Learning and Memory and Sleep 2011

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

Author's personal copy

L e a r n i n g , M e m o r y,
and S le ep i n Humans
Jessica D. Payne, PhD

KEYWORDS
! Slow wave sleep ! Memory ! Hippocampus
! Rapid eye movement sleep

About one-third of a person’s life is spent sleeping, functional consequences but also makes it difficult
yet in spite of the advances in understanding the to know which distinction is critical for memory
processes that generate and maintain sleep, the processing: NREM versus REM sleep or early
functional significance of sleep remains a mystery. versus late sleep.
Numerous theories regarding the role of sleep Neurotransmitters, particularly the monoamines
have been proposed, including homeostatic resto- serotonin (5-HT) and norepinephrine (NE), and
ration, thermoregulation, tissue repair, and immune acetylcholine (ACh), play a critical role in switching
control. A theory that has received enormous the brain from one sleep stage to another. REM
support in the last decade concerns sleep’s role in sleep occurs when activity in the aminergic system
processing and storing memories. This article has decreased enough to allow the reticular
reviews some of the evidence suggesting that sleep system to escape its inhibitory influence.1 The
is critical for long-lasting memories. This body of release from aminergic inhibition stimulates
evidence has grown impressively large and covers cholinergic reticular neurons in the brainstem and
a range of studies at the molecular, cellular, physio- switches the sleeping brain into the highly active
logic, and behavioral levels of analysis. REM state, in which ACh levels are as high as
in the waking state. Overall, REM sleep is also
associated with higher levels of cortisol than
DEFINING SLEEP AND MEMORY NREM sleep. 5-HT and NE, on the other hand,
Stages of Sleep are virtually absent during REM sleep. SWS,
conversely, is associated with an absence of
There are 2 main types of sleep. The first, rapid eye
ACh and low levels of cortisol but detectable levels
movement (REM) sleep, occurs in roughly 90-
of 5-HT and NE.2,3
minute cycles throughout the night and alternates
with 4 additional stages (stages 1–4) known collec-
tively as non-REM (NREM) sleep, which comprises
Stages of Memory Formation, and
the second type of sleep.1 Slow wave sleep (SWS)
Different Memory Types
is the deepest of the NREM sleep phases and is
characterized by high-amplitude, low-frequency Memory consolidation is the process by which
oscillations seen in the electroencephalogram newly acquired information, initially fragile, is inte-
(EEG). REM sleep, on the other hand, is a lighter grated and stabilized into long-term memory.4
stage of sleep characterized by rapid eye move- Evidence overwhelmingly suggests that sleep
ments (REMs), decreased muscle tone, and low- plays a role in the consolidation of a range of
amplitude fast EEG oscillations. More than 80% memory tasks, with the different stages of sleep
of SWS is concentrated in the first half of the typical selectively benefiting the consolidation of different
8-hour night, whereas the second half of the night types of memory.2,5–10
contains roughly twice as much REM sleep than Like sleep, memory is also divided into several
the first half (Fig. 1). key stages and types.11 First, there are various
This domination of early sleep by SWS and of types of memories that can be recalled explicitly,
sleep.theclinics.com

late sleep by REM sleep not only has important including episodic memories, or memories of the

Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, Haggar Hall, Room 122-B, Notre Dame, Indiana 46556, USA
E-mail address: Jessica.payne.70@nd.edu

Sleep Med Clin 6 (2011) 15–30


doi:10.1016/j.jsmc.2010.12.005
1556-407X/11/$ – see front matter ! 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Author's personal copy
16 Payne

Fig. 1. A sleep histogram showing the typical distribution of SWS and REM sleep.

events in our lives. Retrieving an episodic memory differently during sleep. Although somewhat
from one’s past requires access to defining oversimplified,15 there is a general consensus
contextual features of the event, such as specific that NREM sleep, especially SWS, is important
details about the place and time of its occurrence. for the consolidation of explicit episodic and
Because of this emphasis on spatial context, spatial memories, both of which rely on the hippo-
episodic memories and spatial memories are campus for their consolidation,16 whereas REM
closely connected. Second, there are semantic sleep selectively benefits procedural and
memories, which are concerned with the knowl- emotional memories.17,18
edge one acquires during events but is itself sepa- In addition to these distinct types, memory
rated from the specific event in question. Thus, our consolidation also consists of different stages.
knowledge about the meaning of words and facts Although our experience of memory occurs at
about the world, even though acquired in the the moment of retrieval when information about
context of some specific experience, appears to the initial experience returns to conscious aware-
be stored in a context-independent format (eg, ness, successful retrieval is contingent on the
not bound to the originating context). Third, there successful completion of at least 2 earlier stages
are “how to” memories for the various skills, of memory formation. First, the experience or
procedures, and habits that we acquire through information must be properly encoded. Encoding
experience. Because these memories are not so involves transforming new information into a repre-
easily made explicit and are usually only evident sentation capable of being stored in memory, just
in behavior, they are referred to as procedural or as the key presses that are made on a keyboard
implicit memories. Finally, there are emotional must be converted to a format that can be stored
memories for the positive and negative experi- in a computer. Second, the information must be
ences in our lives. This class of memories is espe- consolidated or durably stored in a manner that
cially concerned with learning about fearful and can withstand the passage of time. Only if these
negative stimuli, although evidence suggests it processes occur successfully will it be possible
plays a role in learning about pleasant information for information from the initial event to be retrieved
as well.12 later in time. Although this article mostly focuses
Evidence suggests that each of these memory on memory consolidation, it also briefly reviews
types is subserved by distinct neural systems.11–13 new evidence that sleep is essential for effective
Whereas episodic and spatial memories are gov- memory encoding.
erned by the hippocampus and surrounding
medial temporal areas, procedural or implicit SLEEP BENEFITS IMPLICIT AND PROCEDURAL
memories are thought to be independent of the MEMORIES
hippocampus and anatomically related regions,
relying instead on various neocortical and subcor- Sleep seems to benefit the consolidation of both
tical structures. The emotional memory system is implicit and explicit forms of memory. Converging
centered in the amygdala, a limbic structure that data suggest that most procedural skills and abil-
is richly connected to the hippocampus.14 Impor- ities (eg, performing a surgery, riding a bike) are
tantly, evidence suggests that information depen- acquired slowly and are not attained solely during
dent on each of these systems is processed the learning episode. While some learning certainly
Author's personal copy
Sleep and Memory 17

