Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 689e702

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

Full length article

The role of persuasion knowledge, assessment of benefit and harm,


and third-person perception in coping with online behavioral
advertising
Chang-Dae Ham a, *, Michelle R. Nelson b
a
The Charles H. Sandage Department of Advertising, College of Media, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 330Gregory Hall, MC-462, 810 S. Wright
St., Urbana, IL, 61801, USA
b
The Charles H. Sandage Department of Advertising, College of Media, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 323Gregory Hall, MC-462, 810 S. Wright
St., Urbana, 61801, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Online behavioral advertising (OBA) provides Internet users with potential benefit (relevant ads) and
Received 6 February 2016 harm (privacy infringement) through personalized covert persuasion tactics, making it an interesting
Received in revised form case for understanding perceptions of media effects. In study 1 (survey), we found that subjective
22 March 2016
persuasion knowledge of OBA was positively related to third-person perception (TPP). Importantly, the
Accepted 24 March 2016
assessment of the potential harm and benefit of OBA mediated the relationship between subjective
persuasion knowledge and TPP. Objective persuasion knowledge however was only indirectly related to
TPP via subjective persuasion knowledge. TPP related to personal (accept, avoid OBA) but not social-level
Keywords:
Online behavioral advertising
(pro-regulation) coping measures. Study 2 (experiment) replicated study 1 and showed that the
Persuasion knowledge perceived effect of OBA on self (not others) drove the TPP and the responses to personal outcome
Third-person perception measures. Implications for understanding perceived media effects and response to OBA are discussed.
Benefit-harm assessment © 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction At the same time, the methods for targeting include collecting
individual-level behavior data, which consumers do not always like
“Consumers Conflicted About Privacy Vs. Personalization” (Nill & Aalberts, 2014). Baek and Morimoto (2012, p. 61) warn that
Marketing Daily (Mahoney, 2015) … “highly personalized ad messages may raise consumer fears
about loss of freedom to control the use of private information.”
Indeed, in the survey cited above, only about one third of those
This headline showcases the consumer ambivalence of the harm
same consumers agreed with this statement: “I use free services
and benefit of online behavioral advertising (OBA; Smit, van Noort,
online and on smartphones/tablets and don't mind if my data is
& Voorveld, 2014). OBA tracks online behaviors and generates a
potentially also used for advertising purposes” (Marshall, 2014).
profile based on the user's interests, preferences, etc. (McDonald &
Consumers worry about the potential harm of the use of their
Cranor, 2010; van Noort, Smit, & Voorveld, 2013). The practice
personal information, which is private (Okazaki, Li, & Hirose, 2009)
benefits the audience by providing more relevant advertising (Nill &
and they may not like the idea of being manipulated (Simonson,
Aalberts, 2014). Almost half of U.S. consumers surveyed about on-
2005). The irony is that “while some consumers see benefit in
line advertising agreed that: “Advertising that is tailored to my
tailored ads, most don't want their information to be used to tailor
needs is helpful because I can find the right products and services
them” (Marshall, 2014).
more quickly” (Marshall, 2014). Indeed, attitudes toward person-
Given the ambivalent consumer attitudes toward OBA and
alized advertising have been found to predict purchases online
varying perceptions of its usefulness, OBA provides an interesting
(Eastin, Brinson, Doorey, & Wilcox, 2016).
context for studying perceived advertising effects. In general,
people do not want to believe that they personally are affected by
advertising although they believe that others may be affected
* Corresponding author.
(Eisend, 2015). This perception is based on third-person perception
E-mail addresses: cdham317@illinois.edu (C.-D. Ham), nelsonmr@illinois.edu
(M.R. Nelson). (TPP; Davison, 1983), which refers to differences in perceptions of

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.076
0747-5632/© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
690 C.-D. Ham, M.R. Nelson / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 689e702

the influence of communication on the self and on others (Chang, distinguishes OBA from other personalized advertising, in which
Wei, & Lo, 2014). A robust body of literature has demonstrated consumers know, agree, or permit prior to receiving (e.g., opt-in
TPP effects across various kinds of media (Conners, 2005), including personalized email messages). OBA's covert mechanism poten-
online environments (e.g., Chen & Ng, 2016). However, a key tially harms consumers when they are tracked without their
moderating variable of TPP is the message desirability (Sun, Pan, & recognition of, or full agreement with, the potential risk of privacy
Shen, 2008). If the message is desirable, the perceptions are infringement.
reduced or reversed. As discussed above, consumers' perceptions of As a form of advertising, OBA will likely elicit typical third-
the desirability of OBA may depend on their perception of the person perception (TPP, i.e., people tend to overestimate commu-
benefit and harm associated with the persuasion tactic. nication effects on others whereas they underestimate effects on
Ambivalent attitudes may be caused by the technological themselves; Perloff, 1999). Individuals are motivated to enhance
complexity of OBA and consumers' lack of knowledge. Not many their positive self-esteem by rejecting negative or undesirable
consumers know details about OBA (Jai, Burns, & King, 2013) or persuasion attempts as well as by accepting positive or desirable
how such relevant ads are displayed to them (McDonald & Cranor, ones, according to self-enhancement theory (Perloff, 1999). If OBA
2010; Smit et al., 2014). Thus, knowledge about OBA or how it is deemed to be undesirable, then there is presumed to be a larger
operates (e.g., “persuasion knowledge”; Friestad & Wright, 1994) effect on others, whereas if it is perceived as desirable, the pre-
may relate to the propensity to exhibit TPP. Therefore, we examine sumed effect might be reduced or reversed. However, desirability of
the role of persuasion knowledge of OBA in gauging OBA's OBA seems ambivalent. More importantly, the complexity of OBA
perceived effects on self and others. There may be differences in may be an obstacle for consumers to determine whether OBA will
how people process information, especially if it is technical infor- be beneficial or harmful. Thus, knowledge of how OBA works may be
mation, depending on if they have “objective” knowledge, which is a significant determinant in the perceived desirability of OBA.
“accurate stored information that consumers possess” (Carlson, Knowledge influences TPP. Studies typically show that it is the
Bearden, & Hardesty, 2007) or if they have “subjective” knowl- perception of knowledge and the confidence that arises out of that
edge (e.g., perception of having knowledge about the persuasion or knowledge that may be driving TPP effects (e.g., Driscoll & Salwen,
“persuasion knowledge”; Friestad & Wright, 1994). 1997; Huh & Langteau, 2007). People who perceived themselves as
Finally, consumers may cope with persuasion in many ways more knowledgeable in a communication topic were more likely to
(from accepting to resisting; Fransen, Verlegh, Kirmani, & Smit, consider themselves as more capable of coping with the commu-
2015); therefore, we investigate multiple intended behavioral nication, but to consider others as less capable as they were,
outcome variables for TPP including personal coping responses resulting in eliciting greater TPP. Knowledge about persuasion
(e.g., blocking advertising) as well as “social” coping related to tactics has been explained by the Persuasion Knowledge Model
support of OBA regulation. Understanding the coping mechanisms (PKM; Friestad & Wright, 1994). The PKM proposes that people (the
is particularly timely because the Federal Trade Commission (FTC) persuasion target) have their own knowledge or beliefs about how
is considering regulations for OBA (Nill & Aalberts, 2014). In the persuasion works to help them cope with marketers' (persuasion
meantime, increasing numbers of consumers are blocking adver- agents') persuasion tactics. When perceiving a hidden persuasion
tising, which may damage the advertising industry in the future intent, people are likely to activate this persuasion knowledge,
(Manjoo, 2015). which enables them to actively cope with the tactic, either by
Typically, TPP research measures individuals' perceptions of resisting or accepting the persuasion tactic. We propose that the
undesirable media effects on self and others to examine social level of persuasion knowledge (PK) of the persuasion tactic (OBA)
outcomes such as support for regulation (e.g., Shah, Faber, & Youn, will determine the magnitude of TPP: i.e., when consumers are
1999). Our research examines an ambivalent media context where confronted with a covert advertising tactic, persuasion knowledge
the assessment of harm and benefit is an important factors for influences their perceptions of the ad's effect on themself or others.
understanding perceptions of effects. In addition, we disentangle
perceived effects on self and others to discern which perceived ef- 2.2. Objective and subjective persuasion knowledge
fect drives social and personal outcome measures. Although some
studies have examined the relationship between knowledge and While we believe that PK will be positively associated with TPP,
TPP (e.g., Huh & Langteau, 2007; Driscoll & Salwen, 1997), scholars we argue that we should look at how different dimensions of PK
have not yet investigated how different forms of knowledge may be associated with TPP. Although not specified in the original
(objective, subjective) relate to TPP in this ambivalent media PKM model (Friestad & Wright, 1994), Carlson et al. (2007) pro-
context. Our research therefore contributes to the existing litera- posed that objective persuasion knowledge (PK) should be distin-
ture on PKM and TPP by examining more closely the relationships guished from subjective persuasion knowledge (PK). Subjective PK is
between knowledge and perceptions of effects by assessing harm an individual's self-assessed perception about how persuasion
and benefit and gauging the multiple personal and outcomes that works (e.g., “I know how OBA displays personalized ads to me”)
can occur in this context. whereas objective PK is accurate information about a specific
persuasion tactic (e.g., “Consent is not required for marketers to
2. Literature review track users' online activities to display personalized ad messages”),
which is stored in long-term memory (Carlson et al., 2007). The fact
2.1. Persuasion knowledge and third-person perception in OBA that an individual has subjective PK does not necessarily mean the
person has accurate or objective knowledge about a specific
Online behavioral advertising (OBA) is a technology-driven persuasion tactic. Alba and Hutchinson (2000) argued that sub-
advertising personalization method that enables advertisers to jective and objective knowledge are distinct and can produce
deliver highly relevant ad messages to individuals (McDonald & different effects on consumer assessment.
Cranor, 2010; van Noort et al., 2013). However, OBA's tracking on- Past research shows mixed results with respect to the link be-
line activities, collecting behavioral data, and disseminating infor- tween knowledge and TPP (e.g., Huh & Langteau, 2007; Driscoll &
mation conflicts with consumer privacy because OBA's persuasion Salwen, 1997; Wei & Lo, 2008), but no studies have examined
mechanism is covert, not requiring consumers' recognition, how objective PK, not knowledge of an object or topic, works on
agreement, or permission (Nill & Aalberts, 2014). Such covertness TPP. Further, given that OBA may be a context for which consumers
C.-D. Ham, M.R. Nelson / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 689e702 691

