Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

British Journal of Management, Vol.

00, 1–14 (2018)


DOI: 10.1111/1467-8551.12304

Methodology Corner

Theory as Fantasy: Emotional Dimensions


to Grounded Theory
Annette Clancy and Russ Vince1
UCD College of Business, University College Dublin, Belfield, Dublin, D04 V1W8, Ireland, and 1 School of
Management, University of Bath, Bath, BA2 7AY, UK
Corresponding author email: annette.clancy@ucd.ie

In this paper we discuss emotions and fantasies that inform and influence the project of
theory building. Our argument is that theory building can be improved by engaging di-
rectly with emotions and with fantasies that are defensively and creatively generated by
the researcher. Once acknowledged, these can be transformed into ideas and insights.
We provide an example of the emotional dynamics surrounding a novice researcher’s
use of grounded theory within her doctoral research. We highlight three distinctive re-
searcher fantasies of containment, coherence and purity associated with her experience
of the method. We discuss how engagement with these fantasies deepened the researcher’s
analysis and thereby enhanced the process of building theory from the data. There-
fore, our paper contributes to an understanding of how fantasies mobilized by such an
open-ended research method can help to refine our thinking about emerging theory.

Introduction unconscious processes stimulated by working with


and through this method.
Grounded theory (GT) is a ‘discovery methodol- Any attempt to experience the problem or
ogy that allows the researcher to develop a theo- issue from the perspective of respondents involves
retical account of the general features of a topic the person of the researcher, which necessarily
while simultaneously grounding the account in em- includes the emotional experience (and emo-
pirical observations or data’ (Martin and Turner, tions resulting from inexperience) that he or she
1986: 141). The GT researcher aims to experience brings to the application of the method. Our
the problem or issue from the perspective of the argument is that it is important to reveal emotions
research respondents and to develop an integrated and fantasies that are defensively and creatively
set of conceptual hypotheses about what is going generated by the researcher, so that they can be
on. Much has been written about the best way transformed, once acknowledged, into ideas and
to do grounded theory (Corley, 2015; Gummeson, insights. We provide an example of the emotional
2011; Walsh et al., 2015) but we know little about dynamics surrounding a novice researcher’s use
how GT researchers, and particularly the novice of GT within her doctoral research. We argue
researcher, might connect with the emotional and that working with the emotional and unconscious
dynamics of GT, and with researcher fantasies of
Several people have provided invaluable help in trans- containment, coherence and purity associated with
forming the original iterations of this paper into its cur- the method, deepened the researcher’s analysis
rent, more focused version. We are grateful to Yiannis and thereby enhanced theory building.
Gabriel, Donncha Kavanagh and Ian Miller for their in- Existing scholarship on the emotional aspects
sightful comments and suggestions. Our sincere thanks go
to the Editor and anonymous reviewers for their in-depth
of research assume that researchers know what
reviews and their challenging and supportive engagement they are feeling when they are feeling it (Harlos
with the paper as it evolved. et al., 2003; Saunders, Lewis and Thornhill, 2015).


C 2018 British Academy of Management. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4
2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA, 02148, USA.
2 A. Clancy and R. Vince

Our work focuses on unconscious emotional dy- highlight three unconscious fantasies that arose for
namics which manifest as fantasy and as defences a novice GT researcher, and demonstrate the ways
against emotion. We demonstrate the creative ways in which they afected the work of the researcher
in which unconscious emotion can be made avail- and the GT method. These fantasies were identi-
able for consideration by the researcher. We adopt fied through ‘free association’ within supervision,
a psychodynamic approach to grounded research. through reflections on the researcher’s dreams
What is novel about this approach is that it invites and through auto-ethnographic writing about the
researchers to delve into the internal and imag- lived experience of the research. We found that
ined world of the researcher and the researched, fantasy could be fed back into the research
as an integral and important part of research de- process to strengthen engagement with theory
sign, data collection and analysis. It involves an building.
interest in: unconscious dynamics at work for the We are aware that other concepts, such as para-
researcher and the researched; unconscious pro- dox and ambiguity, can also be used to engage
cesses within the supervisory pair/ triad; fantasies with tensions emerging from the emotional expe-
mobilized within and around the research; and rience of doing research; and to understand how
broader dynamics of relations in the context of the emotions become embedded within research. For
research environment. example, searching out the ‘paradoxical tensions’
Our methodological contribution in this paper (Vince and Broussine, 1996: 4) mobilized by do-
stems from our creation of a framework through ing research helps us to ‘taunt our established
which to understand how emotional, and particu- certainties’ as researchers by acknowledging in-
larly unconscious, aspects of research are enacted evitable contradictions (Schad et al., 2016: 5). Sim-
through fantasy building. There are two elements ilarly, ambiguity alerts us to inconsistencies and
of this that constitute a novel contribution. First, discomforts in the research process, often written
we argue for the importance of working overtly up as ‘limitations’ (Wolgemuth, 2015: 522). We ar-
with and through unconscious defences that gue that a focus on fantasy can help researchers
function to exclude unwanted emotions from to produce a distinctive understanding of the re-
awareness. Therefore, we are not only seeking to lationship between emotions mobilized by doing
identify emotions that are part of the data, which research as well as how emotions become em-
arise in the role of the researcher and influence bedded within the researcher’s understanding and
inductive and abductive analysis. We are also approach. Psychodynamic thinking helps the re-
capturing data on the unconscious processes searcher resist ‘imposing cognitively driven order
mobilized by researchers as their research unfolds, onto always provisional and uncertain knowledge’
and the unconscious dynamics created between (Hollway, 2013: 25).
researchers and others (e.g. respondents, research
supervisors, co-researchers) as they interact.
This allows us to delve deeply into the imagined Why fantasy?
domains generated by doing grounded research
on emotions. We see this as especially helpful Fantasy occurs continuously in daily life (Freud,
in broadening and augmenting the ‘imaginative 1953). It refers to ‘the endless materializations of
interpretations’ (Charmaz, 2008: 157) that are unconscious life’ (Frosh, 2002: 51), to an active un-
central to grounded analysis. conscious mind that is constantly generating ideas
Second, unconscious defences become visible and images through which we see the world. In this
in the fantasies researchers can create to defend paper, fantasy refers to imaginative ideas or stories
against unwanted emotion mobilized by doing connected to the researcher’s psychic life and to
research. Although fantasies can represent de- unconscious relations during a period of research.
fences against emotion, accessing them also pro- As researchers, part of what we do is to ‘take refuge
vides opportunities for creative insights that sup- in plausible stories’ (Phillips, 2014: 9) and in evoca-
port imaginative interpretation. We argue that tive resonances beneath the level of consciousness
the fantasy work of the researcher is an impor- (Bollas, 2009). Such resonances become especially
tant element in the process of analysis through important within the conduct of research when the
which general assertions emerge that provide emotional experience of the task threatens to over-
a basis for theory building. In this paper, we whelm the researcher.