develops quickly, performance on various proce- a 50% greater improvement was observed (see
dural tasks improves further and without additional Fig. 3B). Critically, another group of subjects was
practice simply through the passage of time, sleep deprived on the first night after training.
particularly if these periods contain sleep. These These subjects were allowed 2 full nights of
slow off-line improvements occur as newly recovery sleep before being retested 3 days later.
acquired information, initially fragile, is integrated The subjects failed to show any residual learning,
and stabilized into long-term memory. suggesting that performance enhancements
Early work investigating the effect of sleep on depend on a normal first night of sleep (see
implicit learning used a visual texture discrimination Fig. 3B). Time alone is clearly not enough to
task (VDT) originally developed by Karni and Sagi.19 produce long-term benefits from VDT training. It
The task requires participants to determine the seems that sleep is also required.22
orientation (vertical or horizontal) of an array of diag- Initially, improvement on this task seemed to
onal bars that is embedded in one visual quadrant depend solely on REM sleep, because subjects
against a background of exclusively horizontal who underwent selective deprivation of REM sleep
bars (Fig. 2). At the center of the screen is the fixa- showed no improvement on the task.20 Later
tion target, which is either the letter T or the letter studies, however, showed that optimal perfor-
L. This target screen is succeeded first by a blank mance on this task requires both SWS and REM
screen for a variable interstimulus interval (ISI) and sleep.23
then by a mask (a screen covered with randomly When subjects were trained and their subse-
oriented V’s with a superimposed T and L in the quent sleep monitored in the sleep laboratory,
center). Subjects must determine the orientation of the amount of improvement was proportional to
the array, and the performance is estimated by the the amount of SWS during the first quarter of the
ISI corresponding to 80% correct responses.19,20 night (Fig. 4A), as well as to the amount of REM
Amnesic patients with damage to the hippo- sleep in the last quarter (see Fig. 4B). Indeed, the
campal complex, who cannot acquire knowledge product of these 2 sleep parameters explained
explicitly, show normal performance improve- more than 80% of the intersubject variance (see
ments on the VDT.21 In neurologically normal Fig. 4D). No significant correlations were found
subjects, improvement on the VDT develops for sleep stage during other parts of the night
slowly after training,19,22 with no improvement (see Fig. 4C) or for the amount of stage 2 sleep
when retesting occurs on the same day as training at any time during the night.
(Fig. 3A). Instead, improvement is only observed Gais and colleagues24 came to a similar conclu-
after a night of sleep (see Fig. 3A). This observa- sion by examining improvement after 3 hours of
tion was true even for a group of subjects who sleep either early or late in the night. They found
were retested only 9 hours after training. Impor- that 3 hours of early night sleep, which was rich
tantly, there was not even a trend to greater in SWS, produced an 8-millisecond improvement,
improvement when the training-retest interval but after a full night of sleep, which added REM-
was increased from 9.0 to 22.5 hours, suggesting rich sleep late in the night, a 26-millisecond
that additional wake time after the night of sleep improvement was observed, 3 times that seen
provided no additional benefit. Whereas further with early sleep alone. However, 3 hours of REM-
wake time provided no benefit, additional nights rich, late night sleep actually produced deteriora-
of sleep did produce incremental improvement. tion in performance.24 These results are further
When subjects were retested 2 to 7 days after corroborated by daytime nap studies. Afternoon
training rather than after a single night of sleep naps as short as 60 to 90 minutes also lead to

Fig. 2. (A–C) Sample screens from the VDT.


Author's personal copy
18 Payne

A B 30
Daytime
20 Overnight 25

Improvement (ms)

Improvement (ms)
* 20
*
*
10 15

10

5
0
6 12 18 24
0
0 1 2 3 4 7
Test interval (h) Days

Fig. 3. Sleep-dependent improvement on the VDT. All subjects were trained and then retested only once. Each
point in (A) and each green bar in (B) represents a separate group of subjects. Error bars in (A) and (B) are
SEM. (A) There was no improvement when retesting occurred on the same day as training (open circles), but there
was improvement after a night of sleep (closed circles). Asterisks indicate significant effects. (B) (Green bars)
Improvement in performance of subjects when retested 2 to 7 days of training. (Red bar) No improvement in
performance of subjects who were sleep deprived on the first night of training. ms, milliseconds. (From Stickgold
R, James L, Hobson JA. Visual discrimination learning requires post-training sleep. Nat Neurosci 2000;2
(12):1237–8; with permission; and Stickgold R, Whidbee D, Schirmer B, et al. Visual discrimination task improve-
ment: a multi-step process occurring during sleep. J Cognit Neurosci 2000;12:246–54; with permission.)

performance benefits on the VDT, especially when type of sensory memory that may or may not share
they contain REM and NREM sleep.25,26 its sleep dependency with other procedural tasks.
At this point, sleep’s role in visual discrimination This specificity raises the question of whether the
learning (as measured by VDT performance) is sleep effects observed with the VDT generalize
clear. But the VDT represents a very specific to other forms of procedural memory. Studies of

A B
40
r = 0.70 40 r = 0.76
P = .012 P = .004
Improvement (ms)

30
Improvement (ms)

30

20
20

10
10

0
0
5 15 20 15 20
5 10
1st quartile SWS (% of night)
4th quartile REM (% of night)

C 0.8 D
40
Pearson r value

P = .01 r = 0.89
0.6 P < .0001
Improvement (ms)

P = .05
30
0.4

0.2 20

0.0
10

1 2 3 4
Quartile 0
50 100 150 200

SWS REM SWS 1 x REM 4 (% x %)

Fig. 4. (A–D) REM sleep and SWS dependence of VDT learning. ms, milliseconds; REM4, 4th quartile of REM sleep;
SWS1, 1st quartile of SWS. (From Stickgold R, Whidbee D, Schirmer B, et al. Visual discrimination task improve-
ment: a multi-step process occurring during sleep. J Cognit Neurosci 2000;12:246–54; with permission.)
Author's personal copy
Sleep and Memory 19

sleep-dependent motor skill learning strongly improvement on this task correlated with the
suggest that they do. amount of stage 2 NREM sleep during the night,
As an example, Walker and colleagues27 have especially during the last quarter of the night.
demonstrated sleep-dependent improvements on These findings are in agreement with those of
a finger tapping task. The task requires subjects Fogel and colleagues,28 Smith and MacNeill,29
to repeatedly type the numeric sequence 4-1-3- and Tweed and colleagues,30 who have also
2-4 as quickly and accurately as possible with the shown that stage 2 sleep and possibly the sleep
non-dominant hand. Training consisted of twelve spindles that reach peak density during late night
30-second trials separated by 30-second rest stage 2 sleep are critical for simple motor memory
periods. All subjects show considerable improve- consolidation. This role of sleep spindles seems
ment during the 12 trials of the training session (a plausible because they have been proposed to
fast learning component), but 12 hours later, the trigger intracellular mechanisms that are required
subjects performed differently depending on for synaptic plasticity.31
whether the 12-hour interval was spent sleeping This motor sequence task has been examined to
or awake. When trained in the morning and determine where precisely in the motor program
retested 12 hours later, only an additional nonsig- this sleep-dependent improvement occurs.32 In
nificant 4% improvement was seen in perfor- the sequence mentioned earlier (4-1-3-2-4), there
mance, but when tested again the next morning, are 4 unique key-press transitions: 4 to 1, 1 to 3,
a large and robust (14%) improvement was seen 3 to 2, and 2 to 4. When the speed between tran-
(Fig. 5A). The failure to improve during the daytime sitions was analyzed for individual subjects before
could not be caused by interference from related sleep, “sticking points” emerged. Whereas some
motor activity because subjects who were required transitions were easy (ie, fast), others were prob-
to wear mittens and refrain from fine motor activi- lematic (ie, slow), as if the sequence was being
ties during this time showed a similar pattern of parsed or chunked into smaller bits during
wake/sleep improvement (see Fig. 5B). presleep learning.21 After a night of sleep, these
In contrast, when subjects were trained in the problematic points were preferentially improved,
evening, improvement was observed the following whereas transitions that had already been
morning (after sleep) but not across an additional mastered before sleep did not change. Subjects
12 hours of wake (see Fig. 5C). Thus, improved who were trained and retested after a daytime
performance resulted specifically from a night of wake interval showed no such improvements.
sleep as opposed to the simple passage of time. These findings suggest that the sleep-
Curiously, unlike the findings for the VDT, overnight dependent consolidation process involves the

