Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2006)

xx:1–9 DOI 10.1007/s00787-006-0550-7 ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION

Barbara W.C. Zwirs Ethnic differences in parental detection of


Huibert Burger
Jan K. Buitelaar externalizing disorders
Tom W.J. Schulpen

j Abstract Background Previous and Surinamese parents when


Accepted: 28 March 2006 /
Published online: 9 May 2006 research has reported lower treat- compared to Dutch parents. Sen-
ment rates for externalizing dis- sitivity to detect ADHD tended to
orders among non-Western be lower among Turkish parents.
children as compared to Western Specificity to detect any external-
children. Ethnic differences in izing disorder was higher among
parental detection may be an Moroccan and Turkish parents.
explanation for this discrepancy. Specificity to detect ADHD was
Aims In a cross-sectional study higher among Moroccan parents
among the four largest ethnic and tended to be higher among
B.W.C. Zwirs, MD (&) Æ T.W.J. Schulpen, groups in the Netherlands, namely Turkish parents. Conclusions The
MD Dutch, Moroccan, Turkish and detection rate of externalizing
Dept. of Paediatrics Surinamese, we examined the disorders is markedly lower
University Medical Centre influence of ethnicity on parental among non-Dutch parents than
P.O. Box 85090
3508 AB Utrecht, The Netherlands
detection of behavioural disorders. among Dutch parents. This find-
Tel.: +31-30/2504-932 Method A total of 270 children ing emphasizes the importance of
E-Mail: b.zwirs@wkz.azu.nl (aged 6–10 years) and their par- taking parents’ cultural context
H. Burger, MD ents were interviewed regarding into account when appraising
Julius Centre for Health Sciences and psychiatric disorders and socio- their report on possible external-
Primary Care demographic data. Sensitivity and izing disorders in their children.
University Medical Centre Utrecht specificity were calculated by
Utrecht, The Netherlands
using standard definitions, with j Key words externalizing disor-
J.K. Buitelaar, MD adjustment for parental educa- ders – ethnicity – parental detec-
Radboud University Nijmegen Medical tional level. Results Sensitivity to tion – best-estimate diagnosis –
Centre
Dept. of Psychiatry, and Academic
detect any externalizing disorder Strengths and Difficulties Ques-
Centre for Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and ADHD in particular was sig- tionnaire (SDQ)
Nijmegen, The Netherlands nificantly lower among Moroccan

quency [25]. The combination of stimulant and


Introduction psychosocial treatment, has been shown to be effec-
Externalizing disorders, such as attention-deficit tive in reducing symptoms of externalizing disorders
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), oppositional defiant [19].
disorder (ODD) and conduct disorder (CD) are However, both in the US [24] and as well as in
associated with substantial unfavourable long-term Europe [35] the treatment rates for behavioural dis-
outcomes. Among these are impairments in academic orders have reported to be lower in non-Western
ECAP 550

and psychosocial functioning [1], substance use dis- children as compared to Western children. Since
order, antisocial personality disorder [17] and delin- parental identification of the child’s problem behav-
2 Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry, (2006) Vol. xx, No. x
Ó Steinkopff Verlag 2006

