Wisdom 2

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Journals of Gerontology: Social Sciences

cite as: J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci, 2018, Vol. 73, No. 8, 1514–1525
doi:10.1093/geronb/gby017
Advance Access publication February 14, 2018

Special Article

Early and Midlife Predictors of Wisdom and Subjective

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/73/8/1514/4857263 by guest on 04 September 2020


Well-Being in Old Age
Monika Ardelt, PhD1, Kathryn R. Gerlach, MA1, and George E. Vaillant, MD2
Department of Sociology and Criminology & Law, University of Florida, Gainesville. 2Harvard Medical School, Massachusetts
1

General Hospital, Boston.

Address correspondence to Monika Ardelt, PhD, Department of Sociology and Criminology & Law, University of Florida, PO Box 117330,
Gainesville, FL 32611–7330. E-mail: ardelt@ufl.edu

Received: June 20, 2017; Editorial Decision Date: January 25, 2018

Decision Editor: Deborah Carr, PhD

Abstract
Objectives:  We explored whether wisdom and well-being in old age are the result of early personality traits related to per-
sonality growth or personality adjustment, respectively, or successful human development as outlined by Erikson’s stage
theory and the life course paradigm.
Method:  Structural equation models were applied to analyze 60-year longitudinal data of 98 white male Harvard gradu-
ates born between 1915 and 1924. Different sets of judges rated the men’s childhood and adolescence, early adult person-
ality, and midlife generativity. Items measuring wisdom and well-being in old age were self-assessed.
Results:  Although wisdom and subjective well-being at age 80 were positively correlated, early life predictors differed.
Openness to experiences in early adulthood predicted wisdom 60  years later, whereas greater emotional stability and
extraversion predicted subjective well-being. Additionally, old age wisdom could be traced back to psychosocial growth
throughout life, facilitated by a supportive childhood, adolescent competence, emotional stability in early adulthood, and
generativity at midlife.
Discussion:  Personality traits indicative of personality adjustment or growth differentially predict late-life well-being and
wisdom. Yet a balance between personality adjustment and growth, aided by social support and competence during the
formative years, might be required to promote wisdom development throughout life.
Keywords:  Longitudinal data, Personality adjustment, Personality growth, Psychosocial development, Social support, Structural equation
model, Successful aging

Elder’s (1994) life course paradigm states that human be predicted by enduring personality traits that already
development is a lifelong process that is shaped by social emerge in early adulthood. Although several studies have
context, the interconnectedness of lives, and human agency. found a positive correlation between wisdom and well-
Similarly, Erikson’s (1963) stage model of psychosocial being in old age (Ardelt, 2003; Ardelt & Edwards, 2016;
development divides the life course into a series of eight Ardelt & Jeste, in press; Le, 2011), early personality predic-
developmental tasks or crises, whose successful resolutions tors of wisdom and well-being might differ.
cumulates in wisdom. From these perspectives, wisdom and The purpose of the current study was to test whether
subjective well-being in old age are the result of cumulative wisdom and well-being at age 80 could be predicted by
successful human development as it unfolds across the life (a) young adult personality traits or (b) a model of suc-
course. Alternatively, old age wisdom and well-being could cessful human development, using retrospective data from

© The Author(s) 2018. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of The Gerontological Society of America. All rights reserved. 1514
For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com.
Journals of Gerontology: SOCIAL SCIENCES, 2018, Vol. 73, No. 8 1515

childhood and 60-year prospective longitudinal data from personality adjustment and personality growth. Personality
young adulthood to old age of Harvard-educated white adjustment is a form of socialization toward society’s roles,
men. If wisdom and well-being in old age are the result of norms, and expectations that helps individuals to func-
early personality traits, psychotherapy and psychological tion effectively and successfully in society and, therefore,
interventions that target personality characteristics might enhances subjective well-being. The personality traits of
also increase wisdom and well-being (Roberts et al., 2017). emotional stability (the reverse of neuroticism), conscien-
If, however, wisdom and well-being are due to successful tiousness, and agreeableness in the “Big Five” factor theory
human development, early childhood interventions might of personality (Costa & McCrae, 1992) are expected to
plant the “seeds of wisdom” that bear fruits later in life. aid in the process of personality adjustment with possible
short-term and long-term beneficial effects on well-being.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/73/8/1514/4857263 by guest on 04 September 2020


Personality growth, by contrast, goes beyond personal-
Definition and Operationalization of ity adjustment and requires a deep understanding of life,
Subjective Well-Being and Wisdom self-insight, self-transcendence expressed as concern for the
Subjective well-being is defined and operationalized as gen- well-being of others, tolerance of ambiguity, and striving
eral life satisfaction and a positive overall evaluation of life, toward wisdom (Staudinger & Kunzmann, 2005). Rather
emphasizing a relatively stable cognitive orientation toward than simply adjusting to societal norms, individuals who
life rather than less stable emotional states (George, 2010). grow in personality might challenge norms and social
Wisdom has been defined by contemporary researchers structures that perpetuate social injustice, and this chal-
in a variety of ways (Staudinger & Glück, 2011). These lenging would not necessarily contribute to greater well-
include cognitive-focused conceptions, such as wisdom- being. Among the “Big Five” personality traits, openness to
related knowledge in the fundamental pragmatics of experiences is predicted to foster personality growth.
life (Baltes & Staudinger, 2000); integrative conceptions Several studies of the Big Five personality traits (e.g.,
that combine cognitive and affective components, such DeYoung, 2006; Digman, 1997) have found two higher-
as “deep accurate insight and understanding of oneself order factors, distinguishing between stability/socialization
and the central existential issues of life, plus skillful ben- (emotional stability, conscientiousness, and agreeableness)
evolent responsiveness” (Walsh, 2015, p.  282); and non- and plasticity/personal growth (openness to experiences
cognitive conceptions, such as the combination of critical and extraversion), which correspond to the distinction
life experiences, emotional regulation, reflectiveness/rem- between personality adjustment and growth (with the
iniscence, openness to experience, and humor (Webster, exception of extraversion). Personality traits related to
2003) and self-transcendence (Levenson, Jennings, Aldwin, adjustment are associated with conformity and less open-
& Shiraishi, 2005). We conceptualize personal wisdom as ness to change but, contrary to the model of personality
an integration of cognitive, reflective, and compassionate adjustment and growth, also seem to foster self-transcen-
(affective) personality characteristics (Ardelt, 2003). dence and benevolence rather than self-enhancement val-
The cognitive element refers to an individual’s ability ues. Conversely, individuals characterized by openness and
to understand life, grasping the deeper significance of phe- extraversion appear to value self-direction and stimulation
nomena and events. In particular, this entails being aware of rather than conformity, tradition, and security but also
positive and negative aspects of human nature, recognizing hedonism, achievement, and power (Vecchione, Alessandri,
the limits of knowledge, and being able to make important Barbaranelli, & Caprara, 2011). Moreover, plasticity
decisions despite life’s unpredictability and uncertainty. To traits have been shown to lead to maladaptive behavior
reach this insight requires the reflective wisdom dimension, among male adolescents if not balanced by stability traits
defined as reflective thinking and self-reflection to perceive (DeYoung, Peterson, Seguin, & Tremblay, 2008).
phenomena and events from multiple perspectives. Mindful Hence, the development of wisdom might require a bal-
reflection reduces self-centeredness, lessens the tendency to ance between personality adjustment and growth. Although
direct blame outwards when circumstances turn unpleas- the goal of personality growth is promotion of the greater
ant, and increases insight into one’s own and other people’s good by transcending one’s self-centeredness, emotional
motives and behavior, which results in the realization of a stability helps wise persons approach this goal by either
common humanity and fosters the compassionate wisdom shaping and transforming their environment or by adjust-
dimension. The compassionate dimension refers to sym- ing to circumstances that cannot be changed (Sternberg,
pathy and compassion for all beings and the motivation to 1998), which preserves contentment and well-being even
act on these emotions to enhance others’ well-being. in difficult times (Ardelt, 2005; Ardelt & Edwards, 2016;
Ardelt & Jeste, in press).
Consistent with the theory of personality growth, open-
Personality Adjustment, Personality Growth, ness to new experiences has been shown in several cross-
Wisdom, and Well-Being sectional studies to correlate with all types of wisdom
Staudinger and colleagues (Staudinger & Kunzmann, 2005; measures (Le, 2011; Levenson et al., 2005; Neff, Rude, &
Wink & Staudinger, 2016) advanced the notion that posi- Kirkpatrick, 2007; Zacher, McKenna, Rooney, & Gold,
tive human development consists of two distinct pathways: 2015) and is even included in the operationalization of
1516 Journals of Gerontology: SOCIAL SCIENCES, 2018, Vol. 73, No. 8

