Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Energy Policy 115 (2018) 171–180

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Energy Policy
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/enpol

Exploring the effects of non-cognitive and emotional factors on household T


electricity saving behavior

Shanyong Wangb, Shoufu Lina,b, , Jun Lib
a
School of Economics, Fujian Normal University, Fuzhou, Fujian Province 350007, PR China
b
School of Management, University of Science and Technology of China, Hefei, Anhui Province, 230026, PR China

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Understanding and promoting household electricity saving behavior is vital to reduce electricity consumption
Theory of planned behavior and carbon emissions. The main purpose of this research is to analyze the effects of non-cognitive (personal
Electricity saving behavior moral norm and habit) and emotional factors (positive anticipated emotion) on household electricity saving
Personal moral norm behavior using a comprehensive model integrating the theory of planned behavior and the theory of inter-
Habit
personal behavior. The model is empirically tested using questionnaire survey data collected from 320 house-
Positive anticipated emotion
holds. The results indicate that personal moral norm, habit and positive anticipated emotion are important
determinants of residents’ intention to save electricity. Furthermore, it is find that habit is also positively as-
sociated with electricity saving behavior. However, positive anticipated emotion is negatively associated with
electricity saving behavior, which means that residents who present more positive anticipated emotion about
electricity conservation intend to save electricity in their daily lives but actually end up consuming more
electricity. In addition, the results also suggest that residents with positive attitude towards saving electricity and
greater senses of control have stronger intention to save electricity, while the role of subjective norm is not
significant. Based on these results, policy implications for research and practice and suggestions for further
research are discussed.

1. Introduction electricity consumption pattern and promoting electricity saving be-


havior by the year of 2020. Thus, considering the negative con-
With the rapid development of economy and the improvement of sequences and the large saving potential, several measures and relevant
living standards in the past decades, the electricity consumption in researches should be taken to reduce household electricity consumption
Chinese household sector is increasing in an incredible speed (Du et al., and promote electricity saving behavior.
2017; Ding et al., 2017). According to a report issued by National En- Currently, the measures and research efforts focus on household
ergy Administration of China,1 the average household electricity con- electricity consumption behavior can be divided into three major ca-
sumption in 2000 is only 232.8 kWh, but by 2015 it increased to tegories, namely economic oriented perspective, technological oriented
698.3 kWh in China. The annual growth rate is nearly about 7.60%. perspective and psychological behavior oriented perspective (Zhou and
There is no doubt that the household electricity consumption in China Yang, 2016; Arawomo, 2017; da Silva and Cerqueira, 2017). Economic
will continue increase in the next few years because the electricity oriented perspective mainly focuses on price-based measures and fi-
consumption and economic development exhibit a significant positive nancial incentive-based measures (Zhou and Yang, 2016). Theoreti-
causal nexus (Yuan et al., 2007). The increased household electricity cally, it seems that price-based measures such as multi-part tariffs and
consumption intensifies the energy crisis in China and brings huge peak-load pricing can reduce household electricity consumption dra-
number of carbon emissions and other toxic gases, which may damage matically and promote electricity saving behavior. However, in prac-
the environment and individuals’ health (Pothitou et al., 2016; Zhang tice, most studies have found that the effects of these measures is re-
et al., 2013). Meanwhile, it is worth noting that household electricity latively limited (Reiss and White, 2005; Zhou and Teng, 2013). This is
consumption has a large saving potential (Ouyang and Hokao, 2009). because that compared with other household consumption ex-
Murata et al. (2008) indicated that 28% reduction in household elec- penditures, the expenditure on electricity consumption is rather small
tricity consumption in China could be achieved through changing and electricity consumption usually exhibits low price elasticity (Zhou


Correspondence to: School of Economics, Fujian Normal University, No. 32, Shangsan Road, Fuzhou, Fujian Province 350007, PR China.
E-mail address: linshf2003@126.com (S. Lin).
1
See detail at http://data.stats.gov.cn/easyquery.htm?cn=C01.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.enpol.2018.01.012
Received 22 August 2017; Received in revised form 26 December 2017; Accepted 6 January 2018
0301-4215/ © 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
S. Wang et al. Energy Policy 115 (2018) 171–180

