Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/370002570

Domain-specific and cross-domain effects of the home literacy and numeracy


environment at 3 years on children's academic competencies at 5 and 9 years

Article  in  Developmental Psychology · April 2023


DOI: 10.1037/dev0001515

CITATIONS READS

0 108

3 authors:

Mirela Conica Elizabeth Nixon


Trinity College Dublin Trinity College Dublin
5 PUBLICATIONS   7 CITATIONS    113 PUBLICATIONS   1,596 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Jean Quigley
Trinity College Dublin
41 PUBLICATIONS   265 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Caregivers and peers as support-givers: An exploration of responses to adolescents showing signs of depression View project

Peer-to-peer motivational interview intervention for smoking, alcohol and physical activity among at-risk adolescents in low SES communities: A feasibility trial. View
project

All content following this page was uploaded by Mirela Conica on 14 April 2023.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Developmental Psychology
Domain-Specific and Cross-Domain Effects of the Home Literacy and
Numeracy Environment at 3 Years on Children's Academic Competencies at
5 and 9 Years
Mirela Conica, Elizabeth Nixon, and Jean Quigley
Online First Publication, April 13, 2023. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0001515

CITATION
Conica, M., Nixon, E., & Quigley, J. (2023, April 13). Domain-Specific and Cross-Domain Effects of the Home Literacy and
Numeracy Environment at 3 Years on Children's Academic Competencies at 5 and 9 Years. Developmental Psychology.
Advance online publication. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0001515
Developmental Psychology
© 2023 The Author(s)
ISSN: 0012-1649 https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001515

Domain-Specific and Cross-Domain Effects of the


Home Literacy and Numeracy Environment at 3 Years on Children’s
Academic Competencies at 5 and 9 Years
Mirela Conica, Elizabeth Nixon, and Jean Quigley
School of Psychology, Trinity College Dublin

This study examined whether children’s formal and informal home literacy (HLE) and home numeracy
(HNE) environments at 3 years old demonstrated domain-specific, and cross-domain effects on children’s
academic performance at 5 and 9 years old. Participants were 7,110 children (49.4% male; 84.4% Irish),
recruited between 2007 and 2008 in Ireland. Structural equation modeling revealed that only the informal
HLE and HNE demonstrated both domain-specific and cross-domain positive effects on children’s language
and numeracy outcomes but not on socioemotional outcomes, at 5 and 9 years old. Effect sizes ranged from
small (β = 0.020) to moderate (β = 0.209). These results suggest that even casual cognitively stimulating
activities that do not directly focus on active teaching may benefit children’s educational outcomes.
Findings bear implications for cost-effective interventions with far-reaching, and enduring, effects across
multiple child outcomes.

Public Significance Statement


This study suggests that informal home numeracy and literacy activities such as playing board games,
card games, having access to books, and shared reading during toddlerhood may contribute to children’s
school performance during the preschool and early childhood years. These findings suggest that even
home learning activities that are associated with fun and do not actively focus on teaching can positively
influence children’s skills across literacy and numeracy domains of development.

Keywords: home literacy environment, home numeracy environment, early academic performance,
longitudinal, language outcomes, social-emotional competencies

Supplemental materials: https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001515.supp

Upon entry into the formal school system, children’s cognitive with teachers reporting that approximately half of the children
and social skills are important not only for immediate adjustment who begin school lack the skills expected by the academic environ-
and performance but also for their subsequent academic trajectories ment (Kindred Squared, 2022). Conversely, in Ireland, although
(Duncan et al., 2007; LeFevre et al., 2010). In accordance with the teachers believe that children do not necessarily require basic numer-
“skill begets skill” assumption (Cunha & Heckman, 2007), children acy and literacy skills upon school entry, they express concerns that
can capitalize upon rudimentary skills to engage with the school children lack the necessary social, emotional, and self-management
environment, to further refine their skills, and to ultimately enter a skills such as regulating emotions and making friends (Sloan et al.,
perpetual cycle of academic achievement. However, approximately 2022). Given that children’s individual differences in school perfor-
25% of American children begin school without the necessary skills mance are apparent as early as school entry, it is important to identify
and, as a result, begin their academic journeys at a disadvantage which aspects of children’s early environments—prior to any formal
(Isaacs, 2012). This figure is even larger in the United Kingdom, schooling—might account for this variability.

Nixon served in a supporting role for conceptualization, formal analysis,


This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in investigation, and writing–original draft. Jean Quigley served in a support-
the public, commercial, or nonprofit sectors. We have no conflicts of interest ing role for conceptualization, formal analysis, investigation, and
to disclose. writing–original draft. Mirela Conica, Elizabeth Nixon, and Jean Quigley
Open access funding provided by Irish Research e-Library: This work is contributed equally to methodology, writing–review and editing, and visu-
licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License alization. Elizabeth Nixon and Jean Quigley contributed equally to
(CC BY 4.0, https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). This license per- supervision.
mits copying and redistributing the work in any medium or format, as well Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Mirela
as adapting the material for any purpose, even commercially. Conica, School of Psychology, Áras an Phiarsaigh, Trinity College Dublin,
Mirela Conica served as lead for conceptualization, data curation, Dublin 2, Ireland. Email: conicam@tcd.ie
formal analysis, investigation, and writing–original draft. Elizabeth

1
2 CONICA, NIXON, AND QUIGLEY

The Early Home Learning Environment and Academic Similarly, shared reading at 1–2 years old has been linked with aca-
Performance demic achievement at 8–11 years old (Brown et al., 2022).
Furthermore, meta-analytic evidence from 16 intervention studies
As Vygotsky’s (1978) sociocultural theory suggests, children aimed at promoting dialogic reading (e.g., highlighting words,
develop within social interactions with more experienced counterparts, emphasizing sounds, asking questions during shared-book reading)
who encourage and aid children’s acquisition of skills beyond what indicates that children whose parents received the intervention dis-
they could achieve independently. In the context of this theoretical played greater vocabulary growth compared to their counterparts
framework, examining how early aspects of children’s home learning whose parents engaged in “reading-as-usual” (Mol et al., 2008).
environment may promote their academic competencies can provide Notably, larger effect sizes were observed among younger children
important insight into how caregivers can prepare children for school. (aged 2–3 years old) suggesting that the home learning environment
The home learning environment refers to the quality and quantity may differentially influence child outcomes across different stages
of cognitively stimulating resources and activities that promote of development. These differential associations between the formal
children’s learning. Broad indices of the home learning environment and informal HLE with child outcomes bear implications for early
(including numeracy, literacy, painting, and drawing activities) have interventions that could be tailored according to children’s needs.
been consistently linked with children’s literacy and numeracy skills
(e.g., Melhuish et al., 2008; Niklas and Schneider, 2017). Although
several studies suggest that the home learning environment should
The Home Numeracy Environment
be operationalized as a global measure (e.g., Baker, 2015; Dearing The HNE refers to activities such as teaching numbers, shapes
et al., 2012; Melhuish et al., 2008), there is compelling evidence (LeFevre et al., 2010), spatial skills, measurement, and patterning
to indicate that children’s early learning environments may be better skills (e.g., Purpura et al., 2020) and may be similarly operationalized
understood in terms of the home literacy (HLE) and home numeracy in terms of formal and informal learning experiences. The formal
(HNE) environment (e.g., Anders et al., 2012; Napoli & Purpura, HNE may include teaching children to count, recognize numbers,
2018; Skwarchuk et al., 2014). and calculate simple sums. In contrast, the informal HNE may include
number games and numeracy-oriented activities that are inherent in
The Home Literacy Environment daily tasks such as cooking (Skwarchuk et al., 2014). Compared to
the HLE, findings regarding the formal and informal HNE have
The HLE refers to language-related cognitively stimulating activi- been less consistent (for a meta-analysis, see Daucourt et al., 2021).
ties and resources such as teaching letters, shared-book reading, and Formal aspects of the HNE such as frequently teaching children
provision of children’s books (Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2014). The simple sums at 4 years have been found to predict better performance
HLE may influence children’s academic outcomes through two dis- on arithmetic tasks and growth in nonsymbolic and symbolic number
tinct pathways (e.g., Sénéchal, 2006; Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2014). comparison at 5 years (Susperreguy et al., 2020). However, other stud-
First, children may be exposed to formal learning experiences. ies found no evidence for a statistically significant association between
Specifically, parents may engage in didactic behaviors such as teaching formal numeracy activities and children’s math skills (Blevins-Knabe
children to recognize letters and read words. Second, children may be et al., 2000). Similarly, formal numeracy activities such as counting
exposed to informal learning experiences. These include experiences and performing elementary calculations were not statistically signifi-
that are not explicitly oriented toward teaching, but nonetheless result cantly related to math skills among 5- and 6-year-old children (De
in children’s acquisition of literacy and numeracy skills. Such activities Keyser et al., 2020). These inconsistent findings may be attributed
may include shared reading and providing access to children’s books. to the differential operationalization of the formal HNE across studies
The distinction between formal and informal HLE may have prac- and the type of numeracy activities examined. For instance, a recent
tical implications given that these aspects of children’s early environ- systematic review of 37 studies focusing on the HNE found that com-
ments may promote different skills. For example, teaching literacy at plex, but not basic, formal HNE activities were associated with better
home at 2 years old has been linked with children’s emergent literacy numeracy skills in children (Mutaf Yildiz et al., 2018). This finding
at 5 years old whereas shared reading has been linked with children’s aligns with Vygotsky’s sociocultural learning theory which posits
vocabulary at 5 years old and reading development from 8 to 15 years that children’s skills develop during interactions with more experi-
old (Torppa et al., 2022). Similarly, teaching children to read during enced social partners. However, such interactions can only promote
preschool has been found to predict children’s emergent literacy skills children’s skill development if children’s interactional partners operate
during primary school whereas shared reading has been linked with within their zones of proximal development (ZPD) and appropriately
children’s oral language skills (Silinskas et al., 2020). However, challenge them to develop more complex skills. Thus, teaching chil-
both teaching children to read and engaging in shared reading during dren basic numeracy skills such as counting may promote numeracy
preschool have been linked with children’s early letter knowledge dur- skills in young children who are only beginning to encounter numer-
ing primary school (Silinskas et al., 2020). The formal HLE has also ical concepts. However, such activities may no longer be relevant to
been linked with children’s alphabet knowledge (Sénéchal, 2006) and older children who have already mastered basic numeracy skills and
reading performance throughout the early school years (Hood et al., are more likely to benefit from more complex numeracy activities.
2008; Sénéchal, 2006; Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2014). In contrast, the In addition to the formal HNE, the informal HNE, albeit less studied
informal HLE such as shared-book reading has been linked directly (Mutaf Yildiz et al., 2018), may also positively influence children’s
with children’s vocabulary knowledge at 6 years (Sénéchal, 2006; numeracy skills. In fact, some evidence suggests that the informal
Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2014) and indirectly (through vocabulary medi- HNE may show stronger links with children’s math skills than the for-
ation) with reading skills in second (Hood et al., 2008), third mal HNE (Dunst et al., 2017). It is likely that the casual nature of infor-
(Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2014), and fourth (Sénéchal, 2006) grade. mal numeracy activities may allow parents to introduce more complex
HOME LEARNING ENVIRONMENT AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE 3