C 2018 British Academy of Management.
Emotional Dimensions to Grounded Theory 3

Table 1. Examples of fantasies that might emerge in grounded theory research

Example of unconscious
Fantasy building Rationalization Example of defensive behaviour feelings managed
Fantasy of containment: The Deploying the GT method Initial written drafts complied Underlying knowledge
anxieties of being a novice correctly will eradicate with GT conventions but insecurities and persistent
confusion in the data. were ‘unreadable’. anxieties about lack of
research expertise.
Fantasy of coherence: The Reliance on the GT method Intellectual word spinning: 150 Strong and potentially
anxieties of being will create order amongst labels masquerading as overwhelming feelings which
overwhelmed chaotic data. categories. unsettled and, at times,
undermined the capacity to
think.
Fantasy of purity: The anxieties Blaming the GT method will Becoming stuck and blaming Insistent feelings of fear,
of incompetence alleviate researcher feelings the methodology. incompetence and shame.
of shame and incompetence.

Our illustrations come from a single researcher’s rational thought and intensify fantasy. Anxiety
use of Glaser’s version of GT (Glaser, 1998). The alerts us to the possible existence of threats, but
first author was using this method, feeling con- also (potentially) how to deal with them (Gabriel,
fused by it, learning how to apply it and becoming 2008). Our narrative of research experience shows
aware of her emerging critique of it, all at the same how researcher anxieties were managed through
time. Two insights emerged from this experience. the generation of three fantasies (of containment,
First, it led to questioning the assumption that GT coherence and purity). These ofered data about
unfolds in sequential steps (Strauss and Corbin, how the emotional dynamics of researcher ex-
1990) along a research path of systematic guidance perience and emergent method informed and
towards theory building. As researchers, we are influenced the project of theory building. These
constantly involved in the fantastical pursuit fantasies are summarized in Table 1.
of theory as a container of random experience.
Theory represents the fantasy of order emergent
from confusion. It creates perspective, introduces Diferences between our perspective and other
coherence and ofers interpretation of experiences. approaches to researcher emotion
It illuminates that which is confusing through The theme of our paper falls within the broader
the application of our ‘disciplined imagination’ domain of how emotions are mobilized in the pro-
(Weick, 1989). The coherence that theory ofers cess of doing research. For example, Harlos et al.
is imagined as much as it is logically derived (2003: 313) explain how their emotions intruded
from data. We found that articulating a fantasy- on the task of meaning making: ‘ . . . we were ag-
building process alongside the theory-building itated, nervous, and uneasy as we tried to articu-
process transformed the lived experience of using late our struggles to make a cursory meaning of
GT into insights that extended the potential for the text’. They describe making ‘explicit their ini-
imaginative interpretations within our analysis. tial thoughts and feelings about the data’ (308).
We recognized that the fantasies mobilized by the Saunders, Lewis and Thornhill (2015) invite re-
emotional experience of doing GT become the ba- searchers to ‘consider your emotions and how to
sis for resonances that illuminate theory building. manage these during this process of being an in-
Second, we highlight anxieties generated ternal researcher’. They suggest that a researcher
through the researcher’s employment of GT and needs to learn to ‘cope with the degree of detach-
reflect on the ways in which these anxieties may ment’ (Saunders, Lewis and Thornhill, 2015: 209)
be applied back into the research to promote to manage feelings of becoming overwhelmed by
insights about the depth and value of this method. large amounts of data.
Here, anxiety refers to the underlying anticipation The general advice is to acknowledge one’s
that something will go wrong. We do not know emotions, to expect their impact on the research
if something bad waits around the corner, but project and to have a coherent strategy for man-
we expect it nonetheless. Such feelings disrupt aging potential incoherence. A psychodynamic


C 2018 British Academy of Management.
4 A. Clancy and R. Vince

perspective on emotion additionally acknowledges Calling such dependency into question encourages
unconscious attachment to emotions that are cam- the novice researcher to ask about the emotions
ouflaged or avoided; that ‘it is diicult to conceive that are associated with such dependency, as well
of the research relationship without considering as how emotional responses contribute to reflexive
transference and counter-transference’ (Gabriel, engagement with the research process.
1999: 276) and that states of mind are both
hidden from subjects yet shape their thoughts and
behaviour (Rustin, 2009). Grounded theory: a popular
Our fantasy of coherence engages with these and contested method
ideas by highlighting the hidden ways in which
emotion presents. It may not be possible (or wise) Grounded theory is a popular method that has
to ‘manage’ emotion if it presents in unusual and been used to research a diverse range of topics
creative ways. Exploring the fantasy of research as (O’Callaghan, 2012; Sare and Bales, 2014). The
a coherent process allows for the unknown, the in- method is widespread in qualitative business
coherent and the unexpected to emerge as research and management research (O’Reilly, Paper and
insights rather than disruptions. Marx, 2012; Partington, 2000). The purpose of
‘Consider your emotions’ (Saunders, Lewis a grounded theory study is to experience the
and Thornhill, 2015) is important advice for problem, issue or meaning from the perspective of
researchers. However, it can be useful to move the research respondents and to develop an inte-
beyond the assumption that a researcher always grated set of conceptual hypotheses about what is
knows what she is feeling when she is feeling it. going on. GT fits well with a study aimed at the
Strategies for managing the anxieties and feelings development of a suggestive theory, where there
associated with being a researcher (such as keeping is no strong theoretical basis from which to de-
a fieldwork diary) rely on the researcher being velop well-focused research questions. Grounded
conscious of what was felt, what has worked and theorists start their research process with data
what has not. A researcher must not only notice and develop theories that are generated from their
the emotional impact of participation in a research analysis and conceptualization of data, as distinct
encounter, but also be able to reflect on the raw from logical deduction from a priori assumptions.
emotional experience evoked by it (Hollway, 2016). However, there are continuing and passionate
Our experience was of tension between aware- debates about the way GT is deployed. The uses
ness and ignorance, knowing and not knowing, of GT vary widely across the spectrum of possible
and the subsequent fear that a novice researcher application, ‘from orthodox and classic GT, to
may not know what she is doing. Our fantasy of GT light . . . to one calorie-only GT’ (Gumme-
purity reflects the complexity of emotional experi- son, 2011: 232). For some researchers, the GT
ence for the novice researcher; the anxiety of do- debate has taken on a ‘life of its own’ (Corley,
ing things ‘the wrong way’. Exploring this fan- 2015: 5) with diferences in approach and use
tasy invited questions about whether these tensions of the methodology tending ‘to blur the overall
and uncertainties can ofer insight on the chosen scope and reach of GT’ (Walsh et al., 2015:
method and the orthodoxy that surrounds its use. 2). Complicating matters, but not inconsistent
Researchers can become attached to specific with the debate over the identity of GT, these
methods, and to the idea of ‘brand identity’ same variations and adaptations, when viewed
(Pritchard, 2012); that adherence to one method- from a diferent perspective, signal assessment of
ological choice will ‘secure legitimacy and credibil- GT as containing the ‘hallmarks of a successful
ity with reviewers and examiners’ (Pritchard, 2012: methodology’ (Corley, 2015: 5).
132). This idea is an acknowledgement of a feel- Researchers must decide which version of GT
ing of connection to a method, of an understand- to follow. This may not be a problem for expe-
ing that ensures it is being used in expected and rienced researchers. For the novice, it provides a
acceptable ways. Our fantasy of containment rec- background of continuing uncertainty about the
ognizes that it is important to articulate a clear correct approach to take, as well as feeding the
and legitimate methodological choice, but also that sense that there is an ideal form of GT that is some-
the novice researcher can experience this as depen- where to be found. Unconsciously, disagreements
dency on the infallibility of her chosen method. that have characterized the method can become an


