Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 71 (2020) 101200

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jappdp

Teacher-student relationships across the first seven years of education and T


adolescent outcomes☆
Arya Ansaria,b, , Tara L. Hofkensc, Robert C. Piantac

a
Department of Human Sciences, The Ohio State University, United States of America
b
Crane Center for Early Childhood Research and Policy, The Ohio State University, United States of America
c
Center for Advanced Study of Teaching and Learning, University of Virginia, United States of America

ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT

Keywords: Despite the significance of teacher–student relationships during the early years of school, questions remain about
Teacher-student relationships its long-term importance and whether the timing and variability of relationship quality matter. To address these
Closeness gaps, data from the NICHD SECCYD were used to determine whether teacher-student relationships between
Conflict kindergarten and sixth grade were associated with the achievement, social-behavior, and educational beliefs and
NICHD SECCYD
aspirations of 1364 ninth graders (52% male, 80% White). Multivariate regression analyses revealed that when
teachers reported closer relationships with students, in turn, students demonstrated modestly stronger outcomes
across all domains. In contrast, more conflictual relationships were largely associated with underachievement
and variability in relationship quality was not consistently associated with adolescent outcomes. Finally, al­
though the benefits of teacher-student closeness were largely cumulative, teacher-student conflict in the later
years was more strongly associated with student outcomes than earlier conflict. Collectively, results affirm the
importance of teacher-student relationships for students' long-term development.

Introduction optimal outcomes. Notwithstanding this evidence we know less about


the long-term and cumulative trends in teacher-student relationships
Educational and developmental research has shown that the social across grades and possible implications for optimizing learning en­
and relational aspects of the classroom setting particularly qualities of vironments for students across the K-12 educational pipeline. For ex­
relationships between teachers and students are a modest but robust ample we do not know whether the benefits of earlier relationships
predictor of students’ academic achievement (Hamre & Pianta, 2001; documented in the extant literature have persisting benefits for students
Lippard, La Paro, Rouse, & Crosby, 2018; McCormick & O'Connor, or whether those benefits fade over time
2015; Pianta & Stuhlman, 2004) and socioemotional development Given the potential importance of teacher-student relationships as
(Heatly & Votruba-Drzal, 2017; Howes, 2000; Lippard et al., 2018; an educational and developmental asset, we consider the longer-term
Pianta & Stuhlman, 2004; Spilt, Hughes, Wu, & Kwok, 2012). Indeed ramifications of these relationships between kindergarten and sixth
students whose relationships with their teacher are characterized by grade for students' academic achievement, educational beliefs and as­
higher levels of closeness and lower levels of conflict demonstrate more pirations, and social-behavior in ninth grade, which represents: (a) a


This study was directed by a steering committee and supported by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) through a cooperative
agreement (U10), which calls for scientific collaboration between the grantees and the NICHD staff. Participating investigators, listed in alphabetical order, are: Jay
Belsky, Birkbeck University of London; Cathryn Booth-LaForce, University of Washington; Robert Bradley, University of Arkansas, Little Rock; Celia A. Brownell,
University of Pittsburgh; Margaret Burchinal, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill; Susan B. Campbell, University of Pittsburgh; K. Alison Clarke-Stewart,
University of California, Irvine; Sarah L. Friedman, CNA Corp., Alexandria, Virginia; Kathyrn Hirsh-Pasek, Temple University; Renate Houts, Research Triangle
Institute; Aletha Huston, University of Texas, Austin; Jean F. Kelly, University of Washington; Bonnie Knoke, Research Triangle Institute, Research Triangle, NC;
Nancy Marshall, Wellesley College; Kathleen McCartney, Harvard University; Fred Morrison, University of Michigan; Marion O’Brien, University of North Carolina at
Greensboro; Margaret Tresch Owen, University of Texas, Dallas; Robert Pianta, University of Virginia; Wendy Robeson, Wellesley College; Susan Spieker, University
of Washington; Deborah Lowe Vandell, University of California, Irvine; Marsha Weinraub, Temple University. We express our appreciation to the study coordinators
at each site who supervised the data collection, the research assistants who collected the data, and especially to the families and child-care providers who welcomed
us into their homes and workplaces and cooperated willingly with our repeated requests for information.

Corresponding author at: The Ohio State University, 1787 Neil Avenue, Columbus, OH 43210, United States of America.
E-mail address: ansari.81@osu.edu (A. Ansari).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2020.101200
Received 21 February 2020; Received in revised form 1 September 2020; Accepted 28 September 2020
Available online 07 October 2020
0193-3973/ © 2020 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
A. Ansari, et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 71 (2020) 101200

critical period for students' long-term educational success elementary school can be a promotive, protective, or a risk factor for
(McCallumore & Sparapani, 2010) and (b) outcomes that are predictive students to develop the foundational skills and resources they need to
of long-term life success and well-being (Jones, Greenberg, & Crowley, succeed and thrive in school.
2015; Magnuson, Duncan, Lee, & Metzger, 2016; Susperreguy, Davis-
Kean, Duckworth, & Chen, 2018). As part of this effort, we consider Teacher-student relationships and student outcomes
teacher-student relationships from both cumulative and timing specific
perspectives. We also examine whether variability in relationships Guided by these conceptual frameworks, Pianta (1999, 2006, 2016)
across grade-levels, above and beyond the average levels of teacher- has outlined and summarized the growth of scientific inquiry in­
student relationships, matters for students' later learning and develop­ vestigating the qualities, features, and theoretical advances in the study
ment. of teacher-student relationships. As with the a focus on any social re­
lationship (Ainsworth, 1989), teacher-student relationships are con­
Teacher-student relationships from a theoretical perspective ceptualized as coordinated systems of transacting components, such
that both teacher and student behaviors and characteristics inform
The average student in the United States spends approximately one- these relationships. Pianta (1999) posited that teacher-student re­
sixth (or 1000 h) of their waking hours in school. Moreover, even lationships are composed of: (1) teacher and student beliefs and ex­
though classrooms and schools are often thought of as academic in­ pectations about self, other, and the relationship; (2) behavioral ex­
stitutions, there is a longstanding recognition of the fact that the re­ changes that give rise to experience and beliefs; and (3) individual
lationships that students develop in this context are of consequence to characteristics (e.g., temperament) and experiences (e.g., prior attach­
their development personally, academically, and socially (Hamre & ments) that shape other components. Based on this conceptualization, a
Pianta, 2001; Longobardi, Prino, Marengo, & Settanni, 2016; McGrath large body of research has been devoted to describing the nature and
& Van Bergen, 2015; Pakarinen et al., 2018). In fact, once children consequences of teacher and student beliefs as a marker of relationship
begin their educational careers, the relationships they develop with quality (e.g., Jerome, Hamre, & Pianta, 2009; McCormick & O'Connor,
their teachers become increasingly important for their success in school 2015). In the context of this focus on participants' beliefs, teachers are
(Heatly & Votruba-Drzal, 2017). of particular importance, given the asymmetric nature of these re­
Teacher student relationships in the early years of school appear to lationships and their role as both attachment figure and educator
be particularly salient for student success because they engage with (Pianta, 2006).
multiple developmental processes that influence long-term outcomes. In this context, a large and growing correlational literature has
During these early years, children need to develop foundational cog­ shown that two particularly salient dimensions of teacher-student re­
nitive, social, and self-regulatory skills that they will need to be suc­ lationship beliefs (assessed through teacher report) are closeness (i.e.,
cessful throughout their educational careers and abundant evidence high levels of warmth, positive affect, and approachability between
demonstrates that features of adult-child relationships – sensitivity, student and teacher) and conflict (i.e., negativity and lack of rapport
responsivity, warmth, perspective-taking – shape children's develop­ between student and teacher), both of which modestly predict students'
ment of these skills through moment-to-moment interactions that take academic test scores (e.g., math, literacy; Hamre & Pianta, 2001;
place daily. Such findings confirm the that adult-child relationships and Lippard et al., 2018; McCormick & O'Connor, 2015; Pianta & Stuhlman,
interactions represent critical proximal processes that cumulate to 2004; Spilt et al., 2012), which may also extend to other academic
shape development over time (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006). Aside benchmarks, such as the rigor of students' coursework and their school
from the time children spend at home with their parents, interactions grades. There is also evidence that student-teacher relationships are
with teachers form the next largest share of the day spent with an adult. associated with students' social-behavioral development, including
Thus, a focus on relationships between teachers and students is war­ their sociability, internalizing behavior, externalizing behavior, and
ranted, and can be informed by research and theory on parent-child their engagement in risky behavior (Heatly & Votruba-Drzal, 2017;
relationships. Howes, 2000; Kobak, Herres, Gaskins, & Laurenceau, 2012; Lippard
More specific theoretical support for examining the role of teacher- et al., 2018; Pianta & Stuhlman, 2004; Spilt et al., 2012). Other studies
student relationships as proximal process supports for students' devel­ have shown that teachers' reports of their relationships with students
opment can be drawn from attachment theory (Ainsworth, 1989). At­ are also correlated with students' educational beliefs (e.g., self-con­
tachment theory posits that adult-child relationships support cognition cepts) and aspirations (Clem, Rudasill, Hirvonen, Aunola, & Kiuru,
and self-regulation in part through providing children with a sense of 2020; McFarland, Murray, & Phillipson, 2016; Verschueren, Doumen, &
security that enables them to explore the object world and establishes Buyse, 2012). A meta-analysis by Roorda, Koomen, Spilt, and Oort
an “internal working model” for engaging in interactions with an at­ (2011) suggests that teacher-student relationship quality in the early
tachment figure, or caregiver/teacher that have consequences for ac­ years of school is associated with higher levels of engagement and
quiring information and learning skills (Ainsworth, 1989). Thus at­ achievement outcomes (with effect sizes of 0.15–0.30), with conflict
tachment theory, and by extension theories of teacher-student serving as a stronger predictor of suboptimal outcomes such as elevated
relationships (Pianta, 1999), predicts that students who experience problem behaviors and internalizing problems (O'Connor, Collins, &
positive relationships with their teachers are better able to rely on them Supplee, 2012), associations that appear stronger among boys, lower
as a secure base and developmental asset and, as such, more likely to income children, and ethnic minority students (Roorda et al., 2011).
cooperate and engage with the teacher in the types of learning and Although the literature on teacher-student relationships has largely
developmental activities provided in the classroom and to persist in focused on the early years (Roorda et al., 2011), investigations on the
opportunities to learn and master challenging tasks. In fact, such pre­ nature and developmental salience of students' relationships with tea­
dictions have garnered considerable empirical support (Hamre & chers extends through adolescence (Allen, Pianta, Gregory, Mikami, &
Pianta, 2001; Lippard et al., 2018; Heatly & Votruba-Drzal, 2017). Lun, 2011) and confirms findings from the early grades, that relation­
Conversely, students who do not feel a sense of safety with their teacher ships characterized by closeness support the development of academic,
may withdraw and disengage from learning or engage in emotional or social-behavioral, and aspirational outcomes, while conflictual re­
behavioral conflict with the teacher. Moreover, a close relationship lationships undermine adolescent success (Pianta, 2016). This general
with a warm and caring teacher can buffer students from the negative pattern of results, across the K-12 years, is evident when relationships
effects of poor attachment with primary caregivers (Yan, Zhou, & are assessed through teacher report, observation, and as children reach
Ansari, 2016), indicating the unique promise of such relationships. adolescence, student report (Pianta, 2006). Accordingly, teachers' per­
Thus, from a theoretical perspective, teacher-student relationships in ceptions of their conflict and closeness represents an important, albeit

