Accidental Awareness During General Anesthesia

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 29

Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

Official reprint from UpToDate® www.uptodate.com


©2023 UpToDate®

Accidental awareness during general anesthesia


Author: Jaideep J Pandit, MA, BM, DPhil, FRCA, FFPMRCA, DM
Section Editor: Girish P Joshi, MB, BS, MD, FFARCSI
Deputy Editor: Nancy A Nussmeier, MD, FAHA

Contributor Disclosures

All topics are updated as new evidence becomes available and our peer review process is complete.

Literature review current through: Feb 2023. | This topic last updated: Jul 19, 2021.

INTRODUCTION

The term "accidental awareness during general anesthesia" (AAGA) encompasses both
intraoperative consciousness and later explicit recall of intraoperative events. Although the
incidence of AAGA may be reduced with preventive measures, it may not be eradicated
completely. However, AAGA with recall is specifically associated with use of neuromuscular
blocking agents (NMBAs), and is virtually unknown in patients who are not paralyzed. Thus,
while all patients undergoing general anesthesia should be informed that AAGA is rare but
can occur, specific focus should be on those who will receive NMBAs.

This topic will discuss incidence, risk factors, prevention, recognition, and management of
AAGA.

INCIDENCE AND CHARACTERISTICS OF AWARENESS

The incidence of AAGA varies widely due to methodologic differences in postoperative


assessment of awareness and differences in anesthetic practice [1,2]. The key difference in
reported rates depends on whether specific questions were asked in the postoperative
interview (eg, "what was the last thing you remember before anesthesia, then the next
thing, and do you remember anything in-between?"), rather than relying only on patients'
self-reporting of AAGA [3-5]. Most AAGA events occur during induction and emergence,
rather than during the maintenance phase [5]. Experiences range from isolated auditory
perceptions to being fully awake, immobilized, and in pain.
- Page 1 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

Large, prospective, multicenter studies of adults undergoing surgery with general


anesthesia in North America and Europe have consistently reported an incidence of one to
two AAGA occurrences per 1000 patients who were specifically queried [6-11]. Studies
relying on retrospective review of patient-initiated reports or surveys of anesthesiologists
note a much lower incidence of approximately 1 in 14,000 [5,11-14]. The largest such
review relying on patient self-reporting is the fifth National Audit Project (NAP5), which
reported an incidence of 1 in 19,000 occurrences of AAGA in an estimated 3 million general
anesthetics performed across the United Kingdom during 2013 [5,13-15]. Incidence was
much higher in obstetric patients (1 in 600). AAGA events occur more frequently if a
neuromuscular blocking agent (NMBA) is used during the anesthetic [5,6,15,16] (see
'Neuromuscular blockade' below). Also, total intravenous anesthesia (TIVA) has been
associated with a higher risk compared with techniques based on a volatile inhalation
anesthetic agent [5,17,18]. (See 'Total intravenous anesthesia' below.)

RISK FACTORS

Neuromuscular blockade — The most important risk factor for AAGA is the use of a
neuromuscular blocking agent (NMBA) [5,6,16]. Findings from the 5th National Audit
Project (NAP5) in the United Kingdom confirmed that the incidence of AAGA was
approximately 1 in 8000 if an NMBA was administered, compared with approximately 1 in
136,000 when no NMBA was used [5].

This association is understandable. Muscle paralysis removes one of the physiologic signs
of patient awareness (ie, purposeful movement). Since complete paralysis worsens the
psychologic trauma of an AAGA experience, even more so than pain, the potential for long-
term psychologic sequelae may be increased [10,16,19,20]. Explanations of this include the
fact that pain is an experience recognized by all; although unpleasant, the patient has a
pre-existing understanding of it. In contrast, whole body paralysis is an extremely rare
human experience that is described as panic due to being "buried alive," or "being dead"
[21]. In fact, the need for general anesthesia is determined, in part, by the need to prevent
awareness of the distress of paralysis.

The risks posed by use of NMBAs are exacerbated by:

● Failure to employ monitoring of neuromuscular blockade. (See "Monitoring

- Page 2 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

neuromuscular blockade".)

● Inadequate reversal of neuromuscular blockade with sugammadex or neostigmine


[22]. Full reversal of neuromuscular blockade reduces potential for AAGA, as well as
aiding recovery of respiratory function, as discussed in other topics [23,24]. (See
"Clinical use of neuromuscular blocking agents in anesthesia", section on 'Reversal of
neuromuscular block' and "Clinical use of neuromuscular blocking agents in
anesthesia", section on 'Avoidance of residual neuromuscular blockade'.)

Total intravenous anesthesia — Total intravenous anesthesia (TIVA) is associated with a


higher risk for AAGA compared with techniques based on a volatile inhalation anesthetic
agent [5,17,18]. A survey of 172 anesthesiologists in Japan that included 85,156 anesthetics
noted TIVA had been used in 21 of 24 patients who had definite (n = 14) or possible (n = 10)
awareness [17]. However, most of the 85,156 patients included in the survey received a
volatile inhalation agent rather than TIVA alone.

Increased risk with use of a TIVA technique is likely due to lack of availability of monitors of
blood concentration of intravenous (IV) anesthetic agents, which may lead to underdosing.
This is in contrast to inhalation anesthetics, for which continuous monitoring of end-tidal
(ie, exhaled) anesthetic concentration (ETAC) allows real-time dose adjustments [10,11] (see
'End-tidal anesthetic concentration' below). During TIVA, detection of AAGA has relied in
part on patient responses to noxious stimuli to indicate insufficient anesthetic depth.
However, if an NMBA has been administered, movement may be prevented.

Furthermore, disconnection of the IV tubing from the IV catheter or subcutaneous


infiltration of the IV catheter may occur when the limb containing an IV catheter is tucked
at the patient's side or not continuously visible due to surgical draping, with resultant
failure to deliver the intended medication. (See "Intravenous infusion devices for
perioperative use", section on 'Extravasation of a continuous infusion'.)

Use of a smart IV infusion pumps can theoretically prevent pump programming errors,
although it is still possible for a clinician to administer the wrong medication or wrong
concentration (see "Intravenous infusion devices for perioperative use", section on 'Smart
pumps' and "Intravenous infusion devices for perioperative use", section on 'Risks for
medication errors'). Theoretically, use of target-controlled infusion (TCI) devices may
reduce risk of dosage miscalculations that can result in inadequate anesthesia ( figure 1)
[25]. A TCI device was not used to administer TIVA for any of the patients who developed
- Page 3 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

AAGA in a large survey of anesthesiologists [17]. However, no study has conclusively


demonstrated that use of TCI lowers risk of AAGA during TIVA. Furthermore, TCI
technology is not approved by the US Food and Drug Administration (FDA) for use in the
United States. (See "Intravenous infusion devices for perioperative use", section on 'Target-
controlled infusion systems'.)

Brain monitoring such as processed electroencephalography (EEG) monitors (eg, bispectral


index [BIS]) may improve clinical ability to ensure that general anesthesia has produced
unconsciousness during use of TIVA (or other anesthetic techniques) [17,18,26]. (See 'Brain
monitoring' below.)

Other developing technology may allow prediction of propofol concentrations in serum by


determining its concentration in exhaled air, but such methods are not widely available
[27]. (See "Intravenous infusion devices for perioperative use", section on 'Likely future
developments'.)

Surgery-related risk factors — As noted above, surgical procedures associated with


higher risk for AAGA typically involve use of NMBAs (see 'Neuromuscular blockade' above).
Patients undergoing certain procedures (eg, trauma and emergency surgical procedures,
cardiac surgery with cardiopulmonary bypass [CPB], cesarean delivery) are at particularly
high risk [10,16,28].

