S2 When Lying Changes Memory For The Truth

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

MEMORY, 2018

VOL. 26, NO. 1, 2–14


https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2017.1340286

INVITED REVIEW

When lying changes memory for the truth


Henry Otgaara,b and Alysha Bakerc
a
Section Forensic Psychology, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands; bDepartment of Psychology, City, University of London,
London, UK; cDepartment of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Kelowna, Canada

ABSTRACT KEYWORDS
In the legal field, victims and offenders frequently lie to avoid talking about serious incidents, Lying; memory; false
such as past experiences of sexual abuse or criminal involvement. Although these individuals memory; omission;
may initially lie about an experienced event, oftentimes these same people eventually commission
abandon their lies and are forthcoming with what truly happened. To date, it is unclear
whether such lying affects later statements about one’s memory for the experienced event.
The impetus of the present review is to compile the current state of knowledge on the
effects of lying on memory. Based on existing literature, we will describe how deceptive
strategies (e.g., false denials) regarding what is remembered may affect memory in
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:20 14 November 2017

consequential ways, such as forgetting of details, falsely remembering features that were not
present, or a combination of both. It will be argued that the current literature suggests that
mnemonic outcome is contingent on the type of lie and we will propose a theoretical
framework outlining which forms of lying likely result in certain memory outcomes. Potential
avenues of future research also will be discussed.

Notorious serial killer, Ted Bundy, who was charged and an event can be revised spontaneously (e.g., Otgaar,
convicted with several kidnappings, rapes, and gruesome Howe, Brackmann, & Smeets, 2016; Roediger & McDermott,
murders in the United States during the 1970s, originally 1995) or altered when suggestive information is provided by
denied involvement in the crimes throughout his trials credible others (e.g., Loftus & Palmer, 1974; Loftus & Pickrell,
that spanned over a decade due to escapes and additional 1995; Otgaar, Candel, Smeets, & Merckelbach, 2010; Valen-
crimes. But, before his execution in the late 1980s, Bundy tine & Mesout, 2009). The imperfections of memory have
decided to confess to several of the murders he was con- been demonstrated in several contexts with various types
victed of as well as others unknown to the police (see Car- of stimuli and populations. For example, research has
lisle, 2014 for overview). A pertinent issue is how Bundy’s found that participants can be led to falsely recollect
denials for several years affected his memory for the minute details of events but, more drastically, can also
actual details of the crimes. This issue will be the key falsely remember entire events (Loftus & Pickrell, 1995;
focus of the present review paper in which we will Otgaar, Candel, Merckelbach, & Wade, 2009). Although the
discuss the effects of lying on memory. finding of suggestive information threatening the reliability
The act of lying1 draws substantially on cognitive of one’s memory for past events is well documented, this
resources (e.g., Christ, Van Essen, Watson, Brubaker, & typically has been suggestive information provided by
McDermott, 2009; Sporer & Schwandt, 2006; see also others (i.e., external; whether it be an experimenter, confed-
Walczyk, Harris, Duck, & Mulay, 2014; Zuckerman, erate, or co-participant) rather than the memorial effects of
DePaulo, & Rosenthal, 1981). False details must be con- false accounts provided by the rememberer themselves. At
structed and communicated in a credible manner by moni- the moment, a comprehensive understanding about what
toring one’s verbal and nonverbal channels. Cognitive happens to one’s memory for the past when the truth is
resources can be even more under demand because suc- withheld,2 such as being less than forthcoming during
cessful attempts at lying typically require that the details crime-relevant questioning, is unclear.
of the original truth be remembered as well as the lie.
Although lying requires adequate performance from
The intimate relationship between lying and
one’s memory, a wealth of research stemming from the
memory
last several decades has revealed that memory is reconstruc-
tive and malleable in nature (see Loftus, 2005). Several There are several forensically relevant contexts in which
studies have demonstrated that one’s original memory for lying about what one remembers may occur. For

CONTACT Henry Otgaar henry.otgaar@maastrichtuniversity.nl


© 2017 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-
nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, and is not altered, transformed, or
built upon in any way.
MEMORY 3

example, victims of sexual abuse often delay disclosing an loss for the event (feigning amnesia3; sometimes combined
abusive experience through strategies such as falsely with an alternative fabricated account), or provide an entire
denying the abusive event altogether (Block et al., 2012; alternative/false account (fabrication/confabulation4).
Goodman-Brown, Edelstein, Goodman, Jones, & Gordon, Generally, it appears that there has recently been a
2003; Sjöberg, & Lindblad, 2002; but see Lyon, 2007 and greater appreciation of context or lie type when it comes
Otgaar, Howe, Memon, & Wang, 2014), which has put to studying lies, the processes involved, and the various con-
false denials at the forefront of discussions about sexual sequences (e.g., DePaulo et al., 2003; Frank & Feeley, 2003;
abuse allegations. Furthermore, false denials often are Ganis, Morris, & Kosslyn, 2009; see Sporer, 2016; Warren,
communicated by offenders trying to avoid culpability Schertler, & Bull, 2009). For example, fMRI research has
for a particular crime (e.g., Gudjonsson, Sigurdsson, & found that there may be distinct neural pathways associated
Einarsson, 2004). Another memory-related claim that regu- with different types of lies (such as whether the lie fits into a
larly occurs in investigative contexts is when a rememberer coherent story or whether it was previously memorised;
asserts that he/she has no memory for an entire event or Ganis, Kosslyn, Stose, Thompson, & Yurgelun-Todd, 2003;
parts of an event – that is, claims complete or partial see also Ganis et al., 2009). In a similar vein, theoretically,
amnesia which could, for example, be the result of neuro- there is reason to believe that different types of lies
logical damage (e.g., Bourget & Bradford, 1995; Kalant, demand varying degrees of cognitive resources (e.g., Vrij &
1996; Kopelman, 1995) – and then sometimes comes up Heaven, 1999; Walczyk et al., 2014). The cognitive resources
with a fabricated variant of what ostensibly occurred. involved in the different strategies aimed at withholding the
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:20 14 November 2017

When perceived to be genuine, accepted amnesia claims truth – including feigning amnesia, falsely denying, or fabri-
can have serious legal ramifications as they can lead cating – appear to vary such that more cognitive resources
to questions surrounding criminal responsibility (Cima, are likely used when an entire (alternative) account of an
Merckelbach, Nijman, Knauer, & Hollnack, 2002; Porter, Birt, event is fabricated versus simply claiming memory loss or
Herve, & Yuille, 2001) or competency to stand trial (see falsely denying an event took place (Walczyk et al., 2014;
Smith & Resnick, 2007). Not surprisingly, feigning amnesia – see Figure 1). Of course, this may not be so easily teased
claiming memory loss when in reality they do remember – apart because deceptive strategies are often used in
is an appealing option for those wanting to reap the potential tandem; one may claim amnesia or falsely deny a critical
“benefits” associated with accepted amnesia claims (see part of the experience while also providing a truthful or fab-
Cima, Merckelbach, Hollnack, & Knauer, 2003). ricated account for the rest of the experience, for example.
Rememberers also may be motivated to fabricate Although these deceptive strategies can be used together,
specific details about an experience, and the act of doing when examined individually it appears they have unique
so can even vary by motivation (external or internal) and effects on memory which we theorise to be a result of the
intent. For example, witnesses may be forced to speculate varying cognitive resources required.
about how an event unfolded after (honestly) claiming not There are several ways in which memory can be potentially
to have any knowledge. Similarly, false details may be pro- influenced by deceptive strategies, including hits (correctly
vided in response to (suggestive) pressures by the inter- identifying a previous object/detail; i.e., correct memory),
viewer; for example, certain coercive interview tactics can false alarms/commission errors (reporting an object/detail
persuade suspects/offenders to commit information that that was not present; i.e., false memory), misses/omission
they do not actually remember or that did not happen errors (failing to report a present object/detail), and correct
altogether (see Garven, Wood, Malpass, & Shaw, 1998). Of rejections (accurately rejecting an object/detail that was
relevance to the current discussion is a fabrication gener- not present). To begin our discussion of the effects of
ated with the intent to deceive the listener (i.e., when deceptive strategies on these various memory outcomes,
false details about an original experience are committed we will now turn to the literature that has documented the
that the rememberer knows to be untrue with the goal effect of false denials and feigning amnesia on memory.
of deceiving), such as when an offender provides a false
alibi for the critical time a crime occurred so that he/she
is removed as a suspect in the police inquiry. In conclusion, Lying and omission errors
there are several strategies that a deceiver can use in inves-
False denials
tigative settings when asked to provide an account of a
crime. Rememberers can simply deny details or the entirety Empirical investigation into the relationship between false
of an event (false denial), claim complete or partial memory denials and memory is limited compared to research on

Figure 1. Relationship between cognitive resources used and lying strategy.


