The Development and Implications of A Personal Problem-Solving Inventory

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/232490851

The Development and Implications of a Personal Problem-


Solving Inventory

Article  in  Journal of Counseling Psychology · January 1982


DOI: 10.1037/0022-0167.29.1.66

CITATIONS READS
1,321 14,301

2 authors, including:

Puncky Paul Heppner


University of Missouri
191 PUBLICATIONS   11,257 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Mentoring View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Puncky Paul Heppner on 17 November 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Counseling Psychology Copyright 1982 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
1982, Vol. 29, No. 1,66-75 0022-0167/82/2901-0066$00.75

The Development and Implications


of a Personal Problem-Solving Inventory
P. Paul Heppner Chris H. Petersen
University of Missouri—Columbia Concordia University

Only a few studies have explicitly attended to the personal problem-solving


process within the counseling literature, perhaps due in part to the dearth of
relevant assessment instruments. This article examines _the dimensions
underlying the applied problem-solving process and describes the develop-
ment of a problem-solving instrument based on factor-analysis results. In ad-
dition, the article delineates initial reliability and validity estimates of the in-
strument. Data were collected from four samples of students (ns = 150, 62,
31, and 18). The factor analysis revealed three distinct constructs: problem-
solving confidence, approach-avoidance style, and personal control. Al-
though previous research has focused on stages within problem solving, these
results suggest the existence of underlying dimensions across stages within
people's perceptions of their real-life, personal problem solving. Reliability
estimates of the 32 items revealed that the constructs were internally consis-
tent and stable over time. Initial estimates of validity suggest that the instru-
ment is measuring constructs that are (a) amenable to change through specific
skill training in problem solving, (b) unrelated to conceptualizing means to
solving a hypothetical problem situation, (c) related to general perceptions of
problem-solving skills, (d) unrelated to intelligence or social desirability, and
(e) related to personality variables, most notably locus of control. The inves-
tigation has implications for future research on the personal problem-solving
process and provides a research instrument that is easily administered and
scored.

Problem solving is of special concern for psychology has been concerned with prob-
professionals who are interested in helping lem solving (e.g., Davis, 1966; Gagne, 1964;
others solve problems that are particularly Maier, 1970; Newell, Shaw, & Simon, 1958),
troublesome. Counselors are such profes- most of the research within counseling has
sionals, as Krumboltz (1965) so aptly remained at the conceptual level (e.g.,
states, Clarke, Gelatt, & Levine, 1965; Urban &
Ford, 1971). In addition, a review of the
The central reason for the existence of counseling is counseling literature revealed that only a few
based on the fact that people have problems that they
are unable to resolve by themselves. They come to a .studies have explicitly attended to the
counselor because they have been led to believe that the problem-solving process (Heppner, 1978).
counselor will be of some assistance to them in resolving In short, researchers within counseling have
their problems. The central purpose of counseling, not developed a technology for helping
then, is to help each client resolve those problems for
which he requests help. (pp. 383-384) clients-with problem solving and decision
making (Horan, 1979). Perhaps contribut-
Although a great deal of research within ing to the lack of research is the dearth of
instruments that measure aspects of the
personal problem-solving process. The
An earlier version of this paper was presented at the Means-Ends Problem Solving Procedure
annual meeting of the American Psychological Associ-
ation, Los Angeles, August 1981. (MEPS) developed by Platt and Spivack
The authors extend their gratitude to Lynne Reeder (1975) is a notable exception.
and Janet Hibel for their assistance in some phases of Another reason for the lack of research on
the data collection, and to Charles J. Krauskopf for his problem solving in counseling is the appar-
technical assistance in developing the instrument.
Requests for reprints should be sent to P. Paul ent irrelevancy of the problem-solving re-
Heppner, 210 McAlester Hall, Psychology Department, search for the practitioner. For example,
University of Missouri, Columbia, Missouri 65211. the utility of research that uses water jar
66
PROBLEM-SOLVING INVENTORY 67