and Teng, 2013; Yu and Guo, 2016). Most households are insensitive to habit) and emotional factors (e.g., positive anticipated emotion) are
electricity price change and unwilling to save electricity to sacrifice incorporated into TPB model to better understand household electricity
their comfortable living style and welfare in their daily lives (Cheung saving behavior (Hassin et al., 2009; Webb et al., 2013; Fornara et al.,
et al., 2017). Financial incentive-based measures aim to provide sub- 2016). The main goals of this research are to explore whether these
sidies to households to compensate for their sacrifices (Handgraaf et al., factors and variables significantly affect household electricity saving
2013). Previous studies have suggested that financial incentive-based behavior and what measures can be taken to promote household elec-
measures can only be effective in a short period of time unless the tricity saving behavior.
measures are long lasting (Handgraaf et al., 2013; Frederiks et al., There are several theoretical and applied contributions of this re-
2015). Hence, financial incentive-based measures may not encourage search. Theoretically, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first time
electricity saving behavior effectively in the long run. Technological to incorporate non-cognitive factors and emotional factors together into
oriented perspective aims to improve electricity consumption efficiency TPB to understand household electricity saving behavior, which en-
and promote energy-efficient technology and appliance to reduce and riches the existing understanding of household electricity saving be-
save electricity consumption (Zhou and Yang, 2016). However, relying havior. Meanwhile, this research provides a paradigm for under-
on energy-efficient technology and appliance are not sufficient to re- standing household electricity saving behavior in pro-environmental
duce electricity consumption due to the ‘rebound effect2’ (Sorrell and research filed. This paradigm can help refine and strengthen future
Dimitropoulos, 2008; Georges et al., 2017). For example, Nilsson et al. research on electricity saving behavior. In addition, this research
(2014a) noted that the energy-efficient technology has improved highlights the importance of non-cognitive and emotional factors in
greatly and technical energy efficiency of appliance has improved promoting household electricity saving behavior and provides new di-
substantially over the past decades, while in the same period the rections for future research in pro-environmental behavior field. In
household electricity consumption has also increased. practice, based on the research findings, measures and intervention
Considering the limitations of economic oriented perspective and strategies can be taken to promote household electricity saving beha-
technological oriented perspective, scholars have been more and more vior.
recognized that psychological behavior oriented perspective is of great The remainder of this research is organized as follows. Section 2
significance in achieving electricity saving goals (Martinsson et al., focuses on literature review. Section 3 proposes the conceptual frame-
2011; Hori et al., 2013; De Leeuw et al., 2015; Bertoldo and Castro, work and hypotheses. Section 4 focuses on data and the research
2016; Shi et al., 2017). Psychological behavior oriented perspective method. Data analysis and results are presented in Section 5. In Section
aims to achieve household electricity conservation by underscoring 6, we discuss the results and implications. In Section 7, we conclude the
some psychological factors, such as attitude, social norms and en- research and address the research limitations.
vironmental awareness to promote electricity saving behavior
(Martinsson et al., 2011; Fornara et al., 2016; Zhou and Yang, 2016; 2. Literature review
Ding et al., 2017). In previous studies, scholars have paid attention to
psychological factors and many psychological behavior models and TPB is first proposed by Ajzen in 1991 and now it is the most
theories have been developed to understand household electricity popular theory to predict and explain individual's behavior in a wide
consumption behavior and explore the influencing factors (Barr et al., range of fields, especially in pro-environmental domain (Ajzen, 1991).
2005; De and Steg, 2009; Hori et al., 2013; Nilsson et al., 2014b; Klöckner (2013) noted that approximately 40% of all papers published
Fornara et al., 2016; Yu and Guo, 2016; Ding et al., 2017). Among these in environmental psychology domain have employed TPB as their basic
models and theories, theory of planned behavior (TPB) is widely used. theoretical framework. In fact, TPB has been successfully applied to
Rivis et al. (2009) indicated that TPB is probably the most influential explore household environmentally friendly behavior, such as energy
theory in explaining environmentally relevant behavior. In this re- conservation behavior, green purchasing behavior and other sustain-
search, we also try to adopt TPB to understand household electricity able consumption behavior (Kaiser and Scheuthle, 2003; Chen and
behavior from the psychological behavior oriented perspective. Tung, 2014; Wang et al., 2014; De Leeuw et al., 2015; Yadav and
In fact, in the pro-environmental behavior domain, theory of rea- Pathak, 2016).
soned action (TRA) and norm activation model (NAM) have also often Though TPB has received strong support in explaining en-
been used to study pro-environmental behavior, such as recycling be- vironmentally friendly behavior and own several advantages compared
havior, green buying behavior and energy conservation behavior with TRA and NAM, it also has several shortcomings and these short-
(Davies et al., 2002; Wahid et al., 2011). However, they have often been comings can be solved by adding additional factors and variables
questioned due to their own limitations (Shi et al., 2017). TRA has been (Bamberg, 2003; Gao et al., 2017). One of the shortcomings of TPB is
questioned since this theory only focuses on volitional control factors that as for the social norm, TPB overestimates the effect of it and under
and ignores the non-volitional factors, such as time, opportunities and represents the contribution of internal moral norm, particularly per-
resources (Lam and Hsu, 2004). NAM has been criticized since that it sonal moral norm (Webb et al., 2013; Cheung et al., 2017). Abrahamse
only focuses on internal factors and ignores the external factors, such as and Steg (2009) and Bertoldo and Castro (2016) argued that most pro-
social environment, time and resources (Shi et al., 2017). Compared environmental behaviors are motivated by personal moral norm rather
with TRA and NAM, TPB not only considers the non-volitional factors than social norm. Meanwhile, unlike other pro-environmental behavior
(e.g., perceived behavioral control) but also external factors (e.g., (e.g., green buying behavior or green travel behavior), household
subjective norm). Hence, it is appropriate to select TPB as the basic electricity saving behavior has less visibility to other people (e.g.,
theoretical framework in this research to understand household elec- friends, relatives or neighbors). Intuitively, it can be predicted that the
tricity saving behavior. In addition, several scholars have noted that effect of social norm on household electricity saving behavior is likely
additional psychological factors and variables can also be added to TPB to be of less importance.
to improve the model explanatory power (Kaiser and Scheuthle, 2003; Another shortcoming is that the variables in TPB are cognitive and
Shi et al., 2017; Gao et al., 2017). In this research, two additional rational predictors and TPB largely relies on the assumption that in-
factors, namely non-cognitive factors (e.g., personal moral norm and dividuals make rational choices (Abrahamse and Steg, 2009; Demarque
et al., 2015). As we know, to some degree, environmentally friendly
behavior cannot be just considered as a result of rational choice (Kals
2
Rebound effect refers to an increase in energy use efficiency by 1% will cause a re-
et al., 1999). Many behaviors are guided more by automatic, repeated
duction in energy consumption that is far below 1% or, sometimes, it can even cause an and positive affective processes, which means that some non-cognitive
increase in energy consumption. and emotional factors, such as personal moral norm, habit and emotion

172
S. Wang et al. Energy Policy 115 (2018) 171–180

should be considered (Steg and Vlek, 2009; Quested et al., 2013). predictor of household electricity saving behavior. The greater the in-
Klöckner and Blöbaum (2010) and Klöckner (2013) noted that habit or tention to save electricity, the higher the electricity saving behavior will
past behavior plays an important role in forming behaviors that are be performed. Thus, it is hypothesized that:
repeated over time, such as environmentally friendly behavior. Kals
H2:. Resident's intention to save electricity is positively related to
et al. (1999) noted that emotion about environmental issues is parti-
household electricity saving behavior.
cularly important in explaining environmentally friendly behavior. In
addition, the theory of interpersonal behavior also argues that emotion
is likely to play an important role in conservation behavior (Russell 3.2. Non-cognitive and emotional factors
et al., 2017). Failing to explore and explain the role of emotion hampers
the understanding of individuals’ pro-environmental behavior (Morris This research responds to the shortcomings of TPB and advances the
et al., 2002). research on household electricity saving behavior by examining both
In this research, non-cognitive factors are defined as individuals’ non-cognitive and emotional factors. Two non-cognitive factors (per-
patterns of thought and feelings towards household electricity saving sonal moral norm and habit) and one emotional factor (positive an-
behavior and the thought and feelings are depended on their own ticipated emotion) are added into TPB.
characteristics such as conscientiousness, skill and experience.
Considering that personal moral norm is originated from one's own 3.2.1. Personal moral norm
judgment and conscientiousness, habit is originated from one's past Personal moral norm refers to individuals perform a particular be-
experience, and they are all belong to the variables of personal char- havior based on their own moral responsibilities or obligations (Stern,
acteristics, this research regards personal moral norm and habit as non- 2000; Kaiser and Scheuthle, 2003; Bertoldo and Castro, 2016). In fact,
cognitive factors. Taking these reasons and viewpoints into account, personal moral norm is the core factor in norm activation model (Gao
this research tries to add non-cognitive factors (e.g., habit and personal et al., 2017; Shi et al., 2017). According to this model, individual's pro-
moral norm) and emotional factors into the basic theoretical framework environmental behavior is determined not only by social norms but also
to further and better understand household electricity saving behavior. personal moral norm. High personal moral norm can motivate in-
dividuals to perform pro-environment behavior, while low personal
3. Research hypotheses and conceptual framework moral norm can impede the behavior. A number of previous studies
have found that personal moral norm positively affects individual's pro-
3.1. Basic variables in TPB environmental behavior. For example, Bamberg et al. (2007) noted that
individuals with high personal moral norm are more likely to choose
TPB predicts that behavior is directly determined by behavioral public transportation for traffic than those with low personal moral
intention and behavioral intention is predicted by attitude towards the norm. Fornara et al. (2016) found that personal moral norm positively
behavior, subjective norm and perceived behavioral control (Yadav and affects household energy consumption behavior.
Pathak, 2016). Attitude refers to positive or negative evaluations about Similarity, in this research, it is reasonable to predict that residents
certain specific behavior (Gao et al., 2017). When individuals hold a with high personal moral norm will have more moral obligations and
positive attitude towards the behavior, their intentions to engage in a responsibilities to save electricity. Wasting electricity or not saving
specific behavior are increased and vice versa. Subjective norm refers to electricity will disobey their moral norms and obligations which may
perceptions of social pressures from significant others to perform or not lead them to feel guilty and uncomfortable. Taken together, it can be
perform the behavior (Cheung et al., 2017). Individuals tend to comply speculated that residents with high personal moral norm are more
with the expectations or viewpoints of these important people (e.g. likely to form intention to save electricity. Thus, it is hypothesized that:
relatives or friends). If individuals think that the important other people
H3:. Personal moral norm is positively related to resident's intention to
expect them to engage in a specific behavior, they are more likely to
save electricity.
perform the behavior. Perceived behavioral control refers to the per-
ceived ease or difficulty of performing the behavior (Fornara et al.,
2016). If individuals perceive that they are easy to perform the beha- 3.2.2. Habit
vior, they are more intended to perform the behavior (Shi et al., 2017). Psychological research increasing suggests that the behavior which
Consistent with TPB, in the context of electricity conservation, it can is repeated or habitual is more easily affected by past behavior or habit
be inferred that residents’ intention to save electricity will be increased (Hassin et al., 2009). When behavior is repeatedly performed, it re-
when residents hold a positive attitude towards saving electricity, if quires less deliberate consideration so that it becomes habit (Russell
residents think that important people (e.g., friends or neighbors) expect et al., 2017). According to Verplanken and Holland (2002) and Russell
them to save electricity, and if residents perceive that they have an et al. (2017), habit refers to relatively stable behavioral patterns, which
adequate level of control, such as ability, time, skills and opportunities can be triggered by past behavior and executed without rational eva-
to be able to perform the electricity saving behavior. In this research, luation. Habit is important and once individuals form the habit, they
attitude, subjective norm and perceived behavioral control is con- will be more likely to perform the behavior.
sidered as the positive predictors of residents’ intention to save elec- In relation to electricity conservation, we argue that electricity
tricity. Hence, we hypothesize that: saving behavior is likely to have a strong habitual element. Electricity
saving behavior is usually performed automatically and unconsciously.
H1a:. Attitude towards saving electricity is positively related to
Given that the electricity saving behavior is a repeated and habitual
resident's intention to save electricity.
behavior, we therefore consider that residents’ electricity saving habit
H1b:. Subjective norm is positively related to resident's intention to will play an important role in saving electricity. In daily lives, once the
save electricity. residents own the electricity saving habit, they are more intended to
save electricity and perform the electricity saving behavior naturally
H1c:. Perceived behavioral control is positively related to resident's
and continually. Hence, we expect that the greater electricity saving
intention to save electricity.
habit residents own, the more likely they are willing to save electricity
Based on the same logic, in line with TPB, we also predict that re- and report high levels of current electricity saving behavior. The hy-
sident's intention to save electricity is a significant and positive potheses are:

173
S. Wang et al. Energy Policy 115 (2018) 171–180

H4a. : Habit is positively related to resident's intention to save permanent resident population in Hefei is more than 7 million. Along
electricity. with the population growth and the improvement of residents’ living
standards, the household electricity consumption has increased sharply.
H4b. : Habit is positively related to household electricity saving
For example, the total household electricity consumption in 2015 is
behavior.
2.95 billion kWh, which has increased by 10% over 2014.3 In addition,
the electricity resource is insufficient in Hefei and it needs other cities’
3.2.3. Emotion electricity support. Hence, it is urgent and necessary for Hefei to mo-
Emotion refers to the feelings (e.g., positive or negative) or reac- tivate household to save electricity in their daily lives.
tions to an event or issue (Russell et al., 2017). An individual is likely to In general, it is difficult for researchers to collect data directly from
engage in an event or issue when he presents a positive anticipated households in China because many residents are reluctant to cooperate
emotion on this event or issue, otherwise he is unwilling to engage in. with the research team. Thus, to conduct the survey smoothly and ea-
In this research, positive anticipated emotion refers to the positive sily, the research team sough assistance from the local neighborhood
psychological states when performing a certain behavior (e.g., house- committee, which is the community management organization of re-
hold electricity saving behavior). Positive anticipated emotion consists sidential quarter in China (Wang et al., 2011). To obtain a re-
of expected feelings of pride, exciting and confidence (Han and Hyun, presentative sample, the research team randomly selected 30 re-
2018). Han and Hyun (2018) stressed that positive anticipated emotion sidential quarters in Hefei to conduct the survey. The research team
firstly signals the importance and significance of an event or issue and sent 30 copies of printed questionnaires to the contact person of each
then provides stimulus for performing the behavior. According to the neighborhood committee, and then the contact person random selected
theory of interpersonal behavior, it is known that positive anticipated 30 households in residential quarter and distributed these ques-
emotion plays an important role in performing environmentally tionnaires to the householders. The questionnaire is consisted of 5 se-
friendly behavior, particularly conservation behavior (Russell et al., parate modules: demographic information (e.g., family size, educa-
2017). Several past researches have shown the relationship between tional level and age), three basic constructs of TPB (attitude towards
positive anticipated emotion and environmentally friendly behavior. saving electricity, subjective norm and perceived behavioral control),
Bamberg et al. (2007) observed that individuals are willing to take three additional constructs (personal moral norm, habit and positive
public transportation for traffic when they present positive anticipated anticipated emotion), intention to save electricity, and electricity
emotion about this behavior. Rivis et al. (2009) conducted a meta- saving behavior.
analysis and found that positive anticipated emotion is positively re- Moreover, some scholars have noted that relies on cross-section
lated to individuals’ intention to perform environmentally friendly be- design may lead to common method bias which can inflate the re-
havior. Webb et al. (2013) showed that positive anticipated emotion lationships between constructs (Podsakoff et al., 2003). By measuring
has a positive effect on intention to engage in energy saving behavior. the independent and dependent constructs at different points in time
These studies have identified positive anticipated emotion as a de- can overcome this limitation. Following this logic, a four-stage ques-
terminant of intention to perform environmentally friendly behavior. tionnaire survey is conducted. In the first stage (conducted in August
Based on the same logic, in the context of electricity conservation, it can 2016), the contact person asked the participants to answer modules 1
be predicted that when residents think that saving electricity in their and 2. In the second stage (conducted in September 2016), participants
daily lives is beneficial to the environment and they present a positive are required to answer model 3. In the third (conducted in October
anticipated emotion about saving electricity, they are more intended to 2016) and fourth stage (conducted in November 2016), participants are
save electricity. In addition, according to affective events theory, we required to answer modules 4 and 5 separately. Participants are in-
know that positive anticipated emotion is a direct predictor of actual cluded in the sample if they respond to all four-stage surveys.
behavior because it provides a motivational impetus for action (Weiss Furthermore, to avoid data bias, participants are told that their re-
and Beal, 2005; Han and Hyun, 2018). Consistent with affective events sponses are strictly anonymity and confidentiality and never be leaked
theory, this research therefore postulates that positive anticipated out, and there is no right or wrong answers. To improve the response
emotion has a direct effect on household electricity saving behavior and rate, participants are told that they can obtain 40 RMB as remuneration
we expect that the more positive anticipated emotion residents present upon finishing the questionnaire survey. In total, 492 finished ques-
the more likely they perform the actual electricity saving behavior. tionnaires are received. The research team reviewed the questionnaires
Given these points of view, we hypothesize that: and discarded the incomplete and missing data questionnaires. Finally,
320 useable questionnaires are obtained, with a response rate of
H5a:. Positive anticipated emotion is positively related to resident's 35.56% (320/900). The demographic profiles of participants are pre-
intention to save electricity. sented in Table 1. As shown in Table 1, among the participants, nearly
H5b:. Positive anticipated emotion is positively related to household half (53.44%) of the participants are female and 60% are aged between
electricity saving behavior. 25 and 45 years old. The participants are relatively well-educated, and
70% earns an associate degree or a bachelor's degree. About 48.44%
Based on above analysis, the research framework is depicted in earns a monthly income ranging from 4000RMB to 8000RMB, and
Fig. 1. 35.31% earns a monthly income ranging from 8001RMB to 12000RMB.
More than half (60.31%) of the participants have medium family size
4. Data and research method within 3–4 members.
In addition, to test the potential non-response bias, T-tests are
4.1. Sample and data collection conducted to compare the early responses (those who returned the
completed questionnaires within 10 days) and late responses (those
The research hypotheses are tested using questionnaire survey data who returned the completed questionnaires during the last 10 days)
collected from residents in Hefei, China from mid-July to mid- (Armstrong and Overton, 1977). The results show no significant dif-
December of 2016. Hefei is the provincial capital of Anhui province and ferences between these responses at 0.05 level. Thus, it can be con-
is also the political, economic and cultural center of the province. With cluded that non-response bias is not a major problem in this research.
the reform and opening up policy, the speed of economy development is
amazing. According to “The Economist”, Hefei is the fastest growing
cities in China in 2015. With the development of economy, the human
3
population has increased rapidly. At the end of 2015, the size of the See detail at http://www.ahtjj.gov.cn/tjj/web/index.jsp.