C 2018 British Academy of Management.
Emotional Dimensions to Grounded Theory 5

aspect of the lived experience of being a novice GT that might account for them’ (Charmaz, 2008:
researcher. GT researchers are encouraged to de- 157). GT therefore requires ‘imaginative interpre-
ploy their theoretical sensitivity: ‘the sensitive in- tations’ (Charmaz, 2008: 157) as the researcher
sight of the observer himself’ (Glaser and Strauss, both reasons and imagines possible theoretical
1967: 251) to distinguish core categories and re- accounts in the data to identify the most plausible
lationships from extraneous detours. In practice, explanation. This process has become characteris-
theoretical sensitivity is the process by which re- tic of much qualitative research. We illustrate this,
searchers lived experience is deployed as a lens for example, in Figure 1, taken from a well-known
through which to interrogate data. However, this is qualitative research textbook (Saldaña, 2016).
where anxieties begin, because it can be diicult for This process is presented as a model of ‘progress
the novice researcher to trust her own judgement. toward the thematic, conceptual and theoretical’
The tension between deploying the method in and as an ‘ideal and streamlined scheme’ (Saldaña,
the correct manner and the invitation to bring per- 2016: 14). It is a very good general illustration of
sonal experience to the analysis of data is both an the process of analysis through diferent elements
enticing and an anxiety-producing prospect. The of coding and categorization. The model comes
contested nature of grounded theory can create un- with a caveat, to ‘keep in mind that the actual act
certainty about the best way to put it into practice, of reaching theory is much more complex than
and (as we illustrate in the examples below) this un- illustrated’ (Saldaña, 2016: 14). Our experience
certainty is reflected in emotional responses from as researchers certainly supports this caveat,
the researcher. To understand the consequences although we also believe that the complexity is as
of the lived experience of the method, we think much emotional as it is ‘in mind’.
that it is important to engage with a key question: Theory building is future orientated, and man-
How do the emotional dynamics of researcher ex- agement researchers have been invited to develop
perience and emergent method inform and influence ‘theoretical prescience’, to engage with ‘incipient
the project of theory building? The starting point for organizational, managerial and societal issues and
answering this question is to consider what is in- problems’ (Corley and Gioia, 2011: 23). Another
volved in theory building and how emotions and way to put this is that theory building is about the
unconscious dynamics (in the form of fantasies process of discerning what we need to know by
about the method) may be integral to this process. ‘making informed projections’ (Corley, 2015: 25).
In addition, theory building is dynamic not static,
it provides highly relevant but also necessarily pro-
Theory building visional insights into connections that are likely
to evolve and change. Despite the practical util-
Theory advances knowledge through original ity of existing models, imaginative interpretation
insights into the connections among phenomena, requires more than the systematic identification of
and informs and supports developments in prac- codes, categories and assumptions. Our capacity to
tice. ‘A good theory explains, predicts and delights’ imagine and interpret is tied to the person of the
(Sutton and Staw, 1995: 378). This simple, elegant researcher, as well as to the emotions mobilized by
description captures the intersection between the putting the role of researcher into practice. We re-
explanatory power of theory to inform and guide, fer to this additional aspect as ‘fantasy building’.
to be relevant to current and emerging issues
and to excite our interest through the discovery
of novel, perhaps counter-intuitive, connections. Fantasy building
This raises the question of how to build theory to
explain, predict and delight. The underlying emotional dynamics within a
The way in which GT helps us to do this is by researcher role can involve projections of a dif-
providing the researcher with a systematic, induc- ferent sort. These emerge as an unconscious
tive approach for collecting and analysing data to defensive response to the anxieties generated by
develop theoretical analyses (Strauss and Corbin, the role, through the emotional highs and lows of
1998). GT starts with an inductive logic but doing research, and in association with others –
becomes abductive, involving ‘intuitive interpreta- including respondents, co-researchers or academic
tion of empirical observations and creative ideas supervisors. In using the term ‘fantasy building’


C 2018 British Academy of Management.
6 A. Clancy and R. Vince

Figure 1. A streamlined codes-to-theory model for qualitative inquiry [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
Source: Saldaña (2016: 14).

we do not mean to imply that the generation of decision-making, particularly hedge funds, that
unconscious fantasy is a logical process akin to touch a potent underlying desire for wealth. They
placing building blocks on top of each other. emphasize the role (and dangers) of unconscious
Fantasy gathers its threads by relying on our fantasy as ‘the excitement of investing in what
psychic and physical limits and capabilities, our hedge funds represented became divorced from the
consciousness of mental and embodied percep- anxiety associated with the potential consequences
tion. Knowledge from this perspective begins in of taking excessive investment risk’ (Eshraghi and
‘wishful unconscious desire’ (Phillips, 2014: 51). Taler, 2012: 1245).
For example, a common doctoral student fear is The experience of doing research mobilizes com-
that someone else will write a PhD in the same plex and contradictory feelings. The researcher is
area (perhaps even with the same title) before they challenged to tolerate paradoxical feelings and (at
have finished, thereby undermining their unique the same time) to complete the task that prompted
contribution to knowledge. This fantasy captures the feelings in the first place. Emotional resonances
several intersecting anxieties and other mixed feel- are integral to the research encounter (Prasad,
ings generated around the seemingly monumental 2014; Ulus, 2015). By paying attention to defences,
task of doctoral research and writing. to projective processes, to the anxieties of doing re-
Various scholars (Armstrong, 2005; Ekman, search, unconscious emotions can become avail-
2013; Gabriel, 1995; Glynos, 2008; Lapping, 2016) able for interrogation and interpretation by the
argue for the value of fantasy as a form of in- researcher (Ogden, 1994). Fantasies that are emer-
dividual and systemic intelligence that cannot be gent from anxiety can be recognized as produc-
accessed by purely ‘rational’ approaches. These tive research insights, and as potentially creative in
scholars outline not only the operational concep- their imagining of future possibility.
tion of fantasy but also its importance in theoriz- In our view, theory building and fantasy build-
ing organizational dilemmas that are hidden from ing are parallel processes, sitting side by side as col-
view. For example, Eshraghi and Taler (2012) laborative and disruptive partners in the process
consider the role of the unconscious in investment of doing research. The desired outcome of theory