4 H. OTGAAR AND A. BAKER

the mnemonic consequences of other forms of lying, such et al., 2014). For example, Otgaar and colleagues (2014)
as feigned amnesia or fabrication. However, findings from a questioned participants (children and adults) about their
handful of studies on the topic to date suggest that false memory concerning several presented and non-presented
denials may lead to omission errors. In all of these details of a video they had watched previously (e.g., “What
studies, the effect of denying was compared to a group did the man steal?”). After this, a subgroup of participants
that was instructed to tell the truth. In the first investigation was instructed to (falsely) deny having seen each detail
of its kind, Vieira and Lane (2013) examined the effects of in response to these questions (e.g., “The man did not
false denials (relative to fabrications) on memory for the steal anything”). After a one-week delay, participants
source of an experience. After an encoding phase during received a source memory test in which they were asked
which several pictures (e.g., of an apple) were viewed/ whether they had seen certain details in the video and
studied, participants were instructed (in a random whether they had talked about these details during the
fashion) to deny or describe either studied (i.e., old) or first session with the interviewer. False denials did not
unstudied (i.e., new) objects. Following a delay, participants impact memory for the details of the video. Interestingly,
completed a source memory test by viewing both new and however, false denials did impair memory for having dis-
old pictures during which they were instructed to reply cussed certain details in the initial interview (i.e., partici-
honestly (i.e., report what was actually remembered from pants later falsely denied having discussed a previously
the original studying phase and ignore any previous denied detail). The researchers termed this effect: denial-
instruction to respond deceptively). More specifically, par- induced forgetting (DIF).
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:20 14 November 2017

ticipants had to report their memory for two aspects: (1) In two subsequent experiments (Otgaar, Howe, Smeets,
whether they had studied the item in the earlier phase of & Wang, 2016), the previous work was expanded upon by
the study and (2) the way in which they lied about the examining DIF with pictures rather than video (Experiment
item in the earlier phase (i.e., whether they had sub- 1) and the addition of an external denial group (i.e.,
sequently falsely denied or described that item). Of great- someone else indicating that the participant’s memory is
est interest to the current discussion is that the authors incorrect by denying the existence of a previously
found participants’ memory for whether they had studied present detail; Experiments 1 and 2). These follow-up
an item was affected by the type of lie they had been experiments allowed for the determination of how reliable
assigned to in the rehearsal phase for that item. Specifi- DIF was when a different source of denial was used with
cally, when asked whether they had studied the items pre- different stimuli. In both experiments, the authors repli-
viously, participants had greater difficulty remembering cated DIF: the false deniers demonstrated impaired
whether they had studied an item in the first session if memory for having discussed details in the previous inter-
they had later falsely denied having studied it rather than view while in fact these details were mentioned in the
if they had to tell the truth about the studied item. In initial interview. Overall, this collection of research seems
sum, poorer memory for whether an item was studied pre- to show that false denials lead to omission errors concern-
viously was evidenced for items that were initially falsely ing the content of stimuli, but only when false denial
denied relative to studied items that had to be truthfully effects are compared with material that is both studied
described. and rehearsed. The work on DIF demonstrates that there
A relevant point of discussion concerning the results of is strong support that false denials lead to omission
Vieira and Lane (2013) is that false denials led to impaired errors of having lied during an interview.
memory performance relative to items that were studied
and truthfully rehearsed in the first place. However,
Feigned amnesia
although this situation mirrors real-world scenarios (i.e.,
people lie or tell the truth in a police investigation) and Studies investigating feigned amnesia typically follow a
this comparison implies that false denials result in impaired standard procedure: participants witness an event or
memory performance, the obtained results do not provide perform a certain act and are subsequently instructed to
conclusive evidence that false denials lead to omission simulate amnesia or report the account of their experience
errors. Strong evidence for such would be false denials honestly in an initial interview. Previous studies have used
leading to worse memory performance compared to various types of instructions in this regard. For example,
items that were studied but not rehearsed. However, some have instructed participants to “describe in writing
Vieira and Lane (2013) did not find evidence for this in the course of events so as to evade responsibility, as if
their study. Nonetheless, recent evidence does seem to you did not remember very well” (Christianson & Bylin,
show that false denials affect memory; however, this 1999), “minimize your responsibility by simulating a
effect does not seem to be related to memory of the memory disorder. Thus, try to describe events in such a
stimuli but to memory of what was discussed. way so it looked like you have great difficulties in remem-
That is, the effect of impaired memory performance bering what happened” (Van Oorsouw & Merckelbach,
following false denials has since been investigated more 2004), or simply “feign amnesia” (Van Oorsouw & Merckel-
comprehensively with crime-relevant stimuli, including bach, 2006). Then, (after a delay) participants return for a
video footage (Otgaar, Howe, Smeets, & Wang, 2016; Otgaar second interview at which point all groups are instructed
MEMORY 5

to answer the interviewer’s questions honestly and the investigative process. The majority of studies examin-
memory performance for the original event is assessed. ing the effect of (self-generated) fabrications on memory
Several studies using crime-relevant stimuli have have employed what is often referred to as the forced con-
revealed that feigning amnesia leads to impaired fabulation paradigm (Ackil & Zaragoza, 1998, 2011; Frost,
memory in the form of increased rates of omission errors LaCroix, & Sanborn, 2003; Hanba & Zaragoza, 2007; Zara-
at the follow-up interview [i.e., failure to report critical goza, Payment, Ackil, Drivdahl, & Beck, 2001). In this para-
details (Christianson & Bylin, 1999; Van Oorsouw & Merck- digm, participants (children or adults) view certain stimuli
elbach, 2004)]. Although differences in omission rates or are subjected to a particular event and, after a delay,
have been found between participants who feign are forced to falsely describe an object/event (including
amnesia and those who report honestly during both inter- those that were absent during the exposure phase). More
views, no differences in omission errors have been demon- specifically, participants assigned to the forced confabula-
strated when compared to delayed, genuine responders tion condition are typically instructed to provide answers
(i.e., participants who do not recall at the initial interview to all posed questions (some answerable and others unans-
and respond genuinely only at the second interview; Sun, werable) even if they do not know the answer (i.e., forced
Punjabi, Greenberg, & Seamon, 2009; Van Oorsouw & guess), whereas those assigned to other conditions are
Merckelbach, 2004). This pattern of finding at times has either instructed not to guess (e.g., Otgaar et al., 2014) or
been considered to be support for a “feigned amnesia only guess when they would like to (e.g., Gombos,
effect” (e.g., Christianson & Bylin, 1999; Van Oorsouw & Pezdek, & Haymond, 2012). For example, in response to
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:20 14 November 2017