iour has found to be a prerequisite for mental health (ADEON-study), which was designed to examine
service use [26], ethnic differences in parental detec- prevalence of behavioural disorders and service use in
tion of behavioural problems may be an explanation children of the four largest ethnic groups in the
for the observed discrepancy in treatment status be- Netherlands. For the first phase of the ADEON-study
tween Western and non-Western children. [35], parents of 2802 children, enrolled in grade three
Although few studies have examined the extent of through five of mainstream schools in two large cities
ethnicity bias in reporting on behavioural problems, in the Netherlands, Amsterdam and Utrecht, were
there is some evidence to suggest that cross-cultural asked permission for their child’s teacher to be
differences in perceptions do exist. For instance, administered the teacher version of the Strengths and
within the vignette approach perceptual differences Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) [6]. The SDQ is a
towards externalizing problems have been observed brief and comprehensive questionnaire that has been
between Chinese and Indonesian clinicians on the one translated into more than 40 languages and assessed
hand and Japanese and American clinicians on the in several cultures yielding evidence of good psy-
other hand [16], but also between Jamaican and chometric properties across different countries [23],
American teachers and parents [14]. Furthermore, in including the Netherlands [20, 31].
observational studies, perceptual differences on To obtain a sample with all four ethnicities rep-
problem behaviour have been reported for African resented, we solely sampled schools from areas with a
American and Jamaican teachers [22], and for Thai large immigrant population, which are characterized
and American teachers [32]. Nevertheless, in a sample by a low SES [12]. Children with another ethnic origin
of bicultural families, each with a Thai and a Cauca- than Dutch, Moroccan, Turkish or Surinamese, were
sian-American parent, no differences were observed excluded from the study (N = 336). Information by
in parental perspectives on problem behaviour [33]. teacher report was gathered about problem behav-
Notwithstanding the relevance of these studies, iour, level of impairment and treatment status of 2185
they suffer from some limitations. For instance, the children out of 2466 children (89%). Teachers in the
vignette approach used to assess the possible effect of Netherlands are accurately informed about the treat-
parental culture on reports of problem behaviour ment status of the child, as the mental health care
suffers from the limitation that it is not clear to what system cooperates with them during the treatment
extent parents’ vignette responses are in line with process e.g. teachers are asked to fill in questionnaires
their judgements on their own children in real life about the behaviour of the children in the school.
[33]. In addition, in observational research children For the second phase of the study, which is de-
are observed in specific settings during a restrictive scribed in the present article, we excluded children
time period. As a result, the information obtained by who were treated for behavioural problems or other
observers may not be as comprehensive as that of psychiatric disorders (N = 104). Subsequently, we
teachers or parents [32]. Finally, intercultural couples selected all children scoring above the 90th percentile
may hold cultural values that differ from their cultural on the Hyperactivity Scale and the Conduct Problems
mainstream, blurring the intercultural differences that Scales on the SDQ (N = 153) and a random sample of
are the focus of bicultural parent-versus-parent de- children scoring below this cut-off (N = 223) result-
sign [33]. Cross-cultural research that compares ing in 376 eligible children. The 90th percentile cut-
parental judgements of their own child’s problem off-score was not the same as the UK-sample, but
behaviour to a standard of validity does not exist. based on the present sample. A total of 72% of the
The present study was designed to examine whe- parents agreed to participate (N = 270), with no sig-
ther ethnicity influences sensitivity and specificity of nificant variation across ethnicity. For logistic rea-
parents to detect behavioural disorders. We compared sons, one child and 13 parents were unable to
parental reports to best-estimate diagnoses in a cross- participate in the interview. Consequently, 269 child
sectional study among the four largest ethnic groups interviews and 257 parent interviews were left for the
in the Netherlands, namely: Dutch, Moroccan, Turk- analyses. From 256 children both parent and child
ish and Surinamese. interviews were obtained.

Methods j Instruments
j Subjects Parents were administered at the Diagnostic Interview
Schedule for Children–Parent Version. The DISC-P is
The sample consisted of a subset of participants of a a highly structured diagnostic interview and can be
larger study on the detection of ADHD among chil- used by lay-persons to generate valid DSM-IV diag-
dren of Different Ethnic Origins in the Netherlands noses [28]. The instrument consists of six modules,
B.W.C. Zwirs et al. 3
Ethnic differences in parental detection of externalizing disorders