C 2018 British Academy of Management.
Emotional Dimensions to Grounded Theory 7

building is to make a diference by disconfirming resonances within the research process were fun-
the obvious and challenging existing knowledge, damental to the emergence of these insights about
finding gaps and cracks between established frame- disappointment.
works, while also ofering an alternative, com-
pelling narrative (Alvesson and Sandberg, 2013).
Methods used to explore the emotional dimensions
Our experience of using the grounded theory
to grounded theory
method to study emotion in organizations is that
the unsettled and unsettling environment of theory As researchers, we are afected by the tension be-
building is fertile ground for the generation of un- tween the theoretical outcomes we are hoping for
conscious fantasy. We are therefore taking a first and the process of their discovery. The study of
step in the research documentation of a relation- disappointment mobilized strong emotions and
ship between theory building and fantasy building. vivid unconscious fantasies for me as a researcher.
Along with my academic supervisor (second au-
thor), I became interested in how the emotional dy-
Research context and approach namics of researcher experience were afecting the
project of theory building. I reflected (individually
This section of the paper provides the reader with and in discussion with my supervisor) on my pro-
a first-person (first-author) description of the re- crastinations, excuses, missed deadlines and inter-
search situation from which the present ideas were mittent desire to quit. An entry from my research
drawn. The subject of my doctoral research was journal highlights one example:
disappointment within organizations, and how
this feeling enters individuals’ ways of being and I was due to fly to the UK today to meet Russ. At
acting in their work lives. The design and theoret- the last minute, the flight was cancelled by the airline.
ical results of this study can be read in a previ- My emotional response to the cancellation caught
ously published paper (Clancy, Vince and Gabriel, me by surprise. I was both thrilled and angered in
2012). However, I want to say a little about the equal measure. I was prepared for the meeting and
core findings, because they emerged concurrently had a list of items I wanted to discuss face-to-face.
from my analysis of my data and my emotional ex- At the same time, I was relieved not to meet because
perience as a researcher. First, I found that feel- it would have meant confronting the part of me that
ings of disappointment are processed internally is feeling disappointed in my progress.
much more than they are given voice. Second, I
found that disappointment is strongly associated Feelings such as failure, anger, blame, incompe-
with anger projected onto others as blame or am- tence, at first rejected or relegated to the margins
bivalence. Third, I found that disappointment is of the research, were brought to the fore and ex-
bound up with conflictual feelings of failure (e.g. amined as elements of the overall data. This aspect
the tension between acknowledging failure in/of of my experience highlighted the complex role of
the organization and maintaining positive feelings researcher emotion. It became clear that my emo-
towards the organization and its goals). tional experience mirrored the emotional experi-
Using these core findings as a basis for theory ence of research participants who had expressed
building, I determined that disappointment is similar feelings in relation to disappointment at
experienced either as failure of self (I am disap- work.
pointing) or as failure of other (I am disappointed). Alongside my grounded theory method, I
I also discovered something counter-intuitive, wanted to capture emotional and unconscious
which is that when disappointment is owned as processes at work in my approach to the research.
an ordinary aspect of experience (I disappoint), it I did this in three interconnected ways: through
is transformed. The fantasy of a failed self/other, supervisory sessions focused on free association,
once acknowledged, loses its disabling grip, and a process by which the individual speaks without
disappointment can be understood as a core censoring their thoughts; by recording and reflect-
part of relating. Indeed, disappointment can be ing on my dreams as a form of wish-fulfilment
reframed as tolerable and ordinary rather than (Freud, 1953); and through auto-ethnographic
an emotion that needs to be hidden, avoided or engagement with my lived experience of emotions
displaced elsewhere. My experiences of emotional and unconscious processes within the research.


C 2018 British Academy of Management.
8 A. Clancy and R. Vince

Drawing on Kvale’s (2003) approach, Russ participants. It also reminded me of a statement


and I reframed some of our conversations (10 made by a research participant in which he said,
in total over the period of the research) by ap- ‘all great literature is about disappointment’. I
plying psychodynamic theory to my emotional made an association between established litera-
experience. We explored researcher/researched ture (Jane Austen) and prospective literature (a
and supervisor/student relationships as a way PhD thesis). The inevitability of the relationship
of surfacing fantasy, anxiety and unconscious between expectation and disappointment pointed
associations. I became aware of the value of the way to strong feelings when something of
impasses, which usually occurred at a moment significance was at stake (an opening night, publi-
when I was confronted with the fantasy of how cation or Viva Voce examination). The dream also
things ‘should’ be. We tried to notice unconscious ofered insight into an anxiety and subsequent
thoughts and feelings to bring out the emotional fantasy (coherence). Would I succeed in becoming
landscape of the research. Supervision sessions proficient in a new (academic) language to com-
that were dedicated to free association encouraged municate a story? Would that story gain credible
me to articulate my inner monologue about my (or damming) reviews on its opening night (the
experience of the research, and to bring this into Viva Voce examination)?
dialogue. This was not about saying whatever was My emotions, associations and dreams were de-
on my mind, but rather provided an opportunity veloped through auto-ethnographic reflections on
for me to become immersed in the details of this my lived experience of the research. ‘When re-
inner monologue and to link one set of emerging searchers write auto-ethnographies, they seek to
ideas about the research to the next. produce aesthetic and evocative thick descriptions
I also documented dreams as a way of ‘voicing of personal and interpersonal experience’ (Ellis,
the unspoken’ (Finlay, 2002: 531). These were ex- Adams and Bochner, 2010: 4). Reflection from a
plored in three ways. (1) I recorded and reflected psychodynamic perspective is not the same as cog-
on dreams in my research diary. (2) The dreams nitive activity, ‘it requires keeping an open mind . . .
were re-explored through the lens of ‘disappoint- [and that] is a supremely emotional process’ (Holl-
ment’. This was accomplished by the first author as way, 2016: 21). This was an important distinction
an individual writing task. (3) The dreams were re- for me. I did not wish to simply indulge myself in
explored within academic supervision as relational my own view of the world, and allow my feelings
dynamics between the researcher/researched and to lead me towards certainties that did not reflect
the supervisor/student. An example of a dream the complexities of what was observed (Hollway,
fragment documented by the researcher provides 2016). Rather, through this writing, I could go be-
an illustration of the type of data gathered: yond description and begin to articulate the un-
conscious fantasies that emerged for me in doing
I am waiting for the curtain to rise at the opening
GT research.
night of a theatre production of Pride and Prejudice
by Jane Austen. (In my waking life, this is one of my
favourite novels.) A friend who has not previously Three fantasies mobilized by my
seen the production accompanies me (she is not fa-
miliar with the book). I am full of anticipation and
experience of GT research
excitement about seeing the play and about sharing
As a novice researcher, I embraced GT like an am-
the experience. As the curtain rises I realize that the
ateur cook following a renowned chef’s recipe for a
actors are speaking a foreign language. My feeling in
delicious stew. My belief was more attuned to the
the dream is one of shame . . . I should have known
authority of the method, its guarantees of success
the play was not in English. What will my friend
and reliability, than any certainty about the out-
think of me for inviting her to something she cannot
come of my research enterprise. Initially, I was not
possibly understand?
thinking about how to utilize GT, but rather of my
The dream is a simple story about disappointment own anxieties. It would only dawn on me slowly
– wanting to share something of importance that my methods might have something to do
with a close friend and feeling shamed when with what I was feeling. Often, I simply wanted to
the experience does not match expectation. This drop the whole enterprise, to flee back to well-trod
dream mirrored many stories shared by research avenues of life experience where I remembered