2
A. Ansari, et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 71 (2020) 101200

modest, gauge of the health of students' relationships with their tea­ Variability in relationships characterized by conflict and closeness
chers, and as some have suggested, a lasting indicator of the value of
the classroom setting for a specific child (Howes et al., 2008). In addition to the cumulative nature of teacher-student relationships,
the present study also investigates whether variability in conflict and
closeness with students matters for students' long-term adjustment and
The long-term outcomes of teacher-student relationships and student well-being. In other words, in addition to the accumulation of close or
outcomes conflictual interactions with teachers over time, the variability in the
affective quality of each relationship could also be consequential for
Despite theoretical and empirical support for the importance of student learning. The above is of note because, even though it is gen­
teacher-student relationships in the early years of school, we know little erally accepted that early relationships are a developmental and edu­
about their cumulative and long-term effects, especially as students cational resource across the K-12 school years, developmental theory
transition to ninth grade and start their high school education. Students' suggests that children's characteristics and experiences can be described
academic achievement, social-behavioral functioning, and educational both in terms of continuity (e.g., stability of early experiences and ef­
aspirations during this period can support or impair students' ability to fects across time) and discontinuity (e.g., such as when later experi­
successfully complete and benefit from their formal education ences reverse early experiences and effects; Lerner, 2006; Schulenberg
(Appleton, Christenson, Kim, & Reschly, 2006; Li & Lerner, 2011; Wang & Maslowsky, 2015). Thus, even though studies show that students'
& Peck, 2013). Yet, student engagement bottoms out in the high school long-term educational and life trajectories are established in the early
years (e.g. Martin & Dowson, 2009), with one- to two-thirds of high years (e.g., Duncan et al., 2007; Moffitt & Caspi, 2001), early experi­
school students reporting that they feel disengaged, with qualities of ences and trajectories are not deterministic. Reflecting the above, stu­
teacher-student relationships described as a factor in (dis) engagement dies have found that some students' experience stability in the types of
(National Research Council and Institute of Medicine, 2004). Ninth relationships they develop with teachers over time, whereas others
grade in particular represents a critical point in students' educational experience variability (Jerome et al., 2009).
careers, with many researchers and educators considering it to be a Because students change teachers from year to year it is plausible
turning point for whether students will drop out of school that children experience discontinuities and variability in relationship
(McCallumore & Sparapani, 2010). Although evidence does support the quality over time and these discontinuities may cause challenges (and
importance of concurrent teacher-student relationships with outcomes opportunities) for students. Students who experience variability in re­
for youth (Allen et al., 2011; Granot, 2014), there are few, if any, lationship quality may face not only the immediate lack of social re­
studies of longitudinal patterns of students' relationships with teachers sources but also the disruption in expectations and beliefs about the
across the first several years of school with student behavior and per­ stability of relationships, which may add to challenges in the classroom.
formance after the transition to high school. This is a significant over­ Moreover, a high degree of inconsistency in relational quality year to
sight because these earlier relationships may represent fundamental year may further erode students' trust and confidence in teachers as a
supports for students and represent salient markers for students' adap­ resource. Given that the extant literature has not considered rigorously
tation to high school. the potential drawbacks of variability in relationship quality over time,
Reflecting the above, both bioecological and attachment theory there is little empirical evidence to guide our understanding. The above
imply that a history of positive relationships with teachers across the is particularly problematic because the average quality reported in the
early grades could be an enduring resource that supports students extant literature may conflate students who have middle of the road
across different developmental epochs, such as when they begin high experiences with all of their teachers with students who have extreme
school. Bioecological theory suggests, for example, that teacher-student experiences (e.g., some high and some low) with their teachers.
interactions build developmental skills that then form the basis for later Accordingly, as part of this study, we also consider whether the varia­
skill development. In this way, performance when student enter high tion in closeness or conflict in the first seven years of education matters
school could be influenced by relationships with teachers in the early for students through secondary school.
years because students developed relevant foundational skills in the
context of relationships with their teachers in prior years of school
Timing of teacher-student relationships
(Heckman, 2006). In one of the only known studies that considered this
question, Hamre and Pianta (2000) found that these relationships not
In addition to the cumulative nature and variability of teacher-
only matter for students' concurrent functioning in the early years, but
student relationships, it has long been known that developmental
these relationships with children as early as kindergarten were linked
timing is critical in understanding how environments are experienced.
with students' academic performance, classroom engagement, and dis­
Given the above, another important consideration is whether there are
ciplinary infractions through eighth grade. Similarly, attachment theory
specific times in students' educational careers during which certain
posits and there is evidence that early attachment figures and styles can
aspects of development are more strongly influenced by the quality of
have long-term effects on development in school (O'Connor et al.,
interactions and relationships with teachers. In other words, to the
2012). Although there is evidence that teachers can function as sec­
extent that teachers serve as key sources that affect students' develop­
ondary attachment figures (Van Ryzin, 2010), we have not yet studied
mental trajectories, then these influences may be stronger at some
the enduring effects of closeness (or conflict) with teachers across the
stages than others. As part of the present investigation, we focus on
elementary school years as students transition into high school.
teacher-student relationships developed in the early grades (i.e., kin­
Given the above and given the recent attention to the value of the
dergarten through second grade) and relationships developed in the
classroom as a social and emotional resource for students (The ASPEN
later grades (i.e., third through sixth grade). We consider these epochs
Institute, 2019), examining associations between earlier relationships
because an increasing focus in third grade and beyond is test prepara­
and outcomes in the first year of high school could illuminate pathways
tion, which some studies suggest may be at the expense of building high
toward promoting success from the first to the last years of students'
quality and strong relationships (Blazar & Pollard, 2017).
formal education. In particular, studying long-term associations of
To this end, prior studies have revealed that teachers' perceptions of
closeness and conflict with the high school transition year could inform
conflict with a student is moderately stable across grade levels, whereas
the design, implementation, and expansion of social-emotional learning
closeness is somewhat more variable and specific to a particular tea­
interventions throughout schooling, while testing tenets of develop­
cher-student dyad (Jerome et al., 2009; O'Connor & McCartney, 2007;
mental theories underlying teacher student relationships.