For trauma or emergency surgery, the time interval between anesthetic induction and
surgical incision is necessarily brief (eg, due to the need to control hemorrhage). Also,
anesthetic depth may be deliberately reduced to limit adverse consequences of
hemodynamic instability. In some institutions, an additional factor for after-hours
emergency or trauma surgery is the need for rapid sequence induction and intubation
(RSII) that may be performed by a junior anesthesia team member [5,29]. Use of thiopental
during anesthetic induction, and not using an opioid (eg, fentanyl) during induction were
other factors in such cases. Specific considerations for these cases are discussed in
separate topics. (See "Anesthesia for adult trauma patients", section on 'Strategies to
minimize risk of awareness' and "Rapid sequence induction and intubation (RSII) for
anesthesia".)

During cardiac surgery, use of CPB may alter pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of
anesthetic agents, which may lead to lighter depth of anesthesia than anticipated and
increased risk for AAGA. (See "Management of cardiopulmonary bypass", section on
- Page 4 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

'Maintenance of anesthesia and neuromuscular blockade'.)

Cesarean section is another procedure associated with higher risk for AAGA [30-32]. In one
study that employed standardized interviews to detect AAGA, the incidence was 1 in 256
patients [32]. Factors that have been associated with AAGA after cesarean section include
the need for RSII that may be performed by a junior anesthesia team member, difficult
airway due to airway changes associated with pregnancy and/or obesity (see 'Patient-
related risk factors' below), the brief time interval between anesthetic induction and
surgical incision (due to the need to immediately deliver the neonate), and use of
thiopental or ketamine to induce general anesthesia [5,29-33]. (See "Anesthesia for
cesarean delivery", section on 'General anesthesia'.)

Patient-related risk factors

● Difficult intubation – Difficulty with intubation in any surgical procedure may lead to
AAGA, presumably due to insufficient anesthesia during prolonged intubation
attempts [16]. Maintenance of adequate anesthesia during attempts is addressed
elsewhere [5,34]. (See "Management of the difficult airway for general anesthesia in
adults", section on 'Planning the anesthetic approach' and "Management of the
difficult airway for general anesthesia in adults", section on 'Induction of anesthesia'.)

If "awake intubation," is planned for a patient with a potentially difficult airway, it is


critically important the need to explain the planned sedation technique to avoid
patient expectation for complete amnesia prior to induction of general anesthesia [5].
(See 'Management of patient expectations' below and "Management of the difficult
airway for general anesthesia in adults", section on 'Awake intubation'.)

● Obesity – The association of obesity with a higher incidence of AAGA may be related
to difficulty with intubation [33,35]. (See "Anesthesia for the patient with obesity",
section on 'Difficulty with airway management'.)

● Resistance or tolerance to anesthetics – Patients with a history of AAGA may be at


higher risk for future events. One observational study noted an adjusted fivefold
higher incidence in patients with a previous AAGA event [36].

• Genetic resistance to anesthetic agents – The NAP5 study reported that 1 in 20


AAGA patients either had a previous event or genetic relative(s) with such event(s)

- Page 5 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

[5]. Genetic variations may result in resistance to hypnotic or amnesic actions of


certain anesthetic agents, although such variations have not been identified in
humans [37].

• Acquired tolerance to anesthetic agents may occur:

- Chronic ethanol use – Cytochrome P450 2E1 is induced by alcohol and


isoniazid [38]. Since the inhalation agents, benzodiazepines, and opioids are
metabolized by the cytochrome P450 category of enzymes, patients with
habitual alcohol intake may require higher doses of these agents. However,
acute alcohol intoxication is typically associated with decreased anesthesia
requirements since alcohol is a central nervous system (CNS) depressant.
Details regarding recognition and perioperative management of excessive
alcohol use are described separately. (See "Identification and management of
unhealthy alcohol use in the perioperative period".)

- Chronic opioid use – Opioid tolerance and/or opioid-induced hyperalgesia


may increase opioid dosing requirements in the perioperative period.
Inadequate analgesia during a surgical procedure results in a higher level of
cortical stimulation due to pain, and may increase risk of AAGA [39,40]. Details
regarding anesthetic management of patients with acute or chronic opioid
use are discussed separately. (See "Perioperative uses of intravenous opioids
in adults: General considerations", section on 'Acute opioid intoxication' and
"Perioperative uses of intravenous opioids in adults: General considerations",
section on 'Chronic opioid use'.)

- Chronic benzodiazepine use – There is an increased expression of excitatory


glutamatergic receptors upon benzodiazepine withdrawal after chronic
exposure, which could underlie the symptoms observed in benzodiazepine
withdrawal syndrome. This overexpression of receptors could theoretically
increase anesthetic requirement and risk of AAGA. (See "Benzodiazepine use
disorder", section on 'Physical dependence'.)

- Other prescription and nonprescription medications – Several prescription


and nonprescription medications induce cytochrome P450 3A (eg, efavirenz,
nevirapine, barbiturates, carbamazepine, glucocorticoids, phenytoin,
rifampicin, St. John's wort), which is involved in the metabolism of opioids [38].
- Page 6 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

Similar to chronic opioid use, chronic administration of these medications may


increase opioid dosing requirements [41].

● Pediatric patients – Although some reports have suggested that the incidence of
AAGA may be slightly higher in children (between 0.2 to 1.2 percent) [42], the NAP5
report noted a negligible incidence in children [5,43]. Assessing the presence of AAGA
in children is particularly challenging due to age-related developmental factors and
questionable accuracy of postoperative interviews [44,45].

Technical issues — Malfunction or misuse of the anesthesia machine is an infrequent


primary cause of AAGA [6]. Problems with anesthetic machines are generally detected by
appropriate alarms and machine checks [46]. (See "Anesthesia machines: Prevention,
diagnosis, and management of malfunctions".)

During a TIVA technique, malfunction or misuse of the IV infusion pumps can lead to
failure to deliver the intended anesthetic agent or underdosing with possible awareness
[25]. Also, disconnection of the IV tubing from the IV catheter or subcutaneous infiltration
of the IV catheter may occur when the limb containing an IV catheter is tucked at the
patient's side table or not continuously visible due to surgical draping, with resultant
failure to deliver the intended medication. In such cases, brain monitoring (eg, with
processed EEG [BIS]) may aid in recognition of inadequate anesthetic depth. (See 'Brain
monitoring' below and "Intravenous infusion devices for perioperative use", section on
'Risks for medication errors'.)

Human factors are more common than technology failure in errors in anesthetic
administration or dosing (see "Prevention of perioperative medication errors", section on
'Types and incidence of errors') [47]. Examples of human error include:

● During administration of IV anesthetic agents, a syringe swap may result in


administration of an NMBA before the anesthetic induction agent [47]. Also, it is
possible for a clinician to administer the wrong concentration of an IV anesthetic even
when a smart IV infusion pump is used due to miscalculation or programming error.
(See "Prevention of perioperative medication errors", section on 'Intravenous
medication errors' and "Intravenous infusion devices for perioperative use", section
on 'Programming errors'.).

● During administration of a volatile inhalation anesthetic agent, it is possible to forget


- Page 7 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

to turn on a vaporizer. However, the low inspired anesthetic agent alarm should
sound ( table 1). (See "Anesthesia machines: Prevention, diagnosis, and
management of malfunctions", section on 'Vaporizer malfunction'.)

PREVENTION

Management of patient expectations — In the preanesthetic consultation, patients


should be provided with brief written information as well as a verbal explanation regarding
their anesthetic plan and what to expect. In particular, patients who report prior episodes
of AAGA may be at higher risk; thus, reassurance regarding additional dosing and
fastidious monitoring is appropriate [48]. (See 'Patient-related risk factors' above.)

The majority of AAGA occur during induction and emergence, rather than during the
maintenance phase of general anesthesia [5,32]. The concept of "neural inertia," (ie,
resistance to changes in consciousness induced by general anesthesia) may explain partial
memory during these periods in some patients [49]. During the preanesthetic
consultation, explaining that the patient may recall experiences such as "a mask on face,"
"tube in mouth," or "weakness as you wake up," will help to manage expectations and
mitigate distress if any such memories are retained.