do not know objective technical information about how it works this knowledge will be activated after inferring the persuasion
(e.g., Smit et al., 2014), we pose a research question with respect to intent/tactic (subjective PK) and will significantly influence their
the relationship between objective PK and TPP. coping responses.
This benefit-harm assessment should significantly influence
RQ1. How will be objective persuasion knowledge of OBA be
third-person perception. Gunther and Mundy (1993) asserted “the
related to third-person perception?
concept of harmful vs. beneficial outcome is a central one in
Studies in TPP have typically shown that it is the perception of theoretical research on the optimistic bias phenomenon e the
knowledge (i.e., subjective knowledge) and the confidence that tendency for people to think that they are less likely to have
arises out of that knowledge that may be driving TPP effects negative or undesirable experiences than others” (p. 60). In
(Driscoll & Salwen, 1997; Huh & Langteau, 2007). Relevant in our essence, people tend to boost their ego by thinking they are smarter
context, we assume that subjective PK about OBA will elicit TPP, than others and consider others to be more vulnerable to harmful
assuming that as people gain subjective PK, they are more likely to persuasion attempts. The researchers found that TPP is largely
perceive the effect of OBA is larger on others than on themselves. influenced by the result of benefit-harm assessment. People pre-
Beyond objective knowledge, subjective knowledge relates to the dicted the media effect was greater on others than on themselves
knowledge about the tactic that respondents believe they have. In when an outcome was predicted to be more harmful than benefi-
response to a candy and a finance advertisement, Eisend (2015) cial. Thus, we assume that once consumers have PK about OBA, they
showed that as PK increased, TPP also increased. Thus, we posit. will weigh OBA's benefits (e.g., relevance of advertising) versus
perceptions of harm (e.g., privacy infringement; Nill & Aalberts,
H1. Subjective persuasion knowledge of OBA will be positively
2014), which will determine a negative or positive association
associated with third-person perception.
with TPP. Thus, we posit:
H2a, b. Assessment of (a) benefits (relevance) will be negatively,
whereas their assessment of (b) harm (privacy infringement) will
2.3. Outcomes of persuasion knowledge: assessing harm and
be positively, associated with third-person perceptions.
benefits of OBA
In addition to individual-level predictions for PK and TPP, we
Most PKM studies examined how persuasion knowledge leads examine relationships among the multiple dimensions of persua-
consumers to resist or reject persuasion attempts (Campbell & sion knowledge and TPP. First, objective knowledge about a specific
Kirmani, 2008), but subjective PK does not always lead to resis- persuasion tactic should be a basis for eliciting and elaborating
tance against persuasion. For example, van Reijmersdal, Tutaj, and subjective PK. Online persuasion tactics such as OBA, in particular,
Boerman (2013) found that subjective persuasion knowledge, require some basic technological knowledge to correctly recognize
which was activated by a sponsorship disclosure in a movie, did not and understand how the tactic works (e.g., third-party cookie,
result in more skeptical attitudes toward product placements. behavioral tracking, etc.). Most studies have revealed a positive
Similarly, Campbell, Mohr, and Verlegh (2013) showed that sub- relationship between objective and subjective knowledge (e.g.,
jective PK did not elicit significant attitude correction in a negative Brucks, 1985; Carlson et al., 2007). Therefore, we expect that
manner when the persuasion tactic was disclosed to the viewers objective PK will be related to subjective PK, which in turn, is
before they saw the covert tactic in a TV show. Finally, Boerman, associated with TPP, assuming that subjective PK will mediate the
van Reijmersdal and Neijens (2012) found that sponsorship dis- relationship in between. We however do not hypothesize but ask if
closures did not directly lead to consumers' skepticism or disliking objective PK is related to TPP. Thus, we ask a research question
of a tactic, but indirectly influenced these outcomes through sub- instead of hypothesis.
jective PK. All of these examples support the idea that subjective PK
RQ2. Will subjective persuasion knowledge mediate the rela-
may not always lead to attitudinal and behavioral change. We as-
tionship between objective persuasion knowledge and third-
sume that there should be mediating variables.
person perception?
In this regard, Kirmani and Campbell (2004) argued that in-
dividuals actively interact with persuasion agents to achieve their Second, we postulate that subjective PK will relate to assess-
goals, rather than simply accepting or resisting persuasion at- ments of the privacy harm and relevance benefit because, as we
tempts. Persuasion knowledge allows people to infer the persua- proposed, both assessments are outcomes of PK. Goal-pursuing
sion's potential cooperative and manipulative intent, resulting in individuals actively manage a marketplace interaction, choosing
taking either a compliance or resistance strategy. When a persuasion the best response strategies to cope with persuasion attempts, once
attempt is assessed as beneficial to achieve the goal, activated PK they perceive the persuasion intent (Kirmani & Campbell, 2004);
would not lead to resistance but to a compliance strategy; activated thus, subjective PK is an antecedent to assess benefit/harm. Taken
PK would generate a resistance strategy when the persuasion is together, we expect that subjective PK is positively related to
assessed as harmful, however (Kirmani & Campbell, 2004). We benefit and harm assessment, which in turn, is associated with TPP,
conceptualize these inferences e the perception of harm or benefit assuming that the assessment of the privacy harm and relevance
- as an outcome of PK, and propose that the perception mediates benefit will mediate the relationship between subjective PK and
the effect of PK on coping behaviors. This construct relates to tar- TPP. Thus, we posit that.
gets' beliefs about how the persuasion attempts influence them
H3. Assessment of benefit and harm will mediate the relationship
representing “the nature of the causal relationship between an
between subjective persuasion knowledge and third-person
agent's actions, and the psychological effects those actions produce
perception.
in targets” (Friestad & Wright, 1994, p. 5) and “the idea about what
makes persuasion tactics effective” (Kirmani & Campbell, 2009, p.
302). Upon these definitions, we specify that this construct is goal-
pursuing individuals' intuitive knowledge that enables them to 2.4. Third-person perception and social and personal level coping
assess how and to what extent the persuasion attempt will be responses
beneficial or harmful to them. Benefit/harm assessment is con-
sumers' general response to any persuasion situation. Accordingly, Most studies have shown that third-person perception is
692 C.-D. Ham, M.R. Nelson / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 689e702