Merckelbach, 2004). Alternatively, in response to these the question of where a child was bleeding after falling
findings, some researchers have argued that there is off a chair (when in reality there was no blood visible) par-
nothing unique about the cognitive processes involved ticipants in the forced confabulated condition have to
in simulating amnesia specifically, that differences are provide some detail such as (falsely) reporting that the
only apparent due to variations in the opportunity to prac- child was bleeding on his knee (Ackil & Zaragoza, 2011).
tice the material, and that the memory-debilitating effects This act of forced confabulation is similar to many forensic
are purely methodological in nature (e.g., Sun et al., 2009). contexts in which a rememberer is coerced to speculate
In other words, Sun et al. (2009) claim that it may not be the how events occurred, as there is not necessarily any mali-
act of explicitly reporting a lack of memory for the experi- cious intent on the rememberer’s behalf.
ence but rather just simply a lack of practicing of the A fairly robust finding is that forced fabrications lead to
material that catalyses the memory-impairing effects of an increased rate of commission errors (e.g., Ackil & Zara-
feigned amnesia (e.g., Bornstein, Liebel, & Scarberry, 1998). goza, 2011; Chroback & Zaragoza, 2008, 2012; Gombos
Recent research by Otgaar, Prudon, and Jelicic (2017) et al., 2012; Van Oorsouw & Giesbrecht, 2008; Zaragoza
used the DIF method with one group feigning amnesia et al., 2001), but does not seem to affect number of
for the event. Interestingly, they found that the group correct details. For example, a typical result that has
who had to feign amnesia had impaired memory perform- emerged is that participants who are forced to confabulate
ance for the interview, similar to the DIF effect. This seems a detail (e.g., state that they saw a dog in the picture while
to indicate that feigning amnesia negatively affects in reality no dog was present) will state after a delay that
memory for the interview as well. Collectively, the studies they remember seeing that particular detail. Moreover,
on feigning amnesia suggest that this type of lying will this effect can be exacerbated by confirmatory feedback
result in the form of failing to remember aspects of the (e.g., Hanba & Zaragoza, 2007).
content of the event (i.e., omission errors), undermining Although forced confabulation leads to commission
overall memory performance during later attempts to errors, it is not entirely evident whether it results in false
provide truthful accounts – albeit that this effect only memories of completely new details (i.e., reporting an
appears when participants who have to feign amnesia object that was not present) or also in distortions of the
are compared with honest responders. Furthermore, original memory (i.e., alteration of a quality/detail of a
there is preliminary evidence showing that feigning present object/experience). Although this has not been
amnesia leads to omissions errors of the interview. teased apart in the forced confabulation research, it is
likely that it depends on what is being confabulated. If par-
ticipants confabulate entirely new details which do not
Lying and the creation of false details and
refer to the experienced event then a false memory is
memories
most likely to occur. However, if a participant confabulates
As noted above, there are various circumstances in which a detail related to something that has been present (see
rememberers themselves may generate incorrect infor- above example about the bleeding knee), a distortion is
mation to deceive a listener and that has the potential to likely to occur. Furthermore, there are mixed results for
be incorporated into one’s prospective memory reports whether self-generated misinformation is more readily
as well. In other words, self-generated fabrications can incorporated into memory than other-generated misinfor-
serve as another source of misinformation that might mation. For example, Pezdek, Lam, and Sperry (2009) found
threaten the reliability of a memory being queried during that misinformation provided by another person at initial
6 H. OTGAAR AND A. BAKER

questioning was more likely to be incorporated into fabricated detail may be reported at this point, and
memory at the follow-up interview than fabricated doing so undermines the overall accuracy of the
details (i.e., self-generated misinformation). However, memory, this new detail does not necessarily overwrite
others have found conflicting results, such as false (true) details of the experience. For example, if a victim fab-
memory rates that are comparable for the two sources, ricates that the culprit had a knife when in reality no
except that false recall rates associated with a narrative weapon was present, it is unlikely that the true memories
were higher for self-generated fabrications than inter- of the crime (the culprit stealing money) are adversely
viewer suggestions when the lie was given after overt affected. In this case, only the addition of a new false
resistance and pre-test warning (e.g., Ackil & Zaragoza, detail will undermine the overall accuracy of a statement.
2011). As such, examining omission and commission errors only
Nonetheless, the forced confabulation research to date provides partial information about whether memory has
does suggest that forcing individuals to falsely describe a been affected by one’s lying but does not say much
non-existent detail/object increases the likelihood of that about (1) whether lying might also lead to memory distor-
detail/object being remembered later. Although this scen- tion of a particular detail and (2) whether lying might lead
ario represents some real-world contexts, research to increases or decreases in believing that the event truly
suggests that forcing rememberers to fabricate details occurred to oneself.
and voluntarily fabricating details may differentially influ- If we acknowledge this nuance, there has been some
ence memory with information being less likely to be preliminary research investigating whether attempts at
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:20 14 November 2017

remembered when one is forced to guess (e.g., Pezdek, lying distort one’s beliefs (e.g., Polage, 2004, 2012) and atti-
Sperry, & Owens, 2007). This suggests that false memories tudes (e.g., Brady, 2011) of having experienced a false
may be fostered even further with deliberate fabrication event. An important distinction is that these studies
and, more generally, that considering whether attempted include deliberate attempts at lying during which those
fabrication has occurred may be important when examin- whose memory is being examined intentionally misrepre-
ing resulting memories. sent their opinions or lie about their beliefs (rather than
An important issue related to the literature on confabu- being forced like in the forced confabulation paradigm).
lations is whether forced confabulations can be considered For example, in two studies, Polage (2004) had participants
a form of lying because participants are instructed to come write false accounts of events (that they had previously
up with a confabulatory response. However, this procedure rated as unlikely to have ever happened) and completed
does not vary substantially from a plethora of studies on an interview a week later during which they were ques-
deception detection in which participants are also tioned about the false events. After another one week
instructed to come up with a false statement just as is (Experiments 1 and 2) or five-week (Experiment 2) delay,
the case in work on forced confabulations (e.g., Bogaard, participants again were asked to honestly rate the likeli-
Meijer, Vrij, & Merckelbach, 2016). Given that lying is seen hood of events that were previously queried (as well as a
as a (un)successful attempt to create in someone else a control event that was not written about in the earlier
belief of which the communicator of the lie knows it is session). Interestingly, over half of participants substantially
not true (see Note 1), the act of forced confabulations reduced their likelihood ratings for lied-about items com-
approaches the way in which lying is defined in the decep- pared to likelihood ratings for the control item.5
tion detection field. Of course, one difference between However, there was also a small proportion of participants
studies on forced confabulations and lying is that in the who increased their belief in the occurrence of the lied-
former, participants sometimes resist the pressure to con- about event to the maximum possible (i.e., strongly
fabulate because they do not receive the literal and specific believed the event did occur). This research suggests that
instruction to deceive someone else. lying about an event may actually solidify the truth, as
Besides the studies reported above, there is additional opposed to undermine it, but that there may be individual
research showing that the act of lying has subtler effects variability associated with this effect.
than mere omissions and/or commissions. These studies To further investigate explanations for the small subset
speak more so to the question of whether lying truly of participants who came to strongly believe their lies in
alters the original memory and whether the rememberer earlier research (Polage, 2004), Polage (2012) employed a
genuinely believes the revised memory to be a true similar paradigm but assessed potential moderators associ-
account. Consider, for example, a witness to a crime who ated with several underlying cognitive mechanisms. In the
provides a false description of the event in question out follow-up study, lied-about events were associated with an
of fear of the perpetrator. Based on the literature reviewed increase in belief ratings compared to control events,
in the previous section on forced fabrications, there is evi- termed “fabrication inflation”. Additionally, the study
dence to suggest that details from this false account are revealed important information about individual differ-
incorporated into the witness’ memory for the event, ences that may be associated with an increased propensity
such that he/she may integrate the false detail and be to believe self-generated lies. For example, lied-about
more likely to report it later on if a decision is made to events were more likely to be believed later by remem-
abandon the lie for the truth. However, although a berers who self-reported lying more often within any
MEMORY 7

given week, had higher levels of dissociation, and who specific effect rather than having a negative impact on
experienced greater discomfort while lying. Interestingly, the memory more generally. Interestingly, approximately
subsequent research discovered that just the thought of one-third of fabricated details provided during the initial
having to lie or tell the truth about a past action had an interview were carried over to the questionnaire during
influence on memory for that action. Findings by Colwell which participants were supposed to be answering truth-
et al. (2011) demonstrated that when participants were fully based on what they remembered.7
simply told that they will later be required to lie about
their action of entering a professor’s office (such as
Theoretical frameworks
whether they stole an exam key while in the office) after
having already committed the act, their memory for the The above-reviewed literature indicates that certain decep-
experience in the office was characterised by anxiety and tive strategies are associated with different memorial out-
greater motivation to complete the task – even though comes. A pertinent next question concerns the
while in the office they were unaware of the fact that underlying mechanisms responsible for these differences.
they would later need to lie or tell the truth about this task. There are several theoretical perspectives that can help
Also, some research suggests that the effect of deliber- shed light on the ways in which lying affects memory. If
ate misrepresentation of previously held opinions on one’s we start by focusing on the memory effects of the arguably
memory may not be so clear-cut and may be a function of least complex form of lying discussed here – that is, false
certain target characteristics, such as attractiveness. Brady denials – then the work to date seems to suggest that
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:20 14 November 2017

