Muller 2007

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Original article

doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2729.2007.00248.x

Saying the wrong thing: improving learning with


multimedia by including misconceptions
D.A. Muller,* J. Bewes,* M.D. Sharma* & P. Reimann†
*School of Physics, University of Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
†Faculty of Education and Social Work, University of Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

Abstract In this study, 364 first-year physics students were randomly assigned to one of four online mul-
timedia treatments on Newton’s First and Second Laws of Motion: (1) the ‘Exposition’, a
concise lecture-style presentation; (2) the ‘Extended Exposition’, the Exposition with addi-
tional interesting information; (3) the ‘Refutation’, the Exposition with common misconcep-
tions explicitly stated and refuted; or (4) the ‘Dialogue’, a student–tutor discussion of the same
material as in the Refutation. Students were tested using questions from mechanics conceptual
inventories before and after watching the multimedia treatments. Results show the Refutation
and Dialogue produced the greatest learning gains, with effect sizes of 0.79 and 0.83, respec-
tively, compared with the Exposition. Students with low prior knowledge benefited most,
however high prior knowledge learners were not disadvantaged by the misconception-based
approach. The findings suggest that online multimedia can be greatly improved, promoting
conceptual change in students with all levels of experience, by including a discussion of
misconceptions.

Keywords cognitive load theory, conceptual change, misconceptions, multimedia learning, physics edu-
cation research, vicarious learning.

1999; McDermott & Shaffer 2001). These often involve


Introduction
strategically planned tutorials, concept checks in lecture
With students no longer perceived as passive recipients classes and increased opportunities for student
of knowledge, the role of misconceptions as central discussion. However, limited research has been con-
obstacles to effective science education has been well ducted on how resources like linear multimedia can be
established (Chi et al. 1994; Vosniadou 1994; Duit & altered to promote conceptual change.
Treagust 2003; diSessa 2006). Over the past 30 years, Multimedia research has investigated student learn-
physics education researchers have documented mis- ing of scientific topics like the formation of lightning
conceptions, attempted to overcome them, and evalu- (Mayer et al. 1996) and deep-sea waves (Mayer &
ated interventions with conceptual inventories (Hake Jackson 2005), but the issue of misconceptions has
1998; Crouch & Mazur 2001). Successful teaching rarely been addressed. Studies have also typically been
practices have been implemented in a small number of conducted in controlled laboratory environments, with
physics classrooms internationally (Redish & Steinberg learners who have little or no prior knowledge about the
subject matter nor experience in the ways of knowing,
learning and thinking in the domain.
Accepted: 24 May 2007
Research on simulations and other interactive multi-
Correspondence: Derek A. Muller, Physics Building (A28) Camper-
down Campus, University of Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia. Email: media is addressing the issue of conceptual change. On
muller@physics.usyd.edu.au the topic of electric circuits, Ronen and Eliahu (2000)

144 © 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd Journal of Computer Assisted Learning (2008), 24, 144–155
Misconception-based multimedia instruction 145

found that simulation feedback helped students recog-


a b
nize and change their misconceptions. A further study
confirmed that over the course of a semester, students
who used both real and virtual experiments developed a
stronger conceptual understanding than those who used
real experiments alone (Zacharia 2007). However, an
important finding in interactive settings is that learners
often require more scaffolding to focus on conceptual
issues (Lowe 2004; van Joolingen et al. 2007). With a
projectile motion simulation, Yeo et al. (2004) found
that students interacted superficially and retained their c
intuitive conceptions. Only after researcher intervention
did they focus on the salient conceptual issues in the
program. Linear multimedia could serve this scaffold-
ing function, emphasizing important aspects of a simu-
lation and guiding learner manipulations. If it is
possible to promote conceptual change effectively with
multimedia, it could be used on its own, as part of online
collaborative exercises, or with simulations. Linear
multimedia is one of the most common forms of multi-
media online because it is inexpensive and straight- d

forward to create. With the proliferation of online lec-


tures, and websites like http://YouTube.com registering
100 million downloads per day, video has become a
central communication medium facilitated by
computers.
How then should misconceptions be addressed in
non-interactive multimedia? In this study we sought to
investigate the following questions in a real learning
context:
Fig 1 All multimedia treatments contained (a) diagrams, (b) ani-
mations, (c) live action demonstrations and (d) graphs.
• Does explicit discussion of misconceptions in multi-
media lead to enhanced learning compared with
concise explanations? single speaker mentioned common misconceptions
• Does the level of students’ scientifically accurate while describing Newton’s Laws and explained how the
prior knowledge influence the degree to which differ- misconceptions were inadequate. In the Dialogue treat-
ent teaching methods are effective? ment, a student raised the same misconceptions and
then engaged in discussion with a tutor to resolve incon-
These questions were investigated by comparing four sistent ideas. Screen shots from the multimedia are
instructional multimedia treatments. The Exposition, shown in Fig 1.
Extended Exposition, Refutation and Dialogue were Misconceptions added to multimedia treatments have
designed to resemble learners’ experiences in different the potential to help or hinder learning. They may
physics instructional environments. The Exposition impose an extraneous cognitive load on the learner,
provided a concise summary of the correct physics inhibiting him or her from building a correct, coherent
involved in Newton’s First and Second Laws. The mental model. Or they may help by highlighting pos-
Extended Exposition contained additional interesting sible differences between scientific theories and a learn-
information related to the topic, which was not exam- er’s prior knowledge, increasing germane cognitive
ined in the post-test. In the Refutation treatment, a load. These considerations are outlined in detail below.