Notably, patients frequently do not understand the difference between conscious sedation
with monitored anesthesia care versus general anesthesia ( table 2 and table 3) [50].
In the fifth National Audit Project (NAP5) conducted in the United Kingdom, a striking
feature was that reported AAGA was more common with intended sedation compared with
intended general anesthesia [5]. This has also been reported after gastrointestinal
procedures [51] and after dental surgery [52]. Thus, when sedation for monitored
anesthesia care or awake intubation is planned, the consent process should emphasize the
fact that the patient will be somewhat "awake," and "not under deep anesthesia," with an
explanation that some recall is to be expected even if deep sedation is planned [53].

Monitoring — Prevention of AAGA involves ensuring continuous delivery of an


appropriate anesthetic concentration [54]. Historical explanations suggesting that
underdosing of anesthetic agents was the primary problem were oversimplified [20].
Development of risk recognition and mitigation strategies that involve monitoring
techniques aid in ensuring adequate anesthetic depth while avoiding overdoses of

- Page 8 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

anesthetic agents that may have life-threatening consequences ( figure 2).

Three widely available monitoring modalities directly aid in prevention of AAGA:

● End-tidal anesthetic concentration (ETAC) of inhalation agents (when these agents are
in use). (See 'End-tidal anesthetic concentration' below.)

● Neuromuscular blockade monitoring (when a neuromuscular blocking agent [NMBA]


is administered). (See "Monitoring neuromuscular blockade".)

● Electroencephalographic (EEG) monitoring, which is typically a processed EEG such as


the bispectral index (BIS) monitor, which is especially important when a total
intravenous anesthetic (TIVA) technique is used [54]. (See 'Brain monitoring' below.)

Although not widely used or available in the United States, a target-controlled infusion
(TCI) device is likely useful to avoid AAGA during administration of a TIVA technique [17,25].
The TCI device is designed to ensure that appropriate plasma or effect-site concentration(s)
of anesthetic agents are maintained (see "Intravenous infusion devices for perioperative
use", section on 'Target-controlled infusion systems'). Other monitors of awareness that
are not widely used include the isolated forearm technique (IFT) and nociception monitors,
as noted below. (See 'Other monitors and techniques' below.)

Of course, physiologic parameters such as heart rate (HR), blood pressure (BP), respiratory
pattern, muscle tone, and purposeful movement in response to a noxious stimulus are
always monitored [55,56]. However, HR and BP are unreliable for determining
unconsciousness or the degree of anesthetic depth. Although tachycardia and/or
hypertension may occur as a response to pain (indicating insufficient anesthetic depth),
patients with hypovolemia or significant beta-adrenergic blockade often have minimal
change in HR or BP. Conversely, sympathetic activation due to surgical stimuli may cause
hypertension or tachycardia in some patients, even when anesthetic depth is adequate.
Furthermore, purposeful movement as a sign of AAGA, as well as respiratory pattern and
degree of muscle tone usually cannot be assessed in patients who have received an NMBA.
(See 'Neuromuscular blockade' above.)

End-tidal anesthetic concentration — Continuous monitoring of ETAC when inhalation


anesthetic(s) are administered may decrease risk for AAGA because this allows assessment
of whether the concentration of an inhalation agent is close to the desired minimum

- Page 9 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

alveolar concentration (MAC) value (ie, the exhaled concentration of the agent at which 50
percent of patients do not respond with movement to a noxious stimulus) [57]). The MAC
requirement for movement suppression (mediated at the level of the spinal cord) is usually
higher than MAC for suppression of consciousness (ie, MAC-awake) or memory (ie, MAC-
amnesia), which provides a margin of safety to avoid AAGA [58]. Use of inhalation agents
with low blood solubility (eg, sevoflurane and desflurane) allows relatively rapid
adjustments in anesthetic concentration to achieve the desired anesthetic depth.

While ETAC <0.7 MAC for the selected anesthetic agent has been proposed as an
appropriate alarm threshold level, it is not known whether hypnosis and amnesia are
consistently achieved at concentrations ≥0.7 MAC [59-61]. Other important limitations of
ETAC as a monitor of appropriate anesthetic depth include:

● MAC is associated with movement responses, which are diminished or absent if an


NMBA has been administered. (See 'Neuromuscular blockade' above.)

● MAC varies among and even within individuals, and is dependent on age,
pharmacogenetics, temperature, and other factors [62-65]. (See 'Patient-related risk
factors' above and "Inhalation anesthetic agents: Clinical effects and uses", section on
'Sedation and anesthesia'.)

● MAC is not relevant for intravenous (IV) agents. Although administration of IV


anesthetics such as propofol involves analogous concepts (ie, determining a plasma
concentration at which 50 percent of patients respond [Cp50]), direct plasma
concentrations are not available in most clinical settings [66]. Thus, TCI devices that
use pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic mathematical modeling were developed
to improve maintenance of the targeted concentration of an IV anesthetic agent at
the effect site (typically the brain) [25]. (See 'Total intravenous anesthesia' above and
"Intravenous infusion devices for perioperative use", section on 'Target-controlled
infusion systems'.)

Brain monitoring — Brain monitoring such as processed EEG monitors (eg, BIS) may
improve clinical ability to ensure that general anesthesia has produced unconsciousness
during use of TIVA (or other anesthetic techniques) [17,18,26,67].

● Processed EEG – Processed EEG monitors translate the raw EEG signal from the time
domain to the frequency domain via fast Fourier transformation [68,69]. The analysis
- Page 10 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

of the contribution of each frequency band results in a frequency spectrum.


Characteristics of this spectrum are then analyzed to assess anesthetic depth.
Commercially available devices include the BIS, density spectral array, entropy, and
cerebral state index monitors, with continuing development of new models
( table 4) [69-72]. Threshold alarms for possible AAGA are set according to the
device manufacturer's specifications. Evidence that any one monitor performs better
than others is lacking, although most research involves use of the BIS monitor.

In one randomized study of 5228 patients receiving a TIVA anesthetic technique,


AAGA incidence was lower in patients who received BIS-guided monitoring to a target
of 40 to 60 compared with those in which BIS monitoring data was recorded but was
not available to the anesthesiologist (0.14 versus 0.65 percent) [18]. A 2019 meta-
analysis examined comparisons of BIS-guided anesthetic depth versus monitoring
clinical signs only (eg, HR, BP, lacrimal tearing), and/or use of ETAC monitoring [73].
The incidence of AAGA was lower in patients with BIS monitoring (3 per 1000 versus 9
per 1000; odds ratio [OR] 0.36, 95% CI 0.21-0.60; 27 randomized trials with 9765
participants) compared with monitoring clinical signs. Furthermore, in some of these
studies, BIS monitoring reduced time (by a few minutes) to reach indices of recovery
such as eye opening, time to orientation, and time to discharge from the post-
anesthesia care unit (PACU). However, the studies that compared BIS versus ETAC
monitoring to guide anesthetic depth noted no differences in incidence of AAGA (five
studies; 26,572 participants) [73]. Similar results had been noted in a 2014 meta-
analysis comparing BIS monitoring with clinical signs only, or BIS versus ETAC
monitoring [74].