positively associated with social protective behaviors (Perloff, 1999) (M ¼ 12.7; SD ¼ 4.85).
such as intention to support censorship of alcohol ads (Shah et al.,
1999) and sexually explicit media (Lee & Yang, 1996). The perceived 3.2. Procedure
threat of such undesirable messages motivated individuals to pro-
tect more vulnerable and susceptible others in the society, called Participants visited the online survey website (Qualtrics.com)
“gullible publics,” (Gunther, 1995; Gunther & Hwa, 1996; Salwen, where they agreed with the consent form, read the definition and
1998). Paternalism, defined as acting like parents to protect chil- examples of OBA, and answered questions about OBA. They first
dren from potential harm, explains why people are willing to answered two screening questions that asked if they knew OBA and
protect others who are perceived as more vulnerable than them- if they experienced OBA in the last six months. Those who
selves (McLeod, Eveland, & Nathanson, 1997) and to support answered ‘no’ in either screening question (32 students, 53 non-
censorship (Chia, Lu, & McLeod, 2004). students) were guided to exit the survey and were excluded.
Not all the studies supported such a positive relationship,
however (e.g., Price, Tewksbury, & Huang, 1998; Driscoll & Salwen, 3.3. Measurements
1997). The positive relationship was not observed when pater-
nalism was not sufficiently evoked. Even a negative relationship can Each instrument was slightly modified for the OBA context and
occur (between TPP and social protective actions) when the measured with 7-point Likert scales (1: strongly disagree; 7:
persuasion is considered a personal level issue; that is, when the strongly agree) except Objective PK. It was measured by eight items,
perceived harm was expected to significantly influence the self adopted from previous studies (FTC, 2009; McDonald & Cranor,
(e.g., H1N1 swine flu), individuals were more concerned about their 2010; Turow, King, Hoofnagle, Bleakley, & Hennessy, 2009) and
own rather than others' safety, resulting in them being less likely to screened by two industry experts. A point was allocated to the right
take social level protective actions (Liu & Lo, 2014). answer among three choices (true; false; I don't know); points were
OBA presents an interesting case for examining the relationship accumulated to represent individuals' objective PK. Subjective PK
between TPP and behavioral coping. Consumers' coping responses was measured by six items (M ¼ 4.97; SD ¼ 1.07; Cronbach's
will be determined by whether they perceive OBA's negative effect a ¼ 0.811) modified from Bearden, Hardesty, and Rose (2001).
as a social (i.e., affects others more than self) or a personal (affects self Privacy harm (six items; M ¼ 5.24; SD ¼ 1.35; Cronbach’s a ¼ 0.921)
more than others) issue, and further, whether the effects are and benefit (six items; M ¼ 3.85; SD ¼ 1.47; Cronbach’s a ¼ 0.936)
considered to be beneficial or harmful. When individuals perceive were modified from Woon, Tan, and Low (2005). Third-person
the OBA effect is more a personal issue than social, they will be more perception was calculated by subtracting the perceived OBA effect on
concerned about their own personal rather than others' risk or others (two items; M ¼ 4.33; SD ¼ 1.37; Cronbach’s a ¼ 0.920) from
benefit. People could enlist their own personal responses to that of OBA on self (two items; M ¼ 3.34; SD ¼ 1.65; Cronbach’s
respond to persuasion tactics, which range from acceptance to a ¼ 0.905), following prior research (e.g., Tewksbury et al., 2004).
resistance (e.g., Fransen et al., 2015), by accepting or actively Intention to support OBA regulation (two items; M ¼ 4.96;
avoiding, ignoring or blocking the ads. Since the desirability of the SD ¼ 1.60; Cronbach's a ¼ 0.922) was modified from Paradise and
OBA effect is determined by individuals' persuasion knowledge and Sullivan (2012) and Golan and Banning (2008); Intention to block
assessment of harm and benefit (rather than simply being a priori OBA (three items; M ¼ 4.96; SD ¼ 1.63; Cronbach’s a ¼ 0.951) was
positive or negative), we propose the following research questions, modified from Paradise and Sullivan (2012); OBA acceptance (two
rather than establishing a hypothesis, for how people cope with items; M ¼ 4.11; SD ¼ 1.62; Cronbach’s a ¼ 0.792) was modified
OBA in both social (intent to support regulation) and personal (intent from McDonald and Cranor (2010) and Turow et al. (2009); OBA
to block) responses across behavioral (intent) and perceptual avoidance (five items; M ¼ 4.56; SD ¼ 1.50; Cronbach’s a ¼ 0.900)
(avoidance and acceptance) levels. was modified from Cho and Cheon (2004) and Speck and Elliott
(1997). In the questionnaire, objective PK was measured first, fol-
RQ3a-d. How will third-person perception be related to (a) intent
lowed by subjective PK, assessment of benefit and harm, third-
to support OBA regulation, (b) intent to block OBA, (c) acceptance of
person perception, and coping responses. Descriptive statistics,
OBA, and (d) avoidance of OBA? Specifically, will the perceived
measurement items, and reliability indices are described in Table 1.
effect on self or effect on others be a stronger predictor of those
Finally, inter-construct validity was checked by bivariate correla-
coping responses?
tions. The results are found in Table 2.
Combining all the hypotheses and research questions together,
we visualize a holistic conceptual model that we postulate in this 4. Study 1 e results
study (see Fig. 1).
Research question 1 and Hypotheses 1 and 2 ab examined how
3. Study 1 (survey) e method objective PK, subjective PK, and outcomes of PK (benefit and harm
assessment) were related to TPP. We ran a hierarchical regression
3.1. Participants analysis with model 1 in which demographic variables were
controlled, and model 2 in which objective PK, subjective PK, and
Using convenience sampling, we collected 378 samples across harm and benefit assessment were tested. In model 1 (R2 ¼ 0.059;
two demographic groups. Students (N ¼ 191) were recruited from a F ¼ 5.82; p < 0.001), Internet experience (b ¼ 0.166; p < 0.01) and
Midwestern university in exchange for course extra credit, and student status (b ¼ 0.159; p < 0.05) were significant and controlled.
non-students (N ¼ 187) were recruited via Amazon Mechanical More experienced people (years of using Internet) and non-
Turk (non-student U.S. residents) in return for a small monetary students (vs. students) showed greater TPP. First, the result of
compensation. Age varied from 18 to 76 (M ¼ 28.11; SD ¼ 11.53); model 2 (R2 changes ¼ 0.150; F ¼ 12.11; p < 0.001) answered RQ1
females (N ¼ 243; 64.3%) outnumbered males (N ¼ 133; 35.4%); and that objective PK was not significantly associated with TPP
Caucasian (N ¼ 240; 63.5%) was the major ethnicity, followed by (b ¼ 0.006; n.s.); see Table 3.
Asian (N ¼ 66; 17.5%), Asian American (N ¼ 27; 7.1%), African Second, the result of the H1 test revealed that subjective PK (in
American (N ¼ 17; 4.5%), Hispanic (N ¼ 11; 2.9%), and others model 2: b ¼ 0.175; p < 0.001) was positively associated with TPP.
(N ¼ 17; 4.4%). Internet experience varied from 2 to 20 years People who perceived that they have more PK about OBA perceived
C.-D. Ham, M.R. Nelson / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 689e702 693

Fig. 1. Conceptual model.

greater TPP. Thus, H1 was supported. Post-hoc analysis (each hi- intent to support OBA regulation, (RQ2b) intent to block OBA,
erarchical analysis for effect on others and on self: see Table 3) (RQ2c) acceptance of OBA, and (RQ2d) avoidance of OBA. After
revealed that subjective PK was positively associated with the effect controlling demographic variables in model 1 and persuasion
on others (b ¼ 0.122; p < 0.05) but not with the effect on self knowledge variables in model 2, the relationships of TPP with
(b ¼ 0.047; n.s.). The positive subjective PK-TPP association coping responses were tested in model 3 (see Table 3).
occurred mainly due to a greater perceived effect on others. The result of RQ2a showed that TPP was not significantly related
Third, the result of the H2a test showed that the perceived to regulation intention (in model 3: b ¼ 0.105; n.s.). The result of
(relevance) benefit (b ¼ 0.329; p < 0.001) was negatively related the RQ2b test demonstrated that TPP was positively related to
with TPP. Follow-up post-hoc analyses (see Table 3) revealed that intent to block OBA (model 3: b ¼ 0.152; p < 0.01). Post-hoc ana-
the perceived (relevance) benefit positively related to both lyses showed that the positive relationship was caused mainly by a
perceived effects on others and on self, but the magnitude of effects perceived effect on self (b ¼ 0.251; p < 0.001) but not by an effect
was greater on self (b ¼ 0.690; p < 0.001) than on others (b ¼ 0.490; on others (b ¼ 0.088; n.s.). Considering that effect on self was
p < 0.001). The negative relationship between benefit assessment perceived as more beneficial (post-hoc analysis of H3ab), we
and TPP occurred mainly because the benefit was perceived to be concluded that respondents perceiving greater TPP (i.e., perceiving
greater on self than on others. On the other hand, the result of the lesser beneficial effect on self) were more likely to block OBA.
H2b test showed that the perceived (privacy) harm (b ¼ 0.100; The result of the RQ2c test showed that TPP was positively
p < 0.05) was positively associated with TPP. Post-hoc analyses related to OBA avoidance (in model 3: b ¼ 0.150; p < 0.001). Post-
revealed that (privacy) harm was only positively associated with hoc analyses showed that the positive relationship was caused
effect on others (b ¼ 0.071; p < 0.05) but not with effects on self mainly by a perceived effect on self (b ¼ 0.272; p < 0.001) but not
(b ¼ 0.026; n.s.). The positive harm-TPP relationship occurred by the effect on others (b ¼ 0.067; n.s.). If they perceived greater TPP
mainly due to a greater perceived harm effect on others. Thus, both (i.e., as they perceived that OBA was a less beneficial effect on self),
H2a and H2b were supported. respondents were more likely to avoid OBA. The result of the RQ2d
Fourth, mediation (RQ2) was examined using the Preacher and test showed that TPP was negatively related to OBA acceptance (in
Hayes' (2008) PROCESS macro. The result revealed that objective model 3: b ¼ 0.124; p < 0.01). Post-hoc analyses revealed that the
PK was not significantly related to TPP as we found in RQ1, so the negative relationship was caused mainly by effect on self (b ¼ 0.209;
mediation was not established. But we found that objective PK was p < 0.001) but not by effect on others (b ¼ 0.069; n.s.). If they
significantly related to subjective PK (r ¼ 0.20; P < 0.001). The perceived less TPP (i.e., as they perceived a greater beneficial effect
answer to RQ2 was that subjective PK did not mediate the rela- on self), respondents were more likely to accept OBA.
tionship between objective PK and TPP. Next, multiple mediation
(benefit and harm) analyses for H3 was tested using the Preacher
and Hayes' (2008) bootstrapping analysis with the PROCESS 5. Study 1 e discussion
macro. As seen in Fig. 2, subjective PK was significantly related to
the assessment of benefits (r ¼ 0.17; p < 0.05) and harm (r ¼ 0.35; Given that OBA is a persuasion tactic, we used persuasion
p < 0.001); benefit (r ¼ 0.28; P < 0.001) and harm (r ¼ 0.12; knowledge (PK) as the type of knowledge with two constructs:
p < 0.05) were significantly related to TPP; when the dependent objective and subjective PK. Whereas objective PK was positively
variable (TPP) was regressed on both the independent (PK) and related to subjective PK, only subjective PK was positively related to
mediating variables (benefit: 0.37 to 0.17; harm: 0.02 to 0.21), TPP. Consistent with Eisend (2015), the greater the subjective PK,
PK's relationship with TPP was still significant but weakened the larger the TPP gap. We, however, found that objective PK (i.e.,
(r ¼ 0.24; p < 0.01). The result revealed that assessments of benefit what people actually know about OBA) was not directly related to
and harm partially mediated the relationship between subjective consumers' TPP but indirectly related via subjective PK. This finding
PK and TPP. Specifically, benefit negatively and harm positively is similar to Lasorsa (1989) who showed that those people who had
mediated the relationship. Thus, H3 was supported (see Fig. 2). real (objective) political knowledge did not exhibit TPP effects
Research questions 3a to d asked how TPP related to (RQ2a) about a television series, whereas those with perceived knowledge
demonstrated TPP effects. The belief that people know about the
694 C.-D. Ham, M.R. Nelson / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 689e702