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


146 D.A. Muller et al.

and discussion. It has little been investigated, however,


Why should misconception treatments
whether or how non-interactive resources with which
be superior?
students learn can facilitate conceptual change.
Although learning is often thought of as a process of The Dialogue treatment in this study was inspired by
accretion, as mentioned in the Introduction, in introduc- the emphasis on student discussion in successful reform
tory science education reorganization of concepts teaching methods. Studies of ‘vicarious learning’ in
seems to be the essential mode of learning. Posner et al. which students learn by observing a recorded student–
(1982) proposed that dissatisfaction with existing tutor interaction have shown that this technique can be
mental models is the first step towards conceptual as effective as didactic teaching (Cox et al. 1999; for
change. This has been supported by a number of experi- theoretical background see McKendree et al. 1998; Lee
ments in which different methods have been used to et al. 1999). These studies did not consider, however,
create cognitive conflict (Guzzetti et al. 1993; Limon the role vicarious learning could play in confronting and
2001). Although there is debate over which methods are changing misconceptions.
most effective, many cognitive conflict tactics used in In non-interactive media, studies have been carried
classrooms have demonstrated improved performance out on so-called ‘refutation texts’, in which misconcep-
compared with traditional instructional approaches tions are discussed and rejected (for a review, see
(Duit & Treagust 2003; Vosniadou & Verschaffel 2004). Guzzetti et al. 1993). Although some conflicting find-
Therefore, misconception treatments should activate ings were reported, in general text that attempted to
students’ prior knowledge and help them recognize any create cognitive conflict resulted in greater learning
disparity between their ideas and correct scientific gains than non-refutational text. A study of elementary
theories. science students found that a refutational text passage
In the classroom, teaching interventions developed to on energy, which addressed two prominent preconcep-
overcome misconceptions almost universally use inter- tions, was much more effective as an adjunct to standard
active teaching and learning methods. Interactive class teaching than an expository text (Diakidoy et al.
engagement lectures aim to engage students in 2003). One extensive qualitative study (Guzzetti et al.
‘heads-on (always) and hands-on (usually) activities 1997) found that refutation texts do induce cognitive
which yield immediate feedback through discussion conflict and over a period of months can help students
with peers and/or instructors’ (Hake 1998, p. 65). Peer develop correct scientific understandings. In some
Instruction (Mazur 1997; Crouch & Mazur 2001) and cases, however, students found support for their alterna-
Interactive Lecture Demonstrations (Thornton & tive conceptions in refutation texts when the refutation
Sokoloff 1998) involve students in discussions about was not direct enough or when students lacked neces-
their ideas in small groups before reporting back to the sary reading strategies.
lecturer. Workshop Tutorials (Sharma et al. 2005) and
Tutorials in Introductory Physics (McDermott &
Shaffer 2001) promote discussion outside of lectures in Why should concise treatments be superior?
small groups as students work through physics prob-
In research on multimedia, the finding that succinct
lems under the supervision of tutors.
instruction leads to more learning has been confirmed in
All of these methods have demonstrated improved
a variety of empirical contexts (Chandler & Sweller
performance, often measured with the Force Concept
1991; Mayer 2001, 2003, 2005). Instructional messages
Inventory (Hestenes et al. 1992; Halloun & Hestenes
that contain redundant information sources inhibit
1995; Mazur 1997) or the Force and Motion Conceptual
learning in what is called the redundancy effect (Sweller
Evaluation (Thornton & Sokoloff 1998). Interactive
et al. 1998). A related recommendation – that all non-
engagement lectures, for example, showed almost two
essential information be removed from instructional
standard deviations of improvement over traditional
messages is sometimes called the coherence principle:
teaching methods in a survey of 62 courses (Hake
1998). The challenge of addressing and changing Adding extraneous words or pictures to a multimedia
student misconceptions has been approached in small message can interfere with . . . cognitive processes . . . by
classrooms, tutorials, and in lectures through activities encouraging learners to pay attention to words or images

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Misconception-based multimedia instruction 147