mathematical concepts than they would feel comfortable teaching in a might be a stronger predictor of children’s language outcomes
formal environment (e.g., Daucourt et al., 2021). As such, parents may than the HLE (Napoli & Purpura, 2018). Arguably, this association
be more likely to operate within children’s ZPD and to scaffold child- stems from the complex verbal interactions inherent in numeracy
ren’s numeracy skills during informal numeracy activities. This activities. It is likely that parents invoke verbal explanations to scaf-
assumption is corroborated by findings that show that informal numer- fold children’s numeracy development, which in turn benefits child-
acy activities such as playing dice and number games at the start of kin- ren’s vocabulary (Napoli & Purpura, 2018). Math talk may also
dergarten are linked with children’s math skills (e.g., complex sums represent one form of decontextualized talk, which is a known pre-
and subtraction tasks, calculating coins, completing patterns, and solv- dictor of child language development (Ebert et al., 2020).
ing geometry tasks) both concurrently and at the end of first grade, con- Moreover, aspects of the HLE may influence children’s academic
trolling for children’s IQ (Niklas & Schneider, 2014). Playing number performance by fostering non-academic skills that are nevertheless
board games has been identified as a particularly strong predictor of necessary for optimal school performance. Specifically, early HLE
children’s numeracy skills (Benavides-Varela et al., 2016; Ramani & has been found to influence children’s socioemotional competencies
Siegler, 2011). In fact, playing board games with children, who have at 12 months (O’Farrelly et al., 2018), at 4 years (Bierman et al.,
no experience with such games at home, results in significant growth 2015), and at 8 years (Rose et al., 2018). From a Vygotskian perspec-
in children’s early numeracy skills (Ramani & Siegler, 2011). tive, practices such as shared-book reading provide an opportune
Similarly, engaging in art activities such as coloring and drawing infrastructure within which parents can expand the boundaries of
may positively contribute to children’s math skills. For instance, indi- children’s ZPD by encouraging them to undergo various social and
vidual differences in children’s drawing skills at 4 years old have been emotional experiences through the perspectives of book protagonists.
linked with children’s math ability at 12 years old (Malanchini et al., As a result, children may experience growth in their own socioemo-
2016). Arguably, art can represent a means through which children tional skills and engage in prosocial behavior such as cooperating
learn about patterns, shapes, sizes, and perspectives (Dooley et al., and comforting peers (e.g., Aram et al., 2017). Indeed, some evidence
2014). Moreover, giving children opportunities to play with modeling suggests that shared-book reading is directly linked with children’s
compounds such as “play-doh” can enhance children’s understanding socioemotional outcomes (Kozak & Recchia, 2019).
of conservation (Piaget, 1965), for instance, how specific quantities However, it is also plausible that the HLE influences children’s
can change shape (Dooley et al., 2014). socioemotional outcomes through language development. Shared-
It is noteworthy that although a distinction has been made book reading may promote children’s ability to articulate emotions,
between the formal and informal HNE there is no consensus regard- which in turn may benefit socioemotional development (Rose et al.,
ing how best to operationalize children’s early numeracy environ- 2018). In accordance with this hypothesis, some findings suggest
ments. While some evidence supports studying the HNE in terms that children’s language competence at 3 years predicts children’s
of formal and informal activities (e.g., Manolitsis et al., 2013), cooperation, self-regulation, and aggressiveness at 7 years (Rose
other findings favor a bifactor conceptualization consisting of one et al., 2016). Similarly, the HLE of 3-year-old German children
general numeracy factor, one direct and indirect numeracy factor was found to predict children’s language abilities 6 months later
and one spatial activities factor (Purpura et al., 2020). The various which subsequently predicted children’s socioemotional skills
ways in which the HNE has been measured may explain the variabil- (e.g., adequate display of shame and pride, ability to play alone
ity of research findings and highlight the need for further efforts and behavioral difficulties) as reported by teachers a further
toward the development of a robust HNE model. 6 months later (Wirth et al., 2022).
Similarly, the informal HNE may also contribute to children’s
Domain-Specific and Cross-Domain Effects of the HLE socioemotional development. Activities such as board games, card
and HNE games, and cooking all require children to follow instructions and
adhere to rules which theoretically may foster children’s social
Although there is wide support for domain-specific effects of the skills. Numeracy-related games in particular necessitate turn-taking
HLE and HNE (e.g., Lehrl et al., 2020), evidence is emerging to sug- and may evoke competitiveness thereby providing children with
gest cross-domain effects also. This is not surprising given that child- opportunities to experience failure and success and learn how to self-
ren’s literacy and numeracy skills are associated (Purpura et al., 2011). regulate and engage in prosocial behaviors.
That is, children who struggle with literacy skills tend to also have dif-
ficulties with numeracy skills and vice versa (Light & DeFries, 1995). The Present Study
It follows that promoting one domain of child development may have
transferable effects on other domains of development. This conjecture The present study examines timing-specific associations between
is supported by findings that show that formal literacy activities can the home learning environment at 3 years and children’s school per-
predict math skills just as well as formal numeracy activities formance at 5 and 9 years old, thereby capturing the time-period dur-
(Manolitsis et al., 2013). Likewise, the HLE, comprising formal and ing which children transition to formal schooling. The first research
informal activities, has been identified as a stronger predictor of aim is to examine domain-specific associations between the early
numerical skills than a measure of formal and informal numeracy home learning environment comprising both formal and informal
activities (Anders et al., 2012). It is likely that HLE activities such HLE and children’s language skills at 5 and 9 years and between
as shared reading may also involve enumerating items in books, iden- the formal and informal HNE and children’s numeracy skills at 5
tifying patterns, and assessing characteristics such as size and depth, and 9 years. The second research aim is to examine cross-domain
all of which may promote children’s numeracy development. associations between the formal and informal HLE and children’s
Cross-domain effects have also been documented with regard to numeracy and socioemotional skills and between the formal and
the HNE. For instance, some evidence suggests that the HNE informal HNE and children’s language and socioemotional skills
4 CONICA, NIXON, AND QUIGLEY