Table 1
Measurement and descriptive statistics.

Construct and measurement items M(SD)

Objective Persuasion Knowledge


When a user visits some websites, the same ads will be displayed as when someone else visits the same websites 243/64.3%a
Consent is not required for marketers to track users' online activities to display personalized ad messages 117/31.0%a
Internet users can select an opt-out choice to stop advertisers from tracking their online activities 285/75.4%a
Users can choose to select the type of online activity data that can be collected by marketers 271/71.7%a
If a user does not accept cookie tracking, advertisers are not able to tell where the user has browsed on the web 122/32.3%a
If the same ad is consistently displayed while a user browses the web, that means the user's online activities are being tracked by marketers 186/49.2%a
Advertisers can use cookie files on multiple websites in order to learn which websites the user visits 288/76.2%a
Now there is a federal law that regulates online marketers' tracking of users' online browsing history 31/8.2%a
Subjective Persuasion Knowledge (M ¼ 4.97; SD ¼ 1.07; Cronbach's a ¼ 0.811)
I know how OBA displays personalized ads to me 4.75(1.59)
I can tell OBA has strings attached, requiring my online behavioral information 0.96(1.44)
I understand how a marketer shows the personalized ads to me using behavior tracking 4.87(1.50)
I know how online marketers offer the tailored information to me 5.28(1.27)
I can see through ad personalization technology used to get me to buy in the online advertising 4.99(1.30)
I can separate benefit and harm for the persuasion tactic of OBA 4.96(1.06)
Privacy Harm Assessment (M ¼ 5.24; SD ¼ 1.35; Cronbach's a ¼ 0.921)
I feel uncomfortable when my online behaviors are tracked without permission 5.44(1.57)
I am concerned about misuse of my online behaviors 5.38(1.61)
It bothers me to receive too much advertising material through tracking of my online behaviors 5.23(1.61)
I fear that my online behavior information may not be safe while stored 5.06(1.63)
I believe that my online behavioral data is often misused 5.02(1.59)
I think companies share my online behavioral data without permission 5.29(1.54)
Relevance Benefit Assessment (M ¼ 3.85; SD ¼ 1.47; Cronbach's a ¼ 0.936)
By disclosing my online browsing history in OBA, I could get more relevant information 4.42(1.63)
By disclosing my online browsing history in OBA, I could get more relevant entertainment 4.19(1.68)
By disclosing my online browsing history in OBA, I could get more relevant messages to my social image 4.11(1.67)
OBA allows me to get more valuable information 4.02(1.65)
OBA allows me to get more exciting entertainment 3.26(1.74)
OBA allows me to improve my social image 3.27(1.79)
Third-person perception (me: M ¼ 3.34; SD ¼ 1.65; others: M ¼ 4.33; SD ¼ 1.37; Cronbach's a: me ¼ 0.905; others ¼ 0.920)
How much influence do you think OBA has on you? (1: not at all e 7: very much) 3.57(1.74)
OBA has a powerful effect on me 3.11(1.70)
How much influence do you think OBA has on others in this society? (1: not at all e 7: very much) 4.33(1.42)
OBA has a powerful effect on others in this society 4.34(1.43)
Intention to support OBA regulation (M ¼ 4.96; SD ¼ 1.60; Cronbach's a ¼ 0.922)
I'm willing to support that government should regulate OBA 4.98(1.67)
My willingness to support government's regulation of OBA is (1: very unlikely e 7: very likely) 4.94(1.64)
Intention to block OBA (M ¼ 4.96; SD ¼ 1.63; Cronbach's a ¼ 0.951)
How much are you willing to block OBA?
(1: unlikely e 7: likely) 4.85(1.78)
(1: impossible e 7: possible) 5.16(1.57)
(1: improbable e 7: probable) 4.86(1.77)
Ad (OBA) Acceptance (M ¼ 4.11; SD ¼ 1.62; Cronbach's a ¼ 0.792)
I prefer OBA that target my interests 4.50(1.72)
It's okay to see OBA that are based on my online surfing history 3.74(1.85)
Ad (OBA) Avoidance (M ¼ 4.56; SD ¼ 1.50; Cronbach's a ¼ 0.900)
I intentionally ignore OBA delivered by behavioral targeting tactics 4.56(1.82)
I hate OBA message delivered by behavioral targeting tactics 4.10(1.81)
It would be better if there were no OBA, behavior-tracking advertising 4.41(1.79)
I discard (leave the webpage of, scroll down to hide) OBA's behavioral tracking 4.88(1.76)
I intend to ask OBA marketers to take me off their behavioral targeting lists 4.85(1.69)
a
Note: No. of right answers/% of right answer; OBA: online behavioral advertising (the abbreviation is only used in the table); Correct answer: 1(no); 2(yes); 3(yes); 4(no);
5(no): 6(yes); 7 (yes); 8 (no).

Table 2
Correlation matrix of constructs.

Objective PK Subjective PK Impact on Impact on Relevance benefit Privacy harm Intent to Intent to Avoidance Acceptance
self others regulate block

Objective PK 1
Subjective PK 0.196** 1
Impact on self 0.077 0.013 1
Impact on others 0.024 0.208** 0.587** 1
Relevance benefit 0.070 0.093 0.691** 0.467** 1
Privacy harm 0.003 0.246** 0.229** 0.011 0.286** 1
Intent to regulate 0.099 0.039 0.04 0.007 0.014 0.222** 1
Intent to block 0.012 0.107* 0.368** 0.093 0.359** 0.557** 0.351** 1
Avoidance 0.096 0.152** 0.517** 0.197** 0.548** 0.577** 0.214** 0.676** 1
Acceptance 0.072 0.055 0.607** 0.298** 0.624** 0.461** 0.091 0.486** 0.641** 1
*** **
Note: p < 0.001; p < 0.01; *p < 0.05; PK: Persuasion Knowledge.
C.-D. Ham, M.R. Nelson / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 689e702 695

Table 3
Summary of hierarchical regression analyses.

Third-person perception (Impact on Others) (Impact on Self) Intent to support Intent to block OBA Avoidance of OBA Acceptance of OBA
OBA regulation

Control (model 1)
Age 0.036 0.055 0.091 0.055 0.091 0.024 0.017
Gender 0.034 0.018 0.026 0.018 0.026 0.004 0.014
Student status 0.159* 0.228** 0.224** 0.228** 0.224** 0.191** 0.100
Experience 0.166** 0.094 0.092 0.094 0.092 0.152** 0.213***
R2 0.059 0.039 0.047 0.039 0.047 0.066 0.070
F 5.824*** 3.718* 4.541** 3.718* 4.541** 0.024 6.988***
Predictors (model 2)
Objective PK 0.006 0.064 0.012 0.064 0.012 0.065 0.021
Subjective PK 0.175*** 0.122* ¡0.047 0.038 0.011 0.029 0.037
Outcomes of PK
Relevance benefit ¡0.329*** 0.490*** 0.690** 0.037 0.222*** 0.426*** 0.657***
Privacy harm 0.100* 0.071* ¡0.026 0.227*** 0.481*** 0.426*** 0.257***
R2 changes 0.150 0.050 0.321 0.050 0.321 0.446 0.540
F 12.111*** 4.444*** 26.598*** 4.444*** 26.598*** 48.074*** 71.619***
Predictors (model 3)
Third-person Perception ¡0.105 0.152** 0.150*** ¡0.124**
(on others) ¡0.095 0.088 0.067 ¡0.069
(on self) 0.135 ¡0.251*** ¡0.272*** 0.209***
R2 changes 0.009 0.018 0.012 0.018
F 4.725*** 25.476*** 66.816*** 45.764***

Note: ***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05; ap < 0.1; N ¼ 378; PK: Persuasion Knowledge; Gender (1: male; 2: female); Student status (1: student; 2:non-student); (Effect on
others) and (Effect on self) are results of post-hoc tests; Bold numbers are used for result interpretations.