Processed EEG monitors do provide information that supplements physiologic


parameters to aid in assessing anesthetic depth, and may be particularly important
during TIVA techniques since ETAC monitoring is unavailable. Although a general
relationship between ETAC and BIS values has been derived using data obtained from
a large clinical trial, BIS frequently correlates poorly with ETAC, is insensitive to
changes in ETAC, and exhibits wide interindividual variability [75,76]. Furthermore,
BIS values are influenced by electrical activity generated by the muscles, which is
eliminated in patients with complete neuromuscular blockade. In one study in awake
volunteers who received an NMBA but no anesthesia, BIS values declined from
baseline values of approximately 100 to values as low as 40 [77]. Another limitation of

- Page 11 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

most processed EEG monitoring studies has been statistical modelling as if the
monitor is a "therapy," rather than a diagnostic modality. Studies that model
processed EEG as a diagnostic tool that determine sensitivity, specificity, and receiver
operating curves reveal that such brain monitoring cannot distinguish between
awake versus anesthetized states [78]. Thus, processed EEG monitors do not reliably
confirm that a patient is unaware, and are not consistently useful to guide anesthetic
titration or assess adequacy of anesthetic depth and subsequent recovery [79,80].

Importantly, dosing of anesthetic agents in an attempt to maintain adequate


anesthetic depth should not produce processed EEG values that are consistently very
low, particularly if such dosing results in hypotension or blood pressure significantly
lower than baseline values [81,82]. Maintenance of hemodynamic stability with
preservation of vital organ function is ultimately a more important anesthetic goal
since hemodynamic instability can lead to immediate harm [28]. (See "Perioperative
neurocognitive disorders in adults: Risk factors and mitigation strategies", section on
'Avoid excessive depth during general anesthesia' and "Perioperative neurocognitive
disorders in adults: Risk factors and mitigation strategies", section on 'Avoid extremes
of blood pressure'.)

● Raw (unprocessed) EEG – Raw (unprocessed) EEG monitoring provides more useful
information regarding anesthetic depth compared with processed EEG indices, and
provides some evidence of an anesthetic state even after administration of an NMBA
[80,83,84]. When a patient is awake, the raw EEG typically has lower-power, higher-
frequency beta activity (ie, 20 to 30 Hz), and does not have persistent spindles in
theta (4 to 8 Hz) or alpha (8 to 12 Hz) frequencies [55]. The raw EEG waveform during
general anesthesia varies depending on the classes and combinations of anesthetic
agents administered. With administration of propofol or a volatile inhalation
anesthetic (eg, desflurane, sevoflurane, isoflurane) at general anesthetic
concentrations, the EEG waveform typically has a slow delta pattern (0 to 4 Hz), often
coinciding with persistent spindles in theta or alpha frequencies. These EEG markers
of unconsciousness can help distinguish between patient responses to noxious
stimuli that produce only autonomic activity versus those that produce both an
autonomic response and arousal [55]. (See "Neuromonitoring in surgery and
anesthesia", section on 'Electroencephalography' and "Neuromonitoring in surgery
and anesthesia", section on 'Anesthetic effects on neuromonitoring'.)

- Page 12 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

However, use and interpretation of raw rather than processed EEG monitoring is
impractical in many settings since the technology is not available in all operating
rooms, is unfamiliar to many anesthesiologists, and is susceptible to artifact [74,83].

Other monitors and techniques

Isolated forearm technique — The IFT, although not widely used, is another monitoring
technique developed to provide supplemental information to monitor for awareness. One
forearm is isolated from the systemic circulation using a cuffed upper arm tourniquet
inflated after anesthesia induction, but before administration of any NMBA [85].
Theoretically, an awake patient can move their hand/fingers to alert the caregivers.
Interestingly, in a 2015 systematic review that included 1131 patients, 31 percent of the
patients moved in response to verbal command, but no patient ever moved spontaneously
during surgery, and no patient had AAGA [86]. In another study of 100 consecutive patients
who did not receive an NMBA, none moved in response to verbal command, although 24
did move during surgery (and required additional dosing of anesthetic agent) [87]. These
results have led to speculation that a state of dysesthesia may occur, in which sensation is
uncoupled from perceptions [88,89]. Patients in such a dissociated state may have no
recognition or pain due to the surgery (of which they remain technically aware) but can still
attend to their name being called as part of a verbal command. Such studies emphasize
the distinction between awareness at the time of surgery versus awareness with
postoperative recall of intraoperative events. Thus, although IFT monitoring is the only way
in which intraoperative awareness can be contemporaneously detected, such monitoring
may not be useful to assess wakefulness in the absence of neuromuscular blockade.

Nociception monitors — Nociception monitors that measure sympathetic activation in


response to nociception (eg, surgical plethysmographic index [SPI], nociception level index
[NOL], analgesia-nociception index [ANI], pupillary pain index [PPI], and pupillometry
[measurements of pupillary diameter]) have been developed to guide administration of
analgesics (eg, opioids) and sedatives (eg, propofol), and potentially avoid "too light"
anesthetic depth that could result in AAGA [90]. Some studies using these monitors have
noted lower analgesic or sedative doses or shorter emergence times in the groups with
nociception monitoring compared with control groups monitored with clinical signs only
[90-93]. However, nociception monitors are not widely available, and there is no evidence
that AAGA is preventable with their use.

- Page 13 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

Avoidance of complete neuromuscular blockade — Avoiding complete neuromuscular


blockade when possible may minimize the risk of AAGA. Notably, severe postoperative
distress and long-term psychologic harm are most frequently associated with sensations of
paralysis rather than pain. (See 'Neuromuscular blockade' above.)

Complete neuromuscular blockade is not necessary for many surgical procedures [20].
Some degree of muscle relaxation facilitates certain procedures (eg, thoracic and
abdominal surgery). However, in many cases, partial skeletal muscle paralysis (frequently
assessed with a peripheral nerve stimulator) can be used to improve operating conditions
for the surgeon while still allowing some purposeful movement to minimize the risk of
AAGA.

Avoidance of misuse of technical equipment — During administration of inhalation


anesthetic agents, meticulous adherence to published guidelines for a pre-use anesthesia
machine and equipment checkout would prevent most episodes of accidental delivery of
inadequate anesthesia, as well as other critical incidents due to anesthesia workstation
misuse or failure, including failure to deliver adequate levels of anesthesia. Details are
available in another topic. (See "Anesthesia machines: Prevention, diagnosis, and
management of malfunctions".)

During administration of bolus doses of IV anesthetics, incorrect dosing or administration


of the wrong drug may result in underdosing, overdosing, or failure to administer an
appropriate amount of the desired anesthetic agent. Possible solutions include
standardization and improvements in technology, as described in detail in a separate topic.
(See "Prevention of perioperative medication errors", section on 'Intravenous medication
errors' and "Prevention of perioperative medication errors", section on 'Technology
solutions'.)

During infusion of IV anesthetic agents, possible awareness can result from failure to
deliver the intended agent or anesthetic underdosing, although overdosing can also occur.
Although use of smart pumps may reduce risk, safety and efficacy depends primarily on
appropriate use of the medication library incorporated within the pump for (rather than
manual programming that is possible to bypass these safeguards). Avoidance of errors in
anesthetic delivery is discussed in other topics. (See "Intravenous infusion devices for
perioperative use", section on 'Risks for medication errors'.)

Administration of adjuvant medications — If an episode of AAGA is suspected, adverse


- Page 14 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

consequence may be limited by administering an adjuvant sedative or analgesic agent as


soon as possible, as well as deepening anesthesia if appropriate [54]. Typical adjuvant
agents include:

● Benzodiazepines – A benzodiazepine may achieve an amnestic effect. The extent to


which a preoperative dose of a benzodiazepine (eg, 2 mg IV midazolam) may prevent
AAGA or its consequences is unknown.

● Opioids and other analgesics – Analgesic agents may minimize pain in response to
noxious stimuli during and immediately after an AAGA event [94-96]. Thus, it is often
appropriate to administer an opioid dose if AAGA is suspected, although large opioid
doses are avoided [97].

Professionalism — To reduce risk for extreme distress due to later recall of intraoperative
events, the operating room staff should maintain professionalism in all discussions
regarding the patient, regardless of whether or not the patient is assumed to be
unconscious.