at 5 and 9 years. The study includes parent report, teacher report, and convened by the Department of Children and Youth Affairs. Full
direct assessments of child performance and therefore minimizes the details of the GUI study design and data collection methodology
possibility of common method variance. The study further includes have been described elsewhere (Quail et al., 2019). In brief,
relevant control variables such as family socioeconomic status, pri- 11,134 children born between December 2007 and May 2008,
mary caregiver education, child gender, child baseline language along with their primary and secondary caregivers were recruited
skills, preschool attendance, and whether English is the child’s from the Republic of Ireland Child Benefit Register (Quail et al.,
first language. 2019). Data collection occurred when children were aged 9 months,
This study hypothesizes that both the formal and informal HLE 3 years (n = 9,073), 5 years (n = 9,001), 7–8 years (n = 5,344), and
at 3 years will demonstrate direct domain-specific links with child- 9 years (n = 8,032). The present study uses data that were collected
ren’s language skills at 5 and 9 years (hypothesis 1a) and indirect when children were 3, 5, and 9 years old. Participants who did not
domain-specific links with children’s language skills at 9 years participate at all three time points were not included in this study
through language, numeracy, and socioemotional skills at 5 years (n = 1,891). Data were weighted to statistically adjust for systematic
(hypothesis 1b); both the formal and informal HNE at 3 years will nonresponse and attrition based on the most recent census (detailed
show direct domain-specific links with children’s numeracy skills data set of the entire population in Ireland carried out by the Central
at 5 and 9 years (hypothesis 2a) and indirect domain-specific asso- Statistics Office) and Child Benefit Register (a universal monthly
ciations with children’s numeracy skills at 9 years through language, social welfare payment to families with children in Ireland). This
numeracy, and socioemotional skills at 5 years (hypothesis 2b). statistical adjustment accounts for any differences (e.g., in terms of
Furthermore, based on evidence that formal and informal literacy education, social class) between the true population and sample
activities are important precursors of children’s language outcomes that participated in the study. Minimum information loss algorithm
(e.g., Sénéchal, 2006; Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2014) but also of socio- was used to generate the weighting scheme.
emotional development (e.g., Rose et al., 2018), and that numeracy In addition, 262 participants were excluded from the analysis due
activities are important not only for math-related outcomes but also to a diagnosed speech or language problem. The final sample con-
for language outcomes (e.g., Manolitsis et al., 2013), it is hypothe- sisted of 7,110 primary caregivers and their children. All primary
sized that both the formal and informal HLE will demonstrate direct caregivers were the child’s parents (98.5% = mothers; 1.5% =
cross-domain associations with children’s numeracy and socioemo- fathers). Participant demographic information is presented in Table 1.
tional skills at 5 and 9 years (hypothesis 3a) and indirect cross-
domain associations with children’s numeracy and socioemotional
skills at 9 years through academic competencies at 5 years (hypoth- Measures
esis 3b); both the formal and informal HNE will demonstrate direct The Home Learning Environment at 3 Years
cross-domain associations with children’s language skills at both 5
and 9 years; but only the informal HNE will show direct cross- The home learning environment was measured using parent report
domain links with children’s socioemotional skills at 5 and on a home learning environment scale (see Table S1 in the online
9 years (hypothesis 4a) as informal activities such as board and supplemental material). On a scale from 0 to 7 parents indicated
card games may promote social development through teaching chil- the number of days per week during which they engaged in cogni-
dren self-regulation, rule-following, and turn-taking skills. Given the tively stimulating activities with children. In addition, parents
lack of theoretical and empirical evidence, the formal HNE is not reported the number of children’s books present in the home.
expected to relate to children’s socioemotional competencies. Formal Home Literacy Environment. Two items from the
Lastly, the study hypothesizes that: both the formal and informal home learning environment scale, namely the frequency with
HNE will show indirect cross-domain associations with children’s which parents taught children the alphabet and songs, poems, or
language and socioemotional skills at 9 years through academic rhymes, were used to create a latent formal HLE variable within a
competencies at 5 years (hypothesis 4b). structural equation modeling (SEM) framework. Factor loadings
were 0.608 and 0.590, respectively (comparative fit index [CFI] =
Method 0.986, Tucker-Lewis index [TLI] = 0.957, root mean square error
of approximation [RMSEA] = 0.030, standardized root mean square
Transparency and Openness residual [SRMR] = 0.018).
We report how we determined our sample size, all data exclusions, Informal Home Literacy Environment. Two items from the
all manipulations, and all measures in the study, and we follow home learning environment scale, namely the frequency with
Journal Article Reporting Standards (Kazak & Recchia, 2019). which parents read to children and the number of books present in
As per Irish Social Science Data Archive (ISSDA) regulations, the home were used as indicators of the informal HLE. The number
data cannot be made publicly available and may only be accessed of books was categorized as a dichotomous variable which assessed
by researchers indicated on the Growing Up in Ireland (GUI) data whether participants had less or more than 30 children’s books in the
request form. Data were analyzed using STATA, Version 17. This house.
study’s design and its analysis were not preregistered. Formal Home Numeracy Environment. One item from the
home learning environment scale, namely the frequency with
Participants which parents taught children numbers and counting, was used as
an index of children’s formal HNE.
Participants in the present study are part of the GUI study, a large Informal Home Numeracy Environment. Two items from the
representative cohort study of children in Ireland. Ethical approval home learning environment scale, namely the frequency with which
for this study was obtained from the Research Ethics Committee parents played games (e.g., board games, card games, jigsaw
HOME LEARNING ENVIRONMENT AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE 5

Table 1 less) to 9 with higher scores indicating better pragmatic language


Descriptive Statistics of Demographic Variables at 3 Years skills.
Literacy and Numeracy Skills. Children’s numeracy and liter-
Variable M (SD) or % N
acy skills were assessed via teacher report on the Achievement
Primary caregiver age Scales from the U.K. Millennium Cohort Study. With regards to
Years 30.16 (5.79) 7,110 numeracy, teachers responded to nine “yes” or “no” questions that
Marital status
Married/cohabiting 75.1% 5,339 included items such as “Uses developing mathematical ideas and
Married and separated 2.2% 155 methods to solve practical problems” and “Recognises, counts,
Divorced/widowed 1.3% 89 orders, writes, and uses numbers up to 20.” The nine items were
Never married 20.8% 1,481 combined to create an overall “Numeracy” score (α = .76). With
Unknown 1.2% 92
Primary caregiver education
regards to literacy, teacher report on two scales composed of nine
No qualification 20.6% 1,464 items, namely “Linking Sounds and Letters” and “Child Reading”
Technical/vocational qualification 14.3% 1,025 were combined to form a latent literacy score. Items for “Linking
Nondegree 22.8% 1,624 Sounds and Letters” included “Uses phonic knowledge to read sim-
University degree 24.2% 1,720 ple and regular words,” “Attempts to read more complex words,
Postgraduate 17.4% 1,199
Doctorate 1.1% 78 using phonic knowledge,” and “Uses knowledge of letters, sounds
Household socioeconomic status and words when reading and writing independently” (α = .85).
Professional 19% 1,348 Items for “Child Reading” included “Shows an understanding of
Managerial 34.4% 2,445 the elements of stories, such as main character, sequence of events,
Nonmanual 16.6% 1,180
Skilled/unskilled manual 8.8% 629
and openings,” “Shows an understanding of how information can be
Never worked/unknown 7.8 553 found in nonfiction texts to answer questions about where, who,
Child sex why, and how,” and “Reads books of own choice with some fluency
Male 49.4% 3,510 and accuracy” (α = .77). Scores ranged from 3 (indicating 3 or less)
Language to 9 with higher scores indicating better literacy and numeracy
English not first language 7.3% 519
Preschool attendance skills. Factor loadings for linking sounds and letters and child read-
Yes 68.7% 4,888 ing were 0.805 and 0.877, respectively (CFI = 0.986, TLI = 0.957,
No 2.8% 200 RMSEA = 0.030, SRMR = 0.018).
Missing 28.4% 2,022 Socioemotional Skills. Children’s prosocial behavior was
Primary caregiver ethnicity
Irish 84.4% 6,002
obtained from teacher ratings on the Prosocial Behavior subscale
Other White 10.8% 765 from the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ;
African or other Black 2.3% 164 Goodman, 1997). The subscale consists of five questions regarding
Chinese or other Asian 2% 145 the child’s prosocial behavior over the past 6 months and is rated on
Other (including mixed) 0.5% 32 a 3-point scale ranging from 0 (not true) to 2 (certainly true). Higher
scores indicate greater levels of prosocial behavior (α = .81).
puzzles, etc.) and engaged in art activities (e.g., painting, drawing,
etc.), were used to create a latent informal HNE variable. Factor Academic Competencies at 9 Years
loadings were 0.595 and 0.502, respectively (CFI = 0.986, TLI =
0.957, RMSEA = 0.030, SRMR = 0.018). Vocabulary. Children’s vocabulary was directly assessed by an
interviewer using the Drumcondra Primary Reading Test–Revised
Academic Competencies at 5 Years (DPRT-R). The DPRT-R is a standardized test administered to chil-
dren in Ireland in second (8–9 years old), fourth (10–11 years old),
Vocabulary. Children’s expressive vocabulary was assessed and sixth (12–13 years old) class annually. The test consists of 40
directly by an interviewer in the home using the Naming multiple-choice items aimed at assessing children’s understanding
Vocabulary scale of the British Abilities Scales (Elliott et al., of word meanings. Sample items include asking children to select
1996). Children were shown pictures of various objects and asked the corresponding synonym for the underlined word in “The chil-
to verbally name them. Assessment continued until a child erred dren reached the end of the road.” Response options include “A.
on five consecutive trials. Higher scores indicate more developed crossed,” “B. arrived at,” “C. looked for,” and “D. saw.” Each cor-
vocabularies. rect answer is awarded a score of “1” and all final scores are summed
Pragmatic Language Skills. Children’s pragmatic language to provide an overall score of child vocabulary. The DPRT-R has
skills were assessed via teacher report on the Achievement Scales high reliability with KR20 = 0.93. The vocabulary measure demon-
from the U.K. Millennium Cohort Study. Specifically, teachers strated modest test–retest reliability, r(7, 108) = 0.382, p , .001.
responded to nine “yes” or “no” questions that included items Literacy and Numeracy Skills. Children’s literacy and numer-
such as “Initiates communication with others, displaying greater acy skills were assessed via teacher report of children’s competence
confidence in more informal contexts,” “Uses language to imagine relative to developmental counterparts at 9 years. Reading and writing
and to recreate roles and experiences,” and “Uses talk to organise, scores were used to create a latent literacy skills variable. Factor load-
sequence and clarify thinking, ideas, feelings, and events; explores ings for reading and writing in English were 0.856 and 0.845, respec-
the meanings and sounds of new words.” The nine items were com- tively (CFI = 0.986, TLI = 0.957, RMSEA = 0.030, SRMR =
bined to create an overall “Language for Communication and 0.018). Test–retest reliabilities for literacy and numeracy were, r(7,
Thinking” score (α = .88). Scores ranged from 3 (indicating 3 or 108) = 0.733, p , .001, and r(7, 108) = .745, p , .001, respectively.
6 CONICA, NIXON, AND QUIGLEY

Socioemotional Skills. Children’s prosocial behavior was participants who did not have data at all three timepoints were
obtained from teacher ratings on the five-item Prosocial Behavior more likely to be of lower socioeconomic class, χ2(4) = 15.956,
subscale from the SDQ (α = .80; Goodman, 1997), p = .026, and less likely to have children enrolled in preschool edu-
r(7,108) = .277, p , .001. cation, χ2(1) = 8.391, p = .004. Full Information Maximum
Likelihood (FIML) was used to estimate the missing values via
Covariates the “mlmv” command in STATA.

The study also adjusted for a number of covariates including fam- Results
ily socioeconomic status, primary caregiver education, child sex,
child baseline language skills (measured using the Naming Descriptive statistics are presented in Table S2 in the online
Vocabulary subscale of the British Abilities Scale; Elliott et al., supplemental material. Pearson’s correlations between all study
1996), preschool attendance, child non-singleton status as well as variables are presented in Tables S3 and S4 in the online supplemen-
whether English was the child’s first language. tary material.