problems (e.g., Jacobus & Johnson, 1964), psychology class during the spring of 1980. They vol-
anagram problems (e.g., Tresselt & Mayzner, unteered to respond to three instruments: The Prob-
lem Solving Inventory (PSI), Level of Problem Solving
1960), or arithmetic problems (e.g., Klaus- Skills Estimate Form (LPSSEF), and Rotter Internal-
meier & Loughlin, 1961) may not be readily External (I-E) Locus of Control, Subjects were told
apparent to the practitioner. Wickelgrin that the experimenter was interested in how people
(1974) noted that such research methodol- solve personal problems and that their responses to the
ogies examine how people solve predefined three questionnaires were part of the investigation. In
addition, the experimenter asked permission to collect
laboratory problems (i.e., formal problems), the subjects' entrance examination scores: The School
which may be different or less complex than and College Ability Test (SCAT, Series II), Missouri
how people solve real-life, applied personal College English Test (MCET), and Missouri Mathe-
problems. matics Placement Test (MMPT). All subjects received
laboratory credit for their participation. The subjects'
Earlier investigators postulated the exis- mean age was 19.2 years, they were primarily freshmen
tence of several "stages" within the prob- and sophomores (45% and 41%, respectively), 38% were
lem-solving process (Clarke et al, 1965; male and 62% were female, and they were largely single
Dewey, 1933; D'Zurilla & Goldfried, 1971; (96%) and largely Caucasian (95%).
A second sample consisted of an additional 62 un-
Goldfried & Goldfried, 1975; Urban & Ford, dergraduate university students enrolled in an intro-
1971). Five stages are common to most ductory psychology class during the fall of 1980; these
models of problem solving: general orien- subjects volunteered to respond to six instruments:
tation, problem definition, generation of PSI, LPSSEF, Social Desirability Scale (SDS), Myers-
alternatives, decision making, and evalua- Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI), Means-Ends Problem
Solving procedure (MEPS), and the Unusual Uses Ac-
tion. In addition, training programs de- tivity (UUA) from the Torrance Test of Creative
signed to enhance subjects' problem-solving Thinking. They were told that the experimenter was
skills often are developed around various interested in how people solve personal problems; all
stages (e.g., Dixon, Heppner, Petersen, & subjects received laboratory credit for their participa-
Ronning, 1979; D'Zurilla & Goldfried, 1971; tion. The subjects' mean age was 18.9 years, they were
primarily freshmen (77%), 24% were male and 76% were
Mendonca & Siess, 1976). Although some female, and they were largely single (93%) and largely
evidence has suggested that problem solving Caucasian (91%). Half of this group (n = 31) consti-
is a function of different activities (Johnson, tuted the third sample of subjects; they were asked to
Parrott, & Stratton, 1968; Spivack & Shure, return 2 weeks later and .were readministered the
PSI.
1974), no research has empirically investi- A final group of subjects (n = 18) consisted of un-
gated the existence of these stages and con- dergraduate students who registered to participate in
comitant problem-solving skills in applied a problem-solving workshop offered by the guidance
problem-solving situations. In addition, it center at a Canadian university. The subjects' mean
is unclear whether there are dimensions age was 33.9 years; 39% were male and 61% were fe-'
male.
underlying the applied problem-solving \
process, and whether the process is most
accurately described in terms of distinct Instruments
stages or perhaps dimensions that cut across The present PSI consisted of a 6-point, Likert-type
format of 35 items constructed by the authors as face
The purpose of this investigation was to valid measures of each of the five problem-solving
examine the dimensions underlying the stages, based on a revision of an earlier problem-solving
real-life, personal problem-solving process inventory (Heppner & Petersen, Note 1). The items
through an exploratory factor analysis. We were randomly ordered and written to contain an equal
number of positive and negative statements about
also describe in this article the development problem solving. Low scores indicate behaviors and
of a problem-solving instrument based on attitudes typically associated with successful problem
the factor-analysis results and delineate re- solving.
liability and validity estimates of the in- On the LPSSEF, subjects rated their1 level of prob-
lem-solving skills (1-9) in comparison with other stu-
strument. dents (1 = very much less, 9 = very much more;
Heppner, 1979). In addition, students rated their level
Method of satisfaction or dissatisfaction (1-6) with their present
level of problem-solving skills (1 = very dissatisfied, 6
Subjects = very satisfied). The respective test-retest reliability
coefficients over a 2-week period were .99 and .93 (n =
The initial sample (n - 150) consisted of under- 53).
graduate university students enrolled in an introductory The Rotter Internal-External (I-E) Locus of Control
68 P. PAUL HEPPNER AND CHRIS H. PETERSEN