Fig. 5. (A–C) Sleep-dependent motor learning. Improvement in speed was seen in all 3 groups over a night of
sleep but not over 12 hours of daytime wake. (From Walker M, Brakefield T, Morgan A, et al. Practice with sleep
makes perfect: sleep dependent motor skill learning. Neuron 2002;35:205–11; with permission.)
Author's personal copy
20 Payne

unification of smaller motor memory units into improvement on the task (compared with
a single motor memory representation, thereby randomly generated sequences) reflects implicitly
improving problem points in the sequence. Impor- acquired knowledge of this grammar.37
tantly, this finding suggests that the role of sleep is Neuroimaging was performed on 3 groups of
subtle and complex and that sleep does more subjects. One group was scanned while they
than simply strengthen memories; sleep may were awake, both at rest and during performance
encourage the restructuring and reorganization of of the task, providing information about which
memories, an important and often-overlooked brain regions are typically activated by the task.
aspect of memory consolidation. The author re- A second group of subjects was trained on the
turns to this idea later in the article. task during the afternoon and then scanned the
Moving to another type of motor memory, motor night after training, both while awake and during
adaptation, Maquet and colleagues33 showed that various sleep stages. Thus, group 2 was included
sleep benefits performance on a motor pursuit to determine if similar brain regions were reacti-
task. Participants were trained to use a joystick vated during sleep. A postsleep session was also
to keep the cursor on a target when the target conducted to verify that learning had indeed
trajectory was predictable only on the horizontal occurred. Finally, a third group, never trained on
axis, which meant that optimal performance could the task, was scanned while sleeping to ensure
only be achieved by developing an implicit model that the pattern of activation present in natural
of the motion characteristics of the learned trajec- sleep was different from the pattern of activation
tory. Half of the subjects were sleep deprived on present after training.
the first posttraining night, whereas the other half Results showed that during REM sleep, as
were allowed to sleep normally. Three days later, compared with resting wakefulness, several brain
after 2 full nights of recovery sleep, performance areas used during task performance were more
was superior in the sleep group compared with active in trained than in nontrained subjects. These
the sleep-deprived group, and functional magnetic areas included occipital, parietal, anterior cingu-
resonance imaging (fMRI) revealed that that the late, motor, and premotor cortices and the cere-
superior temporal sulcus (STS) was differentially bellum, all activations that are consistent with the
more active for the learned trajectory in subjects component processes involved in the visual and
who slept than in the sleep-deprived subjects. motor functioning involved in this task. Behavioral
Moreover, increased functional connectivity was data confirmed that trained subjects improved
observed between the STS and the cerebellum significantly more across the night.
and between the supplementary and frontal eye Peigneux and colleagues,38 using the same task,
fields, suggestive of sleep-related plastic changes showed that the level of acquisition of probabilistic
during motor skill consolidation in areas involved in rules attained before sleep was correlated with the
smooth pursuit and eye movements. increase in activation of task-related cortical areas
Admittedly, visual discrimination, finger tapping, during posttraining REM sleep. This observation
and motor adaptation are all relatively basic low- suggests that cerebral reactivation is modulated
level procedural tasks that may become auto- by the strength of the memory traces developed
mated fairly quickly. What about the more during the learning episode, and as such, these
complex implicit and procedural tasks? Work on data provide the first experimental evidence linking
animals clearly demonstrates that complex tasks behavioral performance to reactivation during
(eg, instrumental conditioning, avoidance, or REM sleep.38 Together with animal studies,39,40
maze learning) benefit from sleep, with rats these findings suggest that it is not simply experi-
showing increases in REM sleep that continue until encing the task, but the process of memory
the tasks are mastered.34–36 consolidation associated with successful learning
REM sleep has been implicated in complex of the task that modifies sleep physiology.
procedural learning in humans as well. In a positron These results support the idea that implicit/
emission tomographic study of visuomotor skill procedural memory traces in humans can be reac-
memory using the serial reaction time task,37 6 tivated during REM sleep, and that this reactiva-
spatially permanent position markers were shown tion is linked to improved consolidation. Indeed,
on a computer screen and subjects watched for looking a bit more closely at the literature, human
stimuli to appear below these markers. When REM sleep has also been linked to memory for
a stimulus appeared in a particular position, complex logic games, to foreign language acquisi-
subjects reacted as quickly as possible by tion, and to intensive studying.41 It is interesting
pressing a corresponding key on the keyboard. that these more-complex conceptual-procedural
Because the stimuli were generated in an order tasks often show REM sleep relationships,
defined by a probabilistic finite-state grammar, whereas more basic procedural tasks benefit
Author's personal copy
Sleep and Memory 21

mainly from NREM sleep. The above-mentioned There is a general consensus that NREM sleep,
findings provide encouraging evidence that especially SWS, is important for the consolidation
sleep-based processes can aid in procedural of hippocampus-dependent episodic and spatial
memory consolidation, not only for basic forms memories.16 In a landmark study, Plihal and
of sensory and motor memory in humans but Born50 assessed the recall of word pairs (an
also for complex procedural and conceptual episodic memory task) and improvement in mirror
knowledge. Moreover, these findings indicate tracing (a procedural memory task) after retention
that the consolidation of familiar skills, or those intervals of early sleep (the first 3–4 hours of the
that are similar to other well-learned skills, may sleep cycle), which, as noted in the previous
be reliant on NREM sleep stages (particularly sections, is dominated by SWS, and late sleep
stage 2 NREM sleep), whereas REM sleep may (the last 3–4 hours of the sleep cycle), which is
be required for the integration of new concepts dominated by REM sleep. Recall of word pairs
or skills with preexisting information stored in improved more significantly after a 3-hour sleep
memory.42 This is an important concept that period rich in SWS than after a 3-hour sleep period
warrants future investigation. rich in REM sleep or a 3-hour period of wakeful-
Clearly, sleep is crucial to the consolidation of ness. Mirror tracing, on the other hand, improved
procedural memory. Although both the stabilization significantly more after a 3-hour sleep period rich
of explicitly learned motor skills and off-line in REM sleep than after 3 hours spent either in
improvements in pure implicit tasks may evolve SWS or awake.
over time spent awake,43,44 the posttraining Similarly, using a nap paradigm, Tucker and
enhancement of motor skills seems to be exclu- colleagues51 found that naps containing only
sively dependent on sleep.27 Participants with NREM sleep enhanced memory for word pairs
insomnia show impaired sleep-related procedural but did not benefit mirror tracing. After an after-
memory consolidation compared with participants noon training session, performance on these tasks
with undisturbed sleep,45 and schizophrenia, which was assessed after a 6-hour delay, either with or
is associated with changes in sleep, particularly in without an intervening nap. The subjects who
stage 2 NREM sleep spindles,46,47 is also associ- were allowed a nap not only recalled more word
ated with procedural memory deficits.48 More pairs than the subjects who remained awake
recently, Dresler and colleagues49 demonstrated (Fig. 6) but also showed a weak correlation
that off-line procedural memory consolidation is between improved recall and the amount of SWS
also disturbed in depression, another condition in the nap.
associated with changes in sleep architecture. These results are consistent with neurophysio-
logical evidence derived from electrophysiological
SLEEP BENEFITS EXPLICIT MEMORIES studies in rodents, which demonstrate that
patterns of hippocampal place cell activity first
This section describes the relationship between seen during waking exploration are later reex-
sleep and the consolidation of explicit memories. pressed during postlearning SWS.52–54 Similarly,