each containing related diagnoses (Anxiety, Mood, tionnaire included 10 DSM-IV items: 2 ADHD items
Disruptive, Substance Use, Schizophrenia and Mis- on inattention (Often loses things needed for tasks
cellaneous Disorders), which can be administered and activities (e.g. toys, school assignments, pencils,
independently. Due to time constraint, our special books or tools)’; Is often forgetful in daily activities’),
interest in behavioural disorders, and expected diag- 2 ADHD items about hyperactivity (Often runs about
nostic prevalence in children, we included the fol- or climbs when and where it is not appropriate’;
lowing modules only: Attention-deficit Hyperactivity Often has trouble playing or enjoying leisure activi-
Disorder (ADHD), Oppositional Defiant Disorder ties quietly’), 1 ADHD on impulsivity (Often inter-
(ODD), Conduct Disorder (CD), Separation Anxiety rupts or intrudes on others (e.g. butts into
Disorder (SAD), Generalized Anxiety, Post-traumatic conversations or games’)), 2 items on oppositional
stress disorder (PTSS), Major Depressive disorder behaviour (Often blames others for his mistakes or
(MDD) and Bipolar Disorder (BD). The internalizing misbehaviour’; Often deliberately annoys people’), 1
disorders, SAD, PTSS, MDD and BD, were only item about conduct behaviour (Has deliberately de-
examined because of differential diagnosis, as the stroyed others’ property’) and 2 items about depres-
focus of our study were externalizing disorders. sive symptoms (Markedly diminished interest or
Schwab-Stone et al. [27] reported good to excellent pleasure in all, or almost all, activities most of the day,
test–retest reliability for ADHD (j = 0.60), moderate nearly every day’; Low energy or fatigue’).
test-retest reliability for ODD (j = 0.68), CD
(j = 0.56) and MDD (j = 0.55) and poor reliability
for SAD (j = 0.45). Agreements between the DISC j Procedure
and clinician ratings were moderate to very good
except for SAD [27]. Interviews with both parents and children took place
In addition to the DISC-P, general information was in school in separate private rooms. When parents
collected including developmental history, psychiatric were not willing to attend the interview at school, they
disorders in the family and information about par- were visited at home. The children were interviewed
ents’ highest educational level. The educational level by an experienced child psychiatrist and the parents
of the parents was used as a proxy measure for socio- were interviewed by a trained psychologist or a
economic status (SES) and was delineated on a 5- trained medical student. When necessary, interviews
point scale: 0 = no education; 1 = elementary school; with Turkish and Moroccan parents were conducted
2 = secondary vocational; 3 = secondary general; and in Turkish, Moroccan Arabic or Berber. The trained
4 = tertiary. bilingual and bicultural interviewers had translated
The Semi-structured Clinical Interview for Chil- the DISC into Turkish, Moroccan Arabic and Berber.
dren and Adolescents (SCICA) was used as a frame- All interviews with children were videotaped. After
work for the clinical interview with children in order complete description of the study to the subjects,
to inform the consensus diagnosis. The SCICA, which written informed consent was obtained from all par-
was conducted by an experienced child psychiatrist, ents. The study protocol was approved by the Medical
provides a flexible semi-structured interview format Ethical Committee of University Medical Centre
for assessing children aged 6–18 regarding 9 broad Utrecht.
areas of functioning: 1. Activities, school and job; 2. Clinical diagnoses were consensually formulated
Friends; 3. Family Relations; 4. Fantasies; 5. Self per- by a committee chaired by a board-certificated psy-
ception and Feelings; 6. Parent or Teacher-Reported chiatrist and consisting of two other child psychia-
Problems; 7. Achievement Tests (optional); 8. For trists who conducted the SCICA interviews and a
ages 6–11: Screen for Fine and Gross Motor Abnor- psychologist who conducted DISC-P-interviews. The
malities (optional) and 9. For ages 12–18: Somatic best-estimate procedure for diagnosis was used:
complaints, Alcohol, Drugs and Trouble with the Law SCICA results were considered in conjunction with
[18]. The good reliability and validity of the SCICA, as the DISC-P-results and school-information. We ar-
established in an American sample [18], have been rived at a best-estimate diagnosis when this was
replicated in a Dutch sample [11]. Moreover, SCICA- indicated by at least two informants i.e. parent–
items have found to be very consistent with DSM-IV child, parent–teacher or teacher–child. When no
diagnostic categories [18]. As our study subjects were agreement was achieved in this clinical conference
aged 6–11 years, we left out the questions about work (N = 15) the child was recommended for the expert
and alcohol, and we adapted the questions about so- panel, consisting of the aforementioned persons
matic complaints, drugs and trouble with the law. accompanied by two other psychiatrists experienced
With regard to area 6 of the SCICA, we adminis- in working with patients from different ethnic ori-
tered a short questionnaire to teachers that was gins. Best-estimate diagnoses were considered as
completed prior to the SCICA interview. This ques- gold standard.
4 Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry, (2006) Vol. xx, No. x
Ó Steinkopff Verlag 2006

j Analysis 0.81, 0.61, 0.68 and 0.69 for the ODD scale among the
Dutch, Moroccan, Turkish and Surinamese group,
To obtain more statistical power we increased the respectively. As most items on the CD scale showed
number of DISC-P-cases by determining subthreshold zero variance, it was not possible to compute Cron-
DISC-P-diagnoses [9]. This was accomplished by bach’s alpha for this scale.
choosing a cut-off of one symptom below the com-
mon criterion level. We calculated parental sensitivity
as the proportion of children with a DISC-P-diagnosis j Overall parental detection of behavioural
out of the total number of children who received that disorders
diagnosis as the best-estimate diagnosis. The sensi-
tivity in our study is the probability that the corre- Parental detection rates of children with any exter-
sponding DISC-P-diagnosis is given to children with a nalizing disorder, ADHD and ODD are presented in
particular disorder (as identified by the best-estimate Table 2. A total of 92 children had a best-estimate
diagnosis). Likewise, we calculated specificity as the diagnosis of a behavioural disorder. Of these, 36
proportion of those without a DISC-P-diagnosis out children (39%) received a DISC-P-diagnosis of a
of the total number of children without that best- behavioural disorder. Consequently, 56 (61%) had a
estimate diagnosis. The specificity is the probability best-estimate diagnosis of an externalizing disorder
that the corresponding DISC-P-diagnosis is not given that was not identified by parents. A total of 24 (15%)
to children without a particular disorder. Sensitivity received a DISC-P-diagnosis of a behavioural disor-
and specificity were supplied with an exact binomial der, but did not have one, while 140 (85%) were
95% confidence interval (95% CI). correctly identified as having no externalizing disor-
We further calculated the risk ratio of a Moroccan, der. This pattern was not essentially different for
Turkish or Surinamese ethnic background for a ADHD and ODD. As only one (Dutch) child received a
parental DISC diagnosis in the presence of a best- DISC-P-diagnosis of CD these data are not presented.
estimate diagnosis (sensitivity), as well as the risk
ratio of no parental DISC diagnosis in the absence of a
best-estimate diagnosis (specificity). Dutch ethnicity j Parental detection according to ethnicity
served as the reference group. Logistic regression was
used to see whether parents’ educational level ex- Table 2 also shows the influence of ethnicity on the
plained differences in sensitivity and specificity. The sensitivity and the specificity to detect behavioural
measures of association in these analyses were odds disorders. Compared with a Dutch background, a
ratios with 95% CI. Moroccan and a Surinamese background reduced
sensitivity to detect an externalizing disorder with a
factor 0.23 (95% CI = 0.10–0.53) and 0.52 (95%
Results CI = 0.28–0.97), respectively. Between Dutch and
Turkish parents no significant difference was ob-
Table 1 shows subjects’ demographic characteristics. served in this measure (Risk Ratio = 0.67; 95%
Across ethnicities no substantial differences were CI = 0.37–1.23).
observed, with the exception of lower parental edu- A Moroccan and Turkish background increased
cational level for Moroccan and Turkish children. the specificity to detect any externalizing disorders
Across ethnicity the DISC’s externalizing modules with 1.27 (95% CI = 1.04–1.56) and 1.32 (95%
demonstrated acceptable to good levels of internal CI = 1.09–1.61), respectively. No significant differ-
consistency with Cronbach’s alpha coefficients of 0.85, ence was observed in this measure between Dutch and
0.83, 0.73 and 0.83 for the ADD scale, 0.78, 0.81, 0.66 Surinamese parents (RR = 1.19; 95% CI = 0.95–1.50).
and 0.69 for the Hyperactivity-Impulsivity Scale and Table 3 presents the odds ratios for any externalizing
Table 1 Demographic characteristics of the study population