174
S. Wang et al. Energy Policy 115 (2018) 171–180

Fig. 1. Research framework of household electricity


saving behavior.
Habit
Habit
Attitude

H1a H4a H4b

Subjective norm
Subjective norm
H1b
Intention to save Electricity
Intention to save H2 Electricity
electricity saving behavior
electricity saving behavior
H1c
Perceived behavioral
Perceived behavioral
control
control H5b
H5a
H3

Personal moral norm Positive anticipate


Personal moral norm Positive anticipate
emotion
emotion

Table 1 adapted from Nilsson et al. (2014a) and Cheung et al. (2017).
Demographic profiles of participants. Considering the survey is conducted in China, we firstly developed
an English version questionnaire and then translated it into Chinese
Demographics Frequencies Percentages (%)
version. In addition, to verify and improve the adapted survey items, a
Gender pilot survey is conducted. Based on the feedbacks, several modifications
0. Female 171 53.44% and refinements of the items are made such as deleting some items,
1. Male 149 46.56% changing wordings of items and adjusting the format of the ques-
Age
1. Under 25 51 15.94%
tionnaire.
2.25–35 111 34.69%
3.36–45 83 25.94%
4.46–55 46 14.38% 4.3. Descriptive statistics
5.56 and over 29 9.06%
Educational level
1. High school or below 20 6.25% The results of descriptive statistics for all the constructs are shown
2. Associate degree 125 39.06% in Table 3. The mean of construct reflects the level of participants’
3. Bachelor's degree 102 31.88% perception to the constructs studied (see Fig. 1). The participants show
4. Master's degree 58 18.13% higher levels of subjective norm (Mean = 3.96), habit (Mean = 3.75),
5. PhD 15 4.69%
Monthly income
perceived behavioral control (Mean = 3.63), personal moral norm
1. Less than4000 RMB 20 6.25% (Mean = 3.60), attitude towards saving electricity (Mean = 3.51) and
2.4000–8000 RMB 155 48.44% electricity saving behavior (Mean = 3.51). The levels of positive an-
3.8001–12000 RMB 113 35.31% ticipated emotion (Mean = 3.16) and intention to save electricity
4. More than12000 RMB 32 10.00%
(Mean = 3.01) are relatively smaller and near to the midpoint of the
Family size
1.1–2 79 24.69% scale (namely 3). In addition, as presented in Table 3, there exist as-
2. 3–4 193 60.31% sociations between each of the construct and all the correlations among
3. More than 4 48 15.00% the constructs are lower than 0.60, which means that the multi-
Total 320 100 collinearity is not a serious problem and it is appropriate to conduct
further analysis (Gao et al., 2017).
4.2. Measures

4.4. Data analysis method


Constructs in this research are latent variables and they are mea-
sured with multiple item scales. All the items are developed from prior
Structural equation modeling (SEM) technique is a commonly used
research papers. Some of the wordings of the items are slightly modified
method to test the models with latent variables (Chin, 1998). Thus, in
to fit the context of this research. The items are measured on a five-
this research, SEM technique is adopted to analyze the collected data.
point Likert scale ranging from 1 (“strongly disagree”) to 5 (“strongly
There are two kinds of models in SEM analysis, namely measurement
agree”). The constructs and items are shown in Table A1.
model and structural model (Anderson and Gerbing, 1988). The mea-
The measurement items of attitude towards saving electricity, sub-
surement model is used to test the relationships between latent vari-
jective norm, perceived behavioral control and intention to save elec-
ables and the corresponding items, and the structural model is used to
tricity are developed based on Wang et al. (2014), Cheung et al. (2017)
examine the relationships between these latent variables.
and Ding et al. (2017). The measurement items of electricity saving
In the following data analysis, Amos 22.0 software packages are
behavior are developed based on Barr et al. (2005) and Hori et al.
used to estimate the measurement model to assess the reliability and
(2013). The measurement items of personal moral norm are adapted
validity of the constructs, and then assess the structural model to test
from Bamberg et al. (2007) and Fornara et al. (2016). The measurement
the relationships among the hypothesized constructs.
items of habit are adapted from Zhang et al. (2013) and Russell et al.
(2017). The measurement items of positive anticipated emotion are

175
S. Wang et al. Energy Policy 115 (2018) 171–180

Table 2
Results of confirmatory factor analysis.

Construct Item Standardized factor Loading Cronbach's alpha Composite reliability AVE

Attitude (AT) AT1 0.86*** 0.88 0.93 0.81


AT2 0.90***
AT3 0.93***
Subjective norm (SN) SN1 0.81*** 0.80 0.88 0.71
SN2 0.85***
SN3 0.86***
Perceived behavioral control (PBC) PBC1 0.84*** 0.79 0.87 0.70
PBC2 0.85***
PBC3 0.82***
Personal moral norm (PMN) PMN1 0.85*** 0.83 0.90 0.74
PMN2 0.88***
PMN3 0.86***
Habit (HAT) HAT1 0.82*** 0.83 0.89 0.74
HAT2 0.85***
HAT3 0.90***
Positive anticipated emotion (EMO) EMO1 0.92*** 0.83 0.89 0.74
EMO2 0.93***
EMO3 0.71***
Intention to save electricity (INT) INT1 0.85*** 0.80 0.88 0.71
INT2 0.85***
INT3 0.83***
Electricity saving behavior (BEH) BEH1 0.83*** 0.85 0.91 0.77
BEH2 0.90***
BEH3 0.90***

Note: Significant at *** p < 0.001.