that are not relevant, by disrupting how learners organize rienced in the area, in what is called the expertise rever-
words or pictures into a causal chain, and by priming sal effect (Kalyuga et al. 1998, 2003). Therefore, one
inappropriate schemas to be used to assimilate the
incoming words and pictures. (Mayer 2003, p. 133)
might expect a discussion of misconceptions to be ben-
eficial for novice learners but detrimental for those more
In short, multimedia messages that include non- experienced.
essential material often distract learners from the cogni- Multimedia containing misconceptions is signifi-
tive processes required for learning. At the heart of the cantly different from refutation text because multimedia
redundancy effect and the coherence principle is cogni- is transient in time. While reading refutation texts,
tive load theory (Sweller 1994; Sweller et al. 1998). learners can easily refer back and forth between mis-
Cognitive load theory is a theory of instructional design conceptions and correct scientific ideas. In contrast,
that is based on the limited capacity of humans to viewers of misconception-based multimedia must
process novel information. It asserts that instructional develop their understandings as the multimedia
design must be primarily concerned with allocating progresses. This increases the likelihood that the added
cognitive resources to those activities which lead to material may misdirect or overload learners.
desired changes in long-term memory. Thus far, Finally, some researchers may be sceptical of non-
researchers in this area have focused on how this may interactive media promoting conceptual change in any
best be achieved. Cognitive load can be broken down sense. Often behavioural activity is believed necessary
into three types: intrinsic, extraneous and germane. to inspire cognitive activity, making it essential for con-
Intrinsic cognitive load is a property of the material to ceptual change. ‘Learning is an effortful and mindful
be learned and therefore cannot be altered by instruc- process and students should be encouraged to construct
tional design. Extraneous cognitive load, on the other their own knowledge and skills through active process-
hand, reflects the additional cognitive effort required to ing, rather than being passive listeners.’ (Vosniadou
learn from poorly designed instruction. Germane cogni- et al. 2001, p. 382) This statement begs the question:
tive load refers to the cognitive effort involved in can one make learners active listeners, and if so, how?
schema formation and is therefore most important for
learning. The goal of instructional design is to minimize Method
extraneous cognitive load and maximize germane load.
Participants and design
From the perspective of cognitive load theory, there is
a risk that adding misconceptions to a concise scientific The participants were 678 first-year physics students at
presentation may interfere with learning. When viewing the University of Sydney from three physics streams:
a misconception treatment, learners must select from a Fundamentals, for students with little prior formal
greater number of words and pictures to form coherent instruction in physics; Regular, for students with senior
mental models. They must also pay attention for a high school physics backgrounds; and Advanced, for
longer duration to see the same amount of correct students who excelled in senior high school subjects and
scientific information. Furthermore, in both of the physics in particular. As part of their first assignment,
misconception-based treatments, force diagrams and students were asked to access a website to receive a par-
animations were shown to illustrate multiple ticipation mark. At the website they completed a pre-
misconceptions. In the Dialogue, misconceptions were test, received a randomly assigned multimedia treatment
presented as the genuine beliefs of one of the dialogue and completed a post-test. Participants’ answers and the
participants without cautioning students that not all of times at which they were submitted were written to a
the information in the treatment was correct. Resolu- mysql database. This allowed for determination of the
tions were reached later, through discussions between time spent on the pre- and post-tests and the time spent
the student and tutor. watching the multimedia treatment.
In addition, not all students have the same miscon- As the experiment was conducted in an authentic
ceptions, so a discussion that might be useful for some setting and participants were allowed to withdraw at any
students would likely be irrelevant for others. Multime- time, the data required filtering prior to analysis. Partici-
dia researchers have found that some instructional pants were removed from the data set for failing to com-
guidance that benefits novices can hinder learners expe- plete the post-test (n = 116), watching more than one

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


148 D.A. Muller et al.

multimedia treatment (n = 75), not watching the multi- transfer questions. The answers to 12 questions were
media in its entirety (n = 30), spending less than 4 min directly stated in each treatment, while the other 14
completing the pre- or post-test (n = 57), failing to required application of learned principles to new
answer all questions (n = 6), or scoring higher than 95% situations. Each question was worth one mark.
on the pre-test (n = 30). Students were able to watch
more than one multimedia treatment by using the back
Multimedia treatments
button in their browser or by manually changing the
website URL. Newton’s First and Second Laws are fundamental to vir-
Using a website to administer the materials for this tually all introductory physics courses, but they are noto-
study had several advantages. It allowed for large riously difficult to learn because they contradict
numbers of students from authentic lecture courses in common sense ideas about motion (McClosky 1983).As
physics to be surveyed. The times of submission for the subject matter is common and so widely misunder-
each question were easily recorded and participants stood, extensive research has been carried out to docu-
were able to complete the study in their own time at their ment common student misconceptions (McDermott
own pace. Assignments to video treatments were com- 1991; Mayer 2004). Hence, Newton’s First and Second
pletely randomized using a Hypertext Preprocessor Laws were selected as the focus of the multimedia treat-
script. Some drawbacks of the website set-up included ments in this study, with design informed by misconcep-
the high bandwidth required to view the multimedia tion research (Trowbridge & McDermott 1980, 1981;
treatments over the Internet. Participants either required Clement 1982; McClosky 1983; Halloun & Hestenes
broadband at home or had to complete the study from an 1985; diSessa 1996). The Refutation and Dialogue
on-campus access lab. included the most common misconceptions involving
Many variables were out of the researchers’ control. Newton’s First and Second Laws, which are summa-
Participants accessed the experiment wherever and rized below:
whenever they liked. They may have used resources or
consulted with peers when answering the pre- or post- • believing an unbalanced force is required to keep an
test questions. Although the time between the start and object moving with constant velocity;
end of the multimedia was calculated, an appropriate • confusing velocity and acceleration;
length of time was no guarantee that a student actually • confusing position and velocity;
watched the treatment. These were features of the meth- • confusing momentum with force; and
odology; the ability of students to participate as they • believing that an increasing force is required to
saw fit ensured the results could be generalized to achieve constant acceleration.
authentic learning environments.
To explicate the physics concepts, all of the multimedia
treatments examined three examples: a book pushed
Materials and apparatus
across a table at constant speed, a juggling ball thrown
The pre- and post-tests consisted of the same 26 mul- upwards and caught, and a toy car rolling up and down a
tiple choice questions. Twenty-two questions were from ramp. The treatments ranged in length from 7 to
the Force and Motion Conceptual Evaluation (Thornton 11.5 min. The scripts were written with reference to
& Sokoloff 1998). Three questions were from the Force several textbooks (Hewitt 1997; Young & Freedman
Concept Inventory (Hestenes et al. 1992; Mazur 1997) 2000; Halliday et al. 2003) and were critiqued by a
and one question was written by the researchers. panel of three physics educators, each with over
Reliability estimates of both tests have been made 30 years of experience. The scripts were iteratively
(MacIsaac et al. 2002; Ramlo 2002) with Cronbach’s a compared and contrasted throughout the writing
ranging from 0.85 to 0.94. Previous experiments with process to ensure all treatments contained exactly the
these tests (see Henderson 2002) have shown that no same accurate physics information. All treatments
measurable learning occurs simply by taking the test, included diagrams, animations and live action demon-
eliminating the need for a non-intervention control strations and were created in line with multimedia
group. The test involved a mixture of retention and design principles (Mayer 2005). Specifically, words