Fig. 6. (A, B) A brief daytime nap benefits episodic memory. Note that the nap (which did not contain REM sleep)
benefited episodic, but not procedural memory. Asterisks indicate significant differences between groups. (From
Tucker MA, Hirota Y, Wamsley EJ, et al. A daytime nap containing solely non-REM sleep enhances declarative but
not procedural memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2006;86(2):241–7; with permission.)
Author's personal copy
22 Payne

several neuroimaging studies in humans have sug- during NREM SWS, a process that in turn
gested an important role for SWS in hippocampus- enhances behavioral performance.
dependent memory consolidation. The first of
these studies investigated performance on a hippo-
campally-dependent virtual maze task.55 Daytime SLEEP BENEFITS EMOTIONAL MEMORY
learning of the task was associated with hippo- CONSOLIDATION
campal activity. During posttraining sleep, there
was a reemergence of hippocampal activation, Sleep also contributes to the consolidation of
and this activation occurred specifically during emotional episodic memories. As noted in the
SWS. The most compelling finding, however, is previous section, sleep clearly benefits memory
that the increase in hippocampal activation seen for neutral episodic memory materials across
during posttraining SWS was proportional to the both verbal and spatial modalities.5,16 However,
amount of improvement seen the next day. This when emotional (especially emotionally negative)
finding suggests that the reexpression of hippo- episodic items are intermixed with neutral items
campal activation during SWS reflects the off-line in a study, sleep disproportionately benefits the
reprocessing of spatial episodic memory traces, consolidation of emotional memories relative to
which in turn leads to the plastic changes under- neutral memories. For example, Hu and
lying the improvement in memory performance colleagues58 examined the effect of a full night’s
seen the next day. Similarly, Rasch and sleep on both axes of emotional affect, valence
colleagues56 exposed human subjects to an odor (positive/negative) and arousal (high/low), across
while they were learning object-location pairings both “remember” and “know” judgments of
in a task similar to the memory game “concentra- memory for pictures. Results showed that a night
tion;” subjects who were reexposed to the odor of sleep improved memory accuracy for emotion-
during SWS (but not REM sleep) showed enhanced ally arousing pictures relative to an equivalent
hippocampal activity and enhanced memory for period of daytime wakefulness, but only for know
the memory pairings. judgments. No differences were observed for
Another study57 investigated the time course of remember judgments. Moreover, memory bias
episodic memory consolidation across 90 days. changed across a night of sleep relative to wake,
Subjects studied 360 photographs of landscapes such that subjects became more conservative
and were then tested on subsets of the photo- when making remember judgments, especially
graphs either after a nap the same day or after 2, for emotionally arousing pictures. No bias differ-
30, or 90 days. Before each test, subjects studied ences were observed for know judgments
80 new pictures and then were tested on 80 of the between sleep and wake.
original pictures and the 80 new ones as well as REM sleep may be particularly important for
80 pictures that they had never seen before. All the consolidation of emotional memories, which
memory retrieval sessions occurred during fMRI rely critically on the amygdala for their
scanning. After the initial 90-minute nap, stage 2 consolidation.7,59 Wagner and colleagues59 found
sleep was positively correlated with successful that 3 hours of late night REM-rich sleep (but not 3
recall of both remote and recent items, indicating hours of early night slow wave–rich sleep or 3
a nonspecific benefit of stage 2 NREM sleep on hours of wakefulness) facilitated memory for nega-
episodic memory. This is an intriguing finding, given tive arousing narratives, an effect that could still be
that stage 2 is when sleep spindles are most prom- observed years later when the subjects were re-
inent (see section on neurophysiological and contacted for a follow-up memory test.60 Consis-
neurochemical evidence for sleep’s role in memory tent with these findings, the amygdala and
consolidation later). SWS, on the other hand, was hippocampus are among the most active brain
correlated only with memory for remote (but not regions during REM sleep, with some evidence
recent) items. Because performance on remote suggesting that they are more active during REM
items increased with longer SWS duration, but sleep than during wakefulness.61 This observation
performance for recent items did not, the effect suggests that emotional memory processing may
on memory performance for remote items cannot be a primary goal of REM sleep. Moreover, several
be explained by a general effect of SWS on memory studies have correlated features of REM sleep,
retrieval processes. These findings strongly including oscillatory activity in the theta frequency
suggest that episodic memories can undergo initial band range,17 with enhanced emotional memory
consolidation within a rather short time frame and consolidation.10,62 These findings strongly
that this consolidation is promoted by SWS. suggest a role for sleep, especially REM sleep, in
These observations suggest that learning trig- the processing of memory for emotional
gers the reactivation of episodic memory traces experiences.
Author's personal copy
Sleep and Memory 23

SLEEP TRANSFORMS MEMORIES IN negative arousing memories change across


USEFUL WAYS periods of sleep versus wakefulness. Emotional
scenes could be stored as intact units, suffering
The findings reviewed in the previous sections some forgetting over time but retaining the same
provide compelling evidence that sleep plays an relative vividness for all components. Alternatively,
important role in solidifying experience into long- the components of an experience could undergo
term memory in a veridical manner, more or less differential memory processing, perhaps with
true to its form at initial encoding.63 However, it a selective emphasis on what is most salient and
has long been known that memories change with worthy of remembering.
the passage of time,64 suggesting that the process Participants viewed scenes depicting negative
of consolidation does not always yield exact repre- or neutral objects embedded on neutral back-
sentations of past experiences. On the face of it, grounds at 9 AM or 9 PM. Twelve hours later, after
this process may seem strikingly maladaptive, a day spent awake or a night including at least
yet such flexibility in memory representation 6 hours of sleep, they were tested on their memory
allows the emergence of key cognitive abilities, for objects and backgrounds separately to
such as generalization and inference,65 future examine how these individual components of
thought,66 and selective preservation of useful emotional memories change across periods of
information extracted from a barrage of incoming sleep and wake (see Fig. 7 for example stimuli).
stimulations and experiences.67,68 Consistent Daytime wakefulness led to forgetting of nega-
with these ideas, growing evidence suggests that tive arousing scenes in their entirety, with both
sleep does more than simply consolidating objects and backgrounds being forgotten at
memories in a veridical form; it also transforms similar rates. Sleep, however, led to a selective
them in ways rendering the memories less accu- preservation of negative objects but not their
rate in some respects but more useful and adap- accompanying backgrounds, suggesting that the
tive in the long run. Sleep leads to flexible 2 components undergo differential processing
restructuring of memory traces so that insights during sleep. This finding suggests that rather
can be made,69 inferences can be drawn,64 and than preserving intact representations of scenes
both integration and abstraction can occur.68 In the sleeping brain effectively unbinds scenes to
each of these cases, sleep confers a flexibility to consolidate only their most emotionally salient,
memory that may at times be more advantageous and perhaps adaptive, elements (see68,69 for addi-
than a literal representation of experience. tional examples of potential unbinding during
As a specific example of such qualitative sleep).
changes in memory representation, recent studies Paralleling these behavioral findings, a recent
demonstrate that sleep transforms the emotional fMRI study provided evidence that a single night
memory trace. Payne and colleagues7,67 exam- of sleep is sufficient to provoke changes in the
ined how the different components of complex emotional memory circuitry, leading to increased