3
A. Ansari, et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 71 (2020) 101200

Pianta, Steinberg, & Rollins, 1995). More specifically, in the early years, social-behavior?
students usually experience close and positive relationships with their 2. To what extent is variability in teacher-student conflict and closeness
teachers, which declines over time. In contrast, conflict appears to be between kindergarten and sixth grade predictive of students' aca­
somewhat more stable in part because, even though teachers change demic achievement, educational beliefs and aspirations, and social-
from year to year, students' characteristics and dispositions appear to behavior?
play a larger role in the development of conflict (Jerome et al., 2009; 3. With respect to students' academic achievement, educational beliefs
McKinnon et al., 2018). Even with the modest stability in conflict and and aspirations, and social-behavior, does the timing of teacher-
variability in closeness, there has been little exploration of the im­ student conflict and closeness matter?
portance of the timing of teacher-student relationships across the K-12
school years. To address these research questions, we used data from the NICHD
Two studies are of note, however. First, Burchinal, Peisner-Feinberg, Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (SECCYD). Even
Pianta, and Howes (2002) found that teachers perceptions of relation­ though the NICHD SECCYD constituted primarily White, middle class
ships in the early years were more important for student learning in the participants, the data are one of the few that include annual reports of
immediate term as compared with the later elementary school years. teacher-student relationships between kindergarten and sixth grade and
Second, Pianta and Stuhlman (2004) found that concurrent teacher- that have a comprehensive set of student outcomes assessed in ado­
student relationships mattered more than past relationships for stu­ lescence. As such, the NICHD SECCYD provides the breadth and depth
dents' academic and socioemotional functioning in first grade. Although necessary to answer our research questions. When taken together, re­
both studies highlight the importance of teachers' perceptions of re­ sults obtained related to these questions have implications for educa­
lationships, both also provide only a narrow view of development early tional and developmental conceptualizations of the classroom setting as
in children's educational trajectories. One study focuses on relation­ well as for efforts to improve the value and impact of educational ex­
ships at a single point in time as a predictor of development across time periences on students' outcomes. Given the focus on the long-term
(i.e., Burchinal et al., 2002), whereas the other focuses on relationships outcomes of teacher-student relationships, existing evidence in the
across a three-year window in predicting development in first grade education and developmental literature more generally regarding the
(i.e., Pianta & Stuhlman, 2004). As such, the scope of these studies fadeout of education effects (e.g., Ansari, 2018; Bailey, Duncan, Odgers,
limits our understanding of the role of relationships as part of long-term & Yu, 2017), and exploratory nature of our work, we did not make
developmental cycles that start early in students' educational careers directional hypotheses.
and that could have long-term ramifications. Thus, an additional area of
interest that has yet to be methodically examined is how the timing of Method
relationships influences the links between teacher-student conflict and
closeness in elementary school and longer-term functioning. Participants were drawn from the NICHD SECCYD (for other studies
Specifically, teacher-student relationships early in elementary published with these data see: Belsky et al., 2007; Vandell, Belsky,
school could have different ramifications for development than re­ Burchinal, Steinberg, & Vandergrift, 2010; Vandell, Burchinal, & Pierce,
lationships the older grades. In the early school years, children are 2016), which sampled mothers who gave birth in 1991 across ten dif­
oriented toward their teachers in the classroom and seek teachers out ferent sites in the U.S. (Little Rock, AR; Irvine, CA; Lawrence, KS;
for comfort and security (Verschueren & Koomen, 2012). As children Boston, MA; Philadelphia, PA; Pittsburgh, PA; Charlottesville, VA;
get older, they become increasingly oriented toward interactions with Morganton, NC; Seattle, WA; and Madison, WI). As part of the data
their peers (Eccles, 1999). In the older years, teachers also tend to focus collection, healthy newborns of English-speaking mothers discharged
more on academic curriculum and programming, and students are more within one week of birth were contacted. Attempts were made to
likely to have different teachers for core content areas. Therefore, some contact 3015 families who met eligibility criteria. Contact was un­
decline in children's closeness with their teacher and increase in conflict successful for 512 families and 151 families were deemed ineligible
throughout elementary school may be developmentally normative as because the child remained in the hospital more than seven days or the
children expand their network of social resources by seeking closer family planned to move. An additional 641 families refused to partici­
relationships with peers and spend time with more teachers at school pate and 1-month interviews could not be scheduled for 185 families for
(e.g. Jerome et al., 2009; Spilt et al., 2012). These elevated levels of other reasons. Out of 1526 families scheduled, 1364 families completed
conflict may also be more problematic because, as children get older, the 1-month home visit and became study participants (for sample
they begin to value autonomy and teachers may focus on controlling descriptives, see Table 1). Although there were no significant differ­
aspects of their relationship, which leads to conflict and disengagement ences between these 1364 families and the U.S. population based on
(Skinner, Furrer, Marchand, & Kindermann, 2008). Given students' ethnicity (80% White in U.S. population vs. 80% in cohort) and
expanding social networks, children's relationships with their teachers household income ($36,520 in U.S. population and $37,781 in cohort),
in the early grades may play a more powerful role in development than the children in the NICHD SECCYD had a somewhat higher percentage
their relationships with teachers in subsequent grades (Bergin & Bergin, of parents who were married than the U.S. population (77% vs. 74%,
2009). With that said, children's need to feel socially connected and p < .05).
accepted in school continues to be important throughout childhood and Of the 1364 individuals who participated at the beginning of the
adolescence (Furrer & Skinner, 2003) and, as such, teacher-student study, 1226 participated in Phase II (through first grade; 1995 through
closeness and conflict may continue to be equally important regardless 1999), 1061 participated in Phase III (through sixth grade; 2000
of timing. through 2004), and 1009 participated in Phase IV (through ninth grade;
2005 through 2007). To ensure that our sample is representative of the
The current study full study sample, we use missing data procedures (more details below)
to retain the full sample of 1364 children. The data are not publicly
The purpose of the present investigation is to address key gaps in available, but are accessible for download at the Inter-University
knowledge regarding the long-term outcomes of teacher-student clo­ Consortium for Political and Social Research (ICPSR) upon signing a
seness and conflict. We addressed the following research questions: restricted data use agreement.

1. To what extent are teacher-student conflict and closeness between Procedures


kindergarten and sixth grade cumulatively associated with students'
academic achievement, educational beliefs and aspirations, and Information on participating families and their children were

4
A. Ansari, et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 71 (2020) 101200

Table 1 Table 1 (continued)


Sample descriptives for study participants.
Variable Mean (SD) or proportion
Variable Mean (SD) or proportion
Sexual risk taking 0.07 (0.19)
Child characteristics and experiences Risky behavior 0.19 (0.16)
Preschool functioning
Language and literacy Notes. Estimates in brackets correspond to standard deviations. Proportions may
Letter word identification 369.36 (21.41)
not sum to 1.00 due to rounding.
Picture vocabulary 459.54 (14.09) a
Although math and science coursework is treated as a continuous variable
Math 424.72 (19.27)
Externalizing behavior 51.69 (9.39) in all analyses, the breakdown of courses is provided to illustrate the dis­
Internalizing behavior 47.29 (8.88) tribution.
Social skills 98.27 (13.53)
Health 3.39 (0.65) collected when children were 1 month of age through age 15. More
Race
specifically, as part of the larger study, data were gathered from par­
White 0.80
Black/other 0.20
ticipants at home, in the laboratory, at school, and via regular phone
Male 0.52 calls by trained personnel. All scientific decisions about the larger study
Birth weight (pounds) 7.69 (1.12) were reviewed and approved by the NICHD SECCYD steering com­
Birth order 1.83 (0.95) mittee. All assessment and data collection procedures were also piloted
Temperament 3.18 (0.40)
extensively before use in the field. Data collectors from all study sites
Proportion of time in center care 0.21 (0.26)
Proportion of time in maternal care 0.30 (0.27) followed detailed data collection procedures. Centralized training was
Child care quality 2.92 (0.42) conducted on all data collection procedures and, as part of the data
Family characteristics during early childhood training, all data collectors were required to pass a videotaped certifi­
Mothers age 28.11 (5.63)
cation test on the corresponding procedures to which they were as­
Mothers psychological adjustment 59.00 (13.95)
Maternal education 14.23 (2.51)
signed. The following sections describe the specific measures used in
Maternal vocabulary 99.01 (18.35) the present analyses and the time points of administration. However,
Parenting quality −0.03 (0.73) additional details about all data collection procedures can be found in
Income to needs ratio 3.60 (2.85) the study's Manuals of Operation and Instrument Documentation
Proportion employed 0.66 (0.37)
(http://secc.rti.org).
Maternal depression 9.36 (6.76)
Proportion two parent household 0.84 (0.32)
Mother-child relationship
Closeness 50.04 (3.67) Measures
Conflict 27.35 (7.63)
Father-child relationship Teacher-student relationships
Closeness 47.93 (4.31) We used the Student Teacher Relationship Scale (STRS; Pianta,
Conflict 26.33 (6.87)
Neighborhood characteristics during early childhood
1992) to assess teacher perceptions of the quality of their relationship
% of households in poverty 10.29 (10.03) with students for each year between kindergarten and sixth grade. The
% of single parent households 8.90 (8.21) STRS was included as part of the yearly teacher surveys, whereby
% of households receiving assistance 5.94 (6.55) children's primary teacher in each grade rated the extent to which 15
% of individuals unemployed 4.96 (4.20)
statements characterized their current relationship with the study child
% of adults with less than a high school education 20.16 (13.79)
% of adults White 82.91 (21.06) on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = definitely does not apply to 5 = definitely
Ninth grade outcomes applies). The STRS includes sub-dimensions of closeness and conflict (the
Academic achievement average within time correlation across subscales within the study
Math 524.57 (16.77) sample = 0.33). The closeness subscale consists of eight items and
Language and literacy
Verbal analogies 525.55 (14.18)
measures the warmth and communication of the relationship, whereas
Passage comprehension 520.36 (12.47) the conflict subscale consists of seven items and measures the extent to
Picture vocabulary 518.60 (13.15) which the relationship is marked by negative interactions. In terms of
Grade point average 3.03 (0.71) reliability, prior studies have established the test-retest reliability over a
Math course levela 2.33 (0.90)
four-week period, and high internal consistency for both conflict and
No math course (0) 0.05
Below algebra I (1) 0.04 closeness subscales has been demonstrated (Pianta, 2001). Moreover,
Algebra I (2) 0.51 the Cronbach's alphas for the current study sample were strong for both
Geometry (3) 0.31 the closeness (α = 0.86, 0.85, 0.85, 0.85, 0.94, 0.85, 0.86, kinder­
Algebra II (4) 0.08 garten, first, second, third, fourth, fifth, and sixth grade, respectively)
Advanced math (5) 0.00
Science course levela 2.05 (1.24)
and conflict subscales (α = 0.90 0.88, 0.89, 0.91, 0.91, 0.90, 0.91,
No science course (0) 0.08 kindergarten, first, second, third, fourth, fifth, and sixth grade, re­
Survey Science (1) 0.31 spectively).
Earth Science (2) 0.21 For the purposes of the present study, we use these teacher-student
Biology (3) 0.34
relationship subscales in several different ways (presented in the order
Chemistry (4) 0.01
Physics (5) 0.05 of our research questions). First, we considered the consequences of
Advanced Science (6) 0.00 cumulative closeness and conflict between kindergarten and sixth grade
Educational beliefs and aspirations as associated with adolescent outcomes. In these analyses we took the
Educational aspirations 4.60 (0.69) average of conflict between kindergarten and sixth grade and the
Mathematics self-concepts 5.64 (1.07)
Literacy self-concepts 5.14 (1.18)
average of closeness between kindergarten and sixth grade as the
Social behavior parameterization of our cumulative index (for similar approaches to
Externalizing behavior problems 45.51 (10.46) cumulative effect analyses see: Coley & Kull, 2016). The second way in
Internalizing behavior problems 46.64 (9.86) which we conceptualized teacher-student relationships is in terms of the
Social skills 103.89 (14.80)
variability in closeness and conflict. For this benchmark, we used the
standard deviation of students' closeness and conflict scores between