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Misconception-based multimedia instruction 149

Table 1. Summary of multimedia treatment attributes.

Treatment Exposition Extended Exposition Refutation Dialogue

Number of speakers 1 1 1 2
Length (minutes:seconds) 7:02 11:22 9:33 11:22
Addresses misconceptions No No Yes Yes

were presented as narration rather than on-screen text;


Procedure
related concepts were presented contiguously in time
and space; and extraneous sounds and images were All students taking first year physics at the University of
avoided as much as possible. After the treatments were Sydney were asked to access a website for one mark
completed, they were again critiqued by physics educa- towards their first assignment. A consent form on the
tors to ensure there existed no inconsistencies in physics opening page informed students that
content.
The Exposition was designed to be very similar to a • the study would take between 30 and 45 min to
concise presentation a well-prepared lecturer might complete;
make on the topic of Newton’s Laws. It included • the study should be completed individually without
graphs, force diagrams, animations and live action dem- referring to textbooks or online resources;
onstrations, along with ‘talking head’ narration. The • performance on the pre- and post-tests would be kept
Extended Exposition and the Refutation consisted of confidential; and
the Exposition plus additional material. Interesting • participation in the study was voluntary and that they
information beyond the assessed learning outcomes was could withdraw at any time with no penalty.
added in the Extended Exposition to make it equal in
length with the Dialogue. Thus it served as a control for Between pre- and post-tests, participants were ran-
the time students spent thinking about the physics and, domly assigned to one of the four multimedia
in conjunction with the Exposition, it allowed for an treatments. After completing the post-test, each student
investigation of the applicability of the coherence prin- received their mark on that test along with helpful sug-
ciple (Mayer 2001, 2003) in an authentic physics gestions about resources they could use to improve their
setting. Common misconceptions were explicitly raised understanding. Students who scored below 40% were
and refuted in the Refutation to investigate this method not told their exact mark, and additional aids were
of recognizing anomaly between prior knowledge and recommended. A record of all students who accessed
scientific theory. Previous studies on refutation texts the website, regardless of whether they completed the
were used to inform the writing of this script (Guzzetti study, was sent to the course coordinator who allocated
et al. 1997; Diakidoy et al. 2003). participation marks.
The Dialogue was entirely different in structure to the
other three treatments, utilizing a simulated discussion
Results and analysis
between an inquisitive student and a tutor. Over the
course of the discussion, the student’s misconceptions, The scores on the pre- and post-tests were not normally
the same as those in the Refutation, were revealed and distributed. This was due to the large number of students
corrected through Socratic dialogue (Hake 1992). Parts with widely varied abilities. Non-parametric tests
of the dialogue script were inspired by transcripts of a revealed no significant differences among the pre-test
student’s interviews on Newton’s Laws (diSessa 1996). results across the four treatment groups; however, post-
Where possible the same phrases as in the Expositions test results were significantly different (Kruskal–Wallis
were used in the Dialogue. A summary of the similari- c2 = 8.625, P = 0.035, Median Test c2 = 9.565, P =
ties and differences between the four treatments is 0.023). Gender composition was not significantly dif-
shown in Table 1. Sample corresponding script seg- ferent across the four treatment groups, nor was the time
ments are included in Appendix I. spent completing the pre- or post-tests.

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


150 D.A. Muller et al.

Table 2. Gain statistics for the four multimedia treatments.