is a 29-item forced-choice format test that measures a the first three stories between judges used in this study
person's belief or expectations about how reinforcement were .83, .95, and .90, respectively.
is controlled (Rotter, 1966). Low scores indicate an The Unusual Uses Activity (Form A, Cardboard
internal locus of control. Rotter (1966) reported several Boxes) in the Torrance Test of. Creative Thinking
estimates of internal consistency (a = .69 to .73), and measures aspects of the creative-thinking process
several estimates of testr-retest reliability (r = .55 to .78). (Torrance, 1966). High scores indicate greater fluency,
.Several studies have provided empirical support for flexibility, and originality. Estimates of test-retest
construct validity of the scale. reliability ranged from .60 to .75 across the three scores
The SCAT, Series II is an intelligence test that pro- (Mackler, 1963; Yamamoto, 1962). In addition, Tor-
vides estimates of basic verbal and mathematical ability rance (1966) provided additional data supporting the
(Educational Testing Service, 1967). High scores in- construct, concurrent, and predictive validity of his tests
dicate greater verbal and mathematical ability. The of creative thinking. Interrater reliabilities on scoring
Educational Testing Service (1967) provides several the fluency and flexibility scores between two raters
estimates of predictive validity between scores on the used in this study were .99 and .97, respectively.
SCAT and later academic achievement (r = .41 to .69),
concurrent validity between scores on the SCAT and the Procedure
Scholastic Aptitude Test (r = .52 to .86), and test-retest
reliability (r = .87 to ,94). Data were collected from the four samples of stu-
The MCET is a 90-item test that assesses students' dents. A total of 150 students initially responded to the
knowledge of the mechanics and effectiveness of written PSI, and these data served as the basis for the factor
communication (Callis & Johnson, 1965). Content analysis. On the basis of the factor analysis, the 35-item
validity of the items and scoring was established by questionnaire was reduced to 32 items. Additional data
using college English teachers as judges. Split-half were collected from other students to establish an es-
reliability coefficients range from .92 to .95 (Callis & timate of test-retest reliability and estimates of con-
Johnson, 1965), High scores indicate greater knowl- current validity (rcs = 31, 62). Finally, a study was
edge. conducted to provide further support for the construct
The MMPT provides information about the algebraic validity of the instrument.
skills of entering college freshmen (Krauskopf, Bur-
cham, & Willis, 1967). High scores indicate greater
mathematical knowledge. Estimates of reliability Results
provided a split-half reliability coefficient of .84, and
a test-retest correlation of .81 (Krauskopf et al., 1967). Factor Analyses
The authors provided several estimates of predictive
validity and concurrent validity. A principal-components factor analysis
The SDS, a 33-item, true-false instrument, locates
individuals who describe themselves in favorable, so- was performed using the Statistical Package
cially desirable terms in order to achieve the approval for the Social Sciences (Nie, Hull, Jenkins,
of others (Crowne & Marlowe, 1964). High scores in- Steinbrenner, & Bent, 1970). The analysis
dicate more socially desirable responses. Internal yielded 10 factors having eigenvalues greater
consistency was estimated to be .88 and a test-retest
correlation over a 1-month interval also was reported than 1.00 and accounting for 65% of the
to be .88 (Crowne & Marlowe, 1964). common variance. Using a scree test to
The MBTI is a personality inventory that assesses identify only the major common factors
basic differences in the way people prefer to use per- (Cattell, 1965), three factors were extracted.
ception and judgment (Myers, 1962). The indicator Using a varimax rotation and applying the
contains separate indices for determining each of the
four basic preferences: extraversion or introversion, rule of retaining only those factor loadings
sensing or intuition, thinking or feeling, judgment or above .3, three factors were identified with
perception. Split-half reliability estimates of the four 11,16, and 5 items. A summary of the items
indices ranged from .44 to .94 (Myers, 1962). Myers that contribute to each of the three factors
also presented a wide range of data that provide esti-
mates of construct and concurrent validity. is presented in Table 1. The first factor was
The MEPS consists of 10 stories involving interper- labeled by the experimenters "problem-
sonal problem situations and provides a measure of an solving confidence," and as Table 1 indicates
individual's ability to conceptualize means of moving it includes items that seem to assess confi-
toward an effective solution (Platt & Spivack, 1975). dence in engaging in a wide range of prob-
High scores indicate a greater ability to conceptualize
the means in solving problems. Estimates of reliability lem-solving activities. The second factor
include coefficients of internal consistency (.80-.84) and was labeled "approach-avoidance style,"
test-retest correlations (.43-63). Platt & Spivack because a central theme among most of these
(1975) also provided some evidence of discriminant and items involved whether an individual ap-
concurrent validity, and considerable evidence for
construct validity (Platt, Scura, & Hannon, 1973; Platt, proaches or avoids different problem-solving
Spivack, Altman, Altman, & Peizer, 1974). Interrater activities. The third factor was labeled
reliabilities on scoring the number of means in each of "personal control," since most of the items
PROBLEM-SOLVING INVENTORY 69

Table 1
Summary of Items Contributing to Each Factor, Factor Loadings, and Item-Part Correlations