5. Data analysis and results AVE values range from 0.70 to 0.81, both of which exceed the bench-
mark value of 0.50 (Fornell and Larcker, 1981). These results provide a
5.1. Measurement model analysis strong support for unidimensionality and convergent validity.
Furthermore, we need to test the discriminant validity of the con-
Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) is performed to examine each structs. According to Paulraj et al. (2008), the relationships between
construct's measurement properties such as reliability, uni- shared variances among constructs and AVE values are compared. As
dimensionality, convergent validity and discriminant validity. The CFA shown in Table 3, all the correlations between constructs are less than
results indicate an acceptable fit between the measurement model and the square roots of AVE values, which support the discriminant validity
the data set. The model fit indices are listed as follows: the ration of Chi- of the constructs. Therefore, it can be concluded that the reliability and
square to degree of freedom (χ2/df) is 1.69, Comparative fit index (CFI) validity of the latent constructs are acceptable.
is 0.96, Incremental fit index (IFI) is 0.96, Normed fit index (NFI) is
0.91, Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) is 0.94, Goodness of fit index (GFI) is
0.93, and Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) is 0.04. 5.2. Structural model and hypothesis analysis
In general, Cronbach's alpha values and composite reliability values
are used to assess the reliability of the constructs (Fornell and Larcker, Though a four-stage questionnaire survey is conducted to alleviate
1981). As shown in Table 2, all Cronbach's alpha values range from the common method bias, it is necessary to further assess the common
0.79 to 0.88 and composite reliability values range from 0.87 to 0.93. method bias in structural model analysis. According to Harman (1976),
They are all higher than the recommended threshold value of 0.70 and Harman's one-factor test is performed. The results indicate that all the
support for reliability (Fornell and Larcker, 1981). To evaluate the measurement items could be divided into seven factors, with eigenva-
convergent validity of the constructs, individual item loading and lues greater than 1.0 and accounting for 71.80% of the variance. The
average variance extracted (AVE) are used (Fornell and Larcker, 1981). first factor only explains 23.33% of the variance and less than the
As shown in Table 2, the CFA results indicate that all items are sig- benchmark value of 50.0% (Harman, 1976). The results indicate that
nificantly loaded on the constructs. The loadings are significant at the common method bias is not a major concern in this research.
p < 0.001 level and greater than the threshold value of 0.70. All the In order to test the proposed research hypotheses, the structural
model is analyzed. Overall, the model fit for the structural model is

Table 3
Means, Standard Deviations and Correlations (n = 320).

Construct Mean SD AT SN PBC PMN HAT EMO INT BEH

AT 3.51 0.78 0.90


SN 3.96 0.75 0.29** 0.84
PBC 3.63 0.74 0.19** 0.48** 0.84
PMN 3.60 0.78 0.07 0.08 0.09 0.86
HAT 3.75 0.68 0.03 0.06 0.02 0.11* 0.86
EMO 3.16 0.65 −0.06 −0.06 0.04 0.06 0.01 0.86
INT 3.01 0.86 0.24** 0.30** 0.45** 0.21** 0.11 0.11 0.84
BEH 3.51 0.88 0.09 0.34** 0.55** 0.18** 0.19** 0.04 0.53** 0.88

Note: (1) The diagonal (bold) elements are the square roots of AVE values and the off-diagonal elements are the correlations among the constructs. (2) Significant at * p < 0.05 and **
p < 0.01.

176
S. Wang et al. Energy Policy 115 (2018) 171–180

Fig. 2. Results of hypothesis testing.

Habit
Habit
Attitude

0.15** 0.07* 0.14**

Subjective norm
Subjective norm
0.08
Intention to save Electricity
Intention to save 0.53*** Electricity
electricity saving behavior
electricity saving behavior
0.14**
Perceived behavioral
Perceived behavioral
control R2=0.26 R2=0.30
control -0.03*
0.11*
0.37***

Personal moral norm Positive anticipate


Personal moral norm Positive anticipate
emotion
emotion

good (χ2/df = 1.84, CFI = 0.94, IFI = 0.95, NFI = 0.91, TLI = 0.93, behavior and recycling behavior (Klöckner, 2013; Webb et al., 2013;
GFI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.05). The model explains 26% of the variance Cheung et al., 2017; Russell et al., 2017). However, contrary to our
in intention to save electricity and 30% of the variance in electricity expectations, positive anticipated emotion is negatively associated with
saving behavior. Fig. 2 presents the results of hypothesis testing. The electricity saving behavior. This is the inverse of the expected re-
results suggest that, in line with TPB, attitude towards saving electricity lationship and indicates that the more positive anticipated emotion
(β = 0.15, p < 0.01) and perceived behavioral control (β = 0.14, residents present, the less electricity saving behavior they will perform
p < 0.01) are significantly related to intention to save electricity, thus in practice. This is particularly surprising because the results also de-
providing support for Hypotheses 1a and 1c. The path coefficient from monstrate that positive anticipated emotion has a positive effect on
subjective norm to intention to save electricity is not significant (β = intention to save electricity. These results suggest that positive antici-
0.08, p > 0.05), thus Hypothesis 1b is not supported. Consistent with pated emotion does not translate into actual electricity saving behavior,
TPB, intention to save electricity is significantly and positively related despite residents intend to do so.
to electricity saving behavior (β = 0.53, p < 0.001), thus providing These findings are confusing but interesting to explain. The reasons
support for Hypothesis 2. can be explained as follows. As we know, some scholars such as Chen
Hypothesis 3 posits that personal moral norm positively affects the and Tung (2014), Wang et al. (2014), Russell et al. (2017) and Gao
intention to save electricity. According to Fig. 2, it can be observed that et al. (2017) have noted that there exit difference between behavioral
the path coefficient is 0.37 (p < 0.001), thus supporting Hypothesis 3. intention and actual behavior. Behavioral intention means that the
Hypotheses 4a and 4b discuss the relationships between habit and in- specific behavior will occur sometime in the future, while the actual
tention to save electricity (4a) and electricity saving behavior (4b). The behavior means that the specific behavior will occur immediately or has
results in Fig. 2 suggest that habit positively affects the intention to save already occurred. According to Lerner and Keltner (2001), the beha-
electricity (β = 0.07, p < 0.05) and electricity saving behavior (β = vioral responses of positive anticipated emotion are quite different.
0.14, p < 0.01), thus, supporting Hypotheses 4a and 4b. Hypotheses 5a Individuals are more likely to state that they are willing to perform the
and 5b posit that positive anticipated emotion positively affects inten- behavior that occurs in the future and reluctant to perform the behavior
tion to save electricity (5a) and electricity saving behavior (5b). Ac- that occurs immediately. Based on the same logic, in this research, it is
cording to Fig. 2, Hypothesis 5a is supported (β = 0.11, p < 0.05) and known that residents’ intention to save electricity occurs in the future
Hypothesis 5b is not supported (β = −0.03, p < 0.05). The results and therefore the positive anticipated emotion can lead to strong and
suggest that the experience of more positive anticipated emotion is positive intention. However, considering electricity saving behavior is
associated with higher intention to save electricity but contrary to our imminent, residents may unwilling to sacrifice their comfortable living
predictions, is associated with lower actual electricity saving behavior. style to perform the electricity saving behavior at once. What’ more,
some residents may have avoidance-oriented beliefs and think that they
can save electricity in the future so not perform the electricity saving
6. Discussion and implication
behavior right now is forgivable. Thus, the positive anticipated emotion
can lead to reduction in performing actual electricity saving behavior.
The present research uses a comprehensive model which in-
Another reason for why this result differs from other studies’ results is
corporates not only traditional cognitive factors (attitude, subjective
due to the difference in data collection process. Other studies such as
norm and perceived behavioral control) but also the neglected non-
Webb et al. (2013) and Han and Hyun (2018) rely on cross-section
cognitive and emotional factors (personal moral norm, habit and po-
design and collect data at the same point in time. Under this situation,
sitive anticipated emotion) to understand household electricity saving
participants tend to give the same answers when they evaluate inten-
behavior. This research highlights the importance of non-cognitive and
tion to save electricity and actual electricity saving behavior simulta-
emotional factors as drivers of electricity saving behavior.
neously. However, in this research, a four-stage questionnaire survey is
The results suggest that personal moral norm, habit and positive
conducted and the participants evaluate intention to save electricity
anticipated emotion are positively and significantly related to residents’
and actual electricity saving behavior at different points in time. Par-
intention to save electricity. Meanwhile, we find that habit is also po-
ticipants’ perceptions to these two constructs are quite different. Thus,
sitively associated with electricity saving behavior. These results are
positive anticipated emotion has different effects on intention to save
consistent with previous studies in understanding pro-environmental
electricity and actual electricity saving behavior.
behavior such as energy conservation behavior, green purchasing