Statistical Approach Domain-Specific Associations Between the Formal and


Informal HLE and HNE and Child Academic
First data were analyzed for normality. Results revealed skewness
and kurtosis values within + 2 for all study variables, indicating Competencies at 5 and 9 Years Old
normal univariate distribution (George & Mallery, 2010). Second, Factor loadings for latent variables and direct effects from the for-
a series of descriptive and correlational analyses were performed mal and informal HLE and HNE to child competencies at 5 and
using SPSS 26. Next, a SEM was modeled to address the research 9 years are presented in Table 1. Similarly, indirect effects from
questions (see Figure S1 in the online supplemental material). the formal and informal HLE and HNE to child competencies at
The model controlled for family socioeconomic status, primary 9 years through child competencies at 5 years are presented in
caregiver education, child gender, child baseline language skills, Table 2.
preschool attendance, child non-singleton status as well as whether
English was the child’s first language. Correlations of residual vari- Formal and Informal HLE
ances of all academic competencies at 5 and 9 years were also per-
formed to account for overlap between variables. Indirect paths were Results indicate that the formal HLE did not show significant
estimated post hoc using the Karlson-Holm-Breen (KHB) command domain-specific associations with children’s literacy skills at either
(Karlson & Holm, 2011). The KHB command disentangles each 5 or 9 years.
mediator’s contribution to the indirect effect, net of covariates. All In contrast, with regards to the informal HLE, shared reading
indirect paths were tested, regardless of whether there was a signifi- showed significant positive direct domain-specific associations
cant main effect to allow potential suppressor effects to be identified. with children’s literacy (β = 0.077, p = .003) and pragmatic lan-
Model fit was assessed using Hu and Bentler’s (2009) criteria guage (β = 0.060, p = .008), whereas the number of books in the
including RMSEA , 0.05, SRMR , 0.08, and CFI and TLI values house was linked with children’s vocabulary (β = 0.052, p = .018)
.0.90. Given the multiple comparisons performed, to minimize at 5 years old. Similarly, shared reading showed significant positive
both Type I and Type II error rates, Lakens et al.’s (2018) approach direct associations with children’s literacy skills (β = 0.051,
was employed to calculate an α level of .02. All paths were conse- p = .012) and vocabulary (β = 0.056, p = .001), whereas number
quently tested against this new p value. of books was directly positively related only with child vocabulary
(β = 0.052, p = .008) at 9 years old (hypothesis 1a).
Missing Data In addition, shared reading showed significant positive indirect
associations with children’s literacy skills at 9 years (β = 0.025,
As is common in longitudinal research, there was some missing p = .001) through literacy skills (β = 0.09, p , .001), pragmatic lan-
data due to attrition and nonresponse. Analyses of missingness pat- guage (β = 0.004, p , .001) and numeracy skills (β = 0.005,
terns using Little’s MCAR test indicated that data were missing p , .001) at 5 years. As illustrated in Table 2, literacy skills
completely at random (MCAR), χ2(9) = 9.323, p = .408. Given explained 29.28% of the indirect effect, pragmatic language
that complete case analysis is an appropriate and robust approach explained 1.48%, and numeracy skills explained 14.68%. There
for data that is MCAR (Von Hippel, 2007), only participants who were significant positive indirect associations between the informal
took part in the study at 3, 5, and 9 years old were included. HLE and children’s vocabulary at 9 years (β = 0.027, p = .002)
Among these, at 3 years old, there were 0.10% missing cases for through literacy skills (β = 0.018, p , .001), pragmatic language
the HLE. At 5 years old, there were 0.60% missing cases for child (β = 0.008, p , .001), and numeracy skills (β = 0.008, p , .001)
vocabulary, 6.00% missing cases for pragmatic language, 6.50% at 5 years. Literacy skills explained 37.94% of the indirect effect,
missing cases for numeracy, 17.50% missing cases for literacy, pragmatic language explained 4.26%, and numeracy skills explained
and 5.70% missing cases for socioemotional skills. At 9 years old, 12.15% of the indirect effect (hypothesis 1b).
there were 2.50% missing cases for vocabulary, 9.10% missing
cases for numeracy, 8.90% missing cases for literacy, and 8.50% Formal and Informal HNE
missing cases for socioemotional skills.
There were no differences between participants whose data were There was no evidence for statistically significant domain-specific
and whose were not available at all three timepoints in terms of pri- associations between either the formal or informal HNE and children’s
mary caregiver education, χ2(5) = 14.319, p = .216. However, numeracy skills at either 5 or 9 years (see Table 2; hypothesis 2a).
HOME LEARNING ENVIRONMENT AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE 7

Table 2
Standardized β Coefficients for Direct and Indirect Effects From the Formal and Informal HLE and HNE at 3 Years to Child Competencies at
5 and 9 Years
Latent variable factor loadings β p
ABCs → formal HLE 0.608 ,.001
Songs, poems, rhymes → formal HLE 0.590 ,.001
Board games → informal HNE 0.595 ,.001
Art → informal HNE 0.502 ,.001
Linking sounds and letters at 5 years → literacy skills at 5 years 0.804 ,.001
Child Reading at 5 years → literacy skills at 5 years 0.877 ,.001
Reading in English at 9 years → literacy skills at 9 years 0.856 ,.001
Writing in English at 9 years → literacy skills at 9 years 0.845 ,.001
Direct effects β p 95% bootstrapped CIs R2
Academic competencies at 5 years
Literacy skills .126
Formal HLE → literacy skills at 5 years −0.235 .039 [−0.599, −0.076]
Informal HLE → literacy skills at 5 years 0.077 .003 [−0.022, −0.080]
Number of books → literacy skills at 5 years −0.002 .945 [−0.151, 0.030]
Formal HNE → literacy skills at 5 years 0.098 .114 [0.008, 0.169]
Informal HNE → literacy skills at 5 years 0.209 .011 [0.008, 0.387]
Vocabulary .285
Formal HLE → vocabulary at 5 years 0.014 .850 [−1.434, 2.026]
Informal HLE → vocabulary at 5 years 0.016 .429 [−0.104, 0.452]
Number of books → vocabulary at 5 years 0.052 .018 [0.087, 1.44]
Formal HNE → vocabulary at 5 years 0.022 .549 [−0.320, 0.719]
Informal HNE → vocabulary at 5 years 0.024 .643 [−0.626, 1.756]
Pragmatic language .097
Formal HLE → pragmatic language at 5 years −0.160 .144 [−0.785, −0.066]
Informal HLE → pragmatic language at 5 years 0.060 .008 [0.019, 0.104]
Number of books → pragmatic language at 5 years −0.009 .740 [−0.140, 0.102]
Formal HNE → pragmatic language at 5 years 0.050 .362 [−0.013, 0.187]
Informal HNE → pragmatic language at 5 years 0.190 .017 [0.136, 0.629]
Socioemotional skills .048
Formal HLE → socioemotional skills at 5 years 0.005 .954 [−0.279, 0.283]
Informal HLE → socioemotional skills at 5 years −0.002 .940 [−0.051, 0.048]
Number of books → socioemotional skills at 5 years 0.020 .389 [−0.060, 0.115]
Informal HNE → socioemotional skills at 5 years 0.032 .583 [−0.106, 0.239]
Numeracy Skills .057
Formal HLE → numeracy skills at 5 years −0.110 .290 [−0.479, 0.014]
Informal HLE → numeracy skills at 5 years 0.074 .003 [−0.132, 0.034]
Number of books → numeracy skills at 5 years 0.001 .981 [−0.019, 0.055]
Formal HNE → numeracy skills at 5 years 0.054 .305 [−0.018, 0.118]
Informal HNE → numeracy skills at 5 years 0.105 .186 [0.037, 0.318]
Academic competencies at 9 years
Literacy skills .208
Formal HLE → literacy skills at 9 years −0.107 .429 [−0.229, 0.037]
Informal HLE → literacy skills at 9 years 0.051 .012 [0.003, 0.023]
Number of books → literacy skills at 9 years 0.035 .107 [−0.001, 0.032]
Formal HNE → literacy skills at 9 years 0.046 .515 [−0.012, 0.064]
Informal HNE → literacy skills at 9 years 0.088 .346 [−0.007, 0.139]
Vocabulary .301
Formal HLE → vocabulary at 9 years −0.080 .454 [−0.375, 0.071]
Informal HLE → vocabulary at 9 years 0.056 .001 [0.016, 0.043]
Number of books → vocabulary at 9 years 0.052 .008 [0.006, 0.073]
Formal HNE → vocabulary at 9 years 0.022 .688 [−0.023, 0.105]
Informal HNE → vocabulary at 9 years 0.027 .686 [−0.059, 0.103]
Socioemotional skills .122
Formal HLE → socioemotional skills at 9 years −0.011 .914 [−0.347, 0.323]
Informal HLE → socioemotional skills at 9 years −0.011 .605 [−0.037, 0.048]
Number of books → socioemotional skills at 9 years −0.006 .812 [−0.144, 0.061]
Informal HNE → socioemotional skills at 9 years 0.035 .626 [−0.151, 0.206]
Numeracy skills .137
Formal HLE → numeracy skills at 9 years −0.141 .139 [−0.178, 0.010]
Informal HLE → numeracy skills at 9 years 0.030 .126 [−0.009, 0.014]
Number of books → numeracy skills at 9 years 0.012 .545 [−0.007, 0.031]
Formal HNE → numeracy skills at 9 years 0.066 .191 [0.004, 0.051]
Informal HNE → numeracy skills at 9 years 0.085 .226 [−0.001, 0.120]
(table continues)
8 CONICA, NIXON, AND QUIGLEY

Table 2 (continued)

