Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

doi:10.

1093/brain/awh659 Brain (2005), 128, 2858–2871

Right anterior superior temporal activation


predicts auditory sentence comprehension
following aphasic stroke
Jenny Crinion and Cathy J. Price

Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, Institute of Neurology, London, UK


Correspondence to: Jenny Crinion, Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, Institute of Neurology,
12 Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
E-mail: j.crinion@fil.ion.cul.ac.uk

Previous studies have suggested that recovery of speech comprehension after left hemisphere infarction may
depend on a mechanism in the right hemisphere. However, the role that distinct right hemisphere regions play
in speech comprehension following left hemisphere stroke has not been established. Here, we used functional
magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate narrative speech activation in 18 neurologically normal
subjects and 17 patients with left hemisphere stroke and a history of aphasia. Activation for listening to
meaningful stories relative to meaningless reversed speech was identified in the normal subjects and in each
patient. Second level analyses were then used to investigate how story activation changed with the patients’
auditory sentence comprehension skills and surprise story recognition memory tests post-scanning. Irrespect-
ive of lesion site, performance on tests of auditory sentence comprehension was positively correlated with
activation in the right lateral superior temporal region, anterior to primary auditory cortex. In addition, when
the stroke spared the left temporal cortex, good performance on tests of auditory sentence comprehension was
also correlated with the left posterior superior temporal cortex (Wernicke’s area). In distinct contrast to this,
good story recognition memory predicted left inferior frontal and right cerebellar activation. The implication of
this double dissociation in the effects of auditory sentence comprehension and story recognition memory is that
left frontal and left temporal activations are dissociable. Our findings strongly support the role of the right
temporal lobe in processing narrative speech and, in particular, auditory sentence comprehension following left
hemisphere aphasic stroke. In addition, they highlight the importance of the right anterior superior temporal
cortex where the response was dissociated from that in the left posterior temporal lobe.

Keywords: aphasia; auditory sentence comprehension; stroke

Abbreviations: CAT = comprehensive aphasia test; fMRI = functional MRI; SPM = statistical parametric mapping
Received March 17, 2005. Revised August 5, 2005. Accepted September 12, 2005. Advance Access publication October 18, 2005

Introduction
The neural basis of speech recovery following aphasic stroke and Gordon, 1990). The inverse correlation between the size
remains unknown. It is generally accepted that there will be of the left hemisphere lesion and the degree of language
some degree of functional reorganization following cerebral recovery (Demeurisse et al., 1985; Heiss et al., 1993) has
damage. This will either involve changes within the led some to suggest that it is the left rather than right
pre-existing language network (e.g. Leff et al., 2002) or the hemisphere that plays an important role in recovery. How-
recruitment of supplementary brain areas in the left or right ever, there are also examples of patients with left hemisphere
hemisphere. Clinical observations combined with structural strokes, whose language impairments were impaired further
neuroimaging have suggested that if the lesion is large the when a second stroke damaged the right hemisphere (Basso
prognosis is poor; particularly, it has been proposed, if there is et al., 1989). This suggests that, at least in these patients,
ventral extension of the infarct into the middle and inferior the right hemisphere was also contributing to language
temporal gyri (Selnes et al., 1983; Naeser et al., 1987; Hart processing.
# The Author (2005). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email: journals.permissions@oxfordjournals.org
Right activity predicts aphasics’ comprehension Brain (2005), 128, 2858–2871 2859

Functional imaging studies of aphasic speech recovery have In the present study, we investigated the brain regions that
differed in their conclusions concerning the contribution of contribute to speech comprehension processes by regressing
left and right hemisphere activation. This inconsistency may activation evoked when patients listened to short stories with
partly reflect the tasks tested. During auditory repetition tasks, (i) auditory sentence comprehension and (ii) subsequent
for example, the majority of functional imaging studies have memory abilities, measured outside the scanner. The func-
emphasized that good language recovery depends on left tional MRI (fMRI) paradigm engaged subjects in automatic
hemisphere activation, although right hemisphere activation ‘on-line’ narrative speech comprehension without any
may be observed when left hemisphere regions are no longer requirement to make a response. The intention was to reduce
available (Heiss et al., 1997, 1999; Karbe et al., 1998; Kessler activation related to working memory and other executive
et al., 2000). In contrast, during auditory comprehension functions that are not directly related to normal, on-line
tasks, a greater emphasis has been placed on the role of comprehension of narrative speech. We predicted that the
the right hemisphere. For example, when patients with left activation pattern would vary, across subjects, according to
temporal lobe damage listened to words presented at varying their auditory comprehension abilities. In addition, we asse-
rates, right posterior temporal activation was more correlated ssed how activation changed with auditory recognition mem-
with increasing word rate than in control subjects (Leff ory as measured by a post-scanning surprise story recognition
et al., 2002). Right hemisphere activation in patients with test. This allowed us to distinguish linguistic (auditory com-
left temporal damage has also been shown to correlate with prehension) from metalinguistic executive (auditory recogni-
response accuracy when subjects respond to simple and repet- tion memory) processes that are not directly related to
itive auditory instructions (Musso et al., 1999) or perform normal, on-line narrative speech comprehension. Although
semantic decisions on auditory words (Sharp et al., 2004). speech comprehension proficiency may well depend on work-
Critically, however, these results do not necessarily reflect ing memory capacity (Just and Carpenter, 1992; MacDonald
a ‘laterality shift’ because the same right hemisphere areas et al., 1992) and the speech comprehension problems of some
are also engaged by neurologically normal subjects patients with aphasia have been attributed to verbal working
(Warburton et al., 1999; Zahn et al., 2002, 2004). This is memory deficits (Hermann et al., 1992; Wilson and Baddeley,
clearly illustrated in the study by Sharp et al. (2004) who 1993; Aziz et al., 2002) previous functional imaging studies
found that right anterior fusiform activation correlated have not dissociated activation that is related to understand-
with performance accuracy in neurologically normal controls ing speech (primarily at a linguistic level) from executive
as well as the patients. (metalinguistic) aspects of speech processing.
An outstanding question that we wish to address is whether In summary, previous functional imaging studies have
right hemisphere activation contributes to long-term lan- reached divergent conclusions concerning the neural mech-
guage functioning after aphasic stroke. Previous studies anisms of recovery and this may reflect the very variable
have addressed this issue by investigating how activation stimuli, tasks and patient groups that have been investigated.
changes over the course of recovery (Cardebat et al., 2003; It, therefore, remains unknown as to whether right hemi-
Fernandez et al., 2004); by correlating activation with beha- sphere activation reflects a maladaptive strategy or a beneficial
vioural responses on the task used during scanning (Musso one. In non-fluent patients, it has been proposed that the
et al., 1999; Blasi et al., 2002; Sharp et al., 2004) or by cor- pattern of overactivation may limit, rather than account
relating activation with language assessments conducted for, aphasia recovery (Belin et al., 1996; Rosen et al., 2000).
outside the scanner (Rosen et al., 2000; Blank et al., 2003; Each hemisphere may be important, depending on the type of
Noppeney et al., 2005). Although significant activation language behaviour and when it was examined (Weiller et al.,
changes over time or within scanning session have been 1995; Basso et al., 1996; Belin et al., 1996; Mimura et al., 1998;
observed, correlations between activation and language per- Cao et al., 1999; Ansaldo et al., 2002; Hund-Georgiadis et al.,
formance outside the scanner have not revealed significant 2002; Ansaldo et al., 2004). In this study we aimed to differen-
effects in aphasic patients. For example, neither Blank et al. tiate auditory sentence comprehension (linguistic) processes
(2003) nor Rosen et al., (2000) observed a relationship from auditory recognition memory processes (executive,
between right hemisphere activation and speech production metalinguistic) in a group of chronic aphasic patients by
ability measured outside the scanner. These authors, there- regressing activation evoked when patients listened to short
fore, concluded that right hemisphere activation may repre- stories with (i) auditory sentence comprehension and (ii)
sent behavioural strategies (Rosen et al., 2000) or transcallosal subsequent memory abilities, measured outside the scanner.
disinhibition (Blank et al., 2003) that do not necessarily
contribute to the recovery of speech production processes.
The role of the right hemisphere, however, is likely to be Methods
critically dependent on the task. For example, according to Participants
the model of right hemisphere language proposed by Zaidel Seventeen patients (12 males, mean 62 years, SE 2.7) and 18
(1976), the right hemisphere should selectively contribute neurologically normal subjects (8 males, mean 58 years, SE 2.8)
to the recovery of language comprehension but not to the were studied. Each had English as their first language, was right-
recovery of language production. handed and gave informed consent to participate in the study.
2860 Brain (2005), 128, 2858–2871 J. Crinion and C. J. Price