Item-part
Item Loading correlation

Problem-solving confidence
5. I am usually able to think up creative and effective alternatives to solve a
problem. .42 .42
10. I have the ability to solve most problems even though initially no solution
is immediately apparent. .63 .62
11. Many problems I face are too complex for me to solve. .45 .42
12. I make decisions and am happy with them later. .51 .52
19. When I make plans to solve a problem, I am almost certain that I can make
them work. .63 .53
23. Given enough time and effort, I believe I can solVe most problems that
confront me. .73 .71
24. When faced with a novel situation I have confidence that I can handle
problems that may arise. .75 .70
27. I trust my ability to solve new and difficult problems. .71 .68
33. After making a decision, the outcome I expected usually matches the
actual outcome. .49 .47
34. When confronted with a problem, I am unsure of whether I can handle the
situation. .65 .64
35. When I become aware of a problem, one of the first things I do is to try to
find out exactly what the problem is. .46 .48
Approach avoidance style
1. When a solution to a problem was unsuccessful, I do not examine why it
didn't work. .33 .37
2. When I am confronted with a complex problem, I do not bother to develop
a strategy to collect information so I can define exactly what the problem
is. .40 .42
4. After I have solved a problem, I do not analyze what went right or what
went wrong. .38 .40
6. After I have tried to solve a problem with a certain course of action, I take
time and compare the actual outcome to what I thought should have
happened. .41 .39
7. When I have a problem, I think up as many possible ways to handle it as I
can until I can't come up with any more ideas. .53 .47
8. When confronted with a problem, I consistently examine my feelings to
find out what is going on in a problem situation. .30 .25
13. When confronted with a problem, I tend to do the first thing that I can
think of to solve it. .59 .57
15. When deciding on an idea or possible solution to a problem, I do not take
time to consider the chances of each alternative being successful. .49 .50
16. When confronted with a problem, I stop and think about it before deciding
on a next step. .46 .47
17. I generally go with the first good idea that comes to my mind. .45 .38
(table continued)
70 P. PAUL HEPPNER AND CHRIS H. PETERSEN

Table 1 (continued)
Item-part
Item Loading correlation

18. When making a decision, I weigh the consequences of each alternative and
compare them against each other. .71 .71
20. I try to predict the overall result of carrying out a particular course of
action, .44 .44
21. When I try to think up possible solutions to a problem, I do not come up
with very many alternatives. .37 .43
28. I have a systematic method for comparing alternatives and making
decisions. , .43 .43
30. When confronted with a problem, I do not usually examine what sort of
external things my environment may be contributing to my problem. .41 .40
31. When I am confused by a problem, one of the first things I do is survey the
situation and consider all the relevant pieces of information. .58 .58
Personal control
3. When my first efforts to solve a problem fail, I become uneasy about my
ability to handle the situation. .44 .40
14. Sometimes I do not stop and take time to deal with my problems, but just
kind of muddle ahead. .42 .46
25. Even though I work on a problem, sometimes I feel like I am groping or
wandering, and am not getting down to the real issue. .71 .61
26. I make snap judgments and later regret them. ;53 .46
32. Sometimes I get so charged up emotionally that I am unable to consider
many ways of dealing with my problems. .60 .51

that loaded on this factor involved elements A second sample (n = 62) from the same
of self-control. The factor loadings for each universe, undergraduate students from an
item are also presented in Table 1; an anal- introductory psychology class, provided
ysis of the factor loadings revealed the fol- cross-validation data. The similar norma-
lowing ranges of loadings: problem-solving tive data are as follows: Factor 1—f actorM
confidence, .42 to .75; approach-avoidance = 26.16, SD = 7.90 (item M = 2.38, SD =
style, .30 to .71; personal control, .42 to .71. .38); Factor 2—factor M = 43.68, SD = 11.40
(item M = 2.71, SD = .45); Factor 3—factor
M
Normative Data = 18-32, SD = 5.19 (item M = 3.66, SD =
.51); total inventory—inventory M = 88.16,
Based on the responses from 150 under- SD = 19.03 (item M = 2.75, SD = .54).
graduate students, the following normative
data based on exact scores were obtained for Estimates of Reliability
each of the three factors and the total in-
ventory:1 Factor 1 (problem-solving confi- An estimate of internal consistency was
dence)—factor M = 24.47, -SD = 7.53 (item computed for each of the three factors and
M = 2.44, SD .= .33); Factor 2 (approach-
avoidance style)—factor M = 46.21, SD = —
1
11.51 (item M = 2.88, SD = .47); Factor 3 Although the factors are orthogonal, a total PSI
(personal control)—factor M = 18.40, SD = acore was used as a conglomerate of the three factors
5.06 (item M = 3.68, SD = .44); total inven- (analogous to an IQ score) and thus a more general index
+ -~ „ 4. us nt CA on on oe of problem solving. Additional research is needed to
tory—inventory M = 91.50, SD = 20.65 determine the comparative utility of the total score
(item M = 2.86, SD = .58). versus factor scores.
PROBLEM-SOLVING INVENTORY 71