(2011) examined whether misrepresentation of attitudes the act of falsely denying has adverse effects on memory
to either attractive or unattractive opposite-sex targets performance (e.g., Otgaar et al., 2016; Vieira & Lane,
altered one’s own memory for originally held opinions 2013). Specifically, there is evidence showing that false
(about a particular topic relevant to the student partici- denials lead to omissions for the content of stimuli but
pants such as mandatory examinations). Indeed, when par- only when compared to material that is studied and
ticipants were asked to report their original opinion about rehearsed. Furthermore, a recent line of research has
the topic, after misrepresenting their opinion to an attrac- demonstrated that false denials mainly impair memory
tive member of the opposite sex (whom held a substan- for what has been discussed during an interview. To
tially more positive opinion), participants’ originally some extent, these memory effects are linked to memory
reported opinion was altered such that it was more positive phenomena such as directed forgetting. The memory
and more in line with the target’s opinion. effects associated with directed forgetting have been pri-
Although these findings refer to deliberate misrepre- marily examined by using two methodologies (Basden,
sentation of beliefs and attitudes, other research has Basden, & Gargano, 1993): the list method and the word
looked at the memorial consequences of deliberate lying method. In the list method, participants are told to forget
using a procedure analogous with the forced fabrication a previously studied list of words before receiving a
research mentioned earlier. For example, in the only second list of words which they are instead instructed to
study to our knowledge to do so, Pickel (2004) assessed remember. In the word method, participants receive
the effect of intentional lying (about the appearance of instructions to forget or remember each presented word.
either a perpetrator or witness from a videotaped crime) The standard finding evidenced in both paradigms is that
on one’s memory. More specifically, participants assigned participants are less likely to recollect words that received
to fabrication conditions were instructed to answer all a cue to forget than words that had to be remembered
questions correctly except any questions about the (e.g., Bjork, 1972; Conway, Harries, Noyes, Racsma’ny, &
robber’s appearance (target fabrication condition) or the Frankish, 2000). The underlying mechanism associated
clerk’s appearance (i.e., a witness; clerk fabrication con- with the directed forgetting effect has commonly been
dition). For these questions, they were asked to construct ascribed to retrieval inhibition (Anderson & Neely, 1996;
a false description. The authors found that participants Levy & Anderson, 2002), such that the cue to forget
who fabricated details provided less correct information items inhibits the retrieval of those items thereby making
(i.e., omission errors) and more incorrect information (i.e., them less accessible when they need to be recollected.
commission errors) on a later memory test when instructed Although studies on false denials do not explicitly instruct
to respond honestly based on their actual memory (relative participants to forget items, they might try not to think
to truthful witnesses) but that any impairments to one’s about the items they have just witnessed and are falsely
memory was directly related to the target of their fabrica- denying as a strategy to aid their deceptive attempt.
tion.6 In other words, impaired memory of the perpetrator Further, it has been shown that such “no think” processes
(in the form of omission and commission errors) was are related to retrieval inhibition which might lead to a
shown only when participants had been instructed earlier decrease of unwanted memories entering consciousness
to fabricate details of the perpetrator but no memory (Anderson & Green, 2001; McWilliams, Goodman, Lyons,
impairment regarding the perpetrator’s appearance was Newton, & Avila-Mora, 2014).
apparent for those who fabricated details about the An alternative explanation for the impaired memory
witness in the video – suggesting that it may be a target resulting from false denials might be that doing so leads
8 H. OTGAAR AND A. BAKER

to a lack of rehearsal of details associated with the original memories for imagined events (i.e., those that are internally
event. Studies on feigned amnesia have frequently attribu- generated) which are assumed to contain more references
ted the deleterious memory effects of doing so to a lack of to cognitive operations (e.g., elaboration and retrieval).
rehearsal (e.g., Christianson & Bylin, 1999; Van Oorsouw & According to this framework, when people have to judge
Merckelbach, 2004), considering rehearsal of information whether a memory refers to an experienced or imagined
is one of the strongest ways to boost memory performance event, they will automatically attribute its status from the
(e.g., Dark & Loftus, 1976). According to the lack of rehear- characteristics of the memory in question. Subsequently,
sal explanation, feigning amnesia and even falsely denying source monitoring errors occur, for example, when a
might interfere with the rehearsal of information and this memory for an imagined event contains the same details
may lead to impoverished memory of the original event. (e.g., perceptual and contextual) as a memory for an experi-
Regarding feigned amnesia, Van Oorsouw and Merckel- enced event which might lead to an imagined event being
bach (2004) found support for this explanation during misattributed to an experienced event. In such case, a false
a study examining differential memory performance for a memory is produced.
mock crime (i.e., knocking down a mannequin with a Source monitoring is relevant to the current discussion
pool cue and theft). The critical findings were that the because in several studies mentioned earlier, participants
feigning amnesia and delayed testing group (those who were instructed to confabulate a false answer or even fab-
responded genuinely only one week after the event and ricate an entire story. Such fabrications require many cog-
not directly after) remembered less about the event than nitive resources in the sense that rememberers have to
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:20 14 November 2017

the honestly responding group (those who responded come up with new details while thinking about what
genuinely directly after the event and one week later), truly happened. The mere act of such fabrications can
but that there was no statistical difference in memory per- lead to an increase in characteristics (perceptual, affective,
formance between the feigning amnesia and delayed etc.) that are diagnostic of memories of experienced events
testing group. As the latter group did not engage in elab- because rememberers often rely on true past events or
orate rehearsal of the event, lack of rehearsal was proposed schemas to generate their false description and, hence,
as the most likely mechanism of the memory-undermining might lead to the creation of false memories. Sub-
effect associated with feigning amnesia. sequently, the side effect of increasing cognitive resources
Besides the theoretical explanations behind the effects during deceptive attempts such as fabrication is that the
of false denials and feigned amnesia, both deceptive strat- lying will have similar characteristics as genuine memories.
egies have shown to lead to some impairing effects of Indeed, a wealth of research on the imagination
memory. One potential commonality that may explain inflation effect has shown that imagining non-experienced
why these deceptive strategies seem to lead to similar events can elevate true memory characteristics such as
memory effects is that the cognitive resources required sensory details (e.g., Thomas, Bulevich, & Loftus, 2003).
for both denying and feigning amnesia are quite low (rela- Scientific work on implanted false memories has produced
tively speaking). That is, a person who denies or feigns similar findings; that is, participants who receive the sug-
amnesia uses a rather simple strategy (e.g., “I did not gestion from an interviewer that they experienced a false
steal anything”, “I cannot remember”) which does not childhood event and subsequently have to imagine experi-
require as many cognitive resources as fabricating an encing that event later falsely report experiencing it. The
entire story, for example. Indeed, as evidenced by forced standard finding is that 30–40% of participants fall prey
fabrication studies, other and more elaborate forms of for this suggestion (e.g., Loftus & Pickrell, 1995; Otgaar, Sco-
lying can draw substantially on our available cognitive boria, & Smeets, 2013; Shaw & Porter, 2015; Porter & Baker,
resources and appear to lead to the occurrence and report- 2015; Wade, Garry, Read, & Lindsay, 2002). The same might
ing of false details and memories. apply for deceptive strategies such as fabrication. When
A dominant theoretical framework that has been used participants are instructed to lie and tell a false story, this
to explain findings of reported false details and memories story might have the contours and characteristics of a
is the source monitoring framework (SMF; Johnson, Hash- memory for an experienced event. Of course, studies
troudi, & Lindsay, 1993) which elucidates the processes using the forced confabulation procedure provide a good
involved in making attributions concerning the origins of demonstration of fabrications leading to false memory
memories. Source monitoring refers to the processes (e.g., Ackil & Zaragoza, 2011; Nourkova, Bernstein, &
needed to differentiate between memories from various Loftus, 2004; Van Oorsouw & Giesbrecht, 2008). In these
sources, such as internal (e.g., thoughts, dreams, and studies, participants misattribute a made-up detail as
imaginations) and external (e.g., statements from something that they viewed during an encoding phase
someone else) sources. To monitor and differentiate and it’s likely that this source misattribution happens
these sources, several characteristics of memories and because the false details consist of similar memory charac-
judgmental processes come into play. The SMF posits teristics of authentic details. Evidence of this suggestion is
that, on average, memories for perceived events contain that previous lies that participants generated during first
more perceptual (e.g., sound and colour), contextual (e.g., session interviews have been found to be highly related
spatial and temporal), and affective (e.g., emotions) than to the experienced mock crime and it was found that
MEMORY 9