C 2018 British Academy of Management.
Emotional Dimensions to Grounded Theory 9

myself as competent. Gradually, I began to link Clues about a containment fantasy emerged in
my emotions to the inquiry in which I was en- the initial written drafts about the project. The
gaged. In so doing, I began to see that my wish drafts were ‘technically’ correct, they adhered
for GT as a rational research recipe was a defence to the formal conventions of GT writing, but
against the anxieties mobilized in me by doing ultimately the wording was so opaque that it
research. killed of the light of curiosity. Investing GT with
The feelings associated with being a novice infallibility protected me from the anxiety and
researcher promoted and sustained considerable uncertainties of being a novice GT researcher,
defensive energy. As I became more familiar with from the damage that my inexperience might
researcher reflexivity (Alvesson and Sköldberg, inflict on the research process. Awarding authority
2009), I also became curious about how the archi- to the method helped me to manage uncomfort-
tecture of failure I had constructed might help me able feelings associated with my inexperience.
to make sense of the emotional dynamics asso- Unfortunately, this meant that I also inadver-
ciated with my research project. As the ‘primary tently constrained key features of the research
instrument’ (Pezalla, Pettigrew and Miller-Day, process that I eventually found helpful – curiosity,
2012: 183) in the research process, I embarked ignorance and experimentation. In my illusory
on a reflexive inquiry to uncover fantasies I had search for containment, I developed an uncritical
constructed to manage the anxieties of doing GT. reliance on the GT method and language, invest-
Each of these fantasies is outlined in the following ing them with implicit efectiveness and loading
subsections. them with responsibility for managing my fear of
failure.
A fantasy of containment: the anxieties of being a
novice A fantasy of coherence: the anxieties of being
overwhelmed
The emotional experience of trying to ‘make
sense’ of data was at times overwhelming. I felt My anxiety was related to an uncontained mass
confused, ignorant, lacking in competence and, at of thought and observations; and moved through
a very simple level, blind to the richness contained an initial stage of self-condemnation, ultimately
in participant descriptions of their relationship resulting in incoherence. Around these feelings
with workplace disappointments. My anxieties I created a fantasy of coherence, that the GT
were linked to this ever-present sense of confusion method creates order from chaos. Having col-
and my attempts to manage my anxieties were lected a large amount of data, I was confronted
supported by a fantasy of containment. My con- with a dilemma – how would I know what I was
fusion about the task of making sense of data as looking for? This dilemma conflicted directly
a novice GT researcher was managed through a with adopting a rigorous approach to analysis
fantasy about the infallibility of the method. If I to maintain theoretical control over what is
relied on the GT method, and deployed it in the emerging from the data (Glaser, 1998). While
correct way, then the confusion in the data would advocating that ‘all is data’, Glaser (2002) is
be manageable (the confusion was, of course, still quite specific about the steps involved in data
there). The following extract captures some of this analysis: coding, constant comparative analysis,
emotional confusion: theoretical sampling and memoing. The purpose
of these steps is to guide the researcher towards
I’ve completed and transcribed five interviews at this the inter-relationship between emergent concepts
point. I know the data are rich but there’s such a lot (Glaser, 1998). Unfortunately, despite constant
of material here. I am reading and rereading Glaser’s comparative analysis, theoretical sampling and
advice on working with the data. It seems straightfor- memoing, no relationships seemed immediately
ward enough so I’ll be guided by the advice and start apparent. The method ofered me little help in the
working on codes as my next phase. I’m worried in organization of my thinking. Psychodynamic the-
case I can’t make sense of this material . . . and I feel ory provided conceptual elements corresponding
a very long way away from coming up with anything to emergent data. This cast important light upon
that resembles ‘theory’. I guess it’s one foot in front my findings, but the pragmatic importation of
of the other? an existing theoretical perspective to supplement


C 2018 British Academy of Management.
10 A. Clancy and R. Vince

gaps in GT was my first indication that I might be How is it possible at this stage in my life to feel so
stepping outside of the orthodoxy. For example: stupid? I never realized that this would be such a
painful process . . . moving from some semblance
I’m drowning in data. I even had a dream last night of competence in the ‘real’ world to this feeling
in which I got into trouble swimming in the sea. Ev- of utter incompetence in this new world of re-
eryone I speak to has a story about disappointment. search. I feel as though I have to learn a new
I’ve gone from loving talking about what I’m doing language (Russian for example) with which to
to hiding from it. I love Glaser’s idea that everything communicate the most basic of concepts. I thought
is ‘data’ but I’m realizing that I don’t know what I’m GT would be more dictionary/map than ‘rules of
looking for or, more to the point, I’m not sure how grammar’.
to use the method to find what I am looking for. I
don’t want any more examples, or data, or thoughts, I found it increasingly diicult to progress my
or feelings . . . theorizing. I felt lost in a roundabout without
exit, and I came to relate more and more to a
Started coding interviews this week so it feels good pure form of GT. Furthermore, because ‘theory’
to be busy and engaged with the data. But when is is in the title of the method, ‘theory’ would surely
a code a code? Or a label? Or a category? I started result if only I could deploy GT in the appropriate
out being confused with the data and now I’m con- manner. The circular feeling under this fantasy
fused with the method. Beginning to regret having boiled down to this: I had generated the idea of
chosen GT . . . I wonder if it’s too late to change to an ideal GT against which I blamed myself for
some other method that might ofer a bit more co- shameful incompetence and, at the same time,
herence? All I can think about now is whether or not blamed the impurity of my own GT deployment.
I’ll manage to eke out theory from this jumble of un- Every attempt to penetrate this fantasy deepened
certainty. I’m exhausted feeling confused! my sense of shame and blame. I gradually realized
that multiple interpretative possibilities (e.g.
My fantasy that GT would create order and coher- psychodynamic theory) split the GT approach
ence amongst chaotic data was illusory. The chaos into (a) method and (b) an interpretive tool. This
still existed even though I acted as if order had produced an important insight. My initial fantasy
been imposed. For example, I generated 150 ‘cate- of a robust container for all types of data and anal-
gories’, which increased my confusion. The fantasy ysis met the reality of a method open to multiple
of coherence helped me to manage strong and dis- interpretations.
ruptive feelings, which threatened at times to over-
whelm me emotionally, thereby undermining my
capacity to think. The methodological steps I had These fantasies are interesting!
taken – importing aspects of another theory, hasty I began to recognize that exposing the presence
attention to coding and debate about the coding of fantasy constructions within my conception of
process itself – were insuicient to dispel my appre- GT allowed me both to contain anxiety and to
hensions. I experienced myself as a disappointing think productively about my fantasies as contri-
researcher. I was also on to something. I had begun butions to theory building. My anxieties started
to blame the structure of my inquiry, GT itself. to be transformed through a process of reflexive
engagement with the lived experience of utilizing
GT. Fantasies are always with us. They tend to
A fantasy of purity: the anxieties of incompetence
occur in moments of anxiety. However, I could
The notion that there is an ideal form of method relate to them by surfacing their active presence
and a correct way to proceed arises from the ortho- as ongoing, internally generated obstacles to clear
doxy that surrounds GT (particularly Glaser’s ver- thinking. By naming them, I could clarify their
sion). This illusion of an ideal, intuited from GT’s function, maintain a position of curiosity in rela-
contested methodology, alerted me to a fantasy of tion to their presence and decode their relevance
purity. I feared that I would contaminate the re- in relation to the method and subject of inquiry.
search by deploying a version other than its purist My curiosity about fantasy allowed these stories
form. I felt frustration, anger and guilt with myself to inform my thinking rather than obscure it
and with GT. For example: completely.