5
A. Ansari, et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 71 (2020) 101200

kindergarten and sixth grade. The correlation between the cumulative or twice, 2 = more than twice). This instrument was adapted from
and variability indicators were − 0.55 for closeness (variance inflation Conger and Elder (1994), the Fast Track project (McMahon et al.,
factor [VIF] =1.47) and 0.75 for conflict (VIF = 2.51). Then, for the 1999), and the New Hope project (Bos et al., 1999). Items captured two
series of analyses that considered the importance of timing, the average dimensions of risk taking: sexual risk taking (e.g., vaginal and oral sexual
of closeness and conflict were separated into epochs: (a) the early behavior; 4 items, α = 0. 58) and other risky behavior (e.g., gotten into
grades (i.e., kindergarten through second grade) and (b) the later fights, drank, smoked, 36 items; α = 0.87). Given the distribution of
grades (third through sixth grade). The correlation between early and responses, we top-coded the top 2–3% of responses so that outliers do
later grade closeness and conflict were 0.44 (VIF = 1.24) and 0.63 not unduly influence our estimates.
(VIF = 1.66), respectively. It is important to note that we examined
grade-specific associations as well, and in the main, they support the Covariates
general patterns reported below (results available from authors). To isolate the unique associations between teacher-student re­
lationships and adolescents' functioning and well-being, we adjusted for
Ninth grade outcomes a rich set of covariates from the period before children entered kin­
In total, 13 dimensions of adolescents' functioning and well-being dergarten. Although many of these variables were time invariant, a
that were assessed at age 15 toward the end of the ninth grade (see handful were measured at multiple points in time before kindergarten
Table 1 for descriptive statistics). These 13 dimensions fell under three entry. For variables assessed on multiple occasions, we followed past
broader domains, namely: (1) academic achievement, (2) educational protocol (e.g., Belsky et al., 2007; Vandell et al., 2010, 2016) and took
beliefs and aspirations, and (3) social behavior. We discuss these three the average of children's and families' experiences when children were
domains and the specific dimensions in more detail below. 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months of age. Moreover, although not shown, we
also estimated additional models with time varying family, neighbor­
Academic achievement. To begin, adolescents' math (Applied Problems, hood, and teacher/school covariates from the elementary school years
70 items, α = 0.87) and language and literacy (Verbal Analogies [35 and adolescences and all results were the same as those presented
items, α = 0.86], Passage Comprehension [43 items, α = 0.81], and below (results available from authors). Thus, the findings reported
Picture Vocabulary [58 items, α = 0.81]; composite α = 0.86) skills below were not sensitive to factors such as teacher experience, educa­
were assessed with the Woodcock-Johnson (Woodcock & Johnson, tion, and changes in family income and circumstance.
1989). The math battery captured adolescents' skill and ability in
analyzing and solving practical problems in mathematics, whereas the Child characteristics. At the child-level, we included indicators of:
language and literacy battery captured students' ability to recognize gender, minority status, birthweight, birth order, temperament at
pictured objects, complete phrases, and ability in reading short 6 months of age (measured with the Infant Temperament
passages. For the purposes of the present study, we use the W scores. Questionnaire, α = 0.81; Medoff-Cooper, Carey, & McDevitt, 1993),
In addition to these direct assessments of children's achievement and maternal report of child health at 54 months of age, the proportion of
cognition, records of student's overall grade point average at the end of time spent in center care and maternal between birth and 54 months of
ninth grade and their math (0 = no math, 1 = below algebra I, age, and child care quality (as measured with Observational Record of
2 = algebra I, 3 = geometry, 4 = algebra II, 5 = advanced math) and the Caregiving Environment; α's > 0.80). We also adjusted for
science coursework (0 = no science course, 1 = survey science, children's skills and functioning at 54 months of age, which were
2 = earth science, 3 = biology, 4 = chemistry, 5 = physics, measured with the age appropriate versions of the same tools used at
6 = advanced science) were also available through administrative age 15. In doing so, we account for any confound between teachers who
records. report better relationships with students who do better in school.

Educational beliefs and aspirations. As part of the data collection, Family characteristics. In terms of family factors, we included indicators
students reported on the likelihood they would complete high school, of: mothers age at birth of child, mothers years of education at birth of
attend college, and complete college (1 = not sure at all to 5 = very sure). child, a composite of mothers' psychological adjustment at 6 months of
We used students' responses to these three questions to create an age (measured with the NEO Personality Inventory; Costa & McCrae,
indicator for educational aspirations (α = 0.83). As part of these 1985), mothers' vocabulary skills at 36 months of age (measured with
surveys, adolescents also reported on their mathematics (α = 0.84, 5 the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised; Dunn & Dunn, 1981),
items) and literacy self-concepts (α = 0.83, 5 items) with items derived household income-to-needs, maternal employment, maternal
from the Self and Task Perception Questionnaire (1 = not at all good to depressive symptoms (measured with the Center for Epidemiological
7 = very good; Jacobs, Lanza, Osgood, Eccles, & Wigfield, 2002). Studies Depression Scale; Radloff, 1977), mother closeness and conflict
Sample items from this questionnaire include “How good at SUBJECT with child at 54 months of age, father closeness and conflict with child
are you?” and “How well do you expect to do in SUBJECT this year?” at 54 months of age, an indicator of whether children lived in a two
parent home, and parenting quality (a composite of scores on the Home
Social behavior. In addition to the above outcomes, study mothers also Observation for Measurement of the Environment Scale [Caldwell &
reported on their children's externalizing (33 items, α = 0.91) and Bradley, 1984] and videotaped interactions of maternal sensitivity).
internalizing behavior problems (31 items, α = 0.86) with the Child
Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1991; 0 = not true [as far as you know] Neighborhood characteristics. In addition to the above child and family
to 2 = very true or often true). The externalizing scale measure consisted covariates, we also adjusted for site fixed effects (i.e., dummy indicators
of information related to youths' delinquent and aggressive behavior, for Little Rock, Irvine, Lawrence etc.) and neighborhood characteristics
whereas the internalizing scale consisted of information from the as measured by the census, namely the percent of: households in
syndrome scales designated as withdrawn, somatic complaints, and poverty, single parent households, households receiving government
anxious/depressed. As part of the parent surveys, mothers also reported assistance, unemployed adults, adults with less than a high school
on adolescent's social skills (40 items, 0 = never to 2 = very often, education, and non-minority adults.
α = 0.91) with the Social Skills Rating System (Gresham & Elliott,
1990). This measure captured cooperation, assertiveness, and self- Analytic strategy
control.
Finally, a series of audio-recorded questions were asked of adoles­ All analyses were estimated in Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, 1998-
cents about their risk taking during the past year (0 = never, 1 = once 2013) within a multivariate regression framework in which all