story length of the fabrication was positively related to the omissions or commissions. For example, one can exert a
number of commissions (Van Oorsouw & Giesbrecht, 2008). simple lie that does not draw many cognitive resources
A related study in which entire fabricated stories such as a false denial or simply feigning amnesia (“I do
resulted in the formation of false memories and, impor- not remember”) whereas telling more detailed lies that
tantly, provide theoretical support for the role of source entail fabricating details (forced confabulation) or even
monitoring was conducted by Chroback and Zaragoza entire stories requires more cognitive resources. We
(2008). Specifically, participants had to view a movie and, acknowledge that the boundaries associated with the
two days later, had to answer several questions concerning types of lying are not clearly defined and that using strat-
the movie (some of which referred to details from a section egies in tandem alters the complexity and amount of
of the video that was actually not presented during the resources. However, we postulate that when the draw
viewing). Participants who received these false event ques- of cognitive resources is less (such as in simple versions
tions were instructed to describe fictitious events that they of false denials), issues such as a lack of rehearsal come
had never seen but that were related to the progression of into play and lead to omissions but, when more cognitive
the movie. At an eight-week memory test many partici- resources are required, participants must remember
pants recalled having seen their own fabricated stories exactly what they lied about and remain consistent. As a
during the presentation of the movie – a finding that can result, the threat of source monitoring errors is lurking
be explained by relying on the SMF. That is, when partici- which potentially leads to commission errors/false
pants fabricated a portion of the story, following a delay memories.
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:20 14 November 2017

they are unable to monitor the source of those details A secondary component of the current MAD framework
and, because the fabricated stories were related to the is the way in which deceptive strategies alter beliefs. There
actual movie, source monitoring failures were more likely is a plethora of research showing that when participants
to occur. Interestingly, the explanation provided by Chro- are asked to imagine a fictitious childhood event, this
back and Zaragoza (2012) was that the fabricated stories increases the belief that the event was actually experi-
helped participants to achieve a causal explanation for enced by themselves (e.g., Garry, Manning, Loftus, &
the encoded stimuli they had witnessed. In this way, par- Sherman, 1996) and preliminary research on fabrication
ticipants obtained a coherent account of the perceived has found that it can lead to increases in belief that the
event. By doing so, the fabricated stories resulted in a false event took place (Polage, 2012). Again, this idea is
more complete and richer explanation of what occurred consistent with the SMF but is also linked with a new line
during the event (i.e., the movie) which may also be a ten- of research showing that believing in the occurrence of
dency for those lying about forensically relevant events. an event is the first step in recollecting that the event
Similar findings emerge from related research on the occurred (Otgaar, Scoboria, & Mazzoni, 2014; Scoboria,
effects of speculation on children’s memory (Schreiber, Memon, Gawrylowicz, & Clark, 2015). Arguably, such
Wentura, & Bilsky, 2001), arguably another form of intern- increases in belief may contribute to changes in recollec-
ally generated misinformation. The principal finding was tion when repeated deceptive attempts are used.
that after 5–6 months, children (aged three to six years Although this MAD model provides a general idea of
old) falsely remembered that their own speculative how lying affects memory for past experiences, this
answers occurred during a clown show event that they model is a starting point for future research endeavours
had previously witnessed. Collectively, the research to and, as such, needs refinement as our understanding of
date on fabrication and even speculation indicates that the relationship between lying and memory develops.
source monitoring failures may be the cause of the For example, one ill-explored issue is elucidating the mech-
obtained effects in the studies. anism behind DIF (see Figure 2). Secondly, some studies
have shown that feigning amnesia leads to omissions or
commissions but the exact reason for this remains
Memory and deception (MAD) framework
unclear. Although the most probable explanation for
Based on the findings and theoretical accounts discussed these divergent effects is that commission errors only
above, a picture is emerging regarding under which cir- occur when participants fabricate details in addition to
cumstances lying leads to omissions or commissions feigning amnesia, this issue has not been empirically
(false memories), and what underlying mechanisms may tested to such an extent yet. Third, one might argue that
be responsible for these memory outcomes. In Figure 2, false denials and fabrication differ on many other dimen-
we propose a theoretical framework illustrating memory sions and that they can impact memory performance as
outcomes as a function of different deceptive strategies. well. For example, one issue that is relevant here is what
In this MAD framework, we consider the different or how much precisely is denied or fabricated. Specifically,
memory outcomes and lying strategies as being part of a a victim of repeated sexual abusive incidents might simply
continuum. More specifically, we conceptualise the act of use the strategy to deny all occasions or could choose to
lying as on a continuum such that different types of lies deny a certain event. One might assume that the first
vary in the degree of cognitive resources that they denial (complete false denial) requires fewer cognitive
require which helps to predict when lying leads to resources than the second one because in the second
10 H. OTGAAR AND A. BAKER
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:20 14 November 2017

Figure 2. Theoretical memory and deception (MAD) framework outlining the memorial effects as a function of type of lying.

denial the victim has to simultaneously make sure that he/ more elaborate by adding an alternative account (e.g.,
she is denying a specific event and, for example, disclosing “that did not occur because I was on a holiday and could
the other episodes. Of course, future research should not be there”).
examine other factors that might affect the act of false Future research also can address individual deceptive
denials and fabrication and their influence on memory. strategies and subsequent effects on memory to help
refine our current understanding. For example, the appar-
ent different results between studies on false denials and
Future directions and predictions memory provides an opportunity for further investigation.
A logical next step to examine the predictive value of MAD More specifically, there have been discrepancies in regards
is to conduct a study in which the effects of different to whether false denials lead to forgetting of what was lied
deceptive strategies (false denial, feigning amnesia, and about or a more global forgetting such as forgetting details
fabrication) on memory are examined at once. Such a associated with how/when the lie was told. For example,
study could provide a more advanced understanding and memory of the interview was affected by the act of
may provide additional evidence that false memory pro- falsely denying in the studies on DIF (Otgaar et al., 2016),
duction is elevated when lying involves more cognitive while other research found subtle forgetting effects of
resources. Further, to thoroughly investigate the role of the falsely denied stimuli (Vieira & Lane, 2013). Sub-
cognitive resources, an interesting next direction would sequently, the features by which this forgetting effect
be to manipulate the amount of cognitive resources exists needs to be researched further.
required in a more refined way, by making simple lies Similarly, another relevant research avenue concerns
such as false denials more elaborate (thereby requiring the effects of feigning amnesia on memory. As can be
more cognitive resources). According to the MAD frame- seen in the MAD framework, the evidence so far seems
work, omissions likely occur when a false denial is quite to show that feigning amnesia leads to omissions but
simple (e.g., “that did not occur”) but commissions may there also is research showing that it might lead to false
actually be elicited by false denials when they become memories/commissions (Van Oorsouw & Giesbrecht,
MEMORY 11