Fig. 7. Example stimuli for emotional trade-off task, where a neutral (intact car, A) or negative (crashed car, B)
object is embedded on a neutral background scene (street). (From Payne JD, Kensinger EA. Sleep’s role in the
consolidation of emotional episodic memories. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2010;19(5):290–5; with permission.)
Author's personal copy
24 Payne

activity within the amygdala and the ventromedial sleep stages but are not perfectly correlated with
prefrontal cortex and strengthened connectivity them. For example, SWS is associated with
between the amygdala and both the hippocampus cortical slow oscillations (slow, <1 Hz oscillatory
and ventromedial prefrontal cortex during activity during SWS), sleep spindles (faster,
retrieval.70 These findings are consistent with 11–16 Hz, bursts of coherent brain activity), and
a study by Sterpenich and colleagues71 and hippocampal sharp wave-ripple complexes
suggest that sleep strengthens the modulatory (approximately 200 Hz), all of which have been
effect of the amygdala on other regions of the associated with episodic memory consolidation.
emotional memory network as memories undergo Indeed, the co-occurrence of these electrophysio-
consolidation.14 Whether these selective effects of logical events may underlie the coordinated infor-
sleep on emotional memory consolidation depend mation flow back and forth between the
on REM sleep is an interesting question for future hippocampus and neocortex as memories are
research. integrated within neocortical long-term storage
sites.13
BEYOND SLEEP STAGES For example, slow oscillations are intensified
when SWS is preceded by a learning
The above-mentioned results should not be taken experience73–75 and hippocampus-dependent
to mean that SWS strictly mediates the consolida- memories are specifically enhanced when slow
tion of episodic memories, whereas REM sleep oscillations are induced during SWS by transcrani-
only mediates the consolidation of procedural al electrical stimulation at 0.75 Hz (but not 5 Hz).76
and emotional memories. Matters are clearly not Several human studies have shown a correlation
so simple. As mentioned in the previous sections, between hippocampus-dependent episodic learn-
improvement on a visual discrimination task ing and cortical sleep spindles. In one such study,77
depends on SWS as well as REM,24 and improve- subjects studied a long list of unrelated word pairs 1
ment on a motor task correlates with stage 2 hour before sleep, on 2 separate occasions at least
NREM sleep.29 Moreover, emotionally charged a week apart. In one case, they were instructed to
episodic memories may rely on both REM sleep imagine a relationship between the 2 nominally
and SWS for their consolidation.7,72 unrelated words, whereas in the other they were
There are 2 possible interpretations of these simply asked to count the number of letters contain-
apparent contradictions. First, the sleep stage ing curved lines in each word pair. Such instructions
dependency of these various memory tasks may lead to deep hippocampally mediated encoding
depend on aspects of the task other than simply and shallow cortically mediated encoding, respec-
whether they are episodic or procedural, perhaps tively. During the subsequent nights of sleep,
depending more on the intensity of training, the subjects showed significantly higher spindle densi-
emotional salience of the task, or even the manner ties on the nights after deep encoding, averaging
in which information is encoded (eg, deep vs 34% more spindles in the first 90 minutes of sleep.
shallow encoding or implicit vs explicit). The Moreover, sleep spindle density was positively
second possibility involves an inherent oversimpli- correlated both with immediate recall tested in the
fication in correlating performance improvements final stage of training and with recall the next
with sleep stages as they are classically defined. morning, after sleep. Thus, those who learned better
Indeed, mounting evidence points to several elec- had more spindles the following night and those
trophysiological, neurotransmitter, and neuroen- with more spindles showed a greater performance
docrine mechanisms that may underlie these gain the next morning.78–80
effects and that do not necessarily correlate with Hippocampal network oscillations such as
any single sleep stage, and sleep staging, as it sharp wave-ripple complexes may also help
has been defined for 40 years, may not capture promote the synaptic plasticity necessary for
all the key elements that lead to memory consoli- memory consolidation. Such events can accom-
dation enhanced by sleep. pany the reactivation of hippocampal neuron
ensembles that were activated during prior waking
NEUROPHYSIOLOGICAL AND training. Moreover, inducing long-term potentia-
NEUROCHEMICAL EVIDENCE FOR SLEEP’S tion, a neurophysiological mechanism of learning
ROLE IN MEMORY CONSOLIDATION and memory, can trigger the generation of sharp
wave-ripple complexes in the rat hippocampus,81
Each of the above-mentioned sleep stages is char- suggesting that strengthened synaptic coupling
acterized by a unique collection of electrophysio- can lead to neurons firing synchronously during
logical, neurotransmitter, and neuroendocrine formation of sharp wave-ripples complexes. Con-
properties that tend to overlap with the different necting these events to behavior, Eschenko and
Author's personal copy
Sleep and Memory 25

colleagues82 demonstrated that rats learning is at its nadir during early-night slow wave–rich
odor-reward associations produced an increase sleep and reaches its zenith during late-night
in the number and size of ripple events for 2 hours REM-rich sleep. Indeed, the difference between
during subsequent SWS. Moreover, episodic the cortisol level in the blood at sleep onset and
memory consolidation in human epileptic patients at awakening is so great that the interpretation of
correlates with the number of ripples recorded cortisol blood levels is meaningless without
from the major output regions of the hippocampus knowing exactly when the sample was taken.89–91
(perirhinal and entorhinal cortices),83 and selective Moreover, the secretion of cortisol is not contin-
disruption of hippocampal ripples by electric stim- uous but composed of gradually increasing peaks
ulation during postlearning rest phases in rats that tend to coincide with REM sleep (Fig. 8). REM
impairs formation of long-lasting spatial memories, sleep thus tends to co-occur with cortisol
suggesting that ripples could have a causal role in elevations.90,92
sleep-based memory consolidation.84 The early night reduction in ACh and cortisol
Recent work also suggests that there is a close levels may be necessary for hippocampus-
temporal relationship between the occurrence of dependent memories to undergo effective consol-
slow oscillations, spindles, and sharp wave- idation because experimentally elevating the
ripple complexes during SWS that may coordinate levels of either substance during early sleep
bidirectional information flow between the hippo- impairs performance on episodic memory tasks.
campus and neocortex as memories are inte- Gais and Born93 trained subjects on word pair
grated within long-term storage sites. task and mirror tracing tasks before 3 hours of
nocturnal sleep or wakefulness during which they
NEUROTRANSMITTERS AND received a placebo or an infusion of the cholines-
NEUROHORMONES terase inhibitor physostigmine (which increases
cholinergic tone). When tested after 3 hours of
There is also evidence to suggest that nocturnal early sleep rich in SWS, recall on the paired asso-
changes in neurotransmitter and neurohormone ciates task was markedly impaired in the physo-
levels contribute to memory consolidation. ACh, stigmine group, while the procedural memory
norepinephrine, 5-HT, and cortisol play important performance was unaffected. Using a similar
roles both in modulating sleep85 and memory design, Plihal and Born94 showed that when
function.86–88 Cortisol levels, for instance, follow cortisol was infused during the early SWS-rich
a marked circadian rhythm, whereby the hormone interval, retention of episodic information that is