Eligible Included Complete cases Age (years) Boys (%) PEL


N (%) N (%) Mean (Min; max) Median (Min; max)

Dutch 98 69 (70) 69 (100) 7.6 (6; 10) 57 3 (0; 4)


Moroccan 109 84 (77) 78 (93) 8.0 (6; 11) 67 2 (0; 4)
Turkish 84 60 (71) 53 (88) 8.0 (6; 10) 51 2 (1; 4)
Surinamese 85 57 (67) 56 (98) 7.9 (6; 10) 57 3 (0; 4)
Total 376 270 (72) 256 (95) 7.6 (6; 11) 59 3 (0; 4)

Note: PEL = Parental Educational Level


Table 2 Parental sensitivity and specificity to detect behavioural disorders (95% confidence intervals)

Best-estimate diagnosis any externalizing disorder Best-estimate diagnosis ADHD Best-estimate diagnosis ODD

Yes No Yes No Yes No

Total group
DISC Yes 36 SE = 39% (29–50) 24 26 SE = 35% (24–47) 16 9 SE = 20% (10–35) 27
DISC No 56 140 SP = 85% (79–90) 48 166 SP = 91% (86–95) 35 185 SP = 87% (82–91)
Total 92 164 74 182 44 212
Dutch
DISC Yes 16 SE = 70% (47–87) 13 14 SE = 64% (41–83) 8 4 SE = 50% (16–84) 14
DISC No 7 33 SP = 72% (57–84) 8 39 SP = 83% (69–92) 4 47 SP = 77% (65–87)
Total 23 46 22 47 8 61
Moroccan
DISC Yes 5 SE = 16% (5–33) 4 5 SE = 20% (7–41) 3 0 SE = 0% (one sided:-24) 4
DISC No 27 42 SP = 91% (79–98) 20 50 SP = 94% (84–99) 14 60 SP = 94% (85–98)
Total 32 46 25 53 14 64
Turkish
DISC Yes 7 SE = 47% (21–73) 2 3 SE = 27% (6–61) 1 3 SE = 38% (9–76) 4
DISC No 8 36 SP = 95% (82–99) 8 41 SP = 98% (87–100) 5 41 SP = 91% (79–98)
Total 15 38 11 42 8 45
Surinamese
DISC Yes 8 SE = 36% (17–59) 5 4 SE = 25% (7–52) 4 2 SE = 14% (2–43) 5
B.W.C. Zwirs et al.

DISC No 14 29 SP = 85% (69–95) 12 36 SP = 90% (76–97) 12 37 SP = 88% (74–96)


Total 22 34 16 40 14 42

Note: DISC = Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children; ADHD = Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; ODD = Oppositional Defiant Disorder; SE = Sensitivity; SP = Specificity
Ethnic differences in parental detection of externalizing disorders
5
6 Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry, (2006) Vol. xx, No. x
Ó Steinkopff Verlag 2006

Table 3 Odds Ratios for any externalizing disorders across ethnicity, unadjusted and adjusted for parental educational level

Sensitivity Specificity

Unadjusted Adjusted Unadjusted Adjusted


OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)

Dutch Reference Reference Reference Reference


Moroccan 0.08 (0.02–0.30)* 0.09 (0.02–0.35)* 4.14 (1.23–13.87)* 3.68 (1.06–12.84)*
Turkish 0.38 (0.10–1.47) 0.40 (0.10–1.55) 7.09 (1.49–33.81)* 6.70 (1.39–32.20)*
Surinamese 0.25 (0.07–0.87)* 0.29 (0.08–1.02)** 2.29 (0.73–7.19) 2.14 (0.68–6.79)