C 2018 British Academy of Management.
Emotional Dimensions to Grounded Theory 11

Discussion and conclusion researchers to become aware of the fantasies


we generate, as well as their function and ap-
Our interest in the emotional dimensions of GT propriateness within our research. Fantasy can
was stimulated by a gradual understanding of have complex and competing roles in our stud-
the similarity between the emotions expressed by ies, and function both defensively and creatively.
research participants and the emotions evoked for It can mask potentially disruptive emotions, keep-
the researcher by using the chosen method. This ing them safely contained so that the research en-
is not surprising because ‘the (GT) method does deavour may continue. It also reflects emotional
not stand outside the research process; it resides responses that, when brought to awareness, can
within it’ (Charmaz, 2008: 160). A key value in deepen and enrich the theories we are seeking to
the method is that it is ‘inductive, indeterminate generate from GT research. A key question there-
and open-ended’ (Charmaz, 2008: 156). What is fore is, using unconscious emotions and fantasies
surprising is that the method does not explicitly generated by research, what should we be doing to
seek to make links with the emotional dynamics be imaginative in our theory building?
that are inevitably stimulated by such uncertainty.
To start to address this issue, we suggest that Implications for practice
there can be a parallel relationship between theory
building and fantasy building. We have provided We have emphasized throughout this paper the
examples of three dimensions of fantasy that were importance of the ‘researcher’s subjectivity as
integral to the experience of a novice researcher in an instrument of knowing’ (Hollway, 2009: 463)
the context of her research. These fantasies arose and we have also shown the value of unconscious
from anxieties about how to apply GT efectively, fantasy as a powerful resource of research in-
but the same anxieties informed insights about the telligence. This raises the question of how the
method itself and about the theoretical dimensions researcher gives ‘an authentic account’ if so much
generated within her study. In this final section of ‘is hidden from view’ (Nicholls, 2017: 27). How do
the paper we discuss some key points that have researchers identify and work with unconscious
emerged from our experience to further clarify processes in the service of theory building? We
our contribution. ofer the following suggestions.
The fantasies emergent from anxiety in this Keeping a dream journal is one way in which
study provided imaginative interpretations of the the ‘unconscious embrace’ (Hollway, 2013: 170) of
relationship between the researcher and the re- the research task can be identified. The dream nar-
search, as well as supporting theory building on ratives can be explored by questioning: why this
the theme of disappointment in organizations. We dream, why now? We found that looking for asso-
propose that emotions, fantasies and the processes ciations between dreams and the research ofered
surrounding their management are likely to be cen- both an unsettling and a productive focus on re-
tral to our own and others’ experience of using searcher experience. In addition, dreams ofer a
GT and should be attended to as an integral el- method for uncovering our unconscious motives in
ement of the overall data. This is consistent with ways that strengthen reflexivity because our expe-
common procedures in GT research. For example, rience is ‘invariably complex, ambiguous, ambiva-
if the aim of utilizing the GT method is the pro- lent’ (Finlay, 2002: 186). There are several helpful
duction of ‘rich descriptions’ (Corbin and Strauss, readings on how researchers can use dreams to in-
1990: 3) then their richness and depth can be en- form research (Back, 2007; Nicholls, 2017; Ogden,
hanced by data that aligns with emotions and fan- 2004).
tasies mobilized by the researcher’s investigation The supervisory relationship is one area in
of a particular research context. It becomes possi- which the emotional dynamics of doing research
ble therefore to identify patterns and variations of can be explored as data. It can provide a holding
emotional resonances that inform and enrich the space in which the unmentionable, unthinkable
emerging themes of our research. and unknowable are contained until such time as
The fantasy work of the researcher is an im- the researcher can inquire into and reincorporate
portant element in the production of creative them as research data (French, 1997). We used
hunches and ideas that can arise from using GT. the technique of free association, which creates an
We think that it is possible and desirable for environment where it is possible for the researcher