Fig. 8. The relationship between sleep stage architecture and circulating levels of growth hormone and cortisol.
Note both the linear increase in cortisol levels across the night and also the cortisol peaks riding on top of the
REM periods.
Author's personal copy
26 Payne

normally facilitated during this time was impaired. memory processing. Thus, the neurobiological
Thus, enhancing plasma cortisol concentrations properties of early sleep and late sleep, as
during early sleep eradicated the benefit typically opposed to SWS and REM sleep per se, may be
observed for episodic memory while leaving essential for the consolidation of different types
procedural memory unimpaired (Fig. 9). of memory.
Plihal and Born94 concluded that because In line with the above-mentioned findings, eleva-
episodic, but not procedural, memory relies on tion of cortisol levels during wakefulness can also
hippocampal function, cortisol inhibition during impair performance on episodic memory.89,95
early nocturnal sleep is necessary for episodic The cortisol level in the Plihal and Born94 study
memory consolidation. Thus, because cortisol (15.2 1 0.68 mg/dL) was elevated just enough to
release is normally inhibited during early night mimic the late night peak of circadian cortisol
periods dense in SWS, this time window may activity and is proportionate to the amount of
provide the ideal physiologic environment for cortisol typically released in response to a mild to
episodic memory consolidation. REM sleep, on moderate stressor (approximately 10–30 mg/dL)
the other hand, is an inefficient time to consolidate and is a sufficient dose to disrupt episodic memory
episodes because of the deleterious effect of function during wakefulness,89,92,96,97 particularly
elevated cortisol on hippocampus-dependent when administered prior to consolidation or at

Fig. 9. (A, top) Experimental design for examining the effect of cortisol on memory consolidation during a 3-hour
period of early nocturnal sleep rich in SWS, illustrated by an individual sleep profile. Before sleep, subjects
learned episodic and procedural memory tasks to a criterion. Recall was tested 15 to 30 minutes after awakening.
Infusion of cortisol or placebo began at 11 PM and was discontinued after 2.5 hours. (Bottom) Mean plasma
cortisol concentrations during administration of placebo (dotted line) and cortisol (solid line). (B) Cortisol infu-
sion during sleep, compared with placebo, impaired hippocampus-dependent episodic memories for word pairs
across sleep (top), but did not affect hippocampus-independent procedural memory (speed in mirror tracing,
bottom). N 5 14, *P<.05, **P<.01. Aw, awake; IV, intravenous, Li, lights; S, stage; W, wake. (Data from Plihal W,
Born J. Memory consolidation in human sleep depends on inhibition of glucocorticoid release. Neuroreport
1999;10(13):2741–7.)
Author's personal copy
Sleep and Memory 27

retrieval.95,98 Elevations in cortisol levels seen remain to be answered about sleep-dependent


during late night REM sleep may help explain why memory consolidation in the clinical realm. Future
replay of episodic memories in REM sleep research in the field should thus target clinical
dreaming is so scarce99 and perhaps also why conditions that are characterized by sleep deficits,
dreams are difficult to remember on assessing to what degree sleep dysfunction is
awakening.87,100 associated with impaired memory consolidation,
In addition to cortisol, other hormones (eg, and, equally importantly, determining whether
growth hormone) are also known to affect memory sleep treatment will yield cognitive, emotional,
function in the waking state and vary across sleep, and physical benefits.
suggesting that they might modulate sleep-based
memory consolidation as well. Although initial
studies of growth hormone have failed to find REFERENCES
such an effect,101 further investigation of the
neurochemistry underlying the relationship 1. Hobson JA, McCarley RW, Wyzinski PW. Sleep
between sleep and memory consolidation is cycle oscillation: reciprocal discharge by two
a productive avenue for future research. Indeed, brainstem neuronal groups. Science 1975;189:
it seems especially important to forge ahead into 55–8.
precisely this neuromodulatory realm, in which 2. Walker MP, Stickgold R. Sleep, memory and plas-
the chemical basis of the sleep/memory consoli- ticity. Annu Rev Psychol 2006;57:139–66.
dation connection can be examined. 3. Stickgold R, Hobson JA, Fosse R. Sleep, learning,
and dreams: off-line memory reprocessing.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS Science 2001;294:1052–7.
4. McGaugh JL. Memory – a century of consolidation.
Over the past 10 years, the field of sleep and Science 2000;287:248–51.
memory has grown exponentially, with reports of 5. Diekelmann S, Born J. The memory function of
sleep-memory interactions emerging from myriad sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci 2010;11:114–26.
disciplines and ranging from cellular and molecular 6. Payne JD, Ellenbogen JM, Walker MP, et al. The
studies in animals to behavioral and neuroimaging role of sleep in memory consolidation. In:
studies in humans. Correspondingly little is known, Byrne JH, editor. Concise learning and memory:
however, about sleep-dependent memory consol- the editor’s selection. Oxford (UK): Elsevier Press;
idation in diverse clinical conditions that are asso- 2008. p. 547–69.
ciated with sleep impairment. These conditions 7. Payne JD, Kensinger EA. Sleep’s role in the consol-
range from physical conditions, such as sleep idation of emotional episodic memories. Curr Dir
apnea, to psychiatric conditions, such as depres- Psychol Sci 2010;19(5):290–5.
sion, and each condition may affect sleep- 8. Stickgold R. Sleep-dependent memory consolida-
dependent memory processing in profound but tion. Nature 2005;437:1272–8.
different ways. 9. Smith C. Sleep states, memory processes and
For example, obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) is synaptic plasticity. Behav Brain Res 1995;78:
characterized by repetitive breathing cessations 49–56.
during sleep that can produce hundreds of short 10. Walker MP. The role of sleep in cognition and
EEG arousals each night. Although the cognitive emotion. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009;1156:168–97.
deficits associated with OSA are well 11. Schacter DL, Tulving E. Memory systems 1994.
documented102 and the hypoxia associated with Cambridge (MA): MIT Press; 1994.
OSA is thought to alter brain regions (eg, hippo- 12. Hamann SB. Cognitive and neural mechanisms
campus) that underlie the cognitive deficits,103,104 of emotional memory. Trends Cogn Sci 2001;5:
very little research has directly assessed decre- 394–400.
ments to sleep-dependent memory consolidation 13. Moscovitch M, Rosenbaum RS, Gilboa A, et al.
in this population (see105 for a notable exception). Functional neuroanatomy of remote episodic,
Similarly, depression and schizophrenia, both of semantic and spatial memory: a unified account
which are associated with marked changes in based on multiple trace theory. J Anat 2005;
sleep architecture,106 have recently been associ- 207(1):35–66.
ated with disrupted procedural memory 14. McGaugh JL. The amygdala modulates the consol-
consolidation,48,49 yet a careful study of sleep- idation of memories of emotionally arousing experi-
related episodic, and especially emotional, ences. Annu Rev Neurosci 2004;27:1–28.
memory consolidation in these populations have 15. Giuditta A, Ambrosini MV, Montagnese P, et al. The
yet to be conducted. These are but 3 examples sequential hypothesis of the function of sleep. Be-
of the vital and potentially fruitful questions that hav Brain Res 1995;69:157–66.
Author's personal copy
28 Payne