Fig. 1 Infarct location in aphasic patients. The lesion overlap maps show the extent of damage across patients. Sagittal, transaxial and
coronal projections are displayed rendered onto a single-subject brain T1 template. The range of the colour scale relates to the absolute
number of patients in each group. In the patients with temporal lobe damage (Temporal patients), lesion overlap is highest in the left
superior temporal lobe. In the Control patients, lesion overlap is highest in the left motor cortices.

All patients acquired their aphasia following a single left hemi- story was 64 words (25 s duration). All stories were recorded by a
sphere stroke. They had a volumetric MRI to aid co-registration single female narrator. A typical example is:
with their fMRI scan and to define their allocation to the appropriate ‘Yesterday one person was killed. There was a big storm. The
patient group based on the site of their lesions (Fig. 1). One group storm blew down many trees. John was driving home after work.
of eight patients had left temporal lobe damage (Temporal patients); The storm was very bad. A tree fell on the road. John did not see the
mean age 58 years, mean time since infarct 41 months. The remain- tree. John’s car hit the tree. The police found John. The police called
ing nine patients who had left hemisphere lesions sparing the tem- the doctor. It was too late. John died on the way to the hospital.’
poral lobes acted as an aphasia control group (Control patients); Using SoundEdit16 computer software the waveform for each
mean age 66 years, mean time since infarct 48 months. There was no story was reversed (RSt) and used as the baseline condition. Revers-
significant difference between the groups in terms of age (P = 0.09, ing the speech signal destroys intelligibility while retaining the overall
SE = 4.3) or time of study after infarct (P = 0.69, SE = 17.9). spectrotemporal complexity of the original speech signal. The
Patients’ language abilities were assessed using the comprehensive reversed versions of the narratives were expected to control for
aphasia test (CAT) (Swinburn et al., 2005). For single words, subsets early acoustic processing of the speech signal in both left and
of items manipulate variables such as word frequency, imageability, right superior temporal cortex. However, as reversing speech des-
animacy and length, with other confounding variables controlled. troys normal segmental stress patterns, suprasegmental prosody and
Imageability values were drawn from the MRC Psycholinguistic voice identity, it was expected that the activation patterns in the
Database (Coltheart, 1981) and word frequency from Francis and contrast of St with RSt would not reflect linguistic processing alone.
Kucera (1982). In the sentence level subtests, clausal and syntactic During each subject’s study they heard 32 stories (St) and 8
complexities have been manipulated (varying factors like sentence reversed stories (RSt), with each story and its reversed version
‘reversibility’ and active/passive structures). With all spoken output used only once per subject. The order of presentation was random-
tasks in the CAT the focus of interest is in the linguistic rather than ized, both within and between subjects. Presentation was binaural
motor aspects of speech production. Therefore, articulatory errors with the volume set at a comfortable level for each subject. The
not affecting the perceptual identity of the target are scored correct subjects were asked to simply listen and try to understand the
so that patients with dysarthria would not be penalized. story. They were aware that the reversed stories were unintelligible
but were asked to pay attention to the sounds.
To ensure that the subjects had attended to the stimuli, an
Scanning stimuli eye-tracker was used to check that the subjects were awake through-
Subjects listened to two conditions during their fMRI scans: stories out scanning acquisition; and a surprise story recognition test was
(St) and reversed version of the same stories (RSt). The length of each presented to each subject after scanning. In this post-scanning test,
Right activity predicts aphasics’ comprehension Brain (2005), 128, 2858–2871 2861