6
A. Ansari, et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 71 (2020) 101200

outcomes were modeled simultaneously. These models included robust As can be seen in Table 2, results from these analyses revealed that, on
standard errors to safeguard against violations of normality. Similar to average, teacher-student closeness ranged from 30.31 to 34.23, which
past studies that have used the ninth grade outcomes from the NICHD on the STRS 1 to 5 scale corresponds to scores of roughly 3.79 to 4.28.
SECCYD (e.g., Burchinal, Lowe Vandell, & Belsky, 2014; Martin-Storey Despite these moderate to moderately high scores on teacher-student
& Benner, 2019; Vandell et al., 2010) missing data (mean of 20%; range closeness, a series of t-tests revealed that there was a significant de­
of 0–47%) were accounted for with full information maximum like­ crease between kindergarten and sixth grade: Children in sixth grade
lihood estimation, which utilizes all available data points from each were reported to have a less close relationships with their teachers than
individual in estimating model parameters. In the main, missing data children in kindergarten, with an effect size (ES) difference of ap­
were highest for the high school transcript data (e.g., coursework and proximately 0.67 (p < .001). In terms of teacher-student conflict, there
GPA). And even though it is not possible to determine whether data are appeared to be somewhat greater stability over time, with average
missing at random or missing not at random, prior studies show that the conflict scores ranging from 10.60 to 11.62, which on the STRS scale
inclusion of background variables (e.g., child and family demographics) corresponds to scores of 1.51 to 1.66. Although statistically significant
that may help explain the variation in missingness yields unbiased es­ differences also emerged across grade levels, these differences were
timates (Kline, 2010). It is also important to acknowledge that our two- considerably smaller, with the largest difference (kindergarten versus
coursework outcomes are ordinal categorical variables, such that each third grade) corresponding to roughly an ES of 0.17.
unit increase links to a more difficult course. Because studies show that
ordinal scales can be treated as continuous as long as they have four or Teacher-student closeness and conflict and adolescent outcomes
more categories (Bentler & Chou, 1987), we modeled these variables as
continuous. However, as a robustness check, we estimated models with Having established the descriptive patterns of teacher-student re­
coursework specified as an ordinal variable and all conclusions were lationships between kindergarten and sixth grade, the next set of ana­
the same. Finally, it is also important to note that all models included lyses considered outcomes of these relationships in ninth grade. Results
all covariates outlined above and, given the study design (i.e., there indicated that students who experienced closer relationships with their
were rarely multiple children per classroom/school), nesting of chil­ K-6 teachers, in turn, demonstrated better academic outcomes and
dren in classrooms/schools was not an issue. educational beliefs and aspirations. Specifically, these children earned a
With the above analytic framework in mind, our analyses proceeded higher GPA (ES = 0.12, p < .01), enrolled in a higher level science
in several steps. Given the acceptable VIF values reported above (VIF course (ES = 0.12, p < .05), held higher educational expectations of
values ranged from roughly1.25–2.5; values over 5 indicates issues of themselves (ES = 0.17, p < .001), and demonstrated greater math
multicollinearity that are problematic), our first set of analyses used the (ES = 0.14, p < .01) and English (ES = 0.09, p < .05) self-concepts
cumulative indicator of teacher-student closeness and conflict as a relative to students with less close relationships (see Table 3). More­
predictor of adolescent outcomes in addition to indicators for the over, students with closer relationships with their teachers exhibited
variability in teacher-student closeness and conflict. To determine stronger social skills (ES = 0.08, p < .05) and engaged in fewer risky
whether the timing of closeness and conflict mattered, we estimated behavior (ES = −0.11, p < .05). In contrast, students who were
additional models that separated teacher-student relationships into the perceived to have more conflict with their teachers between kinder­
early grades (i.e., K-G2) and the later grades (i.e., G3-G6) while con­ garten and sixth grade performed worse academically, including on
trolling for variability in closeness and conflict. To determine whether direct assessments of math (ES = −0.16, p < .001), language and
the magnitude of associations were statistically different from one an­ literacy (ES = −0.09, p < .05),and their GPA (ES = −0.15,
other across these two periods, we used a post hoc coefficient com­ p < .05).
parison (Paternoster, Brame, Mazerolle, & Piquero, 1998). If the post
hoc coefficient comparison was significant, we would conclude that the Variability in relationship closeness and conflict
associations between teacher-student relationships were significantly
different across time. Above, we presented the cumulative associations between teacher-
student relationships and adolescent outcomes. When examining the
coefficients for variability in relationships (see Table 3), we found that
Results variability in closeness was not predictive of student outcomes, above
and beyond than the mean-level of closeness. In contrast, students who
Although not a focal study objective, we begin by discussing the experienced greater variability in conflict from year to year demon­
descriptive patterns of teacher-student closeness and conflict between strated less optimal educational beliefs and social behavior. More spe­
kindergarten and sixth grade to provide context for our study findings. cifically, these students demonstrated less optimal self-concepts (both
math [ES = −0.12, p < .05] and literacy [ES = −0.11, p < .05])
Table 2 and social skills (ES = −0.11, p < .05) along with elevated levels of
Descriptive statistics for teacher-student closeness and conflict between kin­ externalizing behavior (ES = 0.11, p < .05) than those who experi­
dergarten and sixth grade.
enced less variability. In the main, however, the cumulative levels of
Teacher-student Teacher-student relationship quality were more consistently associated with student
closeness conflict outcomes than the variability in relationship quality.
Grade level
Kindergarten 34.23 (5.34) 10.60 (5.36) Timing of closeness and conflict and adolescent outcomes
First grade 33.96 (5.04) 10.92 (5.17)
Second grade 33.67 (5.17) 10.94 (5.41) Our final set of analyses considered whether the timing of re­
Third grade 33.08 (5.15) 11.62 (6.03)
lationships mattered. As can be seen in Table 3, results from these
Fourth grade 32.52 (5.11) 11.14 (5.73)
Fifth grade 31.85 (5.36) 11.44 (5.74) timing-specific models indicated that the associations between re­
Sixth grade 30.31 (5.74) 11.07 (5.64) lationship closeness and outcomes were largely cumulative: Teacher-
Relationship quality in early 33.95 (4.04) 10.94 (4.39) student closeness did not matter more in the early versus later grades.
elementary school (K-G2) The sole exception was for math self-concepts, where there was a
Relationship quality in late elementary 31.95 (3.98) 11.46 (4.83)
stronger association between teacher-student closeness in the later
school (G3-G6)
Overall relationship quality (K-G6) 32.91 (3.53) 11.25 (4.26) grades (ES = 0.19, p < .001) than the early grades (ES = −0.00, ns).
In contrast, conflict with teachers in the later grades appeared to be

7
A. Ansari, et al.

Table 3
Multivariate regression results for the links between teacher-child relationships and ninth grade outcomes.
Model 1 Model 2

Cumulative closeness K-G6 Cumulative conflict K-G6 Variability in closeness K- Variability in conflict K-G6 K-G2 closeness G3-G6 closeness K-G2 conflict G3-G6 conflict
G6

Academic outcomes
⁎⁎⁎ ⁎⁎
Math 0.03 (0.03) −0.16 (0.05) −0.01 (0.03) 0.05 (0.04) −0.00 (0.03) 0.04 (0.04) −0.04 (0.04) −0.13 (0.04)
⁎ ⁎⁎
Language and literacy −0.01 (0.03) −0.09 (0.05) −0.03 (0.03) 0.05 (0.04) 0.02 (0.03) −0.04 (0.03) 0.00 (0.03) −0.12 (0.05)
⁎⁎ ⁎ ⁎ ⁎⁎⁎
Grade point average 0.12 (0.04) −0.15 (0.06) −0.04 (0.04) −0.00 (0.05) 0.09 (0.04) 0.05 (0.04) 0.06 (0.05) −0.23 (0.05)
⁎⁎
Math class 0.07 (0.04) −0.07 (0.05) 0.03 (0.04) −0.03 (0.05) 0.06 0.03 (0.04) 0.07 (0.05) −0.16 (0.05)
(0.04)

Science class 0.12 (0.06) −0.01 (0.07) 0.07 (0.04) −0.01 (0.06) 0.05 (0.04) 0.09 (0.05) 0.06 (0.05) −0.08 (0.06)
Educational beliefs and aspirations
⁎⁎⁎ ⁎ ⁎
Educational aspirations 0.17 0.08 0.06 −0.06 0.08 0.13 0.01 0.08

8
(0.05) (0.05) (0.04) (0.06) (0.04) (0.05) (0.05) (0.06)
⁎⁎ ⁎ ⁎⁎⁎
Self-concepts Math 0.14 (0.05) 0.09 (0.06) 0.05 (0.45) −0.12 (0.05) 0.00 (0.04) 0.19 (0.05) 0.04 (0.05) 0.07 (0.06)
⁎ ⁎⁎ ⁎ ⁎
Self-concepts English 0.09 (0.04) 0.14 (0.05) 0.03 (0.04) −0.11 (0.05) 0.07 (0.04) 0.01 (0.05) 0.11 (0.05) 0.01 (0.06)
Social-behavior outcomes
Internalizing −0.07 (0.04) −0.04 (0.05) 0.04 (0.04) 0.06 (0.05) −0.02 (0.04) −0.07 (0.04) 0.01 (0.05) −0.05 (0.05)
⁎ ⁎⁎
Externalizing 0.02 (0.04) 0.09 (0.05) 0.05 (0.03) 0.11 (0.04) 0.00 (0.04) 0.02 (0.04) −0.03 (0.05) 0.13 (0.05)
⁎ ⁎
Social skills 0.08 (0.04) −0.03 (0.05) −0.04 (0.04) −0.11 (0.05) 0.02 (0.04) 0.08 (0.04) 0.02 (0.04) −0.06 (0.05)