Fig. 2. Results of a Mediation Analysis (Study 1: H3).

tactic is driving the TPP effects more than what they really know. understanding of the PK-TPP relationship for a form of advertising
Post-hoc analysis, in addition, revealed that subjective PK-TPP that has beneficial and harmful aspects for consumers.
relationship was mainly driven by the perceived effect on others. Finally, TPP was not significantly associated with social-level
These findings replicate past research (e.g., Eisend, 2015) with a coping (intent to support OBA regulation) but was positively
new form of advertising, which is perceived as ambivalent in its related with personal-level coping (e.g., intent to block OBA). Most
desirability (having both benefit and harm). We however extend TPP studies have hypothesized TPP-social level regulation (e.g.,
this past research by demonstrating processing effects. We show Shah et al., 1999) in response to controversial media effects. Other
how the perceived effects of OBA - the assessment of harm (privacy studies have reported that when individuals perceived the impact
infringement) versus benefit (relevant messages) - mediate the of the media on self was greater than that on others, it is the impact
relationship between PK and TPP. We revealed that the perceived on self that is driving the protective actions (e.g., Liu & Lo, 2014;
benefit of OBA was negatively, whereas the perceived harm was Price et al., 1998). We, however, added new implications: when
positively, associated with TPP. This first finding indicates a first- individuals perceived the media effect on self was greater than that
person perception effect. That is, as people recognize the personal on others, the impact on self is driving the actions (acceptance).
benefits of OBA, they believe that they would be more influenced That is, for personal coping actions, when OBA was perceived as
(because it is ‘smart’ to know this) than others would be. The sec- more impactful on self than on others, and as the impact is
ond finding indicates third-person perception when people believe perceived as more beneficial than harmful, individuals were more
that OBA may cause harm; this harm may impact others more than likely to accept, and less likely to avoid, OBA. These findings suggest
it would impact themselves. These constructs fit with the self- that the links between TPP and coping mechanisms are not
enhancement explanation. When it is ‘smart’ to be persuaded straightforward; the consideration of how the media affects the self
(because of benefits), effects on self are imagined, but when it is is similar or more important than the consideration of media effects
‘not smart’ to be persuaded (due to harm), effects on others are on others, particularly when the media effect is perceived as being
imagined. As a whole, these constructs add a nuanced more beneficial than harmful.
696 C.-D. Ham, M.R. Nelson / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 689e702

Despite the interesting positive relationships noted among the cope with OBA in a personal way rather than a social way. In this
variables in this study, a survey does not allow an examination of study, we will test intent to click the ad and intent to close the ad page
causal relationships. In addition, our survey measured dispositional because these measures are more realistic responses in an experi-
PK, but we were interested in examining the PK impact that is mental situation.
elicited in a situation when the behavioral tracking tactic is dis-
RQ3a-d. When people see a relevant display ad, how will the
closed. Therefore, we designed a follow-up study, using an exper-
behavioral tracking disclosure (vs. default exposure) influence (a)
imental approach, to test some of the relationships among
intent to support OBA regulation, (b) intent to block OBA, (c) intent
variables. We wondered, for example, if respondents empirically
to close OBA page (avoidance), and (d) intent to click OBA (accep-
experienced a persuasion situation where they were exposed to
tance)? If significant, will third-person perception mediate the
OBA, how their activated subjective persuasion knowledge might
relationship between the disclosure and each coping response?
relate to perceptions of effects on self and others and subsequent
coping mechanisms.
7. Study 2 e method

6. Study 2 (experiment) e hypotheses 7.1. Participants

To see the causal relationship, we manipulate different levels of We ran a one-condition between-subject design experiment
situational PK by disclosing how the relevant ad message is deliv- using an online survey. The experimental setting was created
ered (behavior-tracking vs. default exposure) in an empirical situ- similar to a real OBA situation. It provided a website browsing sit-
ation. Dispositional PK refers to the individual's lifetime developed uation in which participants visited various websites to search for
knowledge about a certain persuasion tactic (e.g., OBA) whereas specific information about a computer, and then they saw a related
situational PK is an individual's inference of how a hidden display banner ad when they visited a YouTube website. The situ-
persuasion tactic works in a particular situation (Ham, Nelson, & ation was pretested and found to be one of college students'
Das, 2015). In Study 2, we create a situation in which the same habitual web browsing (visiting YouTube) and information-seeking
relevant ad is shown to goal-pursuing consumers (who search for a (seeking information about a laptop computer) behaviors. Using
specific information) across two conditions, but those in the convenience sampling, 178 students from a Midwestern university
experimental condition were informed that the relevant ad was participated for extra credit. Females (N ¼ 94; 51.9%) slightly out-
delivered by behavior tracking (OBA-persuasion tactic) whereas numbered males (N ¼ 85; 47.0%) and age varied from 18 to 24
those in control condition were notified that it was a default (M ¼ 21).
exposure (without using any persuasion technology). The default
exposure is a general display ad format, not using any technological 7.2. Procedure and stimuli
tactic to deliver relevant ad messages; so we use ‘default exposure’
to represent the “no persuasion knowledge activation” condition. In After signing a consent form, participants were randomly
this experiment, first, we retest H2 (relationship between subjec- assigned into one of the two conditions (behavior-tracking disclo-
tive PK and TPP), positing that consumers will perceive a greater sure: N ¼ 89; default exposure: N ¼ 89). To create such an OBA
impact of OBA on others than on themselves when a relevant ad situation, participants were requested to imagine that they had
message was shown by behavioral tracking tactic (vs. default). searched information to buy a new laptop computer, browsing
Thus, we posit: many websites related to laptop computers (e.g., Apple.com;
lenovo.com) during the last couple of days. After that, they were
H4. When behavioral tracking (vs. default exposure) is disclosed
guided to visit a fictitiously created YouTube main page where the
in receiving the relevant ad, respondents will exhibit greater third-
same fictitiously created laptop ad (a fictitious brand - Willy's
person perception.
computer: see Appendix) was displayed for both groups. In the ad,
Second, we retest Hypotheses 3 (b-d) in an experimental setting, the same relevant message was displayed to the participants (“We
predicting that subjective PK will influence the assessment of offer the cheapest price with the best quality of all types of lap-
benefit and harm of OBA, which in turn will influence TPP. In Study tops”). After they saw the ad on the web page, the following pop-up
1, we found that the assessment of benefits of OBA was negatively, messages were shown to the behavior tracking disclosure group:
and harm was positively, associated with TPP because people are “the banner ad was selectively shown to you because ad network
likely to perceive that beneficial effects are more significant on self, companies tracked your online surfing behaviors and inferred that
but the harmful effect is greater on others. In Study 2, we posit the you are interested in the laptop (OBA)” and to the default exposure
same relationships except reversed PK-benefit assessment because group: “the banner ad was displayed to everyone who visits this
the behavioral tracking tactic is disclosed in a certain situation website like you.”
when the benefit is already given (i.e., relevant ad). Thus, we posit:
7.3. Measures
H5a-c. When behavioral tracking (vs. default exposure) is dis-
closed in receiving the relevant ad, respondents will exhibit (a)
Most measures used the same instruments as Study 1. Item
lower perceived benefit and (b) higher perceived harm; (c) the
reliability (Cronbach's a) was: benefit assessment (a ¼ 0.89), harm
effect of disclosure (behavior tracking vs. default exposure) on
assessment (a ¼ 0.92), third-person perception (effect on me:
third-person perception will be mediated by the assessment of
a ¼ 0.93; effect on others: a ¼ 0.96), intent to support regulation
benefit and harm.
(a ¼ 0.92), and intent to block ads (a ¼ 0.83). Subjective PK
Third, we re-assess research question 2 (a-d) to see if there is a (a ¼ 0.91) was measured by the same items used in Study 1 but
causal relationship between subjective PK and coping responses slightly modified from ‘OBA in general’ to ‘this ad.’ Intent to close
throughout TPP. In Study 1, consumers were more concerned about the OBA page (avoidance) was measured by two items (I'm willing
the OBA effect on self than others; accordingly, they coped with to close the webpage of the banner ad that appeared; I'm willing to
OBA more personally than socially. We, therefore, expect that immediately leave the webpage when the ad is exposed; 7-point
behavioral tracking disclosure (vs. default) will lead consumers to Likert scale; Cronbach's a ¼ 0.94) modified from Cho and Cheon
C.-D. Ham, M.R. Nelson / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 689e702 697