Treatment Sample size (n) Median pre-test Median post-test Mean gain Standard deviation

Dialogue 92 8.5 16 4.77* 4.59


Refutation 86 7.5 14 4.41* 4.01
Extended Exposition 95 8.0 12 2.41 3.72
Exposition 91 8.0 9 1.77 2.65

*Dialogue and Refutation mean gains were significantly greater than the Exposition and Extended Exposition at P < 0.01.

mean gains for each treatment, separated by physics


Differences between treatments
stream, are shown in Fig 2.
To determine the relative effectiveness of the multime- Fundamentals students who watched the Dialogue or
dia treatments, a gain score for each student was com- Refutation had significantly greater gains than those
puted by subtracting their pre-test mark from their post- who watched the Exposition (F(3, 109) = 6.609, P <
test mark (each of which had a maximum of 26 marks). 0.001). Similarly, in the Regular stream the Dialogue
Gain was normally distributed for each treatment group. and Refutation students achieved significantly greater
The sample size, median pre- and post-test scores, mean gains than Exposition students (F(3, 163) = 7.262,
gain and unbiased standard deviation, are shown in P = 0.0001). Students from the Advanced stream did
Table 2. not show significantly different gains between treat-
Using a one-way anova the gains of the treatments ments, although the trends in means observed are
were compared yielding a significant difference similar to those above (F(3, 83) = 2.069, P = 0.111).
between treatments (F(3, 361) = 13.625, P < 0.00001). The lack of significant difference might be due to the
The Games-Howell post hoc procedure, which does not small sample size in this stream and a possible ceiling
assume equal variance, showed the gains for students effect on the post-test. The median score for the
who watched the Dialogue or the Refutation were sig- Advanced stream on the post-test was 85%, compared
nificantly greater than those who received the Exposi- with 23% and 54% for the Fundamentals and Regular
tion (P < 0.0001) or Extended Exposition treatments streams, respectively.
(P = 0.001 for the Dialogue, P = 0.004 for the
Refutation). The effect size for these differences in Discussion
comparison to the Exposition was 0.83 for the Dialogue
and 0.79 for the Refutation. Theoretical implications

Using questions from standard mechanics conceptual


inventories, students from three lecture courses in first
Gain dependence on prior knowledge
year physics were tested before and after watching a
Students from the three physics streams had different short multimedia treatment about Newton’s First and
levels of prior physics instruction, allowing for an inves- Second Laws. Results show that overall students
tigation of the dependence of gain on prior knowledge. achieved greater gains by watching a treatment that
It was expected that the Fundamentals students, with addressed misconceptions than one which presented
the least prior physics instruction, would hold the only correct scientific information. This suggests
most misconceptions and therefore benefit most from that the increased cognitive load incurred with
misconception-based instruction. Regular students, misconception-based treatments was germane rather
having completed Newtonian mechanics in high school, than extraneous, on the average, for students with all
represented a mixture of prior knowledge. It was there- levels of prior knowledge.
fore unclear which treatment would be most advanta- The results of this study are consistent with the
geous for them. Advanced students, with significant findings of conceptual change research that suggest
accurate prior knowledge, were expected to achieve cognitive conflict is essential to conceptual change
greatest learning gains with the concise treatment. The (Guzzetti et al. 1993). As both Refutation and Dialogue

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Misconception-based multimedia instruction 151

Fig 2 Mean gains (pre-test – post-test) for


the four multimedia treatments after sepa-
rating by physics stream. Error bars: ⫾1
standard error.

treatments produced similar effect sizes, it seems that their peers in the concise treatment. Although studies
both methods of recognizing the anomaly between prior have shown misconceptions to be quite persistent, it is
knowledge and scientific theory are equally effective in unlikely that Advanced students held misconceptions
non-interactive multimedia. Follow-up interviews will to anywhere near the same degree as Fundamentals
investigate student perceptions of Refutation and Dia- students. The explicit discussion of misconceptions
logue methods. seems to be an effective instructional strategy whether
The findings highlight the need to consider what con- students actually hold the misconceptions or not.
stitutes extraneous information in the context of cogni- With respect to the Exposition and the Extended
tive load theory. In the standard practice of teaching Exposition, the effects of the coherence principle were
physics, misconceptions are not considered essential not observable using the multiple-choice pre- and post-
teaching material. They are addressed when the need tests in this setting. This result suggests that interesting
arises, in response to student questions or answers on but irrelevant information might encourage students to
assessment. Even then, feedback may only address the pay attention to online multimedia when they are watch-
specific problem without clearly explaining a miscon- ing it in their own time. Alternatively, the multiple-
ception in its entirety. Almost all textbooks, including choice tests may not have been sensitive to the
those used by the students in this study, do not include differences in learning between the two treatments.
discussions of misconceptions. However, the addition Replications of laboratory studies that investigated the
of incorrect information to form the Dialogue and Refu- impact of additional interesting information conducted
tation treatments was essential for students to engage in in authentic learning environments could shed light on
germane processing; it did not impose an onerous extra- the issue. This highlights another possible area in which
neous load on students. seemingly extraneous information in a laboratory
One might expect the discussion of misconceptions setting might yield a germane cognitive load in authen-
to be particularly irrelevant for the Advanced students, tic learning contexts. In a laboratory, a learner’s intrin-
given their experience with Newtonian mechanics and sic motivation to engage with instructional material is
excellent performance on high school assessment tasks. likely less important than in an unstructured environ-
However, no analogue of the expertise reversal effect ment where, without researcher supervision, he may
was found. Advanced students in the misconception freely decide how to direct his attention. A previous
treatments achieved non-significantly greater gains than study has found that some well-established multimedia