Sexual behavior −0.05 (0.05) −0.04 (0.06) 0.01 (0.04) 0.07 (0.06) −0.05 (0.04) 0.00 (0.05) −0.11 (0.05) 0.06 (0.07)

Risky behavior −0.11 (0.05) 0.03 (0.06) −0.05 (0.04) 0.08 (0.05) −0.06 (0.04) −0.06 (0.05) −0.02 (0.05) 0.07 (0.06)

Notes. All models controlled for the covariates listed in Table 1. Bolded coefficients in each row indicate significant differences in the associations between relationship quality in the early and late elementary school years
at p < .05 when using a post hoc coefficient comparison (Paternoster et al., 1998). All continuous predictors and outcomes have been standardized to have a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1 and, thus, estimates
correspond to effect sizes. All estimates in parentheses correspond to standard errors.
⁎⁎⁎
p < .001.
⁎⁎
p < .01.

p < .05.
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 71 (2020) 101200
A. Ansari, et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 71 (2020) 101200

more strongly associated with less optimal adolescent outcomes than from a longitudinal perspective. Third, even though schooling is most
conflict in the early grades, especially for assessments of academic often assumed to produce academic outcomes as a function of in­
achievement (i.e., language and literacy, grade point average, math struction, it is clear that the qualities of students' relationships, in­
coursework) and externalizing behavior (for specific estimates, see dicative of the value of the classroom as a social and relational setting,
Table 3). In these cases, the “effects” of conflict in the later years were have unique influences on students' school success and well-being in the
roughly 20% of a standard deviation greater than earlier conflict. long-term, including academic achievement (The ASPEN Institute,
2019). Accordingly, in light of large-scale investments in teacher pro­
Discussion fessional development in an effort to improve student achievement
(Allen et al., 2011; Egert, Fukkink, & Eckhardt, 2018; Pianta et al.,
The results of the present study support the general premise that 2017), the results reported herein suggest that targeting such invest­
teacher-student relationships are consequential for student success and ments to improve teachers' relationship capacity and interaction skills
well-being (Hamre & Pianta, 2001; Howes, 2000; Howes et al., 2008; across grade levels could be a valuable path toward addressing
McCormick & O'Connor, 2015; Pianta & Stuhlman, 2004; Sabol & achievement shortfalls and gaps (see Allen et al., 2011 for an example).
Pianta, 2012). The results also address gaps in understanding of the Such findings also suggest that the development, implementation, and
long-term educational, social, and behavioral outcomes of teacher- expansion across the K-12 years of social-emotional learning interven­
student relationships across students' educational careers by leveraging tions, that focus on both students' and teachers' relational skills can
a prospective longitudinal sample of 1364 American children. The have persisting benefits.
study examines relational closeness and conflict, as reported by stu­ Students form relationships with different teachers each year, and
dents' primary teachers, across the first seven years of school, as related each of these relationships is unique. Given this dynamic, we also
to students' outcomes at the end of their first year in high school, ninth considered the importance variability of closeness and conflict between
grade. Three important themes emerged from the analyses presented. kindergarten and sixth grade. In general, our results revealed that
To begin, even though prior studies have focused on teacher-student variability in closeness was not predictive of students' academic
relationships and young children's concurrent functioning (Burchinal achievement, educational beliefs and aspirations, nor their social-be­
et al., 2002; Heatly & Votruba-Drzal, 2017; Howes, 2000; Lippard et al., havioral outcomes, but variability in conflict (unlike cumulative con­
2018; McCormick & O'Connor, 2015; Nguyen et al., 2020; Pianta & flict) was predictive of students' educational aspirations and social be­
Stuhlman, 2004), the results reported herein add to the literature by havior. Put another way, when students experienced different levels of
highlighting longer-term associations of these early relationships. More conflict from year to year, this had negative consequences for their
specifically, teachers' reports of closeness and conflict with students beliefs about themselves and their behavior. These results regarding
between kindergarten and sixth grade were both uniquely and cumu­ conflict underscore the salience of discontinuity in students' experi­
latively associated with students' academic outcomes, educational as­ ences, and in particular, the importance of shifts in the levels of teacher-
pirations, and social behavioral skills in ninth grade. These associations student conflict (e.g., Emde & Harmon, 2012; Schulenberg &
were above and beyond the contributions of students' characteristics, Maslowsky, 2015). Thus, with respect to child and adolescent devel­
skills and dispositions, and their relationships with their mothers and opment, what these results suggest is that experiencing variability in
fathers. Importantly, however, there were important nuances related to relationships characterized as more (or less) conflictual may have in­
the prediction of closeness and conflict for students' outcomes. Teacher- terrupted students' overall schema of relationships with their teachers
student closeness between kindergarten and sixth grade had cumula­ as supportive and predictable, which in turn had consequences for their
tive, albeit modest, benefits for students' coursework placement, GPA, beliefs and behavior. The above could be particularly harmful if the
educational beliefs and aspirations, and social-behavior in ninth grade, change in conflictual relationships was preceded by acute or chronic
whereas conflict was only cumulatively associated with direct assess­ life stressors that impact children's emotional wellbeing and their be­
ments of achievement and students' GPA. In the main, these results havior and relationships at school (Kim, Conger, Elder Jr, & Lorenz,
suggest that teacher-student closeness has a more differentiated effect, 2003). Thus, this set of results highlight the need for future research to
in the sense of span of influence, on student development (i.e., aca­ more carefully consider shifts and variability in relationship quality
demic achievement, educational beliefs and aspirations, and social over time, especially with regards to students' educational beliefs and
behavior), than does teacher-student conflict. Thus, the outcomes of social behavior. Such work might provide important insight for inter­
teacher-student closeness are not domain specific and not limited to vention and prevention efforts aimed at ensuring that students have
social/motivational processes but also to achievement. In this context continuous high quality experiences.
one might hypothesize that closeness promotes student engagement, Finally, the findings from this study suggest that the benefits of
which has consequences for attention and particularly in instructional teacher-student closeness were largely cumulative, whereas teacher-
activities (hence the achievement effect), perhaps as a consequence of student conflict in the later school years was more strongly predictive of
beliefs and motivation. students' academic achievement and externalizing behavior than earlier
The above pattern of findings has several implications relevant for conflict. Such findings related to closeness are consistent with other
research, theory, and practice. First, it appears that teacher-student results that teacher-student relationships predict student academic en­
closeness and conflict are not two sides of the same coin. This is im­ gagement and acceptance by peers the following year (Hughes & Kwok,
portant for educators and practitioners because what it would suggest is 2006), a pattern that could stabilize and lead to improved performance
that monitoring or implementing interventions aimed at improving in later grades. However, as academic and peer demands intensify in
teachers' closeness and conflict with students is a valuable endeavor the later years of school, conflict with teachers may more readily
that may result in improvements in different domains of student de­ emerge and could initiate a cycle of disengagement from classroom
velopment. With respect to theory, these findings lend support for at­ learning and increasing engagement in interactions and relationships
tachment theory (Ainsworth, 1989), which predicts that students who with peers who are similarly disengaged, which can contribute to
experience positive relationships with adults are better able to rely on feelings and behaviors that perpetuate conflict with teachers (Howes,
them as a developmental asset. Our work adds to this theory by high­ 2000; Hughes, Cavell, & Willson, 2001). There could also be a recency
lighting the importance of relationship's students develop with different effect of conflict on transitions into and through secondary school; for
teachers over time. Put another way, even though teachers are transient example, prior studies have shown that children's relationships with
figures in students' lives, the strength and influence of these relation­ their teachers in fifth grade is linked with the subsequent relationship
ships can (and do) have lasting consequences. Thus, from a theoretical quality and school adjustment over the transition to secondary school
perspective, it is critical to view students' experiences in the classroom (Davidson, Gest, & Welsh, 2010). These associations could persist