wisdom in the Self-Assessed Wisdom Scale (Webster, 2003). vs isolation). Therefore, a supportive social environment
Emotional stability and extraversion have also been found and family relationships are likely to play a pivotal role
to be positively associated with integrative and noncog- in facilitating or impeding the resolution of the first six
nitive measures of wisdom, such as three-dimensional developmental crises (Vaillant, 1977) and also tend to con-
wisdom, wisdom assessed as self-transcendence, and the tribute to the child’s academic and social success in school
Self-Assessed Wisdom Scale, and agreeableness and con- (Morrison, Rimm-Kauffman, & Pianta, 2003). This is con-
scientiousness were positively correlated with three-dimen- sistent with the principle of linked lives in the life course
sional wisdom and self-transcendence (Levenson et  al., paradigm (Elder, 1994), which stresses that individuals are
2005; Neff et al., 2007; Webster, Westerhof, & Bohlmeijer, social beings who live their lives interdependently.
2014; Zacher et  al., 2015). Wisdom-related knowledge From young to middle adulthood, many adults grow

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/73/8/1514/4857263 by guest on 04 September 2020


was also positively associated with openness but negatively more mature in their personalities, as indicated by aver-
with extraversion in one study (Staudinger, Maciel, Smith, age increases in emotional stability, agreeableness, and
& Baltes, 1998) and uncorrelated with the Big Five per- conscientiousness, which might be the result of adult
sonality traits in another study (Zacher et al., 2015). Only responsibilities in family and work roles (Roberts, Walton,
few studies have examined the long-term effects of early & Viechtbauer, 2006). A more mature personality, in turn,
personality characteristics on later life wisdom. Among facilitates the successful resolution of the seventh develop-
college-educated women, openness to experience at age 21 mental task (Cox, Wilt, Olson, & McAdams, 2010; de St
was positively related to transcendent wisdom at age 52 Aubin & McAdams, 1995), which Erikson (1963) termed
(Wink & Helson, 1997) and to a composite wisdom meas- generativity versus self-absorption or stagnation. During
ure (practical and transcendent wisdom and scores on a midlife, many (but not all) adults develop an interest in
wisdom task) at age 61 (Helson & Srivastava, 2002). guiding the next generation by focusing more on the well-
In accordance with the model of personality adjust- being of others than their own success. Through a reduction
ment, an extensive meta-analysis has shown that emo- in self-centeredness, generative persons contribute to the
tional stability and extraversion are consistent predictors success and well-being of future generations, not just their
own children or grandchildren. This is also in alignment
of subjective well-being, followed by conscientiousness
with the reflective and compassionate dimensions of wis-
and agreeableness, but less consistently openness to experi-
dom and the life course principle of human agency, which
ences, as weaker predictors of well-being (Steel, Schmidt,
emphasizes that people are active agents in shaping their
& Shultz, 2008). The positive association between extra-
life course through the choices they make within the limits
version and well-being might be explained by the greater
of social, personal, and historical constraints. Moreover,
social involvement of extraverted individuals and its posi-
concern for the well-being of others might bring forth a
tive impact on well-being. Emotional stability, extraversion,
sense of gratification and well-being. Generativity has been
conscientiousness, and agreeableness also predicted greater
found to be positively associated with cognitive and non-
subjective well-being over a 4-year period after controlling
cognitive measures of wisdom (Webster, 2003; Wink &
for baseline scores (Soto, 2015). Hence, the constellation
Dillon, 2003; Wink & Helson, 1997; Wink & Staudinger,
of personality traits in young adulthood might have long-
2016) and also subjective well-being (Cox et al., 2010; de
ranging effects on wisdom and well-being in old age.
St Aubin & McAdams, 1995).
The achievement of generativity facilitates the eighth psy-
chosocial task, ego integrity versus despair (James & Zarrett,
Erikson’s Stage Theory of Human
2006), during which older adults struggle to accept and inte-
Development and Late-Life Wisdom and
grate their lives instead of dwelling on past failures, mis-
Well-Being
takes, and missed opportunities. The successful resolution
Erikson’s (1963) stage theory of psychosocial develop- of this task results in wisdom and presumably also greater
ment balances personality adjustment and personality well-being if despair is overcome. Ego-integrity has been
growth. The model divides the life course into a series of shown to be positively correlated with noncognitive wisdom
eight fundamental crises that need to be mastered for suc- (Webster, 2003, 2010) and subjective well-being (Dezutter,
cessful human development to take place. The successful Wiesmann, Apers, & Luyckx, 2013; James & Zarrett, 2006).
resolution of a crisis is rewarded with a specific strength or According to Erikson’s theory, the dissolution of des-
virtue that facilitates the mastery of the next and all sub- pair and the emergence of wisdom depend on the suc-
sequent developmental crises. In Erikson’s theory, early life cessful mastery of all psychosocial tasks. Hence, wisdom
experiences are vital for successful human development. is assumed to develop gradually over the life course with
Erikson (1963) locates four of the eight developmental the seeds of wisdom firmly planted in childhood (Ardelt &
tasks in childhood (basic trust vs basic mistrust, autonomy Jacobs, 2009). This dovetails with the life course paradigm,
vs shame and doubt, initiative vs guilt, and industry vs which states that any given stage of life, such as old age, can
inferiority) and the two subsequent crises in adolescence only be understood when considering the trajectory of all
(identity vs role confusion) and young adulthood (intimacy previous life stages (Elder, 1994).
Journals of Gerontology: SOCIAL SCIENCES, 2018, Vol. 73, No. 8 1517