the total inventory (n = 150), which are as tively). Again, all correlations were statis-
follows: problem-solving confidence, a = tically nonsignificant (p s > .05). Thus, the
.85; approach-avoidance style, a. = .84; per- instrument is not correlated with intelligence
sonal control, a = .72; and total inventory, a measures or academic achievement.
= .90. The item to part correlations are also Campbell (1960) also maintained that in
presented in Table 1. Estimates of test- establishing construct validity each new test
retest reliability were established by ad- should be correlated with a general measure
ministering the inventory to an additional 31 of social desirability. Scores on the three
undergraduate students on two occasions factors and total PSI were correlated with
approximately 2 weeks apart. The test- the SDS (n = 62; rs = -.09, -.09, -.24, -.16,
retest reliabilities were as follows: prob- respectively). The correlations were sta-
lem-solving confidence, r = .85; approach- tistically nonsignificant (ps > .05), except
avoidance style, r = .88; personal control, r with the third factor (r = -.24, p < .05).
- .83; and the total inventory, r = .89. Thus scores on the PSI do not seem to be
highly correlated with scores on a general
Estimates of Validity measure of social desirability.
Validity coefficients were also computed
Estimates of concurrent and construct by correlating the scores on the three factors
validity were established through several and the total PSI with scores on the Rotter
means. First, scores on the three factors I-E scale (n = 33), the UUA (n = 62), and
and the total PSI were correlated with the MBTI (n = 62). All correlations were
the LPSSEF (n = 150), specifically with stu- statistically significant (ps < .01) with the
dents' ratings of their levels of problem- Rotter I-E scale (rs = .64, .53, .40, .61, re-
solving skills (rs = —.44, —.29, —.43, and spectively). Correlations between scores on
-.46, respectively), and students' perceived Factor 1, Factor 2, the total PSI and both the
satisfaction/dissatisfaction with their fluency and flexibility scores on the UUA
problem-solving skills (rs = -.42, -.24, -.39, were not statistically significant (all ps >
and -.42, respectively). All correlations .05). Correlations between scores on Factor
were statistically significant (ps < .0001). 3 (personal control), however, were moder-
Thus, scores on the PSI correlate moderately ately low but statistically significant (ps <
well with scores on a simple self-rating scale. .02) with both the fluency and flexibility
Scores on the PSI were also correlated with scores (rs = -.27 and -.34, respectively).
scores on the first three stories of the MEPS All correlations with the continuous scores
(n = 62). All correlations were statistically ' of each of the four type indicators on the
nonsignificant (ps > .05). MBTI were statistically nonsignificant (ps
Campbell (1960) maintained that in esr > .05), except between scores on the third
tablishing construct validity for any new test, factor and the thinking-feeling scores (r =
it is useful to correlate the new instrument .25,p<.05).
with an intelligence test. Scores on the PSI Finally, a small study (n = 18) was con-
were correlated with the SCAT, Series II (n ducted that supported the construct Validity
— 98); the scores on all three factors and the of the instrument. The study used a Post-
total PSI were correlated with scores on the test Only - Control Group Design (Campbell
Verbal section (rs = .09, .08, .11, .09, re- & Stanley, 1966; Design No. 6), and involved
spectively), with scores on the Quantitative a comparison of subjects who had received
section (rs = .14, .10, .12, .15, respectively), problem-solving training versus a control
and with the total score (rs = .13, .11, .11, .13, sample. Training consisted of a workshop
respectively). Scores on the three factors emphasizing a process approach to problem
and total PSI were also correlated with solving, and entailed six 1-hour sessions.
MCET scores (n = 98; rs = -.03, -.12, .16, Subjects received group and individual in-
-.02), and MMPT scores (n = 99; rs = .04, struction as well as homework assignments
-.02, .11, .08, respectively). In addition, covering a wide range of problem-solving
scores on the three factors and the total PSI skills. Since it was predicted that the ex-
were correlated with subjects' high school perimental subjects would rate themselves
rank (n = 88; rs = .14, -.01, .18, .06, respec- more positively on the PSI than the control
72 P. PAUL HEPPNER AND CHRIS H. PETERSEN

group would, a one-tailed test of significance confidence in one's problem-solving ability