16. Marshall L, Born J. The contribution of sleep to 34. Hennevin E, Hars B, Maho C, et al. Processing of
hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation. learned information in paradoxical sleep: relevance
Trends Cogn Sci 2007;11:442–50. for memory. Behav Brain Res 1995;69:125–35.
17. Nishida M, Pearsall J, Buckner RL, et al. REM 35. Smith C, Wong PT. Paradoxical sleep increases
sleep, prefrontal theta and the consolidation of predict successful learning in a complex operant
human emotional memory. Cereb Cortex 2009;19: task. Behav Neurosci 1991;105(2):282–8.
1158–66. 36. Smith C, Young J, Young W. Prolonged increases in
18. Walker MP, Stickgold R. Overnight alchemy: sleep- paradoxical sleep during and after avoidance-task
dependent memory evolution. Nat Rev Neurosci acquisition. Sleep 1980;3(1):67–81.
2010;11:218. 37. Maquet P, Laureys S, Peigneux P, et al. Experi-
19. Karni A, Sagi D. The time course of learning a visual ence-dependent changes in cerebral activation
skill. Nature 1993;365:250–2. during human REM sleep. Nat Neurosci 2000;
20. Karni A, Tanne D, Rubenstein BS, et al. Dependence 3(8):831–6.
on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a percep- 38. Peigneux P, Laureys S, Fuchs S, et al. Learned
tual skill. Science 1994;265(5172):679–82. material content and acquisition level modulate
21. Stickgold R. Human studies of sleep and off-line cerebral reactivation during posttraining rapid-
memory reprocessing. In: Maquet P, Smith C, eye-movements sleep. Neuroimage 2003;20(1):
Stickgold R, editors. Sleep and plasticity. London: 125–34.
Oxford University Press; 2003. p. 41–65. 39. Datta S. Avoidance task training potentiates phasic
22. Stickgold R, James L, Hobson JA. Visual discrimi- pontine-wave density in the rat: a mechanism for
nation learning requires post-training sleep. Nat sleep-dependent plasticity. J Neurosci 2000;
Neurosci 2000;2(12):1237–8. 20(22):8607–13.
23. Stickgold R, Whidbee D, Schirmer B, et al. Visual 40. Datta S, Mavanji V, Ulloor J, et al. Activation of phasic
discrimination task improvement: a multi-step pontine-wave generator prevents rapid eye move-
process occurring during sleep. J Cogn Neurosci ment sleep deprivation-induced learning impair-
2000;12:246–54. ment in the rat: a mechanism for sleep-dependent
24. Gais S, Plihal W, Wagner U, et al. Early sleep trig- plasticity. J Neurosci 2004;24(6):1416–27.
gers memory for early visual discrimination skills. 41. Smith C. Sleep states and memory processes in
Nat Neurosci 2000;3(12):1335–9. humans: procedural versus declarative memory
25. Mednick S, Nakayama K, Stickgold R. Sleep systems. Sleep Med Rev 2001;5(6):491–506.
dependent learning: a nap is as good as a night. 42. Greenberg R, Pearlman CA. Cutting the REM
Nat Neurosci 2003;6:697–8. nerve: an approach to the adaptive function of
26. Mednick SC, Nakayama K, Cantero JL, et al. The REM sleep. Perspect Biol Med 1974;17:513–21.
restorative effect of naps on perceptual deteriora- 43. Robertson EM, Cohen DA. Understanding consoli-
tion. Nat Neurosci 2002;5:677–81. dation through the architecture of memories.
27. Walker M, Brakefield T, Morgan A, et al. Practice Neuroscientist 2006;12:261–71.
with sleep makes perfect: sleep dependent motor 44. Robertson EM, Pascual-Leone A, Press DZ. Aware-
skill learning. Neuron 2002;35:205–11. ness modifies the skill-learning benefits of sleep.
28. Fogel S, Jacob J, Smith C. Increased sleep spindle Curr Biol 2004;14:208–12.
activity following simple motor procedural learning 45. Nissen C, Kloepfer C, Nofzinger EA, et al.
in humans. Actas de Fisiologı́a 2001;7:123. Impaired sleep-related memory consolidation in
29. Smith C, MacNeill C. Impaired motor memory for primary insomnia – a pilot study. Sleep 2006;29:
a pursuit rotor task following Stage 2 sleep loss in 1068–73.
college students. J Sleep Res 1994;3:206–13. 46. Manoach D, Thakkar KN, Stroynowski E, et al.
30. Tweed S, Aubrey JB, Nader R, et al. Deprivation of Reduced overnight consolidation of procedural
REM sleep or stage 2 sleep differentially affects learning in chronic medicated schizophrenia is
cognitive procedural and motor procedural related to specific sleep stages. J Psychiatr Res
memory. Sleep 1999;22:S241. 2009;44(2):112–20.
31. Sejnowski TJ, Destexhe A. Why do we sleep? Brain 47. Maquet P. The role of sleep in learning and
Res 2000;886(1–2):208–23. memory. Science 2001;294(5544):1048–52.
32. Kuriyama K, Stickgold R, Walker MP. Sleep-depen- 48. Manoach DS, Cain MS, Vangel MG, et al. A failure
dent learning and motor-skill complexity. Learn of sleep-dependent procedural learning in chronic,
Mem 2004;11(6):705–13. medicated schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2004;56:
33. Maquet P, Laureys S, Perrin F, et al. Festina lente: 951–6.
evidences for fast and slow learning processes 49. Dresler M, Kluge M, Genzel L, et al. Impaired off-
and a role for sleep in human motor skill learning. line memory consolidation in depression. Eur Neu-
Learn Mem 2003;10(4):237–9. ropsychopharmacol 2010;20:533–61.
Author's personal copy
Sleep and Memory 29

