote, H. (2007). The Behavioural Inhi- Fujita, K. (2011). On conceptualizing self-control as more than the
bition System and Behavioural Approach System (BIS/BAS) effortful inhibition of impulses. Personality and Social Psychol-
Scales: Measurement and structural invariance across adults and ogy Review, 15(4), 352–366. doi:10.1177/1088868311411165.

13
Motiv Emot (2017) 41:431–442 441

Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Con- does MAM predict individual differences in physiologi-
strual levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social cal indicators of appetitive and aversive activation? Com-
Psychology, 90(3), 351–367. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.351. munication Methods and Measures, 1(2), 113–136.
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., & Elliot, A. J. (2003). Evidence for bivari- doi:10.1080/19312450701399370.
ate systems: An empirical test of appetition and aversion across Lang, A., Kurita, S., Rubenking, B., & Potter, R. F. (2011). Mini-
domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(5), 349–372. MAM: Validating a short version of the motivational action
doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00580-9. measure. Communication Methods and Measures, 5(2), 146–
Gray, J. A. (1987). The neuropsychology of emotion and personality. 162. doi:10.1080/19312458.2011.568377.
Cognitive Neurochemistry. 395xiv, 171–190. Lang, A., Shin, M., & Lee, S. (2005). Sensation seeking, motivation,
Gray, J. A. (1994). Personality dimensions and emotion systems. In P. and substance use: A dual system approach. Media Psychology,
Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Funda- 7(1), 1–29. doi:10.1207/s1532785xmep0701_1.
mental questions (pp.  329–331). New York: Oxford University Lang, A., & Yegiyan, N. (2011). Individual differences in motiva-
Press. tional activation influence responses to pictures of taboo prod-
Hewig, J., Hagemann, D., Seifert, J., Naumann, E., & Bartussek, D. ucts. Journal of Health Communication, 16(10), 1072–1087. doi:
(2006). The relation of cortical activity and BIS/BAS on the 10.1080/10810730.2011.571336.
trait level. Biological Psychology, 71(1), 42–53. doi:10.1016/j. Lang, P. J. (1995). The emotion probe: Studies of motiva-
biopsycho.2005.01.006. tion and attention. American Psychologist, 50(5), 372–385.
Hofmann, W., Baumeister, R. F., Förster, G., & Vohs, K. D. (2012). doi:10.1037/0003-066X.50.5.372.
Everyday temptations: an experience sampling study of desire, Lang, P. J., & Bradley, M. M. (2013). Appetitive and defensive moti-
conflict, and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- vation: Goal-directed or goal-determined? Emotions Review,
chology, 102(6), 1318–1335. doi:10.1037/a0026545. 5(3), 230–234. doi:10.1177/1754073913477511.
Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Friese, M., Wiers, R. W., & Schmitt, Lee, S., & Lang, A. (2009). Discrete emotion and motivation: relative
M. (2008). Working memory capacity and self-regulatory behav- activation in the appetitive and aversive motivational systems
ior: Toward an individual differences perspective on behavior as a function of anger, sadness, fear, and joy during televised
determination by automatic versus controlled processes. Jour- information campaigns. Media Psychology, 12(2), 148–170.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(4), 962–977. doi:10.1080/15213260902849927.
doi:10.1037/a0012705. Leone, L., Perugini, M., Bagozzi, R. P., Pierro, A., & Mannetti, L.
Hofmann, W., Kotabe, H., & Luhmann, M. (2013). The spoiled pleas- (2001). Construct validity and generalizability of the Carver–
ure of giving in to temptation. Motivation and Emotion, 37(4), White behavioural inhibition system/behavioural activation sys-
733–742. doi:10.1007/s11031-013-9355-4. tem scales. European Journal of Personality, 15(5), 373–390.
Hofmann, W., Rauch, W., & Gawronski, B. (2007). And deplete us doi:10.1002/per.415.
not into temptation: Automatic attitudes, dietary restraint, and Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behav-
self-regulatory resources as determinants of eating behavior. ior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(3), 497–504. 65(3), 272–292. doi:10.1006/obhd.1996.0028.
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.05.004. Lopez, R. B., Hofmann, W., Wagner, D. D., Kelley, W. M., & Heath-
Irwin, J. R., & McClelland, G. H. (2003). Negative consequences of erton, T. F. (2014). Neural predictors of giving in to tempta-
dichotomizing continuous predictor variables. Journal of Market- tion in daily life. Psychological Science, 25(7), 1337–1344.
ing Research, 40(3), 366–371. doi:10.1509/jmkr.40.3.366.19237. doi:10.1177/0956797614531492.
Ito, T. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2005). Variations on a human Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of
universal: Individual differences in positivity offset and delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological
negativity bias. Cognition and Emotion, 19(1), 1–26. Review, 106(1), 3. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.3.
doi:10.1080/02699930441000120. Mischel, W., Cantor, N., & Feldman, S. (1996). Principles of self-reg-
Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., Henderson, A. S., Jacomb, P. A., Korten, ulation: The nature of willpower and self-control. In E. T. Hig-
A. E., & Rodgers, B. (1998). Using the BIS/BAS scales to meas- gins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook
ure behavioural inhibition and behavioural activation: Factor of basic principles (pp. 329–360). New York: Guilford.
structure, validity and norms in a large community sample. Per- Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and
sonality and Individual Differences, 26(1), 49–58. doi:10.1016/ depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resem-
S0191-8869(98)00143-3. ble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 247–259.
Joseph, J. E., Liu, X., Jiang, Y., Lynam, D., & Kelly, T. H. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.126.2.247.
(2009). Neural correlates of emotional reactivity in sen- Patrick, V. M., Chun, H. H., & Macinnis, D. J. (2009). Affective fore-
sation seeking. Psychological Science, 20(2), 215–223. casting and self-control: Why anticipating pride wins over antici-
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02283.x. pating shame in a self-regulation context. Journal of Consumer
Kornmeier, J., Ehm, W., Bigalke, H., & Bach, M. (2007). Discontinu- Psychology, 19(3), 537–545. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2009.05.006.
ous presentation of ambiguous figures: How interstimulus-inter- Payne, B. K., Cheng, C. M., Govorun, O., & Stewart, B. D. (2005).
val durations affect reversal dynamics and ERPs. Psychophysiol- An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit meas-
ogy, 44(4), 552–560. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00525.x. urement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(3),
Kotabe, H. P., & Hofmann, W. (2015). On integrating the components 277–293. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.277.
of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(5), Potter, R. F., LaTour, M. S., Braun-LaTour, K. A., & Reichert, T.
618–638. doi:10.1177/1745691615593382. (2006). The impact of program context on motivational sys-
Krcmar, M., Farrar, K. M., Jalette, G., & McGloin, R. (2015). Appe- tem activation and subsequent effects on processing a fear
titive and defensive arousal in violent video games: Explaining appeal. Journal of Advertising, 35(3), 67–80. doi:10.2753/
individual differences in attraction to and effects of video games. joa0091-3367350305.
Media Psychology, 18(4), 527–550. doi:10.1080/15213269.2014 Potter, R. F., Lee, S., & Rubenking, B. E. (2011). Correlating a moti-
.888007. vation-activation measure with media preference. Journal of
Lang, A., Bradley, S. D., Sparks, J. V., & Lee, S. (2007). Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 55(3), 400–418. doi:10.1080/
The Motivation activation measure (MAM): How well 08838151.2011.597468.