The Present Study worked during college to support themselves, and 65% of


the mothers and 26% of the fathers had not attended col-
Taking the distinction between personality adjustment and
lege themselves. Of the 98 men who completed the ques-
personality growth into account (Staudinger & Kunzmann,
tionnaire at age 80, 70% obtained a graduate degree. By
2005; Wink & Staudinger, 2016), the paths to wisdom and
age 80, 103 men (38.4%) of the original sample had died
well-being might depend on people’s personality traits in
and nine (3.4%) had dropped out of the study.
young adulthood, particular if personality is relatively stable
throughout life. An openness to new experiences that chal-
lenges existing norms and societal institutions is necessary
Procedure
for personality growth and might lead to wisdom but not
greater well-being in old age (Hypothesis 1). By contrast, During the men’s sophomore year at Harvard, every study

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/73/8/1514/4857263 by guest on 04 September 2020


emotional stability and extraversion are the most consist- member was thoroughly examined by an interdisciplinary
ent predictors of subjective well-being. Emotionally stable team of internists, psychiatrists, psychologists, and anthro-
individuals who conform to the norms and expectations of pologists. At that time, the students’ parents were also
society and extraverted people who are outgoing, sociable, interviewed to collect the family’s social and medical his-
and friendly are likely rewarded by success in friendship, tory. Follow-up interviews with the men took place when
family life, and employment (Steel et al., 2008), which might they were about 25, 30, 50, and 65 years old. Standardized
yield contentment and well-being in old age (Hypothesis 2). questionnaires with open and closed-ended questions were
Yet, based on Erikson’s (1963) stage theory of psycho- mailed to the men every 2 years, starting at the age of 45. In
social development and the principles of life-span devel- 2001, 98 men completed the Gallup Wellsprings of a Positive
opment, linked lives, and human agency in the life course Life Survey (Isaacowitz, Vaillant, & Seligman, 2003).
paradigm (Elder, 1994), wisdom and well-being in old
age might also be the result of successful psychosocial
development across the whole life course (Hypothesis Measures
3). Accordingly, the following path model was tested: Measures consisted of ratings of the men’s or their parents’
A benign childhood environment conducive to the mastery in-depth qualitative interviews and questionnaire items
of Erikson’s first four psychosocial tasks was predicted to (for details see Vaillant, 1977). Due to limited informa-
lead to social and academic success in school (Morrison tion available in this secondary analysis of archival data,
et  al., 2003) and more mature personality characteristics, interrater reliability or reliability coefficients of the vari-
particularly greater emotional stability. As documented by ous scales could not always be computed but were reported
Rogosch and Cicchetti (2004), abused and/or neglected whenever possible.
children were less likely to develop emotionally stable,
agreeable, and conscientious personalities than children
who grew up in happy and warm families. Mature person- Childhood/Adolescence Measures
ality characteristics were expected to be correlated with
The 10–20 h of retrospective qualitative interviews with the
academic, social, and athletic competence in school (Hair
participants’ parents at the beginning of the study were the
& Graziano, 2003) and to help young adults to continue
source for quality of childhood. Two independent judges
on the trajectory of successful psychosocial development to
who were blind to the men’s data after age 19 provided
reach the stage of generativity in midlife. The achievement
a global impression of the men’s childhood (0  =  overall
of midlife generativity was assumed to increase the likeli-
hunch is negative; non-nurturing environment; 1 = evoked
hood of attaining ego integrity and, therefore, wisdom and
in rater neither positive nor negative feeling; 2 = positive,
subjective well-being in old age.
intact childhood; good relationship with parents, siblings,
others of importance; environment seems to be condu-
cive to developing basic trust, autonomy, and initiative).
Method Two independent judges also rated the men’s school and
Sample sports success in adolescence (0  =  no sports; school fail-
The sample consisted of 98 white men, born between ures; marked social problems; 1 = average; no competitive
1915 and 1924 (with a mean and median birth year of sports; 2  =  does well at school; competitive sports). For
1921), from the longitudinal Study of Adult Development each of the two scales, one was added to the sum of the
for which old age data were available. The original sam- two ratings to obtain 1–5 scales.
ple was taken from the graduating classes of 1940–1944
and contained 268 Harvard college sophomores with a
satisfactory freshman academic record and no history of Young Adulthood Measures
physical and mental illness (Vaillant, 1977). Although the Personality characteristics at around age 21 were based on the
majority of these students had privileged socioeconomic participants’ college personality traits (e.g., sociable, well inte-
family backgrounds, about half had a scholarship and/or grated, shy, pragmatic, humanistic, etc.) derived from ratings by
1518 Journals of Gerontology: SOCIAL SCIENCES, 2018, Vol. 73, No. 8

the clinical staff at the beginning of the study. These traits were The definition and operationalization of wisdom as a
weighted and factor analyzed to create the equivalent of Costa three-dimensional personality characteristic guided the
and McCrae’s (1992) Five-Factor Personality Inventory (Soldz construction of the wisdom measure. Considering that
& Vaillant, 1999). The scale for emotional stability (reverse of the measurement of wisdom was not the intention of the
neuroticism) ranged from 0 (low) to 34 (high), for extraver- original investigators, self-assessed items from the Gallup
sion from 4 (low) to 43 (high), for openness to experience from Wellsprings of a Positive Life Survey were selected to assess
12 (low) to 41 (high), for agreeableness from 14 (low) to 47 the cognitive, reflective, and compassionate dimensions
(high), and for conscientiousness from 13 (low) to 46 (high). of wisdom. This approach has been used successfully in
The correlations of these scales with the respective scales of prior longitudinal wisdom research (Ardelt, 2000; Wink &
the Five-Factor Personality Inventory, obtained when the men Dillon, 2003; Wink & Helson, 1997).