subjects were presented with 38 individual phrases and asked to activation and the normal activation inclusively masked (P <
indicate after each one whether they recognized it as being part of 0.001 uncorrected) with the same contrasts.
a story played to them in the scanner or not. (ii) The impact of auditory sentence comprehension on story
activation. Within a single analysis, each patient group was
fMRI scanning modelled independently and story activation was regressed
A Siemens 1.5 T scanner was used to acquire T 2*-weighted echo- (in each group separately) on the auditory sentence compre-
planar images with BOLD contrast. Each echoplanar image com- hension score from the CAT. This multiple regression within
prised 35 axial slices of 2.0 mm thickness with 1 mm inter-slice ANOVA analysis enabled us to find common effects of auditory
interval and 3 · 3 mm in-plane resolution. Volumes were acquired sentence comprehension and differences between patient
with an effective repetition time (TR) of 3.15 s/volume and the first groups. For both common and differential effects, we identified
six (dummy) volumes of each run were discarded to allow for the effect of sentence comprehension in conjunction with the
T1 equilibration effects. A total of 460 volume images were taken main effect of story activation, in order to limit the search to
in two separate runs, each of 10 min duration. After the two func- areas that responded positively to intelligible speech. As neuro-
tional runs, a T1-weighted anatomical volume image was acquired logically normal control subjects are all at ceiling on the CAT
for all subjects. auditory sentence comprehension test, they were excluded from
the analysis.
Behavioural analyses Unless otherwise indicated, we report and discuss regions
that showed significant effects at P < 0.05 (corrected for mul-
All patients’ individual performance on the comprehensive battery of
tiple comparisons across the whole brain) with an extent
language tests and the recognition memory test post-scan are tabu-
threshold, for each cluster, of 5 voxels.
lated in Table 1. To investigate group differences, the data were
(iii) Activation associated with good story recognition memory.
entered into SPSS 10.0 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL) and group means
Within a single analysis, each patient group and the neurolo-
for each behavioural variable were compared using independent
gically normal controls were modelled independently and story
sample t-tests. Bivariate correlations estimated significance of
activation (for each subject within each group) was regressed on
Pearson correlations between the behavioural variables. Significance
subsequent story recognition memory established from the
was set at P < 0.05 (two-tailed) for all analyses.
surprise recognition test. This multiple regression within
ANOVA analysis enabled us to find common effects of sub-
Statistical parametric mapping sequent recognition memory as well as group differences. For
Statistical parametric mapping was performed using SPM2 software both common and differential effects, we identified the effect of
(Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK), subsequent story recognition in conjunction with the main
running under Matlab 6.5 (Mathworks Inc., Sherborn, MA, USA). effect of story activation, in order to limit the search to
All volumes from each subject were realigned using the first as ref- areas that responded positively to the narrative speech.
erence and resliced with sinc interpolation. Within SPM, using Imcalc
an 8 mm smoothed study specific template was created using the
images from both the normals and patients to aid the accuracy of the
Results
normalization of this subject group rather than using the standard Behavioural data
SPM template based on normal healthy young volunteers. The func- The effect of left hemisphere lesions on
tional images were then spatially normalized to the study specific language function
template (Friston et al., 1995a) with SPM2 normalization software,
using non-linear-basis functions with high regularization that limits Although many of the patients continued to have significant
the local distortions within the image. Functional data were spatially difficulties on tests of speech production and verbal fluency,
smoothed, with a 12 mm full width at half maximum isotropic all patients performed above chance on tests of auditory sen-
Gaussian kernel, to compensate for residual variability after spatial tence comprehension (minimal score 73% accuracy) (see
normalization and to permit application of Gaussian random field Table 1).
theory for corrected statistical inference. A direct comparison of the patient groups indicated
First, the statistical analysis was performed in a subject-specific lower performance for patients with left temporal lobe
fashion. To remove low-frequency drifts, the data were high-pass damage on both auditory (P = 0 .009, SE: 1.9) and written
filtered using a set of discrete cosine basis functions with a cut-off sentence comprehension (P = 0.002, SE: 2.7) tasks (see
period of 128 s. Each experimental condition was modelled inde-
Table 2). However, there was considerable variability within
pendently by convolving each block with a synthetic haemodynamic
groups and no clear relationship between performance on the
response function. The parameter estimates were calculated for all
brain voxels using the general linear model, and contrast images language tests, and the site and extent of damage (see Table 1).
comparing each story against reversed stories (i.e. the baseline) For example, Patients 7 and 5 (WC and GH) had extensive left
were computed (Friston et al., 1995b). These subject-specific contrast temporal lobe damage but still scored 28/30 and 30/30 on
images were then entered into three separate second level ANOVAs single word comprehension (chance = 7.5/30) and 25/32 and
that identified: 23/32 on auditory sentence comprehension (chance = 8/32).
(i) Group differences and commonalities in story activation. Analysis of the extent and site of the lesion was, therefore,
Differences were identified by comparing story activation in not a good predictor of language comprehension. There
neurologically normal subjects, patients with left temporal were no significant correlations between age of the patients
lobe damage and patients without left temporal lobe damage. or time post-stroke, and performance on any of the
Commonalities were identified with the conjunction of patient language tests.
2862

Table 1 Patients’ behavioural profiles


Patient Patient Patient Age Sex Lesion Months Auditory word Auditory Naming Written word Written Repetition Recognition
group number initials (years) location post-stroke comprehension sentence comprehension sentence single word memory
comprehension comprehension post-scan

Temp. 1 GE 64 M pST 4 20* 21* 4* 6* 7* 32 18


Temp. 2 GB 69 M pST 7 30 27* 16* 28 7* 6* n/c
Brain (2005), 128, 2858–2871

Temp. 3 VW 48 M pST 6 27 29 48 30 30 28* 21


Temp. 4 LH 34 F aST 125 30 29 46 30 24 32 20
Temp. 5 GH 41 F a + pST 80 30 23* 47 30 24 30 27
Temp. 6 MG 67 M a + pST 17 16* 18* 2* 27* 9* 0* 12
Temp. 7 WC 75 M a + pST 36 28 25* 37* 24* 29 32 18
Temp. 8 DD 64 M a + pST 51 26 16* 28* 22* 12* 25* 30
Control 9 AK 66 M Frontal 86 28 26* 48 28 29 32 27
Control 10 BC 63 F Frontal 42 30 32 48 30 32 32 25
Control 11 DR 75 F Frontal 37 29 30 31* 27* 30 15* 16
Control 12 DC 69 M Parietal 78 30 24* 45 28 28 24* 33
Control 13 PB 71 M Parietal 95 26 30 48 30 29 29* 31
Control 14 RJ 59 M Parietal 38 30 32 48 30 32 32 33
Control 15 SB 59 F Subcortical 20 30 32 48 30 32 32 27
Control 16 TS 66 M Subcortical 13 28 29 48 29 31 32 33
Control 17 AM 66 M Subcortical 24 28 28 48 29 31 32 19
Non-aphasic Mean = 29.15 Mean = 30.17 Mean = 46.37 Mean = 29.63 Mean = 29.78 Mean = 31.73 Mean = 32.29
performance SD = 1.35 SD = 1.85 SD = 1.6 SD = 0.79 SD = 2.5 SD = 0.67 SD = 3.26
Range = 25–30 Range = 26–32 Range = 42–48 Range = 27–30 Range = 24–32 Range = 30–32 Range = 24–37
Cut off = 25 Cut off= 27 Cut off=43 Cut off= 27 Cut off= 23 Cut off=29 (max = 38)
(max = 30) (max = 32) (max = 48) (max = 30) (max = 32) (max = 32) (n = 18)
(n = 27) (n = 23) (n = 27) (n = 27) (n = 23) (n = 26)

The individual scores for each patient on the language subtests of the comprehensive aphasia test battery (CAT) and the surprise story recognition memory test post-fMRI-scanning. The
cut-off where mentioned is the score that at least 95% of normal subjects exceed; as a result the scores that are below the cut-off represent aphasic performance. Individual test scores
highlighted in bold italic typeface with an asterisk (*) represent aphasic performance. aST = anterolateral superior temporal lobe; pST = posterior superior temporal lobe; a + pST =
damage to both anterior and posterior superior temporal cortices; Recognition memory post-scan = surprise recognition memory test post-scanning; Temp. = patients with left temporal
lobe damage; n/c= not collected.
J. Crinion and C. J. Price
Right activity predicts aphasics’ comprehension Brain (2005), 128, 2858–2871 2863

Table 2 Group language comprehension data


Patient group Auditory word Auditory sentence Written word Written sentence
comprehension comprehension comprehension comprehension

Mean SE Mean SE Mean SE Mean SE

Temporal (n = 8) 25.9 1.8 23.5* 1.7* 24.6 2.9 17.8* 3.5*


Control (n = 9) 28.8 0.5 29.2* 0.9* 29 1.1 30.4* 0.5*
Maximum score 30 32 30 32

The table displays mean scores and standard error of the patient groups with left superior temporal lobe damage (Temporal patients)
and those without damage to the left temporal lobe (Control patients) on auditory and written single word and sentence comprehension
testing. We performed independent t-tests for each variable, and significance was set at P < 0.05 (two-tailed). The Control patients
were as a group significantly better on tests of auditory and written sentence comprehension than the Temporal patients (P = 0.009, SE: 1.9;
P = 0.002, SE: 2.7, respectively), these scores are highlighted in bold italic typeface with an asterisk (*). There was no difference between
the patient groups on tests of single word comprehension, with both groups’ mean performance >80% accuracy on these tests.