177
S. Wang et al. Energy Policy 115 (2018) 171–180

Furthermore, consistent with TPB, attitude and perceived beha- household electricity saving behavior. A four-staged questionnaire
vioral control are positively related to intention to save electricity. survey is conducted to collect data from 320 households in Hefei,
However, the effect of subjective norm on intention to save electricity is China. SEM is adopted to analyze the collected data and test the re-
not significant, which means that important others (e.g., relatives or search hypotheses. The results indicate that attitude, perceived beha-
neighbors) do not influence residents’ intention to save electricity. This vioral control, personal moral norm, habit and positive anticipated
result is inconsistent with previous studies (Yadav and Pathak, 2016; emotion are significantly related to intention to save electricity, while
Fornara et al., 2016; Cheung et al., 2017; Shi et al., 2017). One rea- subjective norm has no significant effect on intention to save electricity.
sonable explanation is that the household electricity saving behavior is Meanwhile, it is worth noting that positive anticipated emotion is ne-
almost invisible to friends, relatives or neighbors. Residents’ electricity gatively associated with actual electricity saving behavior. Our findings
saving behavior almost relies on themselves and is not affected by underscore the importance of non-cognitive and emotional factors as
others. Hence, the effect of subjective norm is not significant. In addi- drivers of electricity saving behavior and suggest that these non-cog-
tion, it is worth noting that personal moral nom has the largest impact nitive and emotional drivers deserve much more attention. This re-
on intention to save electricity, which means that residents with high search adds to our understanding of household electricity saving be-
levels of personal moral norm are more inclined to save electricity. This havior and provides important implications for future researchers and
result is consistent with previous studies, which reported that the per- practitioners.
sonal moral norm is associated with a stronger preference for per- Although this research offers some interesting findings and makes
forming pro-environmental behavior (De Groot and Steg, 2009; Webb significant theoretical and practical implications, it has several limita-
et al., 2013; Fornara et al., 2016). tions which must be acknowledged and possibly overcome in future
The findings of this research have several implications for scholars research. Firstly, the data are only collected from one city, namely
and practitioners. Theoretically, this research enriches and contributes Hefei, China, which may restrict the generalizability of results. Though
to household electricity saving literatures by proposing a comprehen- Hefei shares some common characteristics with other areas and regions,
sive model which incorporates cognitive, non-cognitive and emotional it also has some characteristics of its own. Since the 21st century, along
factors. Previous research neglects to explore the role of non-cognitive with the deepening of political and economic system reforms, the speed
and emotional factors in household electricity saving (Cheung et al., of economy development in Hefei is amazing. According to “The
2017; Gao et al., 2017). This research is a timely response and under- Economist”, Hefei is the fastest growing cities in China in 2015.
scores the importance of non-cognitive and emotional factors. Fur- Meanwhile, the human population has also increased rapidly. At the
thermore, this research suggests that behavioral intention cannot be end of 2015, the size of the permanent resident population in Hefei is
considered as a proxy measure for behavior. Indeed, if we only measure more than 7 million. With the fast development of economy and the
behavioral intention it will draw the conclusion that the positive an- population increase, and with the improvement of residents’ living
ticipated emotion is an effective way for future interventions designed standards, the household electricity consumption has increased sharply.
to save electricity. However, in fact, this research presents a negative In addition, the natural resource endowment is poor in Hefei and the
relationship between positive anticipated emotion and the self-reported electricity resource is insufficient and it needs other cities’ electricity
behavior. Thus, in the following studies, scholars should be careful to support. Electricity saving has drawn wide attention and residents’
conflate behavioral intention and behavior. In addition, a four-stage environmental awareness is relatively high and they are planning to
questionnaire survey is conducted in this research to collect data at saving electricity in their daily lives. The findings may be effective in
different time points to reduce the common method bias. The ques- specific contexts and may not be suitable to other contexts. In general,
tionnaire survey method in this research is beneficial and referential to the findings of this research are suitable for areas which have the si-
scholars in their following studies. milar characteristics with Hefei, such as economic development level,
In practice, based on the research findings, this research also pro- institutional and economic environment, natural resource endowment,
vides important implications for saving electricity. Firstly, considering population, and residents’ environmental awareness. The findings of
the importance of personal moral norm and positive anticipated emo- this research are not suitable for other areas which have different
tion, national and local education and publicity campaigns about characteristics with Hefei. Thus, in future research, we should attempt
household electricity saving can be set up for residents (Gao et al., to collect data from more areas and regions to make the findings more
2017). These education and publicity campaigns can make residents general and universal. Secondly, this research relies merely on self-re-
realize that every citizen has the responsibility and obligation to reduce ported electricity saving behavior. Participants are inclined to over-
electricity consumption and save electricity which is beneficial to im- estimate their electricity saving behavior and thus affect the accuracy of
prove their personal moral norm (Webb et al., 2013). Meanwhile, re- research conclusions. In the following studies, scholars can conduct
levant government agencies can provide certain reports to residents to observations to measure electricity saving behavior. Finally, this re-
highlight the negative consequences of wasting electricity (e.g., in- search only considers the effects of psychological factors on household
tensifying energy crisis and increasing carbon emissions) and explain electricity saving behavior. Other factors such as information feedback
the social benefits of saving electricity to foster their positive antici- and financial incentive scheme are not considered. This is what we set
pated emotion about electricity conservation (Fornara et al., 2016). out to do in future research.
Secondly, considering the importance of perceived behavioral control
and habit, electricity saving manuals can be provided to residents and
electricity saving lectures and training programs can be also launched Acknowledgements
by residential committees (Shi et al., 2017). These measures can en-
hance residents’ skills and knowledge to save electricity and thus to This work was supported by the National Natural Science
improve their perceived behavioral control. In addition, these measures Foundation of China (Grant number 71601174 and 71571172), China
can also make residents realize that it is not difficult to save electricity Postdoctoral Science Foundation (Grant number 2016M590583),
when they follow the guidelines and help them to develop a good habit Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities, National
to save electricity. Social Science Foundation of China (Grant number 16CJL020) and
Fujian Fund of Soft Science Research (Grant number 2017R0034).
7. Conclusions and limitations