2008). Importantly, past studies often fail to differentiate (required to feign symptoms of a serious psychological
between feigned amnesia and fabrication; that is, instruc- condition in a credible way whereas control participants
tions given to participants do not necessarily reflect com- had to respond honestly), later reported significantly higher
plete (or even partial) amnesia. Moreover, the studies symptoms at the second testing session (when asked to
seem to suggest that participants may have employed respond honestly) than the control group. The authors con-
strategies of withholding the truth which are more cluded that this finding suggests there is a residual effect of
similar to fabrication than amnesia. For example, Christian- feigning symptoms which aligns well with the proposed
son and Bylin (1999) instructed participants to “describe in MAD framework; that is, the prediction based on the MAD
writing the course of events so as to evade responsibility, model is that when fabrication occurs, people tend to
as if you did not remember very well”. Although sub- adopt their own fabrications. However, a logical next step
sequent research did a relatively better job by being would be to assess what would occur when participants
more specific with instruction, such as “minimize your are instructed to deny symptoms. Although perhaps not so
responsibility by simulating a memory disorder. Thus, try relevant for forensic purposes, it would be of theoretically
to describe events in such a way so it looked like you importance and, according to MAD, would result in a
have great difficulties in remembering what happened” decrease in the reporting of symptoms.
(Van Oorsouw & Merckelbach, 2004) and “feign amnesia” Finally, our argument is that forced confabulation, a
(Van Oorsouw & Merckelbach, 2006), the available option component of the MAD framework, parallels certain other
of providing a fabricated alternative story is still present types of lies. One obvious candidate is the domain of false
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:20 14 November 2017

in both cases. Indeed, one study reported that one-third alibis. That is, in legal contexts, it is not uncommon that wit-
of participants instructed to “feign amnesia” claimed their nesses and offenders provide false alibis to increase their
strategy for doing so was to provide an alternative credibility (Allison, Mathews, & Michael, 2012). However,
account of how the events happened (Van Oorsouw & what happens with their memories when a truthful
Merckelbach, 2006). Providing further evidence of the sug- account is provided after the fact is unknown at the
gestion that both attempts at feigning amnesia and fabri- moment (see for a related issue; Sauerland, Mehlkopf, Krix,
cation may be occurring, Sun et al. (2009) found that & Sagana, 2016). We would predict that creating a false
those instructed to feign amnesia made significantly alibi requires cognitive resources and might lead to the cre-
more intrusion errors on the initial memory test than com- ation of distorted beliefs and even false memories. As such,
parison groups. Although it could be argued that this is the work of false alibis would be highly related to work on
representative of simulated amnesia claims outside of the forced fabrications. One other field in which the effects of
lab, such that offenders may claim amnesia for certain lying on memory can be examined is work on children’s
aspects of an experience while simultaneously fabricating recantations (e.g., Malloy, Mugno, Rivard, Lyon, & Quas, in
other aspects, it clouds interpretation because we are press). In many child sexual abuse cases, children report to
unable to determine what findings are being generated have been abused, but some time after this, recant their
by feigning amnesia and which are being driven by initial claims. Such recantations are closely linked to false
active fabrication of details. For example, it could be specu- denials. Studies have shown that recantation rates can be
lated that findings of increased commission errors (e.g., as high as 25% in substantiated child sexual abuse cases
Van Oorsouw & Merckelbach, 2006) may be due to fabrica- (Malloy, Lyon, & Quas, 2007). The crucial question here is
tion and may not be appropriately attributed to feigning what the impact is of such recantations on children’s
amnesia. In sum, to what degree fabrication versus feign- memory for the original events. As they can be seen as
ing amnesia contributes to the mnemonic effects found (false) denials, our expectation would be that such recanta-
in previous studies is not adequately teased apart which tions would have adverse effects on memory and might
is worthwhile because fabrication seems to have different lead to omission errors.
effects on memory than feigning amnesia.
Relatedly, the issue of simulating amnesia can be con-
Conclusion
sidered to fall under the more general umbrella theme of
malingering and the current discussion is linked to recent The present discussion began with the conundrum about
research concerning the residual effects of other types of the effects of lying on memory. After assembling the
malingering (e.g., Merckelbach, Dandachi-Fitzgerald, Van current state of knowledge in this domain, the most compel-
Mulken, Ponds, & Niesten, 2015; Merckelbach, Jelicic, & ling answer appears to be that it depends on the type of
Pieters, 2011). Malingering in a medical and legal setting lying and how many cognitive resources are involved.
refers to the feigning of symptoms motivated by external More specifically, details constructed via lying can become
reasons (e.g., financial compensation; American Psychiatric a (false) memory, but this seems to only be the case in
Association, 2000) and thus can be treated as a type of lie. those situations when rememberers use a deceptive tactic
Previous studies have found that the exaggeration (or fab- that involves many cognitive resources such as fabricating
rication) of symptoms leads to adoption of non-existing a story. When a type of lying is used that does not involve
symptoms. For example, Merckelbach et al. (2011; Exper- many cognitive resources, our review of the literature indi-
iment 1) found that those assigned to a feigning condition cates that is more likely to lead to omissions.
12 H. OTGAAR AND A. BAKER

To our knowledge, the current review is the first com- Disclosure statement
prehensive synopsis on the effects of lying on memory. No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
Given the various motivations perpetrators, victims, or wit-
nesses may have to withhold the truth about crime-rel-
evant details of probative significance, we consider the
current discussion as being of great value to investigative References
settings. Indeed, much of the reviewed studies have Ackil, J. K., & Zaragoza, M. S. (1998). Memorial consequences of forced
been conducted from a pure applied legal interest, such confabulation: Age differences in susceptibility to false memories.
as the issue of feigning amnesia in offenders or false Developmental Psychology, 34, 1358–1372. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.
34.6.1358
denials of abuse by victims. Our outline of these studies Ackil, J. K., & Zaragoza, M. S. (2011). Forced fabrication versus inter-
suggests that besides looking at external factors (e.g., sug- viewer suggestions: Differences in false memory depend on how
gestive interviewing tactics) that might hamper statements memory is assessed. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 933–942.
of witnesses, victims, and offenders, it is also important to doi:10.1002/acp.1785
investigate whether lying might have played a role in their Allison, M., Mathews, K. R., & Michael, S. W. (2012). Alibi believability:
The impact of salacious alibi activities. Social Behavior and
statements. If there are indications that statements have Personality: An International Journal, 40, 605–612. doi:10.2224/sbp.
been altered dramatically, like when an alleged victim 2012.40.4.605
has denied being abused and then after many years American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic criteria from DRM-
comes forward with the statements, then the current dis- IV-TR. Washington, DC: Author.
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:20 14 November 2017