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/73/8/1514/4857263 by guest on 04 September 2020


were about 67  years of age, were high (remotional stability  =  .80, The cognitive wisdom dimension was the average of four
p < .001; rextraversion  =  .62, p < .001; ropenness  =  .67, p < .001; items that assessed the ability and willingness to understand
rconscientiousness = .95, p < .001), documenting rank-order stabil- life thoroughly (e.g., My mind is always asking questions)
ity across time, with the exception of agreeableness (r = .26, and one item that appraised the ability to make important
p = .012). decisions despite life’s unpredictability and uncertainties
Three variables were added to control for possible (e.g., I have confidence in the choices I make). The reflective
effects of general intelligence (IQ) and socioeconomic sta- wisdom dimension consisted of the average of four items,
tus. IQ was the mean of average SAT and MAT scores and which gauged the ability and willingness to look at phe-
the Army Alpha Verbal Intelligence Test, transformed into nomena and events from different perspectives (e.g., Others
a 15-point scale (1 = equivalent to E− and 15 = A+). The come to me for advice.). The affective wisdom dimension
correlation between the two IQ measures was r  =  .43 (p comprised the average of 10 items of which three measured
< .001). Parents’ social class when the men were 18 years benevolent emotions toward others (e.g., I always feel the
old was assessed by the Hollingshead-Redlich Social presence of love in my life) and the remaining seven items
Class Index (Hollingshead & Redlich, 1958), divided into benevolent motivations and behavior toward others (e.g.,
four categories (1 = lower middle class and 4 = upper class). I frequently go out of my way to help people in need). All
Years of education ranged from 14 years (did not graduate) of the items were assessed on 5-point scales (1 = very false
to 20 years (MD or PhD). and 5  =  very true). Cronbach’s α was .70, .67, and .80,
respectively, for the cognitive, reflective, and compassion-
Midlife Measure ate wisdom dimensions. The cognitive wisdom dimension
correlated .41 with the reflective dimension and .56 with
Two independent judges rated the qualitative interviews up the compassionate dimension, and the correlation between
to age 47 for 39 men in the current study according to the the reflective and compassionate dimensions was .54 (all p
first seven of Erikson’s (1963) eight developmental stages, < .001). Old age wisdom was computed as the average of
with an added stage of career consolidation between intim- the three wisdom dimensions, resulting in an α-value of .74
acy and generativity (Vaillant & Milofsky, 1980). Erikson (.87 for the 19 items).
stage rating at around age 47 was the average of the two Three separate confirmatory factor analyses of the indi-
ratings, scored on a scale from 4 (industry achieved/failed vidual wisdom dimensions, using LISREL 9.3, confirmed
identity) to 8 (generativity achieved). The intraclass correl- two-factor solutions for the cognitive and compassionate
ation between the two judges was .50 (p=.017). One judge dimensions and a one-factor solution for the reflective wis-
rated the interviews of the remaining 59 men. dom dimension. The two factors correlated .27 (p  =  .03)
for the cognitive dimension and .52 (p < .001) for the com-
passionate dimension. Standardized factor loadings ranged
Old Age Measures from .48 to .83 (p < .01). Detailed results of the confirma-
Subjective well-being (SWB) was measured by seven items tory factor analyses are available in the Supplementary
from the Gallup Wellsprings of a Positive Life Survey Appendix.
(Isaacowitz et al., 2003) when the men were about 80 years
of age. The men were asked to indicate their overall satis-
faction with life on a “well-being ladder” (0 = worst pos-
Results
sible life and 10 = best possible life) and how satisfied they Independent t-tests showed that the 98 men that comprised
were with the way their life was going (1 = very dissatisfied the study sample were not significantly different from the
and 5  =  very satisfied). Five items assessed general satis- remaining men of the original sample on all study vari-
faction and well-being (e.g., I am completely satisfied with ables, except that they tended to have more years of edu-
my life) on 5-point scales (1 = very false and 5 = very true). cation (M  =  18.31, SD  =  1.65 vs M  =  17.78, SD  =  1.79;
The 0–10 scale was transformed into a 1–5 scale before t = 2.44, p = .016) and higher Erikson stage ratings at mid-
the average of the seven items was computed, resulting in a life (M = 7.57, SD = .60 vs M = 7.23, SD = .86; t = 3.62, p
Cronbach’s α value of .90. < .001).
Journals of Gerontology: SOCIAL SCIENCES, 2018, Vol. 73, No. 8 1519

Bivariate Correlations and influential cases. One outlier/influential case with a


Table  1 displays the bivariate correlations between the standardized residual greater than 3.5 was responsible for
variables. Although wisdom and SWB in old age were the negative association between social class of parents and
positively correlated, the pathways differed. As predicted extraversion. Because social class was not related to any
in Hypothesis 1 and following the personality growth other variables, it was removed from the model.
pathway, openness to experiences in young adulthood The resulting path model in Figure 1 had 20 coefficient
was positively correlated with wisdom in old age, but not estimates, which is acceptable for 98 cases. The model fit the
SWB. Conversely, early adulthood extraversion was asso- data well with a nonsignificant maximum likelihood ratio
ciated with SWB in late life, but not wisdom, supporting chi-square of 22.19 (16 df, p  =  .14), a standardized root
Hypothesis 2. Emotional stability in young adulthood was mean square residual (SRMR) of .078, a root mean square