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


152 D.A. Muller et al.

principles fail to generalize easily to authentic settings understanding. This is an important result for teachers
(Tabbers et al. 2004). Further research is required to who find it difficult to implement interactive methods
determine whether the coherence principle holds in owing to restrictions on time, money and technology,
authentic settings. often coupled with large class sizes. Multimedia inter-
In future studies, an attempt to measure the cognitive ventions that address misconceptions, like those investi-
load of students may help to understand and interpret gated in this study, could be used in lectures to highlight
results. In a setting like that of the present experiment, key conceptual difficulties. Additionally, they could be
this would most likely be achieved through self- used to provide conceptual scaffolding for interactive
reported rating scales, however, other techniques could multimedia.
be used in a laboratory setting (Paas et al. 2003). Despite the verified advantages of reform teaching
This study helps to understand an ‘active ingredient’ methods and refutation texts, uptake of these strategies
in the reform methods developed to achieve conceptual has been quite limited. The practical drawbacks of refu-
change through lecture instruction (e.g. Hake 1998). tation texts are clear; they require more research and
Physics education research has been criticized for com- writing to produce and they result in heavier, bulkier
paring instructional strategies where several variables books. Challenges of implementation for reform teach-
have been altered simultaneously (Guzzetti et al. ing methods are similar. They require substantial invest-
1993). Reform teaching methods include various com- ments of time and money, specialized training, and
binations of hands-on activities, discussions with peers, often result in a decrease in the number of learning out-
increased instructor feedback, demonstrations involv- comes that can be achieved in the same number of
ing learning cycles, written worksheets and classroom contact hours. The Internet offers a new means of cir-
communication systems, leaving in doubt the essential cumventing some of these difficulties. Multimedia is
factors that enhance learning. The results of this study almost as readily available as text and is not cumber-
suggest that part of the benefit of interactive lecture some to carry like a textbook. Adding misconceptions
classes and tutorials is likely derived from students only increases the duration of instruction, which as
observing discussions between other students and demonstrated in this study can dramatically increase the
tutors in which misconceptions are addressed. learning gains in an authentic setting.
Discussions of this sort are quite rare in traditional The success of conceptual change interventions is
lecture classrooms (Graesser & Person 1994; Muller often heavily dependent on the expertise of the teacher
2005). (Limon 2001). Addressing misconceptions through
multimedia rather than teacher-led discussion reduces
the burden on teachers and increases the likelihood of
Practical implications
success. Teachers are often hesitant about conducting
The results of this study suggest that, unlike other non- conceptually challenging discussions owing to con-
essential information, discussing misconceptions does cerns about time constraints or their own mastery of the
not interfere with learning when added to multimedia. subject (Weaver 1998).
When designing multimedia for science education It is important to note that although misconception-
areas, developers should therefore address common based multimedia on average resulted in greater learn-
misconceptions explicitly in their explanations of ing gains, it is not a standalone solution to conceptual
appropriate topics. Although their inclusion in multime- difficulties. The process of moving from alternative
dia results in longer interventions with more words and ideas to a coherent scientific view is complex and it
diagrams, they serve a useful pedagogic purpose, aiding remains only partially understood. Undoubtedly, dis-
learners to consider scientific conceptions in light of cussions among students and between students and
their prior knowledge. teachers are important for developing accurate concep-
In addition, although interactive methods in lectures tual understandings. Multimedia that addresses miscon-
have demonstrated substantial gains in conceptual ceptions is simply one resource that may help students
understanding over traditional methods, this study sug- along the path to scientific reasoning. Furthermore,
gests that raising misconceptions in traditional-style the misconception-based techniques presented in this
lectures should increase student conceptual study may be useful adjuncts to simulations or online

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Misconception-based multimedia instruction 153

discussions, to help focus learner attention on salient to interfere with the development of conceptual
conceptual issues. understandings. However, the extra cognitive load
Previous studies have shown that reducing extrane- involved in raising and refuting common misconcep-
ous cognitive load is vital for learning science with tions seems justified by the increased gain achieved by
multimedia. Even relevant equations have been shown students who received misconception treatments.

Appendix I Comparison of juggling script segments

Exposition Extended exposition1 Refutation1 Dialogue1

While the ball is in the air If you are learning to A misconception is that as Tutor: Can you tell me
(we will ignore air friction juggle it might be nice to the ball travels upwards, what happens when a
because it is so small) only have the balls fall a bit there is an upward force single ball goes around
one force acts on the ball slower to give you more from the juggler’s hand once?
throughout its flight. This time to coordinate your that stays with the ball Student: Well the
is the force of gravity efforts catching and even after the ball has lost juggler’s hand gives the
which is constant and throwing the balls. contact with the juggler’s ball a force that drives it
downwards, accelerating Unfortunately if you use hand – a force in the ball upward against gravity –
the ball in the downward lighter balls, they won’t to keep it moving. This but as it goes up that
direction. After being fall any slower than heavy force gradually dies away force dies away, right? So
thrown up, a ball travels ones. Even though the until it balances at the top then, it
slower and slower force of gravity on them gravitational force at the perfectly balances gravity
upwards, stopping is less, it takes peak. Then gravity takes – then gravity wins and
momentarily before proportionately less force over and pulls the ball the ball falls downward.
speeding up in the to accelerate them by downward. However,
Tutor: Hmm . . . you said
downward direction. Newton’s second law, so there is no upward force
that the force from his
Then it meets with the there is no net effect and on the ball after it has
hand and gravity are
juggler’s hand again and the balls accelerate at the left the juggler’s hand
equal at the top.
the process repeats same rate whether they and gravity is acting all
are heavy or light. The the time. In this Student: Yeah.
only advantage of using misconception we are Tutor: Then why doesn’t
light balls is that you simply confusing velocity the ball keep doing what
won’t expend as much with force it’s doing?
energy throwing them Student: I don’t know.
into the air. Something
Maybe air resistance – no.
you might try to make
I mean they’re only
learning to juggle easier balanced for a split
would be juggling tissues
second – so then gravity
or scarves. These items
wins – I don’t know, I must
have significant air be missing something.
resistance so they don’t
accelerate downwards at Tutor: Does it make sense
that the juggler’s hand
the same rate as balls.
can put a force on the ball
Most beginners start out
this way and work up to after it leaves his hand?
more aerodynamic and Student: No . . . not really.
even dangerous objects But the ball’s still going
later. up, isn’t it? Doesn’t that
mean there’s a force?
1Only extra material is presented.