Note: *P < 0.05; **P = 0.05

Table 4 Odds Ratios for ADHD across ethnicity, unadjusted and adjusted for parental educational level

Sensitivity Specificity

Unadjusted Adjusted Unadjusted Adjusted


OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)

Dutch Reference Reference Reference Reference


Moroccan 0.14 (0.04–0.53)* 0.14 (0.03–0.55)* 3.42 (0.85–13.75)** 3.12 (0.73–13.28)
Turkish 0.21 (0.04–1.05)** 0.20 (0.04–1.01)** 8.41 (1.01–70.39)* 8.09 (0.96–68.19)**
Surinamese 0.19 (0.05–0.79)* 0.23 (0.05–0.98)* 1.85 (0.51–6.66) 1.76 (0.49–6.37)

Note: *P < 0.05; **P < 0.09

disorder, unadjusted and adjusted for parental edu- Dutch parents detected considerably fewer external-
cational level. izing disorders than Dutch parents.
In addition, we examined the influence of ethnicity Even though we used a more flexible cut-off for the
on the sensitivity and the specificity to detect ADHD. DISC-P-diagnoses, the DISC did not perform well as a
Compared to Dutch parents, Moroccan and Suri- diagnostic instrument. Parents only detected 39% of
namese parents were 0.31 (95% CI = 0.14–0.73) and the children with an externalizing disorder. Hence,
0.39 (95% CI = 0.16–0.97) times less likely to identify based on parental reports 61% of children with a
cases of ADHD, respectively. Sensitivity to detect behavioural disorder would not be detected. Con-
ADHD tended to be 0.43 (95%CI = 0.16–1.18) times versely, 15% of children were incorrectly identified as
lower among Turkish parents. having an externalizing disorder, while 85% was
The specificity to detect ADHD was 1.14 (95% appropriately recognized as not having one. By way of
CI = 0.98–1.32) times higher among Moroccan par- comparison, if the more stringent cut-off had been
ents and tended to be 1.18 (95% CI = 1.03–1.35) times used, the sensitivity and the specificity would have
higher Turkish parents. No significant difference was been 25% and 91%, respectively (data not shown).
observed in this measure between Dutch and Suri- In contrast, previous studies [2, 15] reported
namese parents (RR = 1.08; 95% CI = 0.92–1.28). higher prevalence of disruptive behaviour disorders
Table 4 presents the odds ratios for ADHD, unad- based on structured interviews as compared to clini-
justed and adjusted for parental educational level. cal diagnoses. However, these studies evaluated the
It can also be seen from Table 2 that parental Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children and
sensitivity to detect ODD was lower in non-Dutch Adolescents (DICA-R) instead of the DISC-IV exam-
parents. Parental specificity to detect ODD was higher ined in the present study [2], or studied a clinical
among non-Dutch parents as compared to Dutch sample [15] instead of the community sample we
parents. However, too few cases of ODD were iden- studied. In addition, we used the best-estimate pro-
tified for OR’s to be reliable. cedure as comparison, which has found to yield a
higher rate of diagnoses than interviews alone [13].
For instance, in a recent study [6] in which the parent
Discussion SDQ was compared to the Development and Well
Being Assessment (a best-estimate procedure); the
This study is the first to evaluate the effect of ethnicity sensitivity of parental recognition was 53% for CD
on parental sensitivity and specificity to detect and 35% for ADHD. Whereas the sensitivity for
behavioural problems by comparing parental reports ADHD was similar in the present study (i.e. 35%), the
to best-estimate diagnoses. It appeared that non- sensitivity for ODD and CD was lower (i.e. 20% and
B.W.C. Zwirs et al. 7
Ethnic differences in parental detection of externalizing disorders