POSTOPERATIVE RECOGNITION AND MANAGEMENT

Recognizing that AAGA may have occurred is important so that discussion, reassurance,
and further counseling can be offered to mitigate distress. Psychologic symptoms occur in
33 to 69 percent of adult patients who experience AAGA, but severity is variable
[6,7,21,56,98-104]. Patients may experience significant symptoms similar to other causes
of posttraumatic stress such as recurrent dreams or nightmares, sleep disturbance,
intrusive thoughts, flashbacks, exaggerated startle response, hypervigilance, or avoidance
of stimuli associated with the trauma. However, approximately half of patients reporting
AAGA may not experience distress [21].

Early emotional responses after an awareness event are risk factors for later development
of more severe post-event psychologic disturbances such as posttraumatic stress disorder
(PTSD) [21,98,104]. Distress and long-term psychologic harm were most frequently
associated with sensations of paralysis rather than pain (see 'Neuromuscular blockade'
above), and the combination was particularly distressing [21]. A subgroup of patients
resort to formal complaint or register malpractice claims. In a closed claims analysis of
cases of awake paralysis, a judgement of substandard clinical care (eg, administration of a
- Page 15 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

neuromuscular blocking agent [NMBA] before or without general anesthesia due to


syringe swaps or labelling error) was rendered in a higher percentage of cases of awake
paralysis compared with cases of AAGA without paralysis (94 versus 43 percent) [100].

Importantly, contemporaneous intraoperative recognition by the anesthesiologist that


AAGA is possible may be helpful if discussion and reassurance regarding this possibility are
offered in the postoperative period to mitigate distress [21]. Regardless of whether
intraoperative AAGA is suspected, an explicit query about AAGA in the immediate
postoperative period and during the anesthesiologist's follow-up visit is important
[3,56,98]. However, signs and symptoms of distress may be delayed, and patients may not
talk to their clinicians about AAGA unless specifically asked. Thus, after the patient is
discharged from the hospital, the surgeon and primary care clinician should be vigilant in
identifying potential early signs of a psychologic disturbance.

Analysis of the psychologic impact of AAGA in the NAP5 study has led to specific
recommendations for a support pathway, consisting of three broad steps ( figure 3) [53]:

● The "meeting," stage, during which the anesthesiologist involved in the patient's care,
along with others, listen carefully to patient’s story to detail and understand their
experience. The default position is to accept the patient's story as their genuine
experience and express regret that the event has happened (this does not constitute
an admission of liability). This meeting is an opportunity to start arrangements for
consult with a local clinical psychologist or psychiatrist.

● The "analysis," stage, where details of the case are examined to seek a cause for the
patient’s report and determine whether possible or probable AAGA occurred. This
may involve review of charts and records, as well as staff interviews. The NAP5 study
advises recording the degree of likelihood that the AAGA is genuine based on the
report and clinical context, as well as seeking an external expert review.

● The "support," stage seeks early detection of clinical impact in the first 24 hours. The
four cardinal signs are flashbacks, nightmares, new anxiety states, and depression.
Then, active follow up occurs at two weeks. If clinical impact is present and persists,
formal referral to psychiatric/psychologic services.

Further details regarding assessment and treatment of PTSD are available in separate
topics:
- Page 16 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia
● (See "Posttraumatic stress disorder in adults: Epidemiology, pathophysiology, clinical
manifestations, course, assessment, and diagnosis".)
● (See "Management of posttraumatic stress disorder in adults".)
● (See "Psychotherapy and psychosocial interventions for posttraumatic stress disorder
in adults".)

PUBLISHED GUIDELINES

Published guidelines regarding AAGA have been developed, although these are primarily
based on expert opinion.

The American Society of Anesthesiologists (ASA) 2006 guidelines state that the
anesthesiologist should [28]:

● Identify risk factors for AAGA. Patients are informed about this possibility, particularly
those with risk factors. (See 'Risk factors' above.)

● Use a checklist protocol for anesthesia machines and equipment ( table 5). (See
"Anesthesia machines: Prevention, diagnosis, and management of malfunctions",
section on 'Standardized anesthesia machine checkout' and "Anesthesia machines:
Prevention, diagnosis, and management of malfunctions", section on 'American
Society of Anesthesiologists Anesthesia Machine Checkout'.)

● Use brain monitoring such as processed electroencephalography (EEG) in patients


with risk factors for AAGA. However, attempts to maintain brain monitoring targets
should not conflict with patient safety priorities (eg, maintenance of hemodynamic
stability and vital organ protection). (See 'Brain monitoring' above.)

● Administer an intraoperative dose of benzodiazepine as an adjuvant agent if AAGA is


suspected, as well as deepening anesthesia as appropriate. (See 'Administration of
adjuvant medications' above.)

Guidelines from the 5th National Audit Project (NAP5) in the United Kingdom differ
somewhat [5]. The NAP5 study included made 64 recommendations, including seven for
national regulations, 12 for hospitals, and 45 for individual clinicians. These include:

● Ensure that neuromuscular blocking agents (NMBAs) are managed appropriately,


- Page 17 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

including avoiding total neuromuscular blockade when feasible, fastidious


monitoring of the degree of blockade, and ensuring that reversal of blockade is
complete. (See 'Neuromuscular blockade' above and 'Avoidance of complete
neuromuscular blockade' above.)

● Use checklists, communication and alarms to ensure maintenance of anesthesia


delivery. (See "Anesthesia machines: Prevention, diagnosis, and management of
malfunctions", section on 'Standardized anesthesia machine checkout'.)

● Ensure that the consent process manages patient expectations. (See 'Management of
patient expectations' above.)

● Use support pathways for patients who report AAGA [53]. (See 'Postoperative
recognition and management' above.)

Guidelines from the Association of Anaesthetists and the Society for Intravenous
Anaesthesia for use of total intravenous anesthesia (TIVA) include recommendations to
(see 'Total intravenous anesthesia' above) [25]:

● Use a target-controlled infusion (TCI) device for TIVA techniques (notably, these
devices are not available in the United States). (See "Intravenous infusion devices for
perioperative use", section on 'Target-controlled infusion systems'.)

● Use processed EEG or other brain monitoring whenever an NMBA is used with TIVA.
(See 'Brain monitoring' above.)

● Maintain visibility of the peripheral intravenous (IV) or central venous catheter


through which the IV infusion is being delivered. (See "Intravenous infusion devices
for perioperative use", section on 'Extravasation of a continuous infusion'.)

Similar guidelines have been developed by professional societies and national


organizations [67,105].

SUMMARY AND RECOMMENDATIONS

● Accidental awareness during anesthesia (AAGA) is rare (with an incidence of 1 to 2


events per 1000 anesthetics when patients are specifically queried regarding

- Page 18 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

perioperative recall). Most AAGA events during general anesthesia occur during
induction and emergence, rather than during the maintenance phase. Experiences
range from isolated auditory perceptions to being fully awake, immobilized, and in
pain.(See 'Incidence and characteristics of awareness' above.)

● Risk factors for AAGA include:

• Use of a neuromuscular blocking agent (NMBA). The incidence of self-reported


AAGA is approximately 1 in 8000 when an NMBA is used compared with 1 in
136,000 when no NMBA is used. (See 'Neuromuscular blockade' above.)

• Surgical procedures associated with use of an NMBA, rapid sequence induction, or


lighter anesthetic depth (eg, trauma and emergency surgical procedures, cardiac
surgery with cardiopulmonary bypass [CPB], cesarean delivery). (See 'Surgery-
related risk factors' above.)

• Patient-related risk factors (eg, difficulty with intubation, obesity, genetic


resistance or acquired tolerance to anesthetic agents). (See 'Patient-related risk
factors' above.)

• Total intravenous anesthesia (TIVA) technique. (See 'Total intravenous anesthesia'


above.)

• Technical issues such as malfunction or misuse of the anesthesia machine or


intravenous (IV) infusion pumps, which are most commonly related to human
error. (See 'Technical issues' above.)