Proportion of
effect
Indirect effects β p 95% bootstrapped CIs R2 explained
Literacy skills
Indirect effect from formal HLE to literacy skills at 9 years 0.009 .065 [−0.001, 0.017]
Indirect effect from informal HLE to literacy skills at 9 years 0.025 .001 [0.018, 0.029] .290
Informal HLE → literacy skills at 5 years → literacy skills at 9 years 0.009 ,.001 [0.006, 0.011] 29.28%
Informal HLE → pragmatic language at 5 years → literacy skills at 9 years 0.004 ,.001 [0.003, 0.006] 1.48%
Informal HLE → numeracy skills at 5 years → literacy skills at 9 years 0.005 ,.001 [0.003, 0.007] 14.68%
Indirect effect from number of books to literacy skills at 9 years 0.004 .623 [−0.005, 0.009]
Indirect effect from formal HNE to literacy skills at 9 years 0.029 .104 [−0.001, 0.014]
Indirect effect from informal HNE to literacy skills at 9 years 0.063 .005 [0.045, 0.064] .260
Informal HNE → literacy skills at 5 years → literacy skills at 9 years 0.023 ,.001 [0.019, 0.027] 42.72%
Vocabulary
Indirect effect from formal HLE to vocabulary at 9 years −0.064 .089 [−0.108, 0.008]
Indirect effect from informal HLE to vocabulary at 9 years 0.027 .002 [0.033, 0.058] .290
Informal HLE → literacy skills at 5 years → vocabulary at 9 years 0.018 ,.001 [0.005, 0.011] 37.94%
Informal HLE → pragmatic language at 5 years → vocabulary at 9 years 0.008 ,.001 [0.005, 0.011] 4.26%
Informal HLE → numeracy skills at 5 years → vocabulary at 9 years 0.008 ,.001 [0.005, 0.012] 12.15%
Indirect effect from number of books to vocabulary at 9 years 0.010 .288 [−0.008, 0.027]
Indirect effect from formal HNE to vocabulary at 9 years 0.034 .119 [−0.004, 0.036]
Indirect effect from informal HNE to vocabulary at 9 years 0.074 .015 [0.051, 0.096] .280
Informal HNE → literacy skills at 5 years → vocabulary at 9 years 0.052 ,.001 [0.043, 0.063] 70.28%
Socioemotional skills
Indirect effect from formal HLE to socioemotional skills at 9 years −0.007 .726 [−0.078, 0.055]
Indirect effect from informal HLE to socioemotional skills at 9 years 0.003 .660 [−0.009, 0.015]
Indirect effect from number of books to socioemotional skills at 9 years 0.004 .507 [−0.015, 0.029]
Indirect effect from formal HNE to socioemotional skills at 9 years 0.001 .959 [−0.021, 0.022]
Indirect effect from informal HNE to socioemotional skills at 9 years 0.016 .240 [−0.016, 0.055]
Numeracy skills
Indirect effect from formal HLE to numeracy skills at 9 years −0.044 .069 [−0.040, 0.002]
Indirect effect from informal HLE to numeracy skills at 9 years 0.020 .001 [0.008, 0.023] .180
Informal HLE → literacy skills at 5 years → numeracy skills at 9 years 0.008 ,.001 [0.006, 0.101] 38.35%
Informal HLE → pragmatic language at 5 years → numeracy skills at 9 years 0.004 ,.001 [0.002, 0.005] 1.08%
Informal HLE → numeracy skills at 5 years → numeracy skills at 9 years 0.007 ,.001 [0.004, 0.009] 34.08%
Indirect effect from number of books to numeracy skills at 9 years 0.003 .641 [−0.005, 0.008]
Indirect effect from formal HNE to numeracy skills at 9 years 0.021 .125 [−0.002, 0.012]
Indirect effect from informal HNE to numeracy skills at 9 years 0.044 .014 [0.028, 0.056] .140
Informal HNE → literacy skills at 5 years → numeracy skills at 9 years 0.021 ,.001 [0.016, 0.025] 49.80%
Goodness-of-fit indices
RMSEA 0.030
CFI 0.986
TLI 0.957
SRMR 0.018
Note. For indirect effects, only child competencies at 5 years that emerged as significant mediators are reported. HLE = home literacy; HNE = home
numeracy; ABCs = the letters of the alphabet. RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis
index; SRMR = standardized root mean square residual.

However, the informal HNE was indirectly associated with child- There was no evidence for significant cross-domain direct associa-
ren’s numeracy skills at 9 years (β = 0.044, p = .014) through liter- tions between the informal HLE and children’s socioemotional and
acy skills (β = 0.021, p , .001). Literacy skills explained 49.80% of numeracy skills at either 5 or 9 years (hypothesis 3a). However, the
the indirect effect (hypothesis 2b). informal HLE showed significant positive indirect cross-domain asso-
ciations with children’s numeracy skills at 9 years (β = 0.020,
Cross-Domain Associations Between the Formal and p = .001) through literacy skills (β = 0.008, p , .001 pragmatic lan-
Informal HLE and HNE and Child Academic and guage (β = 0.004, p , .001) and numeracy skills (β = 0.007,
Socioemotional Competencies at 5 and 9 Years Old p , .001) skills. Literacy skills explained 38.35%, pragmatic language
explained 1.08%, and numeracy skills explained 34.08% of the indirect
Formal and Informal HLE effect (hypothesis 3b).
The formal HLE did not show any statistically significant cross-
domain associations with children’s numeracy or socioemotional Formal and Informal HNE
skills at either 5 or 9 years. In contrast, the informal HLE showed
significant cross-domain links with children’s numeracy skills There was no evidence for statistically significant cross-
(β = 0.074, p = .003) at 5 years old. domain associations between the formal HNE and children’s
HOME LEARNING ENVIRONMENT AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE 9

language or socioemotional skills at either 5 or 9 years the formal and informal HLE and HNE at 3 years and child academic
(see Table 2). performance at 5 and 9 years. The second research aim was to exam-
Conversely, the informal HNE was significantly positively asso- ine cross-domain effects between the formal and informal HLE and
ciated with children’s literacy skills (β = 0.209, p = .011) and prag- HNE at 3 years and children’s academic and socioemotional compe-
matic language (β = 0.190, p = .017) at 5 years. There were no tencies at 5 and 9 years.
significant direct cross-domain associations between the informal
HNE and any child outcomes at 9 years (hypothesis 4a). Domain-Specific Associations Between the HLE and
However, there was evidence for significant positive indirect Children’s Academic Competencies
cross-domain links between the informal HNE and children’s liter-
acy skills at 9 years (β = 0.063, p = .005) through literacy skills at Research has clearly highlighted the importance of the HLE dur-
5 years (β = 0.023, p , .001) which explained 42.72% of the indi- ing the preschool period (see Niklas et al., 2021 for an editorial).
rect effect. Similarly, the informal HNE was indirectly associated Participation in literacy and language activities (e.g., reading, recit-
with children’s vocabulary at 9 years (β = 0.074, p = .015) through ing rhymes, access to books) with parents has been found to predict
literacy skills (β = 0.052, p , .001) at 5 years. Literacy skills 4-year-olds’ print knowledge and interest in book reading 1 year
explained 70.28% of the observed effect (hypothesis 4b). The later (Weigel et al., 2006). Similarly, among 6-year-olds’ formal lit-
final model is presented in Figure 1. eracy activities (e.g., actively teaching children to read and write)
predicted children’s reading competence and reading motivation
Discussion (Yeo et al., 2014). Paradoxically, in this study, the formal HLE
did not show any significant associations with children’s literacy
Children’s early academic competencies largely influence their skills at either 5 or 9 years. One possible explanation for this null
academic trajectories (Duncan et al., 2007; LeFevre et al., 2010). result may be the complexity of the formal HLE activities assessed
Children who begin school with poorly developed literacy and at 3 years. That is, while teaching children the alphabet and singing
numeracy skills are likely to lag behind throughout their songs and rhymes might benefit toddlers who are less sophisticated
academic journey (e.g., Hooper et al., 2010; Jordan et al., 2007). language users, such activities may have limited influence on older
Accordingly, the present study aimed to explore how early aspects children who can arguably read and write from the age of 5 onwards.
of children’s home learning environment may foster their academic It is therefore of relevance for future research to examine not only
competencies during the first years of school through two research associations between the early HLE and child outcomes but also
aims. First the study examined domain-specific effects between stability and change in the HLE over time. Such an approach can

Figure 1
Pathways From the Formal and Informal HLE and HNE at 3 Years to Child Academic Competencies at 5 and 9 Years

Note. Only significant direct and indirect paths from the formal and informal HLE and HNE are illustrated. Solid lines depict significant direct paths, solid bold
lines indicate significant indirect paths from the informal HLE (through child competencies at 5 years), and dashed lines indicate significant indirect paths from
the informal HNE (through child competencies at 5 years). The model adjusts for covariates. HLE = home literacy; HNE = home numeracy.
10 CONICA, NIXON, AND QUIGLEY