(2004). Intent to click the OBA (Cronbach's a ¼ 0.90) was measured dependent variable (TPP) was regressed on both the independent
by three items (My intention to click the banner ad is: unlikely- (PK) and mediating variables (benefit and harm), the independent
likely; impossible e possible; improbable-probable; 7-point se- variable's (PK) effect on the dependent variable became non-
mantic differential scale; Cronbach's a ¼ 0.95) modified from significant (r ¼ 0.39; n.s.). Assessments of benefit and harm fully
Mackenzie, Lutz, and Belch (1986). In the questionnaire, coping mediated the effect of subjective PK on TPP. Specifically, benefit
responses (intent to click; intent to close page; intent to block; negatively and harm positively mediated the relationship. Thus,
intent to support regulation) were asked first, followed by third- H5c was supported (see Fig. 4(a)).
person perceptions (effect on self and others, by random order), Research questions 3a-d examined the effect of PK on coping
perceived benefit and harm, subjective PK, and manipulation check responses, and, if significant, was followed by mediation analyses. A
items, respectively. series of ANOVAs revealed that the PK effect was (1) not significant
on intent to support OBA regulation (F ¼ 0.35; n.s.; tracking:
8. Study 2 e results M ¼ 5.16; SD ¼ 1.81; default: M ¼ 5.30; SD ¼ 1.62); but (2) sig-
nificant on intent to block OBA (F ¼ 6.17; p < 0.05; tracking:
8.1. Manipulation check M ¼ 5.57; SD ¼ 1.44; default: M ¼ 4.98; SD ¼ 1.71); (3) significant
on intent to close the ad webpage (F ¼ 8.93; p < 0.01; tracking:
One-way ANOVA revealed that the behavioral tracking group M ¼ 5.25; SD ¼ 1.52; default: M ¼ 4.55; SD ¼ 1.58); and (4) sig-
showed significantly higher subjective PK (M ¼ 5.41; SD ¼ 1.62) nificant on intent to click the ad (F ¼ 15.198; p < 0.001; tracking:
than the default group (M ¼ 3.82; SD ¼ 1.90); thus, the PK M ¼ 3.37; SD ¼ 1.95; default: M ¼ 4.50; SD ¼ 1.95). In sum, the
manipulation was successful (F ¼ 30.42; p < 0.001). effect of tracking disclosure (PK) led to a greater intent to block OBA
(RQ3b) and to close the OBA webpage (RQ3c), but a lesser intent to
click the OBA (RQ3d).
8.2. Hypotheses testing
We then ran three mediation analyses, which examined the
mediation role of effect on self and on others between PK and coping
Hypothesis 4 examined the main effect of PK on TPP. The
responses, using the Preacher and Hayes' (2008) bootstrapping
repeated measure MANOVA showed a significant interaction effect
(5000 bootstrap samples) analysis with the PROCESS macro. First,
(Wilky's Lambda: F ¼ 10.65; p < 0.001) such that the effect on others
as presented in Fig. 4(b), only effect on self was a significant medi-
was not significantly different across behavior tracking disclosure
ator between PK and intent to block OBA, as the 95% bias-corrected
(M ¼ 4.48; SD ¼ 1.26) and default exposure (M ¼ 4.56; SD ¼ 1.50)
confidence interval of the variable did not contain zero
conditions, but the effect on self was significantly lower in the
(0.143e0.724) but did for effect on others (0.129 to 0.064). When
behavior tracking disclosure (M ¼ 2.83; SD ¼ 1.78) than in the
the mediators were added, the effect of PK on the intent to block
default exposure condition (M ¼ 3.64; SD ¼ 1.90). Thus, H4 was
disappeared (direct: r ¼ 0.59; P < 0.05; indirect: r ¼ 0.22; n.s.). The
supported (see Fig. 3).
relationship between PK and intent to block OBA was fully medi-
Hypotheses 5a and b examined the effect of PK on the benefit
ated by the perceived effect on self but not by effect on others.
and harm assessment. The results of two ANOVAs showed that the
Similarly, only effect on self was a significant mediator between
perceived benefit was significantly lower (F ¼ 10.19; p < 0.05) in the
PK and intent to close the OBA page, as the 95% bias-corrected
behavior tracking (M ¼ 3.30; SD ¼ 1.67) than default (M ¼ 4.09;
confidence intervals of the variable did not contain zero
SD ¼ 1.60) condition; perceived harm was significantly higher
(0.142e0.732 for effect on self; .080 to 0.040 for effect on others).
(F ¼ 7.85; p < 0.01) in behavior tracking (M ¼ 5.42; SD ¼ 1.50) than
When the mediators were added, the effect of PK on the intent to
default (M ¼ 4.75; SD ¼ 1.69) condition. H5a and b were supported.
close disappeared (direct: r ¼ 0.70; P < 0.05; indirect: r ¼ 0.27; n.s.).
Upon these results, we ran multiple mediation analyses for H5c
The relationship between PK and intent to close the OBA page was
using the Preacher and Hayes' (2008) bootstrapping (5000 boot-
fully mediated by the perceived effect on self (see Fig. 4(c)). Finally,
strap samples) analysis with the PROCESS macro. Fig. 4(a) indicates
only effect on self was a significant mediator between PK and intent
that both benefit and harm are significant mediators, as the 95%
to click OBA, as the 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals of the
bias-corrected confidence intervals of the two variables do not
variable did not contain zero (0.927 to 0.170 for effect on
contain zero (0.086e0.484 for benefit; 0.011 to 0.280 for harm).
self; 0.112 to 0.029 for effect on others). When the mediators were
Specifically, tracking disclosure (PK) was a significant predictor of
added, the effect of PK on intent to click significantly weakened
the assessment of benefits (r ¼ 0.78; p < 0.01) and harm (r ¼ 0.67;
(direct: r ¼ 1.14; p < 0.001; indirect: r ¼ 0.63; n.s.). The
p < 0.01); assessments of benefit (r ¼ 0.32; P < 0.001) and harm
perceived effect on self partially mediated the PK's impact on the
(r ¼ 0.14; p < 0.05) were significant predictors of TPP; when the
click intent but the effect on others did not mediate (see Fig. 4(d)).

9. Study 2 e discussion

In an experimental setting, we identified causal relationships for


some of the descriptive relationships noted in Study 1. First, sub-
jective PK significantly impacted TPP. Specifically, we found that
perceptions of effects on self created a larger self-other TPP gap for
those with PK. PK also affected respondents' perceptions of benefits
(lower in behavior tracking) and harm (greater in behavior
tracking).
Second, in line with Study 1 findings, the assessment of harm
and benefit mediated the relationship between PK and TPP effects.
Therefore, it appears that the direct link between PK and TPP is
mediated by the ways that individuals perceive the impact of the
tactics (benefit/harm). The only difference in study 1 (survey) from
Fig. 3. Results of repeated-measure MANOVA (study 2: H4). study 2 (experiment) is the negative relationship between PK and
698 C.-D. Ham, M.R. Nelson / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 689e702

Fig. 4. Results of Mediation Analyses (Study 2: H5 & RQ3).

benefit assessment in study 2. It is likely because behavioral significantly impacted personal coping responses to the OBA (i.e.,
tracking (vs. default exposure) was disclosed in a situation in which avoidance: intent to block or close; or acceptance: intention to
people already gained the benefit (i.e., relevant ad); thus, the click) but not social-level coping (support to regulate). In addition,
disclosure negatively impacted the perceived benefit but positively the impact of PK on all three personal coping responses was
impacted the perceived harm. In a situation when the desirability of mediated only by effect on self.
a persuasion is ambivalent (like OBA), a significant PK-TPP rela-
tionship does not always occur; rather, we found that the assess- 10. General discussion
ment of benefit/harm mediates this relationship. Based on the idea
of Kirmani and Campbell (2004), we argue that consumers do not Third-person perception (TPP) presents a well-known phe-
simply perceive that persuasion knowledge automatically leads to nomenon where individuals believe others are affected by media
third-person perception, but more deeply process the persuasion more than they themselves are affected (Perloff, 1999). Such effects
tactic and its benefit and harm to cope with the tactic. How and are moderated by social desirability of the message (e.g., Sun et al.,
when they do this may be variable depending on the perceptions of 2008) and influenced by individuals' knowledge (Davison, 1983).
benefit or harm and their own personal goals. Online behavioral advertising (OBA) presents an interesting case to
Finally, similar to Study 1, we found that subjective PK understand perceived media effects for two reasons: (1) due to its
C.-D. Ham, M.R. Nelson / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 689e702 699