was conducted. Results indicated that the and somewhat related to personal control.
group that was posttested immediately fol- Furthermore, earlier investigations revealed
lowing the workshop scored significantly that people who expressed confidence in
lower on the PSI than the control group, their ability to control aspects of their envi-
which was pretested immediately prior to ronment also tended to be better problem
the workshop, t (16) = 1.86, p < .05. solvers (Bloom & Broder, 1950; Lefcourt
& Loughlin, 1966; Rotter, 1966). Rotter
Discussion (1978) also postulated that actively seeking
alternatives was a functional problem-solv-
The results of the factor analysis indicate ing attitude, which seems to be consistent
that there are at least three dimensions with the second construct, approach-
underlying the perceived personal prob- avoidance style. In addition, some research
lem-solving process of college students. An evidence supports the notion that successful
analysis of the items that loaded on each problem solvers are not impulsive and do not
factor revealed constructs such as confidence avoid the problem, but rather systematically
in one's problem-solving ability, an ap- engage in several problem-solving behaviors
proach-avoidance style, and personal con- (Bloom & Broder, 1950; Dollard & Miller,
trol. Estimates of reliability indicate these 1950; Osborn, 1963; Parnes, 1967; Shaftel &
constructs are internally consistent as well Shaftel, 1967). Research on the self-control
as stable over time. In addition, cross vali- process seems to support the third construct,
dation data from two different samples in- personal control; for example, investigators
dicate that the normative data are consis- have found that successful problem solvers
tent across similar samples of college stu- had more strategies to control their behavior,
dents. It is important to note that the items and seemed to be more deliberate in that
depicting each of the five general problem- process (Heffernan & Richards, 1981; Perri
solving stages loaded in an almost random & Richards, 1977).
fashion across each of three constructs. The initial findings with regard to estab-
Although several writers have described lishing concurrent and construct validity
distinct stages within the problem-solving provide additional information about the
process (e.g., Clarke et al., 1965; Dewey, 1933, instrument. First, it appears that the PSI
D'Zurilla & Goldfried, 1971), the results can correlates moderately well with a simple
be interpreted as suggesting the existence of self-rating scale. Subjects who respond to
underlying dimensions across stages within the PSI in ways that reflect behaviors and
students' perception of their real-life per- attitudes typically associated with successful
sonal problem solving. Such a notion may problem solving also tend to rate themselves
more accurately portray the complexity of as better problem solvers and to be more
real-life problem solving; describing applied satisfied with their problem-solving skills.
problem solving only in terms of content- Second, the PSI is able to detect differences
stages not only may be an oversimplification between groups of students who have re-
but also may mask important individual ceived training in problem solving and those
differences in the applied problem-solving who have not. Third, subjects' responses to
process. the PSI do not seem to be related to re-
The three dimensions of confidence in sponses on another measure of problem
one's problem-solving ability, an ap- solving, the MEPS. This finding may in-
proach-avoidance style, and personal control dicate that these two instruments measure
are consistent with some earlier research. In different aspects of the personal problem-
examining generalized expectancies for solving process; conceptualizing means to a
personal problem solving, Rotter (1978) hypothetical problem situation is quite dif-
postulated that the most important prob- ferent from reflecting on what one actually
lem-solving attitude was the expectancy that does in solving real-life personal problems.
one can affect, in part, what happens to Parenthetically, Janis and Mann (1977) note
oneself. This seems to be closely related to that there is a growing body of evidence that
PROBLEM-SOLVING INVENTORY 73

indicates that people respond differently to> of different counseling or career-planning