Recognition memory performance on the surprise The Control patients, i.e. those without temporal lobe dam-
story recognition test age, activated the same system as the Normal group, apart
The Normal group’s mean performance on the surprise recog- from two regions in bilateral motor cortices that the Normal
nition test post-scan (mean = 32.4/38; SE 0.8) was signific- group activated significantly more (Fig. 2, b1 and b2; Table 3, b
antly higher (P < 0.0001) than the group of 16 patients (mean = and b2).
24.4/38; SE 1.7). Note: Patient 2, GB from the group of The patients with left superior temporal lobe damage activ-
patients with temporal lobe damage did not participate in ated the same right temporal lobe system as the Normal group
the surprise recognition test post-scan. The mean perform- (Fig. 2, c1; Table 3, c). However, as expected they had signi-
ance of the seven patients with temporal lobe damage was ficantly less activation within the left superior temporal cor-
lower (20.9/38; SE 2.3) than the nine patients without tem- tices, within three main peaks extending from the STG to
poral lobe damage (27.1/38; SE 2.1) but this mean difference posterolateral and anterolateral temporal lobe centred on
did not quite reach significance (P = 0.06) due to the wide the STS (Fig. 2, c2, c3; Table 3, c2 and c3). Analyses of indi-
variability within each group. vidual patients revealed that left anterior temporal activation
There were no significant correlations between recognition was significantly reduced even when this region was spared in
memory scores and patients with posterior temporal damage, see Fig. 3: Patient
GE (c). Likewise, left posterior temporal activation was
(i) the age of the patients and normal subjects, (P = 0.45, significantly reduced even when this region was spared in
n = 34), patients with left anterior temporal damage, see Fig. 3:
(ii) the time post-infarction in the patients (P = 0.27, n = 16) or Patient 4, LH (c).
(iii) sentence comprehension (P = 0.41, n = 16). Neither the Control patients nor the patients with temporal
However, patient performance on the post-scan recognition lobe damage activated any region significantly more than
test was significantly correlated with auditory single word the Normal group. In particular, there was no evidence
comprehension (P = 0.04, n = 16). of a difference in any right temporal region to suggest a
‘laterality shift’ of function in the patients with left temporal
lobe damage.
Functional imaging data
Activation for listening to stories relative to baseline
In the Normal group, listening to stories relative to baseline, Auditory sentence comprehension
(reversed stories) activated lateral left temporal cortex, with For both patient groups, there was a positive correlation
five main peaks extending from posterolateral to anterolateral between auditory sentence comprehension and activation
temporal lobe centred on the superior temporal sulcus (STS), in the right lateral anterior temporal lobe (x = +62, y =
and at the junction of the superior temporal gyrus (STG) with 8, z = 16; Z = 6.0) with no significant effect of group
the inferior parietal lobe (IPL). Activation in the right in this area (see Fig. 5). For the Control patients, there was
superior temporal cortex did not extend into the STG/IPL also a positive correlation between auditory sentence com-
junction, with four main peaks in the lateral temporal lobe. prehension and activation in the left lateral posterior temporal
Additional activations were observed bilaterally in the ventral cortex (x = 46, y = 56, z = +6; Z = 4.7). This effect was not
temporal lobe, within the occipito-temporal sulcus separating observed in the patients with temporal lobe damage and the
the inferior temporal gyrus (ITG) and fusiform gyrus (FG), difference between the patient groups was significant at
and in five regions of the left frontal and bilateral motor P < 0.001 uncorrected (Z = 3.9). There were no significant
cortices (Fig. 2, a; Table 3, a). negative correlations.
2864 Brain (2005), 128, 2858–2871 J. Crinion and C. J. Price

Fig. 2 Regional activation for stories > baseline. fMRI activation for the contrast Speech (St) > baseline (RSt) in (a) Normal subjects, (b1)
Control patients and (c1) patients with temporal lobe damage (Temporal patients); (b2) and (c2) illustrate where Normal subjects activated
significantly more than Control and Temporal patients, respectively; (c3) illustrates where Control patients activated significantly more
than Temporal patients. For each group, results are shown in colour rendered onto the SPM2 single subject brain template. Details
regarding the location and significance of activations are given in Table 3.

Subsequent recognition memory from surprise story of patients had left temporal lobe damage (Temporal
recognition test post-scan patients); another had left hemisphere damage that spared
Positive correlations between activation and performance on the left temporal lobe (Control patients). We found that aud-
the surprise story recognition test post-scan, common to con- itory sentence comprehension ability was positively correlated
trols and patients, were observed in the left prefrontal cortex with (i) bilateral temporal activation when the temporal lobes
(x = 42, y = +8, z = +20; Z = 5.32) and right cerebellum (x = were spared; but (ii) only a right anterior temporal region
+16, y = 80, z = 40; Z = 5.17) (Fig. 4). There were no when the left temporal lobe was damaged. Below, we discuss
significant differences between subject groups. There were no the implications of our results for understanding speech com-
significant negative correlations. prehension following aphasic stroke.
Lesion site and speech comprehension
Discussion deficits
Clinical observations over many decades have demonstrated Auditory sentence comprehension was significantly more
that verbal comprehension, both lexical and sentential impaired in the group of patients with temporal damage
semantics, is largely lateralized to the left perisylvian cortices compared with the group of patients without temporal
in most individuals. Nevertheless, speech comprehension damage. However, within the group of patients with temporal
abilities can be very well preserved following left hemisphere damage, there was variability in both the lesion site and
lesions. We investigated whether good speech comprehension the speech comprehension scores. On auditory single word
was related to left or right hemisphere activation. One group comprehension tasks, all patients scored significantly above
Table 3 Regional activation for stories > baseline
Region (a) Normal (b) Control pts (c) Temporal pts (b2) Normal > Control pts (c2) Normal > Temporal pts (c3) Control pts > Temporal
pts

x y z Z x y z Z x y z Z x y z Z x y z Z x y z Z

Left superior temporal cortices


1 60 48 8 8.3 60 48 8 6.1 – ns – ns 60 48 8 7.4 60 48 8 4.6
2 54 38 2 8.1 50 40 2 7.5 – ns – ns 54 38 2 6.0 50 40 0 5.8
3 60 8 14 8.3 60 8 14 7.0 – ns – ns 60 10 14 6.5 62 6 16 5.0
4 58 6 18 8.2 58 6 18 7.6 58 6 18 2.3 – ns 58 6 18 5.7 62 6 18 5.0
5 58 8 20 7.4 58 8 20 7.0 ns – ns 54 8 26 4.8 58 10 18 5.1
Right activity predicts aphasics’ comprehension