In this research, a comprehensive model is developed to explain

178
S. Wang et al. Energy Policy 115 (2018) 171–180

Appendix A

See Appendix Table A1

Table A1
Constructs and measurement items.

Constructs Measurement items

Attitude Saving electricity in my home is significant to reduce carbon emissions.


Saving electricity in my home is valuable to improve the air quality.
Saving electricity in my home is a wise move.
Subjective norm My neighbors think that I should save electricity in daily lives.
My relatives think that I should save electricity in daily lives.
Other people who are important to me think I should save electricity in daily lives.
Perceived behavioral control I believe that I am capable of saving electricity in my home.
I have knowledge and skills to save electricity in my home.
It is mostly up to me whether I save electricity in my home.
Personal moral norm Waste electricity in my home would go against my environmental protection principles.
I have a moral obligation to save electricity to contribute to the improvement of the environment.
I would feel guilty about not saving electricity in my home.
Habit Saving electricity in my home has become automatic to me.
Saving electricity in my home has become a habit to me.
Saving electricity in my home is natural to me.
Positive anticipated emotion If I take measures to save electricity in my home, I will feel excited.
If I take measures to save electricity in my home, I will feel relaxed.
If I take measures to save electricity in my home, I will feel proud.
Intention to save electricity I intend to save electricity in my home.
I plan to save electricity in my home.
I will make an effort to save electricity in my home.
Electricity saving behavior I always turn off electrical appliances when no one else is left in the room.
I always turn electrical appliances off completely rather than to a standby mode.
I have purchased energy efficient electrical appliances in the past few years.

References influencing the energy-saving behavior of urban and rural residents in China-A case
study of Jiangsu Province. Energy Policy 100, 252–259.
Du, L., Guo, J., Wei, C., 2017. Impact of information feedback on residential electricity
Abrahamse, W., Steg, L., 2009. How do socio-demographic and psychological factors demand in China. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 125, 324–334.
relate to households' direct and indirect energy use and savings? J. Econ. Psychol. 30 Fornara, F., Pattitoni, P., Mura, M., Strazzera, E., 2016. Predicting intention to improve
(5), 711–720. household energy efficiency: the role of value-belief-norm theory, normative and
Ajzen, I., 1991. The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 50 informational influence, and specific attitude. J. Environ. Psychol. 45, 1–10.
(2), 179–211. Fornell, C., Larcker, D.F., 1981. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable
Anderson, J.C., Gerbing, D.W., 1988. Structural equation modeling in practice: a review variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 18, 39–50.
and recommended two-step approach. Psychol. Bull. 103 (3), 411. Frederiks, E.R., Stenner, K., Hobman, E.V., 2015. Household energy use: applying be-
Arawomo, D.F., 2017. Electricity billing systems and household electricity use behavior in havioral economics to understand consumer decision-making and behavior. Renew.
Ibadan, Nigeria. Energy Sustain. Dev. 40, 77–84. Sustain. Energy Rev. 41, 1385–1394.
Armstrong, J.S., Overton, T.S., 1977. Estimating nonresponse bias in mail surveys. J. Gao, L., Wang, S., Li, J., Li, H., 2017. Application of the extended theory of planned
Mark. Res. 396–402. behavior to understand individual's energy saving behavior in workplaces. Resour.
Bamberg, S., 2003. How does environmental concern influence specific environmentally Conserv. Recycl. 127, 107–113.
related behaviors? A new answer to an old question. J. Environ. Psychol. 23 (1), Georges, E., Cornélusse, B., Ernst, D., Lemort, V., Mathieu, S., 2017. Residential heat
21–32. pump as flexible load for direct control service with parametrized duration and re-
Bamberg, S., Hunecke, M., Blöbaum, A., 2007. Social context, personal norms and the use bound effect. Appl. Energy 187, 140–153.
of public transportation: two field studies. J. Environ. Psychol. 27 (3), 190–203. Han, H., Hyun, S.S., 2018. What influences water conservation and towel reuse practices
Barr, S., Gilg, A.W., Ford, N., 2005. The household energy gap: examining the divide of hotel guests? Tour. Manag. 64, 87–97.
between habitual-and purchase-related conservation behaviors. Energy Policy 33 Handgraaf, M.J., de Jeude, M.A.V.L., Appelt, K.C., 2013. Public praise vs. private pay:
(11), 1425–1444. effects of rewards on energy conservation in the workplace. Ecol. Econ. 86, 86–92.
Bertoldo, R., Castro, P., 2016. The outer influence inside us: exploring the relation be- Harman, H.H., 1976. Modern Factor Analysis. University of Chicago Press, Chicago IL.
tween social and personal norms. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 112, 45–53. Hassin, R.R., Bargh, J.A., Zimerman, S., 2009. Automatic and flexible: the case of non-
Chen, M.F., Tung, P.J., 2014. Developing an extended theory of planned behavior model conscious goal pursuit. Soc. Cogn. 27 (1), 20–36.
to predict consumers' intention to visit green hotels. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 36, Hori, S., Kondo, K., Nogata, D., Ben, H., 2013. The determinants of household energy-
221–230. saving behavior: survey and comparison in five major Asian cities. Energy Policy 52,
Cheung, L.T., Chow, A.S., Fok, L., Yu, K.M., Chou, K.L., 2017. The effect of self-de- 354–362.
termined motivation on household energy consumption behavior in a metropolitan Kaiser, F.G., Scheuthle, H., 2003. Two challenges to a moral extension of the theory of
area in southern China. Energy Effic. 10 (3), 549–561. planned behavior: moral norms and just world beliefs in conservationism. Personal.
Chin, W.W., 1998. The partial least squares approach to structural equation modeling. Individ. Differ. 35 (5), 1033–1048.
Mod. Methods Bus. Res. 295 (2), 295–336. Kals, E., Schumacher, D., Montada, L., 1999. Emotional affinity toward nature as a mo-
Davies, J., Foxall, G.R., Pallister, J., 2002. Beyond the intention-behavior mythology: an tivational basis to protect nature. Environ. Behav. 31 (2), 178–202.
integrated model of recycling. Mark. Theory 2 (1), 29–113. Klöckner, C.A., 2013. A comprehensive model of the psychology of environmental be-
De Groot, J.I., Steg, L., 2009. Morality and pro-social behavior: the role of awareness, havior-a meta-analysis. Glob. Environ. Change 23 (5), 1028–1038.
responsibility, and norms in the norm activation model. J. Soc. Psychol. 149 (4), Klöckner, C.A., Blöbaum, A., 2010. A comprehensive action determination model: toward
425–449. a broader understanding of ecological behavior using the example of travel mode
De Leeuw, A., Valois, P., Ajzen, I., Schmidt, P., 2015. Using the theory of planned be- choice. J. Environ. Psychol. 30 (4), 574–586.
havior to identify key beliefs underlying pro-environmental behavior in high-school Lam, T., Hsu, C.H., 2004. Theory of planned behavior: potential travelers from China. J.
students: implications for educational interventions. J. Environ. Psychol. 42, Hosp. Tour. Res. 28 (4), 463–482.
128–138. Lerner, J.S., Keltner, D., 2001. Fear, anger, and risk. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 81 (1), 146.
Demarque, C., Charalambides, L., Hilton, D.J., Waroquier, L., 2015. Nudging sustainable Martinsson, J., Lundqvist, L.J., Sundström, A., 2011. Energy saving in Swedish house-
consumption: the use of descriptive norms to promote a minority behavior in a holds. The (relative) importance of environmental attitudes. Energy Policy 39 (9),
realistic online shopping environment. J. Environ. Psychol. 43, 166–174. 5182–5191.
Ding, Z., Wang, G., Liu, Z., Long, R., 2017. Research on differences in the factors Morris, J.D., Woo, C., Geason, J.A., Kim, J., 2002. The power of affect: predicting