● Prevention of AAGA involves ensuring continuous delivery of an appropriate


anesthetic concentration. Monitoring techniques developed to recognize and
mitigate risk of inadequate anesthetic depth with AAGA, while avoiding anesthetic
overdosing include:

• End-tidal anesthetic concentration (ETAC) monitoring during use of inhalation


agents to assess whether the anesthetic dose is close to the desired minimum
alveolar concentration (MAC) value for the selected agent, with alarms set to
detect ETAC concentrations that are too low (eg, <0.7 MAC). (See 'End-tidal
anesthetic concentration' above.)

- Page 19 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

• Brain monitoring with processed or raw electroencephalography (EEG) and use of


a target-controlled infusion (TCI) device (if available) when a TIVA technique is
used, particularly if an NMBA is administered. (See 'Brain monitoring' above.)

● Additional prevention strategies to avoid or minimize the consequences of AAGA


include:

• Mentioning the small risk of AAGA during the consent process, with an
explanation that brief recollections at induction and emergence are not
uncommon. If awake intubation or sedation for monitored anesthesia care is
planned (rather than general anesthesia), the consent process emphasizes that
some recall is to be expected. (See 'Management of patient expectations' above.)

• Avoiding complete neuromuscular blockade when possible to minimize risk of


AAGA. Interpret any movement during anesthesia as possible AAGA, and deepen
anesthesia accordingly. Notably, severe postoperative distress and long-term
psychologic harm are most frequently associated with sensations of paralysis
rather than pain. (See 'Neuromuscular blockade' above and 'Avoidance of
complete neuromuscular blockade' above.)

• Using of technical equipment to deliver anesthetic agents appropriately, including


meticulous adherence to published guidelines for anesthesia machine checkout
and use, and appropriate use of IV infusion devices. (See 'Avoidance of misuse of
technical equipment' above.)

• Administering adjuvant sedatives or analgesics that may limit adverse


consequences as soon as possible if an intraoperative AAGA event is suspected.
(See 'Administration of adjuvant medications' above.)

• Maintaining professionalism during all intraoperative discussions even if the


patient is assumed to be unconscious.

● Recognizing that AAGA may have occurred is important so that discussion,


reassurance, and further counseling can be offered to alleviate any psychological
symptoms similar to posttraumatic stress (eg, recurrent dreams or nightmares, sleep
disturbance, intrusive thoughts, flashbacks, exaggerated startle response,
hypervigilance, avoidance of stimuli associated with the trauma). (See 'Postoperative

- Page 20 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

recognition and management' above.)

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The editorial staff at UpToDate acknowledge Jeffrey H Silverstein, MD, now deceased, who
contributed to an earlier version of this topic review.

The editorial staff at UpToDate also acknowledge George Mashour, MD, PhD, and Michael
Avidan, MD, who contributed to an earlier version of this topic review.

REFERENCES

1. Mashour GA, Wang LY, Turner CR, et al. A retrospective study of intraoperative
awareness with methodological implications. Anesth Analg 2009; 108:521.
2. Kerssens C, Klein J, Bonke B. Awareness: Monitoring versus remembering what
happened. Anesthesiology 2003; 99:570.
3. Brice DD, Hetherington RR, Utting JE. A simple study of awareness and dreaming
during anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1970; 42:535.
4. Mashour GA, Kent C, Picton P, et al. Assessment of intraoperative awareness with
explicit recall: a comparison of 2 methods. Anesth Analg 2013; 116:889.
5. Pandit JJ, Andrade J, Bogod DG, et al. 5th National Audit Project (NAP5) on accidental
awareness during general anaesthesia: summary of main findings and risk factors. Br
J Anaesth 2014; 113:549.
6. Sandin RH, Enlund G, Samuelsson P, Lennmarken C. Awareness during anaesthesia: a
prospective case study. Lancet 2000; 355:707.

7. Sebel PS, Bowdle TA, Ghoneim MM, et al. The incidence of awareness during
anesthesia: a multicenter United States study. Anesth Analg 2004; 99:833.
8. Orser BA, Mazer CD, Baker AJ. Awareness during anesthesia. CMAJ 2008; 178:185.
9. Pollard RJ, Coyle JP, Gilbert RL, Beck JE. Intraoperative awareness in a regional medical
system: a review of 3 years' data. Anesthesiology 2007; 106:269.
10. Errando CL, Sigl JC, Robles M, et al. Awareness with recall during general anaesthesia:
a prospective observational evaluation of 4001 patients. Br J Anaesth 2008; 101:178.
11. Xu L, Wu AS, Yue Y. The incidence of intra-operative awareness during general

- Page 21 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

anesthesia in China: a multi-center observational study. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 2009;


53:873.
12. Bombardieri AM, Mathur S, Soares A, et al. Intraoperative Awareness With Recall: A
Descriptive, Survey-Based, Cohort Study. Anesth Analg 2019; 129:1291.
13. Pandit JJ, Cook TM, Jonker WR, et al. A national survey of anaesthetists (NAP5 baseline)
to estimate an annual incidence of accidental awareness during general anaesthesia
in the UK. Br J Anaesth 2013; 110:501.
14. Jonker WR, Hanumanthiah D, O'Sullivan EP, et al. A national survey (NAP5-Ireland
baseline) to estimate an annual incidence of accidental awareness during general
anaesthesia in Ireland. Anaesthesia 2014; 69:969.
15. Sury MR, Palmer JH, Cook TM, Pandit JJ. The state of UK anaesthesia: a survey of
National Health Service activity in 2013. Br J Anaesth 2014; 113:575.

16. Ghoneim MM, Block RI, Haffarnan M, Mathews MJ. Awareness during anesthesia: risk
factors, causes and sequelae: a review of reported cases in the literature. Anesth
Analg 2009; 108:527.

17. Morimoto Y, Nogami Y, Harada K, et al. Awareness during anesthesia: the results of a
questionnaire survey in Japan. J Anesth 2011; 25:72.

18. Zhang C, Xu L, Ma YQ, et al. Bispectral index monitoring prevent awareness during
total intravenous anesthesia: a prospective, randomized, double-blinded, multi-center
controlled trial. Chin Med J (Engl) 2011; 124:3664.
19. Cook TM, Andrade J, Bogod DG, et al. 5th National Audit Project (NAP5) on accidental
awareness during general anaesthesia: patient experiences, human factors, sedation,
consent, and medicolegal issues. Br J Anaesth 2014; 113:560.
20. Tasbihgou SR, Vogels MF, Absalom AR. Accidental awareness during general
anaesthesia - a narrative review. Anaesthesia 2018; 73:112.
21. Cook TM, Andrade J, Bogod DG, et al. The 5th National Audit Project (NAP5) on
accidental awareness during general anaesthesia: patient experiences, human factors,
sedation, consent and medicolegal issues. Anaesthesia 2014; 69:1102.
22. Hristovska AM, Duch P, Allingstrup M, Afshari A. Efficacy and safety of sugammadex
versus neostigmine in reversing neuromuscular blockade in adults. Cochrane
Database Syst Rev 2017; 8:CD012763.
23. Schumacher J, Arlidge J, Dudley D, et al. The impact of respiratory protective
- Page 22 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

equipment on difficult airway management: a randomised, crossover, simulation


study. Anaesthesia 2020; 75:1301.
24. Pandit JJ, Eriksson LI. Reversing Neuromuscular Blockade: Not Just the Diaphragm, but
Carotid Body Function Too. Anesthesiology 2019; 131:453.
25. Nimmo AF, Absalom AR, Bagshaw O, et al. Guidelines for the safe practice of total
intravenous anaesthesia (TIVA): Joint Guidelines from the Association of Anaesthetists
and the Society for Intravenous Anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 2019; 74:211.
26. Soehle M, Kuech M, Grube M, et al. Patient state index vs bispectral index as measures
of the electroencephalographic effects of propofol. Br J Anaesth 2010; 105:172.
27. Perl T, Carstens E, Hirn A, et al. Determination of serum propofol concentrations by
breath analysis using ion mobility spectrometry. Br J Anaesth 2009; 103:822.
28. American Society of Anesthesiologists Task Force on Intraoperative Awareness.
Practice advisory for intraoperative awareness and brain function monitoring: a report
by the american society of anesthesiologists task force on intraoperative awareness.
Anesthesiology 2006; 104:847.