0%, respectively). Possibly, it is less threatening for sponses. For instance, Arab parents in the United
parents to disclose such sensitive information as Arab Emirates have found to be reluctant to
conduct problems in a questionnaire than in a face- acknowledge that a family member had a mental ill-
to-face interview. ness [4]. In addition, since the level of monitoring has
However, as we assessed subthreshold DISC-P- found to be relatively low among Moroccan parents,
diagnoses, the observed low sensitivity and high spec- when compared to Dutch parents [8], the lower
ificity among parents, remains remarkable. Another detection rate of non-Dutch parents may also be re-
explanation for this finding may be the overall low SES lated to the fact that these parents are less well in-
of our sample, since adults belonging to lower social formed about their children’s functioning outside the
classes have found to answer less honestly and more home. Finally, Moroccan, Turkish and Surinamese
defensively than adults of higher social classes [10]. immigrant children may show relatively less problem
The overall low parental sensitivity but high behaviour at home than outside of the home as
specificity observed in the current study can also be compared to native Dutch children, which may
explained by the presence of Moroccan, Turkish and associated with an authoritarian socialization inside
Surinamese immigrant parents, as these measures of the home, but with a lack of supervision outside of
varied significantly according to ethnicity. A Moroc- the home as observed in Moroccan families [21].
can or Surinamese background reduced the sensitivity
to detect any externalizing disorder and ADHD in
particular, substantially. Among Turkish parents, the j Limitations
sensitivity to detect ADHD tended to be lower. Con-
versely, specificity to detect any behavioural disorder Our results have to be evaluated with consideration of
was markedly higher among Moroccan and Turkish some limitations. First, the DISC-P had not been
parents. The specificity to detect ADHD was signifi- translated and validated for a Turkish, Moroccan or
cantly higher for Moroccan parents and tended to be Surinamese sample. As a result, semantic or concep-
higher for Turkish parents. tual differences in the DISC may be responsible for
Studies on the influence of ethnicity on parental the relatively low sensitivity and high specificity
detection of behavioural disorders are rare but con- among non-Dutch parents. However, as 70% of the
sistent in suggesting that non-Western parents report Moroccans in the Netherlands are of Berber origin
less externalizing disorders when compared to Wes- and as Berber is an oral language of which many
tern parents. For instance, Stevens et al. [30] found that dialects do exist, it would be hardly possible to de-
Moroccan immigrant parents rated their children velop a standard translation’ of the DISC in Berber.
lower on attention problems and delinquent behaviour Therefore, we think that our translations, which re-
when compared to native Dutch parents. Likewise, sulted from extensive discussions among the carefully
Turkish children received lower scores from their trained bicultural or bilingual interviewers and con-
parents on delinquent behaviour and aggressive sultation of different experts, are relatively valid
behaviour than American children [5]. Nevertheless, translations. Moreover, as a lower detection rate was
these studies did not include a standard of validity and also observed among Surinamese parents (who were
therefore, it remains unclear as to whether the ob- interviewed in Dutch) and among Dutch speaking
served variations reflect actual differences in children’s Turkish and Moroccan parents, it is unlikely that our
behaviour or ethnic differences in parental detection. results are fully explained by semantic or conceptual
An explanation for the observed lower detection differences. Finally, in practice non-Dutch parents in
rate of children’s externalizing disorders among the Netherlands are assessed with instruments that
Moroccan, Turkish and Surinamese minority parents, are developed and standardized in Western samples
as evident from a relatively low sensitivity and high and therefore our results reflect the reality. Still, fu-
specificity, may be that these parents apply other ture research is needed to validate instruments in
definitions of normal and abnormal behaviour. In- non-Western samples like Moroccans, Turkish and
deed, it is generally believed that parental attitudes Surinamese, and to clarify to what extent a lower
towards acceptable behaviour are influenced by eth- detection rate among these parents is explained by the
nicity. For instance, Gujarati parents have been found instruments that are used.
to tolerate fewer behaviour problems than English As the members of the diagnostic committee and
parents [7]. Likewise Bussing et al. [3] reported that the larger expert panel, who were mainly of Dutch
Caucasian parents were more worried about ADHD- origin, were not blinded to the ethnicity of the child,
related school problems than African-American par- an ethnic bias may have influenced the diagnoses. For
ents. Moreover, non-Western parents like Moroccan, instance, English teachers have been found to score
Turkish and Surinamese parents in the current study Asian immigrant children higher on hyperactivity
may be more likely to provide socially desirable re- symptoms than English native children, whereas the
8 Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry, (2006) Vol. xx, No. x
Ó Steinkopff Verlag 2006