Right superior temporal cortices


1 50 34 8 7.8 50 32 10 5.8 52 32 10 4.7 – ns ns 48 36 8 1.9
2 58 4 22 8.2 60 2 24 6.6 56 0 22 5.1 – ns ns 58 4 22 1.7
3 54 10 28 8.0 54 10 28 5.3 54 10 28 4.4 – ns 54 10 28 3.1 – ns
4 54 10 24 7.6 56 12 24 5.9 54 10 28 4.8 – ns ns – ns
Left frontal cortices
1 46 10 18 4.6 46 8 18 4.8 – ns – ns 46 10 18 2.6 48 8 18 3.8
2 56 20 16 7.4 56 20 16 3.7 – ns – ns 56 20 16 3.8 56 20 16 2.1
3 56 28 4 4.6 56 30 6 5.5 – ns – ns 56 28 4 2.2 56 30 6 4.0
4 56 26 2 5.7 56 26 2 3.7 56 26 2 2.2 – ns 56 26 2 1.7 – ns
5 42 28 14 5.9 42 28 14 2.7 42 28 14 2.6 – ns ns – ns
Left motor cortices
1 46 0 50 7.1 42 6 46 4.3 46 0 50 2.7 46 6 52 5.0 46 0 50 2.3 – ns
Right motor cortices
1 56 10 42 5.3 – ns – ns 46 18 64 4.8 56 10 42 3.6 – ns
Left ventral temporal cortex
1 36 42 28 5.3 36 42 28 1.7 – ns – ns 36 42 28 3.8 36 42 28 1.9
2 30 36 22 5.4 – ns – ns 30 36 22 1.7 30 36 22 3.1 – ns
Right ventral temporal cortex
1 38 38 28 4.6 – ns – ns 38 38 28 2.2 38 38 28 2.7 – ns

The first three columns show the coordinates and Z-scores for the condition effect (St > RSt) in (a) the Normal group (visualized in Fig. 2a), (b), the patient Control group (visualized in
Fig. 2b), and (c) the patients with temporal lobe damage (visualized in Fig. 2c). The next three columns (b2, c2 and c3) show the coordinates and Z-scores for the condition effect (St > RSt)
where the Normal group activated significantly more than Control patients (b2) and Temporal patents (c2), and where the Control patients activated significantly more than the Temporal
patients ( c3), visualized in Fig. 2 (b2), (c2) and (c3), respectively. Coordinates are given as x, y and z according to the atlas of Talairach and Tournoux (1988). Normal = normal subject
group; Control pts = patients whose lesions spared the left temporal cortices; Temporal pts = patients whose lesions involved the left superior temporal cortices. Significant effects at
P < 0.05 (corrected for multiple comparisons across the whole brain) are shown in bold typeface.
Brain (2005), 128, 2858–2871
2865
2866 Brain (2005), 128, 2858–2871 J. Crinion and C. J. Price

Fig. 3 Location of temporal damage and abnormal activation in patients GE and LH. Sagittal (x = 58) and coronal (y = 14 and 50) slices
are from the MRI scans of each patient after spatial normalization and indicate the extent of the lesions and the validity of the normalization.
Arrows highlight the areas of temporal damage. Reduced activation (Stories > baseline) compared with neurologically Normal subjects
are shown below in yellow on the same MRI slices (Normal > GE/LH).

chance (8/32 = 25%) with the lowest score (16/32 = 50%) Narrative speech activation in
coming from Patient 6 (MG) who had both anterior and control subjects
posterior superior temporal lobe damage. On the auditory When listening to narrative speech compared with reversed
sentence comprehension task, again, all patients scored above speech, our neurologically Normal subjects and Control
chance with the lowest score (16/32) coming from Patient 8 patients activated bilateral posterior and anterior temporal
(DD) following extensive left superior temporal damage. regions and the left inferior frontal and premotor cortices.
The remainder of the patients made relatively few errors The role of the right temporal activation in speech compre-
on our word and sentence comprehension tests even when hension remains unclear. As the reversal of speech destroys
left temporal lobe damage was extensive. For example, normal stress patterns, intonation, prosody and many
sentence comprehension remained excellent following features that signal unique voice identity, the bilateral
damage to either posterior (Patient 1: GE) or anterior anterior temporal signals may not be attributed exclusively
(Patient 4: LH) temporal cortices as well as in Patient 7 to linguistic processing of speech. Many authors have asso-
(WC) who had lost virtually all his anterior and posterior ciated right temporal lobe activation in normal subjects
superior temporal cortices. These observations do not predominantly with processing of the non-verbal com-
reveal any appreciable relationship between site of ponents (intonation, prosody, voice identity, etc.) of speech
damage and speech comprehension ability. Instead, they (Buchanan et al., 2000; Belin et al., 2002; Mitchell et al.,
suggest that, when the left temporal lobe is damaged, areas 2003; Wildgruber et al., 2004). This lateralization of differ-
outside the left temporal lobe may be sustaining speech ent functions is the usual clinical view. However, there is
comprehension.
Right activity predicts aphasics’ comprehension Brain (2005), 128, 2858–2871 2867

Fig. 4 Activation increases with story comprehension. Regional activation (stories > baseline) increases with performance on the auditory
sentence comprehension test, from the CAT, are illustrated (above) in colour on models of the brain from SPM2 (P < 0.05 corrected).
Two main peaks were observed in the right (x = 64, y = 10, z = 14) and left (x = 50, y = 44, z = 0) superior temporal lobes.
The x-axes on the graphs (below) indicate the individual subjects’ scores on the auditory sentence comprehension test from the CAT; the
y-axes indicate the mean centred activation for stories more than baseline. In the right temporal region there is a common effect for
Control patients (black diamonds) and patients with temporal lobe damage (red circles), with no significant difference in the
regression slopes (black slope, Rsq = 0.47; red slope, Rsq = 0.73). In the left temporal region, there was a positive correlation for the
Control patients but this effect was not observed in the patients with temporal lobe damage. The regression slope was stronger for
Control patients (black slope, Rsq = 0.66) than patients with temporal lobe damage (red slope, Rsq = 0.0009) and the difference between
the patient groups was significant at P < 0.001 uncorrected (Z = 3.9).

good evidence for participation of the right hemisphere left anterior temporal activation was significantly reduced
in lexical semantic functions in normal subjects (Ellis and even when this region was spared in patients with posterior
Shepherd, 1974; Hines, 1975; Day, 1977, 1979; Young and temporal damage (see Patient GE in Fig. 3). Likewise, left
Ellis, 1985) and in patients with semantic dementia who posterior temporal activation was significantly reduced
are more impaired in lexical semantic processing when even when this region was spared in patients with anterior
anterior and ventral temporal atrophy is bilateral (Chan temporal damage (see Patient LH in Fig. 3). The results from
et al., 2001; Galton et al., 2001; Lambon Ralph et al., 2001). these two patients whose lesions involved either posterior
or anterior superior temporal cortices suggest a mutual
dependence between left anterior and posterior superior
The effect of left temporal lobe damage temporal lobe activation when subjects are listening to nar-
on narrative speech activation rative speech.
As expected, left temporal lobe damage significantly reduced The consequence of left temporal lobe damage was also
left superior temporal activation compared with the neuro- reflected in the observation that activation for narrative
logically Normal subjects and the Control patients. More speech was strongly right lateralized (see Fig. 2, c1). This
remarkably, analyses of individual patients revealed that apparent laterality shift (left dominant activation in Normals
2868 Brain (2005), 128, 2858–2871 J. Crinion and C. J. Price