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


154 D.A. Muller et al.

Halloun I.A. & Hestenes D. (1985) The initial knowledge


Acknowledgements state of college physics students. American Journal of
We would like to thank Cyrus Boadway, Jacqueline Physics 53, 1043–1055.
Hayes and Luke Ryves for their assistance in producing Halloun I.A. & Hestenes D. (1995) Interpreting the Force
Concept Inventory. Physics Teacher 33, 502–506.
the multimedia treatments and online testing
Henderson C.R. (2002) Common concerns about the Force
environment. Thanks also to Paul Ginns and reviewers
Concept Inventory. Physics Teacher 40, 542–547.
for their helpful suggestions regarding this paper.
Hestenes D., Wells M. & Swackhamer G. (1992) Force
Concept Inventory. Physics Teacher 30, 141–158.
References
Hewitt P.G. (1997) Conceptual Physics, 8th edn. Addison-
Chandler P. & Sweller J. (1991) Cognitive load theory and the Wesley, Reading, MA.
format of instruction. Cognition and Instruction 8, 293– van Joolingen W.R., de Jong T. & Dimitrakopoulou A. (2007)
332. Issues in computer supported inquiry learning in science.
Chi M.T.H., Slotta J.D. & De Leeuw N. (1994) From things to Journal of Computer Assisted Learning 23, 111–119.
processes: a theory of conceptual change for learning Kalyuga S., Chandler P. & Sweller J. (1998) Levels of exper-
science concepts. Learning and Instruction 4, 27–43. tise and instructional design. Human Factors 40, 1–17.
Clement J. (1982) Students’ preconceptions in introductory Kalyuga S., Ayres P., Chandler P. & Sweller J. (2003) The
mechanics. American Journal of Physics 50, 66. expertise reversal effect. Educational Psychologist 38,
Cox R., McKendree J., Tobin R., Lee J. & Mayes T. (1999) 23–31.
Vicarious learning from dialogue and discourse. Instruc- Lee J., Dineen F., McKendree J. & Mayes T. (1999) Vicarious
tional Science 27, 431–458. learning: cognitive and linguistic effects of observing peer
Crouch C.H. & Mazur E. (2001) Peer instruction: ten years of discussion. Paper presented at the American Educational
experience and results. American Journal of Physics 69, Research Association, Montréal, Québec, Canada.
970–977. Limon M. (2001) On the cognitive conflict as an instructional
Diakidoy I.N., Kendeou P. & Ioannides C. (2003) Reading strategy for conceptual change: a critical appraisal. Learn-
about energy: the effects of text structure in science learn- ing and Instruction 11, 357–380.
ing and conceptual change. Contemporary Educational Lowe R. (2004) Interrogation of a dynamic visualization
Psychology 28, 335–356. during learning. Learning and Instruction 14, 257–274.
Duit R. & Treagust D.F. (2003) Conceptual change: a power- McClosky M. (1983) Intuitive physics. Scientific American
ful framework for improving science teaching and learning. 248, 122–130.
International Journal of Science Education 25, 671–688. McDermott L.C. (1991) What we teach and what is
Graesser A.C. & Person N.K. (1994) Question asking during learned – closing the gap. American Journal of Physics 59,
tutoring. American Educational Research Journal 31, 301–315.
104–137. McDermott L.C. & Shaffer P.S. (2001) Tutorials in Introduc-
Guzzetti B.J., Snyder T.E., Glass G.V. & Gamas W.S. (1993) tory Physics. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ.
Promoting conceptual change in science: a comparative MacIsaac D., Cole R.P., Cole D.M., McCullough L. & Maxka
meta-analysis of instructional interventions from reading J. (2002) Standardized testing in physics via the World
education and science education. Reading Research Quar- Wide Web. Electronic Journal of Science Education 6.
terly 28, 116–159. Available at: http://ejse.southwestern.edu/original%20site/
Guzzetti B.J., Williams W.O., Skeels S.A. & Wu S.M. (1997) manuscripts/v6n3/articles/art02_macisaac/macisaac.pdf
Influence of text structure on learning counterintuitive (last accessed 12 June 2007).
physics concepts. Journal of Research in Science Teaching McKendree J., Stenning K., Mayes T., Lee J. & Cox R. (1998)
34, 701–719. Why observing a dialogue may benefit learning. Journal of
Hake R.R. (1992) Socratic pedagogy in the introductory Computer Assisted Learning 14, 110–119.
physics laboratory. Physics Teacher 30, 546–552. Mayer R.E. (2001) Multimedia Learning. Cambridge Univer-
Hake R.R. (1998) Interactive-engagement vs. traditional sity Press, Cambridge.
methods: a six-thousand-student survey of mechanics test Mayer R.E. (2003) The promise of multimedia learning: using
data for introductory physics courses. American Journal of the same instructional design methods across different
Physics 66, 64–74. media. Learning and Instruction 13, 125–139.
Halliday D., Resnick R. & Walker J. (2003) Fundamentalss of Mayer R.E. (2004) Teaching of subject matter. Annual Review
Physics, 6th edition. Wiley, New York. of Psychology 55, 715–744.