29. El-Orbany M, Connolly LA. Rapid sequence induction and intubation: current
controversy. Anesth Analg 2010; 110:1318.
30. Odor PM, Bampoe S, Lucas DN, et al. Protocol for direct reporting of awareness in
maternity patients (DREAMY): a prospective, multicentre cohort study of accidental
awareness during general anaesthesia. Int J Obstet Anesth 2020; 42:47.
31. Odor PM, Bampoe S, Moonesinghe SR, et al. General anaesthetic and airway
management practice for obstetric surgery in England: a prospective, multicentre
observational study. Anaesthesia 2021; 76:460.
32. Odor PM, Bampoe S, Lucas DN, et al. Incidence of accidental awareness during
general anaesthesia in obstetrics: a multicentre, prospective cohort study.
Anaesthesia 2021; 76:759.
33. Palmer J, Sury M, Nightingale C, et al. Underestimating the size of the problem? UK
anaesthesia for patients with obesity. Obes Res Clin Pract 2017; 11:703.
34. O'Sullivan E, Laffey J, Pandit JJ. A rude awakening after our fourth 'NAP': lessons for
airway management. Anaesthesia 2011; 66:331.
35. Members of the Working Party, Nightingale CE, Margarson MP, et al. Peri-operative
management of the obese surgical patient 2015: Association of Anaesthetists of Great
- Page 23 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

Britain and Ireland Society for Obesity and Bariatric Anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 2015;
70:859.

36. Aranake A, Gradwohl S, Ben-Abdallah A, et al. Increased risk of intraoperative


awareness in patients with a history of awareness. Anesthesiology 2013; 119:1275.

37. Sleigh JW, Leslie K, Davidson AJ, et al. Genetic Analysis of Patients Who Experienced
Awareness with Recall while under General Anesthesia. Anesthesiology 2019; 131:974.

38. Flockhart D. Drug Interactions: Cytochrome P450 Drug Interaction Table. Indiana Univ
ersity School of Medicine 2007. http://medicine.iupui.edu/clinpharm/ddis/table.asp (Ac
cessed on July 21, 2009).

39. Vadivelu N, Mitra S, Kaye AD, Urman RD. Perioperative analgesia and challenges in the
drug-addicted and drug-dependent patient. Best Pract Res Clin Anaesthesiol 2014;
28:91.

40. Pulley DD. Preoperative Evaluation of the Patient with Substance Use Disorder and
Perioperative Considerations. Anesthesiol Clin 2016; 34:201.

41. Nieminen TH, Hagelberg NM, Saari TI, et al. St John's wort greatly reduces the
concentrations of oral oxycodone. Eur J Pain 2010; 14:854.

42. Davidson AJ, Smith KR, Blussé van Oud-Alblas HJ, et al. Awareness in children: a
secondary analysis of five cohort studies. Anaesthesia 2011; 66:446.

43. Sury MR, Arumainathan R, Belhaj AM, et al. The state of UK pediatric anesthesia: a
survey of National Health Service activity. Paediatr Anaesth 2015; 25:1085.

44. Davidson AJ, Huang GH, Czarnecki C, et al. Awareness during anesthesia in children: a
prospective cohort study. Anesth Analg 2005; 100:653.

45. Blussé van Oud-Alblas HJ, van Dijk M, Liu C, et al. Intraoperative awareness during
paediatric anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 2009; 102:104.

46. Checketts MR, Alladi R, Ferguson K, et al. Recommendations for standards of


monitoring during anaesthesia and recovery 2015: Association of Anaesthetists of
Great Britain and Ireland. Anaesthesia 2016; 71:85.

47. Sivia DS, Pandit JJ. Mathematical model of the risk of drug error during anaesthesia:
the influence of drug choices, injection routes, operation duration and fatigue.
Anaesthesia 2019; 74:992.

48. Kent CD, Mashour GA, Metzger NA, et al. Psychological impact of unexpected explicit
recall of events occurring during surgery performed under sedation, regional
- Page 24 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

anaesthesia, and general anaesthesia: data from the Anesthesia Awareness Registry.
Br J Anaesth 2013; 110:381.

49. Sepúlveda PO, Tapia LF, Monsalves S. Neural inertia and differences between loss of
and recovery from consciousness during total intravenous anaesthesia: a narrative
review. Anaesthesia 2019; 74:801.

50. Esaki RK, Mashour GA. Levels of consciousness during regional anesthesia and
monitored anesthesia care: patient expectations and experiences. Anesth Analg 2009;
108:1560.

51. Kuzhively J, Pandit JJ. Anesthesia and airway management for gastrointestinal
endoscopic procedures outside the operating room. Curr Opin Anaesthesiol 2019;
32:517.

52. Sury MR, Palmer JH, Cook TM, Pandit JJ. The State of UK Dental Anaesthesia: Results
From The NAP5 Activity Survey. A national survey by the 5th National Audit Project of
the Royal College of Anaesthetists and the Association of Anaesthetists of Great
Britain and Ireland. SAAD Dig 2016; 32:34.

53. NAP5 Anaesthesia Awareness Support Pack September 2014. Available at: https://ww
w.nationalauditprojects.org.uk/downloads/NAP5-SUPPORTPACK.pdf (Accessed on Febr
uary 07, 2021).

54. Messina AG, Wang M, Ward MJ, et al. Anaesthetic interventions for prevention of
awareness during surgery. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2016; 10:CD007272.

55. Reshef ER, Schiff ND, Brown EN. A Neurologic Examination for Anesthesiologists:
Assessing Arousal Level during Induction, Maintenance, and Emergence.
Anesthesiology 2019; 130:462.

56. Moerman N, Bonke B, Oosting J. Awareness and recall during general anesthesia.
Facts and feelings. Anesthesiology 1993; 79:454.

57. Eger EI 2nd, Saidman LJ, Brandstater B. Minimum alveolar anesthetic concentration: a
standard of anesthetic potency. Anesthesiology 1965; 26:756.

58. Antognini JF, Schwartz K. Exaggerated anesthetic requirements in the preferentially


anesthetized brain. Anesthesiology 1993; 79:1244.

59. Avidan MS, Zhang L, Burnside BA, et al. Anesthesia awareness and the bispectral
index. N Engl J Med 2008; 358:1097.

60. Myles PS, Leslie K, McNeil J, et al. Bispectral index monitoring to prevent awareness
- Page 25 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