cussion can inform professionals concerning the quality Anderson, M. C., & Green, C. (2001). Supressing unwanted memories by
executive control. Nature, 410, 366–369. doi:10.1038/35066572
of such statements. Deceptive strategies of these kinds Anderson, M. C., & Neely, J. H. (1996). Interference and inhibition in
can have serious legal ramifications as threats to the memory retrieval. In E. L. Bjork & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Memory:
reliability of memory in a criminal context may contribute Handbook of perception and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 237–313). San
to deserving offenders avoiding punishment and sub- Diego, CA: Academic Press.
sequently risking public safety. Our view is that by Basden, B. H., Basden, D. R., & Gargano, G. J. (1993). Directed forgetting
in implicit and explicit memory tests: A comparison of methods..
synthesising the existing literature and summarising our Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
current understanding of the effects of one’s own lies Cognition, 19, 603–616. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.19.3.603
on memory for past experiences future research addres- Bjork, R. A. (1972). Theoretical implications of directed forgetting. In A.
sing gaps in our empirical understanding of this impor- Melton & E. Martin (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory (pp.
tant and forensically relevant relationship will be 217–235). Washington, DC: Winston.
Block, S. D., Shestowsky, D., Segovia, D. A., Goodman, G. S., Schaaf, J. M.,
encouraged. & Alexander, K. W. (2012). “That never happened”: Adults’ discern-
ment of children’s true and false memory reports. Law and
Human Behavior, 36, 365–374. doi:10.1037/h0093920
Notes Bogaard, G., Meijer, E. H., Vrij, A., & Merckelbach, H. (2016). Scientific
content analysis (SCAN) cannot distinguish between truthful and
1. In this paper, we use the definition of lying provided by Vrij fabricated accounts of a negative event. Frontiers in Psychology, 7,
(2008, p. 15): “a successful or unsuccessful deliberate attempt, 243.
without forewarning, to create in another a belief which the Bornstein, B. H., Liebel, L. M., & Scarberry, N. C. (1998). Repeated testing
communicator considers to be untrue”. in eyewitness memory: A means to improve recall of a negative
2. Although typical cognitive models describe lying as inhibiting emotional event. Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology.
the truth, choosing the lie, and executing the lie, we will be Paper 177.
referring to this process as “withholding the truth” to be Bourget, D., & Bradford, J. M. W. (1995). Sex offenders who claim
more encompassing of the different deceptive strategies of amnesia for their alleged offense. Journal of the American
interest to the current discussion. Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 23, 299–307.
3. We will be consistently using the term feigning amnesia but Brady, S. E. (2011). Motivation, misrepresentation, and memory
consider it to be the same as simulated amnesia, another com- (Unpublished thesis). University of Nebraska, Nebraska.
monly used term. Carlisle, A. (2014). I’m not guilty: The case of Ted Bundy. Encino, CA:
4. We will be referring to confabulation and fabrication synony- Genius Books.
mously throughout the paper, in line with most of the Christianson, S. A., & Bylin, S. (1999). Does simulating amnesia mediate
literature. genuine forgetting for a crime event? Applied Cognitive Psychology,
5. Similar findings were maintained at the five-week time interval 13, 495–511. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199912)13:6<495::AID-
in Experiment 2. ACP615>3.0.CO;2-0
6. Importantly, a no rehearsal condition performed better than Christ, S. E., Van Essen, D. C., Watson, J. M., Brubaker, L. E., & McDermott,
the fabrication condition – evidence that suggests the effect K. B. (2009). The contributions of prefrontal cortex and executive
of impaired memory did not simply occur due to lack of control to deception: Evidence from activation likelihood estimate
rehearsal. meta-analyses. Cerebral Cortex, 19(7), 1557–1566. doi:10.1093/
7. A second study conducted by Pickel (2004) assessed how the cercor/bhn189
magnitude of this effect compared to misinformation supplied Chroback, Q. M., & Zaragoza, M. S. (2008). Inventing stories: Forcing
by another person and found that participants exposed to fab- witnesses to fabricate entire fictitious events leads to freely
ricated information generated by an external source carried reported false memories. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 1190–
over more false details to the later memory test; however, 1195. doi:10.3758/PBR.15.6.1190
overall accuracy of memory did not differ between the self- Chroback, Q. M., & Zaragoza, M. S. (2012). The misinformation effect. In
generated and other-generated fabrication groups. A. M. Ridley, F. Gabbert, & D. J. La Rooy (Eds.), Suggestibility in legal
MEMORY 13

contexts: Psychological research and forensic implications (pp. 21–44). Loftus, E. F. (2005). Planting misinformation in the human mind: A 30-
London: Wiley Blackwell. year investigation of the malleability of memory. Learning &
Cima, M., Merckelbach, H., Hollnack, S., & Knauer, E. (2003). Memory, 12, 361–366. doi:10.1101/lm.94705
Characteristics of psychiatric prison inmates who claim amnesia. Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile
Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 373–380. doi:10.1016/ destruction: An example of the interaction between language
S0191-8869(02)00199-X and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13,
Cima, M., Merckelbach, H., Nijman, H., Knauer, E., & Hollnack, S. (2002). I 585–589. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(74)80011-3
can’t remember your honor: Offenders who claim amnesia. German Loftus, E. F., & Pickrell, J. E. (1995). The formation of false memories.
Journal of Psychiatry, 5, 24–34. Psychiatric Annals, 25, 720–725. doi:10.3928/0048-5713-19951201-07
Colwell, K., Hiscock-Anisman, C., Corbett, L., Memon, A., Hauselt, W. J., & Lyon, T. D. (2007). False denials: Overcoming methodological biases in
Bonilla, Y. (2011). Change in suspect’s memory as a result of decep- abuse disclosure research. In M. Pipe, M. E. Lamb, & Y. Orbach (Eds.),
tion. American Journal of Forensic Psychology, 29, 1–9. Child sexual abuse: Disclosure, delay and denial (pp. 41–62). New
Conway, M. A., Harries, K., Noyes, J., Racsma’ny, M., & Frankish, C. R. York, NY: Routledge.
(2000). The disruption and dissolution of directed forgetting: Malloy, L. C., Lyon, T. D., & Quas, J. A. (2007). Filial dependency and
Inhibitory control of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, recantation of child sexual abuse allegations. Journal of the
43, 409–430. doi:10.1006/jmla.2000.2706 American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 162–170.
Dark, V. J., & Loftus, G. R. (1976). The role of rehearsal in long-term doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000246067.77953.f7
memory performance. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Malloy, L. C., Mugno, A. P., Rivard, J. R., Lyon, T. D., & Quas, J. A. (in
Behavior, 15, 479–490. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(76)90043-8 press). Familial influences on recantation in substantiated child
DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Chalrton, K., sexual abuse cases. Child Maltreatment.
& Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin, 129, McWilliams, K., Goodman, G. S., Lyons, K. E., Newton, J., & Avila-Mora, E.
74–118. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.74 (2014). Memory for child sexual abuse information: Simulated
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:20 14 November 2017

Frank, M. G., & Feeley, T. H. (2003). To catch a liar: Challenges for memory error and individual differences. Memory & Cognition, 42,
research in lie detection training. Journal of Applied 151–163. doi:10.3758/s13421-013-0345-2
Communication Research, 31, 58–75. doi:10.1080/00909880305377 Merckelbach, H., Dandachi-Fitzgerald, B., Van Mulken, P., Ponds, R., &
Frost, P., LaCroix, D., & Sanborn, N. (2003). Increasing false recognition Niesten, E. (2015). Exaggerating psychopathology produces
rates with confirmatory feedback: A phenomenological analysis. residual effects that are resistant to corrective feedback: An exper-
The American Journal of Psychology, 116, 515–525. doi:10.2307/ imental demonstration. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 22, 16–22.
1423658 doi:10.1080/23279095.2013.816850
Ganis, G., Kosslyn, S. M., Stose, S., Thompson, W. L., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. Merckelbach, H., Jelicic, M., & Pieters, M. (2011). The residual effect of
A. (2003). Neural correlates of different types of deception: An fMRI feigning: How intentional faking may evolve into a less conscious
investigation. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 830–836. doi:10.1093/cercor/13.8.830 form of symptom reporting. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Ganis, G., Morris, R. R., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2009). Neural processes under- Psychopathology, 33, 131–139.
lying self- and other-related lies: An individual difference approach Nourkova, V., Bernstein, D., & Loftus, E. (2004). Altering traumatic
using fMRI. Social Neuroscience, 4, 539–553. doi:10.1080/ memory. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 575–585. doi:10.1080/
17470910801928271 02699930341000455
Garry, M., Manning, C. G., Loftus, E. F., & Sherman, S. J. (1996). Otgaar, H., Candel, I., Merckelbach, H., & Wade, K. A. (2009). Abducted
Imagination inflation: Imagining a childhood event inflates confi- by a UFO: Prevalence information affects young children’s false
dence that it occurred. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 208–214. memories for an implausible event. Applied Cognitive Psychology,
doi:10.3758/BF03212420 23, 115–125. doi:10.1002/acp.1445
Garven, S., Wood, J. M., Malpass, R. S., & Shaw, J. S. (1998). More than Otgaar, H., Candel, I., Smeets, T., & Merckelbach, H. (2010). ‘You didn’t
suggestion: The effect of interviewing techniques from the take Lucy’s skirt off’: The effect of misleading information on omis-
McMartin preschool case.. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 347– sions and commissions in children’s memory reports. Legal and
359. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.83.3.347 Criminological Psychology, 15, 229–241. doi:10.1348/
Gombos, V., Pezdek, K., & Haymond, K. (2012). Forced confabulation 135532509X471951
affects memory sensitivity as well as response bias. Memory and Otgaar, H., Howe, M. L., Brackmann, N., & Smeets, T. (2016). The malle-
Cognition, 40, 127–134. doi:10.3758/s13421-011-0129-5 ability of developmental trends in neutral and negative memory
Goodman-Brown, T. B., Edelstein, R. S., Goodman, G. S., Jones, D. P. H., & illusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 31–55.
Gordon, D. S. (2003). Why children tell: A model of children’s disclos- doi:10.1037/xge0000127
ure of sexual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27, 525–540. doi:10.1016/ Otgaar, H., Howe, M. L., Memon, A., & Wang, J. (2014). The development
S0145-2134(03)00037-1 of differential mnemonic effects of false denials and forced confa-
Gudjonsson, G. H., Sigurdsson, J. F., & Einarsson, E. (2004). The role of bulations. Behavioral Sciences & Law, 32, 718–731. doi:10.1002/bsl.
personality in relation to confessions and denials. Psychology, 2148
Crime & Law, 10, 125–135. doi:10.1080/10683160310001634296 Otgaar, H., Howe, M. L., Smeets, T., & Wang, J. (2016). Denial-induced
Hanba, J. M., & Zaragoza, M. S. (2007). Interviewer feedback in repeated forgetting: False denials undermine memory, but external denials
interviews involving forced confabulation. Applied Cognitive undermine belief. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and
Psychology, 21, 433–455. doi:10.1002/acp.1286 Cognition, 5, 168–175. doi:10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.04.002
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitor- Otgaar, H., Prudon, C., & Jelicic, M. (2017). Denial-induced forgetting in
ing. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–28. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3 feigning amnesia. Manuscript in preparation.
Kalant, H. (1996). Intoxicated automatism: Legal concept vs. scientific Otgaar, H., Scoboria, A., & Mazzoni, G. (2014). On the existence and
evidence. Contemporary Drug Problems, 23, 631–648. doi:10.1177/ implications of nonbelieved memories. Current Directions in
009145099602300406 Psychological Science, 23, 349–354. doi:10.1177/0963721414542102
Kopelman, M. D. (1995). The assessment of psychogenic amnesia. In Otgaar, H., Scoboria, A., & Smeets, T. (2013). Experimentally evoking
A. D. Baddeley, B.A. Wilson, & F. N. Watts (Eds.), Handbook of nonbelieved memories for childhood events. Journal of
memory disorders (pp. 427–448). New York, NY: Wiley. Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39,
Levy, B. J., & Anderson, M. C. (2002). Inhibitory processes and the 717–730. doi:10.1037/a0029668
control of memory retrieval. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 299– Pezdek, K., Lam, S. T., & Sperry, K. (2009). Forced confabulation more
305. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01923-X strongly influences event memory if suggestions are other-
14 H. OTGAAR AND A. BAKER