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/73/8/1514/4857263 by guest on 04 September 2020


correlated with late-life wisdom and SWB and, therefore, error of approximation (RMSEA) of .063, and a compara-
might be an indicator of the balance between personal- tive fit index (CFI) of 1.00. A nonsignificant chi-square and
ity adjustment and growth. The developmental pathway values lower than .08 for SRMR and RMSEA and higher
outlined in Hypothesis 3 was also supported. The raters’ than .95 for CFI indicate a good model fit (Kline, 2005).
impression of a positive childhood was positively related The variables in the path model explained 19%, 6%, 15%,
to adolescent school and sports success and wisdom in old 14%, and 25%, respectively, of the variance in adolescent
age. Adolescent school and sports success was positively school and sports success, emotional stability, Erikson stage
correlated with emotional stability and conscientiousness, rating at midlife, and old age SWB and wisdom. We also
Erikson stage at midlife, and wisdom in old age. Emotional tested the same path model with a listwise deletion of cases
stability in early adulthood was positively correlated with (n = 90) and obtained very similar results with differences
Erikson stage at midlife, which was related to greater wis- in the standardized coefficient estimates no larger than .03.
dom and SWB in old age. Among the control variables, As hypothesized, openness to experience in young adult-
social class of the men’s parents was negatively associ- hood predicted wisdom in old age (Hypothesis 1), and
ated with extraversion. Next, we analyzed a path model extraversion and emotional stability predicted later life
to examine how the variables were inter-related across the SWB (Hypothesis 2). A  developmental pathway also led
life course. to old age wisdom, confirming Hypothesis 3.  The raters’
positive impression of the men’s childhood was directly
related to school and sports success in adolescence, which
Path Analysis was associated with greater emotional stability in young
The Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) multiple imput- adulthood. Greater emotional stability, in turn, positively
ation method provided in PRELIS 9.3 was used to impute predicted psychosocial development toward generativity at
one to six missing values for the young adulthood vari- midlife, which was related to greater wisdom in old age. In
ables. Because PRELIS results indicated that the variables addition, adolescent school and sports success was directly
did not follow a multivariate normal distribution, covari- related to old age wisdom.
ance and asymptotic covariance matrices were computed Model 1 in Table 2 shows the standardized indirect and
to estimate corrected t-values and chi-square statistics. total effects of the earlier life variables on old age wisdom
Due to the small sample size, robust maximum likelihood and well-being based on the model in Figure 1. The indirect
rather than weighted least squares estimation was applied effect of the raters’ positive childhood impression on old age
to obtain the coefficient estimates (Jöreskog, Sörbom, du wisdom was significant (standardized effect = .14, t = 2.89,
Toit, & du Toit, 1999). p = .003) as was the indirect effect of emotional stability
In structural equation modeling, it is desirable to have at (standardized effect = .09, t = 2.04, p = .041), supporting
least five cases for each coefficient estimate (Kline, 2005). the hypothesis of a successful psychosocial developmental
Therefore, to build a path model that was relatively par- pathway from childhood to adolescence, young adulthood,
simonious but did not ignore any significant paths, we midlife, and finally old age. By contrast, the indirect effect
followed a stepwise procedure that only included earlier of a positive childhood on old age SWB was not statistic-
life variables if they were related to later life variables. ally significant (standardized effect = .03, t = 1.60, p = .11).
First, only SWB and wisdom in old age were introduced The model in Figure  1 did not include a direct path
as dependent variables with all earlier life variables as pre- between late-life wisdom and SWB but showed that the
dictors. Nonsignificant predictors were removed from the residual correlation between the variables was statistically
model in a stepwise procedure until only significant predic- significant. Models 2 and 3 in Table 2 examined the long-
tors remained. Second, the same procedure was repeated term effects of earlier life variables on old age wisdom and
with Erikson stage rating added as dependent variable and SWB, assuming a direct path between the variables (stand-
then again with the young adulthood and adolescent vari- ardized direct effect  =  .21, p < .05). If wisdom increased
ables that were retained as significant predictors of mid- SWB (Model 2 in Table 2), consistent with Erikson’s model
life and old age variables. Supplementary multivariate OLS of psychosocial development, SWB in old age would also
regression analyses were conducted to check for outliers be indirectly affected by a positive childhood (standardized
1520

Table 1.  Bivariate Correlations Between Variables

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10) (11) (12) M SD N

Childhood
(1) Positive childhood impression — 2.70 1.36 98
Adolescence
(2) School and sports success .43** — 3.76 1.14 98
Young adulthood
(3) Social class of parents, age 18 .18 .03 — 3.07 .79 98
(4) IQ .03 −.17 .09 — 9.57 2.30 96
(5) Emotional stability, age 21 .03 .25* −.04 −.08 — 19.64 5.94 92
(6) Extraversion, age 21 .02 .09 −.21* .07 .21* — 26.84 6.04 94
(7) Openness to experiences, age 21 −.06 −.07 .01 .13 −.09 .13 — 29.47 5.77 93
(8) Agreeableness, age 21 −.03 −.06 −.04 .02 .11 .07 −.16 — 32.95 5.89 94
(9) Conscientiousness, age 21 .21 .23* −.06 .05 .17 −.00 −.11 .03 — 34.90 6.35 92
(10) Number of years of education −.09 −.09 .14 .26* .05 −.10 .01 −.07 .03 — 18.31 1.65 97
Midlife
(11) Erikson stage rating, age 47 .15 .25* .13 −.07 .39** .12 .04 −.08 .11 .06 — 7.57 .60 98
Old Age
(12) Subjective well-being, age 80 .11 .06 −.01 .06 .36** .28** .06 −.09 .14 .04 .24* — 3.98 .70 98
(13) Wisdom, age 80 .30** .32** −.02 −.00 .24* .12 .28** .00 .19 −.06 .34** .30** 3.77 .40 98
(14) Cognitive wisdom dimension .27** .23* −.08 −.01 .18 .09 .26* −.00 .27* −.03 .23* .34** 3.99 .50 98
(15) Reflective wisdom dimension .17 .29** −.01 .05 .15 .10 .21* .01 .08 −.01 .29** .14 3.64 .54 97
(16) Compassionate wisdom .31** .28** .05 −.06 .28** .11 .20 −.01 .13 −.11 .33** .28** 3.68 .45 97
dimension

**p < .01. *p < .05.


Journals of Gerontology: SOCIAL SCIENCES, 2018, Vol. 73, No. 8

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/73/8/1514/4857263 by guest on 04 September 2020


Journals of Gerontology: SOCIAL SCIENCES, 2018, Vol. 73, No. 8 1521

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/73/8/1514/4857263 by guest on 04 September 2020


Figure 1.  Results of the path analysis model. Notes. n = 98; Robust maximum likelihood estimation using LISREL 9.30; standardized coefficient esti-
mates; t-values corrected for non-normality. Correlations between the three exogenous variables were included in the model (not shown) but were
not significant. ***t-value > 3.29 (p < .001), **t-value > 2.58 (p < .01), *t-value > 1.96 (p < .05).