C 2018 British Academy of Management.
12 A. Clancy and R. Vince

to speak her inner monologue about her experi- mobilized by researchers as their research un-
ence of the research. For example, in this research folds, and between researchers and others as
about disappointment, this monologue was often they interact. We argue that the fantasy work
associated with how the theme of the research was of the researcher is an important element in
embedded in the researcher’s own emotional and the process of analysis through which general
unconscious experience. In emphasizing free as- assertions emerge that provide a basis for theory
sociation within supervision, we are asserting that building. Researchers have much to learn about
meaning lies not only in the manifest and latent the complex emotional dynamics of using the
content of what is said, but also in the unconscious GT method. We have interpreted Glaser’s dictum
thoughts that link one set of emerging ideas about that ‘all is data’ (Glaser, 2007) as an invitation to
the research to the next. Free association is helpful include unconscious emotion, masked by defences,
(alongside other ways of thinking and feeling) in emergent in dreams and formed into fantasies,
noticing the fantasies connected with the emerging that are intimately tied to the design, process and
nature of the analysis. It encourages unconscious lived experience of the research.
feelings and motivations to surface, allowing for A psychodynamic approach ofers opportuni-
the same level of scrutiny as other data (Clarke, ties for researchers using grounded theory, and
2002). other inductive approaches to thematic analysis,
The research journal is a commonly used to deepen their imaginative interpretations of the
reflexive tool in qualitative research. It provides data by capturing elements of the unconscious
a mechanism through which researchers can dynamics that are part of being a researcher and
document the methodological decisions they of doing research. Our approach is important
make throughout their studies, track their analysis because it is not only about a researcher capturing
process, consider their own emotions and the roles emotions, but also about the ways in which emo-
they play in the process, document insights and tion captures the researcher. In this sense, we are
consider researcher bias (Orange, 2016). From a contributing to broader arguments about research
psychodynamic perspective, journals provide an as both personal involvement and professional
insight into the defended self. By this we mean distance. ‘In telling her own story, a discerning
that what we consciously document is only part scholar can build on her personal involvement to
of the story. Journals also provide an insight into develop insights that can significantly contribute
what is excluded, hidden and not transparent to to and sharpen the analysis. But distance also
ourselves (Hollway, 2009). needs to be upheld for such insights to emerge . . . ’
Psychodynamic approaches are both an ontol- (Anteby, 2013: 1283). The emotional life of the
ogy and an epistemology (Deveroux, 1967). As researcher provides ‘data points for insightful
an ontology, they ‘emphasise the efects of afect, analysis’ (Anteby, 2013: 1283). Unconscious
dynamic conflict, unconscious intersubjective emotions associated with the researcher’s role and
processes and embodied practices’ (Hollway, relations provide insights into the fantasies we
2009: 464). From an epistemological perspective, create. These help us to delve inside at the same
psychodynamic approaches deepen researcher time as allowing us to step back from and analyse
subjectivity as ‘an instrument of knowing’ (Red- the emotional experience of our research.
man, 2016: 464). Psychodynamic perspectives
therefore add an additional ‘layer of interpreta-
Limitations and final thoughts
tion, addressing unconscious communication and
motivation’ (Clarke, 2002: 191). We believe that We are aware that there are limitations to our argu-
the tools we have outlined can help researchers ments and assertions within this paper. First, they
to explore the unconscious and emotional dimen- are based on a single researcher’s use of GT within
sions of carrying out grounded theory in novel her (doctoral) research. We cannot show that other
and interesting ways. researchers would generate similar (or indeed any)
fantasies using the GT method, and this was
not an aim of the paper. However, we have been
GT and beyond
encouraged, as these ideas developed and were
For GT, our approach adds an example of how publicly discussed at conferences and seminars,
to capture data on the unconscious processes by the strong associations and similar experiences


C 2018 British Academy of Management.
Emotional Dimensions to Grounded Theory 13

that were acknowledged by other researchers dynamics of mutual recognition’, Human Relations, 66, pp.
using both GT (not only novice researchers) 1159–1181.
Ellis, C., T. E. Adams and A. P. Bochner (2010). ‘Autoethnog-
and grounded approaches to qualitative analysis.
raphy: an overview’, Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 12,
Second, we do not imagine that our enthusiasm pp. 1–13.
for a psychodynamic approach to researcher Eshraghi, A. and R. Taler (2012). ‘Hedge funds and uncon-
experience and practice is widely shared. We know scious fantasy’, Accounting, Auditing & Accountability Journal,
from experience in research and scholarly writing 25, pp. 1244–1265.
Finlay, L. (2002). ‘Negotiating the swamp: the opportunity and
that not everyone is interested in what is going on
challenge of reflexivity in research practice’, Qualitative Re-
under the surface, or in the emotions associated search, 2, pp. 209–230.
with doing research. We are enthusiastic about our French, R. (1997). ‘The teacher as container of anxiety: psycho-
approach, but we are also aware that it can make analysis and the role of teacher’, Journal of Management Edu-
theory building more complicated than it already cation, 21, pp. 483–495.
Freud, S. (1953). ‘The interpretation of dreams’. In J. Stra-
is. Neither are we attempting to establish an
chey (ed.), The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological
orthodoxy of our own based on a psychodynamic Works of Sigmund Freud, Volumes IV & V, 1900–1901, p. 480.
perspective on GT. Our aim is relatively simple, London: Hogarth Press.
we want to open the possibilities for emotional Frosh, S. (2002). Key Concepts in Psychoanalysis. London:
dimensions to GT and provide a coherent example British Library.
Gabriel, Y. (1995). ‘The unmanaged organization: stories, fan-
to inspire future methodological work.
tasies and subjectivity’, Organization Studies, 16, pp. 477–501.
Gabriel, Y. (1999) Organizations in Depth. London: Sage.
References Gabriel, Y. (2008). Organizing Words. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Alvesson, M. and J. Sandberg (2013). ‘Has management stud- Glaser, B. G. (1998). Doing Grounded Theory: Issues and Discus-
ies lost its way? Ideas for more imaginative and innovative re- sions. Mill Valley, CA: Sociology Press.
search’, Journal of Management Studies, 50, pp. 128–152. Glaser, B. G. (2002). ‘Constructivist grounded theory?’, Forum
Alvesson, M. and K. Sköldberg (2009). Reflexive Methodology: for Qualitative Research, 3, pp. 1–10.
New Vistas for Qualitative Research. London: Sage. Glaser, B. G. (2007). ‘All is data’, Grounded Theory Review, 6,
Anteby, M. (2013). ‘Relaxing the taboo on telling our own sto- pp. 1–22.
ries: upholding professional distance and personal involve- Glaser, B. G. and A. L. Strauss (1967). The Discovery of Grounded
ment’, Organization Science, 24, pp. 1227–1290. Theory: Strategies for Qualitative Research. New York: Aldine
Armstrong, D. (2005). Organization in the Mind: Psychoanaly- de Gruyter.
sis, Group Relations and Organizational Consultancy. London: Glynos, J. (2008). ‘Ideological fantasy at work’, Journal of Polit-
Karnac Books. ical Ideologies, 13, pp. 275–296.
Back, L. (2007). The Art of Listening. London: Berg. Gummeson, E. (2011). ‘Grounded Glaser’. In A. Bynnild and V.
Bollas, C. (2009). The Evocative Object World. New York: Rout- Martin (eds), Grounded Theory: The Philosophy, Method, and
ledge. Work of Barney Glaser, pp. 222–235. Boca Raton, FL: Brown-
Charmaz, K. (2008). ‘Grounded theory as an emergent method’. Walker Press.
In S. N. Hesse-Biber and P. Leavy (eds), Handbook of Emergent Harlos, K. P., M. Mallon, R. Stablein and C. Jones (2003).
Methods, pp. 155–172. New York: Guilford Press. ‘Teaching qualitative methods in management classrooms’,
Clancy, A., R. Vince and Y. Gabriel (2012). ‘That unwanted feel- Journal of Management Education, 27, pp. 304–322.
ing: a psychodynamic study of disappointment in organiza- Hollway, W. (2009). ‘Applying the “experience-near” principle
tions’, British Journal of Management, 23, pp. 518–531. to research: psychoanalytically informed methods 1’, Journal
Clarke, S. (2002). ‘Learning from experience: psycho-social re- of Social Work Practice, 23, pp. 461–474.
search methods in the social sciences’, Qualitative Research, 2, Hollway, W. (2013). ‘Locating unconscious, “societal-collective”
pp. 173–194. processes in psycho-social research’, Organisational & Social
Corbin, J. M. and A. Strauss (1990). ‘Grounded theory research: Dynamics, 13, pp. 22–40.
procedures, canons and evaluative criteria’, Qualitative Sociol- Hollway, W. (2016). ‘Emotional experience plus reflection: coun-
ogy, 13, pp. 3–21. tertransference and reflexivity in research’, The Psychothera-
Corley, K. G. (2015). ‘A commentary on “what grounded theory pist, 62, pp. 19–21.
is . . . ” Engaging a phenomenon from the perspective of those Kvale, S. (2003). ‘The psychoanalytic interview as inspiration for
living it’, Organizational Research Methods, 18, pp. 600–605. qualitative research’. In P. Camic, H. Rhodes and L. Yardley
Corley, K. G. and D. A. Gioia (2011). ‘Building theory about (eds), Qualitative Research in Psychology, pp. 275–297. Wash-
theory building: what constitutes a theoretical contribution?’, ington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.
Academy of Management Review, 36, pp. 12–32. Lapping, C. (2016). ‘Reflexivity and fantasy: surprising encoun-
Deveroux, G. (1967). From Anxiety to Method in the Behavioral ters from interpretation to interruption’, Qualitative Inquiry,
Sciences. The Hague/Paris: Mouton and Co./École Pratique 22, pp. 718–724.
des Hautes Étides. Martin, P. Y. and B. A. Turner (1986). ‘Grounded theory and or-
Ekman, S. (2013). ‘Fantasies about work as limitless potential ganizational research’, Journal of Applied Behavioral Science,
– how managers and employees seduce each other through 22, pp. 141–157.