and Control patients right lateralized activation following For example, neither Rosen et al. (2000) nor Blank et al.
left temporal lobe damage), did not reflect compensatory (2003) found any relationship between right frontal activation
right hemisphere activation because examination of activa- and speech production measures. Our findings, in the context
tion in right hemisphere regions revealed that the patients of speech comprehension, therefore, support previous sug-
with left temporal lobe damage activated within the normal gestions that the right hemisphere might be able to sustain
range. This cannot be attributed to abnormalities in the speech comprehension better than speech production (Zaidel,
BOLD response, because the structural integrity of the 1976; Zaidel et al., 2000).
right hemisphere was not compromised in any of the patients. In patients with left temporal lobe damage, auditory sen-
The results, therefore, suggest that right superior temporal tence comprehension ability was only predicted by activation
activation was dissociated from that in the left superior in a right lateral superior temporal region, anterior to primary
temporal lobe. This contrasts with previous observations auditory cortex. Activation in the same right anterior tem-
that have shown how damage to the left frontal operculum poral area also predicted auditory sentence comprehension
can enhance activation in the right frontal operculum ability in patients with lesions outside the left temporal lobe,
during speech production tasks (Rosen et al., 2000; Blank but in addition, these patients also showed a correlation
et al., 2003). between left posterior superior temporal activation and aud-
itory sentence comprehension performance. One interpreta-
tion of these findings could be that auditory sentence
comprehension relies on a bilaterally-distributed system.
The effect of auditory sentence This is demonstrated in terms of bilateral activations in Nor-
comprehension score on narrative mals and Control patients. The reduced comprehension per-
speech activation formance of the group of patients with left temporal lobe
Right lateralized activation following left temporal lobe dam- damage would, therefore, be reflected in reduced, total tem-
age suggests that narrative speech comprehension might be poral lobe activation—in this case the remaining activation of
sustained by the right hemisphere. Moreover, in some patients the right temporal region, with sentence comprehension per-
(e.g. Patient 7, WC), who had little remaining left temporal formance predicted by the sum of left and right temporal
cortex, right hemisphere activation appeared to be sufficient activation.
for good auditory single word and sentence comprehension. However, this does not explain our data. In the patients
However, there are well-recognized difficulties when drawing with left temporal lobe damage there was no negative cor-
such inferences. First, the absence of left hemisphere activa- relation between the remaining activation in the left temporal
tion may be due to a lack of sensitivity in damaged cortex lobe with the degree of right temporal activation. Patients
(i.e. it could be a null result). Second, there is always a trade- with left temporal lobe damage and good sentence compre-
off between engaging patients in an executive paradigm that hension did not have higher right temporal responses than the
requires a motor response versus engaging patients in more Control patients (who had both left and right activation), and
naturalistic speech comprehension paradigms that do not patients with bilateral temporal activation (e.g. Patients 16
involve a motor response. The disadvantage of the former and 17) did not have better sentence comprehension than
is that performance on the decision-making aspects of the patients with only right temporal activation (e.g. Patients 3
task cannot be controlled across subjects. The disadvantage and 4). An alternative interpretation is that, in this group of
of the latter is that we do not have a measure of how much of chronic patients with left temporal damage, the response in
our narratives were understood while the subjects were in the the right temporal lobe is independent of that in the damaged
scanner. left temporal lobe. In other words, the results suggest that in
Our conclusion that the right hemisphere contributes the chronic phase post-stroke the effect in the right anterior
to speech comprehension is, therefore, dependent on our temporal lobe is independent of that in the left posterior
observation that right anterior temporal activation could temporal lobe: the Control patients showed both effects
be predicted not by lesion site but on the basis of auditory while patients with left temporal lobe damage only showed
sentence comprehension ability measured outside the scanner the effect in the right hemisphere.
(see Fig. 4). This correlational approach is becoming increas- The anterior location of the right hemisphere correlation is
ingly popular in functional imaging studies of patient popu- also interesting. The literature on aphasic stroke patients
lations. For example, Shaywitz et al. (2003) have shown that emphasizes the importance of the posterior superior temporal
left occipito-temporal activation increases with reading ability and inferior parietal cortices in speech comprehension
in neurologically normal and dyslexic children; while (Alexander et al., 1989) as do functional imaging studies of
Noppeney et al. (2005) have shown that right hemisphere aphasia following left hemisphere lesions (Leff et al., 2002;
activation correlates with reading ability following left Musso et al., 1999). However, these studies were not able to
anterior temporal lobe resection. Perhaps surprisingly, the segregate speech perception from auditory comprehension
aphasia literature has not previously reported correlations processes. In contrast, we focused on auditory sentence com-
between right hemisphere activation and speech comprehen- prehension in the regression analysis, which highlighted the
sion and production abilities measured outside the scanner. role of the right anterior superior temporal cortex (and the left
Right activity predicts aphasics’ comprehension Brain (2005), 128, 2858–2871 2869

Fig. 5 Activation increases with story recognition. Regional activation (stories > baseline) increases with performance on the surprise story
recognition memory test post-scan for all subject groups are shown in colour on models of the brain from SPM2 (P < 0.05 corrected). Two
main peaks were observed, in the right cerebellum (x = 16, y = 80, z = 40) and in the left prefrontal cortex (x = 42, y = 8, z = 20). The
x-axes in the graphs indicate the individual subjects’ scores on the surprise story recognition memory test post-scan; while the y-axes
indicate the mean centred activation for stories more than baseline. No significant difference in the regression slopes for Normals (green
stars), Control patients (black tirangles) or patients with temporal lobe damage (red circles) was observed in either region. Green slopes
illustrate Normal regression slopes (right Rsq = 0.32; left Rsq = 0.28). Black slopes illustrate patient regression slopes, Control and
Temporal lobe damaged patients combined as one group (right Rsq = 0.26; left Rsq = 0.26).

posterior superior temporal cortex when it was spared in rates were higher in the patients than the normal controls.
Control patients). The anterior location of the right The implication of this double dissociation in the effects
hemisphere effect is consistent with studies of semantic of auditory sentence comprehension and story recognition
dementia that show progressive and ultimately profound memory is that left frontal and left temporal activations
loss of speech comprehension following bilateral anterior are dissociable. For example, prefrontal and motor activation
and ventral temporal lobe atrophy (Mummery et al., 1999; may be associated with working memory and other executive
Chan et al., 2001; Galton et al., 2001). functions not directly related to normal, on-line speech
processing (Crinion et al., 2003). Indeed, left frontal lesions
do not typically impair simple narrative speech comprehen-
The effect of story recognition memory sion; and the Control patients included in the present
on narrative speech activation study, three of whom had left frontal lesions, performed
In distinct contrast to the correlation of auditory sen- as a group significantly better on language comprehension
tence comprehension with narrative speech activation, tests than the patients with left temporal lobe lesions.
performance on the surprise story recognition test post- Together the dissociation in the effects of speech comprehen-
fMRI-scanning predicted left inferior frontal and right cere- sion and story recognition memory suggest that left
bellar activation (see Fig. 5). This was observed across all inferior frontal activation during speech processing may
subject groups, Normal and both patients groups, with no be associated with ‘top-down’ rather than ‘bottom-up’
significant differences between groups, even though error processing.
2870 Brain (2005), 128, 2858–2871 J. Crinion and C. J. Price