effect = .06, t = 2.01, p = .04), adolescent school and sport Successful psychosocial development across the life
success (standardized effect = .13, t = 2.39, p = .02), and course also benefited later life well-being under the assump-
young adult openness (standardized effect = .07, t = 2.01, tion that wisdom led to well-being, for example, by mas-
p = .04). By contrast, if SWB led to greater wisdom (Model tering the physical, social, and psychological challenges of
3 in Table  2), the results would be similar to those in old age and resolving Erikson’s eighth psychosocial task to
Model 1, except for a slightly increased indirect effect of achieve ego integrity and overcome despair. If, by contrast,
young adult emotional stability on wisdom (standardized well-being is considered a precursor of wisdom (Wink &
effect = .15, t = 2.46, p = .01). Staudinger, 2016), the only early life predictors of late-life
well-being were relatively stable personality traits of emo-
tional stability and extraversion. Because wisdom and SWB
Discussion were assessed at the same time, this study cannot determine
Although wisdom and SWB at age 80 were positively cor- the direction of their relationship, but it can compare the
related for the 98 Harvard men in this study, three sep- consequences. However, a short-term longitudinal study of
arate pathways predicted wisdom and well-being as older adults found that wisdom predicted well-being but
hypothesized. Across 60  years, openness to experiences not vice versa (Ardelt, 2016), consistent with successful
predicted wisdom along the personality growth pathway human development theories of old age well-being.
(Hypothesis 1)  and emotional stability and extraversion A positive childhood affected the men’s old age wisdom
predicted well-being along the personality adjustment through a sequential developmental trajectory that spanned
pathway (Hypothesis 2), corroborating prior longitudinal the whole life course and through the direct mediation of
and cross-sectional study findings (Staudinger & Glück, adolescent school and sports success. Because school and
2011; Steel et al., 2008). Along the successful human devel- sports success included the absence of social problems and
opment pathway that balances personality adjustment and was unrelated to the social class of the men’s parents and
growth, raters’ impression of the men’s childhood as posi- the men’s IQ and years of education, it encompasses more
tive and conducive to mastering Erikson’s (1963) first three than academic and physical success. Contrary to adolescent
developmental tasks of basic trust, autonomy, and initiative school and sports success, the men’s IQ scores and years
was positively associated with adolescent school and sports of education had no impact on later life wisdom, corrobo-
success, which was related to greater emotional stability in rating a similar outcome in a longitudinal study of white
young adulthood. Young men who were more emotional college-educated women (Wink & Helson, 1997) and con-
stable were more likely to reach the stage of generativity firming that cognitive development by itself is no guarantee
at midlife, which was positively related to old age wisdom. for later life wisdom (Staudinger et al., 1998).
This is the first study that traces old age wisdom back to Adolescent school and sports success might indirectly
childhood environment. The developmental pathway cor- assess Clausen’s (1991) concept of adolescent “planful
roborates Hypothesis 3 and lends support to Erikson’s competence,” a measure of maturity and human agency
(1963) stage theory of psychosocial development and that is defined as a combination of self-confidence, depend-
Elder’s (1994) life course principles of linked lives, human ability, intellectual engagement, and the ability to interact
agency, and life-span development. positively with others. According to Clausen, competent
1522

Table 2.  Indirect and Total Standardized Effects of Earlier Life Variables on Wisdom and Subjective Well-Being in Old Age—A Comparison of Three Structural Equation Models

Model 1: wisdom ↔ well-being Model 2: wisdom → well-being Model 3: well-being → wisdom

Dependent variables Wisdom SWB Wisdom SWB Wisdom SWB

Indirect Total Indirect Total Indirect Total Indirect Total Indirect Total Indirect Total
Independent variables effects effects effects effects effects effects effects effects effects effects effects effects

Positive childhood rating .14** .14** .03 .03 .13** .13** .06* .06* .14** .14** .03 .03
Adolescent school and sports .02 .32*** .07 .07 .02 .31*** .13* .13* .04 .32*** .08 .08
success
Extraversion, ~age 21 — — — .22* — — — .21* .05 .05 — .22*
Emotional stability, ~age 21 .09* .09* — .31*** .10* .10* .02 .30** .15* .15* — .33*
Openness to experiences, — .31*** — — — .32*** .07* .07* — .31*** — —
~age 21
Erikson stage rating, ~age 47 — .24** — — — .26** .05 .05 — .21** — —
Wisdom, ~age 80 — — — — .21* — —
Subjective well-being, ~age 80 — — — — — .21** —
Overall model fit statistics
  R2 .25 .14 .26 .19 .29 .16
  ML ratio χ2(df), p-value 22.19(16), p = .137 21.55(16), p = .159 20.79(16), p = .187
 SRMR/RMSEA/CFI .078/.063/1.000 .074/.059/1.000 .073/.055/1.000

Notes: n = 98; Robust maximum likelihood estimation using LISREL 9.30; t-values corrected for non-normality. If the indirect effect is equal to the total effect, only an indirect effect exists. ML = maximum likelihood,
SRMR = standardized root mean square residual, RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation, CFI = comparative fit index.
***t-value > 3.29 (p < .001). **t-value > 2.58 (p < .01). *t-value > 1.96 (p < .05).
Journals of Gerontology: SOCIAL SCIENCES, 2018, Vol. 73, No. 8

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/73/8/1514/4857263 by guest on 04 September 2020


Journals of Gerontology: SOCIAL SCIENCES, 2018, Vol. 73, No. 8 1523

adolescents plan for the future by making wise choices a specific developmental pathway between childhood and
related to family and occupational pathways early in life, old age wisdom and well-being, using expert ratings of
which gives them a head start for success. Using 40-year retrospective childhood and adolescent data collected from
longitudinal data from the Berkeley Longitudinal Studies, the men’s parents, psychologists’ ratings of the men’s per-
Clausen (1991) demonstrated that adolescent competence sonality characteristics during college, a different set of
was related to higher educational attainment and occu- experts to rate the men’s midlife generativity based on their
pational prestige and more stable marriages, careers, and qualitative interviews up to age 47, and finally self-assessed
personalities across the life course, particularly for men. scale measures of wisdom and well-being when the men
Childhood upbringing in the form of consistent discipline were around 80  years of age. The different data sources
and parental support appeared to have contributed to ado- and the prospective nature of the data add confidence that