ratings on more objective measures of hyperactivity detection rate of externalizing disorders was low
were similar for both groups [29]. Likewise, the cli- among all parents, but in particular among non-Dutch
nicians in the present study may have been more parents. If the more stringent cut-off had been used,
likely to identify non-Dutch children as disordered, the detection rate of parents would have been even
when compared to Dutch children. However, as the lower. Thus, the original criterion level for diagnosing
professionals were experienced in working with chil- DISC-P-diagnoses would have been too stringent.
dren of non-Dutch origin, we expect them to be cul- However, as the detection rate was also relatively low
turally sensitive. Moreover, since the prevalence of with the more flexible cut-off, it may be questioned
psychiatric disorders based on the best-estimate whether the DISC is suitable at all for assessment
diagnosis was not higher for non-Dutch children as among parents of low SES and/or non-Dutch origin.
compared to Dutch children [34], it is unlikely that a Additional research is needed to examine whether
strong perceptual bias distorted the results. Indeed, if semi-structured, open interviews or rating scales are
the decision of caseness would have been culturally more appropriate.
biased, than the true’ prevalence of externalizing The finding that non-Western parents, like the
disorders would be lower for non-Dutch children than Moroccans, Turks and Surinamese in the present
for Dutch children, which seems implausible. study, are relatively less likely to detect best estimate
In addition, the best-estimate diagnosis chosen as diagnoses of externalizing disorders, may be an
gold standard’ for this study may not have perfect explanation for the observed lower treatment rates
reliability or validity itself. For instance, the teacher among non-Western children with behavioural dis-
questionnaire included in the best-estimate diagnosis orders [24, 35]. Therefore, researchers and clinicians
consisted of 10 DSM-IV items, which has not been working with an ethnically diverse population should
validated as such. However, as we used an ‘‘and-rule’’ be cautious in deriving conclusions solely based on
to define caseness, a teacher report could only result parental information. When assessing children of
in a case, when this was also indicated by the child non-Western origin, it may be of particular impor-
and/or the parent. Moreover, as this gold standard’ tance to obtain information from other resources than
draws on three important sources (parent, child and the parents, such as the teachers or observations.
teacher), includes the perspectives of different pro- Nevertheless, as it remains unclear to what extent
fessionals, and has been evaluated by an expert panel, the lower detection rate among non-Western parents is
we think it is a reasonable standard of validity. associated with ethnic differences in social desirability,
Finally, as we have sampled from inner-city in parental attitudes towards acceptable behaviour, in
neighbourhoods with a low SES, our findings cannot monitoring children’s behaviour or in the discrepancy
be generalized to other neighbourhoods with middle between children’s indoors and outdoors behaviour,
and higher SES levels without additional study. In a future research is needed to elucidate the mechanisms
middle or high SES sample, sensitivity may be higher underlying the lower detection rate among non-Wes-
and specificity lower [10]. Nevertheless, as most mi- tern parents. Qualitative studies in particular may be
grants in the Netherlands live in urban areas and are required to clarify the meaning of externalizing items
from the lowest SES, generalizing to middle and for non-Western parents and to find out whether they
higher SES neighbourhoods seems of less relevance. hold different concepts of behavioural disorders.

j Implications and future research j Acknowledgments This study was financially supported by The
Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (ZON-MW), grant
number 99–9.1–64, by the Foundation for Children’s Welfare
The current study shows that, although a more flexible Stamps Netherlands and by the Province of Utrecht, The Nether-
cut-off for diagnosing DISC-P-diagnoses was used, the lands.

References
1. Biederman J, Faraone S, Milberger S, 2. Boyle MH, Offord DR, Racine Y, San- 3. Bussing R, Gary FA, Mills TL, Garvan
Guite J, Mick E, Chen L, Mennin D, ford M, Szatmari P, Fleming JE, Price- CW (2003) Parental explanatory mod-
Marrs A, Ouellette C, Moore P, Spencer Munn N (1993) Evaluation of the els of ADHD: gender and cultural
T, Norman D, Wilens T, Kraus I, Perrin Diagnostic Interview for Children and variations. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr
J (1996) A prospective 4-year follow-up Adolescents for use in general popula- Epidemiol 38:563–575
study of attention-deficit hyperactivity tion samples. J Abnorm Child Psychol 4. Eapen V, Ghubash R (2004) Help-
and related disorders. Arch Gen Psy- 21:663–681 seeking for mental health problems of
chiatry 53:437–446 children: preferences and attitudes in
the United Arab Emirates. Psychol Rep
94:663–667
B.W.C. Zwirs et al. 9
Ethnic differences in parental detection of externalizing disorders