provide important insights into the types of HLE activities that most engage in spatial reasoning, and perform calculations to manage
salient to children at different stages of development. resources, all of which are likely to promote the development of
In contrast, the informal HLE demonstrated positive domain- numeracy skills. Similarly, children’s math skills may grow as
specific associations with children’s language skills at both 5 and they explore patterns, shapes, sizes, and perspectives through art
9 years thereby partially supporting the first hypothesis. (e.g., Dooley et al., 2014). In addition, given that both math and
Specifically, the number of children’s books at 3 years was associ- art depend on patterns and relationships between objects, this aspect
ated with children’s vocabulary whereas shared-book reading was of the informal HNE may represent a useful activity that could foster
associated with literacy skills and pragmatic language at 5 years. It children’s early numeracy skills.
is likely that shared reading represents interactive episodes whereby It is noteworthy that although the informal HNE showed signifi-
children actively participate in, rather than passively listen to, the cant domain-specific relations with children’s numeracy skills at
book narrative. Thus, shared reading may offer children the opportu- 9 years, there was no evidence for a significant association at
nity to experience turn-taking, to ask and answer questions, and to 5 years. This finding may be attributable to limitations with regards
explore various perspectives of book characters. These experiences to the measures used to assess numeracy skills. Although in this
in turn appear beneficial for children’s development of vocabulary study children’s numeracy was assessed via teacher report of child
and language for thinking and communication during the preschool numerical competence, it is likely that a more comprehensive assess-
period. ment including children’s problem-solving abilities, mathematical
Moreover, shared reading showed indirect links with children’s language, and spatial awareness (e.g., Purpura et al., 2020) may pro-
literacy skills and vocabulary at 9 years through pragmatic language, vide greater insight regarding the informal HNE and children’s
literacy, and numeracy skills at 5 years. In addition, both shared numeracy development.
reading and number of children’s books at 3 years were directly Similarly, a more comprehensive measure of the formal HNE,
related to children’s literacy skills and vocabulary at 9 years inde- beyond counting, may better capture the link between the early
pendent of children’s prior skills. It is likely that exposure to HNE and children’s numeracy skills.
books and shared reading includes activities such as letter recogni- It may be that activities such as teaching children sums, division,
tion, rhyming, and defining new words, all of which may promote and problem-solving may push children to invoke the upper limits of
children’s language outcomes. These findings align with previous their numerical abilities to a greater degree than counting and may
research which similarly found shared reading to be an important therefore better explain potential relations between the formal
precursor of children’s vocabulary at 6 years (Sénéchal, 2006; HNE and children’s numeracy skills.
Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2014) and of children’s reading skills in sec-
ond (Hood et al., 2008), third (Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2014) and Cross-Domain Associations Between the HLE and
fourth (Sénéchal, 2006) grade. Children’s Academic and Socioemotional Competencies
The nonsignificant associations between the formal HLE and
children’s language skills at 9 years highlight an important limita- Although the link between the HLE and children’s language out-
tion of the present study, namely that aspects of the HLE were comes has been established (e.g., Kumschick et al., 2014; Mol et al.,
only assessed when children were aged 3. Consistent with 2008; Sénéchal, 2015; Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2014), growing evi-
Vygotsky’s sociocultural learning theory, teaching children the dence suggests that the influence of the early HLE may also extend
alphabet and songs, poems, and rhymes during toddlerhood may to children’s socioemotional development (Kumschick et al., 2014).
operate within children’s ZPD and promote early language skills. However, findings have been less consistent among younger
However, such formal literacy activities may no longer challenge populations.
preschoolers who may benefit from more sophisticated aspects of Some studies suggest that the HLE can influence socioemotional
the formal HLE such as actively learning how to read and write competencies in children as young as 12-month-olds (O’Farrelly et
(e.g., Silinskas et al., 2020). al., 2018). However, other findings indicate that among 2–
4-year-olds, shared-book reading and a global measure of the HLE
Domain-Specific Associations Between the HNE and (e.g., frequency of reading, number of children’s books, library vis-
Children’s Academic Competencies its) are not meaningful predictors of child socioemotional outcomes
(Wirth et al., 2022). Similarly, in the present study, neither the for-
Existing research suggests that both the formal and informal HNE mal nor the informal HLE showed significant associations with
are meaningfully, albeit differentially, related with child outcomes children’s socioemotional outcomes at either 5 or 9 years.
(e.g., Skwarchuk et al., 2014; Zippert & Rittle-Johnson, 2020). In Therefore, the hypotheses that the formal and informal HLE
the present study, only the informal HNE was significantly linked would show cross-domain links with children’s socioemotional
with children’s numeracy skills at 9 years through literacy skills at skills were not supported. One possible explanation for these results
5 years. Therefore, the hypotheses that both formal and informal is that socioemotional competencies were defined only in terms of
HNE would demonstrate domain-specific associations with child- teacher-reported prosocial behavior and this measure may limit the
ren’s numeracy skills were only partially supported. This finding extent to which children’s socioemotional skills were captured.
is in concert with previous claims that informal aspects of the Socioemotional skills extend beyond prosociality and include abili-
HNE, such as playing board games and engaging in art activities ties to adapt to different settings, establish boundaries and display
may play a positive role in children’s numeracy development appropriate emotional responses such as shame and pride.
(Benavides-Varela et al., 2016; Malanchini et al., 2016; Ramani & Therefore, more comprehensive assessments alongside observa-
Siegler, 2011). Playing numerical games may require children to tional measures might provide more accurate insight into children’s
count, engage in iterative problem-solving, recognize patterns, socioemotional competencies (e.g., Wirth et al., 2022).
HOME LEARNING ENVIRONMENT AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE 11

In addition, it has been suggested that the HLE is not only impor- sizes providing some support for prior claims that the HNE might be
tant for children’s language and socioemotional development but a stronger predictor of children’s language outcomes than the HLE
may also relate to children’s numeracy skills. For instance, formal lit- (Napoli & Purpura, 2018). Moreover, the informal HNE also showed
eracy activities have been reported to impact children’s numeracy indirect relations with children’s literacy skills and vocabulary at
skills to a similar degree as formal numeracy activities (Manolistis 9 years through literacy skills at 5 years. These findings partially
et al., 2013). There have also been suggestions that formal and infor- support the hypothesis suggesting cross-domain links between the
mal aspects of the HLE are stronger predictors of children’s numer- HNE and child language outcomes. It is possible that engaging in
acy skills than formal and informal aspects of the HNE (Anders et informal numeracy activities such as playing card and board
al., 2012). games promotes language skills in children such as initiating conver-
Similarly, in this study the informal, but not the formal, HLE was sations with others and turn-taking, using language to imagine and
significantly positively directly linked with children’s numeracy recreate experiences and using talk to organize feelings, clarify
skills at 5 years and indirectly with numeracy skills at 9 years ideas, and explore meanings of new words. The lack of such an asso-
through pragmatic language, literacy, and numeracy skills at ciation among younger children (e.g., Kartushina et al., 2021) fur-
5 years. These results partially support the hypothesis that the formal ther highlights that different aspects of the home learning
and informal HLE would show cross-domain links with children’s environment may differentially relate to child outcomes across
numeracy skills. This finding echoes previous research whereby stages of development. These findings bear important implications
the number of books in the home, and the frequency of shared read- for the development of developmentally appropriate interventions
ing was associated with children’s math performance during the first especially given prior suggestions that numeracy interventions
months of kindergarten (Lahaie, 2008). It is likely, that in contrast to may be beneficial to children’s reading (Sarama et al., 2012).
formal literacy activities, reading, like math activities, requires chil- It is possible that the effects of the informal HNE may extend
dren to retrieve associations between symbolic and phonological beyond numeracy and language performance and also promote soci-
concepts and therefore benefits both language and numeracy skills. oemotional development. For example, playing board games and
Alternatively, given that children’s language skills at 5 years card games may encourage children to adhere to social norms,
mediated the association between the informal HLE and engage in self-directed speech for self-regulation (e.g., Prizant &
numeracy skills at 9 years, it may be that children with more devel- Wetherby, 1990) and interact appropriately with peers. Similarly,
oped language skills may be better equipped to participate in informal HNE activities may teach children to follow rules, inhibit
numeracy-related activities during preschool and as a result display strong impulses and remain on task, all of which are necessary for
better math performance during the early school years. These find- successful social functioning in an academic environment.
ings corroborate the claim that while early numeracy skills are However, in this study, there was no evidence for a statistically sig-
important for later numeracy skills, language skills are also crucial nificant association between the informal HLE and children’s socio-
(e.g., Yang et al., 2022). It is known that young children possess emotional development. Thus, the hypothesis regarding the HNE
the ability to distinguish broad quantities (e.g., much, little), and children’s socioemotional skills was not supported. As dis-
which later facilitates children’s higher level numeracy skills cussed above, this finding might be attributed to how socioemotional
(Butterworth, 2005). Language however allows children to link skills were operationalized in this study and it is likely that observa-
number words with quantities and therefore progress from a basic tional measures might yield different results.
understanding of numeracy (e.g., 50 equates much) to a more refined Overall, only the informal HLE and HNE demonstrated positive
one (e.g., 50 equates fifty). Furthermore, informal HLE activities domain-specific effects and cross-domain effects with children’s
such as shared reading may also incorporate enumeration, pattern language and numeracy skills at 5 and 9 years. However, these
recognition, and engagement in math talk thereby providing a plau- results should be interpreted with caution as the effect sizes identi-
sible mechanism through which the HLE may influence numeracy fied in this study were of small to modest magnitude.
skills. Thus, the informal HLE may play a significant role in promot- Nevertheless, these findings suggest that even casual cognitively
ing children’s early language skills which in turn contribute to child- stimulating activities that do not directly focus on active teaching
ren’s later numeracy skills. have demonstrable implications for children’s educational out-
comes. Moreover, there have been suggestions that informal literacy
Cross-Domain Associations Between the HNE and activities instill pleasure and appreciation of reading in children
Children’s Academic and Socioemotional Competencies (Sénéchal, 2006) and informal numeracy activities lead children to
associate math with entertainment and positive quality time with
Growing evidence now suggests that the HNE is not only impor- caregivers (e.g., Daucourt et al., 2021). Collectively, these findings
tant for children’s numeracy skills but may also positively influence are promising for the development of cost-effective and easily
literacy skills (Huntsinger et al., 2016). In fact, meta-analytic evi- implementable early childhood interventions.
dence suggests that early math skills predict children’s reading skills
just as well as early reading skills (Duncan et al., 2007). However, Limitations
more recent evidence from younger populations indicates that play-
ing structured games (e.g., board games, puzzles, and flash cards) Although the findings of the present study shed a positive light on
with children aged 8–36 months is not predictive of children’s the importance of early informal learning activities, a number of lim-
vocabularies (Kartushina et al., 2021). itations must be acknowledged. First, the home learning environ-
In this study, the informal HNE demonstrated cross-domain direct ment was measured using parent report only. Moreover, constructs
relations with children’s literacy skills and pragmatic language at of the home learning environment were measured using a small
5 years. Crucially, these associations demonstrated the largest effect number of items (ranging from 1 to 2). Therefore, the extent to
12 CONICA, NIXON, AND QUIGLEY