Implications for long-term language damaged, the correlation with sentence comprehension
functioning after aphasic stroke was only observed in the right anterior superior temporal
Our results indicate that the right superior temporal cortex cortex. This suggests that right superior temporal
does play a role in auditory sentence comprehension follow- responses are dissociated from those in the left superior
ing aphasic stroke, at least, in the chronic phase. The fact that temporal lobe.
the patients’ deficits with speech production were far more These results have implications for the development of rehab-
enduring (see Table 1) is consistent with the model of right ilitative and pharmacological approaches to the treatment of
hemisphere language proposed by Zaidel (1976) in which the aphasic stroke. Once cerebral infarction is established, rehab-
right hemisphere should selectively contribute to the recovery ilitative techniques, whether behavioural or pharmacological,
of language comprehension but not to the recovery of lan- must be directed at intact cortices. Following extensive left
guage production. Future studies can, therefore, investigate temporal lobe infarction, we are proposing that right antero-
whether right anterior temporal activation changes longitud- lateral, superior temporal cortices, may be a target for such
inally over time during the acute or chronic stage of recovery. therapy.
For example, it may be the case that recovery of speech com-
prehension depends on the pre-morbid function of the right
anterior temporal lobe. Alternatively, the function of the Acknowledgements
right anterior temporal lobe may depend on experience/
This work was funded by the Wellcome Trust. We thank
therapy.
John Ashburner, Uta Noppeney and Andrea Mechelli for
It has been proposed that the left posterior STS and supra-
their advice and assistance.
temporal plane provide a system whereby speech sequences
are briefly held in memory and can be repeated by projection
forwards of the encoded speech sequences to the caudal fron- References
tal lobe via the superior longitudinal fasciculus (Hickok et al., Alexander MP, Hiltbrunner B, Fischer RS. Distributed anatomy of transcor-
2000; Wise et al., 2001). Thus, the normal left lateralization of tical sensory aphasia. Arch Neurol 1989; 46: 885–92.
speech comprehension systems may be more a reflection of Ansaldo AI, Arguin M, Roch LA. The contribution of the right cerebral
left superior temporal connections with speech production hemisphere to the recovery from aphasia: a single longitudinal case
study. Brain Lang 2002; 82: 206–22.
faculties within the left frontal lobe rather than purely aud-
Ansaldo AI, Arguin M, Lecours AR. Recovery from aphasia: a longitudinal
itory speech processing systems per se. To fully dissociate these study on language recovery, lateralization patterns, and attentional
possibilities will require additional behavioural data from resources. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2004; 26: 621–7.
patients with right superior temporal lesions. It will also be Aziz MA, McKenzie JC, Wilson JS, Cowie RJ, Ayeni SA, Dunn BK. The human
important to investigate whether successful speech compre- cadaver in the age of biomedical informatics. Anat Rec 2002; 269:
20–32.
hension rehabilitative techniques are associated with
Basso A, Gardelli M, Grassi MP, Mariotti M. The role of the right hemisphere
increased responsiveness of the right anterolateral superior in recovery from aphasia. Two case studies. Cortex 1989; 25:
temporal lobe. 555–66.
Basso A, Corno M, Marangolo P. Evolution of oral and written confrontation
naming errors in aphasia. A retrospective study on vascular patients. J Clin
Exp Neuropsychol 1996; 18: 77–87.
Conclusions Belin P, van Eeckhout P, Zilbovicius M, Remy P, Francois C, Guillaume S,
In this study, we have shown that: et al. Recovery from nonfluent aphasia after melodic intonation therapy:
a PET study. Neurology 1996; 47: 1504–11.
(i) Auditory speech comprehension both at the single word Belin P, Zatorre RJ, Ahad P. Human temporal-lobe response to vocal sounds.
and sentence level can be relatively well preserved fol- Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2002; 13: 17–26.
lowing left anterior and posterior superior temporal cor- Blank SC, Bird H, Turkheimer F, Wise RJ. Speech production after stroke: the
tical damage. This suggests that alternative neuronal role of the right pars opercularis. Ann Neurol 2003; 54: 310–20.
Blasi V, Young AC, Tansy AP, Petersen SE, Snyder AZ, Corbetta M. Word
mechanisms are available to support speech comprehen- retrieval learning modulates right frontal cortex in patients with left frontal
sion following aphasic stroke. damage. Neuron 2002; 36: 159–70.
(ii) Lesions involving the left superior temporal cortices res- Buchanan TW, Lutz K, Mirzazade S et al. Recognition of emotional prosody
ult in significantly less activation not only in the area of and verbal components of spoken language: an fMRI study. Brain Res Cogn
lesioned cortex but also ipsilaterally along the length of Brain Res 2000; 9: 227–38.
Cao Y, Vikingstad EM, George KP, Johnson AF, Welch KM. Cortical language
the superior temporal lobe even in structurally intact activation in stroke patients recovering from aphasia with functional MRI.
cortices. This suggests that, for processing narrative Stroke 1999; 30: 2331–40.
speech, left posterior and anterior superior temporal Cardebat D, Demonet JF, De Boissezon X, Marie N, Marie RM, Lambert J,
cortices respond together. et al. Behavioral and neurofunctional changes over time in healthy and
aphasic subjects: a PET Language Activation Study. Stroke 2003; 34:
(iii) Auditory sentence comprehension abilities, as measured
2900–6.
outside the scanner, predicted the level of activation in Chan D, Fox NC, Scahill RI, Crum WR, Whitwell JL, Leschziner G, et al.
the left posterior and right anterior superior temporal Patterns of temporal lobe atrophy in semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s
cortices. When the left superior temporal region was disease. Ann Neurol 2001; 49: 433–42.
Right activity predicts aphasics’ comprehension Brain (2005), 128, 2858–2871 2871