5. Erol N, Arslan LB, Akçakin M (1995) 16. Mann EM, Ikeda Y, Mueller CW, Ta- 27. Schwab-Stone ME, Shaffer D, Dulcan
The adaptation and standardization of kahashi A (1992) Cross-cultural differ- MK, Jensen PS, Fisher P, Bird HR,
the Child Behavior Checklist among 6– ences in rating hyperactive-disruptive Goodman SH, Lahey BB, Lichtman JH,
18 year-old Turkish children. In: Ser- behaviors in children. Am J Psychiatry Canino G, Rubio-Stipec M, Rae DS
geant E (eds) Eunethydis: European 149:1539–1542 (1996) Criterion validity of the NIMH
Approaches to Hyperkinetic Disorder. 17. Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Konig PH, Diagnostic Interview Schedule for
Fotorotar, Zurich, pp 97–113 Giampino TL (1989) Hyperactive boys Children Version 2.3 (DISC-2.3). J Am
6. Goodman R (1997) The strengths and almost grown up. IV. Criminality and Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 35:878–
difficulties questionnaire: a research its relationship to psychiatric status. 888
note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Arch Gen Psychiatry 46:1073–1079 28. Shaffer D, Fisher P, Lucas CP, Duldan
38:581–586 18. McConaughy SH, Achenbach TM MK, Schwab-Stone ME (2000) NIMH
7. Hackett L, Hackett R (1993) Parental (2001) Manual for the semistructured Diagnostic Interview Schedule for
ideas of normal and deviant child clinical interview for children and Children Version IV (NIMH DISC-IV):
behaviour. A comparison of two ethnic adolescents. University of Vermont, Description, differences from previous
groups. Br J Psychiatry 162:353–357 Burlington. versions, and reliability of some com-
8. Junger M, Terlouw GJ, van der Heijden 19. MTA Cooperative Group (1999) A 14- mon diagnoses. J Am Acad Child
PGM (1995) Crime, accidents and so- month randomized clinical trial of Adolesc Psychiatry 39:28–38
cial control. Crim Behav Mental Health treatment strategies for attention-defi- 29. Sonuga-Barke EJS, Minocha K (1993)
5:386–410 cit/hyperactivity disorder. The MTA Inter-ethnic bias in teachers’ ratings of
9. Kadesjo B, Gillberg C (2001) The Cooperative Group. Multimodal Treat- childhood hyperactivity. Br J Dev Psy-
comorbidity of ADHD in the general ment Study of Children with ADHD. chol 11:187–200
population of Swedish school-age chil- Arch Gen Psychiatry 56:1073–1086 30. Stevens GW, Pels T, Bengi-Arslan L,
dren. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 20. Muris P, Meesters C, Van Den BF Verhulst FC, Vollebergh WA, Crijnen
42:487–492 (2003) The Strengths and Difficulties AA (2003) Parent, teacher and self-re-
10. Kalliopuska M (1992) Social desirabil- Questionnaire (SDQ)–further evidence ported problem behavior in The Neth-
ity related to social class among adults. for its reliability and validity in a erlands: comparing Moroccan
Psychol Rep 70:808–810 community sample of Dutch children immigrant with Dutch and with Turk-
11. Kasius MC (1997) Interviewing chil- and adolescents. Eur Child Adolesc ish immigrant children and adoles-
dren. Development of the Dutch ver- Psychiatry 12:1–8 cents. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr
sion of the Semi-structured Clinical 21. Pels T (2003) Respect van twee kanten; Epidemiol 38:576–585
Interview for Children and Adolescents Een studie over last van Marokkaanse 31. Van Widenfelt BM, Goedhart AW,
(SCICA) and testing of the psycho- jongeren (The question of respect; A Treffers PD, Goodman R (2003) Dutch
metric properties. Unpublished doc- study on misconduct of Moroccan version of the Strengths and Difficulties
toral dissertation, Erasmus University, boys) Koninklijke van Gorcum, Assen Questionnaire (SDQ). Eur Child Ado-
The Netherlands, Rotterdam 22. Puig M, Lambert MC, Rowan GT, lesc Psychiatry 12:281–289
12. Knol FA (1998) Van hoog naar laag; Winfrey T, Lyubansky M, Hannah SD, 32. Weisz JR, Chaiyasit W, Weiss B, East-
van laag naar hoog (From high to low; Hill MF (1999) Behavioral and emo- man KL, Jackson EW (1995) A multi-
from low to high). Sociaal en Cultureel tional problems among Jamaican and method study of problem behavior
Planbureau, Den Haag African American Children, Ages 6 to among Thai and American children in
13. Kosten TA, Rounsaville BJ (1992) Sen- 11: Teacher Reports versus Direct school: teacher reports versus direct
sitivity of psychiatric diagnosis based Observations. J Emotion Behav Disor- observations. Child Dev 66:402–415
on the best estimate procedure. Am J ders 7:240–251 33. Weisz JR, McCarty CA (1999) Can we
Psychiatry 149:1225–1227 23. Rothenberger A, Woerner W (2004) trust parent reports in research on
14. Lambert MC, Weisz JR, Knight F, Strengths and Difficulties Question- cultural and ethnic differences in child
Desrosiers MF, Overly K, Thesiger C naire (SDQ)–evaluations and applica- psychopathology? Using the bicultural
(1992) Jamaican and American adult tions. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry family design to test parental culture
perspectives on child psychopathology: 13(Suppl 2):II1–II2 effects. J Abnorm Psychol 108:598–605
further exploration of the threshold 24. Safer DJ, Malever M (2000) Stimulant 34. Zwirs BWC, Burger H, Schulpen TWJ,
model. J Consult Clin Psychol 60:146– treatment in Maryland public schools. Buitelaar JK (xxxx) Prevalence of psy-
149 Pediatrics 106:533–539 chiatric disorders among children of
15. Lewczyk CM, Garland AF, Hurlburt 25. Satterfield JH, Schell A (1997) A pro- different ethnic origin. Manuscript
MS, Gearity J, Hough RL (2003) Com- spective study of hyperactive boys with submitted for publication.
paring DISC-IV and clinician diagnoses conduct problems and normal boys: 35. Zwirs BWC, Burger H, Schulpen TWJ,
among youths receiving public mental adolescent and adult criminality. J Am Buitelaar JK (2006) Different treatment
health services. J Am Acad Child Ado- Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry thresholds in non-Western children
lesc Psychiatry 42:349–356 36:1726–1735 with behavioral problems. J Am Acad
26. Sayal K, Taylor E, Beecham J, Byrne P Child Adolesc Psychiatry 45(4):476–483
(2002) Pathways to care in children at
risk of attention-deficit hyperactivity
disorder. Br J Psychiatry 181:43–48

You might also like