which children’s formal and informal HNE and HLE were appropri- to identify additional HLE and HNE activities that may show cross-
ately captured is limited. It would be useful for future research to domain effects. Such an exercise would facilitate the development of
assess children’s early learning environment through alternative cost-effective interventions with far-reaching effects across multiple
methods such as direct observation. Such an approach would child outcomes.
allow additional, and potentially unexamined, aspects of the HLE
and HNE to emerge. Second, children’s home learning environment
References
was only assessed when children were aged 3. However, children
continuously develop and acquire more complex skills and their Anders, Y., Rossbach, H.-G., Weinert, S., Ebert, S., Kuger, S., Lehrl, S., &
ZPD changes accordingly. Hence, aspects of the home learning von Maurice, J. (2012). Home and preschool learning environments and
environment that may have promoted skill development at 3 years their relations to the development of early numeracy skills. Early
may differ from aspects of the home learning environment that Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(2), 231–244. https://doi.org/10.1016/
may encourage skills development at 5 and 9 years. j.ecresq.2011.08.003
Aram, D., Deitcher, D. B., Shoshan, T. S., & Ziv, M. (2017). Shared book
Third, socioemotional skills were assessed using a teacher-
reading interactions within families from low socioeconomic backgrounds
reported measure of child prosocial behavior. However, a more com-
and children’s social understanding and prosocial behavior. Journal of
prehensive understanding of how the home learning environment Cognitive Education and Psychology, 16(2), 157–177. https://doi.org/10
may influence child socioemotional skills may be obtained by .1891/1945-8959.16.2.157
multiple-item questionnaires complemented by direct assessment. Baker, C. E. (2015). Does parent involvement and neighborhood quality mat-
Relatedly, given that all child outcomes were assessed using teacher ter for African American boys’ kindergarten mathematics achievement?
report, it is possible that ceiling effects may have influenced the find- Early Education and Development, 26(3), 342–355. https://doi.org/10
ings of this study. Future research should assess child outcomes .1080/10409289.2015.968238
using a multitude of measures to allow for greater variability to Benavides-Varela, S., Butterworth, B., Burgio, F., Arcara, G., Lucangeli, D.,
emerge and contribute greater insight into the relation between & Semenza, C. (2016). Numerical activities and information learned at
home link to the exact numeracy skills in 5–6 years-old children.
early learning environments and academic competencies. Fourth,
Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 94. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg
although the study controls for children’s baseline language skills,
.2016.00094
it does not account for bidirectional effects between other child indi- Bierman, K. L., Welsh, J. A., Heinrichs, B. S., Nix, R. L., & Mathis, E. T.
vidual characteristics and the home learning environment. It is likely (2015). Helping head start parents promote their children’s Kindergarten
that academically driven children are likely to elicit cognitively stim- adjustment: The research-based developmentally informed parent pro-
ulating activities from parents which in turn may promote their later gram. Child Development, 86(6), 1877–1891. https://doi.org/10.1111/
academic competencies (e.g., Sameroff, 2009). cdev.12448
Fifth, the extent to which these findings may generalize across Blevins-Knabe, B., Austin, A. B., Musun, L., Eddy, A., & Jones, R. M.
orthographies is limited. For example, the effects of formal home (2000). Family home care providers’ and parents’ beliefs and practices
learning activities tend to be significantly smaller across transparent concerning mathematics with young children. Early Child Development
and Care, 165(1), 41–58. https://doi.org/10.1080/0300443001650104
orthographies (e.g., Finnish, Greek) compared to opaque orthogra-
Brown, M. I., Wang, C., & McLeod, S. (2022). Reading with 1–2 year olds
phies (e.g., French, English; Manolitsis et al., 2013). Sixth, it should
impacts academic achievement at 8–11 years. Early Childhood Research
be noted that the effect sizes in the present study were of small mag- Quarterly, 58(1), 198–207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2021.09.008.
nitude, albeit comparable to existing research (e.g., Gibson et al., Butterworth, B. (2005). The development of arithmetical abilities. Journal of
2021). One possibility is that the large sample size may have Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(1), 3–18. https://doi.org/10.1111/j
increased the risk of Type I error which may account for the identi- .1469-7610.2004.00374.x
fication of weak effect sizes. Nevertheless, these findings provide Cunha, F., & Heckman, J. J. (2007). The technology of skill formation.
promising suggestions that even recreational activities without an American Economic Review, 97(2), 31–47. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer
explicit focus on math and literacy domains can partly contribute .97.2.31
to children’s academic competence. Daucourt, M. C., Napoli, A. R., Quinn, J. M., Wood, S. G., & Hart, S. A.
Finally, despite the strengths of the longitudinal nature of the (2021). The home math environment and math achievement: A meta-
analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 147(6), 565–596. https://doi.org/10
study, the analyses reported in the present paper are correlational.
.1037/bul0000330.supp
Thus, the possibility that confounders or reverse-causality may Dearing, E., Casey, B. M., Ganley, C. M., Tillinger, M., Laski, E., &
explain the observed effects cannot be fully eliminated. Montecillo, C. (2012). Young girls’ arithemtic and spatial skills: The distal
and proximal roles of family socioeconomics and home learning experi-
Conclusion ences. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(3), 458–470. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.01.002
Despite limitations, the present study tentatively suggests that De Keyser, L., Bakker, M., Rathé, S., Wijns, N., Torbeyns, J., Verschaffel, L.,
children’s early informal HLE and HNE play an important role for & De Smedt, B. (2020). No association between the home math environ-
their later academic performance. Moreover, the positive influence ment and numerical and patterning skills in a large and diverse sample of
5- to 6-year-olds. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, Article 547626. https://
of the early home learning environment appears to persist until chil-
doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.547626
dren are 9 years old. These findings point to the importance and the
Dooley, T., Dunphy, E., Shiel, G., O’Connor, M., & Travers, J. (2014).
potential effectiveness of promoting cognitively stimulating envi- Mathematics in early childhood and primary education (3–8 years).
ronments early in life. In addition, the evidence for domain-specific Teaching and Learning, 18, 164.
but also cross-domain effects of the informal HLE and HNE Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C.,
bears important implications for early childhood interventions. Klebanov, P., Pagani, L. S., Feinstein, L., Engel, M., Brooks-Gunn, J., &
Specifically, these findings provide an incentive for future research Sexton, H. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental
HOME LEARNING ENVIRONMENT AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE 13