C 2018 British Academy of Management.
14 A. Clancy and R. Vince

Nicholls, L. (2017). ‘Discomforting and disquieting: dreaming Rustin, M. (2009). ‘How do psychoanalysts know what they
to enhance reflexivity’, Journal of Psycho-Social Studies, 10, know?’. In L. Braddock and M. Lacewing (eds), The Academic
pp. 25–46. Face of Psychoanalysis, pp. 172–195. London: Routledge.
O’Callaghan, C. (2012). ‘Grounded theory in music therapy re- Saldaña, J. (2016). The Coding Manual for Qualitative Re-
search’, Journal of Music Therapy, 49, pp. 236–277. searchers, 3rd edn. London: Sage.
O’Reilly, K., D. Paper and S. Marx (2012). ‘Demystifying Sare, L. and S. E. Bales (2014). ‘Professional identity: a grounded
grounded theory for business research’, Organizational Re- theory study of veteran academic librarians’, Library Review,
search Methods, 15, pp. 247–262. 63, pp. 574–589.
Ogden, T. H. (1994). ‘The analytic third: working with intersub- Saunders, M. N. K., P. Lewis and A. Thornhill (2015). Research
jective clinical facts’, International Journal of Psychoanalysis, Methods for Business Students, 7th edn. Essex: Pearson.
75, pp. 3–19. Schad, J., M. W. Lewis, S. Raisch and W. K. Smith (2016). ‘Para-
Ogden, T. (2004). ‘On holding and containing, being and dream- dox research in management science: looking back to move
ing’, International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 85, pp. 1349– forward’, Academy of Management Annals, 10, pp. 5–64.
1364. Strauss, A. L. and J. Corbin (1990). Basics of Qualitative Re-
Orange, A. (2016). ‘Encouraging reflexive practices in doctoral search: Grounded Theory Procedures and Techniques. London:
students through research journals’, The Qualitative Report, Sage.
21, pp. 2176–2190. Strauss, A. L. and J. Corbin (1998). Basics of Qualitative Re-
Partington, D. (2000). ‘Building grounded theories of manage- search: Techniques and Procedures for Developing Grounded
ment action’, British Journal of Management, 11, pp. 91– Theory. London: Sage.
102. Sutton, R. I. and B. M. Staw (1995). ‘What theory is not’, Ad-
Pezalla, A. E., J. Pettigrew and M. Miller-Day (2012). ‘Re- ministrative Science Quarterly, 40, pp. 371–384.
searching the researcher-as-instrument: an exercise in in- Ulus, E. (2015). ‘Workplace emotions in postcolonial spaces: en-
terviewer self-reflexivity’, Qualitative Research, 12, pp. 165– during legacies, ambivalence, and subversion’, Organization,
185. 22, pp. 890–908.
Phillips, A. (2014). Becoming Freud: The Making of a Psychoan- Vince, R. and M. Broussine (1996). ‘Paradox, defense, attach-
alyst. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. ment: accessing and working with emotions and relations
Prasad, A. (2014). ‘You can’t go home again: and other psycho- underlying organizational change’, Organization Studies, 17,
analytic lessons from crossing a neo-colonial border’, Human pp. 1–21.
Relations, 67, pp. 233–257. Walsh, I., J. A. Holton, L. Bailyn, W. Fernandez, N. Levina and
Pritchard, K. (2012). ‘Combining qualitative methods’. In G. B. Glaser (2015). ‘What grounded theory is . . . a critically re-
Symon and C. Cassell (eds), Qualitative Organizational Re- flective conversation among scholars’, Organizational Research
search: Core Methods and Current Challenges, pp. 132–148. Methods, 18, pp. 581–599.
London: Sage. Weick, K. E. (1989). ‘Theory construction as disciplined imagi-
Redman, P. (2016). ‘Once more with feeling: what is the nation’, Academy of Management Review, 14, pp. 516–531.
psychosocial anyway?’, Journal of Psycho-Social Studies, 9, Wolgemuth, J. R. (2015). ‘Driving the paradigm’, Qualitative In-
pp. 73–93. quiry, 22, pp. 518–525.

Annette Clancy is a Lecturer in Organizational Behaviour at University College Dublin, Ireland. She
earned her PhD at the University of Bath, where her doctoral research investigated organizational
disappointment and explored how it might be reimagined as a catalyst for organizational learning
and creativity. The focus of Annette’s research is on emotion in organizations and, in particular, how
unconscious dynamics influence and inform how work is performed.

Russ Vince is Professor of Leadership and Change and Director of the Centre for Strategic Change and
Leadership at the School of Management, University of Bath. He is Honorary Professor of Manage-
ment at the School of Management, University of St Andrews. His research has focused on learning
and reflection in organizations, emotions, management education, leadership development and the
psychodynamic study of organizations.


C 2018 British Academy of Management.

You might also like