13

442 Motiv Emot (2017) 41:431–442

Reuter, M., Stark, R., Hennig, J., Walter, B., Kirsch, P., Schienle, A., Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determi-
& Vaitl, D. (2004). Personality and emotion: Test of Gray’s per- nants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology
sonality theory by means of an fMRI study. Behavioral Neuro- Review, 8(3), 220–247. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1.
science, 118(3), 462–469. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.118.3.462. Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-
Rothman, A. J., Baldwin, A. S., Hertel, A. W., & Fuglestad, P. T. control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades,
(2011). Self-regulation and behavior change: Disentangling and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72(2), 271–
behavioral initiation and behavioral maintenance. In K. D. 324. doi:10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x.
Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Voigt, D. C., Dillard, J. P., Braddock, K. H., Anderson, J. W., Sopory,
Research, theory, and applications (2  edn., pp.  106–124). New P., & Stephenson, M. T. (2009). Carver and White’s (1994) BIS/
York: Guilford Press. BAS scales and riskyhealth behaviors. Personality and Individ-
Rubenking, B., & Lang, A. (2015). Appetitive and impulsive: Exam- ual Differences, 47, 89–93. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.02.003.
ining alcohol use via the motivational and self-control systems. Wang, Z., Lang, A., & Busemeyer, J. R. (2011). Motivational pro-
The Social Science Journal, 52(2), 258–265. doi:10.1016/j. cessing and choice behavior during television viewing: An inte-
soscij.2014.12.001. grative dynamic approach. Journal of Communication, 61(1),
Schönbrodt, F. D., & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do 71–93. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2010.01527.x.
correlations stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality, 47, Zisserson, R. N., & Palfai, T. P. (2007). Behavioral Activation Sys-
609–612. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2013.05.009. tem (BAS) sensitivity and reactivity to alcohol cues among
Schwarz, N. (2007). Attitude construction: Evaluation in con- hazardous drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 32(10), 2178–2186.
text. Social Cognition, 25(5), 638–656. doi:10.1521/ doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.02.016.
soco.2007.25.5.638.

13

You might also like