Psychology, 43(6), Article 1428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6 Kozak, S., & Recchia, H. (2019). Reading and the development of social
.14287 understanding: Implications for the literacy classroom. The Reading
Dunst, C. J., Hamby, D. W., Wilkie, H., & Dunst, K. S. (2017). Meta-analysis Teacher, 72(5), 569–577. https://doi.org/10.1002/trtr.1760
of the relationship between home and family experiences and young child- Kumschick, I. R., Beck, L., Eid, M., Witte, G., Klann-Delius, G., Heuser, I.,
ren’s early numeracy learning. In P. Sullivan & A. Gervasoni (Eds.), Steinlein, R., & Menninghaus, W. (2014). READING and FEELING: The
Engaging families as children’s first mathematics educators (pp. 105– effects of a literature-based intervention designed to increase emotional
125). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-2553-2_7 competence in second and third graders. Frontiers in Psychology, 5,
Ebert, S., Lehrl, S., & Weinert, S. (2020). Differential effects of the home lan- Article 1448. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01448
guage and literacy environment on child language and theory of mind and Lahaie, C. (2008). School readiness of children of immigrants: Does parental
their relation to socioeconomic background. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, involvement play a role? Social Science Quarterly, 89(3), 684–705.
Article 2321. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.555654. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6237.2008.00554.x
Elliot, C. D., Smith, P., & McCulloch, K. (1996). British ability scales II. Lakens, D., Adolfi, F. G., Albers, C. J., Anvari, F., Apps, M. A., Argamon, S.
NFER-Nelson. E., Baguley, T., Becker, R. B., Benning, S. D., Bradford, D. E., Buchanan,
George, D. & Mallery, M. (2010). SPSS for Windows step by step: A simple E. M., Caldwell, A. R., Van Calster, B., Carlsson, R., Chen, S.-C., Chung,
guide and reference, 17.0 update (10th ed.). Pearson. B., Colling, L. J., Collins, G. S., Crook, Z., … Zwaan, R. A. (2018). Justify
Gibson, J. L., Newbury, D. F., Durkin, K., Pickles, A., Conti-Ramsden, G., & your alpha. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(3), 168–171. https://doi.org/10
Toseeb, U. (2021). Pathways from the early language and communication .1038/s41562-018-0311-x
environment to literacy outcomes at the end of primary school; the LeFevre, J. A., Polyzoi, E., Skwarchuk, S. L., Fast, L., & Sowinski, C.
roles of language development and social development. Oxford Review (2010). Do home numeracy and literacy practices of Greek and
of Education, 47(2), 260–283. https://doi.org/10.1080/03054985.2020 Canadian parents predict the numeracy skills of kindergarten children?
.1824902 International Journal of Early Years Education, 18(1), 55–70. https://
Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research doi.org/10.1080/09669761003693926
note. Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 38(5), 581– Lehrl, S., Ebert, S., Blaurock, S., Rossbach, H.-G., & Weinert, S. (2020).
586. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x Long-term and domain-specific relations between the early years home
Hood, M., Conlon, E., & Andrews, G. (2008). Preschool home literacy prac- learning environment and students’ academic outcomes in secondary
tices and children’s Literacy development: A longitudinal analysis. school. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 31(1), 102–124.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(2), 252–271. https://doi.org/10 https://doi.org/10.1080/09243453.2019.1618346
Light, J. G., & DeFries, J. C. (1995). Comorbidity of reading and mathematics
.1037/0022-0663.100.2.252
Hooper, S. R., Roberts, J., Sideris, J., Burchinal, M., & Zeisel, S. (2010). disabilities: Genetic and environmental etiologies. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 28(2), 96–106. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221949502800204
Longitudinal predictors of reading and math trajectories through middle
Malanchini, M., Tosto, M. G., Garfield, V., Dirik, A., Czerwik, A., Arden, R.,
school for African American versus Caucasian students across two sam-
Malykh, S., & Kovas, Y. (2016). Preschool drawing and school mathemat-
ples. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1018–1029. https://doi.org/10
ics: The nature of the association. Child Development, 87(3), 929–943.
.1037/a0018877
https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12520
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (2009). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance
Manolitsis, G., Georgiou, G. K., & Tziraki, N. (2013). Examining the effects
structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural
of home literacy and numeracy environment on early reading and math
Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. https://
acquisition. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(4), 692–703.
doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.05.004
Huntsinger, C. S., Jose, P. E., & Luo, Z. (2016). Parental facilitation of early
Melhuish, E. C., Phan, M. B., Sylva, K., Sammons, P., Siraj-Blatchford, I., &
mathematics and reading skills and knowledge through encouragement of
Taggart, B. (2008). Effects of the home learning environment and pre-
home-based activities. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 37, 1–15.
school center experience upon literacy and numeracy development in
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.02.005 early primary school. Journal of Social Issues, 64(1), 95–114. https://
Isaacs, J. B. (2012). Starting school at a disadvantage: The school readiness
doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00550.x
of poor children. The Social Genome Project, Center on Children and Mol, S. E., Bus, A. G., de Jong, M. T., & Smeets, D. J. H. (2008). Added
Families at Brookings. value of dialogic parent–child book readings: A meta-analysis. Early
Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Locuniak, M. N., & Ramineni, C. (2007). Education and Development, 19(1), 7–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Predicting first-grade math achievement from developmental number 10409280701838603
sense trajectories. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 22(1), Mutaf Yıldız, B., Sasanguie, D., De Smedt, B., & Reynvoet, B. (2018).
36–46. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5826.2007.00229.x Frequency of home numeracy activities is differentially related to basic
Karlson, K. B., & Holm, A. (2011). Decomposing primary and secondary number processing and calculation skills in kindergartners. Frontiers in
effects: A new decomposition method. Research in Social Stratification Psychology, 9, Article 340. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00340
and Mobility, 29(2), 221–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rssm.2010.12.005 Napoli, A. R., & Purpura, D. J. (2018). The home literacy and numeracy envi-
Kartushina, N., Mani, N., Aktan-Erciyes, A., Alaslani, K., Aldrich, N. J., ronment in preschool: Cross-domain relations of parent–child practices
Almohammadi, A., Alroqi, H., Anderson, L. M., Andonova, E., and child outcomes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166,
Aussems, S., & Babineau, M. (2021). COVID-19 first lockdown as a win- 581–603. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.10.002
dow into language acquisition: Associations between caregiver-child Niklas, F., Cohrssen, C., Lehrl, S., & Napoli, A. R. (2021). Children’s com-
activities and vocabulary gains. Language Development Research, 2(1), petencies development in the home learning environment. Frontiers in
1–36. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/5ejwu. Psychology, 12, Article 706360. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021
Kindred Squared. (2022). School readiness: Qualitative and quantitative .706360
research with teaching professionals. YouGov. https://secureservercdn Niklas, F., & Schneider, W. (2014). Casting the die before the die is cast: The
.net/160.153.138.204/457.e40.myftpupload.com/wp-content/uploads/ importance of the home numeracy environment for preschool children.
2022/05/YouGov-Kindred-Squared-School-Readiness-Report-2022-1 European Journal of Psychology of Education, 29(3), 327–345. https://
.pdf doi.org/10.1007/s10212-013-0201-6
14 CONICA, NIXON, AND QUIGLEY

Niklas, F., & Schneider, W. (2017). Home learning environment and devel- Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. A. (2014). Continuity and change in the home
opment of child competencies form kindergarten until the end of elemen- literacy environment as predictors of growth in vocabulary and reading.
tary school. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 49, 263–274. https:// Child Development, 85(4), 1552–1568.
doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.03.006 Silinskas, G., Torppa, M., Lerkkanen, M.-K., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2020). The
O’Farrelly, C., Doyle, O., Victory, G., & Palamaro-Munsell, E. (2018). home literacy model in a highly transparent orthography. School
Shared reading in infancy and later development: Evidence from an Effectiveness and School Improvement, 31(1), 80–101. https://doi.org/10
early intervention. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 54, .1080/09243453.2019.1642213
69–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2017.12.001 Skwarchuk, S.-L., Sowinski, C., & LeFevre, J.-A. (2014). Formal and infor-
Piaget, J. (1965). The child’s conception of number. W. W. Norton. mal home learning activities in relation to children’s early numeracy and
Prizant, B. M., & Wetherby, A. M. (1990). Toward an integrated view of literacy skills: The development of a home numeracy model. Journal of
early language and communication development and socioemotional Experimental Child Psychology, 121, 63–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
development. Topics in Language Disorders, 10(4), 1–16. .jecp.2013.11.006
Purpura, D. J., Hume, L. E., Sims, D. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2011). Early lit- Sloan, S., Devine, D., Martinez Sainz, G., Symonds, J. E., Crean, M., Moore,
eracy and early numeracy: The value of including early literacy skills in the B., Davies, A., Farrell, E., Farrell, J., Blue, T., Tobin, E., & Hogan, J.
prediction of numeracy development. Journal of Experimental Child (2022). Children’s school lives: The preschool to primary school transi-
Psychology, 110(4), 647–658. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.07.004 tion in Ireland, Report No. 4. University College Dublin. https://cslstudy
Purpura, D. J., King, Y. A., Rolan, E., Hornburg, C. B., Schmitt, S. A., Hart, .wpenginepowered.com/wp-content/uploads/2022/08/CSL_Annual-
S. A., & Ganley, C. M. (2020). Examining the factor structure of the home Report-4-_FINAL-1.pdf
mathematics environment to delineate its role in predicting preschool Susperreguy, M. I., Burr, S. D. L., Xu, C., Douglas, H., & LeFevre, J.-A.
numeracy, mathematical language, and spatial skills. Frontiers in (2020). Children’s home numeracy environment predicts growth of their
Psychology, 11, Article 1925. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01925 early mathematical skills in kindergarten. Child Development, 91(5),
Quail, A., O’Reilly, C., Watson, D., McNamara, E., O’Mahony, D., & 1663–1680. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13353
Torppa, M., Vasalampi, K., Eklund, K., & Niemi, P. (2022). Long-term
Murray, A. (2019). Growing up in Ireland a summary guide to cohort
effects of the home literacy environment on reading development:
’08 at 9 years (wave 5 of the infant cohort at 9 years). Economic and
Familial risk for dyslexia as a moderator. Journal of Experimental Child
Social Research Institute.
Psychology, 215, Article 105314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2021
Ramani, G. B., & Siegler, R. S. (2011). Reducing the gap in numerical
.105314
knowledge between low- and middle-income preschoolers. Journal of
Von Hippel, P. T. (2007). Regression with missing Ys: An improved strategy
Applied Developmental Psychology, 32(3), 146–159. https://doi.org/10
for analyzing multiply imputed data. Sociological Methodology, 37(1),
.1016/j.appdev.2011.02.005
83–117. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9531.2007.00180
Rose, E., Ebert, S., & Weinert, S. (2016). Zusammenspiel sprachlicher und
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Socio-cultural theory. Mind and Society, 6, 52–58.
sozial-emotionaler entwicklung vom vierten bis zum achten lebensjahr:
Weigel, D. J., Martin, S. S., & Bennett, K. K. (2006). Contributions of the
Eine längsschnittliche untersuchung [Interrelation between language and
home literacy environment to preschool-aged children’s emerging literacy
socioemotional development from age three to seven: A longitudinal and language skills. Early Child Development and Care, 176(3–4), 357–
study]. Frühe Bildung, 5(2), 66–72. https://doi.org/10.1026/2191-9186/ 378. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430500063747
a000254 Wirth, A., Ehmig, S. C., & Niklas, F. (2022). The role of the home literacy
Rose, E., Lehrl, S., Ebert, S., & Weinert, S. (2018). Long-term relations environment for children’s Linguistic and socioemotional competencies
between children’s Language, the home literacy environment, and socio- development in the early years. Social Development, 31(2), 372–387.
emotional development from ages 3 to 8. Early Education and https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12550
Development, 29(3), 342–356. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2017 Yang, X., Yan, M., Ruan, Y., Ku, S. Y. Y., Lo, J. C. M., Peng, P., & McBride,
.1409096 C. (2022). Relations among phonological processing skills and mathemat-
Sameroff, A. (2009). The transactional model. American Psychological ics in children: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology,
Association. 114(2), 289–307. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000710.supp
Sarama, J., Lange, A. A., Clements, D. H., & Wolfe, C. B. (2012). The Yeo, L. S., Ong, W. W., & Ng, C. M. (2014). The home literacy environment
impacts of an early mathematics curriculum on oral language and literacy. and preschool children’s reading skills and interest. Early Education and
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(3), 489–502. https://doi.org/10 Development, 25(6), 791–814. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2014
.1016/j.ecresq.2011.12.002 .862147
Sénéchal, M. (2006). Testing the home literacy model: Parent involvement in Zippert, E. L., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2020). The home math environment:
kindergarten is differentially related to grade 4 reading comprehension, More than numeracy. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50(Part 3),
fluency, spelling, and reading for pleasure. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.07.009
10(1), 59–87. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr1001_4
Sénéchal, M. (2015). Young children’s home literacy. In A. Pollatsek & R. Received July 8, 2022
Treiman (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of reading (pp. 397–414). Revision received December 2, 2022
Oxford University Press. Accepted December 6, 2022 ▪

View publication stats

You might also like