9
A. Ansari, et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 71 (2020) 101200

throughout secondary school, and/or be exacerbated at subsequent support of teachers' capacities to form positive relationships with their
transitions, such as the transition from middle to high school when the students.
outcome data was collected for study students. As such, more research
is needed to understand the emergence and consequences of teacher- Declaration of Competing Interest
student conflict in the later years and the complex links with other
relational and developmental processes. None.
Despite these contributions to the literature, the present study in­
cluded a number of limitations. First, although the statistical models References
included a wide range of covariates, the interpretation of results re­
quires caution given the potential for omitted variables; that is, the Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the child behavior checklist. Burlington, VT:
results are correlational and not causal. At the same time, however, the Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont.
models included a robust set of child, family, and neighborhood cov­ Ainsworth, M. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44(4),
709–716.
ariates and replicated across different specifications, which lends Allen, J. P., Pianta, R. C., Gregory, A., Mikami, A. Y., & Lun, J. (2011). An interaction-
greater confidence to our conclusions. Second, the investigation in­ based approach to enhancing secondary school instruction and student achievement.
cluded only a modest number of children from minority and very low- Science, 333, 1034–1037. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1207998.
Ansari, A. (2018). The persistence of preschool effects from early childhood through
income homes, which limits the range of experiences reflected in the adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110, 952–973.
assessments and the analytic sample included only students who com­ Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, A. L. (2006). Measuring cognitive
pleted ninth grade in 2007, which may reflect classroom settings and and psychological engagement: Validation of the student engagement instrument.
Journal of School Psychology, 44, 427–445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2006.04.
teacher capabilities that are not consistent with the focus of present-day 002.
education. We note these aspects of the sampling design and period of Bailey, D., Duncan, G. J., Odgers, C. L., & Yu, W. (2017). Persistence and fadeout in the
data collection as it limits the generalizability of our findings. With that impacts of child and adolescent interventions. Journal of Research on Educational
Effectiveness, 10, 7–39.
said, the patterns of closeness and conflict reported here (both within
Baker, J. A., Grant, S., & Morlock, L. (2008). The teacher-student relationship as a de­
grades and across time) resemble those of children who participated in velopmental context for children with internalizing or externalizing behavior pro­
the ECLS-K between 2010 (kindergarten) and 2014 (third grade). Thus, blems. School Psychology Quarterly, 23, 3–15. https://doi.org/10.1037/1045-3830.
despite the potential changes in the school context (e.g., the increasing 23.1.3.
Belsky, J., Vandell, D. L., Burchinal, M., Clarke-Stewart, K. A., McCartney, K., Owen, M.
academic demands of school), it seems like the trends in relationships T., & NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2007). Are there long-term effects
are somewhat similar. of early child care? Child Development, 78, 681–701. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
The fact that we did not have indicators of teacher-student re­ 8624.2007.01021.x.
Bentler, P. M., & Chou, C. P. (1987). Practical issues in structural modeling. Sociological
lationships after sixth grade and that we only had one assessment of Methods & Research, 16, 78–117. https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124187016001004.
teacher-student relationship each grade are also important limitations. Bergin, C., & Bergin, D. (2009). Attachment in the classroom. Educational Psychology
Indeed, despite the test-retest reliability of the STRS, it is possible that Review, 21(2), 141–170.
Blazar, D., & Pollard, C. (2017). Does test preparation mean low-quality instruction?
student's relationship changes substantially over the course of the Educational Researcher, 46, 420–433.
school year. Nonetheless, the findings regarding the nature and out­ Bos, J. M., Huston, A. C., Granger, R. C., Duncan, G. J., Brock, T. W., & McLoyd, V. C.
comes of teacher-student relationships provide an important long­ (1999). New Hope for people with low incomes: Two-year results of a program to reduce
poverty and reform welfare. New York, NY: Manpower Demonstration Research
itudinal snapshot that has been largely absent in the literature. Indeed, Corporation.
longitudinal studies that include seven years of data on teacher-student Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human devel­
relationships and that have such rich information on numerous long- opment. In W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.). Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 1:
Theoretical models of human development (pp. 793–828). (6th ed.). New York: Wiley.
term outcomes are uncommon. It is also important to note that teacher-
Burchinal, M. R., Lowe Vandell, D., & Belsky, J. (2014). Is the prediction of adolescent
student relationships are inherently dyadic in nature and involve both outcomes from early child care moderated by later maternal sensitivity? Results from
the perspective of the teacher and the student and, thus, our reliance on the nichd study of early child care and youth development. Developmental Psychology,
only teacher-reports is a limitation. Future studies should also consider 50, 542–553. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033709.
Burchinal, M. R., Peisner-Feinberg, E., Pianta, R., & Howes, C. (2002). Development of
the mechanism by which positive teacher-student relationships may academic skills from preschool through second grade: Family and classroom pre­
contribute to positive outcomes among students. Next, this study was dictors of developmental trajectories. Journal of School Psychology, 40, 415–436.
exploratory and, thus, we explored a large number of outcomes; hence, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4405(02)00107-3.
Caldwell, B. M., & Bradley, R. H. (1984). Home observation for measurement of the en­
confirmation and replication with different samples and methods are vironment. Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas at little Rock.
necessary. Finally, even though we used state of the art techniques to Clem, A. L., Rudasill, K. M., Hirvonen, R., Aunola, K., & Kiuru, N. (2020). The roles of
address missing data and included a wide set of variables to improve teacher–student relationship quality and self-concept of ability in adolescents’
achievement emotions: Temperament as a moderator. European Journal of Psychology
the precision of our estimation strategy (see Kline, 2010), the rates of of Education, 1–24.
missing data for some variables were high, which is limitation of our Coley, R. L., & Kull, M. (2016). Cumulative, timing‐specific, and interactive models of
study. residential mobility and children's cognitive and psychosocial skills. Child
Development, 87, 1204–1220. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12535.
With these limitations and future directions in mind, the present
Conger, R. D., & Elder, G. H. (1994). Families in troubled times: Adapting to change in rural
study adds to the extant literature that has considered the concurrent America. New York, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.
and short-term outcomes of student- teacher relationships (Baker, Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO personality inventory manual. Odessa, FL:
Psychological Assessment Resources.
Grant, & Morlock, 2008; Hamre & Pianta, 2001; Heatly & Votruba-
Davidson, A. J., Gest, S. D., & Welsh, J. A. (2010). Relatedness with teachers and peers
Drzal, 2017; Howes, 2000; Lippard et al., 2018; McCormick & during early adolescence: An integrated variable-oriented and person-oriented ap­
O'Connor, 2015; Pianta & Stuhlman, 2004; Spilt et al., 2012), by proach. Journal of School Psychology, 48, 483–510. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.
highlighting the high school outcomes of teacher-student closeness and 2010.08.002.
Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., ...
conflict in the first seven years of education. Collectively, the results Sexton, H. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology,
revealed that teachers' perceptions of closeness and conflict with stu­ 43, 1428–1446. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428.
dents were predictive of a range of outcomes in ninth grade, but in Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody picture vocabulary test– Revised. Circle Pines,
MN: American Guidance Service.
nuanced ways. Furthermore, there was evidence to suggest that the Eccles, J. S. (1999). The development of children ages 6 to 14. Future of Children, 9,
“effects” of closeness were largely cumulative, whereas conflict was 30–44.
more problematic in the older grades. These results reinforce recent Egert, F., Fukkink, R. G., & Eckhardt, A. G. (2018). Impact of in-service professional
development programs for early childhood teachers on quality ratings and child
policy initiatives that call attention to the importance of the classroom outcomes: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 88, 401–433. https://doi.
as a relational setting (The ASPEN Institute, 2019), including the need org/10.3102/0034654317751918.
for more focus on socioemotional learning and the preparation and Emde, R. N., & Harmon, R. J. (Eds.). (2012). Continuities and discontinuities in development.