technological complexity and covertness, OBA presents a context them rather than simply resisting it. In a social exchange situation
where people's knowledge is limited; accordingly, their coping such as the marketplace, consumers tend to provide their resources
strategy and the perceived effects on others and self appear to vary (e.g., privacy-related personal information; White, 2004) only to
by their knowledge level; (2) the tactic offers potential benefits the extent that the benefit is equal to or greater than their loss. We
(relevant messages) and harm (privacy infringement) so OBA is not identified that persuasion knowledge, assessment of harm and
clearly undesirable or desirable. Therefore, across two studies, we benefit, and TPP play a role in determining consumers' coping
tested how people's knowledge about OBA and their assessments of mechanisms for the tactic (e.g., accept, avoid).
harm and benefit influenced perceptions of effects on self and Second, the effect of objective persuasion knowledge on third-
others and subsequent personal and social coping behaviors. Our person perception was examined for the first time. Despite the
research extends the TPP and PKM relationship in a new context importance, objective persuasion knowledge has only been exam-
that works by decomposing PK and examining the role of both ined in the pricing tactic context (Carlson et al., 2007). We propose
objective and subjective PK and highlighting the mediating role of that objective knowledge in a specific context (e.g., OBA) should be
harm and benefit assessment in the PK-TPP relationship and its considered because persuasion knowledge is context-specific, and
effects on a greater range of personal coping responses. objective knowledge is significantly associated with subjective
Based on TPP studies (e.g., Gunther & Thorson, 1992), we iden- persuasion knowledge. In developing coping strategies and third-
tified how knowledge influenced TPP. Across a survey and experi- person perceptions, we identified that the subjective confidence
ment, we found that people's assessment of their own knowledge of how persuasion works plays a more important role than the
about how OBA works (subjective PK) is significant in determining objective knowledge of what consumers really know, particularly
the direction and magnitude of TPP in the OBA context. That is, the when the objective knowledge was not sufficient to fully under-
greater the PK, the larger the gap in self-other perceptions of ef- stand how the persuasion works. We however, found that objective
fects. Interestingly, the gap revealed less perceived effect on self knowledge indirectly affected TPP through subjective persuasion
when persuasion knowledge was manipulated through disclosure knowledge, which in turn influenced benefit and harm assessment.
of OBA (vs. Default) in Study 2. This knowledge processing structure implies that different di-
The outcomes of PK related to individuals' assessments of the mensions of “knowledge of how persuasion works” operate
psychological effects and effectiveness of OBA. Across both studies, differently in perceiving the persuasion's effect on self and others.
we found that individuals' assessments of the potential harm On a practical note, our studies revealed that social coping (i.e.,
(privacy infringement) and benefits (relevant messages) played a support for regulation) is not supported yet for OBA, but consumers
strong role in assessing perceived effects of OBA on self and others. may take several personal coping mechanisms for OBA. The FTC has
That is, whereas past research has shown the PK-TPP link (Eisend, considered setting a law to regulate behavior tracking and profiling,
2015), we demonstrated how perceived outcomes of PK in harm but only industry self-regulation and ethical guidelines are rec-
and benefit assessment mediate the relationship between PK and ommended, not dictated, for years (Nill & Aalberts, 2014). In the
TPP. Interestingly but consistent with TPP's underlying self- meantime, more and more consumers are installing ad blockers to
enhancing motive, we identified that the perceived benefit drove avoid tracking and profiling personal data (Manjoo, 2015). Our re-
the effect on self, but the perceived harm drove the effect on others; sults suggest that consumers perceive personal data tracking as
combined together which formed TPP. In addition, it appears to be more of a personal issue rather than a social issue; thus, their social
the perceived effects on self and not effects on others that show the voice to regulate OBA may not yet be significant, but their personal
most influence for predicting personal coping responses. Such coping (e.g., blocking) is silently but consistently increasing,
findings imply that activated persuasion knowledge may allow resulting in a potential cost to the ad industry in the near future. In
consumers to actively elaborate the benefit and harm, processing 2012, regulation in the UK dictated that websites disclose cooking
the perception of effect on self more systematically and taking more tracking behavior to users (Whittaker, 2012). One example of this
personal level coping responses. disclosure, with an opt-in or out feature, reads, “The cookie settings
Different from typical TPP studies, which showed social pre- on this website are set to ‘allow all cookies’ to give you the very best
ventive behaviors (such as likelihood to support regulation) as a experience. If you continue without changing these settings, you
result of TPP, we identified that individuals are more concerned sent to this e but if you want, you can change your settings at any
about personal coping, when they perceived the media effect was time at the bottom of this page.” Such controllability can enhance
more significant on self than on others. When effect on self is self-confidence, which is positively associated with subjective
perceived more significant than effect on others, TPP leads to more persuasion knowledge (Bearden et al., 2001). However, when
personal than social coping responses. Importantly, these re- persuasion knowledge is activated through disclosure, consumers
sponses are not always preventive but also represent some proac- may actively elaborate on the benefit and harm, processing the
tive coping responses (e.g., accept advertising) particularly when perception of effect on self more systematically and resulting in
the tactic is perceived as more beneficial than harmful. taking more personal level coping responses. When persuasion
knowledge is not sufficiently activated, on the other hand, con-
10.1. Implications sumers may be less likely to elaborate benefit-harm assessments,
processing both perceptions of effect on self and effect on others not
First, our model helps explain the relationship between critically. Accordingly, then, the consumers seem to simply resist
knowledge and TPP by highlighting the mediating role of perceived OBA.
benefits and harm. Whereas past TPP studies use messages that are Future research could systematically test these explanations. As
clearly undesirable or desirable, OBA presents a message that may Eisend (2015) identified, simple persuasion knowledge (without
be harmful and beneficial. We show that it is the assessment of elaborating benefit-harm assessments) increases TPP primarily by
those benefits or harm that explain the relationship between overestimating effect on others, which in turn would elicit more
knowledge and TPP. Perloff (1999) asks, “Do people consider it social-level coping responses (e.g., regulation). In effect, the
acceptable to be influenced just when they perceive potential disclosure of OBA, if the disclosure conveys or reinforces benefits of
personal benefits?” (p. 361). It appears that may be the case. As the practice, may actually help consumers to accept OBA. Educating
Kirmani and Campbell (2004) proposed, a goal-pursuing self more consumers by disclosing benefit and harm can allow them to more
actively assesses whether a persuasion tactic benefits or harms systematically process the message and assess whether the
700 C.-D. Ham, M.R. Nelson / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 689e702

message can help them to achieve their marketplace goals, required disclosure of tracking cookies similar to regulation in the
resulting in assisting them to decide whether they accept or avoid UK. Despite these limitations, we hope that our study spurs addi-
OBA. tional research in this area.

11. Conclusion
10.2. Limitations

OBA is a special form of media that is desirable and undesirable


This study has several limitations. First, we used convenience
because it offers benefits and potential harm to consumers. Our
sampling. Second, we used hierarchical regression analyses, even
findings show that consumers' perception of their knowledge
though most variables were measured by ordinal scale. Third, our
about OBA (“subjective PK”) relates to how much they believe they
study tested only behavioral intention but not real behaviors.
personally (vs. others) are affected by the persuasion tactic (i.e., TPP
Although behavioral intentions are typically an antecedent of
effect). Further, our results uniquely capture an important mediator
behavior, especially in situations within the individuals' control
between the PK and TPP relationship: namely, it is the assessment
(e.g., Ajzen, 1985), future study could propose a theoretical rela-
of the potential harm and benefit of OBA that mediates the rela-
tionship between TPP and real behaviors, and to test direct re-
tionship between subjective persuasion knowledge and TPP.
sponses of real behaviors (e.g., click or avoid banner ads).
Importantly, it is the perceived effect on self in TPP that relates to
Fourth, our experiment included only one information search
personal coping behaviors (e.g., accepting or avoiding OBA). No
and advertising scenario. Also, the presumption that TPP operates
significant relationships or effects were found for social level
on individuals' self-esteem is common in the TPP literature (Perloff,
coping such as support for regulation. In sum, this research expands
1999), yet this construct is rarely measured. Future research could
on and links the growing body of research on PK and the research
measure or manipulate self-esteem and see its role in the model.
on TPP in the online advertising context.
For example, if respondents believed they had low (rather than
high) persuasion knowledge, would TPP be the same? Across two
studies we did not find social-level coping with respect to regula- Appendix. Example of stimulus in study 2.
tion. Yet, the regulation measures used were rather vague. Future
research could ask specific questions regarding, for example,
C.-D. Ham, M.R. Nelson / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 689e702 701