Coltheart M. The MRC psycholinguistic database. Q J Exp Psychol 1981; 33A: Leff A, Crinion J, Scott S, Turkheimer F, Howard D, Wise R. A physiological
497–505. change in the homotopic cortex following left posterior temporal lobe
Crinion JT, Lambon-Ralph MA, Warburton EA, Howard D, Wise RJ. infarction. Ann Neurol 2002; 51: 553–8.
Temporal lobe regions engaged during normal speech comprehension. MacDonald MC, Just MA, Carpenter PA. Working memory constraints on
Brain 2003; 126: 1193–201. the processing of syntactic ambiguity. Cognit Psychol 1992; 24: 56–98.
Day J. Right-hemisphere language processing in normal right-handers. J Exp Mimura M, Kato M, Kato M, Sano Y, Kojima T, Naeser M, et al. Prospective
Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1977; 3: 518–28. and retrospective studies of recovery in aphasia. Changes in cerebral blood
Day J. Visual half-field word recognition as a function of syntactic class and flow and language functions. Brain 1998; 121: 2083–94.
imageability. Neuropsychologia 1979; 17: 515–9. Mitchell RL, Elliott R, Barry M, Cruttenden A, Woodruff PW. The neural
Demeurisse G, Capon A, Verhas M. Prognostic value of computed tomo- response to emotional prosody, as revealed by functional magnetic reson-
graphy in aphasic stroke patients. Eur Neurol 1985; 24: 134–9. ance imaging. Neuropsychologia 2003; 41: 1410–21.
Ellis HD, Shepherd JW. Recognition of abstract and concrete words presented Mummery CJ, Patterson K, Wise RJ, Vandenbergh R, Price CJ, Hodges JR.
in left and right visual fields. J Exp Psychol 1974; 103: 1035–6. Disrupted temporal lobe connections in semantic dementia. Brain 1999;
Fernandez B, Cardebat D, Demonet JF, Joseph PA, Mazuax JM, Barat M, et al. 122: 61–73.
Functional MRI follow-up study of language processes in healthy subjects Musso M, Weiller C, Kiebel S, Muller SP, Bulau P, Rijntjes M. Training-
and during recovery in a case of aphasia. Stroke 2004; 35: 2171–6. induced brain plasticity in aphasia. Brain 1999; 122: 1781–90.
Francis, WN, Kucera, H. Frequency analysis of English usage: Lexicon and Naeser MA, Helm-Estabrooks N, Haas G, Auerbach S, Srinivasan M. Rela-
grammar. Boston: Houghton Mifflin; 1982. tionship between lesion extent in ‘Wernicke’s area’ on computed tomo-
Friston KJ, Ashburner J, Frith CD, Poline J-B, Heather JD, Frackowiak RSJ. graphic scan and predicting recovery of comprehension in Wernicke’s
Spatial registration and normalization of images. Hum Brain Mapp 1995a; aphasia. Arch Neurol 1987; 44: 73–82.
2: 165–88. Noppeney U, Price CJ, Duncan JS, Koepp MJ. Reading skills after left
Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Worsley KJ, Poline J-B, Frith CD, Frackowiak RSJ. anterior temporal lobe resection: an fMRI study. Brain 2005;
Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear Jun;128(Pt 6): 1377–85.
approach. Hum Brain Mapp 1995b; 2: 189–210. Rosen HJ, Petersen SE, Linenweber MR, Synder AZ, White DA, Chapman L,
Galton CJ, Patterson K, Graham K, Lambon-Ralph MA, Williams G, et al. Neural correlates of recovery from aphasia after damage to left inferior
Antoun N, et al. Differing patterns of temporal atrophy in Alzheimer’s frontal cortex. Neurology 2000; 55: 1883–94.
disease and semantic dementia. Neurology 2001; 57: 216–25. Selnes OA, Knopman DS, Niccum N, Rubens AB, Larson D. Computed
Hart J, Gordon B. Delineation of single-word semantic comprehension tomographic scan correlates of auditory comprehension deficits in aphasia:
deficits in aphasia, with anatomical correlation. Ann Neurol 1990; 27: a prospective recovery study. Ann Neurol 1983; 13: 558–66.
226–31. Sharp DJ, Scott SK, Wise RJ. Retrieving meaning after temporal lobe infarc-
Heiss WD, Kessler J, Karbe H, Fink GR, Pawlik G. Cerebral glucose meta- tion: the role of the basal language area. Ann Neurol 2004; 56: 836–46.
bolism as a predictor of recovery from aphasia in ischemic stroke. Arch ShaywitzSE,ShaywitzBA,FulbrightRK,SkudlarskiP,MenclWE,ConstableRT,
Neurol 1993; 50: 958–64. et al. Neural systems for compensation and persistence: young adult out-
Heiss WD, Karbe H, Weber-Luxenburger G, Herholz K, Kessler J, Pietrzyk U, come of childhood reading disability. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 54: 25–33.
et al. Speech-induced cerebral metabolic activation reflects recovery from Swinburn, K, Baker, G, and Howard, D. CAT: The Comprehensive Aphasia
aphasia. J Neurol Sci 1997; 145: 213–7. Test. New York: Psychology Press; 2005.
Heiss WD, Kessler J, Thiel A, Ghaemi M, Karbe H. Differential capacity of left Talairach J, Tornoux M. Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. New
and right hemispheric areas for compensation of poststroke aphasia. Ann York: Thieme Medical Publishers, 1988.
Neurol 1999; 45: 430–8. Warburton E, Swinburn K, Price CJ, Wise RJS. Mechanisms of recovery from
Hermann BP, Seidenberg M, Haltiner A, Wyler AR. Adequacy of language aphasia: evidence from positron emission tomographic studies. J Neurol
function and verbal memory performance in unilateral temporal lobe Neurosurg Psychiatr 1999; 66: 155–61.
epilepsy. Cortex 1992; 28: 423–33. Weiller C, Isensee C, Rijntjes M, Huber W, Muller S, Bier D, et al. Recovery
Hickok G, Erhard P, Kassubek J, Helms-Tillery AK, Naeve-Velguth S, Strupp from Wernicke’s aphasia: a positron emission tomographic study. Ann
JP, et al. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of the role of left Neurol 1995; 37: 723–32.
posterior superior temporal gyrus in speech production: implications for Wildgruber D, Hertrich I, Riecker A, Erb M, Anders S, Grodd W, et al.
the explanation of conduction aphasia. Neurosci Lett 2000; 287: 156–60. Distinct frontal regions subserve evaluation of linguistic and emotional
Hines D. Independent functioning of the two cerebral hemispheres for recog- aspects of speech intonation. Cereb Cortex 2004; 14: 1384–9.
nizing bilaterally presented tachistoscopic visual-half-field stimuli. Cortex Wilson BA, Baddeley A. Spontaneous recovery of impaired memory span:
1975; 11: 132–43. does comprehension recover? Cortex 1993; 29: 153–9.
Hund-Georgiadis M, Lex U, Friederici AD, von Cramon DY. Non-invasive Wise RJ, Scott SK, Blank SC, Mummery CJ, Murphy K, Warburton EA.
regime for language lateralization in right- and left-handers by Separate neural subsystems within ‘Wernicke’s area’. Brain 2001; 124:
means of functional MRI and dichotic listening. Exp Brain Res 2002; 83–95.
145: 166–76. Young AW, Ellis AW. Different methods of lexical access for words
Just MA, Carpenter PA. A capacity theory of comprehension: individual presented in the left and right visual hemifields. Brain Lang 1985; 24:
differences in working memory. Psychol Rev 1992; 99: 122–49. 326–58.
Karbe H, Thiel A, Weber-Luxenburger G, Herholz K, Kessler J, Heiss WD. Zahn R, Huber W, Drews E, Specht K, Kemeny S, Reith W, et al. Recovery of
Brain plasticity in poststroke aphasia: what is the contribution of the right semantic word processing in transcortical sensory aphasia: a functional
hemisphere? Brain Lang 1998; 64: 215–30. magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurocase 2002; 8: 376–86.
Kessler J, Thiel A, Karbe H, Heiss WD. Piracetam improves activated blood Zahn R, Drews E, Specht K, Kemeny S, Reith W, Willmes K, et al. Recovery of
flow and facilitates rehabilitation of poststroke aphasic patients. Stroke semantic word processing in global aphasia: a functional MRI study. Brain
2000; 31: 2112–6. Res Cogn Brain Res 2004; 18: 322–36.
Lambon Ralph MA, McClelland JL, Patterson K, Galton CJ, Hodges JR. No Zaidel E. Auditory vocabulary of the right hemisphere following brain bisec-
right to speak? The relationship between object naming and semantic tion or hemidecortication. Cortex 1976; 12: 191–211.
impairment: neuropsychological evidence and a computational model. Zaidel E, Kasher A, Soroker N, Batori G, Giora R, Graves D. Hemispheric
J Cogn Neurosci 2001; 13: 341–56. contributions to pragmatics. Brain Cogn 2000; 43: 438–43.

You might also like