179
S. Wang et al. Energy Policy 115 (2018) 171–180

intention. J. Advert. Res. 42 (3), 7–17. limitations and extensions. Ecol. Econ. 65 (3), 636–649.
Murata, A., Kondou, Y., Hailin, M., Weisheng, Z., 2008. Electricity demand in the Chinese Steg, L., Vlek, C., 2009. Encouraging pro-environmental behavior: an integrative review
urban household-sector. Appl. Energy 85 (12), 1113–1125. and research agenda. J. Environ. Psychol. 29 (3), 309–317.
Nilsson, A., Hansla, A., Biel, A., 2014b. Feeling the green? Value orientation as a mod- Stern, P.C., 2000. Towards a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. J.
erator of emotional response to green electricity. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 44 (10), Soc. Issues 56, 407–424.
672–680. Verplanken, B., Holland, R.W., 2002. Motivated decision making: effects of activation and
Nilsson, A., Bergstad, C.J., Thuvander, L., Andersson, D., Andersson, K., Meiling, P., self-centrality of values on choices and behavior. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 82 (3),
2014a. Effects of continuous feedback on households' electricity consumption: po- 434.
tentials and barriers. Appl. Energy 122, 17–23. Wahid, N.A., Rahbar, E., Shyan, T.S., 2011. Factors influencing the green purchase be-
Ouyang, J., Hokao, K., 2009. Energy-saving potential by improving occupants' behavior havior of Penang environmental volunteers. Int. Bus. Manag. 5 (1), 38–49.
in urban residential sector in Hangzhou City, China. Energy Build. 41 (7), 711–720. Wang, Z., Zhang, B., Yin, J., Zhang, Y., 2011. Determinants and policy implications for
Paulraj, A., Lado, A.A., Chen, I.J., 2008. Inter-organizational communication as a rela- household electricity-saving behavior: evidence from Beijing, China. Energy Policy
tional competency: antecedents and performance outcomes in collaborative buyer- 39 (6), 3550–3557.
supplier relationships. J. Oper. Manag. 26 (1), 45–64. Wang, Z., Zhang, B., Li, G., 2014. Determinants of energy-saving behavioral intention
Podsakoff, P.M., MacKenzie, S.B., Lee, J.Y., Podsakoff, N.P., 2003. Common method among residents in Beijing: extending the theory of planned behavior. J. Renew.
biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended Sustain. Energy 6 (5), 053127.
remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88 (5), 879. Webb, D., Soutar, G.N., Mazzarol, T., Saldaris, P., 2013. Self-determination theory and
Pothitou, M., Hanna, R.F., Chalvatzis, K.J., 2016. Environmental knowledge, pro-en- consumer behavioral change: evidence from a household energy-saving behavior
vironmental behavior and energy savings in households: an empirical study. Appl. study. J. Environ. Psychol. 35, 59–66.
Energy 184, 1217–1229. Weiss, H.M., Beal, D.J., 2005. Reflections on Affective Events Theory. In The effect of
Quested, T.E., Marsh, E., Stunell, D., Parry, A.D., 2013. Spaghetti soup: the complex world affect in Organizational Settings. Emerald Group Publishing Limited, Bingley, UK, pp.
of food waste behaviors. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 79, 43–51. 1–21.
Reiss, P.C., White, M.W., 2005. Household electricity demand, revisited. Rev. Econ. Stud. Yadav, R., Pathak, G.S., 2016. Young consumers' intention towards buying green products
72 (3), 853–883. in a developing nation: extending the theory of planned behavior. J. Clean. Prod. 135,
Rivis, A., Sheeran, P., Armitage, C.J., 2009. Expanding the affective and normative 732–739.
components of the theory of planned behavior: a meta-analysis of anticipated affect Yu, Y., Guo, J., 2016. Identifying electricity-saving potential in rural China: empirical
and moral norms. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 39 (12), 2985–3019. evidence from a household survey. Energy Policy 94, 1–9.
Russell, S., Young, C.W., Hardin, K., Robinson, C., 2017. Bringing habits and emotions Yuan, J., Zhao, C., Yu, S., Hu, Z., 2007. Electricity consumption and economic growth in
into food waste behavior. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 125, 107–114. China: cointegration and co-feature analysis. Energy Econ. 29 (6), 1179–1191.
da Silva, P.P., Cerqueira, P.A., 2017. Assessing the determinants of household electricity Zhang, Y., Wang, Z., Zhou, G., 2013. Determinants and implications of employee elec-
prices in the EU: a system-GMM panel data approach. Renew. Sustain. Energy Rev. tricity saving habit: an empirical study in China. Appl. Energy 112, 1529–1535.
73, 1131–1137. Zhou, K., Yang, S., 2016. Understanding household energy consumption behavior: the
Shi, H., Fan, J., Zhao, D., 2017. Predicting household PM 2.5-reduction behavior in contribution of energy big data analytics. Renew. Sustain. Energy Rev. 56, 810–819.
Chinese urban areas: an integrative model of theory of planned behavior and norm Zhou, S., Teng, F., 2013. Estimation of urban residential electricity demand in China using
activation theory. J. Clean. Prod. 145, 64–73. household survey data. Energy Policy 61, 394–402.
Sorrell, S., Dimitropoulos, J., 2008. The rebound effect: microeconomic definitions,

180

You might also like