10
A. Ansari, et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 71 (2020) 101200

Springer Science & Business Media. grade? Developmental Psychology, 54, 2053–2066. https://doi.org/10.1037/
Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a factor in children's academic dev0000584.
engagement and performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1), 148–162. McMahon, R. J., Bierman, K. L., Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., Greenberg, M. T., Lochman, J.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.148. E., & Pinderhughes, E. E. (1999). Initial impact of the fast track prevention trial for
Granot, D. (2014). Teacher-student attachment and student school adaptation: A variable conduct problems: I. The high-risk sample. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
centered and person centered analytical approaches. American Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 631–647. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.67.5.631.
Research, 2, 1005–1014. Medoff-Cooper, B., Carey, W. B., & McDevitt, S. (1993). The Early infancy temperament
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). The social skills rating system. Circle Pines, MN: questionnaire. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 14, 230–235.
American Guidance Service. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004703-199308010-00004.
Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). Early teacher–child relationships and the trajectory Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2001). Childhood predictors differentiate life-course persistent
of children's school outcomes through eighth grade. Child Development, 72, 625–638. and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways among males and females. Development
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00301. and Psychopathology, 13, 355–375. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579401002097.
Heatly, M. C., & Votruba-Drzal, E. (2017). Parent-and teacher-child relationships and Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998-2013). Mplus user’s guide (6th ed.). Los Angeles, CA:
engagement at school entry: Mediating, interactive, and transactional associations Muthén & Muthén.
across contexts. Developmental Psychology, 53, 1042–1062. https://doi.org/10.1037/ National Research Council and Institute of Medicine (2004). Engaging schools: Fostering
dev0000310. high school students' motivation to learn. Washington, DC: The National Academies
Heckman, J. J. (2006). Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged Presshttps://doi.org/10.17226/10421.
children. Science, 312, 1900–1902. Nguyen, T., Ansari, A., Pianta, R. C., Whittaker, J. E., Vitiello, V. E., & Ruzek, E. (2020).
Howes, C. (2000). Social-emotional classroom climate in child care, child-teacher re­ The classroom relational environment and children’s early development in preschool.
lationships and children’s second grade peer relations. Social Development, 9, Social Development. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12447 in press.
191–204. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00119. O’Connor, E., & McCartney, K. (2007). Examining teacher–child relationships and
Howes, C., Burchinal, M., Pianta, R., Bryant, D., Early, D., Clifford, R., & Barbarin, O. achievement as part of an ecological model of development. American Educational
(2008). Ready to learn? Children's pre-academic achievement in pre-kindergarten Research Journal, 44, 340–369. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831207302172.
programs. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23, 27–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. O’Connor, E. E., Collins, B. A., & Supplee, L. (2012). Behavior problems in late childhood:
ecresq.2007.05.002. The roles of early maternal attachment and teacher–child relationship trajectories.
Hughes, J. N., Cavell, T. A., & Willson, V. (2001). Further support for the developmental Attachment & Human Development, 14, 265–288.
significance of the quality of the teacher–student relationship (3rd ed.). Journal of School Pakarinen, E., Silinskas, G., Hamre, B. K., Metsäpelto, R. L., Lerkkanen, M. K., Poikkeus,
Psychology39, 289–301. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4405(01)00074-7. A. M., & Nurmi, J. E. (2018). Cross-lagged associations between problem behaviors
Hughes, J. N., & Kwok, O. M. (2006). Classroom engagement mediates the effect of and teacher-student relationships in early adolescence. The Journal of Early
teacher–student support on elementary students’ peer acceptance: A prospective Adolescence, 38, 1100–1141. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431617714328.
analysis. Journal of School Psychology, 43, 465–480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp. Paternoster, R., Brame, R., Mazerolle, P., & Piquero, A. (1998). Using the correct statis­
2005.10.001. tical test for the equality of regression coefficients. Criminology, 36, 859–866. https://
Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Changes in doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.1998.tb01268.x.
children’s self-competence and values: Gender and domain differences across grades Pianta, R., Hamre, B., Downer, J., Burchinal, M., Williford, A., LoCasale-Crouch, J., ...
one through twelve. Child Development, 73, 509–527. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467- Scott-Little, C. (2017). Early childhood professional development: Coaching and
8624.00421. coursework effects on indicators of children’s school readiness. Early Education and
Jerome, E. M., Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2009). Teacher–child relationships from Development, 28, 956–975. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2017.1319783.
kindergarten to sixth grade: Early childhood predictors of teacher-perceived conflict Pianta, R. C. (1999). Enhancing relationships between children and teachers. Washington, D.
and closeness. Social Development, 18, 915–945. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- C: American Psychological Associationhttps://doi.org/10.1037/10314-000.
9507.2008.00508.x. Pianta, R. C. (2001). STRS: Student-Teacher Relationship Scale: Professional manual.
Jones, D. E., Greenberg, M., & Crowley, M. (2015). Early social-emotional functioning Psychological Assessment Resources.
and public health: The relationship between kindergarten social competence and Pianta, R. C. (2006). Schools, schooling, and developmental psychopathology.
future wellness. American Journal of Public Health, 105, 2283–2290. Developmental psychopathology, volume 1: Theory and method (pp. 494–529). (2nd ed.).
Kim, K. J., Conger, R. D., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Lorenz, F. O. (2003). Reciprocal influences Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
between stressful life events and adolescent internalizing and externalizing problems. Pianta, R. C. (2016). Classroom processes and teacher-student interaction: Integrations
Child Development, 74, 127–143. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00525. with a developmental psychopathology perspective. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.).
Kline, R. B. (2010). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (3rd ed.). New Developmental psychopathology: Risk, resilience, and intervention (pp. 770–814).
York, NY: Guilford Press. Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125556.
Kobak, R., Herres, J., Gaskins, C., & Laurenceau, J. P. (2012). Teacher–student interac­ devpsy415.
tions and attachment states of mind as predictors of early romantic involvement and Pianta, R. C., Steinberg, M. S., & Rollins, K. B. (1995). The first two years of school:
risky sexual behaviors. Attachment & Human Development, 14(3), 289–303. Teacher-child relationships and deflections in children’s classroom adjustment.
Lerner, R. M. (2006). Developmental science, developmental systems, and contemporary Development and Psychopathology, 7, 295–312. https://doi.org/10.1017/
theories of human development. Handbook of child psychology. S0954579400006519.
Li, Y., & Lerner, R. M. (2011). Developmental trajectories of school engagement across Pianta, R. C., & Stuhlman, M. W. (2004). Teacher-child relationships and children’s
adolescence: Implications for academic achievement, substance use, depression, and success in the first years of school. School Psychology Review, 33, 444–459.
delinquency. Developmental Psychology, 47, 233–247. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CESD scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the
a0021307. general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401. https://doi.org/
Lippard, C. N., La Paro, K. M., Rouse, H. L., & Crosby, D. A. (2018). A closer look at 10.1177/014662167700100306.
teacher-child relationships and classroom emotional context in preschool. Child & Roorda, D. L., Koomen, H. M., Spilt, J. L., & Oort, F. J. (2011). The influence of affective
Youth Care Forum, 47, 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10566-017-9414-1. teacher–student relationships on students’ school engagement and achievement: A
Longobardi, C., Prino, L. E., Marengo, D., & Settanni, M. (2016). Student-teacher re­ meta-analytic approach. Review of Educational Research, 81, 493–529.
lationships as a protective factor for school adjustment during the transition from Sabol, T. J., & Pianta, R. C. (2012). Recent trends in research on teacher–child relation­
middle to high school. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg. ships. Attachment & Human Development, 14, 213–231. https://doi.org/10.1080/
2016.01988. 14616734.2012.672262.
Magnuson, K., Duncan, G. J., Lee, K. T., & Metzger, M. W. (2016). Early school adjustment Schulenberg, J., & Maslowsky, J. (2015). Contribution of adolescence to the life course:
and educational attainment. American Educational Research Journal, 53, 1198–1228. What matters most in the long run? Research in Human Development, 12, 319–326.
Martin, A. J., & Dowson, M. (2009). Interpersonal relationships, motivation, engagement, https://doi.org/10.1080/15427609.2015.1068039.
and achievement: Yields for theory, current issues, and educational practice. Review Skinner, E., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., & Kindermann, T. (2008). Engagement and dis­
of Educational Research, 79, 327–365. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654308325583. affection in the classroom: Part of a larger motivational dynamic? Journal of
Martin-Storey, A., & Benner, A. (2019). Externalizing behaviors exacerbate the link be­ Educational Psychology, 100(4), 765–781. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012840.
tween discrimination and adolescent health risk behaviors. Journal of Youth and Spilt, J. L., Hughes, J. N., Wu, J. Y., & Kwok, O. M. (2012). Dynamics of teacher–student
Adolescence, 48, 1724–1735. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-019-01020-3. relationships: Stability and change across elementary school and the influence on
McCallumore, K. M., & Sparapani, E. F. (2010). The importance of the ninth grade on high children’s academic success. Child Development, 83, 1180–1195. https://doi.org/10.
school graduation rates and student success in high school. Education, 130(3), 1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01761.x.
447–456. Susperreguy, M. I., Davis-Kean, P. E., Duckworth, K., & Chen, M. (2018). Self-concept
McCormick, M. P., & O’Connor, E. E. (2015). Teacher–child relationship quality and predicts academic achievement across levels of the achievement distribution: Domain
academic achievement in elementary school: Does gender matter? Journal of specificity for math and reading. Child Development, 89, 2196–2214.
Educational Psychology, 107, 502. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037457. The Aspen Institute (2019). From a Nation at Risk to a Nation at Hope: Recommendations
McFarland, L., Murray, E., & Phillipson, S. (2016). Student–teacher relationships and from the National Commission on Social, Emotional, & Academic Development. Retrieved
student self-concept: Relations with teacher and student gender. Australian Journal of fromhttp://nationathope.org/report-from-the-nation/.
Education, 60, 5–25. Van Ryzin, M. (2010). Secondary school advisors as mentors and secondary attachment
McGrath, K. F., & Van Bergen, P. (2015). Who, when, why and to what end? Students at figures. Journal of Community Psychology, 38, 131–154.
risk of negative student–teacher relationships and their outcomes. Educational Vandell, D. L., Belsky, J., Burchinal, M., Steinberg, L., & Vandergrift, N. (2010). Do effects
Research Review, 14, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2014.12.001. of early child care extend to age 15 years? Results from the NICHD study of early
McKinnon, R. D., Blair, C., & The Family Life Project Investigators (2018). Does early child care and youth development. Child Development, 81, 737–756. https://doi.org/
executive function predict teacher–child relationships from kindergarten to second 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01431.x.

11
A. Ansari, et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 71 (2020) 101200

Vandell, D. L., Burchinal, M., & Pierce, K. M. (2016). Early child care and adolescent Wang, M.-T., & Peck, S. C. (2013). Adolescent educational success and mental health vary
functioning at the end of high school: Results from the NICHD study of Early child across school engagement profiles. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1266–1276. https://
care and youth development. Developmental Psychology, 52, 1634. https://doi.org/10. doi.org/10.1037/a0030028.
1037/dev0000169. Woodcock, R. W., & Johnson, M. B. (1989). Woodcock-Johnson psycho-educational battery-
Verschueren, K., Doumen, S., & Buyse, E. (2012). Relationships with mother, teacher, and revised. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.
peers: Unique and joint effects on young children's self-concept. Attachment & Human Yan, N., Zhou, N., & Ansari, A. (2016). Maternal depression and children’s cognitive and
Development, 14(3), 233–248. socio-emotional development at first grade: The moderating role of classroom emo­
Verschueren, K., & Koomen, H. M. (2012). Teacher–child relationships from an attach­ tional climate. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25, 1247–1256. https://doi.org/10.
ment perspective. Attachment & Human Development, 14, 205–211. https://doi.org/ 1007/s10826-015-0301-9.
10.1080/14616734.2012.672260.

12

You might also like