50. Plihal W, Born J. Effects of early and late nocturnal 67. Payne JD, Stickgold R, Swanberg K, et al. Sleep
sleep on declarative and procedural memory. preferentially enhances memory for emotional
J Cogn Neurosci 1997;9(4):534–47. components of scenes. Psychol Sci 2008;19:781–8.
51. Tucker MA, Hirota Y, Wamsley EJ, et al. A daytime 68. Payne JD, Schacter DL, Tucker MA, et al. The role
nap containing solely non-REM sleep enhances of sleep in false memory formation. Neurobiol
declarative but not procedural memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2009;92:327–34.
Learn Mem 2006;86(2):241–7. 69. Wagner U, Gais S, Haider H, et al. Sleep inspires
52. Ji D, Wilson MA. Coordinated memory replay in the insight. Nature 2004;427:352–5.
visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep. Nat 70. Payne JD, Kensinger EA. Sleep leads to qualitative
Neurosci 2007;10:100–7. changes in the emotional memory trace: evidence
53. Wilson MA, McNaughton BL. Reactivation of hippo- from fMRI. J Cogn Neurosci 2010. DOI:10.1162/
campal ensemble memories during sleep. Science jocn.2010.21526.
1994;265:676–9. 71. Sterpenich V, Albouy G, Darsaud A, et al. Sleep
54. O’Neil J, Pleydell-Bouverie B, Dupret D, et al. Play it promotes the neural reorganization of remote
again: reactivation of waking experience and emotional memory. J Neurosci 2009;29:5143–52.
memory. Trends Neurosci 2010;33(5):220–9. 72. Payne JD, Nadel L. Sleep, dreams, and memory
55. Peigneux P, Laureys S, Fuchs S, et al. Are spatial consolidation: the role of the stress hormone
memories strengthened in the human hippo- cortisol. Learn Mem 2004;11:671–8.
campus during slow wave sleep? Neuron 2004; 73. Huber R, Ghilardi MF, Massimini M, et al. Local
44:535–45. sleep and learning. Nature 2004;430:78–81.
56. Rasch B, Buchel C, Gias S, et al. Odor cues during 74. Molle M, Marshall L, Gais S, et al. Learning
slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory increases human electroencephalographic coher-
consolidation. Science 2007;315:1426–9. ence during subsequent slow sleep oscillations.
57. Takashima A, Petersson KM, Rutters F, et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2004;101:13963–8.
Declarative memory consolidation in humans: 75. Molle M, Eschenko O, Gais S, et al. The influence of
a prospective functional magnetic resonance learning on sleep slow oscillations and associated
imaging study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006; spindles and ripples in humans and rats. Eur J
103(3):756–61. Neurosci 2009;29:1071–81.
58. Hu P, Stylos-Allan M, Walker M. Sleep facilitates 76. Marshall L, Helgadottir H, Molle M, et al. Boosting
consolidation of emotional declarative memory. slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory.
Psychol Sci 2006;17:891–8. Nature 2006;444:610–3.
59. Wagner U, Gais S, Born J. Emotional memory 77. Gais S, Molle M, Helms K, et al. Learning-depen-
formation is enhanced across sleep intervals with dent increases in sleep spindle density.
high amounts of rapid eye movement sleep. Learn J Neurosci 2002;22(15):6830–4.
Mem 2001;8:112–9. 78. Schmidt C, Peigneux P, Muto V, et al. Encoding
60. Wagner U, Hallschmid M, Rasch B, et al. Brief difficulty promotes postlearning changes in sleep
sleep after learning keeps emotional memories spindle activity during napping. J Neurosci 2006;
alive for years. Biol Psychiatry 2006;60:788–90. 26:8976–82.
61. Maquet P, Péters JM, Aerts J, et al. Functional 79. Clemens Z, Fabo D, Halasz P. Twenty-four hours
neuroanatomy of human rapid-eye-movement retention of visuospatial memory correlates with
sleep and dreaming. Nature 1996;383:163–6. the number of parietal sleep spindles. Neurosci
62. Greenberg R, Pearlman C, Schwartz W, et al. Lett 2006;403:52–6.
Memory, emotion, and REM sleep. J Abnorm 80. Schabus M, Hodlmoser K, Gruber G, et al. Sleep
Psychol 1983;92:378–81. spindle-related activity in the human EEG and its
63. Ellenbogen JM, Payne JD, Stickgold R. Sleep’s role relation to general cognitive and learning abilities.
in declarative memory consolidation: passive, Eur J Neurosci 2006;23(7):1738–46.
permissive, active or none? Curr Opin Neurobiol 81. Behrens CJ, van den Boom LP, et al. Induction of
2006;16:716–22. sharp wave-ripple complexes in vitro and reorgani-
64. Bartlett FC. Remembering: a study in experimental zation of hippocampal networks. Nat Neurosci
and social psychology. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge 2005;8:1560–7.
University Press; 1932. 82. Eschenko O, Ramadan W, Molle M, et al. Sustained
65. Ellenbogen JM, Hu P, Payne JD, et al. Human rela- increase in hippocampal sharp-wave ripple activity
tional memory requires time and sleep. Proc Natl during slow-wave sleep after learning. Learn Mem
Acad Sci U S A 2007;104:7723–8. 2008;15:222–8.
66. Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL. Episodic 83. Axmacher N, Elger CE, Fell J. Ripples in the medial
simulation of future events. Ann N Y Acad Sci temporal lobe are relevant for human memory
2008;1124:39–60. consolidation. Brain 2008;131:1806–17.
Author's personal copy
30 Payne

84. Girardeau G, Benchenane K, Wiener SI, et al. health and disease. Front Neuroendocrinol 2009;
Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples 30:358–70.
impairs spatial memory. Nat Neurosci 2009;12: 96. Kirschbaum C, Wolf OT, May M, et al. Stress and
1222–3. treatment induced elevations of cortisol levels
85. Hobson JA, Pace-Schott EF. The cognitive neuro- associated with impaired declarative memory in
science of sleep: neuronal systems, conscious- healthy adults. Life Sci 1996;58:1475–83.
ness and learning. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;3: 97. Kirschbaum C, Pirke K, Hellhammer D. The ‘Trier
679–93. Social Stress Test’ - A tool for investigating psycho-
86. Cahill L, McGaugh JL. Mechanisms of emotional biological stress responses in a laboratory setting.
arousal and lasting declarative memory. Trends Neuropsychobiology 1993;28:76–81.
Neurosci 1998;21:294–9. 98. de Quervain DJF, Roozendaal B, Nitsch RM, et al.
87. Payne JD. Memory consolidation, the diurnal Acute cortisone administration impairs retrieval of
rhythm of cortisol, and the nature of dreams: long-term declarative memory in humans. Nat Neu-
a new hypothesis. Int Rev Neurobiol 2010;92: rosci 2000;3:313–4.
101–36. 99. Fosse MJ, Fosse R, Hobson JA, et al. Dreaming
88. Hasselmo ME. Neuromodulation: acetylcholine and and episodic memory: a functional dissociation?
memory consolidation. Trends Cogn Sci 1999;3: J Cogn Neurosci 2003;15:1–9.
351–9. 100. Hobson JA. The dreaming brain. New York: Basic
89. Payne JD, Nadel L, Britton WB, et al. The biopsy- Books; 1988.
chology of trauma and memory. In: Reisberg D, 101. Gais S, Hullemann P, Hallschmid M, et al. Sleep-
Hertel P, editors. Emotion and memory. Oxford dependent surges in growth hormones do not
(UK): Oxford University Press; 2004. p. 76–127. contribute to sleep-dependent memory consolida-
90. Lavie P. The enchanted world of sleep. New Haven tion. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2006;31(6):786–91.
(CT): Yale University Press; 1996. 102. Saunamaki T, Jehkonen M. A review of executive
91. Weitzman ED, Fukushima D, Nogeire C, et al. functions in obstructive sleep apnea syndrome.
Twenty-four hour pattern of the episodic secretion Acta Neurol Scand 2007;115:1–11.
of cortisol in normal subjects. J Clin Endocrinol 103. Meerlo P, Mistlberger RE, Jacobs BL, et al. New
Metab 1971;33:14–22. neurons in the adult brain: the role of sleep and
92. Wagner U, Born J. Memory consolidation during consequences of sleep loss. Sleep Med Rev
sleep: interactive effects of sleep stages and HPA 2009;13:187–94.
regulation. Stress 2008;11:28–41. 104. Row BW. Intermittent hypoxia and cognitive func-
93. Gais S, Born J. Low acetylcholine during slow- tion: implications from chronic animal models.
wave sleep is critical for declarative memory Adv Exp Med Biol 2007;618:51–67.
consolidation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2004; 105. Kloepfer C, Riemann D, Nofzinger EA, et al.
101(7):2140–4. Memory before and after sleep in patients with
94. Plihal W, Born J. Memory consolidation in human moderate obstructive sleep apnea. J Clin Sleep
sleep depends on inhibition of glucocorticoid Med 2009;5:540–8.
release. Neuroreport 1999;10(13):2741–7. 106. Armitage R. Sleep and circadian rhythms in
95. de Quervain DJF, Aerni A, Schelling G, et al. mood disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2007;
Glucocorticoids and the regulation of memory in 115(Suppl 433):104–15.

You might also like