hypothetical situations than they do to interventions? Would problem-solving
real-life situations (Collins & Hoyt, 1972; training be more effective if it attended to
Cooper, 1971; Deutsch, Krauss, Rosenau, the three underlying dimensions on the PSI?
1962; Gerard, Blevans, & Malcolm, 1964; And on a theoretical level, how do the three
N,el, Helmreich, & Aronson, 1969; Singer & dimensions relate to the real-life personal
Kornfield, 1973; Taylor, 1975). Fourth, the problem-solving process across the previ-
instrument does not seem to be another ously hypothesized five stages? Additional
variation of an intelligence test, nor does it data are needed to investigate these and
seem to be a mislabeled social desirability many other research problems related to the
inventory. Fifth, correlations with other applied personal problem-solving process.
inventories indicate that scores on the PSI
are not strongly related to measures of cre- Reference Notes
ativity or to personality types as indicated by
the MBTI. People who report being more 1. Heppner, P. P., & Petersen, C. H. The develop-
like "the successful problem solver" also ment, factor analysis, and initial validation of a
problem solving instrument. Paper presented at
tend to report having an internal locus of the meeting of the American Education Research
control. The PSI seems to be more than a Association, Toronto, March 1978.
measure of locus of control; for example, the 2. Harren, B. A. Assessment of career decision-
PSI correlates better with subjects' self- making (ACDM): Counselor/instructor guide.
ratings of problem solving (LPSSEP) than Unpublished manuscript, Southern Illinois Uni-
versity at Carbondale, 1978.
does the Rotter I-E. In short, the findings
as an aggregate provide some initial esti- References
mates of validity for the PSI.
There has been an absence of instruments Bloom, B. S., & Broder, L. J. Problem solving processes
that attempt to measure constructs associ- of college students. Chicago: University of Chicago
ated with applied problem solving. Thus, Press, 1950.
Callis, R,, & Johnson, W. Missouri College English
the PSI may serve as a much needed re- Test. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, 1965.
search tool for investigators who want to Campbell, D. T. Recommendations for APA test
assess people's perceptions of the problem- standards regarding construct, trait, and discriminant
solving process. An advantage of the PSI is validity. American Psychologist, 1960, 15, 546-
the ease with which it is administered and 553.
Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. Experimental and
scored, in contrast to the cumbersome and quasi-experimental designs for research. Chicago:
difficult scoring of the MEPS. In addition, Rand McNally, 1966.
whereas the MEPS provides information Cattell, R. B. Factor analysis: An introduction to es-
about people's ability to conceptualize sentials. Biometrics, 1965,21,190-215.
means in solving hypothetical problems, the Clarke, R., Gelatt, H. B., & Levine, L. A decision-
making paradigm for local guidance research. Per-
PSI provides quite different information sonnel and Guidance Journal, 1965,44,40-51.
about people's self-perceptions. Until fur- Collins, B. E., & Hoyt, M. F. Personal responsibility
ther research is conducted on the PSI, the for consequences: An integration and extension of
instrument would best be restricted to re- the "forced compliance" literature, Journal of Ex-
perimental Social Psychology, 1972,8,558-593.
search functions. Generalizations about the Cooper, J. Personal responsibility and dissonance:
instrument must also be restricted to college The role of foreseen consequences. Journal of Per-
students at this time; research has not been sonality and Social Psychology, 1971,18,354-363.
extended to other populations. Crowne, D., & Marlowe, D. The approval motive.
Finally, the results of the study introduce New York: Wiley, 1964.
Davis, G. A. Current status of research and theory in
a wide range of research problems. What human problem solving. Psychological Bulletin,
are the relationships between people's per- 1966,66, 36-54.
ceptions of their personal problem-solving Deutsch, M., Krauss, R. M., & Rosenau, N.. Dissonance
processes and (a) decision-making styles or defensiveness. Journal of Personality, 1962,30,
16-28.
(e.g., Barren, Note 2), (b) the decision Dewey, J. How we think. New York: Heath, 1933.
whether or not to seek counseling or career- Dixon, D. N., Heppner, P. P., Petersen, C. H., & Ronri-
planning services, and (c) the effectiveness ing, R. R. Problem solving workshop training.
74 P. PAUL HEPPNER AND CHRIS H. PETERSEN

Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1979, 26, 133- (University Microfilms No. 64-68,41)
139. Maier, N. R. F. Problem solving and creativity.
Bollard, J., & Miller, N. E. Personality and psycho- Belmont, Calif.: Brooks/Cole, 1970.
therapy. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950. Mendonca, J. D., & Siess, T. F. Counseling for indeci-
D'Zurilla, T. J., & Goldfried, M. R. Problem solving siveness: Problem-solving and anxiety management
and behavior modification. Journal -of Abnormal training. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1976,
Psychology, 1971, 78,107-126. 23,.339-347.
Educational Testing Service. Handbook: SCAT Se- Myers, I. B. The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator:
ries II. Princeton, N.J., 1967. Manual. Palo Alto, Calif.: Consulting Psychologists
Gagne,R.M. Problem solving. In A. W. Melton (Ed.), Press, 1962.
Categories of human learning. .New York: Aca- Nel, E., Helmreich, R., & Aronson, E. Opinion change
demic Press, 1964. in the advocate as a function of the persuasibility of
Gerard, H. B., Blevans, S. A., & Malcolm, T. Self- his audience: A clarification of the meaning of dis-
evaluation and the evaluation of choice alternatives. sonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Journal of Personality, 1964,32,395-410. chology, 1969,12,117-124.
Goldfried, M. R., & Goldfried, A, P. Cognitive change Newell, A,, Shaw, J. C., & Simon, H. A. Elements of a
methods. In P. H. Kanfer & A. P. Goldstein (Eds.), theory of human problem solving. Psychological
Helping people change. New York: Pergamon Review, 1958,65,151-166.
Press, 1975. Nie, N. H., Hull, C. H., Jenkins, J. G., Steinbrenner, K.,
Heffernan, T., & Richards, C. S, Self-control of study & Bent, D. H. Statistical package for the Social
behavior: Identification and evaluation of natural Sciences (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill,
methods. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1981, 1970.
28, 361-364. Osborn, A, F. Applied imagination: Principles and
Heppner, P.P. A review of the problem-solving liter- procedures of creative problem-solving (3rd ed.).
ature and its relationship to the counseling process. New York: Scribner's, 1963.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1978, 25, 366- Fames, S, J. Creative behavior guidebook. New York:
375. Scribner's, 1967.
Heppner, P. P. The effects of client perceived need and Perri, M. G., & Richards, C. S. An investigation of
counselor role on clients' behaviors (Doctoral dis- naturally occurring episodes of self-controlled be-
sertation, University of Nebraska, 1979). Disserta- haviors. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1977,
tion Abstracts International, 1979,39,5950A-5951A. 23,178-183.
(University Microfilms No. 79-07,542). Platt, J. J., Scura, W. C., & Hannon, J. R. Problem-
Horan, J. J. Counseling for effective decision making: solving thinking of youthful incarcerated heroin ad-
A cognitive-behavioral perspective. North Scituate, dicts. Journal of Community Psychology, 1973, 7,
Mass.: Duxbury Press, 1979. 278-281.
Jacobus, K. A., & Johnson, N. F. An experimental set Platt, J. J., & Spivack, G. Manual for the Means-Ends
to adopt a set. Psychological Reports, 1964, 15, Problem-Solving Procedure (MEPS): A measure
737. of interpersonal cognitive problem solving skill.
Janis, I. L,, & Mann, L. Decision making: A psycho- Philadelphia: Department of Mental Health Sci-
logical analysis of conflict, choice, and commitment ences, Hahnemann Medical College and Hospital,
New York: Free Press, 1977. 1975.
Johnson, D. M., Parrott, G. R., & Stratton, R. P. Pro- Platt, J. J., Spivack, G., Altman, N., Altman, D., &
duction and judgment of solutions to five problems. Peizer, S. B. Adolescent problem-solving thinking.
Journal of Educational Psychology Monograph, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1974,
1968,59,(6,Pt.2). 42,787-793.
Johnson, F. Y. Political attitudes as related to inter- Rotter, J. B. Generalized expectancies for internal
nal and external control. Unpublished master's versus external control of reinforcement. Psycho-
thesis, Ohio State University, 1961. logical Monographs, 1966,80(1, Whole No. 609).
Klausmeier, H. J., & Loughlin, L. J. Behaviors during Rotter, J. B. Generalized expectancies for problem
problem solving among children of low, average, and solving and psychotherapy. Cognitive Therapy and
high intelligence. Journal of Educational Psy- Research, 1978,2,1-10.
chology, 1961,52,148-152. Shaftel, F. R., & Shaftel, G. Role playing for social
Krauskopf, C. J., Burcham, P. B., & Willis, C. G. A values: Decision making in the social studies.
manual for the Missouri Mathematics Placement Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1967.
Test. Testing and Counseling Service Report; 1967, Singer, D. G., & Kornfield, B. Conserving and con-
22(3), 1-13. suming: A developmental study of abstract and ac-
Krumboltz, J. D. Behavioral counseling: Rationale tion choices. Developmental Psychology, 1973, 8,
and research. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 314.
1965,44,383-387. Spivack, G., & Shure, M. B. Social adjustment of
Lefcourt, H. J., & Loughlin, H. M. Internal versus ex- young children. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass,
ternal control of reinforcement: A review. Psy- 1974.
chological Bulletin, 1966,65, 206-220. Taylor, S. E. On inferring one's attitudes from one's
Mackler, B, Creativity and life styles (Doctoral dis- behavior: Some delimiting conditions. Journal of
sertation, University of Kansas, 1963) Dissertation Personality and Social Psychology, 1975, 31,
Abstracts International, 1963, 24, 5571-5572. 1126-1133.
PROBLEM-SOLVING INVENTORY 75

Torrance, E. P. Torrance tests of creative thinking: Wickelgrin, W. A. How to solve problems. San
Norms-technical manual. Princeton, N.J.: Per- Francisco,Calif.: Freeman, 1974.
sonnel Press, 1966.' Yamamoto, K. A study of the relationships between
Tresselt, M. E., & Mayzner, M. S. Switching behavior creative thinking abilities of fifth-grade teachers and
in a problem solving task. Journal of Psychology, academic achievement and personal-social adjust-
1960,50,349-354. ment of their pupils (Doctoral dissertation, Univer-
Urban, H., & Ford, H. Some historical and conceptual sity of Minnesota, 1962). Dissertation Abstracts
perspectives of psychotherapy and behavioral change. International, 1962, 23, 2801. (University Micro-
In A. Bergin & S. Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of psy- films No. 63-18,73)
chotherapy and behavior change. New York:
Wiley, 1971. Received Februaryl2,1981 •

View publication stats

You might also like