References Lasorsa, D. L. (1989). Real and perceived effects of ‘Amerika’. Journalism Quarterly,
66(2), 373e378.
Lee, C., & Yang, S. (1996). Third-person perception and support for censorship of
Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl,
sexually explicit visual content: a Korean case. In Paper presented to the Asso-
& J. Backmann (Eds.), Action-control: From Cognition to Behavior (pp. 11e39).
ciation for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication, Anaheim, CA.
Heidelberg: Springer.
Liu, X., & Lo, V. H. (2014). Media exposure, perceived personal impact, and third-
Alba, J. W., & Hutchinson, J. W. (2000). Knowledge calibration: what consumers know
person effect. Media Psychology, 17(4), 378e396.
and what they think they know. Journal of Consumer Research, 27(2), 123e156.
MacKenzie, S. B., Lutz, R. J., & Belch, G. E. (1986). The role of attitude toward the ad
Baek, T., & Morimoto, M. (2012). Stay away from me. Journal of Advertising, 41(1),
as a mediator of advertising effectiveness: a test of competing explanations.
59e76.
Journal of Marketing Research, 23(2), 130e143.
Bearden, W. O., Hardesty, D. M., & Rose, R. L. (2001). Consumer self-confidence:
Mahoney, S. (2015). Consumers conflicted about privacy vs. personalization. http://
refinements in conceptualization and measurement. Journal of Consumer
www.mediapost.com/publications/article/245405/consumers-conflicted-
Research, 28(1), 121e134.
about-privacy-vs-personaliza.html/ Accessed 15.01.17.
Boerman, S. C., van Reijmersdal, E. A., & Neijens, P. C. (2012). Sponsorship disclosure:
Manjoo, F. (2015). Ad blockers and the nuisance at the heart of the modern web. http://
effects of duration and persuasion knowledge and brand response. Journal of
www.nytimes.com/2015/08/20/technology/personaltech/ad-blockers-and-the-
Communication, 62(6), 1047e1064.
nuisance-at-the-heart-of-the-modern-web.html?_r¼0 Accessed 15.09.18.
Brucks, M. (1985). The effects of product class knowledge on information search
Marshall, J. (2014). Do consumers really want targeted ads?. http://blogs.wsj.com/
behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 12(1), 1e16.
cmo/2014/04/17/do-consumers-really-want-targeted-ads/ Accessed 15.02.10.
Campbell, M. C., & Kirmani, A. (2008). I know what you’re doing and why you're
McDonald, A. M., & Cranor, L. F. (2010). An empirical study of how people perceive
doing it: the use of the persuasion knowledge model in consumer research. In
online behavioral advertising. In Proceedings of the 10' workshop on privacy in
C. P. Haugtvedt, P. M. Herr, & F. R. Kardes (Eds.), The handbook of consumer
the electronic society (WPES'10) (pp. 63e72).
psychology (pp. 549e573). New York, NY: Erlbaum.
McLeod, D. M., Eveland, W. P., Jr., & Nathanson, A. I. (1997). Support for censorship
Campbell, M. C., Mohr, G. S., & Verlegh, P. W. J. (2013). Can disclosures lead con-
of violent and misogynic rap lyrics: an analysis of the third-person effect.
sumers to resist convert persuasion? the important roles of disclosure timing
Communication Research, 24(2), 153e174.
and type of response. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 23(4), 483e495.
Nill, A., & Aalberts, R. J. (2014). Legal and ethical challenges of online behavioral
Carlson, J. P., Bearden, W. O., & Hardesty, D. M. (2007). Influences on what con-
targeting in advertising. Journal of Current Issues & Research in Advertising, 35(2),
sumers know and what they think they know regarding marketer pricing
126e146.
tactics. Psychology & Marketing, 24(2), 117e142.
Okazaki, S., Li, H., & Hirose, M. (2009). Consumer privacy concerns and preference
Chang, C., Wei, R., & Lo, V. H. (2014). Ambivalent versus univalent voters: perceived
for degree of regulatory control. Journal of Advertising, 38(4), 63e77.
media influences and third-person perceptions. Media Psychology, 17(4), 420e450.
Paradise, A., & Sullivan, M. (2012). (In)visible threats? the third-person effect in
Chen, G. M., & Ng, M. Y. M. (2016). Third-person perception of online comments: civil
perceptions of the influence of Facebook. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social
ones persuade you more than me. Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 736e742.
Networking, 15(1), 55e60.
Chia, S. C., Lu, K. H., & McLeod, D. M. (2004). Sex lies, and video compact disc: a case
Perloff, R. M. (1999). The third-person effect: a critical review and synthesis. Media
study on third-person perception and motivations for censorship. Communi-
Psychology, 1(4), 353e378.
cation Research, 31(1), 109e130.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for
Cho, C. H., & Cheon, H. J. (2004). Why do people avoid advertising on the internet?
assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior
Journal of Advertising, 33(4), 89e97.
Research Methods, 40(3), 879e891.
Conners, J. L. (2005). Understanding the third-person effect. Communication
Price, V., Tewksbury, D., & Huang, L. N. (1998). Third-person effects on publication of
Research Trend, 24(2), 2e22.
a holocaust-denial advertisement. Journal of Communication, 48(2), 3e26.
Davison, W. P. (1983). The third-person effect in communication. Public Opinion
Salwen, M. B. (1998). Perceptions of media influence and support for censorship:
Quarterly, 47(1), 1e15.
the third-person effect in the 1996 presidential election. Communication
Driscoll, P. D., & Salwen, M. B. (1997). Self-perceived knowledge of OJ Simpson trial:
Research, 25(3), 259e285.
third-person perception and perceptions of guilt. Journalism & Mass Commu-
Shah, D. V., Faber, R. J., & Youn, S. (1999). Susceptibility and severity: perceptual di-
nication Quarterly, 74(3), 541e556.
mensions underlying the third-person effect. Communication Research, 26(2),
Eastin, M. S., Brinson, N. H., Doorey, A., & Wilcox, G. (2016). Living in a big data
240e267.
world: predicting mobile commerce activity through privacy concerns. Com-
Simonson, I. (2005). Determinants of customers' responses to customized offers: con-
puters in Human Behavior, 58, 214e220.
ceptual framework and research propositions. Journal of Marketing, 69, 32e45.
Eisend, M. (2015). Persuasion knowledge and third-person perceptions in adver-
Smit, E. G., van Noort, G., & Voorveld, H. A. M. (2014). Understanding online
tising: the moderating effect of regulatory competence. International Journal of
behavioural advertising: user knowledge, privacy concerns and online coping
Advertising, 34(1), 54e69.
behavior in Europe. Computers in Human Behavior, 32, 15e22.
Fransen, M. L., Verlegh, P. W. J., Kirmani, A., & Smit, E. G. (2015). A typology of
Speck, P. S., & Elliott, M. T. (1997). Predictors of advertising avoidance in print and
consumer strategies for resisting advertising, and a review of mechanisms for
broadcast media. Journal of Advertising, 26(3), 61e76.
countering them. International Journal of Advertising, 34(1), 6e16.
Sun, Y., Pan, Z., & Shen, L. (2008). Understanding the third-person perception: ev-
Friestad, M., & Wright, P. (1994). The persuasion knowledge model: how people
idence from a meta-analysis. Journal of Communication, 58(2), 280e300.
cope with persuasion attempts. Journal of Consumer Research, 21, 1e31.
Tewksbury, D., Moy, P., & Weis, D. S. (2004). Preparations for Y2K: revisiting the
FTC. (2009). FTC staff revises online behavioral advertising principles. http://www.ftc.
behavioral component of the third-person effect. Journal of Communication,
gov/opa/2009/02/behavad.shtm/ Accessed 14.05.21.
54(1), 138e155.
Golan, G. J., & Banning, S. A. (2008). Exploring a link between the third-person effect
Turow, J., King, J., Hoofnagle, C. J., Bleakley, A., & Hennessy, M. (2009). Americans
and the theory of reasoned action: beneficial ads and social expectations.
reject tailored advertising and three activities that enable it. http://papers.ssrn.
American Behavioral Scientists, 52(2), 208e224.
com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id¼1478214 Accessed 14.05.21.
Gunther, A. C., & Mundy, P. (1993). Biased optimism and the third-person effect.
van Noort, G., Smit, E. G., & Voorveld, H. A. (2013). The online behavioural adver-
Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 70(1), 58e67.
tising icon: two user studies. Advances in Advertising Research, 4, 365e378.
Gunther, A. C. (1995). Overrating the x-rating: the third-person perception and
van Reijmersdal, E. A., Tutaj, K., & Boerman, S. C. (2013). The effects of brand
support for censorship of pornography. Journal of Communication, 45(1), 27e38.
placement disclosures on skepticism and brand memory. Communications e The
Gunther, A. C., & Hwa, A. P. (1996). Public perceptions of television influence and
European Journal of Communication Research, 38(2), 127e146.
opinions about censorship in Singapore. International Journal of Public Opinion
Wei, R., & Lo, V. H. (2008). News media use and knowledge about 2006 US midterm
Research, 8(3), 248e265.
elections: why exposure matters in voter learning. International Journal of Public
Gunther, A. C., & Thorson, E. (1992). Perceived persuasive effects of product com-
Opinion Research, 20(3), 347e362.
mercials and public service announcements: the third-person effect in new
White, T. B. (2004). Consumer disclosure and disclosure avoidance: a motivational
domains. Communication Research, 19(5), 574e596.
framework. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(1), 41e51.
Ham, C., Nelson, M. R., & Das, S. (2015). How to measure persuasion knowledge.
Whittaker, Z. (2012). UK ‘cookie law’ takes effect: What you need to know. http://
International Journal of Advertising, 34(1), 17e53.
www.zdnet.com/article/uk-cookie-law-takes-effect-what-you-need-to-know/
Huh, J., & Langteau, R. (2007). Presumed influence of DTC prescription drug advertising:
Accessed 15.03.12.
do experts and novices think differently? Communication Research, 34(1), 25e52.
Woon, R., Tan, G. W., & Low, R. (2005). Protection motivation theory approach to how
Jai, T., Burns, L. D., & King, N. J. (2013). The effect of behavioral tracking practices on
wireless security. In Proceeding of 26th international conference on information sys-
consumers' shopping evaluations and repurchase intention toward trusted
tems. available at: http://aisel.aisnet.org/icis2005/31 (accessed August 30, 2014).
online retailers. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 901e909.
Kirmani, A., & Campbell, M. C. (2004). Goal seeker and persuasion sentry: how
consumer targets respond to interpersonal marketing persuasion. Journal of
Consumer Research, 31(3), 573e582. Chang-Dae Ham (Ph.D., University of Missouri) is assistant professor in the Charles H.
Kirmani, A., & Campbell, M. C. (2009). Taking the target's perspective: the Sandage Department of Advertising at the University of Illinois-Urbana. His research
persuasion knowledge model. Social Psychology of Consumer Behavior, 297e316. interests are motivation, persuasion and persuasion knowledge relate to advertising
702 C.-D. Ham, M.R. Nelson / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 689e702

in digital area. Chang-Dae has published in International Journal of Advertising, Journal Illinois-Urbana. Her research and public engagement endeavors relate to adver-
of Consumer Behaviour, Current Psychology etc. tising/persuasion literacy and product placement. Nelson has published in
leading advertising (JA, IJA, JAR, JCIRA, JIAD), consumer research (JCP), and
communication (JMCQ) journals. She is on the editorial board of JA and is an
Michelle R. Nelson (Ph.D., University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign) is associate Associate Editor of IJA.
professor in the Charles H. Sandage Department of Advertising at the University of

You might also like