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/73/8/1514/4857263 by guest on 04 September 2020


lescent competence, supporting the current study’s findings. the associations between the variables are not artificially
At first glance, adolescent competence resembles inflated by common method variance.
Staudinger and colleagues’ (Staudinger & Kunzmann, 2005; The longitudinal data support three developmental
Wink & Staudinger, 2016) path of personality adjustment pathways to late-life wisdom and well-being: (a) a per-
that is rewarded by greater success in life. Competent ado- sonality adjustment path to well-being, (b) a personality
lescents were characterized by dependability, responsibility, growth path to wisdom, and (c) a successful human devel-
self-confidence, cheerfulness, equanimity, and the absence opment path across the life course that balances personal-
of rebelliousness, yet they were also more likely than those ity adjustment and growth. Given the long-term effects of
lower on competence to be independent, unconventional early adult personality, psychological interventions that can
in thinking, accepting of uncertainty and complexity, successfully increase emotional stability, extraversion, and
and introspective, which are all indicators of personality openness to experiences (Roberts et al., 2017) are recom-
growth. Clausen’s study of adolescent competence, the mended to foster late-life wisdom and well-being. The third
associations of adolescent school and sports success with pathway of successful psychosocial development does not
emotional stability and conscientiousness in young adult- mean that wisdom only emerges in old age. On the contrary,
hood, and the long-term influence of adolescent school and the development of wisdom takes place across the whole
sport success on old age wisdom support the notion that life course through experiential learning and the mastery of
a balance between personality adjustment and personality crises (Ardelt, 2005; Ardelt & Jacobs, 2009). The research
growth facilitates psychosocial development and growth in suggests that a warm and nurturing childhood, adoles-
wisdom. Hence, it is likely that a positive family environ- cent competence, and mature personality characteristics in
ment fosters not only the successful adaptation to school young adulthood contribute to higher levels of wisdom and
and social life but also adolescent competence and that well-being more than 60 years later in life, indicating that it
role models and mentors in family and school might have is best to plant the seeds of wisdom early.
far reaching influences on psychosocial development and
ultimately wisdom in old age (Baltes & Staudinger, 2000).
This study analyzed data from highly educated white Supplementary Material
men who came of age during WWII, effectively controlling Supplementary data is available at The Journals of
for education, race, gender, and birth cohort. Yet, factors Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social
that contribute to well-being and wisdom might not be Sciences online.
universal. Additionally, if wisdom (or other variables) had
been operationalized differently, the outcome might not be
the same. To assess the results’ generalizability, longitudinal Funding
studies using more diverse samples from different cohorts This work was supported by a grant from the John T.  Templeton
are needed. It would be particularly interesting to replicate Foundation.
this study for the baby boom cohort, who grew up under
dramatically different societal circumstances, norms, and
rules with changing employment, marriage, and family pat- Acknowledgments
terns and a greater interest in personality growth than the This article was presented at the 21st IAGG World Congress of
WWII generation (Lesthaeghe, 2014). Gerontology and Geriatrics in San Francisco, California, July 2017.
Despite its limitations, the study breaks new ground by Author contributions: M.A. planned the study, wrote the article, con-
examining the relative contributions of young adult per- ducted the analysis, and edited the manuscript. K.R.G. contributed to the
sonality and psychosocial development across the whole literature review and edited the manuscript. G.E.V. is the former princi-
life course on wisdom and well-being in old age. Although pal investigator for the Study of Adult Development and gave advice.
the positive correlations between Erikson’s developmental
tasks have been shown in prior studies, most were based
on self-assessed cross-sectional data or only incorporated Conflict of Interest
two time points. By contrast, our study is the first to map The authors do not declare any conflict of interest.
1524 Journals of Gerontology: SOCIAL SCIENCES, 2018, Vol. 73, No. 8

References Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the Big Five.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1246–1256.
Ardelt, M. (2000). Antecedents and effects of wisdom in old age:
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.6.1246
A longitudinal perspective on aging well. Research on Aging, 22,
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1994). Time, human agency, and social change:
360–394. doi:10.1177/0164027500224003
Perspectives on the life course. Social Psychology Quarterly, 57,
Ardelt, M. (2003). Empirical assessment of a three-dimen-
4–15. doi:10.2307/2786971
sional wisdom scale. Research on Aging, 25, 275–324.
Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society. New York, NY:
doi:10.1177/0164027503025003004
Norton.
Ardelt, M. (2005). How wise people cope with crises and obstacles in
George, L. K. (2010). Still happy after all these years: Research
life. Revision: A Journal of Consciousness and Transformation,
frontiers on subjective well-being in later life. The Journals
28, 7–19. doi:10.3200/REVN.28.1.7-19
of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/73/8/1514/4857263 by guest on 04 September 2020