generated than self-generated. Legal and Criminological Psychology, Sporer, S. L., & Schwandt, B. (2006). Paraverbal indicators of deception:
14, 241–252. doi:10.1348/135532508X344773 A meta-analytic synthesis. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 421–
Pezdek, K., Sperry, K., & Owens, S. M. (2007). Interviewing witnesses: 446. doi:10.1002/acp.1190
The effect of forced confabulation on event memory. Law and Sun, X., Punjabi, P. V., Greenberg, L. T., & Seamon, J. G. (2009). Does
Human Behavior, 31, 463–478. doi:10.1007/s10979-006-9081-5 feigning amnesia impair subsequent recall? Memory & Cognition,
Pickel, K. (2004). When a lie becomes the truth: The effects of self-gen- 37, 81–89. doi:10.3758/MC.37.1.81
erated misinformation on eyewitness memory. Memory, 12, 14–26. Thomas, A. K., Bulevich, J. B., & Loftus, E. F. (2003). Exploring the role
doi:10.1080/09658210244000072 of repetition and sensory elaboration in the imagination
Polage, D. C. (2004). Fabrication deflation? The mixed effects of lying inflation effect. Memory & Cognition, 31, 630–640. doi:10.3758/
on memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 455–465. doi:10. BF03196103
1002/acp.995 Valentine, T., & Mesout, J. (2009). Eyewitness identification under stress
Polage, D. C. (2012). Fabrication inflation increases as source monitor- in the London Dungeon. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 151–161.
ing ability decreases. Acta Psychologica, 139, 335–342. doi:10.1016/j. doi:10.1002/acp.1463
actpsy.2011.12.007 Van Oorsouw, K., & Giesbrecht, T. (2008). Minimizing culpability
Porter, S., & Baker, A. (2015). CSI (crime scene induction): Creating false increases commission errors in a mock crime paradigm. Legal and
memories of committing crime. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, Criminological Psychology, 13, 335–344. doi:10.1348/
716–718. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.014 135532507X228539
Porter, S., Birt, A. R., Hervé, H., & Yuille, J. C. (2001). Memory for Van Oorsouw, K., & Merckelbach, H. (2004). Feigning amnesia under-
murder: A psychological perspective on dissociative amnesia in mines memory for a mock crime. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18,
forensic contexts. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 24, 505–518. doi:10.1002/acp.999
23–42. Van Oorsouw, K., & Merckelbach, H. (2006). Simulating amnesia and
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: memories of a mock crime. Psychology, Crime & Law, 12, 261–271.
Downloaded by [EP- IPSWICH] at 02:20 14 November 2017

Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of doi:10.1080/10683160500224477


Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, Vieira, K. M., & Lane, S. M. (2013). How you lie affects what you remem-
803–814. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803 ber. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 2, 173–
Sauerland, M., Mehlkopf, S., Krix, A. C., & Sagana, A. (2016). Deceiving 178. doi:10.1016/j.jarmac.2013.05.005
suspects about the content of their alibis: Consequences for truthful Vrij, A. (2008). Detecting lies and deceit: Pitfalls and opportunities.
and untruthful suspects. The Journal of Forensic Practice, 18, 143– Chichester: Wiley.
154. doi:10.1108/JFP-10-2014-0042 Vrij, A., & Heaven, S. (1999). Vocal and verbal indicators of deception as
Schreiber, N., Wentura, D., & Bilsky, W. (2001). “What else could he have a function of lie complexity. Psychology, Crime & Law, 5, 203–215.
done?” Creating false answers in child witnesses by inviting specu- doi:10.1080/10683169908401767
lation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 525–532. doi:10.1037/0021- Wade, K. A., Garry, M., Read, J. D., & Lindsay, S. (2002). A picture is worth
9010.86.3.525 a thousand lies: Using false photographs to create false childhood
Scoboria, A., Memon, A., Gawrylowicz, J., & Clark, A. (2015). memories. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 597–603. doi:10.3758/
Nonbelieved memories across the adult lifespan. Psychology of BF03196318
Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 2, 461–474. doi:10. Walczyk, J. J., Harris, L. L., Duck, T. K., & Mulay, D. (2014). A social-cog-
1037/cns0000051 nitive framework for understanding serious lies: Activation-
Shaw, J., & Porter, S. (2015). Constructing rich false memories of com- decision-construction-action theory. New Ideas in Psychology, 34,
mitting crime. Psychological Science, 26, 291–301. doi:10.1177/ 22–36. doi:10.1016/j.newideapsych.2014.03.001
0956797614562862 Warren, G., Schertler, E., & Bull, P. (2009). Detecting deception from
Sjöberg, R. L., & Lindblad, F. (2002). Limited disclosure of sexual abuse emotional and unemotional cues. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,
in children whose experiences were documented by videotape. 33, 59–69. doi:10.1007/s10919-008-0057-7
American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 312–314. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp. Zaragoza, M. S., Payment, K. E., Ackil, J. K., Drivdahl, S. B., & Beck, M.
159.2.312 (2001). Interviewing witnesses: Forced confabulation and confirma-
Smith, D., & Resnick, P. R. (2007). Amnesia and competence to stand tory feedback increase false memories. Psychological Science, 12,
trial. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 473–477. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00388
35, 541–543. Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B. M., & Rosenthal, R. (1981). Verbal and non-
Sporer, S. L. (2016). Deception and cognitive load: Expanding our verbal communication of deception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances
horizon with a working memory model. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, in experimental social psychology (pp. 1–59). New York, NY:
261. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00420 Academic Press.
Copyright of Memory is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may not be
copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for
individual use.

You might also like