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Misconception-based multimedia instruction 155

Mayer R.E. (Ed.) (2005) The Cambridge Handbook of Multi- Sweller J. (1994) Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty,
media Learning. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. and instructional design. Learning and Instruction 4, 295–
Mayer R.E. & Jackson J. (2005) The case for coherence in sci- 312.
entific explanations: quantitative details can hurt qualitative Sweller J., van Merriënboer J.J.G. & Paas F.G.W.C. (1998)
understanding. Journal of Experimental Psychology Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educa-
Applied 11, 13–18. tional Psychology Review 10, 251–296.
Mayer R.E., Bove W., Bryman A., Mars R. & Tapangco L. Tabbers H.K., Martens R.L. & van Merrienboer J.J.G. (2004)
(1996) When less is more: meaningful learning from visual Multimedia instruction and cognitive load theory: effects of
and verbal summaries of science textbook lessons. Journal modality and cueing. British Journal of Educational Psy-
of Educational Psychology 88, 64–73. chology 74, 71–81.
Mazur E. (1997) Peer Instruction: A User’s Manual. Prentice Thornton R.K. & Sokoloff D.R. (1998) Assessing student
Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ. learning of Newton’s laws: the force and motion conceptual
Muller D.A. (2005) Inside the quantum mechanics lecture: evaluation of active learning laboratory and lecture
changing practices. Paper presented at the Higher Educa- curricula. American Journal of Physics 66, 338–352.
tion Research and Development Society of Australasia, Trowbridge D.E. & McDermott L.C. (1980) Investigation
Sydney, Australia. Available. at: http://conference.herdsa. of student understanding of the concept of velocity in
org.au/2005/pdf/refereed/paper_260.pdf (last accessed 12 one dimension. American Journal of Physics 48, 1020–
June 2007). 1028.
Paas F., Tuovinen J.E., Tabbers H. & Van Gerven P.W.M. Trowbridge D.E. & McDermott L.C. (1981) Investigation of
(2003) Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance student understanding of the concept of acceleration in one
cognitive load theory. Educational Psychologist 38, 63–71. dimension. American Journal of Physics 49, 242–253.
Posner G.J., Strike K.A., Hewson P.W. & Gertzog W.A. Vosniadou S. (1994) Capturing and modeling the process of
(1982) Accommodation of a scientific concept: toward a conceptual change. Learning and Instruction 4, 45–69.
theory of conceptual change. Science Education 66, 211– Vosniadou S. & Verschaffel L. (2004) Extending the concep-
227. tual change approach to mathematics learning and teaching.
Ramlo S. (2002) The force and motion conceptual evaluation. Learning and Instruction: The Conceptual Change
Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Mid-Western Approach to Mathematics Learning and Teaching 14,
Educational. Research Association, Columbus, OH. 445–451.
Redish E.F. & Steinberg R.N. (1999) Teaching physics: figur- Vosniadou S., Ioannides C., Dimitrakopoulou A. & Papadem-
ing out what works. Physics Today 52, 24–30. etriou E. (2001) Designing learning environments to
Ronen M. & Eliahu M. (2000) Simulation – a bridge between promote conceptual change in science. Learning and
theory and reality: the case of electric circuits. Journal of Instruction 11, 381–419.
Computer Assisted Learning 16, 14–26. Weaver G.C. (1998) Strategies in K-12 science instruction to
diSessa A.A. (1996) What do ‘just plain folk’ know about promote conceptual change. Science Education 82, 455–
physics? In Handbook of Education and Human Develop- 472.
ment: New Models of Learning, Teaching and Schooling Yeo S., Loss R., Zadnik M., Harrison A. & Treagust D.F.
(ed. D.R. Olson), pp. 705–730. Blackwell Publishers, (2004) What do students really learn from interactive mul-
Cambridge, MA. timedia? A physics case study. American Journal of Physics
diSessa A.A. (2006) A history of conceptual change research: 72, 1351–1358.
threads and fault lines. In Cambridge Handbook of the Young H.D. & Freedman R.A. (2000) University Physics with
Learning Sciences (ed. K. Sawyer), pp. 265–282. Modern Physics, 10th edn. Benjamin-Cummings, San
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Francisco, CA.
Sharma M.D., Mendez A. & O’Byrne J.W. (2005) The rela- Zacharia Z.C. (2007) Comparing and combining real and
tionship between attendance in student-centred physics virtual experimentation: an effort to enhance students’
tutorials and performance in University examinations. conceptual understanding of electric circuits. Journal of
International Journal of Science Education 27, 1375–1389. Computer Assisted Learning 23, 120–132.

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd

You might also like