during anaesthesia: the B-Aware randomised controlled trial. Lancet 2004; 363:1757.
61. Shanks AM, Avidan MS, Kheterpal S, et al. Alerting thresholds for the prevention of
intraoperative awareness with explicit recall: a secondary analysis of the Michigan
Awareness Control Study. Eur J Anaesthesiol 2015; 32:346.
62. Liem EB, Lin CM, Suleman MI, et al. Anesthetic requirement is increased in redheads.
Anesthesiology 2004; 101:279.
63. Eger EI 2nd. Age, minimum alveolar anesthetic concentration, and minimum alveolar
anesthetic concentration-awake. Anesth Analg 2001; 93:947.
64. Nickalls RW, Mapleson WW. Age-related iso-MAC charts for isoflurane, sevoflurane and
desflurane in man. Br J Anaesth 2003; 91:170.
65. Kanazawa S, Oda Y, Maeda C, Okutani R. Age-dependent decrease in desflurane
concentration for maintaining bispectral index below 50. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand
2016; 60:177.
66. Smith C, McEwan AI, Jhaveri R, et al. The interaction of fentanyl on the Cp50 of
propofol for loss of consciousness and skin incision. Anesthesiology 1994; 81:820.
67. Chan MTV, Hedrick TL, Egan TD, et al. American Society for Enhanced Recovery and
Perioperative Quality Initiative Joint Consensus Statement on the Role of
Neuromonitoring in Perioperative Outcomes: Electroencephalography. Anesth Analg
2020; 130:1278.
68. Rampil IJ. A primer for EEG signal processing in anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1998;
89:980.
69. Fahy BG, Chau DF. The Technology of Processed Electroencephalogram Monitoring
Devices for Assessment of Depth of Anesthesia. Anesth Analg 2018; 126:111.
70. Bruhn J, Myles PS, Sneyd R, Struys MM. Depth of anaesthesia monitoring: what's
available, what's validated and what's next? Br J Anaesth 2006; 97:85.
71. Bowdle TA. Depth of anesthesia monitoring. Anesthesiol Clin 2006; 24:793.
72. Voss L, Sleigh J. Monitoring consciousness: the current status of EEG-based depth of
anaesthesia monitors. Best Pract Res Clin Anaesthesiol 2007; 21:313.
73. Lewis SR, Pritchard MW, Fawcett LJ, Punjasawadwong Y. Bispectral index for improving
intraoperative awareness and early postoperative recovery in adults. Cochrane
Database Syst Rev 2019; 9:CD003843.

- Page 26 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

74. Punjasawadwong Y, Phongchiewboon A, Bunchungmongkol N. Bispectral index for


improving anaesthetic delivery and postoperative recovery. Cochrane Database Syst
Rev 2014; :CD003843.
75. Whitlock EL, Villafranca AJ, Lin N, et al. Relationship between bispectral index values
and volatile anesthetic concentrations during the maintenance phase of anesthesia in
the B-Unaware trial. Anesthesiology 2011; 115:1209.
76. Gaskell A, Sanders RD, Sleigh J. Using EEG markers to titrate anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth
2018; 121:327.
77. Schuller PJ, Newell S, Strickland PA, Barry JJ. Response of bispectral index to
neuromuscular block in awake volunteers. Br J Anaesth 2015; 115 Suppl 1:i95.
78. Pandit JJ, Cook TM. National Institute for Clinical Excellence guidance on measuring
depth of anaesthesia: limitations of EEG-based technology. Br J Anaesth 2014; 112:385.
79. Epstein RH, Maga JM, Mahla ME, et al. Prevalence of discordant elevations of state
entropy and bispectral index in patients at amnestic sevoflurane concentrations: a
historical cohort study. Can J Anaesth 2018; 65:512.
80. Avidan MS, Graetz TJ. Monitoring the brain strikes a discordant note for
anesthesiologists. Can J Anaesth 2018; 65:501.
81. Aldecoa C, Bettelli G, Bilotta F, et al. European Society of Anaesthesiology evidence-
based and consensus-based guideline on postoperative delirium. Eur J Anaesthesiol
2017; 34:192.
82. Berger M, Schenning KJ, Brown CH 4th, et al. Best Practices for Postoperative Brain
Health: Recommendations From the Fifth International Perioperative Neurotoxicity
Working Group. Anesth Analg 2018; 127:1406.
83. Bennett C, Voss LJ, Barnard JP, Sleigh JW. Practical use of the raw
electroencephalogram waveform during general anesthesia: the art and science.
Anesth Analg 2009; 109:539.
84. Purdon PL, Sampson A, Pavone KJ, Brown EN. Clinical Electroencephalography for
Anesthesiologists: Part I: Background and Basic Signatures. Anesthesiology 2015;
123:937.
85. Russell IF. Fourteen fallacies about the isolated forearm technique, and its place in
modern anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 2013; 68:677.
86. Pandit JJ, Russell IF, Wang M. Interpretations of responses using the isolated forearm
- Page 27 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

technique in general anaesthesia: a debate. Br J Anaesth 2015; 115 Suppl 1:i32.


87. Pandit JJ. An observational study of the 'isolated forearm technique' in unparalysed,
spontaneously breathing patients. Anaesthesia 2015; 70:1369.
88. Pandit JJ. Isolated forearm - or isolated brain? Interpreting responses during
anaesthesia - or 'dysanaesthesia'. Anaesthesia 2013; 68:995.
89. Pandit JJ. Acceptably aware during general anaesthesia: 'dysanaesthesia'--the
uncoupling of perception from sensory inputs. Conscious Cogn 2014; 27:194.
90. Anderson TA. Intraoperative Analgesia-Nociception Monitors: Where We Are and
Where We Want To Be. Anesth Analg 2020; 130:1261.
91. Gruenewald M, Harju J, Preckel B, et al. Comparison of adequacy of anaesthesia
monitoring with standard clinical practice monitoring during routine general
anaesthesia: An international, multicentre, single-blinded randomised controlled trial.
Eur J Anaesthesiol 2021; 38:73.
92. Meijer FS, Martini CH, Broens S, et al. Nociception-guided versus Standard Care during
Remifentanil-Propofol Anesthesia: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Anesthesiology
2019; 130:745.
93. Funcke S, Pinnschmidt HO, Brinkmann C, et al. Nociception level-guided opioid
administration in radical retropubic prostatectomy: a randomised controlled trial. Br J
Anaesth 2021; 126:516.
94. Ghouri AF, White PF. Effect of fentanyl and nitrous oxide on the desflurane anesthetic
requirement. Anesth Analg 1991; 72:377.
95. McEwan AI, Smith C, Dyar O, et al. Isoflurane minimum alveolar concentration
reduction by fentanyl. Anesthesiology 1993; 78:864.
96. Katoh T, Kobayashi S, Suzuki A, et al. The effect of fentanyl on sevoflurane
requirements for somatic and sympathetic responses to surgical incision.
Anesthesiology 1999; 90:398.
97. Levy N, Quinlan J, El-Boghdadly K, et al. An international multidisciplinary consensus
statement on the prevention of opioid-related harm in adult surgical patients.
Anaesthesia 2021; 76:520.
98. Samuelsson P, Brudin L, Sandin RH. Late psychological symptoms after awareness
among consecutively included surgical patients. Anesthesiology 2007; 106:26.
99. Osterman JE, Hopper J, Heran WJ, et al. Awareness under anesthesia and the
- Page 28 of 29 -
Accidental awareness during general anesthesia

development of posttraumatic stress disorder. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 2001; 23:198.


100. Domino KB, Posner KL, Caplan RA, Cheney FW. Awareness during anesthesia: a closed
claims analysis. Anesthesiology 1999; 90:1053.
101. Schwender D, Kunze-Kronawitter H, Dietrich P, et al. Conscious awareness during
general anaesthesia: patients' perceptions, emotions, cognition and reactions. Br J
Anaesth 1998; 80:133.
102. Lennmarken C, Bildfors K, Enlund G, et al. Victims of awareness. Acta Anaesthesiol
Scand 2002; 46:229.
103. Whitlock EL, Rodebaugh TL, Hassett AL, et al. Psychological sequelae of surgery in a
prospective cohort of patients from three intraoperative awareness prevention trials.
Anesth Analg 2015; 120:87.
104. Lennmarken C, Sydsjo G. Psychological consequences of awareness and their
treatment. Best Pract Res Clin Anaesthesiol 2007; 21:357.
105. Nunes RR, Fonseca NM, Simões CM, et al. [Brazilian consensus on anesthetic depth
monitoring]. Rev Bras Anestesiol 2015; 65:427.

Topic 400 Version 45.0

© 2023 UpToDate, Inc. All rights reserved.

- Page 29 of 29 -

You might also like