Ardelt, M. (2016). Disentangling the relations between wisdom and
Sciences, 65B, 331–339. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbq006
different types of well-being in old age: Findings from a short-
Hair, E. C., & Graziano, W. G. (2003). Self-esteem, personal-
term longitudinal study. Journal of Happiness Studies, 17, 1963–
ity and achievement in high school: A  prospective longitu-
1984. doi:10.1007/s10902-015-9680-2
dinal study in Texas. Journal of Personality, 71, 971–994.
Ardelt, M., & Edwards, C. A. (2016). Wisdom at the end of life: An
doi:10.1111/1467–6494.7106004
analysis of mediating and moderating relations between wisdom
Helson, R., & Srivastava, S. (2002). Creative and wise peo-
and subjective well-being. The Journals of Gerontology, Series
ple: Similarities, differences and how they develop.
B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 71, 502–513. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1430–1440.
doi:10.1093/geronb/gbv051 doi:10.1177/014616702236874
Ardelt, M., & Jacobs, S. (2009). Wisdom, integrity, and life satisfac- Hollingshead, A. B., & Redlich, F. C. (1958). Social class and mental
tion in very old age. In M. C. Smith (Ed.), Handbook of research illness: Community study. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
on adult learning and development (pp. 732–760). New York, Isaacowitz, D. M., Vaillant, G. E., & Seligman, M. E. (2003).
NY: Routledge. Strengths and satisfaction across the adult lifespan. International
Ardelt, M., & Jeste, D. (2018). Wisdom and hard times: The ameliorat- Journal of Aging & Human Development, 57, 181–201.
ing effect of wisdom on the negative association between adverse doi:10.2190/61EJ-LDYR-Q55N-UT6E
life events and well-being. The Journals of Gerontology, Series James, J. B., & Zarrett, N. (2006). Ego integrity in the lives of older
B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 73, 1374–1383. women. Journal of Adult Development, 13, 61–75. doi:10.1007/
doi:10.1093/geronb/gbw137 s10804-006-9003-2
Baltes, P. B., & Staudinger, U. M. (2000). Wisdom: Jöreskog, K. G., Sörbom, D., du Toit, S., & du Toit, M. (1999).
A  metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and vir- LISREL 8: New statistical features. Chicago, IL: Scientific
tue toward excellence. American Psychologist, 55, 122–136. Software International.
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.122 Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation
Clausen, J. S. (1991). Adolescent competence and the shaping of modeling (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
the life course. American Journal of Sociology, 96, 804–842. Le, T. N. (2011). Life satisfaction, openness value, self-transcen-
doi:10.1086/229609 dence, and wisdom. Journal of Happiness Studies, 12, 171–182.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality doi:10.1007/s10902-010-9182-1
Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO- Lesthaeghe, R. (2014). The second demographic transition: A con-
FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment cise overview of its development. Proceedings of the National
Resources. Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111,
Cox, K. S., Wilt, J., Olson, B., & McAdams, D. P. (2010). 18112–18115. doi:10.1073/pnas.1420441111
Generativity, the Big Five, and psychosocial adaptation Levenson, M. R., Jennings, P. A., Aldwin, C. M., & Shiraishi, R. W.
in midlife adults. Journal of Personality, 78, 1185–1208. (2005). Self-transcendence: Conceptualization and measure-
doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00647.x ment. International Journal of Aging & Human Development,
de St Aubin, E., & McAdams, D. P. (1995). The relations of genera- 60, 127–143. doi:10.2190/XRXM-FYRA-7U0X-GRC0
tive concern and generative action to personality traits, satisfac- Morrison, E. F., Rimm-Kauffman, S., & Pianta, R. C. (2003). A lon-
tion/happiness with life, and ego development. Journal of Adult gitudinal study of mother–child interactions at school entry and
Development, 2, 99–112. doi:10.1007/bf02251258 social and academic outcomes in middle school. Journal of School
DeYoung, C. G. (2006). Higher-order factors of the Big Five in a Psychology, 41, 185–200. doi:10.1016/s0022-4405(03)00044-x
multi-informant sample. Journal of Personality and Social Neff, K. D., Rude, S. S., & Kirkpatrick, K. L. (2007). An examination
Psychology, 91, 1138–1151. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.6.1138 of self-compassion in relation to positive psychological function-
DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., Séguin, J. R., & Tremblay, R. E. ing and personality traits. Journal of Research in Personality, 41,
(2008). Externalizing behavior and the higher order factors of 908–916. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.08.002
the Big Five. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 947–953. Roberts, B. W., Luo, J., Briley, D. A., Chow, P. I., Su, R., & Hill,
doi:10.1037/a0013742 P. L. (2017). A systematic review of personality trait change
Dezutter, J., Wiesmann, U., Apers, S., & Luyckx, K. (2013). Sense of through intervention. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 117–141.
coherence, depressive feelings and life satisfaction in older persons: doi:10.1037/bul0000088
A closer look at the role of integrity and despair. Aging & Mental Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns
Health, 17, 839–843. doi:10.1080/13607863.2013.792780 of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course:
Journals of Gerontology: SOCIAL SCIENCES, 2018, Vol. 73, No. 8 1525

A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, the life cycle. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 1348–
132, 1–25. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1 1359. doi:10.1176/ajp.137.11.1348
Rogosch, F. A., & Cicchetti, D. (2004). Child maltreatment and Vecchione, M., Alessandri, G., Barbaranelli, C., & Caprara, G. (2011).
emergent personality organization: Perspectives from the Five- Higher-order factors of the big five and basic values: Empirical
Factor Model. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 123– and theoretical relations. British Journal of Psychology, 102,
145. doi:10.1023/b:jacp.0000019766.47625.40 478–498. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02006.x
Soldz, S., & Vaillant, G. E. (1999). The Big Five personality traits and Walsh, R. (2015). What is wisdom? Cross-cultural and cross-disci-
the life course: A 45-year longitudinal study. Journal of Research plinary syntheses. Review of General Psychology, 19, 278–293.
in Personality, 33, 208–232. doi:10.1006/jrpe.1999.2243 doi:10.1037/gpr0000045
Soto, C. J. (2015). Is happiness good for your personality? Concurrent Webster, J. D. (2003). An exploratory analysis of a self-assessed wis-
and prospective relations of the big five with subjective well- dom scale. Journal of Adult Development, 10, 13–22. doi:10.10

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/73/8/1514/4857263 by guest on 04 September 2020


being. Journal of Personality, 83, 45–55. doi:10.1111/jopy.12081 23/A:1020782619051
Staudinger, U. M., & Glück, J. (2011). Psychological wisdom Webster, J. D. (2010). Wisdom and positive psychosocial values in
research: Commonalities and differences in a growing field. young adulthood. Journal of Adult Development, 17, 70–80.
Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 215–241. doi:10.1146/ doi:10.1007/s10804-009-9081-z
annurev.psych.121208.131659 Webster, J. D., Westerhof, G. J., & Bohlmeijer, E. T. (2014).
Staudinger, U. M., & Kunzmann, U. (2005). Positive adult per- Wisdom and mental health across the lifespan. The Journals
sonality development: Adjustment and/or growth? European of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and social
Psychologist, 10, 320–329. doi:10.1027/1016-9040.10.4.320 Sciences, 69, 209–218. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbs121
Staudinger, U. M., Maciel, A. G., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1998). What Wink, P., & Dillon, M. (2003). Religiousness, spirituality, and
predicts wisdom-related performance? A first look at personal- psychosocial functioning in late adulthood: Findings from
ity, intelligence, and facilitative experiential contexts. European a longitudinal study. Psychology and Aging, 18, 916–924.
Journal of Personality, 12, 1–17. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099- doi:10.1037/0882-7974.18.4.916
0984(199801/02)12:1%3C1::AID-PER285%3E3.0.CO;2–9 Wink, P., & Helson, R. (1997). Practical and transcendent wisdom:
Steel, P., Schmidt, J., & Shultz, J. (2008). Refining the relationship Their nature and some longitudinal findings. Journal of Adult
between personality and subjective well-being. Psychological Development, 4, 1–15. doi:10.1007/BF02511845
Bulletin, 134, 138–161. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.138 Wink, P., & Staudinger, U. M. (2016). Wisdom and psychosocial
Sternberg, R. J. (1998). A balance theory of wisdom. Review of General functioning in later life. Journal of Personality, 84, 306–318.
Psychology, 2, 347–365. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.4.347 doi:10.1111/jopy.12160
Vaillant, G. E. (1977). Adaptation to life. Boston, MA: Little, Brown. Zacher, H., McKenna, B., Rooney, D., & Gold, S. (2015). Wisdom
Vaillant, G. E., & Milofsky, E. (1980). Natural history of male psy- in the military context. Military Psychology, 27, 142–154.
chological health: IX. Empirical evidence for Erikson’s model of doi:10.1037/mil0000070

You might also like